You are on page 1of 8

NeuroImage 59 (2012) 872879

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

NeuroImage
j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / y n i m g

Reading the mind's eye: Online detection of visuo-spatial working memory and
visual imagery in the inferior temporal lobe
Carlos M. Hamam a, b, c,, Juan R. Vidal a, b, c, Toms Ossandn a, b, c, Karim Jerbi a, b, c, Sarang S. Dalal a, b, c,
Lorella Minotti d, e, f, Olivier Bertrand a, b, c, Philippe Kahane d, e, f, Jean-Philippe Lachaux a, b, c
a
INSERM U1028, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, Lyon, France
b
CNRS UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, Lyon, France
c
University Lyon 1, Lyon, France
d
Neurology Department, Grenoble University Hospital, Grenoble, France
e
INSERM U836, Grenoble Neuroscience Institute, Grenoble, France
f
University Joseph Fourier, Grenoble, France

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Several brain regions involved in visual perception have been shown to also participate in non-sensory
Received 10 April 2011 cognitive processes of visual representations. Here we studied the role of ventral visual pathway areas in
Revised 25 July 2011 visual imagery and working memory. We analyzed intracerebral EEG recordings from the left inferior
Accepted 28 July 2011
temporal lobe of an epileptic patient during working memory tasks and mental imagery. We found that high
Available online 3 August 2011
frequency gamma-band activity (50150 Hz) in the inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) increased with memory
Keywords:
load only during visuo-spatial, but not verbal, working memory. Using a real-time set-up to measure and
Visual mental imagery visualize gamma-band activity online BrainTV we found a systematic activity increase in ITG when the
Working memory patient was visualizing a letter (visual imagery), but not during perception of letters. In contrast, only 7 mm
Word form area more medially, neurons located in the fusiform gyrus exhibited a complete opposite pattern, responding
Intracerebral EEG recordings during verbal working memory retention and letter presentation, but not during imagery or visuo-spatial
Electrocorticography working memory maintenance. Talairach coordinates indicate that the fusiform contact site corresponds to
Gamma-band activity the word form area, suggesting that this region has a role not only in processing letter-strings, but also in
Brain-computer interface
working memory retention of verbal information. We conclude that neural networks supporting imagination
Epilepsy
of a visual element are not necessarily the same as those underlying perception of that element. Additionally,
we present evidence that gamma-band activity in the inferior temporal lobe, can be used as a direct measure
of the efciency of top-down attentional control over visual areas with implications for the development of
novel brain-computer interfaces. Finally, by just reading gamma-band activity in these two recording sites, it
is possible to determine, accurately and in real-time, whether a given memory content is verbal or visuo-
spatial.
2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction et al., 2002; Le Bihan et al., 1993) and in working memory retention of
visual information (Awh and Jonides, 2001; Haxby et al., 1994;
The denition of certain brain regions as perceptual, motor or Serences et al., 2009).
associative, has been based mostly upon functional criteria. For Visual imagery refers to the maintenance of a stable conscious visual
example, regions of the human brain showing selective responses to representation independent of visual inputs (Moulton and Kosslyn,
visual stimuli would usually be labeled as visual areas. However, the 2009), very much like the maintenance of a visual event in working
human visual system is known to be involved not only in perception memory, a theoretical construct that refers to memory stores and
of environmental stimuli, but also in mental visual imagery (Kreiman executive processes supporting our capacity to keep in mind
information no longer available in the environment (Baddeley, 1992;
Baddeley and Hitch, 1974). Indeed, behavioral studies have shown that
Abbreviations: BCI, brain-computer interface; MVIS, visuo-spatial working memory increasing working memory load impairs the capacity of subjects to
task; MVEB, verbal working memory task; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; TMS, mentally create and/or manipulate visual images (Atkinson and Shiffrin,
transcranial magnetic stimulation; WFA, word form area. 1968; Baddeley and Andrade, 2000; Gyselinck et al., 2007), suggesting
Corresponding author at: INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience
Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, 95, Bd. Pinel, Centre Hospitalier
that visual imagery and visual working memory share common
Le Vinatier (Bt. 452), F-69500, Bron, France. Fax: + 33 4 72 13 89 01. cognitive and neural resources. These ndings are in agreement with
E-mail address: carlos.hamame@inserm.fr (C.M. Hamam). the sensory recruitment hypothesis, which states that information to

1053-8119/$ see front matter 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.087
C.M. Hamam et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 872879 873

be recalled is temporarily stored in the sustained activity of brain


regions involved in perception of that information (Awh and Jonides,
2001; Serences et al., 2009). In fact, the disruption of neural activity in
the early visual cortex with single TMS pulses affects performance in
both visual imagery and short-term memory tasks (Cattaneo et al.,
2009). Moreover, apart from the sensory recruitment hypothesis,
several authors have proposed that visual imagery and visuo-spatial
working memory might rely on the same large-scale network
connecting the frontal executive system with occipito-temporal visual
areas, possibly via the parietal lobes (Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000;
Ranganath and D'Esposito, 2005; Zimmer, 2008). Nevertheless, how
and to what extent visual brain regions participate in imagery and
working memory remains unknown.
In the present study, we focus primarily on the neural implemen-
tation of visual imagery and working memory maintenance in the
inferior temporal lobe. We hypothesized that visual areas which activity
increases with memory-load should also be active during visual
imagery. We tested this hypothesis in an epileptic patient implanted
with intracerebral electrodes for clinical reasons. We recorded the
patient during a simple visuo-spatial working memory task and
searched for cortical sites active during memory maintenance. We Fig. 1. Patient preoperative MRI and electrode positions. A) Coronal and B) horizontal
view of MRI showing recording sites f6 and f8. C) Sagital, view in the preoperative MRI
then monitored the activity of those sites online and found an
highlighting localization of contact site f6 in the fusiform gyrus, Tailarach coordinates:
instantaneous and systematic increase in gamma-band neural activity 42, 55, 11. D) As in C, but highlighting localization of contact site f8 in the
(50150 Hz) when the patient voluntarily engaged in visual imagery. inferior temporal gyrus, Tailarach coordinates: 49, 55, 11.

Methods
study, due to their selective response to load manipulations during
Participant working memory tasks. Activity in these recording sites was veried
to be free from epileptiform activity, eye movements, and electro-
Subject was a right-handed 16 year old female undergoing myographic artifacts. Structural MRIs were acquired before implan-
resective neurosurgical treatment for drug-resistant epilepsy. Neuro- tation, post-implantation (i.e., with electrodes in place) and after
psychological examination was normal, with intellectual abilities and resective surgery. The post-implantation MRI was analyzed with
memory in the normal range. A thickening of the right superior custom Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA) routines to precisely
temporal sulcus was suspected, though repeated MRI proved localize electrodes and contact sites. Talairach and Tournoux (1988)
inconclusive. 18-FDG-PET demonstrated a right posterior temporo- atlas was used to identify and report recorded brain regions as
perisylvian hypometabolism. Interictal EEG mainly showed right standard coordinates.
temporal slow waves. In order to precisely identify the epileptic focus,
the patient was stereotactically implanted with 15 multi-lead depth- Ofine time-frequency analysis
EEG electrodes. The implantation strategy favored the posterior
temporal cortex of both sides with 9 electrodes on the right side, Time courses of gamma-band (50150 Hz) amplitude (i.e.,
and 6 electrodes on the left, chosen solely by clinical criterion. The envelope or band-limited power) were calculated ofine for the
patient was electrophysiologically monitored for a three-week period electrophysiological signal acquired during visuo-spatial and verbal
by a team of clinicians at Grenoble University Hospital (see Kahane et working memory tasks (see below for an explanation of procedures).
al., 2004 for method description). During this period, parents and Continuous data was rst band-pass ltered between 50 and 150 Hz
subject provided informed consent for participation in the study. in steps of 10 Hz to obtain 10 consecutive frequency bands from 50
Research was approved by the Regional Ethical Committee (CPP Sud- 60 Hz to 140150 Hz. Afterward, the envelope of each of these band-
Est V). After clinical examination, the epileptogenic area proved to be passed ltered signals was calculated using the standard Hilbert
right temporo-basal. Fig. 1 depicts the patient's MRI showing location transform (c.f. Le Van Quyen et al., 2001). To normalize across
of the electrode contacts of interest for this study, f6 and f8. frequency bands, amplitude values for each 10 Hz band were divided
by their mean throughout the entire session and then multiplied by
Intracerebral recordings 100. The resulting values are thus presented as a percentage of
increase or decrease in activity relative to the entire recording period.
A multi-channel video-EEG acquisition and monitoring system Finally, the 10 envelopes were averaged across frequency yielding a
(Micromed, Treviso, Italy) was used to record neural activity from 15 single time-series for broad-high-gamma (50150 Hz).
semi-rigid uni-linear electrode arrays. Each array comprises from 6 to Event-related modulations of gamma envelope were obtained by
15 contact sites separated by 3.5 mm (Dixi, Besanon, France) averaging its amplitude across trials for a given experimental
bilaterally spanning temporal and occipital lobes. The intracerebral condition. For visualization purposes, envelopes were smoothed
EEG signal was acquired at a 512 Hz sampling rate and bandpass using a sliding-window moving average of 250 ms and then displayed
ltered between 0.1 and 200 Hz. A contact site placed in white matter relative to a common pre-stimulus baseline of 400 to 100 ms.
was chosen as monopolar reference for the acquisition. However, and All spectral analyses, display and related statistical tests were
in order to increase spatial resolution, a bipolar montage (i.e., performed using Matlab custom routines.
difference between adjacent contacts) was applied for online analysis
and display. After real-time exploration of gamma-band response Real-time visualization of gamma-band activity
(50150 Hz) in different cognitive and behavioral conditions (Jerbi et
al., 2009b; Lachaux et al., 2007a), a left fusiform (f6) and an inferior A real-time visualization system, dubbed BrainTV (detailed descrip-
temporal gyrus (ITG) (f8) recording sites were selected for further tion in Lachaux et al., 2007a, 2007b), was used for online analysis and
874 C.M. Hamam et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 872879

display of instantaneous power variations in gamma activity. Signal remember the identity of the letters and respond after the retention
amplitude was displayed as an increase or decrease (activation score) in period if the presented sample matched the probe. The number of
spectral power relative to baseline. The only difference between online trials per condition was the same as in MVIS.
and ofine time-frequency analysis was that normalization across
frequency bands was done considering the last two minutes of Visual imagery
recording for online computation, while in the ofine analysis the The BrainTV environment (Lachaux et al., 2007a, 2007b) allows
entire recording period was preferred. A computer screen was used to the experimenter to quickly test the reactivity of any given brain
display the gamma-band envelope measured during the last ten region as the participant engages in simple cognitive procedures, such
seconds of recording (refreshed every 250 ms). as mentally exploring her own house, or shifting her attention
between internal representations and external sensory inputs. In the
Experimental procedures example of interest for this study, the participant was initially shown
a blank sheet of paper and was asked to mentally visualize letters
Visual working memory task (MVIS) drawn on it, as vividly as possible (see Supplementary Movie). The
A delayed matched to sample task was used to evaluate brain reactivity of the ITG was then tested while the participant was asked
dynamics during retention of different loads of visuo-spatial infor- to visually imagine real-life objects. The quality of the mental imagery
mation. Each of the 48 trials started with the presentation of a was assessed by interviewing the patient about how she performed
1500 ms central xation cross followed by a 4-by-4 grid with two, the task, what was she actually imagining and how difcult it was for
four or six dots (top-right panel in Fig. 2). This 1500 ms probe- her to follow the instructions.
stimulus was replaced by an empty grid during a retention period of
3000 ms. The patient was instructed to remember the spatial Statistical analysis
arrangement of the dots on the screen until sample presentation.
The sample consisted of a dot shortly (1500 ms) presented on one of The Wilcoxon signed rank test was performed to reveal signicant
the 16 spaces of the grid. The subject was required to respond if the differences in gamma band activity across memory loads and control
sample dot was also present in the probe-stimulus pressing one of condition in both working memory tasks. Alpha value for single
two buttons in a game-pad with her right index nger. Memory load comparison was 0.01, while the Bonferroni correction was applied for
was manipulated by changing the number of dot positions to retain multiple comparisons yielding a more conservative alpha of 0.005.
two for low, four for medium, and six for high load. Additionally, 15
trials consisted of a control condition where one of the dots in the Results
probe was highlighted meaning that it was the only position to retain.
All conditions were randomly assigned across trials. The maintenance period in the visuo-spatial working memory task
was characterized by increased gamma activity for high loads in the
Verbal working memory task (MVEB) inferior temporal gyrus, but not in the fusiform gyrus. More specically,
A very similar delayed matched to sample task was used to a recording site located in the ITG (T3, electrode contact f8, Talairach
evaluate verbal working memory. The only difference was that the coordinates: 49, 55, 11) showed a strong increase in gamma-
probe consisted of a horizontal, central and linear array of two, four, or band energy after presentation of the sample array, which was sustained
six letters (top-right panel in Fig. 3). The participant was asked to throughout the 3-second maintenance interval in medium (load4) and

Fig. 2. Gamma-band activity during visuo-spatial working memory task. Superior right corner of the gure depicts an example of probe stimulus. Each row represents a single-trial of
gamma activity during sample, maintenance and probe periods recorded from electrode A) f8 at the ITG and B) f6 at the fusiform gyrus. Gamma amplitude during a 2-second
window centered in the maintenance period from recording sites C) f8 and D) f6.
C.M. Hamam et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 872879 875

Fig. 3. Gamma-band activity during verbal working memory task. Superior right corner of the gure depicts an example of probe stimulus. Each row represents a single-trial of
gamma activity during task from recording site A) f8 at the ITG and B) f6 at the fusiform gyrus. Gamma amplitude during a 2-second window centered in the maintenance period
from recording sites C) f8 and D) f6.

high (load6) load conditions (Fig. 2). In contrast, the effect was absent in stimulation, we decided to test the reactivity of the recorded visual areas
either control or low load (load2) conditions. When measuring the while the patient was asked to produce vivid images of single letters
gamma-band energy specically within the maintenance period (a 2 while xating on a white sheet of paper (Supplementary Movie 1).
second window centered in that interval), we found that all trials in the The result was a systematic and unambiguous energy increase of
high load condition (12 out of 12) and all but one (11 out of 12) in the gamma-band activity sustained throughout the imagery effort at ITG,
medium load condition had energy values higher than the highest value but not at the fusiform site (Fig. 6). Interestingly, the same effect was
measured in the same interval during the control (High-loadN Control, found when the patient was instructed to imagine other more complex
Medium-loadN Control, p b 0.0001). In contrast, we found no such effect real-life objects. Therefore, the effect may have been not specic to
in the adjacent more medial fusiform gyrus (f6, Talairach coordinates: letters, but related to a visual imagery effort.
42, 55, 11).
An opposite pattern of response was found during the verbal Discussion
working memory task (Fig. 3). There was no increase in gamma-band
energy during memory maintenance or encoding at ITG (f8). In Our guiding intuition was that visual mental imagery and visuo-
contrast, the fusiform site (f6) revealed an increased gamma activity spatial working memory shared a neural substrate. Indeed, we show
with verbal memory load (medium load N control, high load N control, direct evidence that neural populations of human ITG are selectively
p b 0.002). Here again, if we take the gamma energy measured during active during both visuo-spatial working memory and visual mental
the maintenance period, from the fusiform gyrus, we found that all imagery. ITG gamma amplitude modulations during MVIS and visual
values from the high load condition were above the maximal value
observed in the control. In summary, high load could be distinguished
from the control condition on a single-trial basis in ITG during MVIS,
and in the fusiform during MVEB. Furthermore, a comparison
between maintenance values measured in ITG during MVEB and
MVIS revealed that a very strong dissociation between memory types
(Fig. 4): in the high load condition, 11 out of 12 values were higher
than the highest maintenance value measured in the MVEB condition.
In the medium load condition, this proportion was 10/12. This
corresponds to a 92% (resp. 83%) dissociation between the MVEB and
MVIS condition, a direct measure of the ability to distinguish between
verbal and visuo-spatial working memory in ITG.
Inferior temporal and fusiform gyri differed also markedly in their
gamma-band responses to visual stimulation (Fig. 5). In the fusiform
gyrus, we found that letter-strings (pseudo-words and consonant
strings), but not pictures of scenes or faces, induces a strong increase
Fig. 4. Amplitude of gamma activity from the ITG dissociates between visuospatial
of activity. In contrast, we failed to nd any gamma response to letter- (blue) and verbal (red) content of working memory maintenance. Gamma amplitude is
strings, faces or natural scenes at the ITG recording site. Since ITG displayed as a function of difculty in the working memory tasks, i.e. control, low,
responded to visuo-spatial memory maintenance, but not to visual medium and high load conditions.
876 C.M. Hamam et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 872879

Fig. 5. Inferior temporal (red) and fusiform (blue) gyrus selective visual responses to faces, natural scenes, words or pseudowords. Note that while ITG does not exhibit a particularly
strong response to the selected categories, the recording site located in the fusiform gyrus showed a largely selective visual response to written-like stimuli.

imagery were sufciently clear to be readily detected in real-time. representation (Ranganath and D'Esposito, 2005), they might at least
Furthermore, we found that ITG did not respond during visual partially share some of their neural mechanisms as well. For instance,
stimulation or verbal working memory. This dissociation is in fact it has been observed that both working memory (Howard et al., 2003;
sufciently clear to decode the type of material being maintained in Mainy et al., 2007; Tallon-Baudry et al., 2005) and visual imagery
working memory (verbal vs. visuo-spatial) on a single trial basis. The (Kreiman et al., 2002) modulate neural activity in visual areas of the
implications of these results regarding the neurophysiology and medial temporal lobe. Moreover, there is also a body of evidence for a
cognitive models of visual imagery and working memory, together distributed fronto-temporal network sustaining mental imagery
with possible novel BCIs are discussed below. (Ishai et al., 2000; Zimmer, 2008) and thus revealing further
functional and anatomical overlap. Here we show for the rst time
A common neural substrate for imagery and working memory maintenance direct electrophysiological evidence that visuo-spatial working
in the inferior temporal lobe memory and visual imagery activate the same neural population in
the ventral visual pathway. Indeed, the voluntary stabilization of a
Our ndings provide novel insights into the neurophysiology of visual mental image can be seen as functionally equivalent to its
mental imagery and working memory. Given that both phenomena maintenance on the visual sketchpad of working memory. This also
can be described as the voluntary manipulation of a mental visual ts well with the patient's ofine report that she performed our visuo-

Fig. 6. ITG (left, red) selectively increases gamma activity amplitude during visual imagery, while fusiform gyrus (right, blue) remains around baseline level. Letters depicted next to each
plot represent the tokens that subject was instructed to imagine.
C.M. Hamam et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 872879 877

spatial working memory task by mentally stabilizing the dot-array sustain a mental image of a letter, but to identify or code a visual
picture. element as a letter. Since in our verbal working memory task it was
While memory-related activity in the visual cortex is a fairly necessary to remember a given string of letters, rehearsal of the entire
common observation, it is most often found in regions responding sequence would have to involve the repeated identication of its
also to external visual inputs. Our recording site in the ITG did not reveal components, thus yielding a sustained activation of the WFA.
any visual response when the patient was passively presented with In other words, since ITG was involved in both visuo-spatial
letter strings, faces or landscapes (Fig. 5). This observation could almost working memory and visual imagery, we propose that the strategy of
by itself dene that brain region of the ITG as non-visual. Yet, it was maintenance during the visuo-spatial task was the actual retention of
active during the visual presentation of dot arrays during the working an imagined dot-array. On the other hand, the WFA showed a steady
memory task. Our interpretation is that this brain region selectively and increased GBA during verbal working memory, but not during
responds to task-relevant visual stimuli and is almost exclusively driven visual imagery. Therefore, we propose that during the verbal task,
by a top-down process. Stimuli shown passively did not require such a successful maintenance of the letter-string was accomplished not
process, thus yielding no clear visual response. In other words, our through visual imagery, but by means of covert speech or verbal
results suggest that there are regions in the ventral visual pathway that rehearsal, which could explain the observed recurrent activity in the
do not respond to visual stimuli, unless those stimuli are task-relevant. WFA during memory maintenance, but not during imagery.
This means that passive visual stimulation activate some and not all of Although the multicomponent model of working memory has
the areas in the visual pathway. Further research could conrm if there been one of the most fruitful theoretical constructs in cognitive
is a top-down/bottom-up functional organization of lateral and medial neuroscience, a number of critics have raised, specially regarding how
portions of the ventral visual pathway accounting for this nding. xed the proposed storages are (Cowan and Morey, 2006), the role of
attention (Awh and Jonides, 2001; Cowan and Morey, 2006) and the
A subtle anatomical dissociation between verbal and visuo-spatial working possibility of having a modality-independent capacity for maintaining
memory networks in the inferior temporal lobe a given item in working memory (Cowan and Morey, 2006). Our
results do not represent conclusive evidence regarding the exposed
The observed dissociation between verbal and visuo-spatial working issues of the multicomponent model, specially because of our limited
memory in the inferior temporal lobe is consistent with the classical sample size (one patient). Even so, we think our study shows that
multicomponent model rst posed by Baddeley and Hitch (1974). models of working memory could greatly benet from taking into
According to that model (see also Baddeley, 1992, 2010), visual account the complex relationship between working memory, reten-
information is temporarily sustained in a visuo-spatial sketch-pad, tion strategy and the activity in perceptual areas.
which is functionally dissociated from the phonological loop that
momentarily stores verbal information and usually involves covert Intracerebral high-frequency gamma
speech. Further research on the role of attention, have shown that
subjects who can recall a larger number of objects from a visuo-spatial We have found that high and broad gamma activity (50150 Hz)
array are also more efcient in ltering-out irrelevant information from the inferior temporal lobe is actively involved in working
(Conway et al., 2001; Olesen et al., 2007; Vogel et al., 2005; Zanto and memory and imagery. It has been proposed that neural synchroniza-
Gazzaley, 2009), suggesting that attentional processes may determine tion in the gamma-band (3060 Hz) is a fundamental physiological
storage capability. Similar results have been obtained with verbal mechanism for attention and working memory (Jensen et al., 2007).
material only when precluding covert-speech rehearsal during the One inuential hypothesis states that gamma oscillations can organize
maintenance period. This suggests that rehearsal prevention forces neural activity in time by modulating spike probability during
subjects to rely on more attention-demanding strategies (Cowan, 2008; different phases of its cycle (Fries et al., 2007). However, our data is
Cowan and Morey, 2006; Morey and Cowan, 2005). In contrast to the not sufciently precise to demonstrate that our broad-band activity
multicomponent model of working memory, those ndings have been (50150 Hz) corresponds to that synchronization process. An alter-
interpreted as pointing to a modality-independent capability that does native view is that high-frequency broad-band gamma may be the
not depend on pre-dened stores or buffers (Cowan, 2008; Cowan and trace of an increase of neural ring rate (Dalal et al., 2011; Manning et
Morey, 2007). al., 2009; Miller, 2010). In either case, we have found a reliable neural
Here, we found that gamma-band activity in the ITG proportion- marker of visual imagery and visuo-spatial memory maintenance in
ally increases with visuo-spatial working memory load, suggesting the inferior temporal lobe and more evidence for a functional role of
that ITG participates of the proposed visual sketchpad. Similarly, in a high frequency gamma in working memory and the visual system.
previous study by our team, intracerebral EEG were used to localize a
set of perisylvian cortical areas involved in the phonological loop Visual imagery and BCI
(Mainy et al., 2007; Vidal et al., 2010). Regardless of its modality,
maintenance of information in working memory most likely involves Brain Computer Interfaces (BCI) are widely known for transforming
some sort of mental imagery. Indeed, we have shown that ITG had a thoughts into actions. BCI systems take a recorded brain signal, which
sustained gamma response not only during visuo-spatial working can be voluntarily modulated by motor intention (e.g. Daly and
memory, but also during visual imagery. Accordingly, a shared neural Wolpaw, 2008; Hochberg et al., 2006; Jerbi et al., 2009a; Kim et al.,
substrate between working memory and imagery may very well 2008; Leuthardt et al., 2004; Schalk et al., 2007), attention (van Gerven
explain the pattern of gamma-band activity that we found in the ITG. and Jensen, 2009; van Gerven et al., 2009; Vansteensel et al., 2010) or
We have also shown that in the adjacent fusiform gyrus, there was an imagery, and use it to control a computer or robotic device (Birbaumer
increase in gamma amplitude during presentation, maintenance, but et al., 2008; Jerbi et al., 2010). Real-time recognition of a neural signature
not imagery of verbal information (letter strings). Interestingly, this of visual mental imagery serves as critical proof of concept for
recording site was located (Talairach coordinates: 42, 55, 11) implementing BCIs controlled by visual thoughts. An advantageous
very closely if not inside the visual word-form area (WFA), a part of aspect of such device would be that visual mental imagery does not
the visual cortex that is selectively involved in processing letter- require training and can be exercised by patients as a natural cognitive
strings (Cohen et al., 2000; Dehaene and Cohen, 2011; Mainy et al., strategy for controlling a computer or a robotic machine. The next step,
2008; Price and Devlin, 2011). The fact that the WFA increases its for visual imagery to provide an alternative/supplementary approach to
activity during verbal working memory, but not during letter imagery, existing invasive BCI techniques that use motor-related activity, would
suggests that its role in the phonological loop is not to create or be to close the loop between user and operated device. For the time
878 C.M. Hamam et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 872879

being, this can only be achieved invasively using depth electrodes Cowan, N., 2008. What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working
memory? Prog. Brain Res. 169, 323338.
implanted into the infero-temporal cortex. As much as it is more Cowan, C., Morey, C.C., 2006. Visual working memory depends on attentional ltering.
desirable to avoid surgery risks by using non-invasive methods, there is Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 139141.
increasing agreement that intracranial recordings of small neural Cowan, N., Morey, C.C., 2007. How can dual-task working memory retention limits be
investigated? Psychol. Sci. 18, 686688.
populations provide the most stable, clean, and effective input signal Dalal, S.S., Vidal, J.R., Hamam, C.M., Ossandon, T., Bertrand, O., Lachaux, J.P., Jerbi, K.,
for BCI (Moran, 2010). However, since data was acquired from a single 2011. Spanning the rich spectrum of the human brain: slow waves to gamma and
patient and fully operational BCI testing (closed loop between brain and beyond. Brain Struct. Funct. 216, 7784.
Daly, J.J., Wolpaw, J.R., 2008. Brain-computer interfaces in neurological rehabilitation.
computer) was not performed, we are not able to currently deliver the Lancet Neurol. 7, 10321043.
application. Nonetheless, we claim that the presented evidence points to Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., 2011. The unique role of the visual word form area in reading.
the feasibility of BCIs controlled by visual imagery and that further BCI Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 254262.
Fries, P., Nikolic, D., Singer, W., 2007. The gamma cycle. Trends Neurosci. 30, 309316.
development could largely benet from researching how motor-related
Gyselinck, V., De Beni, R., Pazzaglia, F., Meneghetti, C., Mondoloni, A., 2007. Working
signals can be complemented, or even replaced, by visual imagery- memory components and imagery instructions in the elaboration of a spatial
related activity from the inferior temporal lobe. mental model. Psychol. Res. 71, 373382.
Haxby, J.V., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L.G., Maisog, J.M., Pietrini, P., Grady, C.L., 1994. The
functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective
Conclusions attention to faces and locations. J. Neurosci. 14, 63366353.
Hochberg, L.R., Serruya, M.D., Friehs, G.M., Mukand, J.A., Saleh, M., Caplan, A.H., Branner,
We found selective increases in gamma-band (50150 Hz) activity A., Chen, D., Penn, R.D., Donoghue, J.P., 2006. Neuronal ensemble control of
prosthetic devices by a human with tetraplegia. Nature 442, 164171.
of the ITG during visual imagery and visuo-spatial working memory Howard, M.W., Rizzuto, D.S., Caplan, J.B., Madsen, J.R., Lisman, J., Aschenbrenner-
maintenance. Measures from this and the adjacent fusiform gyrus Scheibe, R.A., Schulze-Bonhage, A., Kahana, M.J., 2003. gamma oscillations correlate
revealed a clear distinction between areas involved in the phonolog- with working memory load in humans. Cereb. Cortex 13, 13691374.
Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L.G., Haxby, J.V., 2000. Distributed neural systems for the
ical loop or visual sketchpad, suggesting a novel role of the WFA in generation of visual images. Neuron 28, 979990.
working memory. Moreover, our results suggest that lateral and Jensen, O., Kaiser, J., Lachaux, J.P., 2007. Human gamma-frequency oscillations
medial portions of the inferior temporal visual cortex may selectively associated with attention and memory. Trends Neurosci. 30, 317324.
Jerbi, K., Freyermuth, S., Minotti, L., Kahane, P., Berthoz, A., Lachaux, J.P., 2009a.
respond to top-down or bottom-up modulations of activity in the Watching BrainTV and playing brain ball: exploring novel BCI strategies using real-
gamma range. Finally, we found that neural activity related to time analysis of human intracranial data. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 86, 159168.
voluntary cognitive operations, such as visual imagery and working Jerbi, K., Ossandon, T., Hamam, C.M., Senova, S., Dalal, S.S., Jung, J., Minotti, L., Bertrand,
O., Berthoz, A., Kahane, P., Lachaux, J.P., 2009b. Task-related brain dynamics from an
memory maintenance, can be recorded in real-time, in sites which intracerebral perspective: Review and implications for surface EEG and MEG. Hum.
do not respond to visual stimulation, thus opening a promising Brain Mapp. 30, 17581771.
possibility for novel BCIs. Jerbi, K., Vidal, J.R., Mattout, J., Maby, E., Bauchet-Lecaignard, F., Ossandon, T.,
Hamam, C.M., Dalal, S.S., Lachaux, J.P., Leahy, R.M., Baillet, S., Garnero, L.,
Supplementary materials related to this article can be found online
Delpuech, C., Bertrand, O., 2010. Inferring hand movement kinematics with MEG
at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.087. and EEG: from brain-machine interfaces to motor rehabilitation. IRBM Biomed.
Eng. Res. 32, 818.
Kahane, P., Minotti, L., Hoffmann, D., Lachaux, J.P., Ryvlin, P., 2004. Invasive EEG in the
Acknowledgments
denition of the seizure onset zone: depth electrodes. In: Rosenow, F., Lders, H.O.
(Eds.), Presurgical assessment of the epilepsies with clinical neurophysiology and
We would like to thank the patient who participated in the study functional imaging. : Handbook of Clinical Neurophysiology, vol.3. Elsevier BV,
Amsterdam, pp. 109133.
and the invaluable support of the staff from Grenoble University
Kim, S.P., Simeral, J.D., Hochberg, L.R., Donoghue, J.P., Black, M.J., 2008. Neural control of
Hospital Epilepsy Unit. Funding was provided by: Fondation Fyssen to computer cursor velocity by decoding motor cortical spiking activity in humans
C.M.H., Fellowship from Ministre de l'Education Nationale et la with tetraplegia. J. Neural Eng. 5, 455476.
Recherche (France) to T.O., Fondation pour la Recherche Medicale Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I., 2002. Imagery neurons in the human brain. Nature 408,
357361.
(FRM), the Marie Curie Fellowship (FP7 Grant PIIF-GA-2008-221097) Lachaux, J.P., Jerbi, K., Bertrand, O., Minotti, L., Hoffmann, D., Schoendorff, B., Kahane, P.,
to S.S.D., BrainSync FP7 European Project (Grant HEALTH-F2-2008- 2007a. A blueprint for real-time functional mapping via human intracranial
200728), ANR OPENVIBE2 to J.R.V. and J.P.L. recordings. PLoS One 10, e1094.
Lachaux, J.P., Jerbi, K., Bertrand, O., Minotti, L., Hoffmann, D., Schoendorff, B., Kahane, P.,
2007b. BrainTV: a novel approach for online mapping of human brain functions.
References Biol. Res. 40, 401413.
Le Bihan, D., Turner, R., Zefro, T.A., Cunod, C.A., Jezzard, P., Bonnerot, V., 1993.
Atkinson, R.C., Shiffrin, R.M., 1968. Human memory: a proposed system and its control Activation of human primary visual cortex during visual recall: a magnetic
processes. In: Spence, K.W., Spence, J.T. (Eds.), The Psychology of Learning and resonance study. PNAS 90, 1180211805.
Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, vol. 2. Academic Press, New York, Le Van Quyen, M., Foucher, J., Lachaux, J.P., Rodriguez, E., Lutz, A., Martinerie, J., Varela,
pp. 89195. F.J., 2001. Comparison of Hilbert transform and wavelet methods for the analysis of
Awh, E., Jonides, J., 2001. Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working neuronal synchrony. J. Neurosci. Methods 111, 8398.
memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 119126. Leuthardt, E.C., Schalk, G., Wolpaw, J.R., Ojemann, J.G., Moran, D.W., 2004. A brain-
Baddeley, A.D., 1992. Working memory. Science 255, 556559. computer interface using electrocorticographic signals in humans. J. Neural Eng. 1,
Baddeley, A., 2010. Working memory. Curr. Biol. 20, R136R140. 6371.
Baddeley, A.D., Andrade, J., 2000. Working memory and the vividness of imagery. J. Exp. Mainy, N., Kahane, P., Minotti, L., Hoffmann, D., Bertrand, O., Lachaux, J.P., 2007. Neural
Psychol. Gen. 129, 126145. correlates of consolidation in working memory. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 183193.
Baddeley, A.D., Hitch, G., 1974. Working memory. In: Bower, G.H. (Ed.), The psychology Mainy, N., Jung, J., Baciu, M., Kahane, P., Schoendorff, B., Minotti, L., Hoffmann, D.,
of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, vol. 8. Academic Bertrand, O., Lachaux, J.P., 2008. Cortical dynamics of word recognition. Hum. Brain
Press, New York, pp. 4789. Mapp. 29, 12151230.
Birbaumer, N., Murguialday, A.R., Cohen, L., 2008. Brain-computer interface in paralysis. Manning, J.R., Jacobs, J., Fried, I., Kahana, M.J., 2009. Broadband shifts in local eld
Curr. Opin. Neurol. 21, 634638. potential power spectra are correlated with single-neuron spiking in humans.
Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., 2000. Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and J. Neurosci. 29, 1361313620.
fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 147. Miller, K.J., 2010. Broadband spectral change: evidence for a macroscale correlate of
Cattaneo, Z., Vecchi, T., Pascual-Leone, A., Silvanto, J., 2009. Contrasting early visual population ring rate? J. Neurosci. 30, 64776479.
cortical activation states causally involved in visual imagery and short-term Moran, D., 2010. Evolution of brain-computer interface: action potentials, local eld
memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 30, 13931400. potentials and electrocorticograms. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 741745.
Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Lehricy, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Hnaff, M.A., Morey, C.C., Cowan, N., 2005. When do visual and verbal memories conict? The
Michel, F., 2000. The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization importance of working-memory load retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.
of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain 123, 31, 703713.
291307. Moulton, S.T., Kosslyn, S.M., 2009. Imagining predictions: mental imagery as mental
Conway, A.R.A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M.F., 2001. The cocktail party phenomenon emulation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 12731280.
revisited: the importance of working memory capacity. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, Olesen, P.J., Macoveanu, J., Tegnr, J., Klinberg, T., 2007. Brain activity related to working
331335. memory and distraction in children and adults. Cereb. Cortex 17, 10471054.
C.M. Hamam et al. / NeuroImage 59 (2012) 872879 879

Price, C.J., Devlin, J.D., 2011. The interactive account of ventral occipitotemporal van Gerven, M., Jensen, O., 2009. Attention modulations of posterior alpha as a control signal
contributions to reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 246253. for two-dimensional braincomputer interfaces. J. Neurosci. Methods 179, 7884.
Ranganath, C., D'Esposito, M., 2005. Directing the mind's eye: prefrontal, inferior and van Gerven, M., Bahramisharif, A., Heskes, T., Jensen, O., 2009. Selecting features for
medial temporal mechanisms for visual working memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, BCI control based on a covert spatial attention paradigm. Neural Netw. 22,
175182. 12711277.
Schalk, G., Kubnek, J., Miller, K.J., Anderson, N.R., Leuthardt, E.C., Ojemann, J.G., Vansteensel, M.J., Hermes, D., Aarnoutse, E.J., Bleichner, M.G., Schalk, G., van Rijen, P.C.,
Limbrick, D., Moran, D., Gerhardt, L.A., Wolpaw, J.R., 2007. Decoding two- Leijten, F.S.S., Ramsey, N.F., 2010. Braincomputer interfacing based on cognitive
dimensional movement trajectories using electrocorticographic signals in humans. control. Ann. Neurol. 67, 809816.
J. Neural Eng. 4, 264275. Vidal, J.R., Ossandon, T., Jerbi, K., Dalal, S.S., Minotti, L., Ryvlin, P., Kahane, P., Lachaux, J.P.,
Serences, J.T., Ester, E.F., Vogel, E.K., Awh, E., 2009. Stimulus-specic delay activity in 2010. Category-specic visual responses: an intracranial study comparing gamma,
human primary visual cortex. Psychol. Sci. 20, 207214. beta, alpha, and ERP response selectivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, A195.
Talairach, J., Tournoux, P., 1988. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: Vogel, E.K., McCollough, A.W., Machizawa, M.G., 2005. Neural measures reveal
3-dimensional proportional system - an approach to cerebral imaging. Thieme individual differences controlling access to working memory. Nature 438, 500503.
Medical Publishers, New York. Zanto, T.P., Gazzaley, A., 2009. Neural suppression of irrelevant information underlies
Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Hnaff, M.A., Isnard, J., Fischer, C., 2005. Attention optimal performance. J. Neurosci. 29, 30593066.
modulates gamma-band oscillations differently in the human lateral occipital Zimmer, H.D., 2008. Visual and spatial working memory: from boxes to networks.
cortex and fusiform gyrus. Cereb. Cortex 15, 654662. Neurosci. Behav. Rev. 32, 13731395.

You might also like