You are on page 1of 9

JBR-08159; No of Pages 9

Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Business Research

Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products


Jie Chen a, Lefa Teng b,, Shixiong Liu c, Huihuang Zhu d
a
Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China
b
Department of Marketing and Consumer Studies, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada, N1G 2W1
c
Shenzhen University, Guangdong, China
d
Sun Yat-sen University, Guangdong, China

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: When consumers consider their feelings about a future purchase, these feelings can inuence their preferences
Received 1 April 2012 for the product under consideration and alter their nal purchase decision. This research identies the effect of
Received in revised form 1 October 2013 anticipating regret on consumer preferences for a product that is a conspicuous counterfeit. Study 1 shows
Accepted 1 August 2014
that consumers are more likely to choose non-conspicuous luxury counterfeits after anticipating the potential
Available online xxxx
regret of purchasing a counterfeit product. Study 2 demonstrates the mediating effect of perceived risk on the
Keywords:
anticipation of regret effect. Studies 3 and 4 determine two boundary conditions of the anticipation of regret
Anticipating regret effect, specically, only consumers who have an independent self-view and are less conscious of their social
Non-conspicuous counterfeits face experience the anticipation of regret effect.
Perceived risk 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Social face

1. Introduction purchase an authentic brand because the product reects their person-
ality. Therefore, social-adjustive consumers are more likely to choose a
Crucial factors that inuence the consumption of counterfeit luxury prototypical counterfeit bag that has a noticeable logo.
products include the typicality and the display of brand logos, as well as However, among consumers who will purchase counterfeit goods,
a consumer's attitude toward counterfeit products (e.g., self- will consumers prefer a prototypical counterfeit luxury handbag when
presentation vs. self-expression). In terms of the typicality of the luxury the bag has a logo (conspicuous counterfeit) or doesn't have a logo
product, consumers who purchase counterfeit products have a strong (non-conspicuous counterfeit)? Further, if these consumers anticipate
tendency to choose luxury bags that look prototypicalthat is, bags regretting their choice to purchase a counterfeit, will they prefer a
that look familiar to other consumers (see Fig. 1a). Additionally, conspicuous or a non-conspicuous counterfeit? Existing research has
consumers seldom choose atypical luxury bags, which are unique and addressed the inuence of prototypical versus atypical counterfeits,
not often seen or easily recognized by other consumers (see Fig. 1b). loud versus quiet brand prominence, and social- versus value-
Regarding brand logos, consumers have a stronger preference for coun- adjustive attitudes on consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury
terfeit bags that have a loud brand prominence (a conspicuous brand), products, yet a gap exists as research has yet to address the effects of
because their signals are more decipherable than counterfeit bags that anticipating regret on consumer preferences for counterfeit luxury
have a quiet brand prominence (a non-conspicuous brand) (Han, products. Therefore, the goal of the current study is to determine
Nunes, & Drze, 2010; Han, Suk, & Chung, 2008). Lastly, Wilcox, Kim, whether anticipating regret decreases (or increases) consumers' desire
and Sen (2009) show that social-adjustive consumers (high need to for prototypical counterfeit luxury products that have a logo (do not
convey a high social status) are more likely to purchase a counterfeit have a logo).
brand because the product is a status symbol, whereas value-adjustive This research presents a conceptual model demonstrating that when
consumers (low need to convey a high social status) are more likely to consumers mentally generate hypothetical scenarios where they
anticipate the regret they may feel after the consumption of counterfeit
products (e.g., other consumers discover that their product is a counter-
feit, rather than the authentic luxury brand), consumers will be more
This study was supported by research grants from the National Natural Science likely to choose a non-conspicuous option over a conspicuous option.
Foundation of China (71072062), Shanghai Pujiang Program (13PIC071), Changjiang More specically, given that conspicuous prototypical counterfeits are
Scholars and Innovative Research Teams (PCSIRT) and SMC Chenxing Scholars. The au- associated with a higher probability of regret, this research proposes
thors gratefully acknowledge the nancial support of OMAFRA (#200261). They also
thank Hanwei Wang and Drew J. Harden for their help in this study.
that consumers are more likely to choose a counterfeit luxury product
Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 519 824 4120x53818; fax: +1 519 823 1964. that does not have a logo rather than a counterfeit luxury product that
E-mail address: leteng@uoguelph.ca (L. Teng). does have a logo. Therefore, the objective of this research is to analyze

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
0148-2963/ 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
2 J. Chen et al. / Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx

PROTOTYPICAL AND ATYPICAL LUXURY HANDBAGS

(a) (b)
Fig. 1. Prototypical and atypical luxury handbags.

consumers' decreased desire for conspicuous prototypical counterfeit the authentic prototypical luxury product possesses. Consumers who
luxury products by examining how anticipating regret inuences their cannot afford the authentic, original luxury product will purchase a
preference for purchasing such products. counterfeit that has a discernible logo of the original brand (Louis
The ndings of this research are particularly important because antic- Vuitton, Chanel, etc.). The logo of even the counterfeit prototypical con-
ipating regret relates to a growing interest in behavioral motivation, spicuous luxury product represents a symbolic signal of status and
attitude, and processing of counterfeits (Wilcox et al., 2009), as well as wealth (Han et al., 2010) and conveys the conspicuousness associated
non-commercial campaigns such as the International Anti-Counterfeiting with the authentic brand (Kim & Karpova, 2009). Consumers who pur-
Coalition (IACC) and Quality Brands Protection Committee (QBPC). chase counterfeit luxury brands seek the prestigious symbolism of the
Through a series of four studies, this research tests the aforementioned authentic brand at extreme cost savings (Ehrlich, 1985).
conceptual framework regarding consumer's anticipation of regret In addition, most of the literature on counterfeits focuses on con-
when purchasing counterfeit products. The rst study examines how sumers' motivation and intention to purchase such counterfeits
anticipating regret inuences consumers' choices of conspicuous proto- (Albers-Miller, 1999; Gentry, Putrevu, & Shultz, 2006; Kim & Karpova,
typical counterfeit luxury products (namely with logo vs. without logo). 2009; Leisen & Nill, 2001; Swami, Chamorro-Premuzic, & Furnham,
The conceptual model predicts that consumers who anticipate regret 2009; Tom, Garibaldi, Zeng, & Pilcher, 1998; Wilcox et al., 2009; Yoo &
will choose a product that does not have a logo. Study 2 investigates Lee, 2009), only a limited amount of research has focused directly on
how perceived risk mediates the effect of anticipating regret on con- consumers' consumption of conspicuous counterfeits (e.g., Han et al.,
sumers' preferences for conspicuous counterfeit luxury products. 2008, 2010; Wilcox et al., 2009). For example, Wilcox et al. (2009) dem-
Study 3 evaluates if consumers who have an independent self-view onstrates the inuence of elements of the marketing mix, such as prod-
rather than an interdependent self-view have a more pronounced uct logo design, on the social functions served by consumers'
decrease in preference for conspicuous counterfeit products due to consumption of luxury brands. Given that the logo or emblem creates
anticipating regret. Finally, Study 4 identies how consciousness of so- a luxury brand's conspicuousness, counterfeits that have a prominent
cial face inuences consumers' choice of conspicuously branded luxury logo will serve the social functions of most consumers of counterfeit
counterfeit products. The conceptual model predicts that the effect of products. Wilcox et al. (2009) proposes that the extent to which a coun-
anticipating regret will only decrease the preference for conspicuously terfeit luxury brand fullls a consumer's social goals is likely to depend
branded counterfeit products among consumers who are less conscious on brand conspicuousness. These researchers suggest that the way a
of their social face. luxury brand's meaning becomes accessible to other consumers
through product design can affect the consumers' desire for counterfeit
2. Theoretical background versions of the authentic brand. Wilcox et al. (2009) also suggests that a
prominently displayed brand logo on a counterfeit enables consumers
2.1. Previous research on prototypical conspicuous counterfeits to acquire and display to other consumers the authentic brand's aspira-
tional associations.
One of the most worrying aspects about counterfeiting has always Another study using online authentic brand data and counterfeit
been the threat to human life and safety caused by poorly made brand data from Thailand shows that counterfeit handbags copy loud
counterfeits using potentially dangerous materials (BASCAP, 2009). authentic brands and that brand prominence drives which product
This concern primarily applies to items such as counterfeit medication lines will be counterfeited (Han et al., 2010). These authors dene
and electronics; however, today's counterfeits are mostly low-quality loud branding as a product that has visible markings that ensure
fake copies of luxury apparel, which are virtually harmless to the end consumers can recognize the brand; a product with a loud brand is a
consumer. Therefore, the current strategy of educating consumers conspicuous brand. Conversely, Han et al. (2010) dene a quiet brand
about the risk to human life and safety posed by counterfeits is unlikely as a product that is discreet and does not have any visible markings
to be effective for decreasing purchases of counterfeit luxury products. (e.g., brand logo may be on the inside of a handbag, rather than on the
Counterfeits that resemble the original can comply with the attributes outside); a product with a quiet brand is a non-conspicuous brand.
of genuine prototypical luxury products that convey high status Consumers with prototypical counterfeits that have big logos face a
(brand logo, color, etc.). Additionally, conspicuousness is widely higher risk that other consumers will easily identify their products as
assumed to be the key motivation behind luxury consumption (Han counterfeit.
et al., 2010; O'Cass & McEwen, 2004; Phau & Prendergast, 2000; Piron, Few studies have focused on how consumers choose between proto-
2000; Truong, Simmons, McColl, & Kitchen, 2008; Vigneron & Johnson, typical counterfeits that have a logo (loud, conspicuous brand) or do not
1999; Wang & Chen, 2004). Therefore, consumers purchase counterfeit have a logo (quiet, non-conspicuous brand). In reality, counterfeiters
luxury products because they want to obtain the conspicuousness that often sell consumers conspicuous prototypical bags because consumers

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
J. Chen et al. / Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx 3

who do not have the nancial means to purchase an authentic luxury Conspicuous counterfeits are more familiar to consumers than non-
bag and have a high need for status, will turn to a cheap copy of the conspicuous counterfeits; as such, other consumers are more likely to
original in order to emulate consumers whom they recognize to be detect that a conspicuous counterfeit is fake compared to a non-
wealthy. Additionally, counterfeiters seldom manufacture atypical conspicuous counterfeit. In turn, consumers may anticipate stronger
non-conspicuous luxury bags because they will not signal the owner's feelings of regret should they purchase a conspicuous counterfeit com-
supposedly high social standing to other consumers. Normally, pared to a non-conspicuous counterfeit. This research proposes that
prototypical bags have strong, notable brands; an example is the famous consumers' preference for a conspicuous counterfeit will decrease
and easily recognizable black Chanel bag, easily identied by its quilted when they anticipate feelings of regret caused by their purchase
pattern, gold accents, and gold C's (see Fig. 1a). Counterfeiters frequently decision. This anticipation of regret will make the consumer act more
copy the black Chanel bag due to its recognizability, even a counterfeit cautiously by selecting the non-conspicuous counterfeit rather than
can provide the illusion of luxury to the consumer. An example of an the conspicuous counterfeit.
atypical bag is the Louis Vuitton bag (see Fig. 1b), which is not often
seen in public, or carried by celebrities. These atypical bags are not easily H1. Given the choice between a conspicuous and a non-conspicuous
copied by counterfeiters, thus allowing for a restricted display of luxury counterfeit, consumers are more likely to choose the non-conspicuous
and conspicuousness. Therefore, questions about how consumers will counterfeit due to the anticipation of regret effect.
decrease their preference for conspicuous prototypical counterfeits and A psychological process underlies the anticipation of regret effect
which segment of consumers is more likely to decrease their preference in reducing consumers' preference for conspicuous counterfeits. This
for counterfeit products is the focus of this research. research anticipates the mechanism underlying the decrease in con-
spicuous counterfeit preference is due to an increase in consumers'
perceived risk (e.g., other consumers will discover that the product
2.2. The inuence of anticipating regret on consumer preferences for is a counterfeit brand and not an authentic brand, lower product
prototypical conspicuous counterfeits quality in terms of performance, reliability, and durability). As
such, consumers' perception of risk may lead to the anticipation of
Consumers make their purchase decisions with a concerted effort to regret effect, which, in turn, affects (reduces) their preference for
reduce the likelihood of experiencing regret (Bell, 1982; Brehm & conspicuousness.
Wicklund, 1970; Loomes & Sugden, 1982), and to maximize expected
utility (von Neumann & Morgenstern, 1947). However, given that H2. Consumer's perception of risk mediates the effect of anticipation of
counterfeits are low quality copies of authentic luxury brands, the regret on the choice between a conspicuous or non-conspicuous
performance, reliability, and durability of the counterfeit is unknown prototypical counterfeit option.
nor guaranteed with any certainty (Lai & Zaichkowsky, 1999). When The anticipation of regret effect may also have two boundary condi-
consumers consider a purchase decision, they consider the costs and tions. The rst boundary condition is self-view. Studies in social psy-
benets of the purchase by engaging in mental simulations of a world chology focus on the distinction between the independent self-view
in which they purchase the product and another world in which they and the interdependent self-view; the former views the self as different
do not purchase the product (counterfactual thinking; see Roese, from others, whereas the latter views the self as dened by others
1997, for a detailed review of counterfactual thinking). If a consumers' (Aaker & Lee, 2001). As such, the inuence of the anticipation of regret
mental simulation of the world in which they make the product pur- effect may be moderated by consumers' self-view (independent self-
chase is more positive than reality, they will feel negative affect (regret). view vs. interdependent self-view). In both self-view conditions when
Furthermore, when the outcome of a consumers' purchase decision is consumers anticipate regret, the anticipation of regret effect will likely
unknown, as is often the case when the purchase decision is about a make them consider the consequences of their actions should other
counterfeit product, consumers anticipate that their decision will lead consumers discover that their product is counterfeit. This effect may
to future regret (prefactual). Therefore, consumers' consideration of make them be more cautious in their purchase decision by preferring
prefactuals (mental simulations of possible future events) can affect the non-conspicuous counterfeit. However, when independent self-
their purchase decisions (Gleicher et al., 1995; McConnell et al., 2000). view consumers do not anticipate regret, they are likely to have a great-
Counterfeit conspicuousness, unlike authentic brand conspicuous- er preference for the conspicuous counterfeit because they want their
ness, is a double-edged sword. On the one hand, consumers who cannot conspicuous product choice to stand out and be different from other
afford the authentic brand will purchase a counterfeit with the hope of consumers. Conversely, when interdependent self-view consumers do
engaging in the conspicuous consumption associated with the authentic not anticipate regret, they are likely to consider the social context of
brand. On the other hand, the outcome may be negative because their purchase and prefer the non-conspicuous counterfeit. That is, be-
consumers face a risk of embarrassment if other consumers identify cause the interdependent consumer's self-view is dependent on other
that their luxury product is a counterfeit. Given that consumers seldom consumers, they will prefer the non-conspicuous option because they
anticipate regret prior to purchasing a counterfeit, their choice to want to t in among other consumers.
purchase a counterfeit may result in damage to their self- and social-
image. This damage may be avoidable if consumers anticipate regret H3. Consumers' self-view moderates the inuence of the anticipation
prior to purchase. of regret effect on consumers' choice between a conspicuous and non-
An implication of this effect is that a consumer's anticipation of conspicuous counterfeit.
regret serves as a mechanism to minimize the regret they will feel
after the purchase. When consumers anticipate the potential negative H3a. When consumers' independent self-view is more accessible and
outcomes of purchasing a conspicuous counterfeit, their memories they anticipate regret, they will prefer the non-conspicuous counterfeit
and cognition will inform them not only about the erroneous choices over the conspicuous counterfeit.
they have made in the past, but also about the correct choice to make H3b. When consumers' independent self-view is more accessible and
in the future (Hetts & Boninger, 2000; Shih & Schau, 2011). When con- they do not anticipate regret, they will prefer the conspicuous counter-
sumers imagine the cost and the benet of purchasing conspicuous feit over the non-conspicuous counterfeit.
counterfeits, they will instead opt for the non-conspicuous option in
order to derive maximum utility from the counterfeit purchase, and H3c. When consumers' interdependent self-view is more accessible,
decrease the likelihood that other consumers will discover the product they will prefer the non-conspicuous counterfeit to the conspicuous
is a counterfeit. counterfeit regardless of whether they anticipate regret.

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
4 J. Chen et al. / Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx

The second boundary condition on the anticipation of regret effect is that their product is counterfeit. The design and stimuli of Study 1
the consciousness of social face. Consumers of counterfeit luxury prod- were adapted from Wilcox et al. (2009), and are similar to other
ucts tend to hold social-adjustive attitudes because they desire luxury studies on counterfeit goods (e.g., Ang, Cheng, Lim, & Tambyah,
products for self-presentation and to convey a high social status 2001; Cheung & Prendergast, 2006). The study utilized a between-
(Wilcox et al., 2009). Consumers desire to gain face (e.g., maintain rela- subject design with two levels of regret (regret: anticipation of
tionships, or attain approval in social situations) and fear to lose face regret vs. control). The dependent variable is the participants' choice
(e.g., lose friends, or attain disapproval in social situations) (Wilcox of prototypical counterfeit handbags (counterfeit options: conspicuous
et al., 2009; Zhang, Cao, & Grigoriou, 2011). A consumer who is less vs. non-conspicuous).
face-conscious (low need to present himself/herself with a high social Sixty-nine female undergraduates who have purchased a counterfeit
status) may be more likely to choose a non-conspicuous counterfeit product in the past were randomly assigned to the experimental (antic-
due to the anticipation of regret effect. The underlying psychological ipation of regret) or control condition. Consumers who have experience
process is that consumers who are less face-conscious may not antici- with purchasing counterfeits were recruited because extent research
pate any possibility of regret that other consumers will discover the indicates that such consumers are signicantly more willing to buy
true nature of their product. However, if such consumers receive a counterfeit products in the future (Furnham & Valgeirsson, 2007;
prompt to anticipating regret, they will mentally simulate the possible Swami et al., 2009; Tom et al., 1998). Participants completed a short
negative outcome if other consumers realize that their product is a questionnaire in exchange for a gift.
counterfeit; this anticipation of regret will decrease the consumer's In the control condition, participants viewed pictures of two coun-
original preference for the conspicuous option and will increase their terfeit Chanel handbags, counterbalanced for order. The two handbags
preference for the non-conspicuous option. were identical, except one featured a Chanel logo (conspicuous option)
Conversely, a person who is more face-conscious (high need to and the other did not (non-conspicuous option) (see Fig. 2). Participants
present himself/herself with a high social status) may prefer the non- were then informed that each handbag was an identical counterfeit of
conspicuous counterfeit in case other consumers discover that the prod- an actual Chanel handbag, and the counterfeit bag was affordable.
uct is a counterfeit. Therefore, they will prefer the non-conspicuous After being given this information, participants were asked which hand-
counterfeit option, even without anticipating regret. bag they would rather purchase. Participants were then thanked and
debriefed.
H4. Consumers' consciousness of social face will moderate the inu- The anticipation of regret condition was identical to the control con-
ence of the anticipation of regret effect on a consumers' choice between dition, with the addition of the manipulation for anticipation of regret.
a conspicuous and non-conspicuous counterfeit. Before participants selected which bag they would rather purchase,
H4a. When consumers are less face-conscious and they anticipate they were rst asked to consider how they would feel if they bought
regret, they will prefer the non-conspicuous counterfeit over the con- the handbag without a logo and other consumers later discovered that
spicuous counterfeit. the handbag was counterfeit, and how they would feel if they bought
the handbag with a logo and other consumers later discovered that
H4b. When consumers are less face-conscious and they do not the handbag was counterfeit.
anticipate regret, they will prefer the conspicuous counterfeit over the
non-conspicuous counterfeit.
3.2. Results and discussion
H4c. When consumers are more face-conscious, they will prefer non-
conspicuous counterfeit over the conspicuous counterfeit, regardless A chi-square test was conducted to compare consumer preferences
of whether they anticipate regret. in the experimental and control conditions. As summarized in Fig. 3,
the results support H1. Signicantly more participants chose the
handbag without a logo in the anticipation of regret condition than in
3. Study 1: The anticipation of regret effect the control condition (60.5% vs. 35.5%; 2(1) = 4. 284,p b 0.04).
Study 1 provides initial support for the proposition that anticipation
3.1. Design, participants, and procedure of regret decreases consumer preference for conspicuous counterfeits
and increases consumer preference for non-conspicuous counterfeits.
Study 1 is an empirical test of, H1, which states that consumers However, this study sheds little light on the mechanism underlying
are more likely to choose a non-conspicuous counterfeit product the anticipation of regret effect. Most research on the anticipation of
after they consider how they would feel if other consumers discover regret effect has examined regret on delayed choice (e.g., Simonson,

PROTOTYPICAL CHANNEL COUNTERFEITS WITH LOGO AND WITHOUT LOGO

Fig. 2. Prototypical Channel counterfeits with logo and without logo.

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
J. Chen et al. / Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx 5

STUDY 1 RESULTS participants selected the non-conspicuous option. Mediation analysis


was conducted using the process application and method provided by
Hayes (2013). Anticipating regret was positively associated with
perceived risk, as indicated by a signicant regression coefcient
( = 0.93, t = 1.99, p = 0.05). A negative relationship also exists be-
tween anticipating regret and counterfeit choice ( = 0.50, z =
3.27, p = 0.001). Finally, anticipating regret was found to have a
non-signicant effect on counterfeit choice ( = 0.81, z = 1.46,
p = 0.14). Bootstrap results demonstrated that the indirect effect was
signicant, as the bootstrapped 95% CI around the indirect effect does
not contain zero (1.27, 0.0067). Thus H2 is supported, as perceived
risk mediates the effect of anticipating regret on counterfeit choice
(Fig. 4).
Study 2 provides robust evidence that consumers' perceived risk
mediates their preference for counterfeit products. More specically,
when consumers' associate counterfeit products with risk, they
anticipate the regret that they will feel when, for example, other con-
Fig. 3. Study 1 results. sumers discover that their product is a counterfeit and not an authentic
product or they discover that the counterfeit is low quality. This effect
leads to consumers being more likely to prefer non-conspicuous
1992), behaviors (e.g., Hetts & Boninger, 2000), and decisions (e.g., Shih counterfeit options over conspicuous counterfeit options. Studies 3
& Schau, 2011). Very little research has examined how the anticipation and 4 will continue to develop the anticipation of regret effect through
of regret effect inuences counterfeit purchases. Study 2 investigates investigating potential boundary conditions, namely, self-view and
perceived risk as a potential mechanism for the anticipation of regret social face.
effect,
5. Study 3: Self-view and the anticipation of regret
4. Study 2: Perceived risk and anticipation of regret
5.1. Design, participants, and procedure
4.1. Design, participants, and procedure
Study 3 explored one potential boundary condition of the anticipa-
The main purpose of Study 2 was to further investigate the tion of regret effect, self-view. Participants were primed to adopt either
mechanism underlying the inuence of the anticipation of regret effect an independent or interdependent self-view and then examined the in-
on consumers' choice of counterfeit products. Given that counterfeits uence of the anticipation of regret effect on consumers' selection of
are copies of authentic brands, an inherent risk is associated with coun- conspicuous and non-conspicuous counterfeits. Although the primary
terfeits that is not associated with authentic brands (e.g., other con- objective of Study 3 was to provide the boundary condition of self-
sumers will discover that the product is a counterfeit, or the counterfeit view, the second objective of Study 3 was to rule out an alternative ex-
product is lower quality compared to the authentic product). Thus, the planation of a moral effect. Although data from Studies 1 and 2 conrm
expected result is that the inuence of the anticipation of regret effect the anticipation of regret effect, other effects, such as moral consider-
on consumers' preferences for conspicuous and non-conspicuous coun- ations, could provide an alternative explanation for these ndings.
terfeits operates via consumers' perception of the increased risk associat- Study 3 measured moral items to test whether the moral effect provides
ed with purchasing counterfeit products. a better explanation for the results of Studies 1 and 2 than the anticipa-
Seventy female participants were recruited from a database of con- tion of regret effect. The third goal of Study 3 was to replicate previous
sumers who have experience with purchasing counterfeit products. ndings using different stimuli for prototypical counterfeit bags.
The design and procedure of Study 2 is similar to that of Study 1, with Specically, the stimuli brand was changed from Chanel (Studies 1
the following exceptions. After participants examined the photos of and 2), to Gucci, another luxury brand of handbags (see Fig. 5).
the two counterfeit Chanel handbags, consumers perceived risk was Seventy-one female undergraduates participated in Study 3. Partici-
measured using two 7-point scale questions adapted from Nordgren, pants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 (regret:
Van Der Pligt, and Van Harreveld (2007): What are the risks of anticipation of regret vs. control) 2 (self-view prime: independent
purchasing the bag A /B (the bag with a logo/without a logo)?; and How vs. interdependent) between-subject design. Using the same procedure
great is the risk of purchasing the bag A/B (the bag with a logo/without a as Study 1, participants were presented two images of counterfeit hand-
logo)? (1 = no risk to 7 = extreme risk). Consumers then chose bags. Using the same manipulation as Study 1, one handbag had a logo
which handbag was their preferred option. (conspicuous option), and the other did not (non-conspicuous option).
Self-view was primed using stimulus from Aaker and Lee (2001), which
4.2. Results and discussion consists of pictures that focused on either an individual or a group of
people (i.e., family; independent and interdependent self-view,
A chi-square test was conducted to examine participants' prefer- respectively).
ences for the conspicuous and non-conspicuous counterfeit options. Aaker and Lee's (2001) Self/Others Thoughts Index was adapted to
The results indicate that more participants chose the non-conspicuous measure participants' independent and interdependent self-views.
option in the anticipation of regret condition (55.9%) than in the control The questions in this index consisted of six 7-point scale questions:
condition (30.6%), 2(1) =4.580, p = 0.032. These results replicate the please describe the extent to which (1) you thought just about yourself;
anticipation of regret effect initially found in Study 1. (2) your thoughts about the message were focused on just yourself;
Second, in order to examine H2, which stated that the anticipation of (3) your thoughts were focused on just you; (4) you thought about
regret effect is mediated by perceived risk, the two items that measured your friends; (5) your thoughts about the message were focused on
perceived risk were averaged to create a single item ( = 0.906). The your friends; and (6) your thoughts were focused on your friends
key dependent variable was participants' choice of handbag, which (1 = not at all; 7 = a lot). The moral effect was measured using a
was coded as 1 if participants selected the conspicuous option and 0 if Moral Index, which averaged three 7-point scale questions; these

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
6 J. Chen et al. / Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx

STUDY 2 - MEDIATION OF ANTICIPATING REGRET TO COUNTERFEIT CHOICE VIA


PERCEIVED RISK

Fig. 4. Study 2: Mediation of anticipating regret to counterfeit choice via perceived risk. *p b 0.05; **p b 0.01; ***p N 0.1.

questions were anchored by unethical/ethical, dishonest/honest, and 5.4. Results and discussion
immoral/moral. Participants were told that the questionnaires in the
packet (self-view manipulation, moral effect, anticipation of regret To test H3, category modeling analysis was conducted on partici-
effect, and choice of conspicuous or non-conspicuous counterfeit pants' preference for the conspicuous and non-conspicuous counterfeit
options) were unrelated to one another. options. The analysis revealed a signicant main effect of regret,
2(1) = 7.861, p = 0.05, and a signicant two-way interaction effect,
2(1) = 4.375, p = 0.036. These results indicate that consumers' ac-
5.2. Pretest cessibility of self-view moderates the anticipation of regret effect.
Overall, the main effect of the anticipation of regret found in Study
A pretest was conducted to verify the effectiveness of the self-view 1 was replicated in Study 3. The analysis also showed that the
prime. Participants (n = 19) were randomly assigned to either the inde- proportion of participants who chose the non-conspicuous option
pendent or interdependent self-view condition. The rst three items of in the anticipation of regret condition (80.5%) was signicantly
the Self/Others Thought Index were averaged to form an Independent higher than the proportion of participants in the control condition
Self-View Index ( = 0.791), and the latter three items were averaged (45.7%), 2(1) = 9.282, p = b0.01. However, the proportion of par-
to form an Interdependent Self-View Index ( = 0.972). Participants ticipants who chose the non-conspicuous option did not vary across
given the independent situational prime thought more about them- the independent (54.2%) and interdependent conditions (72.2%),
selves (M = 3.27) than their group of friends (M = 1.94), t = 2.501, 2(1) = 0.117, ns.
p = 0.02, whereas participants given the interdependent situational In addition, the anticipation of regret effect was tested separately in
prime thought more about their group of friends (M = 4.66) than the independent self-view and interdependent self-view conditions.
themselves (M = 1.58), t = 3.947, p b 0.01. Within the independent self-view condition, participants were more
likely to choose the non-conspicuous option in the anticipation of regret
condition (84.2%) more than that in the control condition (18.8%),
5.3. Manipulation check 2(1) = 14.988, p b 0.01. These results support H3a and H3b. However,
in the interdependent condition, the interaction effect disappeared;
The rst three items of the Self/Others Thought Index were averaged participants' preference for the conspicuous and non-conspicuous op-
to form an Independent Self-View Index ( = 0.894), and the other tions did not vary across the anticipating regret condition (76.4%) and
three items were averaged to form an Interdependent Self-View Index the control condition (68.4%), 2(1) = 0.290, ns. These results support
( = 0.989). Participants given the independent situational prime H3c (Fig. 6).
thought more about themselves (M = 3.13) than their group of friends A nal test was conducted for the moral effect on participants'
(M = 2.30), t = 2.102, p b 0.04, whereas participants given the interde- preference for the conspicuous and non-conspicuous options. The
pendent situational prime thought more about their group of friends three items in the Moral Index were averaged ( = 0.889). The re-
(M = 5.15) than themselves (M = 2.38), t = 6.659, p b 0.001. sults showed no difference in the Moral Index between the

PROTOTYPICAL GUCCI COUNTERFEITS WITH LOGO AND WITHOUT LOGO

Fig. 5. Prototypical Gucci counterfeits with logo and without logo.

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
J. Chen et al. / Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx 7

STUDY 3 RESULTS 6. Study 4: Social face and the anticipation of regret


100.0%
6.1. Design, participants and procedure
84.2%
% of individuals Choosing

80.0% 76.4%
The purpose of Study 4 was to test a second boundary condition of
hadbag without a Logo

68.4%
the anticipation of regret effectconsciousness of social faceand to
60.0% provide additional evidence for the underlying psychological process
that drives the anticipation of regret effect. Study 4 explored whether
40.0%
face-consciousness moderates the anticipation of regret effect. Another
goal of Study 4 was to resolve one of the limitations from previous
18.8% studies by recruiting participants from the marketplace.
20.0%
After ltering potential participants' responses regarding their
experiences with purchasing counterfeit products, 128 female par-
0.0% ticipants were recruited for Study 4. Participants were assigned to
control anticipating regret
one of four conditions in a 2 (regret: anticipation of regret vs.
Independent Interdependent
control) 2 (consciousness of social face: high vs. low) between-
subject design experiment. Using the same procedure as Study 1,
Fig. 6. Study 3 results.
participants were presented two images of counterfeit Chanel handbags,
and chose between purchasing a conspicuous or non-conspicuous coun-
terfeit. Participants then answered 11 questions designed to measure
anticipation of regret (M = 4.933) and control conditions (M = their consciousness of social face (Zhang et al., 2011). Participants were
5.185), t = 0.731, ns. Additionally, the results showed no differ- told that the questionnaires in the packet (consciousness of social face
ence in the Moral Index between participants who selected the con- manipulation, anticipation of regret effect, and choice of conspicuous or
spicuous counterfeit option (M = 5.128) and the non-conspicuous non-conspicuous counterfeit options) were unrelated to one another.
counterfeit option (M = 5.022), t = 0.296, ns.
The results of Study 3 are consistent with H3, which stated that the
anticipation of regret effect is moderated by self-view (independent 6.2. Results and discussion
vs. interdependent). In addition, Study 3 provides further evidence for
the anticipation of regret effect. The anticipation of regret effect only The 11 items used to measure participants' consciousness of social
inuences consumers who have an independent self-view, but does face had high reliability ( = 0.825). Participants' responses to all
not inuence those who have an interdependent self-view. That is, questions were averaged to a single item, then a median split was
independent self-view consumers are more likely to choose the non- used to divide participants into low- and high-face consciousness
conspicuous counterfeit option only when they anticipate regret, groups. In order to test H4, that the anticipation of regret effect will be
whereas interdependent self-view consumers are more likely to choose moderated by consumers' consciousness of social face, category model-
the non-conspicuous counterfeit option regardless of whether they ing analysis was conducted on participants' choice for conspicuous and
anticipate regret. The results of Study 3 thereby extend the results of non-conspicuous counterfeit options. The analysis showed that, the
Studies 1 and 2: one boundary condition of the anticipation of regret proportion of participants who chose the non-conspicuous counterfeit
effect is when consumers' self-view is focused on the group. Furthermore, option in the anticipation of regret condition (66.2%) was signicantly
Study 3 ruled out an alternative explanation for the results observed in higher than the proportion of participants in the control condition
Studies 1 and 2: a moral effect did not provide a better explanation (42.9%), 2(1) = 6.00, p b 0.02. This result again replicates the
than the anticipation of regret effect on decreasing participants' anticipation of regret effect found in Study 1 However, the proportion
preference for the conspicuous option. Study 4 will build on the re- of participants who chose the non-conspicuous option did not vary
sults of Studies 1 through 3 through investigating another potential across participants in the low consciousness of social face condition
boundary condition of the anticipation of regret effectconsciousness (49%) and the high consciousness of social face condition (60%),
of social face. 2(1) = 0.70, p = ns).
The analysis also showed a signicant two-way interaction effect,
2(1) = 3.70, p = 0.0543, indicating that participants' consciousness
STUDY 4 RESULTS of social face moderated the anticipation of regret effect. Therefore,
the anticipation of regret effect was tested separately in the low and
high consciousness of social face conditions. In the low consciousness
condition, the anticipation of regret effect was replicated, as participants
preferred the non-conspicuous counterfeit in the anticipating condition
(71.4%) signicantly more than that in the control condition (32.4%),
2(1) = 9.697, p b 0.01. These results support H4a and H4b. However,
in the high consciousness condition, the effect disappeared and partici-
pants' preference for the non-conspicuous counterfeit did not vary be-
tween the anticipation of regret condition and the control condition
(57.7% vs. 62.2%, 2(1) = 0.127, ns). These results support H4c (Fig. 7).
In summary, Study 4 provides evidence that the anticipation of re-
gret effect holds only for those consumers who are less conscious of
their social face. For consumers who are more conscious of their social
face, the anticipation of regret effect does not inuence their preference
for non-conspicuous counterfeits. The results from Study 4 extend the
results of Studies 1, 2, and 3: a second boundary condition exists
when the anticipation of regret effect is not observed, namely, when
Fig. 7. Study 4 results. consumers are more conscious of their social face.

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
8 J. Chen et al. / Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx

7. General discussion anticipation of regret effect occurs only for consumers who have an in-
dependent self-view (Study 3) and are less conscious of their social face
The present research addresses the question of how consumers' (Study 4).
preferences for conspicuous prototypical counterfeit products will de-
crease due to anticipating regret when purchasing such a product.
Study 1 extended previous research about anticipating regret 9. Managerial implications
(e.g., Simonson, 1992; Keinan & Kivetz, 2008; Shih & Schau, 2011) by
demonstrating that if consumers anticipate that they will regret pur- Results of the present research should be of interest to anti-
chasing a counterfeit product should other consumers discover the counterfeiting practitioners. One important nding from these studies
product is counterfeit, they will prefer to purchase a non-conspicuous is the reduction of consumers' likelihood of purchasing a conspicuous
counterfeit (one without a logo) over a conspicuous counterfeit (one counterfeit product when they anticipate the regret they will feel. Fur-
with a logo). This phenomenon is referred to as the anticipation of re- thermore, this research suggests which consumers will be more likely
gret effect. Study 2 showed that the anticipation of regret effect is due to experience a decrease in preference for conspicuous counterfeits, as
to consumers' perceived risk: consumer preference for conspicuous well as how the decrease will happen.
counterfeits decreased when they anticipated the regret associated A seemingly constant stream of counterfeiting incidents is raising
with such a purchase, specically because they perceive the purchase awareness of the issues surrounding counterfeit products among
as risky. Moreover, boundaries to the anticipation of regret effect consumers, regulators, enforcement agencies, and brand marketers.
occur when consumers have an independent self-view (Study 3) or Enforcement agencies and marketers have been charged with the task
when they are less conscious of their social face (Study 4). Across of nding anti-counterfeit solutions. Currently, these solutions have
these studies, evidence for the anticipation of regret effect was robust focused on strengthening enforcement regimes centered on deterring
as the effect occurred in all four studies. the production and trade of counterfeit products. However, focusing
on supply side strategies will not successfully eliminate counterfeit
8. Theoretical implications products.
In the face of escalating global growth of the purchase of counterfeit
Beyond studying demographic and psychographic variables that ex- luxury products, an equally aggressive attempt to weaken the demand
plain consumers' counterfeit purchase intentions, this research sought for counterfeit products should complement current supply side control
to investigate the psychological mechanism that underlies consumers' regimes (BASCAP, 2009). From the demand side, consumers will only
decision processing with regard to anticipating regret. This research stop purchasing counterfeit luxury products when their preferences
makes two important theoretical contributions to understanding this for conspicuous prototypical counterfeits change. This research pro-
mechanism. vides one potential perspective to change these preferences. Although
These studies were the rst to systematically investigate how the an- the present research is not designed to solve all issues associated with
ticipation of regret effect inuences consumers' preferences for counter- counterfeit products, this research emphasizes the importance of exam-
feit products. This research demonstrates that the anticipation of regret ining ways that anticipating regret can decrease consumers' preferences
effect decreases consumers' preferences for conspicuous counterfeits. for counterfeits, especially conspicuous counterfeits. Given that this re-
The bulk of extant research on counterfeits is focused on variables related search found that consumers' perceived risk mediates the anticipation
to purchase intentions (Swami et al., 2009; Gentry et al., 2006; Leisen & of regret effect, identifying ways in which consumers will feel
Nill, 2001; Kim & Karpova, 2009; Tom et al., 1998), or reasons as to why embarrassed after other consumer discover they own a counterfeit
consumers purchase counterfeits (Wilcox et al., 2009). Existing studies will be an effective strategy to reduce the demand for counterfeit
have found that consumers prefer conspicuous counterfeits (counterfeits goods, especially for consumers who have an independent self-view
that have a visible display of a logo) because they provide consumers or are less conscious of their social face.
with the same aspirational associations as the authentic product
(Wilcox et al., 2009). The results of these studies suggest that consumer
preferences for conspicuous counterfeits can be reduced by prompting 10. Future directions and limitations
anticipation of regret and awareness of the risks associated with
purchasing a counterfeit product. The present research represents one of the rst empirical attempts
Most research on consumers' anticipation of regret has focused on to examine the link between anticipating regret and preferences for
topics and products that are unrelated to counterfeits (Simonson, prototypical conspicuous counterfeits. Therefore, a number of impor-
1992; Keinan & Kivetz, 2008; Shih & Schau, 2011). Therefore, a second tant issues remain for future research studies. One future research
theoretical implication of this research is the introduction of the area is examining other boundary conditions by which consumers ex-
anticipation of regret effect into counterfeit product literature with the perience a decrease in preferences for conspicuous counterfeits. Other
goal of examining the possible psychological mechanisms underlying factors such as social power are also worthy of consideration. The
consumers' counterfeit purchase intentions. This research investigated broad cultural context also raises questions about the inuence of antic-
why anticipation of regret reduces consumers' desire for conspicuous ipating regret. Given today's global mobility, the same consumer may
counterfeits. The ndings provide a new perspective on the anticipation behave differently in different cultural contexts. Future research should
of regret effect, namely, consumers' perception of perceived risk also study real-world counterfeit purchasing behavior by examining
mediates the anticipation of regret effect (Study 2). In other words, whether prompting consumers' anticipation of regret decreases their
because consumers associate risk with purchasing counterfeit preference for conspicuous counterfeits. A potential moderator for the
products, they automatically begin to anticipate that their purchase present ndings may be whether consumers have experience with pur-
of a counterfeit product will result in future regret (e.g., other con- chasing counterfeit products.
sumers discover that their product is a counterfeit, or the counterfeit In conclusion, this research began by noting that although coun-
is of inferior performance, reliability, and durability). Providing this per- terfeit consumption and anticipating regret are pervasive, the effect
spective advances the literature's understanding of how to dissuade of anticipating regret on preferences for counterfeits is not widely
consumers from purchasing counterfeit products, especially conspicu- understood. This research has not only provided an initial effort to
ous counterfeits, through the anticipation of regret effect. In addition, answer this question, but has also introduced a new theoretical
this research also uncovered two boundary conditions of the anticipa- mechanism that underlies anticipating regret within the context of
tion of regret effect. More specically, evidence indicated that the counterfeit literature.

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012
J. Chen et al. / Journal of Business Research xxx (2014) xxxxxx 9

References Leisen, B., & Nill, A. (2001). Combating product counterfeiting: An investigation into the
likely effectiveness of a demand-oriented approach. American Marketing Association.
Aaker, J. L., & Lee, A. Y. (2001). I seek pleasures and we avoid pains: The role of self- (pp. 271277).
regulatory goals in information processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1982). Regret theory: An alternative theory of rational choice
Research, 28, 3349. under uncertainty. The Economic Journal, 92(368), 805824.
Albers-Miller, N. D. (1999). Consumer misbehavior: Why people buy illicit goods. Journal of McConnell, A.R., Niedermeier, K. E., Leibold, J. M., El-Alayli, A. G., Chin, P. P., & Kuiper,
Consumer Marketing, 16(3), 273287, http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/07363769910271504. N. M. (2000). What if I nd it cheaper someplace else?: Role of prefactual think-
Ang, S. H., Cheng, P.S., Lim, E. A.C., & Tambyah, S. K. (2001). Spot the difference: Consumer ing and anticipated regret in consumer behavior. Psychology & Marketing, 17(4),
responses towards counterfeits. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 18(3), 219235, http:// 281298.
dx.doi.org/10.1108/07363760110392967. Nordgren, L. F., Van Der Pligt, J., & Van Harreveld, F. (2007). Unpacking perceived control
BASCAP (2009). Research report on consumer attitudes and perceptions on counterfeiting in risk perception: The mediating role of anticipated regret. Journal of Behavioral
and piracy, 1110. Decision Making, 544(20), 533544, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.
Bell, D. (1982). Regret in decision making under uncertainty. Operations Research, 30(5), O'Cass, A., & McEwen, H. (2004). Exploring consumer status and conspicuous consumption.
961981. Journal of Consumer Behavior, 4(1), 2539, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cb.155.
Brehm, J. W., & Wicklund, R. A. (1970). Regret and dissonance reduction as a function of Phau, I., & Prendergast, G. (2000). Consuming luxury brands: The relevance of the rarity
postdecision salience of dissonant information. Journal of Personality and Social principle. The Journal of Brand Management, 8(2), 122138.
Psychology, 14(1), 17. Piron, F. (2000). Consumers' perceptions of the country-of-origin effect on purchasing in-
Cheung, W. -L., & Prendergast, G. (2006). Buyers' perceptions of pirated products in China. tentions of (in)conspicuous products. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 17(4), 308321,
Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 24(5), 446462, http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/07363760010335330.
02634500610682854. Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual Thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 133148.
Ehrlich, B. (1985). Private sector combats products counterfeiting. The Loy (pp. 8). LA Int'l Shih, E., & Schau, H. J. (2011). To justify or not to justify: The role of anticipated regret on
& Comp. LJ. consumers' decisions to upgrade technological innovations. Journal of Retailing, 87(2),
Furnham, A., & Valgeirsson, H. (2007). The effect of life values and materialism on buying 242251, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jretai.2011.01.006.
counterfeit products. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 36(5), 677685, http://dx.doi. Simonson, I. (1992). The inuence of anticipating regret and responsibility on purchase
org/10.1016/j.socec.2007.01.004. decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 105118.
Gentry, J., Putrevu, S., & Shultz, C. (2006). The effects of counterfeiting on consumer Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2009). Faking it: Personality and
search. Journal of Consumer Research, 256, 245256, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cb. individual difference predictors of willingness to buy counterfeit goods. The Journal
Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., Strathman, A., Armor, D., Hetts, J., & Ahn, M. (1995). With an of Socio-Economics, 38(5), 820825, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2009.03.014.
eye toward the future: The impact of counterfactual thinking on affect, attitudes, and Tom, G., Garibaldi, B., Zeng, Y., & Pilcher, J. (1998). Consumer demand for counterfeit
behavior. In N. J. Roese, & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology goods. Psychology & Marketing, 15, 405421.
of counterfactual thinking (pp. 283304). Truong, Y., Simmons, G., McColl, R., & Kitchen, P. J. (2008). Status and conspicuousness
Han, Y. J., Nunes, J. C., & Drze, X. (2010). Signaling status with luxury goods: The role of are they related? strategic marketing implications for luxury brands. Journal of
brand prominence. The Journal of Marketing, 74, 1530. Strategic Marketing, 16(3), 189203, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09652540802117124.
Han, J. -M., Suk, H. -J., & Chung, K. -W. (2008). The inuence of logo exposure in purchasing Vigneron, F., & Johnson, L. W. (1999). A review and a conceptual framework of prestige-
counterfeit luxury goods focusing on consumer values. International DMI Education seeking consumer behavior. Academy of Marketing Science Review, 1999(1), 115.
Conference Design Thinking: New Challenges for Designers, Managers and Organizations, von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1947). Theory of games and economic behavior.
Design Management Institute (DMI). (2nd ed.). Princeton University Press.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: Wang, C. L., & Chen, Z. X. (2004). Consumer ethnocentrism and willingness to buy domestic
A regression-based approach. Guilford Press. products in a developing country setting: Testing moderating effects. Journal of
Hetts, J. J., & Boninger, D. S. (2000). The inuence of anticipated counterfactual regret on Consumer Marketing, 21(6), 391400, http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/07363760410558663.
behavior. Psychology & Marketing, 17, 345368. Wilcox, K., Kim, H. M., & Sen, S. (2009). Why do consumers buy counterfeit luxury
Keinan, A., & Kivetz, R. (2008). Remedying hyperopia: The effects of self-control regret on brands? Journal of Marketing Research, 46, 247259.
consumer behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 45, 676689. Yoo, B., & Lee, S. -H. (2009). Buy genuine luxury fashion products or counterfeits?
Kim, H., & Karpova, E. (2009). Consumer attitudes toward fashion counterfeits: Applica- Advances in Consumer Research, 36, 280286.
tion of the theory of planned behavior. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 28(2), Zhang, X. -A., Cao, Q., & Grigoriou, N. (2011). Consciousness of social face: The develop-
7994, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0887302X09332513. ment and validation of a scale measuring desire to gain face versus fear of losing
Lai, K. K. -Y., & Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1999). Brand imitation: Do the Chinese have different face. Journal of Social Psychology, 151(2), 129149, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
views? Asia Pacic Journal of Management, 16, 179192. 00224540903366669.

Please cite this article as: Chen, J., et al., Anticipating regret and consumers' preferences for counterfeit luxury products, Journal of Business
Research (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.012

You might also like