You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1992, Vol. 60, No. 6, 893-903 0022-006X/92/$3.00

Academic Underachievement, Attention Deficits, and Aggression:


Comorbidity and Implications for Intervention
Stephen R Hinshaw
University of California, Berkeley

Although comorbidity with specific learning disabilities is less frequent than commonly reported,
externalizing behavior disordersparticularly attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
often overlap with various indices of academic underachievement during childhood. Furthermore,
by adolescence, delinquency is clearly associated with school failure. Because the link between
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavioral and learning problems often appears before formal schooling, and because the comor-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

bid problems predict a negative course, early intervention is a necessity. Controlled treatment
investigations with youngsters who show these combined problems are rare, and such studies
present a host of methodologic and practical problems. I discuss issues surrounding multimodality
treatment programs and the potential for long-term interventions to break cycles of school failure
and externalizing behavior.

Overlap between achievement difficulties, particularly in suspension, poor absolute performance on standardized
reading, and behavior problems of an externalizing or acting- achievement tests, and achievement test scores that fall below
out nature has been noted for many decades. My purposes in the level predicted from the child's intelligence. This heteroge-
this article are to (a) highlight diagnostic issues that pertain to neous set of criteria for underachievement presents a major
each domain; (b) cover theoretical issues regarding the overlap methodologic and conceptual problem for those who wish to
of these problem areas, including clarification of the specificity appraise either comorbidity with behavioral disorders or effects
of the linkage; (c) review validated intervention strategies for of interventions.
the single disorders; (d) report on empirical treatment investi- Over the past several decades, considerable research focus
gations of children with comorbid achievement and behavior has been directed to the subgroup of underachieving young-
problems; (e) discuss pertinent clinical, methodologic, and theo- sters whose test scores in academic areas, particularly reading,
retical issues regarding such intervention; and (f) make recom- fall substantially below levels that would be expected on the
mendations for subsequent research. As readers will discover, basis of their intellectual abilities. The underlying conception is
the field has far to go before definitive treatment prescriptions to separate those children with demonstrable reasons for low
can be made with regard to youngsters who display the distress- achievement (i.e., subaverage intelligence) from those whose aca-
ingly persistent combination of externalizing behavior prob- demic deficiencies are not so readily explainable. The latter
lems and academic failure. group is considered to have specific learning disabilities (or, in
the nomenclature of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders [3rd ed., rev; DSM-HI-R; American Psychiat-
Description of Domains ric Association, 1987], specific developmental disorders). Be-
Academic Underachievement cause their deficient academic performance is not attributable
to subaverage cognitive potential, to obvious physical or sensory
Reflecting the variegated conceptions of academic attain- deficits, or to inferior educational opportunities (see Rutter &
ment and academic failure that currently exist, a wide variety of Yule, 1975; Yule & Rutter, 1985), these children are often pre-
terms is used to describe children whose school achievement is sumed to have subtle neurological deficiencies that underlie the
found to be deficient (e.g., school failure, learning difficulty, achievement problems. (Note that because group-administered
underachievement, specific learning disability, dyslexia, and general-ability tests place a premium on readingwhich
specific developmental disorder). Thus, in empirical investiga- would confound a diagnosis on the basis of disparity between
tions, outcome measures that pertain to suboptimal academic intelligence and reading skillit is considered a necessity to
performance are quite divergent; they include (to name a few) diagnose such specific learning disabilities on the basis of indi-
placement in special education classes, retention, low grades, vidually administered IQ tests.)
The divergent validity of such IQ-discrepant learning diffi-
culties from so-called general problems in achievement has
This article was supported by National Institute of Mental Health been examined in a number of investigations. Recent evidence
Grant 45064. indicates that, despite the importance of considering IQ in as-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ste- sessing learning problems (Torgeson, 1989) and despite the sur-
phen P. Hinshaw, Department of Psychology, Tolman Hall, University prisingly slow progress made by children with specific learning
of California, Berkeley, California 94720. problems (Rutter & Yule, 1975), the patterns of difference be-
893
894 STEPHEN P. HINSHAW

tween general and specific learning difficulties are less clear guage delays, neuropsychological strengths and weaknesses)
and less stable over time than once thought (e.g., McGee, Wil- and in the environment (e.g., classroom composition, familial
liams, Share, Anderson, & Silva, 1986; Taylor, 1988), and sole attitudes) is required before lasting gains from intervention can
consideration of children with specific learning problems ne- be expected (Taylor, 1989; but see also Gittelman & Feingold,
glects the very real achievement problems of children with IQ 1983).
scores below the normchildren who are likely to suffer addi-
tional problems of low social class and poor educational oppor-
Disruptive Behavior Disorders
tunities. Furthermore, because the majority of children with
poor achievement scores fall into the "general" category, exclu- Whereas diagnosis of learning/achievement difficulties is
sive focus on specific learning disabilities limits representative- typically based on test scores gained from administration of
ness. In short, the inclusionary criterion of an IQ-achievement individual assessments of IQ and academic performance, chil-
disparity may unduly restrict sampling of achievement prob- dren are categorized as behaviorally disordered chiefly by
lems and may lead to neglect of important motivational and means of parental or teacher ratings (or, more recently, struc-
social factors that pertain to poor academic performance (Tay- tured interviews). Externalizing behavior disorderspresently
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

lor, 1989).' called disruptive behavior disorders in DSM-HI-Rhave a long


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Prevalence estimates for underachievement vary widely, de- psychometric history; they incorporate such features as impul-
pending on the type and stringency of the criteria involved in sivity, defiance, disruption, inattention, overactivity, and a host
diagnosis. The epidemiologic work of Rutter and colleagues in of antisocial acts. These behaviors are psychometrically separa-
the United Kingdom indicated that general reading problems ble from internalizing tendencies, which constitute sadness,
were present at levels of 7% on the Isle of Wight and 20% in withdrawal, somatic concerns, and anxiety. In addition, most
inner London, whereas specific (IQ-discrepant) reading disabil- evidence reveals that externalizing or disruptive behavioral
ities had respective prevalences of 4% and 10% (Rutter et al., problems are more likely to persist over time than are interna-
1974). These data reveal both the greater presence of general, as lizing behaviors. Furthermore, they are quite refractory to typi-
opposed to specific, learning problems and the strong influ- cal interventions and, like severe underachievement, comprise
ence of psychosocial factors, particularly inner-city life and low a major psychological, economic, and social problem (Kazdin,
socioeconomic status (SES), on rates of severe underachieve- 1987; Loeber, 1990; Robins, 1991).
ment. Data from the United States reveal similar prevalence Although the various externalizing behavior problems tend
rates (e.g., American Psychiatric Association, 1987; Taylor, to load together on higher order factorial dimensions, mean-
1989). Although boys typically outnumber girls by a consider- ingful subdivisions exist within this domain. In particular, di-
able margin with respect to diagnosed learning problems, re- mensions or categories of inattentive and impulsive actions are
cent data from unselected populations indicate that this dispar- separable from defiant, aggressive, and antisocial behaviors on
ity may reflect gender-related differences in accompanying the basis of criteria that are both internal (i.e., different factor
behavior problems rather than in absolute levels of underachie- structure) and external (e.g., divergent family history, correlates,
vement (Shaywitz, Shaywitz, Fletcher, & Escobar, 1990). Severe and course; see Hinshaw, 1987). In DSM-HI-R, the pertinent
learning deficits in children are quite costly, in terms of both categories include attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
the enormous expenses related to special education and the (ADHD), which is marked by developmentally inappropriate
personal suffering and frustration for children and families; and persistent dysregulation of attention, impulse control, and
furthermore, they are quite persistent, with continuing achieve- motoric activity. (In several of the investigations reviewed be-
ment difficulties triggering accompanying problems in self-es- low, earlier diagnostic procedures mandate the use of prior
teem, peer relations, and adult adjustment (e.g., Horn, O'Don- terms for ADHD-related syndromes, including both hyperactiv-
nell, & Vitulano, 1983; Spreen, 1988). Thus, development of ity and attention-deficit disorder with hyperactivity, or ADDH.)
effective intervention strategies for underachieving children More frankly antisocial disorders include oppositional-defiant
must be viewed as a major societal issue. disorder (ODD) and conduct disorder (CD). ODD and CD are
To establish a rational basis on which to develop effective syndromes marked by excessive levels of hostility, defiance, and
treatments, investigators have attempted to discern meaningful noncompliance and by recurrent antisocial and aggressive ac-
subgroups of the wide array of children with deficient academic tions, including assault, bullying, stealing, and fighting, respec-
skills (e.g., Lyon, 1985). Indeed, proponents of such subtyping tively. Whereas extensive comorbidity exists between ADHD
efforts contend that a given group of children with similar lev- and these more aggressive disruptive behavior disorders (see
els of deficient academic performance in, say, reading often Abikoff & Klein, 1992; Hinshaw, 1987) and whereas latent vari-
display markedly divergent neuropsychological or motivational ables of these constructs correlate at extremely high levels (Fer-
deficits (Rourke, 1985; Snowling, 1991), differences that are gusson, Horwood, & Lloyd, 1991), their divergence with re-
masked by classification on the basis of achievement-related spect to important external criteria is important to take into
performance per se. Whereas protracted discussion of the account both theoretically and clinically (see Szatmari, Offord,
various theoretical bases for such subtyping is beyond the & Boyle, 1989a). That is, despite their overlap, they constitute
scope of this article, the overarching point is that establishment partially independent dimensions or diagnoses, and such inde-
of a learning disability by way of documentation of low aca-
demic test scores, whether or not IQ is considered, is only the
1
beginning of treatment planning. Consideration of a host of I do not consider the behavioral problems of children with clear
additional factors both within the child (e.g., presence of lan- mental retardation in this article.
SPECIAL SECTION: UNDERACHIEVEMENT, ATTENTION, AND AGGRESSION 895

pendence pertains even to their differential overlap with under- and intraindividual factors are necessary to provide for com-
achievement (see Comorbidity Between Domains section). prehensive treatment of disruptive behavior disorders.
The prevalence of behavioral disorders that are defined
chiefly through quantitative ratings is somewhat arbitrary, re-
flecting the choice of cutoff scores used to define deviant status Comorbidity Between Domains
and the choice of constituent behaviors believed, at any given
time, to constitute the disorder. Thus, prevalence estimates for I recently completed an extensive review of issues pertaining
the disruptive behavior disorders have varied over the past sev- to the Comorbidity between externalizing behavior problems
eral decades. Currently, ADHD is thought to occur with a fre- and academic underachievement (Hinshaw, 1992). Space here
quency of 3% to 6% in school-aged populations (American Psy- allows only a brief summary of the key conclusions from this
chiatric Association, 1987; Szatmari, Offord, & Boyle, 1989b). work. First, overlap between externalizing behavioral syn-
Boys outnumber girls with a ratio of approximately 3:1 in epide- dromes and underachievement occurs at levels that are far
miologic surveys; the ratio is considerably higher in clinic sam- above chance rates, with estimates ranging quite widely (from
ples. Similar figures apply to conduct disorder, which occurs in less than 10% to more than 50%). Critical to such figures is the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

precise definition of underachievement that is used. Indeed,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

3% to 7% of children and which shows a 2:1 to 3:1 male-to-fe-


male predominance (Robins, 1991). ODD, a relatively new the percentage of externalizing children who have specific
diagnostic category containing milder symptoms of noncom- learning disabilities, defined by IQ-achievement discrepancies,
pliant behavior that may represent precursors of later CD is lower than commonly believed, in the range of 6% to 20%
(Loeber, Lahey, & Thomas, 1991), has an apparently higher (e.g., Forness, Youpa, Hanna, Cantwell, & Swanson, 1992). Yet,
prevalence rate, although definitive figures are not available. with broader definitions of school failure that include reten-
The externalizing behavior disorders have several associated tion, low grades, and suspensions, the figures rise dramatically
features that are deserving of mention. First, children with (see Barkley, Fischer, Edelbrock, & Smallish, 1990). It is note-
these conditions have noteworthy problems with peers. Young- worthy that Comorbidity in the other direction is not nearly so
sters with exclusive aggression are often categorized as socio- specific; that is, among carefully diagnosed learning disabled
metrically controversial, meaning that they are liked by some youngsters, the most frequent pattern of accompanying behav-
classmates and disliked by others; children with ADHD are ior problems appears to be in the internalizing domain (Fuerst,
quite frequently rejected by their peers, and those with comor- Fisk, & Rourke, 1989), and only a minority of learning disabled
bid ADHD and aggressive features are almost universally re- children have diagnosable externalizing disorders.
jected by agemates (Milich & Landau, 1989). Given the strong Second, in opposition to many earlier reports that posited
predictive power of peer rejection in childhood for a host of links between aggressive behavior and underachievement, the
negative outcomes later in life (Parker & Asher, 1987), the nega- specific relation in childhood pertains to ADHD (e.g., McGee,
tive peer status of children with externalizing behavior prob- Williams, & Silva, 1985). The overlap between childhood ag-
lems is quite salient. Second, lowered self-esteem is common in gression and underachievement is mediated chiefly through
children with a history of acting-out behavior; by preadoles- the Comorbidity of aggression with ADHD (Frick et al., 1991).
cence, conduct disorder co-occurs at high rates with major de- By adolescence, however, a clear association exists between de-
pression (see Puig-Antich, 1982). The latter association is clini- linquency and underachievement, clarifying the need for aware-
cally important; theoretically, it calls into question any simplis- ness of developmental change factors in specifying comorbidity
tic notions about the separability of severe internalizing from between these domains.
severe externalizing disorders. Third, overlap with academic Third, despite considerable theorizing about unidirectional
underachievement, which is the focus of this article, is widely paths from early underachievement to subsequent behavior
cited (e.g., McGee & Share, 1988). problems, or from early externalizing tendencies to later learn-
Although symptom lists for the disruptive behavior disorders ing difficulties, the overlap between externalizing behavior
continue to be modified and sharpened on the basis of empiri- problems and learning difficulty often begins during the pre-
cal findings, the presence of systematic compilations of acting school years, with significant association documented before
out or inattentive and hyperactive behaviors is not sufficient to the onset of formal schooling (see the review by Hinshaw, 1992).
establish individualized treatment plans for such youngsters. Thus, whereas early behavior problems may precipitate later
For one thing, the symptoms of ADHD, ODD, and CD are underachievement or acting-out behavior may emerge afresh
embedded within familial, peer, and school contexts; consider- from a history of school failure in selected subgroups of young-
ation of such environmental factors is critical if treatment ef- sters (e.g., Maughan, Gray, & Rutter, 1985), the overlap between
forts are to be initiated, much less maintained. In particular, domains typically appears quite early in development, suggest-
the effects of behavioral interventions for acting-out behavior ing the operation of underlying causal factors (e.g., mild to mod-
do not persist or generalize without systematic efforts to pro- erate language deficits, poor verbal skills, familial distress or
mote transfer (e.g., McMahon & Wells, 1989); such efforts must discord, neurodevelopmental delay) in shaping eventual Co-
be directed toward the particular family configuration and morbidity. Developmental progressions are likely to be com-
school setting in which the child interacts. In addition, such plex in this regard: For example, children with the combination
individual variables as readiness to acknowledge problems, of attentional problems, aggression, and verbal/neuropsycho-
motivation for change, and concurrent anxious or depressed logical deficits during the preschool years are at greatly in-
symptoms may weigh heavily in treatment recommendations. creased risk for delinquency by adolescence (Moffitt, 1990).
In short, evaluations that incorporate pertinent social system Attacking preachievement and behavior problems during early
896 STEPHEN P. HINSHAW

developmental phases thus appears crucial for averting negative & Donaldson, 1989). It is important to consider that if the
outcomes. achievement gains are to be maintained, interventionists must
Fourth, youngsters with learning disabilities who show con- also take into account ancillary aspects of academic learning
comitant problem behaviors of an acting-out variety (atten- problems in children, including motivational variables and the
tional difficulties, conduct problems, or both) are at higher risk child's attribution of failures and gains to controllable versus
for displaying continuing deficits in achievement during the uncontrollable causes (e.g., Licht & Kistner, 1986). Indeed, far
grade-school years than are those children with internalizing more research is needed on the ability of remedial interven-
tendencies or those without concurrent problem behavior tions to effect long-term gains for children with learning defi-
(McKinney, 1989). Thus, comorbidity is important prognosti- cits.
cally. The pharmacologic treatment of underachievement is a con-
In summary, externalizing behavior disorders overlap consid- troversial area. Earlier reports contending that stimulant medi-
erably with at least some degree of academic underachieve- cation provided little or no benefit in the academic domain
ment, with the predominant link between ADHD and achieve- were plagued by methodologic and conceptual problems. In
ment delays in childhood giving way to a strong association fact, it is now clear that for children with ADHD, stimulant
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

between delinquency and school failure in adolescence. Be- medications yield a significant and positive impact on a host of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cause inattention and impulse-control problems are correlated variables that are related to academic attainment, including
with readiness difficulties early in development, and because attention span, academic productivity, reading comprehension,
externalizing behavior problems and severe learning failure are and even complex problem solving (e.g., Douglas, Barr, Amin,
quite difficult to treat once established, early intervention ef- O'Neill, & Britton, 1988; see review in Pelham, 1986). Yet,
forts are strongly encouraged, as I will highlight in more depth achievement test performance in reading is not significantly
subsequently. At this point, I turn to coverage of established altered by medication treatment, and the use of stimulants for
intervention strategies for each problem domain. After this nec- children with learning disabilities that are unaccompanied by
essarily brief review, I take up, in some detail, those treatment externalizing behavior problems has not been successful (Git-
studies that have explicitly investigated youngsters with comor- telman, Klein, & Feingold, 1983).2 Whether combinations of
bid externalizing behavior problems and learning deficiencies. stimulant medication with psychosocial and academic inter-
ventions can yield benefits for children with comorbid under-
Intervention Strategies for Underachievement achievement and disruptive behavior patterns is discussed in a
and Learning Disabilities subsequent section.
Several additional comments are in order. First, most of the
The education and treatment of children with severe learning intervention research in the field is directed toward children
deficits entail significant costs. Indeed, in terms of the public with specific (IQ-discrepant) learning disabilities, with reading
education system alone, provision of special education services skill receiving, by far, the most attention. Thus, despite their
to children with learning handicaps is a multibillion dollar massive numbers, very real problems with academic attain-
enterprise each year (Ceci, 1986). Yet the empirical database on ment, and ability to make noteworthy gains (Rutter & Yule,
which remedial programs for such youngsters is based is dis- 1975), children with general achievement difficulties comprise
tressingly thin. Virtually no evidence is available regarding a neglected population with regard to treatment research. As
long-term efficacy of special educational efforts, and only a noted earlier, a related point is that consideration of this large
handful of well-controlled short-term investigations have domain would force confrontation with a host of policy and
yielded promising leads (see the excellent review in Taylor, school system issues and on the need to direct systematic inter-
1989). vention toward the social and environmental factors that are
Perhaps the firmest conclusion that can be drawn from the associated with globally deficient academic performance.
literature is that the long history of intervention efforts directed Next, learning problems other than those in reading (e.g., spel-
toward ameliorating basic perceptual processes, psychological ling, mathematics, and nonverbal learning disabilities) have
processes, or both that allegedly underlie learning difficulties is only recently been recognized as separable problems deserving
misguided. For example, training in perceptual-motor skills is of selective intervention (e.g., Rourke, 1985). Increased research
simply not a sufficient intervention for learning disabled chil- density in these domains is likely. Finally, it has been contended
dren (Kavale & Mattson, 1983). Although holding considerable that gaps in knowledge about effective intervention strategies
promise, more modern efforts to specify and treat relevant pro-
cess variables (e.g., metacognitive skills, A. L. Brown & Cam- 2
pione, 1986; or learning strategies, Deshler, Warner, Schu- A fascinating, recent investigation by Balthazor, Wagner, and Pel-
maker, & Alley, 1983) lack empirical support for children with ham (1991) indicated that methylphenidate's positive effects on read-
severe learning difficulties. ing comprehension and letter-matching tasks were apparently not me-
diated by any ability of the medication to enhance phonologic process-
Those interventions with the most empirical justification ing or to retrieve name codes from long-term memory. Rather, the
are those that provide instruction in the precise academic skills benefits of the medication appeared to be related to nonspecific fac-
that are deficient, such as phonetic word decoding or oral and tors, possibly including enhancement of sustained attention or amelio-
written language skills. Carefully designed investigations using ration of controlled processing. Thus, stimulants apparently do not
these approaches have yielded solid evidence for impressive directly improve the skills thought to underlie reading but enhance
treatment efficacy in children with specific reading disabilities academic functioning as a result of more general facilitation of motiva-
(Gittelman & Feingold, 1983; Lovett, Ransby, Hardwick, Johns, tion, attention, and performance.
SPECIAL SECTION: UNDERACHIEVEMENT, ATTENTION, AND AGGRESSION 897

relate, in part, to mistaken assumptions about the homogeneity yield generalized, durable effects for periods of 1 year or longer,
of children with achievement problems and that intervention with parental motivation a key ingredient in mediating success.
tailored to empirically validated subtypes of learning deficits is This latter issue is far from inconsequential, given the tendency
crucial (Lyon, 1985). On the other side of this debate are those for aggressive externalizing youngsters to have family histories
(e.g., Gittelman & Feingold, 1983) who contend that the search marked by substance abuse and antisocial behavior and family
for Subtype X Treatment interactions is premature until evi- climates highlighted by disruption and discord (Hetherington
dence for any sustained, meaningful improvements from inter- & Martin, 1986; Lahey et al., 1988; Schachar & Wachsmuth,
vention with learning problems can be demonstrated. Indeed, 1990).
on the basis of their controlled outcome investigation, in which Cognitive-behavioral approaches, which combine problem-
systematic instruction in phonics led to significant and lasting solving training with reinforcement, have led to clear improve-
reading gains in severely disabled readers that persisted for up ments in the behavior of young, aggressive children (e.g., Kaz-
to 8 months postintervention, Gittelman and Feingold (1983) din, Bass, Siegel, & Thomas, 1989). Yet, with few short-term
stated that "intensive training of reading skills should be used exceptions in the realms of anger control and playground social
to improve reading performance in children with reading dis- behavior, they have not proven successful for children with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

orders, regardless of other diagnostic refinementswhether ADHD (see Abikoff, 1991; Hinshaw & Erhardt, 1991). Further-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

based on perceptual, motor, or neuropsychological test perfor- more, it is safe to say that these treatments have not lived up to
mance" (p. 189). their initial promise of enhanced generalization and mainte-
nance for clinical populations of externalizing children. Fi-
Intervention Strategies for Externalizing nally, as with most interventions for adolescents with delin-
quent/disruptive behavior, residential treatment options have
Behavior Problems
produced limited benefits.
Like underachievement, the domain of externalizing behav- For ADHD, stimulant medications are the most efficacious
ior problems has proven distressingly resistant to long-term short-term intervention (Gittelman et al., 1980). A host of well-
amelioration with existing treatment regimens. \et, both psy- controlled investigations document the ability of methylpheni-
chosocial and pharmacologic interventions can each lead to date (Ritalin) and dextroamphetamine (Dexedrine) to amelio-
substantial short-term improvement for disruptive and external- rate the disorder's core symptoms of inattention, impulsivity,
izing disorders, and I review highlights of these treatment mo- and overactivity as well as to effect improvements in the impor-
dalities later in this article. tant domains of peer relations and aggressive behavior (see re-
Whereas the most commonly used intervention strategy in views in Barkley, 1990; Hinshaw, 1991).3 Yet, as noted previ-
clinic settings for children with disruptive disorders is probably ously, whereas the stimulants yield impressive gains in aca-
individual play therapy, such dynamic approaches to psycho- demic productivity and accuracy, their effects on standardized
therapy are not effective in ameliorating the core symptoms of achievement test scores are far weaker, and there is still consid-
these conditions (Kazdin, 1987). The wide range of behavioral erable debate as to their clinical impact on the learning of new
approaches, on the other hand, has a solid empirical base for material (Swanson, Cantwell, Lerner, McBurnett, & Hanna,
this population. A rough subdivision of behavioral interven- 1991). Overall, perhaps the most salient issues with regard to
tions includes (a) direct contingency management, in which pharmacologic intervention are that, despite the clear medica-
systematic reward and response cost procedures are imple- tion-related improvements that can be expected for 70% to 80%
mented, usually in classroom settings; (b) clinical behavior ther- of ADHD children with stimulant intervention, stimulants
apy, in which the therapist teaches behavioral programing pro- alone typically do not bring all social and academic behavior
cedures to key adults in the environment (i.e., parents and into the normal range, and the long-range efficacy of pharmaco-
teachers); (c) cognitive-behavioral approaches, in which prob- logic treatment is not impressive (e.g., Pelham & Hinshaw, 1992;
lem solving and mediational strategies are taught directly to the Weiss & Hechtman, 1986). As a result, combinations of behav-
child with the goal of facilitating generalization and mainte- ioral and pharmacologic intervention are receiving attention as
nance; and (d) residential treatment options for conduct-disor- the intervention strategy of choice for ADHD (see Pelham &
dered adolescents (see McMahon & Wells, 1989, and Pelham & Murphy, 1986).
Hinshaw, 1992, for elaboration of these subcategories). In summary, as cogently argued by Kazdin (1987), investiga-
Whereas direct contingency management programs yield tors must begin to view externalizing behavior disorders as
powerful effects on disruptive behavior, they can be costly to chronic behavioral conditions, requiring long-term interven-
implement, and their benefits do not automatically generalize tion strategies across childhood and adolescence. In this re-
outside the demonstration classroom setting. It is noteworthy gard, externalizing behavior is similar to the persistent prob-
that the use of prudent negative consequences (e.g., response lem of severe academic underachievement, which resists signifi-
cost and time-out) is a critical adjunct to reward systems for cant amelioration with treatments that are acute in nature. It
externalizing children, particularly those with ADHD (Bark-
ley, 1990; Pfiffner & O'Leary, in press). Clinical behavior ther-
apy approaches, which are far more commonly used, can also 3
The actions of stimulants are not specific to ADHD. In fact, nor-
produce significant effects, particularly when systematic par- mal children often show a beneficial response, and recent investiga-
ent training is applied with defiant and aggressive youngsters tions with adolescents who have primary diagnoses of conduct dis-
(see the review in McMahon & Wells, 1989). Encouragingly, order reveal gains with stimulant treatment (e.g., R. T. Brown, Jaffe,
parent-based approaches with these populations have begun to Silverstein, & Magee, 1991).
898 STEPHEN P. HINSHAW

should come as no surprise, given this discussion, that I subse- not performed, an experimental single-case intervention study
quently recommend the use of long-term intervention pro- by Ayllon, Layman, and Kandel (1975) is noteworthy for its
grams for the comorbid group of children with externalizing considerable influence on conceptualizations of how to inter-
behavior disorders and underachievement. vene with the academic and behavioral problems of externaliz-
ing youngsters. For the 3 hyperactive subjects, all of whom at-
tended learning disabilities classes, use of the stimulant Ritalin
Intervention for Comorbid \bungsters: was contrasted with the behavioral intervention of rewarding
Empirical Investigations correct performance on academic assignments in a multiple-
Overview baseline design; outcomes included both an observational com-
posite of hyperactive and disruptive behavior in class and per-
Although some treatment studies for learning disabled chil- centage of correct responses on the worksheets. Whereas Rita-
dren include behavioral adjustment as an outcome, and al- lin led to improvement only in terms of classroom behavior, the
though some interventions for externalizing youngsters incor- behavioral intervention both increased the accurate perfor-
porate dependent variables of school performance, most re- mance of academic tasks in math and reading and reduced the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ports contain outcome assessments that are focused rather problematic behavior. Closer examination of the academic
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

narrowly, indexing either achievement or externalizing behav- tasks, however, reveals that the worksheets comprised relatively
ior, but not both. In addition, there are surprisingly few con- simple problems; it seems more accurate to contend that the
trolled intervention studies that explicitly address children behavioral intervention enhanced task completion rather than
with comorbid underachievement and disruptive behavior dis- learning or achievement per se. Furthermore, there was no indi-
orders. One factor contributing to the lack of such investiga- cation that Ritalin dosages were carefully evaluated. Indeed, no
tions may be the difficulty of finding externalizing subjects child was receiving more than 0.17 mg/kg, a dose level that is
with severe discrepancy-based learning disabilities (see Abikoff considered quite low. Nonetheless, the investigation was influ-
et al, 1988). Regardless of the precise reasons, there is an in- ential in promoting the treatment approach of reducing prob-
creasing appeal for intervention researchers to broaden both lem behavior in the classroom indirectly, by way of reinforce-
treatment approaches and outcome evaluations for externaliz- ment of the incompatible behavior of academic performance.
ing or underachieving children into comorbid problem areas An extremely heuristic study by Coie and Krehbiel (1984)
(McMahon & Wells, 1989). In reviewing the few intervention- involved 40 fourth graders who were comorbidly underachiev-
related investigations of comorbid externalizing-underachiev- ing (indexed by group achievement test scores in reading or
ing children, I do not consider a larger number of multimodal- math that were below the 36th percentile of national norms)
ity treatment studies of ADHD children in which academic and socially rejected (assessed by sociometric nominations
measures were obtained but in which formal assessment of co- from peers and teacher nominations of disruption and social
morbid learning disabilities was not performed (e.g., Pelham et difficulties). Although peer rejection does not, in itself, signify
al., 1988; Satterfield, Cantwell, & Satterfield, 1979; see review an externalizing behavior disorder, it is well known that low
in Gadow, 1985). social status with peers significantly predicts a host of negative
outcomes in later life (Parker & Asher, 1987) and covaries posi-
tively with disruptive and particularly aggressive behavior (Er-
Pertinent Investigations
hardt & Hinshaw, 1992). These comorbid youngsters were as-
Conrad, Dworkin, Shai, and Tobiessen (1971) performed a signed randomly to academic skills training, social skills train-
study of 68 low-SES children diagnosed as hyperkinetic (on the ing, combined academic and social skills training, or
basis of a teacher rating scale) and learning disabled (on the no-treatment control conditions, with an explicit goal of ascer-
basis of poor performance on at least one of three perceptual- taining whether intervention with one problem area would lead
motor tests). They crossed the experimental treatment vari- to change in the reciprocal domain. The intervention programs
ables of stimulant medication (dextroamphetamine) and twice- spanned 6 months, with academic tutoring performed twice
weekly prescriptive tutoring (focusing on such process variables per week and social skills training once per week. The tutoring
as fine and gross motor skills, sequencing abilities, and visual focused on direct enhancement of reading and math skills; the
and auditory memory) in this 20-week investigation; outcome social skills curriculum was based on validated procedures for
measures included IQ and achievement tests, behavior ratings promoting prosocial functioning.
from parents and teachers, and performance on individual as- Both immediately after the intervention programs and at 1-
sessments of perceptual-motor skills. Although the statistical year follow-up, children receiving the academic tutoring made
analyses that were performed preclude clear interpretation of significant improvements not only in academic achievement
the pattern of findings, treatment effects were relatively few, but also in observed classroom behavior and in peer sociome-
and none were found for measures of academic achievement. tric status. In contrast, the social skills intervention (which was
Those significant effects that did occur for behavior ratings and admittedly less intensive than the tutoring in terms of contact
for perceptual-motor skills tended to favor medication rather time with adults) produced only transitory change in some so-
than tutoring. On the basis of current knowledge, it can be cial preference measures and no reciprocal effects on academic
argued that the decision to focus the tutoring on underlying skills. Few interactions between the two treatment types were
process variables rather than on academic attainment per se found. Although it could be argued strongly that the most ap-
contributed strongly to the weak pattern of effects. propriate social skills intervention for socially rejected or exter-
Although formal assessment of underachieving status was nalizing children would focus directly on reducing aggressive
SPECIAL SECTION: UNDERACHIEVEMENT, ATTENTION, AND AGGRESSION 899

and antisocial behavior rather than on promoting social ap- ADHD and learning disabilities may be refractory to such ben-
proach skills, these results echo the findings of Ayllon et al. efit.
(1975). That is, direct amelioration of academic skills has recip-
rocal effects on social competence, perhaps mediated through Conclusions
such factors as enhancement of on-task behavior, positive
teacher response, and increased self-esteem. In addition to pro- The divergent subject selection criteria, intervention proce-
viding clinical guidelines for treatment, well-designed inter- dures, and outcome measures used in the previously described
vention programs such as this are potentially valuable theoreti- investigations render premature sweeping generalizations
cally through their specification of patterns of reciprocal rela- about intervention for children with comorbid underachieve-
tions across behavioral and achievement-related domains. ment and externalizing behavior problems (see also Gadow,
The effects of three dosages of methylphenidate, paired with 1985). One point to be made, however, is that promoting scho-
direct tutoring in reading, were assessed on behavior, cognitive lastic success through direct instruction in and reinforcement
performance, and academic achievement in carefully diag- of academic work may yield some benefits in behavioral and
nosed children with comorbid ADDH and reading disabilities peer domains as well (Ayllon et al., 1975; Coie & Krehbiel, 1984;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Kupietz, Winsberg, Richardson, Maitinsky, & Mendell, 1988; see also Gittelman & Feingold, 1983). Furthermore, even when
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Richardson, Kupietz, Winsberg, Maitinsky, & Mendell, 1988). pharmacologic intervention (particularly stimulant medica-
In a between-subjects design, children in these companion re- tion) has a positive influence on achievement-related measures,
ports were assigned randomly to double-blind administration the benefits usually occur in the presence of intervention that
of placebo or Ritalin (0.3 mg/kg, 0.5 mg/kg, or 0.7 mg/kg) for a emphasizes direct teaching of the academic material (e.g., Ku-
6-month period; each child received weekly reading tutoring pietz et al., 1988; Richardson et al., 1988; but see Abikoff et al.,
(by means of a thorough program aimed at sight-word vocabu- 1988). Overall, reducing problem behavior is not a sufficient
lary, phonics, and comprehension skills) and daily instruction intervention for youngsters with overlapping achievement and
in the same skills from parents. In combination with the tutor- behavior problems; the promotion of academic success is criti-
ing, stimulant medication (i.e., Ritalin; particularly the highest cal for these children.
dosage level of 0.7 mg/kg) led to improvements in adult-rated I should make explicit that instruction in academic skills will
behavior and on some cognitive processing measures; in addi- not, in all likelihood, succeed for youngsters with comorbid
tion, the medication enhanced word recognition skills during underachievement and externalizing behavior unless motiva-
postassessments. Methylphenidate's effects on the latter skills tion is enhanced through the use of incentives such as rein-
were correlated with its amelioration of hyperactive, inatten- forcers and response cost procedures. That is, tutoring or in-
tive, and aggressive behaviors, suggesting that pharmacologic struction in phonics, sight-word vocabulary, or reading com-
improvement of externalizing behaviors may mediate achieve- prehension approaches will probably be quite frustrating for
ment-related gains in children with comorbid hyperactivity and children with histories of both academic failure and inattentive
learning disabilities. Thus, although the study design (in which and disruptive behaviors; extrinsic motivational procedures are
all children received tutoring) precluded ascertainment of truly typically necessary, particularly at the outset. In short, children
interactive effects of educational therapy with medication, im- with comorbid achievement and behavior problems require in-
portant change in behavior, cognition, and achievement may tervention that combines the best of behavioral programing
well occur when effective tutoring strategies are combined with and educational instruction.
stimulant medication in comorbid youngsters.4 Next, because the overlap with underachievement in child-
Abikoff et al. (1988) attempted to ascertain the efficacy of a hood most often occurs with the externalizing behavior pattern
16-week, academically oriented, cognitive-behavioral training of ADHD, as opposed to aggression per se, the appropriateness
program for boys with comorbid ADDH and academic defi- of stimulant treatment is an extremely salient issue. As detailed
ciencies (defined as reading or math scores significantly below previously, stimulant medications can clearly enhance attention
expected performance). In the three-group design, in which all and impulse control and many aspects of academic perfor-
subjects received moderate to high dosages of stimulant medica- mance in comorbid youngsters (e.g., Abikoff et al., 1988; Ku-
tion, the boys were assigned randomly to academic cognitive pietz et al., 1988; Richardson et al., 1988). These effects stand in
training, remedial reading and math tutoring, or no-interven- contrast to the limited efficacy of stimulants for children with
tion conditions; the first two conditions included direct in- severe learning problems that are unaccompanied by behavior
struction in academic skills. Although all three groups yielded problems (Gittelman et al., 1983). As just noted, however, the
a number of significant within-subjects improvements in aca- pairing of medication with direct instruction in academic skills
demic measures, there were virtually no between-subjects dif- (accompanied by behavioral procedures to enhance motivation)
ferences across approximately 45 outcome measures at postin-
tervention or 6-month follow-up assessments. These results in- 4
dicate that stimulant treatment alone, when administered over For many of the outcome measures that yielded significant effects
sufficient time periods, can facilitate gains in reading and math of medication, the gains were salient during the first 3 months of the
investigation, but some decrements were noted in the last 3-month
achievement but that remedial tutoring and cognitive training period, suggesting that long-term stimulant treatment is associated
fail to increment such gains, at least when stimulant dosages are with the development of tolerance effects in children. This provocative
high. Thus, despite some earlier evidence for the facilitation of finding may constitute another potential reason for the failure of
academic performance with cognitive-behavioral procedures chronic stimulant treatment to lead to long-term improvements in hy-
(e.g., Cameron & Robinson, 1980), children with comorbid peractive or in hyperactive-learning disabled youngsters.
900 STEPHEN P. HINSHAW

is usually necessary to effect improved performance. Overall, both for (Lyon, 1985) and against (Gittelman & Feingold, 1983)
although supporting evidence is not consistent (e.g., Abikoffet this practice. Regardless of one's stand on this issue, it is clear
al., 1988), children with comorbid underachievement and at- that assessment of such factors as the child's motivation, the
tention deficits will most often require stimulant medication in family's structure and attitudes, and the classroom climate (e.g.,
addition to exemplary educational and behavioral programing. the teacher's readiness to incorporate special materials or ap-
\bungsters with the combination of underachievement and proaches) will be crucial to the success of any intervention pro-
purely oppositional/aggressive behavior have been investigated gram for an underachieving child. In short, the brevity of any
far less frequently. diagnostic label, coupled with the rather focused nature of the
Third, I must reiterate that truly long-term benefits of any inclusionary criteria themselves, mandate thorough evaluation
intervention program have rarely been demonstrated for un- of a wealth of additional factors if optimal intervention is to
derachievement or for externalizing behavior, and much less for ensue.
their combination. Despite the substantial gains produced by A related issue concerns subcategorization of each domain.
phonics-based tutoring for reading disabled children (Gittel- A key goal of such subtyping would be to clarify Treatment X
man & Feingold, 1983) and despite the clear improvements that Subtype interactions, with the hope of specifying more accu-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

can accrue to family-based behavioral programs with external- rately the interventions to be directed toward homogeneous
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

izing youngsters (see McMahon & Wells, 1989)with gains clusters of academic deficit areas or problem behaviors. Al-
from each type of intervention persisting months or even years though discussion of the methodologic and statistical difficul-
after treatmentthe stark fact is that severe underachievement ties that arise from subtyping efforts is well beyond the scope of
and severe levels of ADHD and aggressive behavior are quite this article, I should mention that Subtype X Treatment re-
persistent, and long-range treatment-related benefits are the search models require substantial sample sizes if there is to be
exception rather than the rule. Particularly for the group of any power to detect pertinent interactions; and with more than
youngsters with comorbid underachievement and externaliz- two subtypes or treatment conditions, intervention research de-
ing behavior problems, whose problems are quite likely to be- signs quickly become unwieldy, particularly in the context of
come more rather than less severe over time, the field must the investigation of youngsters who are comorbidly externaliz-
realize the need to extend treatment regimens over lengthy time ing and underachieving. At present, the field is perched on the
periods rather than to expect short-term interventions to pro- horns of a dilemma: Should the top priority be placed on find-
mote lasting change (e.g., Kazdin, 1987). A related realization is ing any effective treatment strategy (see Gittelman & Feingold,
that such intervention programs will, of necessity, be costly to 1983)? Or will the only real success emerge from matching in-
implement. Yet, given the severe risk for delinquency posed by terventions to particular processing deficits (Lyon, 1985)? Reso-
the combination of learning and neuropsychological deficits lution of this issue is far from certain, particularly given the
and externalizing behavior patterns early in life (see Moffitt, nonspecificity of many extant treatment strategies (e.g., stimu-
1990), the costs to individuals, families, and society will un- lant medication and behavior management).
doubtedly be greater without such long-term interventions. As noted in an earlier section, comorbidity between the do-
mains of interest often begins quite early in development, prior
to the start of formal schooling. A clear implication is that
Critical Issues
optimal treatment efforts must begin during such key develop-
A question of clinical and theoretical importance is whether mental periods, before difficulties become entrenched and be-
the present diagnostic criteria for underachievement or for fore other associated problems emerge. In speculating as to
disruptive behavior disorders are sufficient to specify interven- what components early intervention programs for children with
tion strategies. Although there is some apparent evidence for early signs of comorbid underachievement and externalizing
the affirmative (e.g., a diagnosis of ADHD strongly invites con- behavior might entail, I emphasize the caveat that because of
sideration of a stimulant trial, and specific reading disabilities the infancy of research in this area, these ideas are not empiri-
mandate academic remediation that is directed specifically to- cally validated.
ward reading skills and not toward underlying perceptual-mo- For example, how should early intervention for underachieve-
tor processes), the picture becomes less clear on further consid- ment be conceptualized, if, as is the case in preschool years,
eration. First, if ADHD overlaps with significant internalizing formal academic work has not yet begun? In this regard, those
features or with disordered thinking, stimulant medications types of deficits in the preschool years that have been found to
may exacerbate such symptoms rather than improve behavioral predict subsequent underachievement (e.g., mild language de-
regulation (see the discussion in Barkley, 1990). Thus, it is cru- lays, poor visual-motor integration, and subaverage global intel-
cial to record the comorbidity of ADHD with other behavioral ligence) would seem likely targets; yet these are difficult to treat
and emotional disorders in treatment planning. Second, as directly. Nonetheless, language stimulation programs for pre-
noted earlier, the externalizing behavior disorders are often school-aged children with expressive language delays, which are
embedded in networks of discordant family interaction, psy- predictive of both subsequent learning disabilities and behavior
chosocial deprivation, and peer rejection (particularly when se- problems (Beitchman et al., 1992; Hinshaw, 1992) could well be
vere aggression is present); these systemic factors need to be indicated. As kindergarten age approaches, such language stim-
addressed in any comprehensive treatment plan. Third, I have ulation intervention could profitably be combined with inten-
noted the ongoing debate regarding the need for better-speci- sive tutoring of other readiness skills, such as letter recognition
fied interventions to extend evaluation of underachievement and letter-sound correspondence. To the extent that the under-
into neuropsychological domains, with clearly stated opinions lying linguistic skill deficits are predictive of the emerging co-
SPECIAL SECTION: UNDERACHIEVEMENT, ATTENTION, AND AGGRESSION 901

morbidity of learning problems and behavior problems, such 1992), extremely few empirical treatment investigations have
treatment could be truly preventive in nature. been undertaken with children who display this comorbid con-
Yet, because of the distinct possibility that partially indepen- figuration. Whereas some evidence exists that intervention for
dent causal factors operate to shape comorbidity (Hinshaw, academic problems has reciprocal effects on reducing problem
1992), it seems unlikely that intervention in any one modality behavior (Ayllon et al., 1975; Coie & Krehbiel, 1984), youngsters
could circumvent the development of conjoint externalizing be- in the investigations supporting this claim did not meet formal
havior and underachievement, even if delivered sufficiently criteria for the joint presence of disruptive behavior disorders
early. Active engagement of the child and family in home- and and learning difficulties, and the preponderance of evidence
school-based behavior management programs would also seem suggests strongly that multimodal intervention must occur to
a necessity for the young child with language and behavioral address both the learning problems and the behavioral prob-
problems, with the goals of (a) promoting better parent-child lems that comorbid children exhibit. Because the overlap be-
interactions, (b) reducing problem behavior, and (c) preventing tween domains often appears during the preschool years, I ad-
the ensuing cycles of low self-worth and peer relationship diffi- vocate the adoption of early intervention procedures for lan-
culties that often emanate from acting-out behavior. Indeed, guage and readiness skills and disruptive behavioral problems;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

clear behavioral and familial precursors of subsequent atten- yet, such preventive treatments must contend with such prob-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tion-disordered and aggressive patterns have been identified lems as the imperfect prediction of later comorbidity from early
for preschoolers (Campbell, 1990; Richman, Stevenson, & Gra- difficulties and a dearth of supportive empirical evidence for
ham, 1982), and successful behavioral parent education pro- their efficacy. An exciting aspect about the development of
grams have been documented for young externalizing children both preventive and tertiary interventions for comorbid under-
(Pisterman et al, 1989). In general, however, early intervention achievement and externalizing disorders is that well-designed
efforts lag considerably behind tertiary treatments, partly be- and well-executed treatment investigations are potentially ben-
cause salient developmental pathways have yet to be clearly eficial both clinically and theoretically. That is, not only can
elucidated. appropriate interventions provide an extremely important ben-
Finally, how should we best intervene for children in grade efit to children, families, and society by helping to prevent the
school with comorbid underachievement and externalizing be- bleak course faced by comorbid youngsters, but also, because
havior? Given the partial independence of the two domains, of the potential for demonstration of reciprocal benefits across
and given the limited evidence for spillover of intervention domains, they can potentially elucidate causal pathways link-
gains from one area to the other, the main recommendation ing behavior and achievement. The next decade should witness
would be to direct treatment toward each problem area: fo- the accumulation of considerably more supporting evidence
cused educational intervention on academic skills for the un- regarding both clinical efficacy and causal paths.
derachievement and combined pharmacological and psychoso-
cial treatment for the externalizing behavior patterns. Hence,
multimodal treatments should be considered a necessity. Re- References
garding underachievement, I have repeatedly stated that the Abikoff, H. (1991). Cognitive training in ADHD children: Less to it
preponderance of evidence favors direct enhancement of aca- than meets the eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 205-209.
demic skills rather than intervention with underlying pro- Abikoff, H., Ganeles, D., Reiter, G., Blum, C., Foley, C, & Klein, R. G.
cesses; and behavioral incentives will, in all likelihood, need to (1988). Cognitive training in academically deficient ADDH boys
be paired with the educational procedures. As for externalizing receiving stimulant medication. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
behavior, if ADHD appears to be present, any consideration of ogy, 76,411-432.
a stimulant medication trial requires systematic assessment of Abikoff, H., & Klein, R. G. (1992). Attention-deficit hyperactivity and
its efficacy (e.g., Fischer & Newby, 1991); and practitioners conduct disorder: Comorbidity and implications for treatment.
should remember that medication alone rarely provides clini- Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 881-892.
cally sufficient benefits (Pelham & Hinshaw, 1992). Further- American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
more, parents and teachers need to be engaged in behaviorally
Ayllon, T., Layman, D., & Kandel, H. J. (1975). A behavioral-educa-
based intervention programs for children with either attention tional alternative to drug control of hyperactive children. Journal of
deficits or aggressive behavior patterns, as solely child-directed Applied Behavior Analysis, 8,137-146.
intervention is rarely adequate for such youngsters. Although Balthazor, M. J., Wagner, R. K., & Pelham, W E. (1991). The specificity
there is some limited evidence that ameliorating academic per- of the effects of stimulant medication on classroom learning-related
formance will have reciprocal effects on behavioral domains measures of cognitive processing for attention deficit disorder chil-
(Ayllon et al., 1975; Coie & Krehbiel, 1984), such generalization dren. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 35-52.
has largely been confined to classroom behavior, and it would Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A hand-
be naive to expect that improved academic performance alone book for diagnosis and treatment. New \brk: Guilford Press.
can reduce severe acting-out behavior at home or in social envi- Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Edelbrock, C. S., & Smallish, L. (1990). The
ronments. Overall, the types of multimodality treatments nec- adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research
criteria: I. An 8-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of the Amer-
essary to intervene with comorbid youngsters need to be sys- ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 546-557.
tematic, extensive, and lengthy in duration. Beitchman, J. H., Ferguson, B., Schachter, D, Kroll, R., Lancee, B.,
In summary, despite the prognostic, theoretical, and clinical Mathews, R., Wild, J., Inglis, A., Brownlee, E., & Beebakhee, R.
importance of the linkage between externalizing behavior dis- (1992, February). A seven-year follow-up of speech/language im-
orders and severe academic underachievement (Hinshaw, paired and control children: Findings and implications. Poster pre-
902 STEPHEN P. HINSHAW

sented at the annual meeting of the Society for Research in Child ing disorders: II. Effects of methylphenidate in combination with
and Adolescent Psychopathology, Sarasota, FL. reading remediation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24.
Brown, A. L., & Campione, J. C. (1986). Psychological theory and the 193-212.
study of learning disabilities. American Psychologist, 41, 1059- Hetherington, E. M., & Martin, B. (1986). Family interaction patterns.
1068. In H. C. Quay & J. S. Werry (Eds.), Psychopathological disorders of
Brown, R. T., Jaffe, S. L., Silverstein, J., & Magee, H. (1991). Methyl- childhood (3rd ed., pp. 349-408). New York: Wiley.
phenidate and adolescents hospitalized with conduct disorder: Dose Hinshaw, S. P. (1987). On the distinction between attentional deficits/
effects on classroom behavior, academic performance, and impulsiv- hyperactivity and conduct problems/aggression in child psychopa-
ity. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 282-292. thology. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 443-463.
Cameron, M. I., & Robinson, V M. J. (1980). Effects of cognitive train- Hinshaw, S. P. (1991). Stimulant medication and the treatment of ag-
ing on academic and on-task behavior of hyperactive children. Jour- gression in children with attentional deficits. Journal of Clinical
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 405-419. Child Psychology, 20, 301-312.
Campbell, S. B. (1990). Behavior problems in preschool children: Clini- Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems and academic
cal and developmental issues. New \brk: Guilford Press. underachievement in childhood and adolescence: Causal relation-
Ceci, S. J. (Ed.). (1986). Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsycho- ships and underlying mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 127-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

logical aspects of learning disabilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 155.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Coie, J. D., & Krehbiel, G. (1984). Effects of academic tutoring on the Hinshaw, S. P., & Erhardt, D. (1991). Attention-deficit hyperactivity
social statusof low-achieving, socially rejected children. Child Devel- disorder. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Child and adolescent therapy: Cogni-
opment, 55,1465-1478. tive-behavioral procedures (pp. 98-128). New %rk: Guilford Press.
Conrad, W G., Dworkin, E. S., Shai, A., & Tobiessen, J. E. (1971). Horn, W E, O'Donnell, J. P., & Vitulano, L. A. (1983). Long-term fol-
Effects of amphetamine therapy and prescriptive tutoring on the low-up studies of learning-disabled persons. Journal of Learning Dis-
behavior and achievement of lower class hyperactive children. Jour- abilities, 16, 542-555.
nal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 509-517. Kavale, K., & Mattson, P. D. (1983). "One jumped off the balance
Deshler, D. D., Warner, M. M., Schumaker, J. B., & Alley, G. R. (1983). beam": Meta-analysis of perceptual-motor training. Journal of Learn-
Learning strategies intervention model: Key components and ing Disabilities, 16, 165-173.
current status. In J. D. McKinney & L. Feagans (Eds.), Current topics Kazdin, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior in children:
in learning disabilities (Vol. 1, pp. 245-283). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Current status and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 102,
Douglas, V I., Barr, R. G., Amin, K., O'Neill, M. E., & Britton, B. G. 187-203.
(1988). Dosage effects and individual responsivity to methylpheni- Kazdin, A. E., Bass, D, Siegel, T., & Thomas, C. (1989). Cognitive-be-
date in attention deficit disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and havioral therapy and relationship therapy in the treatment of chil-
Psychiatry, 29, 453-475. dren referred for antisocial behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clini-
Erhardt, D., & Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Initial sociometric impressions of cal Psychology, 57, 522-535.
hyperactive and comparison boys: Predictions from social behaviors Kupietz, S. S., Winsberg, B. G, Richardson, E., Maitinsky, S., & Men-
and nonsocial variables. Manuscript submitted for publication. dell, N. (1988). Effects of methylphenidate dosage in hyperactive
Fischer, M., & Newby, R. F. (1991). Assessment of stimulant response reading-disabled children: I. Behavior and cognitive performance
in ADHD children using a refined multimethod clinical protocol. effects. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 232-244. Psychiatry, 27, 70-77.
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lloyd, M. (1991). Confirmatory Lahey, B. B., Piacentini, J. C., McBurnett, K., Stone, P., Hartdagen, S.,
factor models of attention deficit and conduct disorder. Journal of & Hynd, G. (1988). Psychopathology in the parents of children with
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 257-274. conduct disorder and hyperactivity. Journal of the American Acad-
Forness, S. R., Youpa, D., Hanna, G., Cantwell, D. P., & Swanson, J. M. emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27,163-170.
(1992). Classroom instructional characteristics in attention deficit Licht, B. G., & Kistner, J. A. (1986). Motivational problems of learning-
hyperactivity disorder: Comparison of pure and mixed subgroups. disabled children: Individual differences and their implications for
Behavioral Disorders, 17, 115-125. treatment. In J. K. Torgeson & B. Y. L. Wong (Eds.), Psychological
Frick, P., Kamphaus, R. W, Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Christ, M. G, and educational perspectives on learning disabilities (pp. 329-365).
Hart, E., & Tannenbaum, L. E. (1991). Academic underachievement San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
and the disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Consulting and Loeber, R. (1990). Development and risk factors of juvenile antisocial
Clinical Psychology, 59, 289-294. behavior and delinquency. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 1-41.
Fuerst, D. R., Fisk, J. L., & Rourke, B. P. (1989). Psychosocial function- Loeber, R., Lahey, B. B., & Thomas, C. (1991). Diagnostic conundrum
ing of learning-disabled children: Replicability of statistically de- of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. Journal of
rived subtypes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, Abnormal Psychology, 100, 379-390.
275-280. Lovett, M., Ransby, M. J., Hardwick, N., Johns, M. S., & Donaldson,
Gadow, K. D. (1985). Relative efficacy of pharmacological, behavioral, S. A. (1989). Can dyslexia be treated? Treatment-specific and gener-
and combination treatments for enhancing academic performance. alized treatment effects in dyslexic children's response to medica-
Clinical Psychology Review, 5, 513-533. tion. Brain and Language, 37, 90-121.
Gittelman, R., Abikoff, H., Pollack, E., Klein, D. E, Katz, S., & Mattes, Lyon, G. R. (1985). Educational validation studies of learning disabil-
J. (1980). A controlled trial of behavior modification and methyl- ity subtypes. In B. Rourke (Ed.), Neuropsychology of learning disabili-
phenidate in hyperactive children. In C. K. Whalen & B. Henker ties: Essentials of subtype analysis (pp. 228-253). New "Vbrk: Guil-
(Eds.), Hyperactive children: The social ecology of identification and ford Press.
treatment (pp. 221-243). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Maughan, B., Gray, G, & Rutter, M. (1985). Reading retardation and
Gittelman, R., & Feingold, I. (1983). Children with reading disorders: antisocial behavior: A follow-up into employment. Journal of Child
I. Effects of reading instruction. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 741-758.
Psychiatry, 24,167-191. McGee, R., & Share, D. L. (1988). Attention deficit disorder hyperactiv-
Gittelman, R., Klein, D. E, & Feingold, I. (1983). Children with read- ity and academic failure: Which comes first and which should be
SPECIAL SECTION: UNDERACHIEVEMENT, ATTENTION, AND AGGRESSION 903

treated? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent reading-disabled children: II. Reading achievement. Journal of the
Psychiatry, 27, 318-325. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 78-87.
McGee, R., Williams, S., Share, D. L., Anderson, J, & Silva, P. A. Richman, N., Stevenson, J, & Graham, P. (1982). Preschool to school: A
(1986). The relationship between specific reading retardation, gen- behavioural study. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
eral reading backwardness, and behavioural problems in a large sam- Robins, L. N. (1991). Conduct disorders. Journal of Child Psychology
ple of Dunedin boys: A longitudinal study from five to eleven years. and Psychiatry, 32,193-212.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 597-610. Rourke, B. (Ed.). (1985). Neuropsychology of learning disabilities: Es-
McGee, R., Williams, S., & Silva, P. A. (1985). Factor structure and sentials of subtype analysis. New York: Guilford Press.
correlates of ratings of inattention, hyperactivity, and antisocial be- Rutter, M., & Yule, W (1975). The concept of specific reading retarda-
havior in a large sample of 9-year-old children from the general tion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16,181-197.
population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 480- Rutter, M., Yule, B., Quinton, D., Rowlands, Q, Yule, W, & Berger, M.
490. (1974). Attainment and adjustment in two geographical areas: III.
McKinney, J. D. (1989). Longitudinal research on the behavioral char- Some factors accounting for areas differences. British Journal of Psy-
acteristics of children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning chiatry, 125, 520-533.
Disabilities, 22,141-150. Satterfield, J. H., Cantwell, D. P., & Satterfield, B. T. (1979). Multimo-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

McMahon, R. J., & Wells, K. C. (1989). Conduct disorders. In E. J. dality treatment: A one-year follow-up of 84 boys. Archives of Gen-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Treatment of childhood disorders (pp. eral Psychiatry, 36, 965-974.
73-132). New York: Guilford Press. Schachar, R., & Wachsmuth, R. (1990). Hyperactivity and parental
Milich, R., & Landau, S. (1989). The role of social status variables in psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31,
differentiating subgroups of hyperactive children. In L. M. Bloom- 381-392.
ingdale & J. M. Swanson (Eds.), Attention deficit disorder (Vol. 4, pp. Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Escobar, M. D.
1-16). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls: Results of
Moffitt, T. E. (1990). Juvenile delinquency and attention deficit dis- the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. Journal of the American Medi-
order: Boys' developmental trajectories from age 3 to age 15. Child cal Association, 264, 998-1002.
Development, 61, 893-910. Snowling, M. J. (1991). Developmental reading disorders. Journal of
Parker, J. G, & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 49-77.
adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psychological Bulle- Spreen, O. (1988). Prognosis of learning disability. Journal of Consult-
tin, 102, 357-389. ing and Clinical Psychology, 56, 836-842.
Pelham, W E. (1986). The effects of psychostimulant drugs on learning Swanson, J. M., Cantwell, D., Lerner, M., McBurnett, K.., & Hanna, G.
and academic achievement in children with attention-deficit dis- (1991). Effects of stimulant medication on learning in children with
orders and learning disabilities. In J. K. Torgeson & B. Y. L. Wong ADHD. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 219-230.
(Eds), Psychological and educational perspectives on learning disabili- Szatmari, P., Offord, D. R., & Boyle, M. H. (1989a). Correlates, asso-
ties (pp. 259-296). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. ciated impairments and patterns of service utilization of children
Pelham, W E., & Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Behavioral intervention for with attention deficit disorder: Findings from the Ontario Child
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. In S. M. Turner, K. S. Cal- Health Study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 205-
houn, & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of clinical behavior therapy 217.
(2nd ed., pp. 259-283). New York: Wiley. Szatmari, P., Offord, D. R., & Boyle, M. H. (1989b). Ontario Child
Pelham, W E., & Murphy, H. A. (1986). Attention deficit and conduct Health Study: Prevalence of attention deficit disorder with hyperac-
disorders. In M. Hersen(Ed.), Pharmacological and behavioral treat- tivity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 219-230.
ments: An integrative approach (pp. 108-148). New \brk: Wiley. Taylor, H. G. (1988). Learning disabilities. In E. J. Mash & L. G. Terdal
Pelham, W E., Schnedler, R. W, Bender, M. E., Nilsson, R. W, Miller, (Eds.), Behavioral assessment of childhood disorders (2nd ed., pp.
J., Budrow, M. S., Ronnei, M., Paluchowski, C, & Marks, D. A. 402-450). New York: Guilford Press.
(1988). The combination of behavior therapy and methylphenidate Taylor, H. G. (1989). Learning disabilities. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Bark-
in the treatment of attention deficit disorders: A therapy outcome ley (Eds.), Treatment of childhood disorders (pp. 347-380). New York:
study. In L. M. Bloomingdale (Ed.), Attention deficit disorder (Vol. 3, Guilford Press.
pp. 29-48). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. Torgeson, J. K. (1989). Why IQ is relevant to the definition of learning
Pfiffner, L. J., & O'Leary, S. G. (in press). Behavioral interventions in disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 484-486.
classrooms. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity dis- Weiss, G., & Hechtman, L. T. (1986). Hyperactive children grown up:
order: A handbook. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Empirical findings and theoretical considerations. New York: Guil-
Pisterman, S., McGrath, P., Firestone, P., Goodman, J. T, Webster, I, & ford Press.
Mallory, R. (1989). Outcome of parent-mediated treatment of pre- Yule, W, & Rutter, M. (1985). Reading and other learning difficulties.
schoolers with attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. Journal In M. Rutter & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry:
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 628-635. Modem approaches (pp. 444-464). Oxford, England: Blackwell Sci-
Puig-Antich, J. (1982). Major depression and conduct disorder in pre- entific.
puberty. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 21,
118-128. Received December 2,1991
Richardson, E., Kupietz, S. S., Winsberg, B. G, Maitinsky, S., & Men- Revision received February 17,1992
dell, N. (1988). Effects of methylphenidate dosage in hyperactive Accepted June 23,1992

You might also like