You are on page 1of 17

JSPR

Article

Passion for life: Selfexpansion and passionate


love across the life span

Journal of Social and


Personal Relationships
2014, Vol. 31(7) 958974
The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0265407513515618
spr.sagepub.com

Virgil L. Sheets
Indiana State University, USA

Abstract
It is commonly believed that passionate love diminishes over time even as companionate
love may grow. According to the self-expansion model (Aron and Aron (1986) Love and
the expansion of the self: Understanding attraction and satisfaction. New York, NY: Hemisphere Publishing Co/Harper & Row Publishers), this change may reflect changes in
opportunities for self-expansion in the relationship. Early in relationships, as partners
continuously learn new things about each other, self-expansionwhich occurs through
the integration of the qualities and characteristics of the partner into oneselfgenerates
passion for ones partner. Later, as ones partner is completely included in the self, selfexpansion opportunities diminish and less passion is generated. From this perspective, if
self-expansion could be sustained, so could passionate love. Over 500 adults in a Midwestern community responded to a telephone survey about their romantic relationship.
They answered questions about the length of their relationship, experiences of selfexpansion within their relationship, passionate loveincluding both romantic and
obsessive componentsand companionate love. As expected, people in longer lasting
relationships reported lower levels of self-expansion within their relationships. Romantic
and obsessive components of passionate love showed different patterns across time in
relationship, but both were positively associated with self-expansion experiences as
expected by the model. Self-expansion was unrelated to companionate love. Broadly
consistent with the self-expansion model, these findings highlight a need for further theoretical development to explain the specific linkages of self-expansion with different
components of passion.

Corresponding author:
Virgil L. Sheets, Department of Psychology, Indiana State University, Root Hall B-205, Terre Haute, IN 47809,
USA.
Email: vsheets1@indstate.edu

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

Sheets

959

Keywords
Aging, Eros, life span, love, mania, passionate love psychology, relationships, selfexpansion

If love doesnt last forever, then whats forever for asks singer, Michael Martin
Murphy (Van Hoy, 1978). In an era when one-half of all U.S. marriages end in divorce
(Miller & Perlman, 2009), some theorists hypothesize that love is not supposed to last
forever (Fisher, 1992). Yet many couples do sustain mutual attraction for a lifetime
(Acevedo & Aron, 2009). Whats the key that makes love last?
While there is no simple answer to this question, a half-century of relationship
research has begun to identify factors of individuals and relationships that may portend
a lasting love. The current article focuses on one of these proposed factors: selfexpansion. I begin by distinguishing different categories of love that follow different trajectories over time and describe a theory that links the diminution of passionate love in
long-term relationships to reduced opportunity to expand the sense of self. I then report
the results of a cross-sectional community survey that allows a test of the linkage
between love and self-expansion within relationships of varying lengths.

Love
When asked, what makes love last? most social scientists would respond with What
do you mean by love? It is not that they do not believe in love, but social scientists have
conceptualized love in many different ways (Hatfield, Bensman, & Rapson, 2012).
Krafft-Ebing (1886/1945, cited in Berscheid & Regan, 2005), for instance, saw at least
four varieties of love, while Lee (1977) described loves six colors; Fisher (2000)
described loves three functional systems, while Sternbergs Triangular Theory (1986)
identifies eight distinct love states. However, perhaps the most widely recognized taxonomy distinguishes just two types: passionate love and companionate love (Bersheid &
Hatfield, 1969; Hatfield & Rapson, 1993).
Passionate love has been defined as an intense longing for union with another
(Hatfield & Rapson, 1993, p. 5). It is understood to be an emotionally volatile state
characterized by strong attraction, an attraction that brings elation when reciprocated but
that is cause for despair when rejected. People experiencing passionate love may be
obsessed with their partner, experiencing intrusive thoughts and fantasies that distract
them from everything else (Tennov, 1979). Although passionate love often includes a
sexual longing, sex is not its sole focus (Fisher, 2000; Myers & Bersheid, 1997).
Companionate love, in contrast, is the deep affection toward someone with whom
your life is intertwined (Hatfield & Rapson, 1993, p. 9). It is the love of close friends
who have a genuine concern for each other (Grote & Frieze, 1994). Built on intimacy and
trust, companionate love is more prototypical of love than is passion (Fehr, 1994).
Whereas passionate love is hot and arousing, the flames of a fire that move and shift in
unpredictable ways, companionate love is warm and comforting, the embers of a fire
that remain through the night (Hatfield & Rapson, 1993).

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

960

Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 31(7)

It is widely accepted that passion is short-lived (Bersheid & Regan, 2005; Fletcher,
2002; Hatfield & Rapson, 1993; Miller & Perlman, 2009). Some authors give it only
24 months (Myers, 2008). Fisher (2000) argues that this is not only natural but even necessary. After all, one could not live in a state of constant obsession, unable to focus on the
tasks of everyday life. From an evolutionary perspective, the fading of passionate love
allows couples to build a life and raise a family (Kenrick & Trost, 1987). Others, however, see the drop in passionate love as an artifact of other changes in the relationship
(Aron & Aron, 1986; Baumeister & Bratslavsky, 1999) and hypothesize that the ability
to sustain the passion is important for the long-term health of the relationship (Acevedo
& Aron, 2009).

Love and self-expansion


According to the self-expansion model (Aron & Aron, 1986), people have a fundamental
motive to expand their sense of self that is primarily achieved through the formation of
close relationships. As people develop new relationships, they gain access to the
resources, perspectives, and identities of their partners that can expand their knowledge
and increase their efficacy for interacting with the world. Cognitive research suggests
that self-expansion is more than metaphorical; people actually begin to include a partners characteristics into their own self-concept, treating partners as extensions of themselves. For instance, attribution researchers have long observed that people extend
self-serving biases that are intended to protect and enhance their own ego to close others,
protecting and enhancing their partners selves as their own (Sedikides, Campbell,
Reeder, & Elliot, 1998). Recent research using reaction time and resource allocation
measures further suggests that people treat close others as part of themselves, even showing confusion in distinguishing a partners characteristics from their own (Aron, Aron,
Tudor, & Nelson, 1991; Mashek, Aron, & Bencimono, 2003).
Fulfilling the drive for self-expansion within a relationship is expected to increase
attraction for ones partner (Aron & Aron, 1986), and rapid self-expansion is believed
to generate an intense attraction that is experienced as passionate love. But once a partner
is included in the self, opportunities for expansion within the relationship become diminished, and passionate love is expected to drop even though one may continue to feel close
to ones partner (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). Baumeister and Bratslavsky (1999)
have observed that the experience of passionate love seems to correspond with the rapid
growth in intimacy in the early phases of a relationship, and Aron, Paris, and Aron (1995)
have observed that people describe themselves more expansively just after falling in
love. Although the self-expansion model offers a parsimonious account for the especially high levels of passion felt early in a relationship, when possibilities for selfexpansion are maximal, there is little empirical data that directly link these observations.
Does self-expansion really account for the high levels of passion early in a relationship
and reduced self-expansion for observed drops in passionate love after the relationship is
established?
Surveys of college students reveal strong correlations between self-reported selfexpansion in a relationship and feelings of passionate love for a romantic partner
(Paniccia, 2011; Sheets, Paniccia, & Sandrick, 2012). Studies of adult couples find

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

Sheets

961

that boredom predicts lower relationship satisfaction (Tsapelas, Aron, & Orbuch,
2009), while activities associated with expansion predict positive affect and relational quality (Graham, 2008); similarly, interventions that engage couples in novel
and exciting activities (that are believed to promote self-expansion) seem to increase
relationship satisfaction (which is related to passionate love; e.g., Aron, Aron, Heyman, Norman, & McKenna, 2000; Carson, Carson, Gil, & Baucom, 2007; Reissman,
Aron, & Bergen, 1993). However, these studies provide no data on the natural
course of self-expansion that would indicate whether self-expansion and passionate
love follow a common trajectory over the life of a relationship. It may be that selfexpansion only correlates with passionate love in late adolescence/early adulthood
when both the search for a romantic partner and the construction of an adult identity
are salient psychological tasks (McAdams, 2001) or when people are particularly
motivated to seek novel experiences for self- or relationship improvement (Mattingly, McIntyre, & Lewandowski, 2012).

Self-expansion, love, and the life cycle


Questions about the impact of self-expansion on passionate love are further motivated
by recent research on the life course of passionate love. Despite several studies documenting drops in passionate love across the life of a romantic relationship (Bierhoff &
Schmohr, 2004; Hatfield, Pillemer, OBrien, & Li, 2008; Sprecher & Regan, 1998;
Wojciszke, 2002), some studies show that this outcome is not inevitable. Acevedo and
Aron (2009) report that passionate love may be retained throughout a long marriage,
and OLeary, Acevedo, Aron, Huddy, and Mashek (2012) found that 40% of married
adults (and over a quarter of those married more than 10 years) still report intense
love for their partners, long after traditional wisdom says their passion should have
died. Acevedo, Aron, Fisher, and Brown (2008) further found brain scan evidence supporting the possible maintenance of passionate love over time. One explanation for
these seemingly contradictory conclusions may rest in Grahams (2011) observationshared by others (e.g., Acevedo & Aron, 2009)that traditional conceptions
of passionate love confuse two distinct dimensions, romantic love and obsessive love,
which may have different temporal courses. In fact, in a meta-analysis of 81 studies of
love, Graham (2011) found that obsessive love was negatively associated with the
length of a reporters relationship, while romantic love was positively associated with
relationship length. If as expected, self-expansion declines over timetracking the
course of obsessive rather than romantic feelingsself-expansion may not be one of
the keys to maintaining romantic passion; in fact, it could be detrimental if it is linked
to obsessive love, which is negatively associated with relationship satisfaction (Acevedo & Aron, 2009; Graham, 2011).
As noted above, there is currently no specific data showing that self-expansion, in
fact, drops over time with a partner, as predicted. Although the logic that as you include
another in the self, there is less remaining to be included later seems irrefutable, it is possible that partners outside activities continue to support expansion throughout their lives
(OLeary et al., 2012).

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

962

Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 31(7)

The current study


The current study examined the relationship between love and self-expansion within romantic relationships of varying duration. Because different components of love may show different trajectories over time, separate assessments of obsessive and romantic components of
passionate love as well as companionate love were made. Following conceptual development and empirical work of Bierhoff and Schmohr (2004), Acevedo and Aron (2009), and
Graham (2011), items from the mania and eros subscales of the Love Attitudes scale (Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986) were used to represent obsessive and romantic components of passionate love, and an item from the storge subscale was used to assess companionate love.
Based on the self-expansion model, it was predicted that those in long-standing relationships would report lower levels of self-expansion within their relationships, and as a
result, lower levels of passionate love for their partnersalthough it was not predicted
whether this would occur on only one or both obsessive and romantic measures of passionate love. Based on prior claims that romantic love (and opportunities for self-expansion) drop rapidly in the first 24 months, nonlinear effects of relationship duration and
self-expansion were also examined. No relationship was anticipated between levels of
self-expansion in a relationship and feelings of companionate love.

Methods
Participants
Five hundred thirty-nine (41% men) registered voters in a medium-sized (population
60,000) Midwestern community participated in a telephone survey about their current
romantic relationship. Respondents ranged in age from 18 to 92 years (median (Mdn)
49.3; 17 not reporting (nr)). Most (84.2%) respondents were living with their romantic
partner (1 nr), and 74.7% were married to their partner. Of these, 14 (2.6%) were involved
with a same-sex romantic partner (1 nr). Respondents relationships ranged in length from
1 day to 65 years (Mdn 16.6 years; 38 nr).
Where possible, the sample demographics were compared with census data for the
county in which the study was conducted (U.S. Census, 2000). These comparisons
revealed that women (59% of the sample) were overrepresented compared to the local
population (51% female). The median age of the sample (49 years) was slightly greater
than for adults in the area (44 years), although the proportion of older adults (e.g., over
65 years) in the sample (16.7%) was slightly lower than that in the local population
(18.4%). Data were not collected on the samples racial or educational composition, but
census data reveal that the study locale is 91% White and 6% African American and onefifth (21%) have at least a bachelors degree. Recent telephone surveys conducted
through the lab reveal that the respondents tend to be slightly more educated than
expected (with approximately one-third holding a bachelors degree).

Procedures
Students in an undergraduate course in social science research methods randomly called
4774 phone numbers of registered voters in a medium-sized Midwestern community in

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

Sheets

963

west-central Indiana. Most numbers were bad (e.g., disconnected, fax/modem line,
etc.; N 2117) or resulted in no contact with a qualified adult resident (e.g., no
answer, busy signal, no adult present, etc.; N 613). When someone answered, the caller
would indicate that he/she was telephoning from the survey research lab in the psychology department at a local university. Callers would confirm that the potential respondent
was 18 and seek verbal consent to continue with the survey. In total, 2044 calls that were
answered (including immediate hang ups) yielded 854 completed interviews, a 41.8%
compliance rate. Three hundred fifteen individuals (36.9%) were not in a romantic relationship; they were asked questions about a best friendship, if any; these data are not
included in the current report.

Measures
The relationship survey consisted of several parts.
Demographics. Participants were asked their age and sex, the age and sex of their romantic
partner, the length of their romantic relationship (How long have you been romantically
involved with your partner?), whether they were married to and/or living with their
romantic partner, and whether they had any children together.
Love. Six items from the Love Attitudes scale (Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986) were selected
based on their face validity as representations of the three dimensions of love to be
assessed: eros, mania, and storge. Items were modified to allow for presentation via
telephone (e.g., shortened and changed from first person to third person).
Eros refers to romantic/sensual attraction to ones partner that is one of the dimensions of passionate love (Graham, 2011); items were Your partner fits your ideal
standards of beauty/handsomeness and Your partner and you have the right physical
chemistry (r .34, p < .001). Mania assesses a possessiveness or obsession for ones
partner that has also been viewed as a dimension of passionate love (Bierhoff &
Schmohr, 2004); items were If your partner ignores you, you do things to get his/her
attention and Since being with your partner, you find it hard to focus on the routines of
life (r .26, p < .001). Storge represents the love of a close friendship and is used as a
measure of companionate love (Bierhoff & Schmohr, 2004; Graham, 2011); items were
Your love for your partner is really just a deep friendship and You always expect to
be friends with your partner (r .10, not significant (ns)). Responses were recorded on
a 15 scale where 1 doesnt describe your relationship at all and 5 describes your
relationship very well.
A principal components analysis confirmed that these items reflected three distinct
components of love as intended. Three components with eigenvalues greater than 1.0
were obtained, accounting for 65% of variance in participants responses. One item
You always expect to be friends with your partner did not load on the expected
subscale (coincidentally, it was the only item of the six that has no parallel on the Life
Attitudes Schedule: Short Form; Hendrick, Hendrick, & Dicke, 1998) and was dropped
from further analysis.

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

964

Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 31(7)

Table 1. Mean levels of self-expansion and love at various relationship durations.


Duration (years)

Self-expansion

Eros

Mania

Storge

0.02.0
2.14.0
4.110.0
10.120.0
20.130.0
30.140.0
40.165.0

68
41
70
90
77
62
67

4.15
3.88
4.02
3.90
3.70
3.80
3.57

4.38
4.55
4.49
4.43
4.31
4.31
4.54

2.65
2.54
2.32
2.40
2.15
2.07
1.62

2.72
2.39
2.47
2.97
2.88
3.06
3.27

Eros and mania scores were computed as the average of the aforementioned items.
Overall, participants reported very high levels of eros (M 4.42, SD .76) and somewhat
lower levels of storge (M 2.88, SD 1.71) and mania (M 2.25, SD 1.08).
Self-expansion. Three face-valid items were used to measure core aspects of selfexpansion experiences in relationships as represented in a scale developed by Lewandowski and Aron (2002). Items included You are still learning things about your
partner; Being with your partner expands your sense of who you are; and Your
relationship with your partner is the source of new experiences. Responses were given
on a 15 scale where 1 not at all and 5 very much. Responses to these items were
significantly correlated (r .21.41, p < .001), and a principal components analysis confirmed their unidimensionality with the single component accounting for 55% of variation in participants responses. Items were averaged to form a single measure of
self-expansion experiences (M 3.87, SD .97, a .59).

Results
Sixty-four respondents were missing values on one or more measures used in the analyses (mostly age or relationship length) and were excluded from all analyses subsequently. Preliminary analyses that compared these respondents with those included in
the analyses revealed no significant differences in any of the dependent measures examined here.
Table 1 presents the means of self-expansion and three measures of love (eros, mania,
and storge) at various relationship durations (chosen to assure n > 40 per category). To
model their trajectories across time, four sets of regressions were performed in which
self-expansion and the three measures of love were regressed on relationship length. A set
of covariatesrespondent age, sex, cohabitation status (whether they were cohabiting with
their partner), and co-parenting status (whether they had children with their partner)were
also entered to examine the possibility that findings were spurious effects of these other
variables. The latter three, sex (1 male), cohabitation (1 yes), and co-parenting (1
yes), were dummy-coded for these analyses. To test the importance of self-expansion in
explaining variation in love, self-expansion was added as a predictor of the three measures
of love. Finally, in an additional step, nonlinear relationships were explored: a quadratic
term tested for curvilinear effects of relationship length on each dependent measure and

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

Sheets

965

Table 2. Correlations between self-expansion and love at various relationship durations.


Duration (years)
0.02.0
2.14.0
4.110.0
10.120.0
20.130.0
30.140.0
40.165.0

Eros

Mania

Storge

.42***
.34*
.52***
.32**
.28*
.45***
.31*

.39**
.24
.02
.13
.32*
.13
.01

.01
.09
.04
.17
.02
.14
.04

*p < .05; **p < .01, ***p < .001.

an interaction term tested for variability in the association of self-expansion on the measures of love across relationships of varying duration. Table 2 presents the correlations
between self-expansion and each measure of love at various relationship durations. Table 3
presents the means, standard deviations, and correlations among all variables used in these
analyses.

Self-expansion
As seen in Table 1, respondents in longer lasting relationships generally reported less
self-expansion within their relationship. This association was verified in the first regression analysis, b .18, p < .001; however, this effect was reduced by half, and not significant, when the four covariates, age, sex, cohabitation status, and co-parenting status,
were included, b .09, ns. But despite a significant model, R2 .05, p < .001, none of
the covariates showed significant effects either: b .10, ns, for age; b .08, ns, for
cohabitating with partner; and b .08, ns, for having children with partneralthough
there was a marginally significant sex difference, b .08, p < .10, with men reporting
higher rates of self-expansion. This outcome likely reflects multicollinearity among
the predictors as age and relationship length were strongly correlated (r .76, p < .001).
The quadratic term for relationship length, b .02, ns, did not significantly improve the
model.

Eros
As seen in Table 1, romantic love varied little across the life span of participants
relationships. Romantic love was unrelated to relationship length both in the initial
model, b .01, ns, and after the addition of the covariates, b .06, ns. Nor were any of
the covariates related to romantic love: b .06, ns, for age; b .02, ns, for sex; b
.06, ns, for cohabitation status; and b .05, ns, for co-parenting status. However,
self-expansion was strongly predictive of romantic love, b .37, p < .001, even after
controlling for these covariates. As seen in Table 2, self-expansion was significantly
associated with romantic love at every stage of relationship, and the addition of an interaction term to the regression analysis offered no evidence that this association was moderated by relationship length, b .03, ns. There was, however, a significant quadratic

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

966

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

3.86
.96

.13**

4.42 2.24
.77 1.06

.01 .03
2.85
1.71

.02

.23***
.08y
.00
.04
.08y

.01
.01
.04
.01
.07

.16***
.08y
.04
.13**
.10*

.29***
.14**
.12**
.10*
.15**

1.00
.05 1.00
.00 .08y
1.00
.01 .27*** .15**

Storge

1.00
.35***
.20***
.07
.18***

Mania

246.23
206.86

.12*

.76***
.04
.41***
.54***
.59***

1.00

Relationship
length
Sex

48.48
16.97

.08y

.42
.49

.00

1.00
.03
1.00
.31*** .11*
.25*** .08y
.44*** .03

Age

.84
.37

.01

1.00
.47***
.02

.64
.48

.02

1.00
.14**

Cohabitation Co-parent

42,699.27
53,278.78

.25***

1.00

Quadratic

Note. Means for quadratic and interaction effects and all correlations are computed after centering relationship length and self-expansion measures.
yp < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Self-expansion
Eros
Mania
Storge
Relationship length
(months)
Age (years)
Sex (1 male)
Cohabitation (1 yes)
Co-parent (1 yes)
Quad (relationship
length2)
Interaction (SE 
Relationship Length)
Mean
SD

Selfexpansion Eros

Table 3. Correlations, means, and SD of all measures (N 475).

36.29
207.41

1.00

Interaction

Sheets

967

effect for relationship length, b .12, p < .05. This implied that the trajectory of romantic love was curvilinear across time in relationship. Further probing revealed thatas
seen in Table 1romantic love dropped gradually across relationships for the first
20 years (vertex 22) but then rebounded in longer lasting relationships.

Mania
As seen in Table 1 and confirmed via regression, obsessive love dropped in long-lasting
relationships, as expected, b .27, p < .001, although this association was not significant when covariates were added to the model, b .12, ns. Obsessive love was significantly predicted by respondent age, b .22, p < .01, and sex, b .14, p < .01 (with
men reporting greater levels of obsessive love) but not cohabitation status, b .00, ns,
nor co-parenting status, b .03, ns. Self-expansion in a relationship was also significantly related to obsessive love, b .15, p < .01. Although there was no evidence of
a quadratic component of relationship length in this trajectory, b .02, ns, the interaction of self-expansion and relationship length approached significance, b .08, p
.06. Further probing revealedas evident in Table 2self-expansion was positively
associated with obsessive love early in the relationship, but this effect generally diminished over time (except for those married 2030 yearswhich may reflect the particular
cut points used in creating the table).

Storge
As seen in Table 1, companionate love appeared to increase in longer relationships, b
.15, p < .01, but this effect was not significant when the covariates were included in the
model, b .05, ns. Respondent age was positively related to levels of companionate
love, b .28, p < .001, but respondent sex, b .07, ns; cohabitation status, b .08, ns;
and co-parenting status, b .04, ns, were not. Nor was self-expansion related to companionate love, b .04, ns, at any relationship duration (see Table 2) as confirmed
by a nonsignificant interaction term, b .01, ns. The quadratic term for relationship
length, b .00, ns, also failed to achieve significance.

Discussion
As every couple knows, love is not static, but relationships, and the love between
partners, change over time. This study showed that people in long-standing relationships
reported lower levels of self-expansion in their relationships than did people who had
been with partners for shorter periods of time. This pattern is consistent with the selfexpansion model of love (Aron & Aron, 1986), which further predicts corresponding
changes in passionate love. Two measures of passionate love both varied across groups
in relationships for different amounts of time. Romantic love showed a complex trajectory, decreasing slightly with time in relationship for groups of participants in the first
20 years of their relationships and then increasing with time in relationship for participants whose relationships lasted longer. Obsessive love showed a more steady decrease
across time, though it appears that aging, rather than time in relationship, may account

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

968

Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 31(7)

for this pattern. Despite their seemingly different trajectories, self-expansion was positively related to both measures of passionate love. Companionate love, which is not central to the self-expansion model, also appears to change, with older participants reporting
greater levels of friendship with their partners than younger participants; these feelings were unrelated to experiences of self-expansion in their relationships.
Many researchers have examined changes in love across time. This is the first study,
however, to explicitly examine the temporal trajectory of self-expansion and the association of these experiences with romantic and obsessive components of passionate love
across the life span. The results have specific implications for the self-expansion model
and, more generally, enhance existing knowledge on aging in relationships. Subsequently, I discuss both the specific and general implications of the findings and the limitations of the current study.

Implications for the self-expansion model


According to the self-expansion model (Aron & Aron, 1986), as intimacy grows and a
partner is fully included in the self, self-expansion opportunities within the relationship
diminish. This is consistent with the pattern obtained in the current study. Respondents in
the longest lasting relationships reported lower levels of self-expansion than those in relatively newer relationships, although collinearity makes it unclear whether this is independent of age/aging. Contrary to expectations, however, there was no parallel drop in
romantic love as a function of time in relationship or participant age. Although romantic
love tended to drop with increasing time in relationship early on, it appears to rebound
later, and people who had been with their partner for over 40 years reported just as much
romantic love toward their partner as those just a few years into their relationship.
Although these diverging trends seem inconsistent with the self-expansion model,
self-expansion experiences were positively associated with romantic love no matter how
old the relationship, just as expected by the self-expansion model. How is it that romantic
love is maintained at high levels despite diminishing levels of self-expansion, given a
positive association between them? Several explanations seem possible.
First, romantic love may have causes besides self-expansion with ones partner. For
instance, other relationship factors may counter the decline that would be expected with
reduced self-expansion opportunities in long-term relationships (Fingerman & Charles,
2010). Perhaps early retirement or children leaving home permits partners to experience
pleasure in each others company while carrying out the mundane activities of life that
do not engender opportunities for self-expansion (Gorchoff, John, & Helson, 2008).
Alternatively, even as self-expansion opportunities through ones partner diminish in
long-lasting relationships, other opportunities for self-expansion emerge that may positively impact attraction for ones partner and relationship. For instance, ones grown
children or grandchildren may become routes for continued self-expansion that are both
independent of ones relationship with a partner (as measured here) and intimately connected to it. One might speculate that an avocation or other outside source of selfexpansion might also generate positive affectperhaps even passionthat may be
transferred to a supportive partner who does not themselves participate.

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

Sheets

969

A second possibility is that the affective benefit of self-expansion may change over
age or time in relationship. There is no a priori reason why self-expansion in a mature
relationship should generate the same level of positive affect as in a youthful relationship. In fact, it seems possible that small changes to a fully developed self (that includes
ones romantic partner) could evoke stronger responses than larger changes in a still
developing self. Consistent with this, some researchers have observed that older adults
enhance the significance of new love experiences through constructing a sense of their
uniqueness from prior experiences in life (Ben-Ari, Lavee, & Gal, 2006); whether this
pattern of enhancement occurs with self-expanding events in long-term relationships
is mere speculation. However, if it does, lower levels of self-expansion would be needed
to maintain higher levels of romantic passion in later life, which is consistent with the
obtained results.
Finally, it is also possible that there is no decline in self-expansion in long-lasting
relationships. Because of the cross-sectional nature of the current data, it is impossible
to be certain that the lower self-expansion reported by participants from longer lasting
relationships really reflects a change in their experience relative to when their relationships were newer; instead, it could be that this pattern of reports reflects the experience
of participants from different cohorts. Some authors (e.g., Twenge & Campbell, 2008)
have argued that American society is increasingly emphasizing the self, and this may
have resulted in greater emphasis on close relationships as a source of self-expansion. As
a result, the absolute levels of self-expansion reported by those from relatively young
relationships may reflect their different experience (self-expanding through their relationships) relative to those in older cohorts (who may find self-expansion in outside
activities) even as the association between self-expansion and romantic love remains the
same.
Like self-expansion, the obsessive component of passionate love seemed to decline in
longer lasting relationships, although this seems linked to aging rather than time in relationship, per se. While self-expansion experiences were positively associated with this
component of passionate love as implied by the self-expansion model, unexpectedly, this
association was most evident in relatively young relationships and seemed to diminish
over time. Perhaps obsession is necessary to sustain interactions with a partner long
enough for self-expansion to be initiated, but once inclusion of the other is underway,
ones focus can be released to explore other sources of expansion. Alternatively, this
association may represent an attempt to jealously protect the potential for selfexpansion in a partner that has not yet been realized, which may dissipate once it has
(Lewandowski, 2003 similarly notes the affective importance of the potential for selfexpansion). Understanding the parameters of this correlation is important because unlike
romantic love, obsessive love is negatively associated with relationship satisfaction
(Graham, 2011).

Aging in relationships
Although the motivation of this study was to examine the temporal course of selfexpansion in relationships and its association with measures of passionate love, other
trends in the data also merit consideration.

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

970

Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 31(7)

There was no evidence of a precipitous drop in passionate love in the first years of a
relationship, but this may reflect the relatively small number of respondents in relationships of less than 2 years. On the other hand, a large number of respondents were in
relationships of 20 years or longer, and many, much longer, which may have enabled
detection of the rebound in romantic love at older ages. While there was a downward
trend in romantic love in relationships from 0 to 20 years, the trend was positive in longer
lasting relationships. This finding is relatively unique, and its causes are not immediately
obvious. As noted above, family and life cycle transitions (e.g., retirement, children
leaving, etc.) or other factors may allow older partners to enjoy each other more (Fingerman & Charles, 2010; Gorchoff et al., 2008) and alter the experience and trajectory of
romantic love. It is also interesting to speculate whether this trend might be related to
other changes in later life. For instance, researchers have noted a general increase in
happiness and well-being at older ages, which could be the underlying cause (Keyes,
Shmotkin, & Ryff, 2002; Weiss, King, Inoue-Murayama, Matsuzawa, & Oswald,
2012); alternatively, it is worth exploring whether changes in romantic love contribute
uniquely to general well-being in older adults. This finding highlights the importance
of adequately representing older adults and of testing for nonlinear patterns in relationship studies.
There was also a tendency for older respondents to report more companionate love
toward their partners than younger respondents. Although one must be cautious in
interpreting responses to this single-item measure, especially in light of Grahams (2011)
meta-analysis which found that companionate love (practical friendship) was unrelated to relationship length or satisfaction, other researchers (e.g., Bierhoff & Schmohr,
2004) have reported increases in storge in older relationships. It may surprise some
that there was no significant correlation between companionate love and selfexpansion experiences in this study as it may seem logical that romantic love (which
is correlated with self-expansion experiences) eventually becomes companionate love
(Aron et al., 2008; Sprecher & Regan, 1998). However, if such a transition occurs, the
observed relationships are likely to be lagged by an unknown temporal parameter and
difficult to see without an additional measure of achieved expansion or integration
(perhaps the Inclusion of Other in Self scale Aron et al., 1992) to bridge these experiences. Clearly, more research is needed to understand this pattern.

Limitations
Although this study included a larger and potentially more representative sample of
American adults than is typically obtained in relationship studies, it must be noted that
the Midwestern population it represents is itself a culturally homogenous group, showing
relatively little of the variability that might be observed in a larger, urban area. Given
cultural and subcultural variability in the nature and importance of the self, it would be
especially interesting to explore these relationships in a more diverse cultural group or in
a locale that is not as self-focused as the United States and where passion is not seen as a
basis for marriage.
While the use of a telephone survey methodology enabled collection of data from a
broad range of community members, telephone assessment also has limitations, perhaps

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

Sheets

971

the most salient of which is the reliance on brief measures. Although the items used were
carefully selected to represent established constructs, there is almost certainly a loss of
precision and increase in error associated with such brief measures. For instance, the
wording of the single item to represent companionate love, Your love for your partner
is really just a deep friendship may have inappropriately limited endorsement by
respondents who also felt passion toward their partners. It is reassuring that the factor
structure of the measures were consistent with those reported in other samples using
larger versions of the measures. Caution is still warranted in drawing comparisons
between companionate and passionate love types and in interpretation of nonsignificant
findings despite a moderate large sample size.
It is also important to recognize the limitations posed by the cross-sectional nature of
the data. Although the observed patterns may represent changes that typically occur in
relationships across time, there are a number of potential confounding factors. For
instance, the very large correlation between participants age and length of relationship
obscured the role of time in relationship versus age as an explanation for the trajectory of
self-expansion. In addition, as mentioned above, observed trends may not represent
changes in participants life or relationship experiences but differences in the experiences of people who began their relationships at different times over the last 60 years.
A longitudinal design is necessary to begin to disentangle these possibilities.
One further caution is in order. Although the current results support others (e.g.,
Acevedo & Aron, 2009) in questioning the common wisdom that passion and romance
necessarily fade, it is important to observeas have others (e.g., Hatfield et al., 2008)
that those whose passion faded the most are likely missing from the current study (as
their relationships have probably ended). This could easily explain the absence of an
expected downward trend in passionate love across longer lasting relationships, but there
is no obvious reason to suspect that would have affected the significant nonlinear trends
and associations between self-expansion and passionate love reported here. As a result,
while one cannot conclude from the current findings that romantic love does not weaken
over time, one can conclude that it does not have to.

Conclusion
As noted at the beginning of the article, people often wonder, What makes love last?
Research is increasingly pointing to the value of self-expansion experiences. Correlational and laboratory studies suggest that novel and exciting experiences generate
self-expansion and motivate positive affect toward partners (Aron et al., 2000; Sheets
et al., 2012); interventions that engage couples in novel and exciting activities and promote self-expansion (Carson et al., 2007; Reissman et al., 1993) similarly show positive
relationship outcomes. The current study that examined the relationship between selfexpansion and passionate love in a large community sample, further documents the
important relationship of these experiences. Across relationships from 1 day to 65 years,
these data show that self-expansion is strongly, positively associated with feelings of
romantic passion. While self-expansion is not a panacea that will cure all relationship
ills, it is one of the more promising keys to enhancing positive affect between longterm partners who seem to have grown bored.

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

972

Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 31(7)

Authors note
A preliminary version of this article was presented at the International Association for Relationship Research (IARR) conference, Chicago, Illinois, USA, 2012.

Funding
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or
not-for-profit sectors.

References
Acevedo, B. P., & Aron, A. (2009). Does a long-term relationship kill romantic love? Review of
General Psychology, 13, 5965. doi:10.1037/a0014226
Acevedo, B. P., Aron, A., Fisher, H. E, & Brown, L. L. (2012). Neural correlates of long-term
intense romantic love. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 145159.
Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1986). Love and the expansion of the self: Understanding attraction and
satisfaction. New York, NY: Hemisphere Publishing Co/Harper & Row Publishers.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Heyman, R. E., Norman, C. C., & McKenna, C. (2000). Couples shared
participation in novel and arousing activities and experienced relationship quality. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 273284. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.273
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of
interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596612. doi:10.
1037/0022-3514.63.4.596
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as including other in
the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 241253. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.
60.2.241
Aron, A., Fisher, H. E, Strong, G, Acevedo, B., Riela, S., & Tsapelas, I. (2008). Falling in love. In
S. Sprecher, A. Wenzel, & J. Harvey (Eds.), Handbook of relationship initiation (pp 313336).
New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Aron, A., Paris, M., & Aron, E. N. (1995). Falling in love: Prospective studies of self-concept
change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1102112. doi:10.1037/
0022-3514.69.6.1102
Baumeister, R. F., & Bratslavsky, E. (1999). Passion, intimacy, and time: Passionate love as a
function of change in intimacy. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 4967. doi:
10.1207/s15327957pspr0301_3
Ben-Ari, A, Lavee, Y., & Gal, Z. (2006). Midlife perspectives on falling in love: The dialectic of
unique experiences. Journal of Adult Development, 13, 118123.
Berscheid, E., & Hatfield, E. (1969). Interpersonal attraction. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Berscheid, E., & Regan, P. C. (2005). The psychology of interpersonal relationships. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Bierhoff, H.-W., & Schmohr, M. (2004). Romantic and marital relationships. In F. R. Lang & K. L.
Fingerman (Eds.), Growing together: Personal relationships across the life span (pp 103129).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Carson, J. W., Carson, K. M., Gil, K. M., & Baucom, D. H. (2007). Self-expansion as a mediator of
relationship improvements in a mindfulness intervention. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33, 517528. doi:10.1111/j.1752-0606.2007.00035.x

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

Sheets

973

Fehr, B. (1994). Prototype-based assessment of laypeoples views of love. Personal Relationships,


1, 309331. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.1994.tb00068.x
Fingerman, K. L., & Charles, S. T. (2010). It takes two to tango: Why older people have the best
relationships. Current Perspectives in Psychological Science, 19, 172176.
Fisher, H. (1992). Anatomy of love: The natural history of adultery, monogamy, and divorce. New
York, NY: Norton.
Fisher, H. (2000). Lust, attraction, attachment: Biology and the evolution of the three primary
emotion systems for mating, reproduction, and parenting. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 25, 96104.
Fletcher, G. (2002). The new science of intimate relationships. Maiden, MA: Blackwell.
Gorchoff, S. M., John, O. P, & Helson, P. (2008). Contextualizing change in marital satisfaction during middle age: An 18-year longitudinal study. Psychological Science, 19,
11941200.
Graham, J. M. (2008). Self-expansion and flow in couples momentary experiences, Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 679694. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.679
Graham, J. M. (2011). Measuring love in romantic relationships: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 28, 748771. doi:10.1177/026540751389126
Grote, N. K., & Frieze, I. H. (1994). The measurement of friendship-based love in intimate relationships. Personal Relationships, 1, 275300. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.1994.tb00066.x
Hatfield, E., Bensman, L., & Rapson, R. L. (2012). A brief history of social scientists attempts to
measure passionate love. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 29, 143164. doi:10.
1177/0265407511431055
Hatfield, E., Pillemer, J. T., OBrien, M. U., & Yen-Chi, L. L. (2008). The endurance of love: Passionate and companionate love in newlywed, and long-term marriages. Interpersona, 2, 3564.
Hatfield, E., & Rapson, R. (1993). Love, sex, and intimacy: Their psychology, biology, and history.
New York, NY: HarperCollins.
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, S. (1986). A theory and method of love. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 50, 392402. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.392
Hendrick, C., Hendrick, S. S., & Dicke, A. (1998). The Love Attitudes scale: Short form. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 147159. doi:10.1177/0265407598152001
Kenrick, D. T., & Trost, M. R. (1987). A biosocial theory of heterosexual relationships. In
Kelley K. (Ed.), Females, males, and sexuality: Theories and research (pp 59100).
Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Keyes, C. L., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. P. (2002). Optimizing well-being: The empirical encounter
of two theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 10071022.
Krafft-Ebing, R. Von (1945). Psychopathia sexualis (12th ed.). New York, NY: Pioneer Publication Inc. (original work published 1886).
Lee, J. A. (1977). A typology of styles of loving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3,
173182. doi:10.1177/014616727700300204
Lewandowski, G. W., Jr. (2003). Relationship dissolution and the self-concept: The role of interpersonal closeness and self-expansion. [Dissertation Abstracts] International: Section B: The
Sciences and Engineering, Vol. 64(1-B), p. 465.
Lewandowski, G. W., Jr., & Aron, A. (2002). The self-expansion scale: Construction and validation. Paper presented at the Third Annual Meeting of the Society of Personality and Social Psychology, Savannah, GA.

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

974

Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 31(7)

Mashek, D. J., Aron, A., & Boncimino, M. (2003). Confusions of self with close others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 382392. doi:10.1177/0146167202250220
Mattingly, B. A., McIntyre, K. P., & Lewandowski, G. W. Jr (2012). Approach motivation and the
expansion of the self in close relationships. Personal Relationships, 19, 113127. doi:10.1111/
j.1475-6811.2010.01343.x
McAdams, D. P. (2001). The person (3rd ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.
Miller, R. S., & Perlman, D. (2009). Intimate relationships. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Montgomery, M. J., & Sorell, G. T. (1997). Differences in love attitudes across family life stages.
Family Relations, 46, 5561.
Myers, D. G. (2008). Social psychology (9th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Myers, S. A., & Berscheid, E. (1997). The language of love: The difference a preposition makes.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 347362. doi:10.1177/0146167297234002
OLeary, K. D., Acevedo, B. P., Aron, A., Huddy, L., & Mashek, D. (2012). Is long-term love more
than a rare phenomenon? If so, what are its correlates? Social Psychological and Personality
Science, 3, 241249. doi:10.1177/1948550611417015
Paniccia, L.(2011). Self-expansion and couple possessions: The representation of the self and other
in valued possessions. (Unpublished masters thesis). Indiana State University, Terre Haute, IN.
Reissman, C., Aron, A., & Bergen, M. R. (1993). Shared activities and marital satisfaction: Causal
direction and self-expansion versus boredom. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
10, 243254. doi:10.1177/026540759301000205
Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Reeder, G. D., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). The self-serving bias in relational context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 378386. doi:10.1037/
0022-3514.74.2.378
Sheets, V., Paniccia, L., & Sandrick, C. (2012, July). Individual differences in self-expansion
motives, experiences, and passionate love. Paper presented at the International Association for
Relationship Research 2012 Conference, Chicago, IL.
Sprecher, S., & Regan, P. C. (1998). Passionate and companionate love in courting and young married couples. Sociological Inquiry, 68, 163185.
Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93, 119135. doi:10.
1037/0033-295X.93.2.119
Tsapelas, I., Aron, A., & Orbuch, T. (2009). Marital boredom now predicts less satisfaction 9 years
later. Psychological Science, 20, 543545. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02332.x
Tennov, D. (1979). Love and limerence: The experience of being in love. New York, NY: Stein & Day.
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Increases in positive self-views among high school students: Birth cohort changes in anticipated performance, self-satisfaction, self-liking, and self-competence. Psychological Science, 19, 10821086. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02204.x
U.S. Census Bureau. (2000). DP-1. Profile of general demographic characteristics: 2000 (Vigo
County Indiana). Retrieved from http://factfinder2.census.gov
Van Hoy, R. (1978). Whats forever for? [Recorded by Michael Martin Murphy]. On Michael
Martin Murphy [Audiocassette]. London, UK: Liberty/EMI (1982).
Weiss, A., King, J. E., Inoue-Murayama, M., Matsuzawa, T., & Oswald, A. (2012). Evidence for a
midlife crisis in great apes consistent with the U-shape in human well-being. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1994919952.
Wojciszke, B. (2002). From the first sight to the last drop: A six-stage model of the dynamics of
love. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 33, 1525.

Downloaded from spr.sagepub.com at Tumaini Uni-Ingringa University College on December 12, 2014

You might also like