You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, June 2006; 31(2): 7786

Adjustment, sibling problems and coping strategies of brothers and


sisters of children with autistic spectrum disorder

PENELOPE ROSS & MONICA CUSKELLY

School of Education, University of Queensland, Australia

Abstract
Background Siblings of children with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) express more problem behaviours and experience
more difficulties in their relationships than do children in families where all children are developing typically. We know little
about what contributes to these difficulties.
Method Mothers of a child with ASD completed the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991) with respect to a non-
disabled sibling. Siblings responded to a questionnaire tapping their knowledge about their brother or sisters disorder. They
reported on problems they had experienced with their brother or sister with ASD and on the coping strategies they had used
in response to these events. Problems were classified into 1 of 5 problem types.
Results Aggressive behaviour was the most commonly reported interaction problem and anger was the usual response.
Siblings did not generally choose blaming (either self or other) as a coping strategy when facing difficulties with their brother
or sister with ASD. Neither coping strategies nor knowledge of ASD were associated with adjustment. Forty percent of non-
disabled siblings had scores on the Child Behavior Checklist that placed them in the borderline or clinical range.
Conclusions The current study indicated that siblings of children with ASD are at increased risk of developing internalising
behaviour problems. The contributing factors to this outcome are unknown at this point. It is important for research to
focus on dynamic variables in the search for these contributors, as they are open to change.

Keywords: Sibling, autism, autistic spectrum disorder, Asperger syndrome, coping, adjustment, relationship

Introduction neglected area of research. Cuskelly (1999) argued


Interactions with ones siblings provide opportu- that research focusing on dynamic aspects of the
nities for experiencing and expressing many emo- sibling relationship is more likely to contribute to
tions, as well as for the practice, and perhaps ameliorating the impact of a child with a disability on
mastery, of skills such as self-control, sharing, their sibling than research into status variables such
listening, conflict resolution, and fair play (Gibbs, as age and birth-order. Her reasoning was that
1993). Sibling relationships extend into adulthood dynamic variables are open to change and therefore
but their character is generally established in the provide the foci for interventions.
early years. It is likely that the quality of the The study reported here investigated the impact of
relationship contributes to the overall psychological two dynamic variables on the adjustment of siblings
adjustment of children. Given the importance of of children with ASD, namely coping skills and
experiences within the sibling subsystem, it is knowledge of ASD. In addition, it examined the
essential that we increase our understanding of the types of difficulties reported by brothers and sisters
impact on children of possible disturbances within of children with ASD. The term ASD is used to
this subsystem. include children with a range of social and commu-
While it has been established that the siblings of nication problems. Often, a more specific diagnosis
children with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) of autism or Asperger syndrome is made for children
experience more difficulties in their relationships on this spectrum. The present study included
than do children in families where all children are children with either of these diagnoses.
developing typically (Bagenholm & Gillberg, 1991), Research investigating the psychological adjust-
investigation of the day-to-day difficulties experi- ment of siblings of children with ASD (i.e., autism,
enced by siblings of children with ASD and of how Asperger syndrome, or simply ASD) has yielded
they cope with these stressors has been a relatively inconsistent results, with some researchers reporting

Correspondence: Dr Monica Cuskelly, School of Education, The University of Queensland, QLD 4072, Australia. E-mail: m.cuskelly@uq.edu.au
ISSN 1366-8250 print/ISSN 1469-9532 online # 2006 Australasian Society for the Study of Intellectual Disability Inc.
DOI: 10.1080/13668250600710864
78 P. Ross & M. Cuskelly

deleterious outcomes (e.g., Fisman et al., 1996; siblings of children with a disability found to be the
Gold, 1993; Hastings, 2003; Smalley, McCracken, most poorly adjusted in the sample. Alternatively,
& Tanguay, 1995; Verte, Roeyers, & Buysse, 2003) self-directed cognitions (e.g., calm down) were
while others have found no effects (e.g., see negatively associated with depression and anxiety
Kaminsky & Dewey, 2002; Mates, 1990). Based levels.
on analyses of effect sizes from three studies that Multidiagnostic groups may not be very informa-
reported poor outcomes for siblings (i.e., Fisman, tive about the experience of siblings of children with
Wolf, Ellison, & Freeman, 2000; Gold, 1993; a specific disorder such as ASD (Cuskelly, 1999).
Rodrigue, Geffken, & Morgan, 1993), Yirimiya, The only study to date that has looked specifically at
Shaked and Erel (2001) concluded that the overall sibling coping with respect to difficulties with their
effect of having a sibling with autism on childrens brother or sister with ASD was carried out by
level of behavioural and emotional problems was of Roeyers and Mycke (1995). This study was a partial
low magnitude and non-significant. Furthermore, duplication of Gamble and McHales (1989) study,
Kaminsky and Dewey (2002) reported no significant but compared siblings of children with autism,
differences in levels of internalising and external- intellectual disability, and typical development.
ising behaviour problems between siblings of child- Stressor events and coping strategies were measured
ren with autism, Down syndrome, or typical using the same procedure as Gamble and McHale.
development. Overall, the three groups of siblings Consistent with Gamble and McHales report,
were well adjusted, with scores within the normal Roeyers and Mycke found that siblings of children
range. with autism more frequently used other-directed
While a wealth of literature has been devoted to cognitions to cope with stressful sibling incidents.
family coping and adaptation to a child with a Another dynamic factor that may be important in
disability (e.g., Donovan, 1988; McCubbin & determining the impact on children of living with a
Patterson, 1983; Wikler, 1986), this conceptual
brother or sister with ASD is their knowledge and
approach has only rarely been extended to explore
understanding of ASD. Howlin (1988) noted that
the ways children adjust to the difficult aspects of
the extent and openness of parental communication
having a sibling with a disability. As Gamble and her
about ASD appeared to be a major factor in sibling
colleagues have suggested, understanding the coping
adjustment, and hypothesised that sibling under-
processes used by children who have a sibling with a
standing of ASD was the critical mechanism.
disability may help to explain the observed variability
However, this conclusion reflected clinical observa-
in siblings psychological adjustment (Gamble &
tion rather than an empirical finding. Lobato and
McHale, 1989; Gamble & Woulbroun, 1993).
Kao (2002) reported an intervention study that
Gamble and McHale (1989) investigated psycho-
found increased knowledge and improved adjust-
logical wellbeing in siblings of children with a variety
of disabilities in comparison to siblings of typically ment in siblings of children with a disability post
developing children. They examined stressful events, intervention. Although these results are promising,
cognitive appraisals of those events, and the coping no comparison group was included and associations
strategies used by the children. Children were asked between knowledge and adjustment were not
to rate the frequency with which they experienced reported.
seven stressor events with respect to their sibling, As mentioned above, Gamble and McHale (1989)
and to appraise each event by reporting the level of found that other-directed cognitions were the
affect experienced in response to that event. Coping least effective coping strategy for dealing with
strategies were identified by asking the children to stressors originating from the sibling relationship.
nominate the frequency with which they used 16 Use of this coping strategy by brothers and sisters of
different coping strategies when they became angry children with a disability may be more frequent in
with their sibling. These strategies were collapsed children who have a poor understanding of their
into four general categories: environment-directed siblings condition. Only one study investigating the
cognition, self-directed cognition, environment- relationship between siblings knowledge of ASD
directed behaviour, and self-directed behaviour. and the quality of the sibling relationship was
Gamble and McHale identified a trend for siblings identified. Roeyers and Mycke (1995) found that
of children with a disability (and girls in general) to siblings with greater knowledge of ASD had a more
report using other-directed cognitions (e.g., positive relationship with their sibling. To date no
blaming another person) more frequently to cope research has investigated the relationship between
with stressful sibling situations. This form of coping siblings knowledge about ASD and their adjust-
was subsequently linked to poorer adjustment, with ment.
Adjustment, sibling problems and coping 79

Three research questions guided this study: (1) problems such as depression and anxiety), and
What stressors are commonly experienced by the Externalizing (reflecting outwardly directed
siblings of children with ASD? (2) What coping problem behaviours such as aggression). Raw
strategies do children use to deal with these scores are converted to T scores. While the
situations? (3) What associations exist between instrument is not intended to be a stand-alone
childrens adjustment and the coping strategies they diagnostic tool, it can contribute to diagnostic
use and their knowledge about their siblings decisions (Achenbach). Children who have a T
disorder? score of 64 or above on these scales are considered
to be in the clinical range, while those with a T score
of between 60 and 63 are considered to be in the
Method borderline range. Further assessment is warranted
when T scores above 60 are obtained. Australian
Participants
data have shown the structure of the CBCL to be
Participants were 25 typically developing children appropriate for Australian children (Heubeck,
and adolescents who had a sibling with ASD, and 2000), with some studies showing a higher level of
their mothers. All children were from two-parent problem behaviour in Australian children than
families. The children who participated in the study American children (Hensley, 1988; Sawyer et al.,
are referred to as participating siblings, and the 2001), and others showing no difference (Bond,
children with ASD as reference siblings. The Nolan, Adler, & Robertson, 1994).
average age of participating siblings was 10.64 years
(SD52.33), with a range from 815 years. Nineteen Knowledge of Autism/Asperger Syndrome (KAAS). Two
of the participating siblings were male. Fourteen of questionnaires of 21 items were constructed by the
the children were younger than their reference authors of this study to assess childrens knowledge
sibling. The average age of the reference siblings of their siblings disorder (i.e., autism or Asperger
was 11.16 years (SD52.79), with a range from 616 syndrome). Two questionnaires were considered
years. Nineteen were diagnosed with Asperger necessary because in Australia, autism and Asperger
syndrome and six with autism. Twenty (80%) of syndrome are more commonly used as diagnostic
the reference siblings were male. This is consistent labels than ASD. Children are asked to rate
with the male to female ratio of autistic spectrum statements about their siblings disorder as either
disorders reported in the literature (e.g., American true or false. Items are based on definitions and
Psychiatric Association, 1994). There were two criteria provided in the Diagnostic and Statistical
children in 60% of the families, 24% had three Manual of Mental Disorders 4th Edition (American
children, and 16% had four or more children. All Psychiatric Association, 1994), and cover aspects of
mothers and children were English speakers. the disorders such as course, prevalence, aetiology,
cognitive ability, and associated features. Higher
scores indicate greater knowledge of the disorder.
Measures
As far as possible, parallel items were constructed
Gilliam Autism Rating Scale (GARS). The Gilliam for each measure. Three experts in the field of
Autism Rating Scale (Gilliam, 1995) provided ASD contributed to the construction of the
information on the severity of disability for the questionnaires to ensure that the items were
reference siblings. The instrument has a mean of 100 clearly expressed and correctly scored. Cronbach
(SD515). The overall mean level of severity of ASD Alpha was used to test internal consistency for each
was a standard score of 86.32 (SD514.18), measure. Due to poor reliability estimates, 3 items
indicative of an average level of severity. When were removed from the original questionnaire on
diagnostic groups were examined separately, Asperger syndrome, with the parallel items removed
children with autism were rated somewhat higher from the autism questionnaire. Alpha for the
than children with Asperger syndrome (M590.33 remaining questions on the Asperger syndrome
and 85.05 respectively). This difference in severity questionnaire was .67. Alpha could not be
was not statistically significant. calculated for the autism questionnaire due to low
subject numbers. The final measures contained 18
Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). The Behavior items with a maximum score of 18. Questionnaires
Problem Scale of the Child Behavior Checklist are available from the second author on request.
(Achenbach, 1991) gathers information on
childrens problem behaviours. Items contribute to Kidcope. The Kidcope (Spirito, Stark, & Williams,
two broad-band scales, Internalizing (referring to 1988) is a brief paper-and-pencil self-report measure
80 P. Ross & M. Cuskelly

of children and adolescents use of coping strategies. examples of a problem they had experienced with
It assesses 10 common cognitive and behavioural their sibling. This modification was adopted as we
coping strategies, namely distraction, social wished to ascertain whether the initial problem
withdrawal, wishful thinking, self-criticism, identified by the participating siblings in response
blaming others, problem solving, emotional to the prompt was sufficiently representative of their
regulation, cognitive restructuring, social support, concerns about their brother or sisters interactions
and resignation. Two versions have been developed, with them. It seemed probable that there would be a
one for children 712 years (15 items), and one for range of problem types experienced within each
children 1318 years (10 items) (Rodrigue, Geffken, dyad. We were also interested to know whether
& Streisand, 2000). Children are asked to recall a similar coping strategies were chosen, irrespective of
recent problem (i.e., occurred in the past month) the problem type. Coping strategies and efficacy
and are then asked how much (a) sadness (b) information were collected about each of the three
nervousness, and (c) anger it produced. Responses problems in turn. Prior to statistical analyses of the
are on a 5-point scale anchored by not at all and data, information gathered from the Kidcope on
very much. Children are then asked to indicate the types of sibling-related problems identified by
whether they used any of the 10 coping strategies the participants was sorted into specific categories.
with respect to the identified stressor. For every The categorisation system was developed from the
coping strategy children indicate they used, they are data and was designed to reflect the underlying
asked to rate on a 3-point scale (05not at all to problem types. The authors initially developed
25a lot) how much the strategy helped (efficacy). operational definitions for 12 main problem types.
Moderate test-retest reliability estimates (r50.41 These were then collapsed into 5 categories: Aggres-
0.83) have been reported over short time periods sive Behaviours (i.e., physical aggression, verbal
ranging from 37 days (Spirito et al., 1988). aggression, destruction of property, disruption);
Concurrent validity has been displayed by Social Difficulties (i.e., invasion of privacy, lack of
moderate to high correlations of the Kidcope with social reciprocity, lack of sharing); Syndrome-
other common coping measures (Spirito et al., specific Behaviours (i.e., communication impair-
1988). The measure has been used to understand ments, unusual behaviours, inability to cope with
coping strategies used by children facing a number
change; lack of understanding of agency); Concern
of difficulties including their own disability or illness
for Sibling (i.e., worry about outcomes for brother/
(e.g., Edgar & Skinner, 2003; Garralda & Rangel,
sister); and Other (problems which could not be
2004).
categorised into one of the original 12 categories). In
cases where problem descriptions could be cate-
Procedure gorised as both Aggressive Behaviours and another
category, privilege was given to Aggressive
The study was approved by the ethics committee of
Behaviours. This decision was based on the view
the School of Education, University of Queensland.
that aggression, disruption and destructive beha-
Participants were recruited from local parent sup-
viours would have a greater impact on the sibling
port associations. In families where there was more
relationship than other behaviours. Table 1 presents
than one sibling willing to participate, a child was
randomly selected. Participating families were vis- the operational definitions for each category.
ited in their home by the first author. Both mothers The problem descriptions provided by the parti-
and children provided written consent before data cipating siblings were first categorised by the authors
were collected. Mothers initially provided demo- independently of each other. Discrepancies were
graphic information, then completed the GARS and discussed and some definitions were clarified. The
the CBCL in that order. Subsequently, the first authors subsequently reached 100% agreement.
author administered the Kidcope and the KAAS to Two other experts in the field of ASD subsequently
the participating sibling. These questionnaires were coded the problem descriptions using the classifica-
administered in a semi-structured interview format. tion system presented in Table 1. Inter-rater relia-
This allowed younger children who had difficulty bility was 82.6%, an acceptable level.
understanding some items to ask questions for
further clarification. A series of standardised
Results
prompts and examples were used in such cases.
In a change from the usual procedure for Prior to addressing the research questions, descrip-
administration of the Kidcope, children in this study tive data about the participating siblings adjustment
were asked to provide three (rather than one) and knowledge of ASD is presented.
Adjustment, sibling problems and coping 81

Table 1. Problem categorisations and operational definitions

Primary categorisation Problem type Operational definition

Aggressive Behaviours Physical aggression Any behaviour directed towards another


resulting in physical harm or pain (e.g.,
pushing, hitting, kicking, punching, etc.)
Verbal aggression Verbal aggressive statements directed towards
another person (e.g., shouting, swearing,
teasing)
Destruction of property Any physical act resulting in the damage or
destruction of property
Disruption Annoying and provoking behaviours directed
towards the sibling or others (e.g., deliberately
turning off the TV to spite the sibling;
interrupting the sibling when talking, etc.)
Social Difficulties Invasion of privacy Failure to respect the needs of others for
private space and time (e.g., going into
siblings room without permission)
Lack of social reciprocity Impairment in social interactions whereby one
child does not reciprocate in a social manner
(e.g., child doesnt get involved in simple social
play or games; prefers solitary activities; rejects
invitations to engage with others; doesnt
understand game rules)
Lack of sharing Difficulties in understanding the need to
apportion time with property items (e.g.,
television)
Syndrome-specific Behaviours Communication impairments Qualitative impairments in communication
skills and language use (e.g., phrase repetition,
unusual pitch, humming, etc.). Also includes
failure to understand another persons
language or communication directed towards
them.
Unusual behaviours Odd behaviours that are seen as inherent to the
child with ASD and are inappropriate given
the childs chronological age.
Inability to cope with change Any inappropriate behaviours occurring in
response to a disruption of ritualised routines
(e.g., shouting, physical aggression, etc.)
Lack of understanding of agency Child blames sibling for something outside the
siblings control (e.g., tuckshop not open)
Concern for Sibling Concern for sibling General concerns or worries that something
negative may happen to their sibling (e.g., that
they will be teased by other children, etc.)
Other Other Any problems that do not fall into the above
categories.

Psychological adjustment (1 male) were within the borderline range. Three


children were in the clinical range on the
The mean level of T scores for Internalising and
Externalising scale and two were in the borderline
Externalising problem behaviours on the Child
range. These five children were all males who were
Behavior Checklist (CBCL) for the participating
also in the clinical range on the Internalising scale.
siblings was 55.68 (SD511.88) and 48.04
Forty percent of the participants were therefore
(SD510.48) respectively. Scores were normally
identified as experiencing adjustment problems of
distributed and mean scores were well within the
sufficient severity to be of concern.
non-clinical range. Due to the small sample size, sex
comparisons were not undertaken. Using paired t-
tests, a significant difference was found between the
Knowledge of disorder
level of Internalising and Externalising problem
behaviours in the sample: t (24)53.56, p,.01. On average, participating siblings scored moderately
Eight siblings (7 males) were identified to be within well on the questions of the KAAS, indicating a
the clinical range on Internalising problems, and two reasonable understanding of their siblings condition
82 P. Ross & M. Cuskelly

(M514.12; SD52.55). Children correctly answered nervousness (rho5.49) and anger (rho5.40), while
an average of 66% of the questions on the knowledge moderate, did not reach significance. No other type
questionnaire. KAAS scores were not significantly of problem was reported at a high enough rate to
correlated with age of the participating sibling. allow this analysis to be attempted.

Consistency of problem type and affect Coping strategies


We initially examined childrens descriptions of We were interested to know if there was a coping
problem interactions to ascertain whether similar strategy that was typically used following a problem
problem types were reported across the three interaction of a specific type. Twenty-one children
responses. Nine children (36%) gave a response (84%) reported an aggressive incident at least once.
that fell within the same category for all three For the following analysis, we took the first occasion
incidents, nine gave two responses that were of the on which each child reported such an incident and
same type, and seven (28%) gave answers that examined his/her response. All children reported
reflected three different categories. Of the initial using at least three coping strategies in response to
responses, 52% described an incident that was an aggressive incident, with one child reporting
categorised as aggressive, 20% were categorised as using all ten. The median number of strategies
social problems, 16% as syndrome-specific, 4% as reported was six.
concern for the sibling, and 8% were unable to be The most common coping strategies used in
classified. When all 75 responses were classified, response to aggression were emotional regulation
these proportions remained much the same: 53% and wishful thinking, both reported by 91% of the
were categorised as aggressive, 16% as social children. These were followed by social withdrawal
difficulties, 20% as syndrome-specific, 5% as con- (86%) distraction (81%), problem solving (71%),
cern for the sibling, and 5% as other. social support (62%), resignation (57%), cognitive
Correlations of reports of emotional responses restructuring (48%), blaming others (24%), and
were undertaken using p,.01 to guard against Type self-criticism (10%).
1 error. The purpose of the initial analyses was For each coping strategy used, children were
related to the validity of the emotional response asked to rate how efficacious they found the strategy
aspect of the Kidcope. If the information from this in dealing with the problem. For this analysis, scores
element is to be useful, then children need to be able were collapsed into two categories, not efficacious
to discriminate between the different emotional (scores of 0), and efficacious (scores of 1 or 2).
states. High correlations between nervousness, anger The proportion of children who used a strategy and
and sadness experienced in response to an incident who considered it to be helpful were as follows:
would suggest that the instrument was failing in this emotional regulation (84%), wishful thinking (84%)
regard. None of the associations between childrens social withdrawal (89%) distraction (100%), pro-
reports of their emotional response to the problem blem solving (93%), social support (92%), resigna-
they described reached significance, suggesting that tion (58%), cognitive restructuring (100%), blaming
children were in fact discriminating between the others (60%), and self-criticism (50%).
various emotions. The Friedman rank test indicated significant
The second set of analyses was undertaken to differences between the strength of the emotions
ascertain whether children responded to the same felt by the participating siblings after an incident
problem types with the same levels of emotion across of aggression: x2(2)512.99, p,.01. Follow-up
different incidents. Eighteen children identified an Wilcoxon tests showed that anger was a significantly
incident that was categorised as of the same type on stronger response than either sadness or nervousness
at least two occasions. Correlations between levels of (Z522.76 and 22.84 respectively, both p,.01).
nervousness were high and significant (rho5.61, There was no significant difference between sadness
p,.01); for reports of sadness they were moderate and nervousness. The mean for anger following an
but failed to reach significance (rho5.49); and for incident of aggression was 3.57 (SD51.21). The
the experience of anger they did not reach signifi- mean for sadness was 2.24 (SD51.34) and for
cance (rho5.32). A similar analysis was conducted nervousness it was 1.90 (SD51.18).
for incidents that were classified as aggressive, Due to our interest in incidents classified as
however only 11 children provided two incidents of syndrome-specific, we repeated the above analyses
an aggressive interaction with their sibling. Level of for those occasions when such a behaviour was
sadness was highly correlated following an incident reported. As the number of these incidents was small
of aggression (rho5.87, p,.001), but ratings of (n515) and only 12 children reported such an
Adjustment, sibling problems and coping 83

incident, any interpretation must be undertaken with found between the two groups. In addition, when
great caution. The most commonly used coping knowledge scores were correlated with Internalising
strategy for syndrome-specific incidents was wishful and Externalising scores, relationships were non-
thinking (92%), followed by emotional regulation significant (Internalising rho5.13; Externalising
(83%), and distraction (83%). Social withdrawal rho5.23).
and problem solving were used by 75% of the
children, followed by resignation (67%), cognitive
Discussion
restructuring (58%), social support (50%), self-
criticism (17%), and blaming others (8%). The A substantially larger proportion of siblings of
proportion of children who used a strategy and who children with ASD in this study had scores on the
considered it helpful were as follows: wishful CBCL that placed them in the at-risk or clinical range
thinking (73%), emotional regulation (100%), dis- than would be expected. Six percent of the normative
traction (100%), social withdrawal (89%), problem sample are in these categories (Achenbach, 1991),
solving (100%), resignation (75%), cognitive and 13% have been found to fall into these categories
restructuring (100%), social support (100%), self- in a large-scale national study (Sawyer et al., 2001).
criticism (50%), and blaming others (100%). There Although the mean levels of internalising and
was no difference between the strength of emotions externalising behaviour scores were well within the
children reported feeling after such a problem normal range for the participating sibling group, 40%
interaction (Anger: M53.58, SD51.31; Sadness: of the siblings were reported by their mother to have
M52.33, SD51.44; Nervousness: M52.17, significant adjustment problems, predominantly
SD51.64). internalising difficulties. The current findings are
consistent with Gold (1993), who noted clinical levels
of depression in adolescent siblings of children with
Factors associated with adjustment
autism, and with Fisman and colleagues (1996,
To investigate whether either the number of 2000), who identified significantly higher levels of
strategies used or childrens perceptions of coping internalising and externalising behaviour problems in
efficacy were related to the adjustment measures, siblings of children with Pervasive Developmental
correlational analyses were undertaken. The number Disorder over a 3-year period. Smalley et al. (1995)
of strategies used was determined by identifying all also found an increased rate of major depressive
the strategies a child reported using over the three disorders among siblings of individuals with autism,
incidents. For example, a child may not have used and suggested that the familial link of autism and
blame in response to the first reported problem but depressive disorders could be due to shared genetic
may have used it for the second. No strategy was underpinnings. With the knowledge that the inci-
counted more than once, therefore the total could dence of autism increases markedly in first-degree
range from 010. The minimum number of strate- relatives (e.g., Piven et al., 1990), it is possible that
gies reported by participating siblings was 4, and the male siblings are at an increased genetic vulnerability
maximum was 10. A mean efficacy score for each for developing adjustment difficulties. It is important
participating sibling was generated by totalling the to keep in mind, however, that the sample included in
efficacy score for each of the three reported incidents the study reported here was small, included only
and dividing by 3. There was no significant correla- English-speakers, and may therefore be unrepresen-
tion between Internalising and Externalising scores tative. The fact that the sample was comprised of
and either the number of strategies (Internalising volunteers may also contribute to bias, with parents
rho52.10; Externalising rho52.01) or the mean who were concerned about the participating siblings
efficacy score (Internalising rho52.06; Externalising adjustment being more likely to agree to take part in
rho5.01). the study.
Participating siblings were divided into two groups Aggression was identified as the most common
based on their CBCL scores. The 10 whose score type of stressor within the sibling interaction, with
placed them in the borderline or clinical range were 84% of participating siblings reporting it as a
identified as the clinical group, and the remaining concern. Furthermore, 52% of participating siblings
15 as the non-clinical group. Using Mann- reported aggressive behaviours as a problem on the
Whitney U, the two groups were compared on the first occasion when they were asked to identify a
number of strategies used and on their mean efficacy problem they were having with their sibling. This is
scores. No significant difference was found. consistent with Bagenholm and Gillberg (1991),
This group comparison was repeated for KAAS who found that a disproportionate number of
scores and again there was no significant difference siblings of children with autism reported problems
84 P. Ross & M. Cuskelly

with their sibling disturbing them and breaking their Differences in child-rearing practices between coun-
property. However, due to the lack of a comparative tries may contribute to these contrasting findings,
sample of siblings of typically developing children, it however a more likely explanation lies in the
is unclear whether the predominance of aggressive different composition of the groups of children with
problems in the dyad is specific to families with a a disability used in the two studies. Those in the
child with ASD. Aggressive behaviour may well Gamble and McHale study had a range of disabling
reflect typical sibling relationships and interactions. conditions, including cerebral palsy and acquired
Further examination with an appropriate compar- brain injury, and all had an intellectual disability. All
ison group is required to determine whether it is the children in the study reported here had a
particularly characteristic of sibling interactions in diagnosis that placed them on the autism spectrum,
families with a child with ASD. Not surprisingly, and thus they would have been a more homogeneous
anger was found to be the strongest emotional group. It is clear that parents had made some effort
reaction to aggressive behaviours. The fact that two- to assist the non-disabled child to understand the
thirds of the group provided more than one type of impact of the condition on their siblings behaviour,
problem when given the opportunity points to a as they had a good understanding of the condi-
limitation of the Kidcope in identifying the range of tion(s). This understanding may have led children to
problems experienced by the group. consider some behaviour to be outside the control of
Choice of coping strategies did not appear to be their sibling. This may be less likely to occur for
problem specific. Participating siblings reported use children with other disabilities. It is also possible that
of coping strategies was very similar across aggressive the respondents in this study were influenced by a
and syndrome-specific incidents. Emotional regula- desire to present themselves in a good light and
tion and wishful thinking were the most common therefore under-reported their use of blaming others
strategies for both types of problem. The least as a coping strategy. This possibility could best be
common coping strategies were blaming others addressed by a study that triangulated reports from a
(24% and 8% respectively) and self-criticism (10% number of family members.
and 17% respectively). The finding that emotional Investigation of the association between the
regulation was the most frequently used strategy in psychological adjustment of siblings of children with
response to aggression is interesting, as anger was ASD and their knowledge of their brother or sisters
the strongest emotional response to aggressive disorder, as well as their perception of coping
behaviours. It is possible that siblings of children efficacy, found them to be unrelated. The lack of
with ASD are aware of the level of anger that association between the variables may be due to the
aggressive behaviours produce in them and choose small sample size. It may also be that children who
to cope with the situation by actively controlling were in the borderline or clinical range on the CBCL
their emotions. The equally high level of wishful found the coping strategies they used to be as helpful
thinking suggests that siblings of children with ASD as did children with lower scores. It is also possible
have a strong desire for things to be different. that other dynamic variables not assessed in the
The fact that blaming others was one of the least current study for example, parenting style, parent
commonly endorsed coping strategies suggests that distress, and family coping style and adaptation
the participants in this study did not blame their may be more important in influencing adjustment in
sibling for their aggressive behaviours. This is in the siblings of children with ASD. Reliance on self-
contrast to the findings reported by both Gamble report measures could also have contributed to the
and McHale (1989) and Roeyers and Mycke (1995). studys outcomes. For a more in-depth assessment
In both these studies, other-directed cognitions were and understanding of childrens coping styles and
the most frequently used strategy for coping when processes, an interview with the child is warranted
difficulties arose between siblings. However while (Spirito, 1996). A comprehensive interview may
the age range of the children in the study reported highlight differences in childrens coping efficacy
here was very similar to that of the children in the and more clearly identify strategies that are used by
Gamble and McHale study, 14 of the 25 participants well-adjusted children.
in this study were younger than their brother or sister
with ASD, whereas all siblings in Gamble and
Directions for future research
McHales study were older. It is possible that
children who are older than their brother or sister While the current study failed to find an association
with a disability might adopt a more blaming between coping efficacy and adjustment, it is believed
attitude than those who are younger, but it is that a paradigm shift away from focusing on status
difficult to see why this would be the case. variables towards approaches aimed at identifying
Adjustment, sibling problems and coping 85

dynamic variables (e.g., coping skills) should be American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
pursued further. As highlighted by McHale, Sloan, manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington: Author.
Bagenholm, A., & Gillberg, C. (1991). Psychosocial effects on
and Simeonsson (1986), siblings of children with a siblings of children with autism and mental retardation: A
disability are highly variable in their adjustment. The population-based study. Journal of Mental Deficiency, 35,
current study found that the most common difficul- 291307.
ties experienced by siblings of children with ASD in Bond, L., Nolan, T., Adler, R., & Robertson, C. (1994). The
the dyad are related to aggressive behaviours and Child Behavior Checklist in a Melbourne urban sample.
Australian Psychologist, 29, 103109.
syndrome-specific behaviours. However, what
Cuskelly, M. (1999). Adjustment of siblings of children with a
remains unclear is the frequency and severity of these disability: Methodological issues. International Journal for the
stressors on a day-to-day basis. Future research Advancement of Counselling, 21, 111124.
should address this area using a comparative sample Donovan, A. M. (1988). Family stress and ways of coping with
of children with typically developing siblings. adolescents who have handicaps: Maternal perceptions.
Comparisons between the types of problems fre- American Journal on Mental Retardation, 92, 502509.
Edgar, K. A., & Skinner, T. C. (2003). Illness representations and
quently reported by siblings of children with ASD coping as predictors of emotional well-being in adolescents with
with those reported by children who have typically Type 1 diabetes. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 28, 485493.
developing siblings would determine whether siblings Fisman, S., Wolf, L., Ellison, D., & Freeman, T. (2000). A
of children with ASD present with unique stressors in longitudinal study of siblings of children with chronic
their sibling relationships. disabilities. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 45, 369375.
Fisman, S., Wolf, L., Ellison, D., Gillis, B., Freeman, T., &
Further research using the Kidcope, and focusing
Szatmari, P. (1996). Risk and protective factors affecting
on its psychometric properties, would be most adjustment of siblings with chronic disabilities. Journal of
useful. The establishment of a valid tool for American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 15321541.
collecting data about childrens sibling experiences, Gamble, W. C., & McHale, S. M. (1989). Coping with stress in
coping strategies and emotional responses would be sibling relationships: A comparison of children with disabled
very helpful. In this study, childrens reports of and non-disabled siblings. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 10, 353373.
emotion following each individual incident they Gamble, W. C., & Woulbroun, J. E. (1993). Measurement
described were uncorrelated, suggesting that chil- considerations in the identification and assessment of stressors
dren were able to discriminate between the emotions and coping strategies. In Z. Stoneman & P. Waldman-Berman
of nervousness, sadness and anger. There was some (Eds.), The effects of mental retardation, disability, and illness on
consistency between childrens reports of levels of sibling relationships: Research issues and challenges (pp. 287319).
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
nervousness and sadness when separate incidents
Garralda, M. E., & Rangel, L. (2004). Impairment and coping in
were examined. These two findings together support children and adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: A
the view that the emotional response aspect of the comparative study with other pediatric disorders. Journal of
Kidcope has some value in helping us to understand Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 543552.
childrens emotional experiences. Gibbs, B. (1993). Providing support to sisters and brothers of
children with disabilities. In G. Singer & L. Powers (Eds.),
Families, disability, and empowerment: Active coping skills and
Conclusion strategies for family interventions (pp. 343363). Baltimore: Paul
H. Brookes.
The current study indicated that siblings of children
Gilliam, J. A. (1995). Gilliam Autism Rating Scale: Examiners
with ASD are at an increased risk of developing manual. Texas: Pro-Ed.
internalising behaviour problems. The contributing Gold, N. (1993). Depression and social adjustment in siblings of
factors to this outcome are unknown and may boys with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
include a genetic predisposition. It is important for Disorders, 23, 147163.
Hastings, R. P. (2003). Brief report: Behavioral adjustment of
future research to focus on dynamic variables in the
siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and
search for these contributors since, by definition, Developmental Disorders, 33, 99104.
they are open to change. Hensley, V. R. (1988). Australian normative study of the
Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist. Australian Psychologist,
Author note 23, 371382.
Heubeck, B. G. (2000). Cross-cultural generalizability of the
The authors received no funding for this study and CBCL syndromes across three continents: From the USA and
have no conflict of interest in publication of the Holland to Australia. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28,
results. 439450.
Howlin, P. (1988). Living with impairment: The effects on
children of having an autistic sibling. Child: Care, Health, and
References
Development, 14, 395408.
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/ Kaminsky, L., & Dewey, D. (2002). Psychosocial adjustment in
418 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, siblings of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and
Department of Psychiatry. Psychiatry, 43, 225232.
86 P. Ross & M. Cuskelly

Lobato, D. J., & Kao, B. T. (2002). Integrated sibling-parent Roeyers, H., & Mycke, K. (1995). Siblings of a child with autism,
group intervention to improve sibling knowledge and adjust- with mental retardation and with a normal development.
ment to chronic illness and disability. Journal of Pediatric Child: Care, Health and Development, 21, 305319.
Psychology, 27, 711716. Sawyer, M. G., Arney, F. M., Baghurst, P. A., Clark, J. J., Graetz,
Mates, T. E. (1990). Siblings of autistic children: Their B. W., Kosky, R. J., et al. (2001). The mental health of young
adjustment and performance at home and school. Journal of people in Australia: Key findings from the child and adolescent
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 545553. component of the national survey of mental health and well-
McCubbin, H. I., & Patterson, J. (1983). The family stress being. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35,
process: The double ABCX model of adjustment and 806814.
adaptation. In H. I. McCubbin, M. B. Sussman & J. M. Smalley, S. L., McCracken, J., & Tanguay, P. (1995). Autism,
Patterson (Eds.), Social stress and the family: Advances and affective disorders, and social phobia. American Journal of
developments in family stress theory and research. Marriage and Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 60, 1926.
Family Review (pp. 738). New York: Haworth. Spirito, A. (1996). Commentary: Pitfalls in the use of brief
McHale, S., Sloan, J., & Simeonsson, R. J. (1986). Sibling screening measures of coping. Journal of Pediatric Psychology,
relationships of children with autistic, mentally retarded, and 21, 573575.
nonhandicapped brothers and sisters: A comparative study. Spirito, A., Stark, L. J., & Williams, C. (1988). Development of a
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 399413. brief coping checklist for use with pediatric populations.
Piven, J., Gayle, J., Chase, G. A., Fink, B., Landa, R., Wzorek, Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 13, 555574.
M. M., & Folstein, S. E. (1990). A family history study of Verte, S., Roeyers, H., & Buysse, A. (2003). Behavioural problems,
neuropsychiatric disorders in the adult siblings of autistic social competence and self-concept in siblings of children with
individuals. Journal of American Academy of Child Adolescent autism. Child: Care, Health and Development, 29, 193205.
Psychiatry, 29, 177183. Wikler, L. (1986). Chronic stresses of families of mentally
Rodrigue, J. R., Geffken, G. R., & Morgan, S. B. (1993). retarded children. Family Relations, 30, 281288.
Perceived competence and behavioral adjustment of siblings of Yirimiya, N., Shaked, M., & Erel, O. (2001). Comparison of
children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental siblings of individuals with autism and siblings of individuals
Disorders, 23, 665674. with other diagnoses: An empirical summary. In E. Schopler,
Rodrigue, J. R., Geffken, G. R., & Streisand, R. M. (2000). Child N. Yirimiya & L. M. Marcus (Eds.), The research basis for
health assessment: A handbook of measurement techniques. Boston: autism intervention (pp. 5973). New York: Kluwer Academic/
Allyn & Bacon. Plenum Publishers.

You might also like