You are on page 1of 25

Academy ol Managemenl Review, 1990, Vol.

15, No, 1, 113-136,

Cross-Cultural Training Effectiveness:


A Review and a
Theoretical Framework for
Future Research
J. STEWART BLACK
Dartmouth College
MARK MENDENHALL
University of Tennessee, Chattanooga
Increased internationalization in the economic, political, and social
arenas has led to greater interpersonal cross-cultural contact. Be-
cause much of this contact has not been successful, cross-cultural
training has been proposed by many scholars as a means of facili-
tating more effective interaction. A review of the cross-cultural train-
ing literature is presented, and it is determined that cross-cultural
training in general is effective. The article also offers a theoretical
framework based on social learning theory for understanding past
research and for guiding future research; this is important because in
this context variables seem to operate differently in international ver-
sus domestic areas.

The field of international management has tive of the newer subfield of cross-cultural inter-
been observed to be in a nascent, preparadigm national human resource management (Adler,
state of development (Adler, 1983; Beaty & Men- 1983; Beaty & Mendenhall, 1989). Adler (1983)
denhall, 1989; Kyi, 1988; Roberts, 1970; Roberts & found that from 1971 to 1980 only one percent of
Boyacigiller, 1984; Schollhammer, 1975). Kyi the 11,000 articles published in 24 management
(1988) perhaps best summarized these findings journals focused on cross-cultural work interac-
when he stated, "The paucity of papers in the tion. Of the major international business jour-
hypothetico-deductive category . . . is related to nals from 1984 to 1988, only 9 percent of the
the stage of the development of the field and the articles were devoted to international human re-
nature of comparative analysis. At this stage, source management issues, and in the Acad-
there are no deductively developed theories in emy of Management Review, for the same pe-
[international] management and most so-called riod, only one and one half percent of the arti-
'theories' are experienced-based hunches or cles dealt with international human resource
empirical generalizations. Well-integrated de- management issues; however, none were at-
ductive theories with a central core concept, tempts at theory building (Beaty & Mendenhall,
such as 'market and rationality' in economic the- 1989).
ory, have not appeared yet" (p. 209). This is particularly unfortunate because in-
This preparadigm state is especially descrip- creased internationalization in the economic,

113
political, and social arenas has led to a greater ness organizations is not very widespread. Stud-
frequency of, and depth in, cross-cultural inter- ies have found that only 30 percent of managers
actions (contacts between two or more people who are sent on expatriate assignments (one to
from different cultural backgrounds). These in- five years) receive cross-cultural training before
teractions occur in a variety of work-related sit- their departure (Baker 8f Ivancevich, 1971;
uations, including short-term business trips to Black, 1988; Runzheimer Executive Report, 1984;
foreign countries, long-term overseas assign- Tung, 1981). Various reasons have been cited by
ments, and even work in one organization. business organizations for the low use of cross-
However, work-related cross-cultural interac- cultural training, and the most prevalent of
tions are not always successful. For example, these is that such training is not thought to be
studies have found that between 16 and 40 per- effective (Baker & Ivancevich, 1971; Mendenhall
cent of all expatriate managers who are given & Oddou, 1985; Schwind, 1985; Tung, 1981;
foreign assignments end these assignments Zeira, 1975); thus, top management sees no
early because of their poor performance or their need for the training (Runzheimer Executive Re-
inabflity to adjust to the foreign environment port, 1984) and is unwilling to support it, finan-
(Baker & Ivancevich, 1971; Black, 1988; Dunbar cially or otherwise. Essentially, American top
& Ehrlich, 1986; Tung, 1981), and as high as 50 managers believe that a good manager in New
percent of those who do not return early function York or Los Angeles will be effective in Hong
at a low level of effectiveness (Copeland & Kong or Tokyo (Miller, 1973). 'This is illustrated
Griggs, 1985). Other studies have found that ne- not only in the lack of training provided but also
gotiations between businessmen of different cul- in the use of the domestic track record as the
tures often fail because of problems related to primary criterion for selecting candidates for
cross-cultural diflerences (Adler, 1986; Black, overseas assignments (Miller, 1973). Such a cul-
1987; Graham, 1985; Tung, 1984). Unsuccessful turally insensitive perspective seems to be an
cross-cultural interactions become even more important reason for many faulty international
important when the costs of failure are high, and human resource practices and the high expatri-
they often are. For example, studies have esti- ate failure rates (Adler, 1986; Black, 1988; Baker
mated that the cost of a failed expatriate assign- & Ivancevich, 1971; Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985;
ment is $50,000 to $150,000 (Copeland & Griggs, Ronen, 1986; Tung, 1982). It would seem, then,
1985; Harris & Moran, 1979; Misa & Fabricatore, that an attempt to comprehensively review the
1979). For a firm that has hundreds of expatriate empirical literature on cross-cultural training in
employees worldwide, such costs can easily an effort to examine its effectiveness or ineffec-
reach into the tens of millions of dollars. In fact, tiveness and an attempt to advance a theoreti-
Copeland and Griggs (1985) have estimated that cal grounding for valid cross-cultural training
the direct costs to U.S. firms of failed expatriate program development and evaluation is espe-
assignments is over $2 billion a year, and this cially important.
does not include unmeasured losses such as Thus, this article has two purposes. The flrst is
damaged corporate reputations or lost business to review the extant empirical literature on the
opportunities. effectiveness of cross-cultural training. Three
Cross-cultural training (CCT) has long been reasons indicate that this is an appropriate and
advocated as a means of facilitating effective timely effort: (a) the opportunities for and the ne-
cross-cultural interactions (Brislin, 1981; Landis cessity of engaging in cross-cultural interaction
& Brislin, 1983; Bochner, 1982; Harris & Moran, are widespread, (b) the costs of unsuccessful in-
1979; Mendenhall & Oddou, 1986; Tung, 1981). teractions can be substantial, and (c) many flrms
Despite the normative arguments for the use of seem to believe that cross-cultural training is not
cross-cultural training, its use in American busi- effective and that no empirical evidence exists

114
that supports the efficacy of cross-cultural train- some scholars have argued (Adler, 1983; Beaty
ing. The lack of a systematic review of the liter- 8f Mendenhall, 1989; Kyi, 1988; Roberis, 1970;
ature may have contributed to conflicting views Roberts & Boyacigiller, 1984; Schollhammer,
by corporate leaders and academics about 1975), international management in general is
whether cross-cultural training facilitates more in a preparadigmatic, pretheoretical stage of
effective cross-cultural interactions. Most corpo- development, then it is not surprising that such
rate leaders do not provide cross-cultural train- subareas as cross-cultural training also lack
ing for employees and cite doubt of the training's theories and models.
effectiveness as the reason. Academics, in con- In order to delineate the efficacy of cross-
trast, who advocate the necessity of cross- cultural training programs, a review of the ex-
cultural training tout its importance but do not tant literature was performed across a variety of
offer empirical evidence to support their asser- disciplines in the social sciences. Much writing
tions. A comprehensive review of the literature exists on the value of utilizing cross-cultural
would aid in the resolution of the debate. training programs for potential expatriates and
The second purpose is to propose a theoretical others who work in a multicultural milieu; how-
framework for understanding the literature and ever, only studies that empirically evaluated the
guiding future research. This latter aim seems efiectiveness of cross-cultural training programs
particularly important because the empirical as were included. A computer-aided, interdiscipli-
well as prescriptive work on cross-cultural train- nary literature search was used for a number of
ing to date has been criticized for lacking a clear data bases, and after eliminating nonempirical
theoretical base (Adler, 1983; Brislin, 1981; Boch- studies, 29 studies were included in the formal
ner, 1982; Landis, Brislin, & Hulgus, 1985). review process.
Because most studies used a variety of train-
Review of the Literature ing methods and because few studies systemat-
ically tried to compare the efficacy of training
Although no study has attempted a compre- methods, training will be treated herein as a
hensive review, two reviews have examined the generic activity, and the focus will be to ascer-
cultural assimilator type of training and its effec- tain its overall impact on the dependent vari-
tiveness (Fiedler, Mitchell, & Triandis, 1971; ables used to measure its effectiveness. The re-
Mitchefl, Dosseti, Fiedler, & Triandis, 1972), but lationship between training and each of the de-
neither examined other approaches, nor did pendent variables was coded as (a) not
they offer a theoretical framework. Brislin and addressed in the study (lefi blank), (b) positive
Pedersen (1976) conducted a comprehensive re- (recorded as a -t- ), (c) nonsignificant (recorded
view of cross-cultural training approaches, but as 0), and (d) negative (recorded as ). A sum-
they focused on developing a typology of train- mary of the studies reviewed is given in Table 1.
ing methods. Other somewhat-related reviews From the review of the literature, three pri-
include the nature of cross-cultural adjustment mary dependent variables were commonly
(Church, 1982; Mendenhafl & Oddou, 1985; used as indicators of training efiectiveness (a)
Stening, 1979), the impact of cross-cultural as- cross-cultural skifl development, (b) adjustment,
signments on expatriates' careers (Mendenhall, and (c) performance (see Table 1). Each of these
Dunbar, & Oddou, 1987), and the degree to variables and its relationship to training effec-
which cross-cultural training is utilized (Baker & tiveness is discussed next.
Ivancevitch, 1971; Mendenhall et al., 1987;
Tung, 1981). Athough related, none directly at- Cross-Cultural Skill Development
tempts a comprehensive review of the empirical Living and working overseas or within a mul-
literature or offers a theoretical framework. If, as ticultural context in one's home country requires

115
T a b l e 1 -^- - v = - , . : ; ; . : , . , . . - . ,
Review Summary of Cioss-Cultuial Training Effectiveness

Variables oi Training Effectiveness


Subjects b Cross-Cultural Skills Adjustment Perionnance
Study Sample Size Seli Relationship Perceptual Mise'
Befus(1988) Missionaries F,C
n = 64
Chemers (1969) Managers, + /Q F,T
Teachers n = 48
Chemers et al. Students n = 42 -H/O L,C,T
(1966)
Dege(1981) Teaching -1- F
assistants n = 30
Earley(1987) Managers n = 80 + F,C,T,LG
Fernandez Nurses n = 31 L
(1986)
Gudykunst et al. Navy personnel -I- F,C,T,LG
(1977) n = 530
Katz(1977) Students n = 24 -1- L,C,LG
Landis et a l Army officers F,C
(1985) n = 923
Landis et al. Students n = 45 L,T,C
(1985)
Landis et al. Army officers F,C
(1976) n = 170
Lefley (1986) Mental health -1- F,LG
agencies n = 97
Lefley (1985) Psychologists -1- F,LG
n = 122
McDaniel et al. Teachers n = 180 -1- F,LG
(1988)
McGroarty Teachers of F
(1984) English as a
second language
n = 29
Mitchell et al. Peace corps F,C
(1972) volunteers n = 23
Mitchell & Foa Students n = 64 0 L,C
(1969)
Ncryar et al. Students n = 72 -{- L,T
(1968)
Neimeyer et al. Students n = 20 L
(1986)
Newbold et al. Medical interns F,LG
(1982) n =6
O'Brien & Nurses n = 74 L,C,LG
Plooij (1976)
O'Brien et al. Public health F,T,LG
(1970) workers n = 265
PSI (1982) Peace corps F
teachers n = 79

116
Table 1Continued
Review Summary of Cross-Cultural Training Effectiveness

Variables of Training Effectiveness

Subjects & Cross-Cultural Skills Adjustment Performance


Study Sample Size Self Relationship Perceptual Mise*
Randolph et al. Students -1- L,C
(1977) n = 35
Salisbury Eskimo students -1- F,C,LG
(1971) n = 53
Steinkalk College F,C
&Taft administrators
(1979) n = 43
Vigushin College staff F
(1982) n = 126
Weldon et al. Students 0 L,C
(1975) n = 128
Worchel & Mitchell Military officers F,C,T,LG
(1972) n = 110
* The code for the miscellaneous column is: L = laboratory study, F = field study, C = control group was used. T = Training
programs were compared for relative effectiveness. LG = longitudinal study.

an individual to use interaction skills that tran- amined the relationship between cross-cultural
scend those that are effective when dealing with training and the development of greater feelings
others from one's immediate in-group (Adler, of well-being and self-confidence (Dege, 1981;
1986; Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985; Torbiorn, Fernandez, 1986; McGoarty, 1984; Neimeyer,
1982). Thus, the development of cross-cultural Fukuyama, Bingham, Hall, & Mussenden, 1986;
skills in trainees has been a major objective in O'Brien & Plooij, 1976; O'Brien, Fiedler, &
the evaluation studies reviewed here. The link Hewett, 1970; PSI Associates, 1982; Randolph,
between possessing these skills and subsequent Landis, & Tzeng, 1977; Salisbury, 1971; Worchel
success in overseas assignments has been dem- & Mitchell, 1972), and all 10 found a positive re-
onstrated elsewhere (for reviews see Brein &i lationship. In examining the validity of the find-
David, 1971; Church, 1982; Mendenhall & ings of these studies, two points should be noted.
Oddou, 1985). The purpose here is to ascertain First, all 10 studies relied on self-repori mea-
whether cross-cultural training can indeed fos- sures of variables related to the self dimension
ter the development of such skills in trainees. (e.g., O'Brien & Plooij, 1976). This approach may
The skills needed to be successful in a new have inflated the relationship between training
culture can be subsumed under three dimen- and the self dimension dependent variables be-
sions: skills related to the maintenance of self cause of common method variance problems.
(mental health, psychological well-being, stress However, studies by Worchel and Mitchell
reduction, feelings of self-confidence), skills re- (1972), Salisbury (1971), and O'Brien and Plooij
lated to the fostering of relationships with host (1976) included control groups and found signif-
nationals, and cognitive skills that promote a icant differences in pre- and posttest measures
correct perception of the host environment and of self-confidence between the experimental
its social systems (Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985). and control-group subjects. Second, despite the
The self dimension. O the 29 studies, 10 ex- fact that studies that utilized control groups

117
found significant relationships between training another culture, and all 16 studies found a pos-
and self-confidence, it is possible that subjects itive relationship (Chemers, 1969; Chemers et
assigned to the experimental groups through al., 1966; Earley, 1987; Katz, 1977; Landis, Bris-
common method measures may have inflated lin, HulgusetaL, 1985; Landis, Brislin, Swanner
the relationship because of Hawthorne effects of et a l , 1985; Landis et al., 1976; Lefley, 1985; Mc-
knowing they were in an experimental group. Groarty, 1984; Nayar et al., 1968; O'Brien &
The Relationship Dimension. Nineteen of the Plooij, 1976; Randolph et al., 1977; Steinkalk &
29 studies examined the relationship between Taft, 1979; Vigushin, 1982; Weldon et al., 1975;
cross-cultural training and skills related to fos- Worchel & Mitchell, 1972). Again, more than
tering relationships with host nationals (Chem- half the studies examining the perception di-
ers, 1969; Chemers, Lekhyananda, Fiedler, & mension used control groups (Chemers, 1969;
Stoulurow, 1966; Dege, 1981; Earley, 1987; Earley, 1987; Katz, 1977; Landis, Brislin, & Hul-
Gudykunst, Hammer, & Wiseman, 1977; Katz, gus, 1985; Landis, Brislin, Swanner et a l , 1985;
1977; Landis, Brislin, Swanner, Tseng, & Thom- Landis et al., 1976; O'Brien & Plooij, 1976;
as, 1985; Landis, Day, McGrew, Thomas, & Steinkalk & Taft, 1979; Randolph et al., 1977^
Miller, 1976; Lefley, 1985; McGroarty, 1984; Weldon et al., 1975; Worchel & Mitchell, 1972),
Mitchell & Foa, 1969; Mitchell et al., 1972; Nayar, and Earley (1987), O'Brien and Plooij (1976), and
Touzard, & Summers, 1986; Newbold, Carroll, Worchel and Mitchell (1972) were longitudinal in
Hartman, Mitchell, & Story, 1982; PSl Associ- nature. This lends some support to the conclu-
ates, 1982; Randolph et al., 1977; Salisbury, sion that cross-cultural training has a positive
1971; Weldon, Carlston, Rissman, Slobodin, & impact on individuals' correctly perceiving other
Triandis, 1975; Worchel & Mitchell, 1972). Each cultures and members of those cultures. Addi-
of these 19 studies found a significant relation- tionally, Weldon et a l (1975), Landis, Brislin, &
ship between cross-cultural training and self- Hulgus (1985), Randolph et al. (1977), Vigushin
reported measures of relationship skills. (1982), and Worchel and Mitchell (1972) all sup-
More than half of the studies that examined ported this conclusion using objective measures
the effectiveness of cross-cultural training on re- of perception (the comparison of perception or
lational skills used control groups (Chemers et knowledge of the subjects with that of the host
al., 1966; Earley, 1987; Gudykunst et al., 1977; national group). This independent measure of
Landis, Brislin, Swanner et a l , 1985; Landis et the perception dimension overcomes some of
al., 1976; Mitchell et al., 1972; Mitchell & Foa, the limitations related to common method vari-
1969; Randolph et al., 1977; Salisbury, 1971; ance already discussed concerning the self-
Weldon et a l , 1975; Worchel & Mitchell, 1972). In reported measures of the self and relational di-
addition, the studies of Earley (1987), Salisbury mensions.
(1971), and Worchel and Mitchell (1972) were
longitudinal in nature. All of these studies found Adjustment
a significant relationship, and the longitudinal The main argument for using cross-cultural
studies found the relationship to persist over training is that it allows individuals to more rap-
time. However, relational skills were measured idly adjust to the new culture and, therefore, to
through self-reports, which opens these studies be more effective in their new roles. Adjusting to
and their findings to the same limitations and a new culture involves the gradual develop-
issues that concerned the self dimension. ment of familiarity, comfort, and proficiency re-
The Perception Dimension. A total of 16 of the garding expected behavior and the values and
29 studies examined the relationship between assumptions inherent in the new culture, all of
cross-cultural training and the development of which are different from the individual's native
appropriate perceptions relative to members of culture (Torbiom, 1982). A total of 9 studies ex-

118
amined the relationship between cross-cultural and performance (Chemers et al., 1966; Mitchell
training and adjustment (Befus, 1988; Earley, & Foa, 1969; Weldon et al., 1975). Mitchell et al.
1987;Gudykunstetal., 1977; Mitchell et aL, 1972; (1972), in their review of culture assimilator train-
Newbold et al., 1982; O'Brien, Fiedler, & Hewett, ing, argued that in general lab studies do not
1970; Salisbury, 1971; Steinkalk & Taft, 1979; allow sufflcient time for the effects of cross-
Worchel & Mitchell, 1972). All 9 studies found a cultural training to be manifested in perfor-
positive relationship between cross-cultural mance. However, one field study found signifi-
training and adjustment. Seven of these nine cant and nonsignificant relationships between
studies used control groups (Befus, 1988; Earley, cross-cultural training and various measures of
1987;Gudykunstetal., 1977; Mitchell et al., 1972; performance (Chemers, 1969). It should be noted
Salisbury, 1971; Steinkalk & Taft, 1979; Worchel that none of the studies found a significant neg-
& Mitchell, 1972), and Earley (1987), Salisbury ative relationship between cross-cultural train-
(1971), and Worchel and Mitchell (1972) were ing and performance. Studies by Earley (1987),
longitudinal in nature. Earley (1987) and Mitch- Worchel and Mitchell (1972), Lefley (1986), and
ell et al. (1972), in addition to using control O'Brien et al. (1970) afl used independent mea-
groups and longitudinal designs, also used in- sures of performance, and all found a significant
dependent measures of adjustment and found a and positive relationship between training and
significant relationship between cross-cultural performance.
training and adjustment; this use of indepen- Several concluding remarks can be made
dent measures of adjustment increases the con- concerning the effectiveness of cross-cultural
fldence one can place in their findings that training. First, on average about one half of the
cross-cultural training has a positive impact on studies examining each dependent variable
cross-cultural adjustment. used control groups, which allows for some con-
fidence to be placed in the consistency of the
Performance findings. Only 6 of the 29 studies included both
Institutions that send individuals overseas or control groups and a longitudinal design; there-
into a multicultural work setting expect those in- fore, even though it does seem that cross-
dividuals to perform, whether that involves cultural training has a long-term effect on all the
building a dam, managing a factory, negotiat- dependent variables examined, it is clear that
ing a contract, teaching children to read, or con- more longitudinal studies are needed that in-
verting host-nationals to a new religion. Thus, clude rigorous research designs, before defini-
the relationship of cross-cultural training to per- tive conclusions about the impact of training
formance is a critical concern: Does such train- over time can be made. Second, many of the
ing aid work performance in the cross-cultural studies of skill-related dependent variables did
setting, or are its effects negligible? Fifteen of the not use independent measures of dependent
29 studies examined the relationship between variables and, thus, common method variance
cross-cultural training and performance, and 11 limitations must be kept in mind concerning the
of the 15 studies found a significant relationship apparent positive relationship between training
between cross-cultural training and perfor- and skill development. Third, it is important to
mance (Dege, 1981; Earley, 1987; Katz, 1977; Le- note that those studies that included rigorous re-
fley, 1985, 1986; McDaniel, McDaniel, & search designs (e.g., control groups, longitudi-
McDaniel, 1988; Nayar et al., 1968; O'Brien & nal designs, independent measures), such as
Plooij, 1976; O'Brien et aL, 1970; Salisbury, 1971; Earley (1987), Salisbury (1971), and Worchel and
Randolph et al., 1977; Worchel & Mitchefl, 1972). Mitchell (1972), found support for a positive rela-
Three lab studies found generally nonsignificant tionship between cross-cultural training and the
relationships between cross-cultural training following dependent variables: cross-cultural

119
skill development, cross-cultural adjustment, Shanmugam, 1972). If people who have differ-
and performance in a cross-cultural setting. ent culturally based behaviors and beliefs must
Thus, the empirical literature gives guarded interact, difficulties arise because faulty attribu-
support to the proposition that cross-cultural tions are made about the motives and meanings
training has a positive impact on cross-cultural of the others' behaviors since the attributions are
effectiveness. based on the attributor's own cultural norms
and worldview (Bochner, 1982).
Need for a Theoretical Framework Cross-cultural training enables the individual
to learn both content and skills that will facilitate
This review indicates that, in general, cross- effective cross-cultural interaction by reducing
cultural training seems to have a positive impact misunderstandings and inappropriate behav-
on skill development, adjustment, and perfor- iors. If this is accepted as the major objective of
mance; however, the lack of a theoretical frame- cross-cultural training, it becomes necessary to
work leaves unanswered the question about understand how people learn to appropriately
why cross-cultural training is effective. Thus, in interact with others and how they use that
order to systematically understand the condi- knowledge for efiective interactions.
tions that cause cross-cultural training to be ef- In an effori to focus learning theory on the
fective or ineffective a theoretical framework, more specific issue of training, Noe (1986) pro-
which encompasses cross-cultural training and posed a model of how training facilitates perfor-
its relation to cross-cultural interaction, is mance. This suggested that two aspects of an
needed. individual's motivation had an important impact
Any attempt to move toward a theoretical un- on the effectiveness of training. Noe argued that
derstanding of the efiectiveness of cross-cultural an individual's motivation to learn and his or her
training must begin with a basic understanding motivation to transfer what was learned into ac-
of cross-cultural interaction. Although it is be- tion were critical elements in the relationship of
yond the scope of this article to present a com- training and performance. However, Noe did
prehensive review of the theoretical literature not delineate how individuals actually learn or
on cross-cultural interaction, several points can how they transfer that learning to behaviors. In
be summarized based on other, more detailed order to understand the relationship between
reviews (Brislin, 1981; Bochner, 1982). training and performance, these aspects of the
Most discussions of cross-cultural interaction model require additional attention.
begin with a definition of culture. In their classic Cognitive and behavioral theorists have long
review of culture, Kluckhohn and Kroeberg competed to explain how individuals learn and
(1952) cited over 160 definitions of culture. Based how they use this knowledge (for reviews on
on their exhaustive review, Kluckhohn and cognitive and behavioral learning theories and
Kroeberg (1952) concluded that culture consists the debate between them see Bochner, 1982;
of patterns of behaviors that are acquired and Hilgard & Bower, 1975; Swenson, 1980). Accord-
transmitted by symbols over time, which be- ing to the cognitive theories of learning, learn-
come generally shared within a group and are ing takes place through the mental processing of
communicated to new members of the group in information and the determination of subse-
order to serve as a cognitive guide or blueprint quent behavior. Behavioral theories argue that
for future actions. Thus, cross-cultural interac- learning is determined by behaviors and expe-
tions bring people together who have different rienced consequences. In response to this 20-
patterns of behaving and believing and who year debate Latham and Saari (1979) noted, "To
have different cognitive blueprints for interpret- show that behavior is determined only by cog-
ing the world (Triandis, Vassifiou, Tanaka, & nitions, one would have to find a control group

120
consisting of subjects who cannot think. Simi- they will behave before an actual situation.
larly, to prove empirical suppori for the argu- Also, it is argued that individuals learn from ex-
ment that behavior is due to environmental con- perience and that the experienced conse-
sequences alone, one would have the impossi- quences of their behavior shape what they learn
ble task of forming a control group for which as wefl as their future behavior. As described by
there was no environment" (p. 240). Bandura (1977), SLT has four central elements:
Social learning theory (SLT) has been advo- attention, retention, reproduction, and incen-
cated as a synthesis of the cognitive and behav- tives.
ioral learning theories (Bandura, 1977; Hilgard Attention. Before behavior can be modeled,
& Bower, 1975). Davis and Luthans (1980), who the subject must notice it. Several factors influ-
compared SLT with other theories of behavior, ence the attention process of the subject, includ-
argued that it is the most useful in understand- ing: (a) the status of the model, (b) the attractive-
ing organizational behavior. Additionally, gen- ness of the model, (c) the similarity of the model,
eral reviews of learning in particular have ar- (d) the repeated availability of the model, and (e)
gued for the superiority of SLT (Hilgard & Bower, past reinforcement for paying attention to the
1975; Swenson, 1980). For example, Swenson model (actual or vicarious).
(1980) stated that SLT was viewed as a consen- fefenion. Retention is the process by which
sus position on most aspects of learning. SLT not the modeled behavior becomes encoded as a
only integrates cognitive and behavioral theo- memory. Two representational systems are in-
ries, it also encompasses the motivational as- volved in this process. The imaginai system is
pects stressed by Noe (1986) within the concept of utilized during exposure to the model. At this
self-efficacy, and it covers the issues of how in- time the subject associates sequences of corre-
dividuals both learn and utilize what they learn sponding sensory images with the physical con-
during a training situation, aspects that Noe did tiguity of the model. These images are stored as
not emphasize. In addition, SLT is becoming the cognitive maps, which can guide the observer
dominant framework in U.S. management train- when he or she tries to imitate the behavior. In
ing (Latham & Saari, 1979; Manz 8f Sims, 1981). the second system, a verbal system, the coded
These make for compelling reasons to use SLT information is abbreviated into verbal systems,
as the theoretical framework for understanding and groups of constituent patterns of behavior
cross-cultural learning and training. However, are integrated into larger units. It should be
greater depth and breadth of novel behaviors noted that both the repeated modeling of a be-
that trainees must learn during cross-cultural havior and the repeated cognitive rehearsal of
training present a challenge to SLT's explana- the modeled behavior help to secure the reten-
tion of the success or failure of cross-cultural tion process.
training. Consequently, it is important to see if fleproduciion. The third major component in-
the domestic and the foreign contexts can both volves the translating of flie symbolic represen-
be explained through SLT. tations into actions. As individuals try to imitate
the modeled behavior, they check their perfor-
Social Learning Theory: An Overview mance against their memory of what was mod-
eled. Motoric reproduction of the modeled be-
According to SLT, learning is affected by both havior can, of course, be inhibited by physical
observation and experience (Bandura, 1977). A differences between the model and the person
central premise is that individuals use symbols imitating the model, how well the model is ob-
to engage in anticipatory action, that is, they served, and how well the modeled behavior is
anticipate actions and their associated conse- retained.
quences. This enables people to determine how Incentives and the Motivational Processes.

121
The fourth element of SLT, incentives, can come expectations, and outcome expectancies influ-
from the environment, from vicarious associa- ence what learned behaviors are acted out.
tion, and from the individual. Each of these can Although a number of empirical findings are
affect several aspects of the learning process. reviewed by Bandura (1977), three are impor-
For example, incentives (a) can affect which tant to summarize because they provide insight
models are observed and how much attention is about fundamental elements in the learning
paid to them, (b) can influence the degree to process. The first finding is that gradual model-
which the modeled behavior is retained and re- ing is more effective than one-shot modeling,
hearsed, and (c) can influence which learned especially if the modeled behaviors are novel.
behaviors are emitted. It is imporiant to note that Gradual modeling involves providing succes-
Bandura (1977) argued on the basis of empirical sive approximations of the final behavior and it
work that incentives play a much larger role m is more effective because (a) observers pay more
influencing what behavior is emitted as op- attention to models and modeled behaviors that
posed to what behavior is learned. He con- are familiar, (b) observers can more easily re-
cluded that individuals learn numerous behav- tain models that are similar to cognitive maps
iors that are not emitted because they are not they already possess, (c) observers have higher
positively rewarded. However, if the reward expectations of efficacy and outcome of behav-
structure is changed, the behaviors are per- iors that are familiar, and (d) observers are more
formed. likely to be able to reproduce familiar behaviors.
In relation to the motivational processes of The second finding is that individuals can learn
learning, Bandura (1977) distinguished between completely through symbolic modeling, that is,
two types of expectancies: efficacy expectations by watching actions and mentally rehearsing
and outcome expectations. Self-efficacy is the them. As previously mentioned, this symbolic
degree to which the individual believes he or learning process can be facilitated by other vari-
she can successfully execute a pariicular behav- ables and by the use of multiple models. Finally,
ior. This expectation is similar to the effort- Bandura found that participative reproduction,
to-performance expectancy proposed by Vroom in general, is more effective than symbolic pro-
(1964). In his review of the literature, Bandura cesses. Participative reproduction simply means
(1977) found that higher levels of self-efficacy led that the observer actually practices (compared
individuals to persist at imitating modeled be- to only cognitively rehearsing) the modeled be-
havior longer and to be more willing to try to havior. The external, and especially the inter-
imitate novel behavior. The sources for increas- nal, feedback processes serve to refine the ob-
ing self-efficacy, in order of importance, include server's ability to reproduce the modeled behav-
past experience ("I've done it or something like it ior at a later time.
before"), vicarious experience ("other people
have done it"), and verbal persuasion ("people
say I can do it"). Social Learning Theory and
Outcome expectations are people's beliefs Cross-Cultural Training
that the execution of ceriain behaviors will lead SLT provides both a theorefical framework for
to desired outcomes. There is a clear similarity understanding past research and a guide for fu-
between this type of expectation and the expect- ture investigation. Because few of the past em-
ancy-of-performance-to-outcome (instrumental- pirical studies of cross-cultural training have at-
ity expectancies) proposed by Vroom (1964). tempted a systematic study of training methods,
Bandura concluded that incentives influence this section treats cross-cultural training in a ge-
what people learn and that incentives, efficacy. neric manner.

122
Social Learning Theory and the literature indicated that cross-cultural train-
Skill Development ing increased trainees' relational skills. Most
cross-cultural training provides models that both
The review of the literature on cross-cultural describe cognitive maps of individuals from the
training and skill development found that cross- other cultures and explain appropriate and in-
cultural training tended to increase various appropriate behaviors for interacting in these
skills that facilitated effective cross-cultural inter- settings. Because trainees have both modeled
action. This section examines how cross-cultural behavior and information, upon which they can
training might achieve this within the theoretical imagine models of behavior, and because they
framework of social learning theory. have formed expectations about the conse-
The Self Dimension. Cross-cultural training quences of certain behaviors, when they actu-
increased trainees' confidence in themselves ally are confronted with the cross-cultural situa-
and their abiliti' to act effectively in a cross- tion they can recall the models and imitate them
cultural setting. According to SLT, as trainees better than individuals who have not been
receive either verbal or visual models of appro- trained. Additionally, even if the models are not
priate and inappropriate behaviors for the im- perfect or not perfectly imitated, the increased
pending cross-cultural interaction, and for asso- self-efficacy that the trained individuals have
ciations between these behaviors and expected gained would lead them to persist longer than
consequences, that trainees rehearse (cogni- nontrained individuals in imitating more novel
tively or behaviorally) these behaviors and con- behaviors in the actual cross-cultural situation.
sequences. Such cognitive maps will increase This, in turn, would generate both external and
the individual's efflcacy and outcome expecta- internal feedback, which would serve to refine
tions because he or she has learned the appro- the modeled behaviors; all of this would help to
priate behaviors (vicariously) and made associ- facilitate the trained individuals' skills at estab-
ations about the expected outcomes. According lishing relationships with individuals from the
to SLT, higher self-efficacy and outcome expec- other culture. As an example, the workers in
tations would have a positive impact on the re- Earley's (1987) study were subjected to a simu-
tention and reproduction learning processes, lation in which they were required to interact
which, in turn, would have a positive impact on with individuals who represented South Kore-
outcome variables such as adjustment and per- ans (the target culture). Through this simulation,
formance. This learning process would explain trainees had the opportunity to see (attend) and
why individuals who received cross-cultural practice (retain) interpersonal interaction skills,
training reported higher levels of confidence which they later recalled and utilized in South
and other self-hygiene factors than those who Korea.
did not (Church, 1982; Mendenhall & Oddou, The Perception Dimension. Most cross-
1985). For example, as the public health workers cultural training includes information about
in the O'Brien et al. (1970) study were trained how people of other cultures view the world as
through culture assimilator exercises, they were well as their cognitive tendencies (e.g., ethno-
presented with verbal models of how to interpret centrism), which cause problems in cross-
the behavior of the people of Honduras and cultural situations. This information enables the
Guatemala and how to behave appropriately. trainee to make cognitive associations between
The information gained gave the volunteers congruent behaviors and the cognitive maps of
confidence that they could now function appro- members of the other culture. Assuming the in-
priately in these two countries and cultures. formation provided to the trainee is correct, and
The Relationship Dimension. The review of the cognitive maps the trainee creates are accu-

123
rate, when the trainee imagines models of be- cess. Simply stated, cross-cultural adjustment
havior in the actual situation, these behaviors involves the knowledge of which behaviors to
will be appropriate and will enable the individ- execute or suppress in given situations and the
ual to be effective in the cross-cultural interac- ability to effectively actualize this understand-
tion. ing. Cross-cultural training can provide models
Training, which provides a means for model- of appropriate and inappropriate behavior in
ing behaviors such as being tolerant of ambigu- general or specific, hypothetical or simulated
ity or reserving judgment about the actions of situations; it can provide information from which
those of the other culture and the consequences trainees can imagine appropriate and inappro-
of such behavior, can serve as an important vi- priate behavior and associated consequences; it
carious learning experience for the trainee and can increase individuals' efficacy and outcome
can result in cognitive behavior/consequence expectations; and it can facilitate symbolic and/
associations as well as efficacy and outcome ex- or participative rehearsals of the modeled be-
pectations. The social learning process would haviors.
result in the same but opposite results concern- All of these results can lead to several impor-
ing the modeling of inappropriate behaviors tant links between cross-cultural training and
such as ethnocentricity. Thus, according to SLT, adjustment. First, by having models of appro-
cross-cultural training leads both to trainees' priate and inappropriate behavior upon which
perceiving they have accurate perceptions of to base behavior, cross-culturally trained indi-
the target culture and to trainees actually hav- viduals would have a greater ability to imitate
ing correct perceptions of the target culture. For the models in actual situations. Also, because
example, some workers in Earley's (1987) study cross-culturally trained individuals would have
also went through an interaction simulation higher efflcacy and outcome expectations, they
called BaFa BaFa, in which two teams tried to would be more likely to imitate and to persist in
trade with each other without knowing the oth- imitating the modeled behaviors that would be-
er's rules of trading. Debriefing sessions illus- gin the feedback process. Additionally, such
trated that without knowing the rules most par- models of appropriate behavior would enable
ticipants made inaccurate attributions, which trainees to better recognize model behavior in
had a dysfunctional effect on their attempts to host nationals, which would increase the
trade. Thus, trainees were able to make antici- trained individuals' retention of the models and
patory behavioral adjustments by determining their ability to reproduce the behaviors. Also,
to reserve judgment about the actions of South trained individuals would have higher levels of
Koreans until they had more information, self-efflcacy, which would lead them to be more
which, subsequently, had a positive effect on persistent in attempting to execute new behav-
their adjustment in South Korea. iors. This persistence would facilitate the learn-
ing process in the host culture, and it also would
Social Learning Theory and Adjustment
aid in the individual's understanding of, and
Adjustment to a cross-cultural situation gener- ability to execute, appropriate behavior.
ally has been viewed as the process and end As an example. Navy personnel in the study
state of being familiar and comfortable while in- by Gudykunst et al. (1977) were trained by two
teracting in the host culture. Thus, adjustment is methods (factual briefing and integrated
most relevant when an individual will live in method) in preparation for living in Japan. The
another culture for some period of time. Social integrated, multimethod training included both
learning theory also provides a framework for symbolic and participative modeling. This sym-
understanding why various skills are important bolic modeling (e.g., lectures on Japanese cul-
for adjustment and how they facilitate this pro- ture and culture shock) enabled trainees to use

124
their imaginai systems to create anticipatory as- the cultural dimensions of the setiing, then cross-
sociations between appropriate and inappropri- cultural training would have an impact on per-
ate behaviors in the Japanese culture and to un- formance in a number of ways.
derstand likely positive and negative conse- First, based on the previous discussion, SLT
quences. Pariicipative modeling (e.g., contrast provides a means of understanding why cross-
American role play, BaFa BaFa simulation, and cultural training has an impact on the acquisi-
field trips) allowed trainees to utilize both verbal tion of various skills and on adjustment to the
and imaginai systems, to have repeated expo- culture, as well as how certain skills may facili-
sure to the models of desirable behavior, and to tate adjustment. If adjustment is viewed as the
practice those behaviors. Based on SLT, re- person's ability to know and execute appropri-
peated exposure would increase attention pro- ate behaviors relative to the cross-cultural situ-
cesses, which, in turn, would have a positive ation, then to the degree that performance is tied
effect on the retention process. Additionally, to culturally determined behavior, cross-cultural
given the novelty of the Japanese culture (rela- training would also have a positive impact on
tive to American culture), participative model- task performance. Within the SLT framework,
ing would increase the retention of desirable be- cross-cultural training would enable individuals
haviors. As SLT would predict, the trainees ex- to determine in advance appropriate behaviors
posed to this more comprehensive training were and culturally congruent ways of performing job
more adjusted to Japan than trainees who re- tasks. Also, if cross-cultural training allowed for
ceived only the factual briefing. more cognitive and behavioral rehearsal, the
individual would have higher efficacy and out-
Social Learning Theory and Periormance come expectations as well as greater proficiency
The relationship between cross-cultural train- regarding certain behaviors before actually en-
ing and performance in the theoretical frame- tering the cross-cultural setting, all of which
work of SLT is based on the assumption that would facilitate the effective execution of the job
tasks that are performed in a cross-cultural set- tasks. Obviously, to the extent that performance
ting have dimensions that are related to the host is unrelated to culturally determined appropri-
culture. At the least, this assumes a positive re- ate behaviors, then other theoretical models of
lationship between adjustment to living in a for- performance would be more relevant and cross-
eign culture and efiectively performing organi- cultural training would be less relevant. For ex-
zational tasks; that is, that successful adjustment ample, based on empirical evidence. Porter and
to living in a foreign culture requires an under- Lawler (1968) argued that abflity, motivation,
standing of the host culture. At the most, it as- opporiunity, and role perceptions are the key
sumes that every task has cultural dimensions to determinants of performance. If these factors are
it and that effectively performing these tasks re- unrelated to the cross-cultural situation, then
quires an integration of appropriate behavior. logically (as well as within the theoretical frame-
Without one or both of these assumptions, it work of SLT) one would not expect cross-cultural
could be argued that living in a foreign culture training to have an impact.
has no influence on work performance, and that However, the studies reviewed found that
work performance has no relationship with cul- cross-cultural training did lead to greater levels
tural dimensions; therefore, there would be no of performance in various groups of individuals.
reason for cross-cultural training to have an im- This suggests that in most cases performance
pact on work performance in a cross-cultural does have culturally related components and
setiing. Thus, if one can assume that a person's that the processes described with the framework
effective execution of tasks and responsibilities of SLT of how skills are acquired, how modeled
in a cross-cultural situation are determined by behaviors are imitated, and how the adjustment

125
process is facilitated explain why cross-cultural who are university professors and lack expatri-
training facilitates performance. Figure 1 illus- ate or business experience?
trates how elements of cross-cultural training Proposition 1 : The use of similar models results
and SLT would interact in affecting cross- in greater attention to (he modeled behavior
cultural skill development, adjustment, and per- and more effective training.
formance. Individual differences might affect the degree
to which individuals attend to the training and,
therefore, the degree to which the training is
Theoretical Implications effective. Locus of control might be an important
individual difference in this regard. For exam-
The primary focus of the previous section was ple, because individuals with internal locus of
to examine how SLT might explain the empirical control orientations (internals) believe that
findings of the effectiveness of cross-cultural events are determined by their own actions and
training; however, SLT also provides a means abilities, it could be expected that they would
by which cross-cultural training can be system- have higher efflcacy and outcome expectations,
atically investigated in the future. which, in turn, might lead to more motivation to
attend to the training content. Additionally, in a
Attention Processes in review of the locus of control construct, Lefcourt
Cross-Cultural Learning (1983) indicated that internals also attend more
closely to new or ambiguous environments. In
Social learning theory suggests that similar the context of this article, according to SLT indi-
models receive more attention than dissimilar viduals differ in their abilities to anticipate cross-
models. In a cross-cultural training situation, the cultural situations, which by their nature are
more the model or trainer seems similar to the new and ambiguous. Therefore, internals might
trainees, the more the trainees relate to the pay more attention during cross-cultural train-
trainer and the more attention they pay to the ing, which would have a positive impact on the
modeled behaviors. It is important to remember learning process and the effectiveness of the
that the modeling can be in the form of (a) lec- training.
tures, which require symbolic associations Additionally, Ratiu (1983) found that individu-
based on imagined relationships between be- als who were rated as most internationally ef-
haviors and consequences, (b) watching films, fective by their colleagues were more likely to
which requires symbolic associations based on alter their stereotypes of foreigners, whereas
observed relationships between behaviors and those rated as least internationally effective
consequences, and (c) role playing, which re- were more likely to maintain their stereotypes.
quires symbolic associations based on observed Although this individual diflerence factor has
and experienced behaviors and consequences. not been highly researched, it may be evidence
No studies in the cross-culture training literature of what others (Oddou & Mendenhall, 1984)
have examined the impact on trainee learning have referred to as cognifiVe flexibility. Thus, it
of having trainers who are similar to the train- might be that individuals who have a high level
ees. For example, if businessmen are being of cognitive flexibility would be more willing to
trained for overseas expatriate assignments, accept and incorporate fundamental notions
what would be the impact of using trainers who from cross-cultural training; therefore, they
are expatriates, trainers who are consultants might beneflt more from cross-cultural training
(but utilize models from actual expatriates than individuals who have lower levels of cog-
through means such as case studies), or trainers nitive flexibility.

126
M A
0)

a
I
*
I
3 W
en
o
fl "2
.2
"G
-S g
1 ^

S*
(S I I I 2 g" g
x; n, x; D. -c 0)
.3 u ..o -S -o -S

I
1
fl
o
1

sfl e s
O o
> S-
U
c
^ 0
i

cl il
U k..

127
The Retention Process in Proposifion 3: Gradual modeling of novel be-
Cross-Cultural Learning haviors that are needed for cross-cultural inter-
actions has a greater impact on the retention
Social learning theory suggests that modeled processes than one-time modeling.
behavior that is appropriate in the cross-cultural
situation and is perceived as similar to behavior Social learning theory implies that cognitive
the observer has already learned is easier to and behavioral rehearsal of the modeled be-
retain and imitate than dissimilar behavior. This haviors that are appropriate for the cross-
means that new behavior and its consequences cultural situation will facilitate retention. Once
can be incorporated into already existing cogni- the trainee has formed the original cognitive as-
tive maps. Once incorporated, the new sym- sociations between the components of the be-
bolic representations become part of, and are haviors and the related consequences, both
retained in, the past cognitive maps; therefore, cognitive rehearsals and a combination of cog-
they can be retrieved in the future. Thus, pre- nitive and behavioral rehearsals will reinforce
senting new and culturally different behavior and refine the trainee's cognitive map, which, in
and consequences in a way that allows the turn, serves to facilitate the retention of the be-
learner to perceive them as similar to known havior.
behavior and associated consequences would Proposition 4: Cognitive and behavioral re-
facilitate the learners' ability to retain these new hearsal facilitate the retention of models of
behaviors, which in turn would have a positive novel behavior.
impact on the appropriate execution of the new
These retention-oriented propositions evoke
behaviors in the cross-cultural situation.
general questions for investigation in the area of
Proposition 2: Presenting novel behavior in a cross-cultural training; for example, what train-
form which allows the learner to perceive it as ing method or combination of methods (e.g., lec-
similar to his or her own behavior will enable
the learner to more effectively retain the new ture, factual briefing, cultural assimilator, role
behavior. play, field trips, etc.) is best for learning the
Social learning theory also suggests that behaviors of a different and novel culture?
novel behavior is best learned through model- Gudykunst et al. (1977) found that a combination
ing successive approximations of the final be- of lecture and experiential methods may be
havior. Thus, by rst providing a basic model of more effective than training through lecture
the behavior and its consequences in a cross- alone. Based on SLT, very novel behaviors
cultural situation which are appropriate in the might be best modeled through a combination
new culture, a reference or starting point can be of methods. In addition, gradual modeling and
provided for the learner. The gradual addition participative modeling should be incorporated
of successive and more complex approximations so that trainees can easily comprehend the
of the complete model of new and novel behav- novel behavior, make associations between the
ioral norms allows the trainee to build associa- behavior and its consequences, and rehearse
tions between each successive approximation the behaviors; all of these, in turn, will enable
and the previous ones. Essentially, in SLT it is trainees to reach a higher retention level.
argued that cross-cultural trainers should first
The Reproduction Process in
provide the skeleton of the new behavior and
Cross-Cultural Learning
then gradually add the flesh until the complete
model is presented. Once the symbolic associa- The moioric reproduction process, as Bandura
tions have been constructed, the modeled be- (1977) referred to it, is the execution of the
havior can be retained more easily than by pre- learned behaviors in actual situations. In SLT
senting the complex model of the behavior. accurate and effective reproduction of learned

128
behaviors depends on the attention and reten- ducing a learned behavior and has an incentive
tion processes, the person's incentive to repro- for reproducing the behavior, the more novel
duce the behavior, and the person's ability to the modeled behavior, the more important will
reproduce it. Because of the relationships be the attention and retention processes, rela-
among attention and retention processes and tive to the accurate reproduction of the learned
motoric reproduction, it follows that the more behavior in the cross-cultural situation. The
similar the modeled behavior is to what has more attention the trainee pays to the modeled
been learned and reproduced in the past, the behavior, the more information he or she has to
easier the new behavior will be to learn and form associations that will facilitate the retention
reproduce. In contrast, the more novel the mod- of the model. Also, the greater the trainee's re-
eled behaviors, the more difficult they will be to tention of the model, the easier it will be for the
learn and reproduce. individual to recall the modeled behavior and
Proposition 5: The more novel the modeled be- the more useful that recollection will be in guid-
haviors relative to what has been learned and ing the actual motoric reproduction.
reproduced in the past, the more difficult it will
be to reproduce them accurately in the future. Proposition 6; The more novel the modeled be-
haviors, the more important will be the attention
This proposition raises some general ques- and retention processes for accurate and effec-
tions about the relationship between the novelty tive motoric reproductions.
of the culture (and, therefore, the novelty of the
behaviors necessary for effective cross-cultural It is possible to derive a number of general
interactions) and the rigor of the cross-cultural questions for future cross-cultural training re-
training. Past reviews on factors that affect search from this proposition. For example, if the
cross-cultural adjustment have found that the new culture and its attendant behavioral norms
more novel the new culture, or what Menden- are extremely novel, what is the difference in
hall and Oddou (1985) referred to as culture motoric reproduction of purely symbolic versus
toughness, the more difflcult the adjustment for symbolic and behavioral rehearsal during the
the expatriate. Based on this review's flndings, it retention process? Also, what is the trainee's af-
seems reasonable to speculate that the more fective response to different methods during the
novel the culture and behaviors necessary for attention and retention processes?
effective cross-cultural interactions, the greater In addition, past experiences as well as the
will be the necessary rigor of the cross-cultural attention and retention processes influence the
training. Although no clear consensus concern- individual's level of self-efflcacy, which, in turn,
ing the deflnition of training rigor exists, Men- influences several aspects of motoric reproduc-
denhall and Oddou (1986) provided typologies tion. In cross-cultural training, if a person has a
of training methods and ranked the methods in low level of self-efficacy, despite the training to
terms of rigor, where rigor is largely a function learn the new behaviors necessary for effective
of the degree of cognitive involvement required cross-cultural interaction, it would be probable
of the learner. Although these scholars did not that the individual would not attempt to repro-
make specific reference to SLT, the training duce the learned behaviors during appropriate
methods they classified as low rigor (e.g., lec- cross-cultural situations. In contrast, if the indi-
tures and factual briefings) involved only sym- vidual has a high level of self-efficacy, he or she
bolic modeling processes, whereas those they would probably persist at that effort to repro-
classified as high rigor involved symbolic and duce the learned behavior longer than an indi-
participative modeling processes. vidual who has a lower level of self-efficacy.
Social learning theory also suggests that, as- Proposition 7: The higher the person's self-
suming a person is physically capable of repro- efficacy, the more likely the person is to execute

129
the learned behavior and to persist in executing ternal rewards for receiving cross-cultural train-
the behavior. ing on the degree of retention and reproduction
This proposition in combination with others of learned behaviors? What will be the impact of
presents many questions for future study in the vicarious rewards for receiving the training on
cross-cultural training area. For example, is a the efiectiveness of the cross-cultural training
high level of self-efficacy more important for (e.g., feedback regarding how past trainees
learning novel behaviors of a novel host culture have benefited from the training)? What will be
than it is for learning similar behaviors in a sim- the impact of the trainee's reaction to past train-
ar host culture? In other words, is there an in- ing situations or his or her expectations that the
teraction effect between novelty of the behaviors current training will be beneficial?
and the degree of self-efficacy? Relationship Between Adjustment and
Cross-Cultural Training
Motivation and Incentives in
Cross-Cultural Learning Scholars have defined adjustment in three
ways. Some scholars have defined adjustment
Within SLT, incentives can have an imporiant in terms of the psychological comfori and famil-
impact on the attention, retention, and repro- iarity an individual feels for the new culture
duction processes. Incentives can be actual or (Black, 1988; Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985; Tor-
vicarious, immediate or expected; they can biorn, 1982). In contrast, culture shock has been
have an external or internal origin; and they can used as a measure of adjustment and has been
influence the extent to which individuals attend described as a state of not knowing how to be-
to the modeled behavior. Incentives can influ- have appropriately in the new culture and be-
ence the extent to which modeled behavior is ing overwhelmed by this anxiety (Oberg, 196(D).
cognitively or behaviorally rehearsed; there- Other scholars have defined adjustment in
fore, they can influence the extent to which the terms of the individual's ability to get along with
behavior is retained. Additionally, and strongly and effectively interact with host nationals
emphasized by Bandura (1977), even if behav- (Brem & David, 1971; Brislin, 1981; Landis & Bris-
iors are learned, incentives largely determine if lin, 1983).
the learned behaviors will be executed. For ex- Because cross-cultural training familiarizes
ample, even if an individual learns how to ef- the individual with the culture (and the new and
fectively interact with Japanese subordinates, if appropriate behaviors in that culture), the indi-
there are no incentives for exhibiting those be- vidual would be expected to feel comfortable
haviors, and if the individual is instead re- and familiar with the culture more easily and
warded via praise, recognition, promotions, or quickly. Likewise, because of the subsequent
salary raises for behaviors that have nothing to cultural familiarity and advanced cross-cultural
do with interacting with Japanese subordinates understanding on the part of the trainee, cross-
(or is rewarded for actions that contradict the cultural training would lead to less anxiety and
learned behaviors necessary for effective inter- culture shock and thereby facilitate adjustment.
actions with Japanese subordinates), it is likely In terms of adjustment (if it is defined as effective
that the pserson will not execute these behaviors. interactions with host nationals), the trainee
Proposition 8: Incentives influence the trainees' learns through cross-cultural training the appro-
attention, retention, and especially their repro- priate behaviors for effective interactions and in-
duction of modeled behavior in cross-cultural creases his or her self-efficacy, both of which
training. enable the individual to reproduce the behav-
This proposition also generates general re- iors that would lead to effective interactions
search questions. What will be the impact of ex- more quickly than a nontrained individual.

130
Proposition 9: Cross-cultural training reduces faction, lower intentions to return early, and less
the severity of normal culture shock and re-
duces the time necessary to reach a level of failure.
cultural proficiency. Proposition 10: Cross-cultural training would
This proposition in combination with proposi- lead to more realistic expectations, greater job
satisfaction, lower intentions of returning early,
tions about the novelty of new behavior sug- and lower rates of early returns from cross-
gests general questions for future research. For cultural assignments.
example, would the impact that rigorous cross-
cultural training has on reducing culture shock However, because it is possible that an indi-
be the largest when the host culture is quite vidual would not return early, yet would be in-
novel and the individual interacts with host na- effective, performance also was operationalized
tionals on a frequent and involved basis? Would in terms of performance evaluations, usually
rigorous cross-cultural training have a less dra- given by the individual's supervisor (Earley,
matic effect on culture shock when the host cul- 1987; Hawes & Kealey, 1981). If the individual's
ture is novel but the individual has litfle or no job has dimensions that are related to effective
need to interact wifli host nationals? cross-cultural interaction, and if cross-cultural
training facilitates effective relationships with
The Relationship Between Performance and host nationals, it also should lead to high per-
Cross-Cultural Training formance evaluations. Preliminary empirical
Although no universally accepted deflnition evidence suggests that the hypothesized rela-
of performance exists in the cross-cultural liter- tionship between adjustment and performance
ature, at least two major operationalizations is significant (Earley, 1987). Extending this logic,
were found. First, past researchers have dis- cross-cultural training would have the greatest
cussed cross-cultural performance in terms of impact on individuals (e.g., top executives)
whether the individual stayed in the new culture whose jobs require a large amount of interac-
the expected length of time or whether the indi- tion with host nationals and deep integration
vidual requested or was requested to return into the culture.
early (Baker & Ivancevich, 1971; Tung, 1981). Proposition 11 : Cross-cultural training leads to
Through cross-cultural training trainees should higher performance, especially for individuals .
gain a realistic expectation of their jobs and lives whose jobs require a high level of interaction
in the foreign country and culture. Realistic job with host nationals.
expectations in general have been associated Based on the framework presented, there is at
with greater satisfaction and lower job turnover least one more general issue to consider. Al-
(see Steers & Mowday, 1981, or Wanous, 1980, though none of the studies reviewed attempted
for a review). to examine and compare the impact of training
Cross-cultural training also would allow the for different subjects (businesspeople, military
trainee to make anticipatory determinations of personnel, educators, etc.) or the impact of dif-
what behaviors to act out. If those determina- ferent environmental contexts (e.g., job charac-
tions were correct, the training would enable teristics that require a high demand for interact-
the trainee to execute appropriate behavior ing with host nationals versus a low demand for
without having to learn by trial and error. There- such interaction), it is possible that moderating
fore, the person could avoid some instances of effects exist. For example, it is possible that be-
inappropriate behavior and the associated neg- cause many military personnel are isolated from
ative consequences that might be experienced the foreign environment through base com-
by a nontrained individual. Such experience pounds, that training may be less effective for
could, in turn, lead to higher levels of job satis- them than for expatriates who are involved in

131
the transfer of technology and who live and context of domestic training challenges any fun-
work among host nationals. However, it seems damental assumptions of SLT or presents any
that rather than focusing on differences in sub- totally new variables that should be incorpo-
jects, that it would be more productive to exam- rated into SLT, the international context does
ine the impact of differences in context. For ex- seem to rearrange the imporiance of various
ample, would cross-cultural training have the factors within SLT.
same effect on a senior military ofiicer and a The first difference concerns the variable of
senior business executive who both would need novelty. Although novelty plays an important
to interact frequently with host nafionals in order role in SLT within the domestic training context,
to be effective? Tung (1982) and Mendenhall et it plays a critical role in the international train-
al. (1987) argued that the degree to which cross- ing context. Generally, the degree of novelty of
cultural training is needed and is effective de- new behaviors is significantly higher in the in-
pends upon the degree to which the individual ternational context. In both domestic and inter-
must interact with members of other cultures. national contexts new work-related behaviors
The framework of SLT complements this argu- must be learned; however, in most cross-
ment; accordingly, not only would training have cultural training situations new nonwork behav-
the greatest impact when the new culture and iors must be learned as well. For example, an
context are novel compared to the individual's individual preparing for an assignment in Japan
home culture, but also when the individual must might need to learn new accounting proce-
interact frequently with individuals of the for- dures, how to make decisions by consensus,
eign culture. how to work within the rules of sempai(senior)/
kohai (junior) relationships, how to give and re-
ceive gifts, and how to behave appropriately on
Conclusion extremely crowded trains and subways. Thus,
the depth and breadth of the role of novelty in
The first purpose of this paper was to provide SLT seems to be much greater in international
a comprehensive review of the extant empirical contexts.
literature on cross-cultural training. This review This also raises the importance of two related
suggests that cross-cultural training has a posi- aspects of SLT. First, given that in a cross-
five impact on the individual's development of cultural training situation, in general, an indi-
skills, on his or her adjustment to the cross- vidual will be required to learn behaviors of
cultural situation, and on his or her job perfor- greater breadth and depth of novelty, the atten-
mance in the cross-cultural situation. However, tion and retention aspects of the SLT become
compared to the training literature in general much more imporiant in the international con-
(see Latham, 1988, for a review), the area of text. For example, in a domestic training con-
cross-cultural training has received little empir- text, it is likely that low levels of attention and
ical attention. retention will negafively affect the trainee's ac-
Because most past empirical research on curate reproduction of the modeled behavior,
cross-cultural training has lacked theoretical but it is also likely that the behavior could be
grounding, this article presented SLT as a theo- reproduced in some form because the modeled
retical framework. In the process of examining behavior is familiar. In an international context,
SLT in relafion to cross-cultural training, some because of the generally higher level of behav-
important new insights about SLT have ior novelty, low levels of attenfion and retenfion
emerged. Although it does not seem that the may render the trainee unable to reproduce the
context of international training compared to the modeled behavior in any fashion (Gudykunst et

132
al., 1977). Thus, a higher minimum threshold of are greater than in domestic training contexts, a
attention and retention may be necessary in in- higher minimum level of motivation may be nec-
ternational contexts. essary in order for effective attention and reten-
In addition, there is reason to believe that the tion processes to occur during cross-cultural
role and sequencing of symbolic and participa- training situations. Cross-cultural training effec-
tive modeling processes of SLT are different in tiveness in the studies reviewed may be ex-
international versus domestic contexts. Because plained partly by almost all the trainees being
most domestic training situations involve behav- volunteers for the cross-cultural work situation
iors that are familiar, the sequencing of sym- and, therefore, being more likely to be moti-
bolic or participative modeling processes and vated than randomly selected individuals.
whether the processes are used in combination Finally, another variable of SLT that seems to
or separately seem less important in domestic operate differently in an international context is
versus international contexts. The results of the self-efficacy. In both the domestic and interna-
study by Gudykunst et al. (1977) suggest that tional contexts it seems that the greater the level
symbolic modeling should precede participative of self-efficacy, the more likely the individual is
modeling; symbolic modeling alone may not be to persist in trying to reproduce the modeled be-
sufficient for the training to have a positive and haviors. However, given the greater level of
significant impact on outcomes, such as cross- novelty associated with cross-cultural training
cultural adjustment. For example, although situations, it seems that there may need to be a
symbolic modeling processes may be sufficient higher minimum level of self-efficacy expecta-
for training a person to handle a new reporting tions in the international context before an indi-
procedure or even a new set of job responsibil- vidual will attempt to reproduce the modeled
ities in the domestic context, symbolic learning behavior at all, let alone persist in trying to re-
alone is not likely to be sufficient for training a produce it. There is some evidence that individ-
person to speak a different language or for train- uals who dropped out of cross-cultural training
ing him or her to interact effectively with people situations lacked the confidence that they would
from a different culture. Thus, while the theoret- need to successfully execute the new behaviors
ical axioms of SLT do not necessarily change (PSI, 1982).
with respect to symbolic and participative mod- Several conclusions are worth noting. First,
eling in the cross-cultural training context, the based on the published empirical evidence, it
importance of participative modeling in SLT, rel- seems that cross-cultural training is effective in
ative to successful cross-cultural training and developing important cross-cultural skills, in fa-
the sequencing of symbolic and participative cilitating cross-cultural adjustment, and in en-
modeling, does change. hancing job performance. Second, most past
In addition, if the greater depth and breadth of empirical research on this subject has lacked
novelty in international contexts make the atten- theoretical grounding. The area of cross-cultural
tion and retention processes more important, training will be facilitated by correcting this
then the motivational factors also increase in im- practice, and using SLT as a heuristic frame-
portance. It may be that even though greater work is a first attempt to move the field in a more
motivation to attend to and retain new behav- theoretically based direction. Finally, although
iors has a positive impact on the learning pro- SLT seems to be a robust theory that can be
cess in the domestic context, the required mini- applied to both domestic and international train-
mum threshold level is lower than that required ing contexts, the importance of certain variables
in the international context. Given that the depth of SLT is different in cross-cultural training situ-
and breadth of novelty in international contexts ations.

133
References
Adler, N. (1983) Cross-cultural management research: The training program. Paper presented at the meeting of the
ostrich and the trend. Academy o Management Review, Intemational Communication Association, Minneapolis.
8, 226-232. Dunbar, E., & Ehrlich, M. (1986) nernafionai practices, se-
Adler, N. (1986) International dimensions of organizational lection, training, and managing the international staff: A
behavior. Boston, MA: Kent. survey report. New York: Columbia University, Teachers
Baker, ]. C, & Ivancevich, J. M. (1971) The assignment of College, Project on International Human Resource.
American executives abroad: Systematic, haphazard, or Earley, P. C. (1987) Intercultural training for managers: A
chaotic? Caii/ornia Managemen/Review, 13(3), 39-44. comparison of documentary and interpersonal methods.
Bandura, A. (1977) Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, Academy of Management Journal, 30, 685-698.
NI: Prentice-Hall. Fernandez, T. M. (1986, Oct 9-11) Guide design: Sensitivity
Beaty, D., & Mendenhall, M. (1989) /nernaionai manage- to black-white interactions when giving nursing care. Pa-
ment research: Toward an agenda. Unpublished manu- per presented at the 15th Annual Conference of the Inter-
script. national Society for Individualized Instruction, Atlanta.
Befus, C. P. (1988) A multilevel treatment approach for cul- Fiedler, F., Mitchell, T., & Tdandis, H. (1971) The culture
ture shock experienced by sojourners. /niernafionaJ Jour- assimilator: An approach to cross-cultural training. Jour-
nal of Intercultural Relations, 12, 381-400. nal of Applied Psychology, 55, 95-102.
Black, J. S. (1987) Japanese/American negotiations: The Jap- Graham, J. (1985) The influence of culture on the process of
anese perspective. Business and Economic Review, 6(1), business negotiations: An exploratory study. Journal of In-
27-30. ternational Business Studies, 16(1), 81-95.
Black, J. S. (1988) Work role transitions: A study of American Gudykunst, W. B., Hammer, M. H., & Wiseman, R. L. (1977)
expatriate managers in Japan, /ournai o International An analysis of an integrated approach to cross-cultural
Business Siudies, 19, 277-294. training. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,
1, 99-110.
Bochner, S. (1982) Cuifures in contact: Studies in cross-
cultural interaction. New York: Pergamon Press. Hammer, M. R., Gudykunst, W. B., & Wiseman, R. L. (1978)
Brein, M., & David, K. H. (1971) Intercultural communication Dimensions of intercultural effectiveness: An exploratory
and adjustment of the sojourner. Psychoiogicai BuiJeiin, study, /nferna/ionai Journal of ntercultural Relations, 2,
76, 215-230. 382-393.
Brislin, R. W. (1981) Cross-cuifuraJ encouniers. New York: Harris, P., & Moran, R. T. (1979) Managing cultural dier-
Pergamon Press. ences. Houston, TX: Gulf.
Brislin, R. W., & Pedersen, P. (1976) Cross-cultural orienta- Hawes, F., & Kealey, D. J. (1981) An empirical study of Ca-
tion programs. New York: Gardner Press. nadian technical assistance. International Journal o In-
tercultural Relations, 5, 239-258.
Chemers, M. M. (1969) Cross-cultural training as a means of
improving situational favorableness. Human Relations, Hilgard, E. R., & Bower, G. H. (1975) Theories of earning.
22, 531-546. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Chemers, M. M., Lekhyananda, D., Fiedler, F. E., & Katz, J. (1977) The effects of a systematic training program on
Stolurow, L. (1966) Some effects of cultural training on the attitudes and behaviors of white people. International
leadership in heterocultural task groups. International lournal of ntercutural Relations, 1, 77-89.
Journal o Psychology, 1, 301-314. Kluckhohn, C , & Kroeberg, A. L. (1952) CuJfure; A critical
Church, A. T. (1982) Sojourner adjustment. Psychological review o concepts and deiinitions. Vintage Books.
Bulletin, 91, 540-571. Kyi, K. M. (1988) APJM and comparative management in
Copeland, L., & Griggs, L. (1985) Going internaftonai. New Asia. Asia Pacific/ournai o/Managemen/, 5, 207-224.
York: Random House. Landis, D., & Brislin, R. (1983) Handbook on intercutura
Davis, T., & Luthans, F. (1980) A social learning approach to training (Vol 1). New York: Pergamon Press.
organizational behavior. Academy o Management ffe-
Landis, D., Brislin, R. W., & Hulgus, J. F. (1985) Attributional
view, 5, 281-290.
training versus contact in acculturative learning: A labo-
Dege, D. B. (1981, May 21-25) Forma/ and eva/ua(ion o the ratory study. 7ournai of Applied Social Psychology, 15,
cross-cultural component of a foreign teaching assistant 466-482.

134
Landis, D., Brislin, R., Swanner, G., Tseng, O., & Thomas, Mitchell, T. R., & Foa, U. G. (1969) Diffusion of the effect of
I. (1985) Some effects of acculturative training: A field eval- cultural training of the leader in the structure of hetero-
uation, /nternationa/ 7ournai of Group Tensions, 15, 68- cultural task groups. Austraiian Journal of Psychology, 21,
91. 31-43.
Landis, D., Day, H. R., McGrew, P. L., Thomas, J. A., & Nayar, E. S. K., Touzard, H., & Summers, D. A. (1968)Train-
Miller, A. B. (1976) Can a black "culture assimilator" in- ing, tasks and mediator orientation in heterocultural ne-
crease racial understanding? Journal of Social Issues, gotiations. Human Relations, 21, 283-310.
32(2), 169-183.
Neimeyer, G. J., Fukuyama, M. A., Bingham, R. P., Hall,
Latham, G. (1988) Human resource training and develop- L. E., & Mussenden, M. (1986) Training cross-cultural
ment. Annuai Review of Psychology, 39, 545-582. counselors: Comparison of the pro-counselor and anti-
Latham, G., & Saari, L. (1979) Application of social learning counselor trial models. Journal of Counseling and Devel-
theory to training supervisors through behavior modifica- opment, 64, 437-439.
tion, journai of Applied Psychology, 64, 239-246.
Newbold, R. C. m., Carrol, J. G., Hartman, M. E., Mitchell,
Lefcourt, H. M. (1983) Research with the locus of control con- H. E., & Story, M. R. (1982) A training program in medical
struct (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press. communications for foreign native medical graduates.
Lefley, H. (1985) Impact of cross-cultural training on black Project for the Educational Commission for Foreign Med-
and white mental health professionals, /nternaiionai Jour- ical Graduates, ERIC ED 219010-HE015252.
nal of ntercultural Relations, 9, 305-318. Noe, R. A. (1986) Trainee's attributes and attitudes: Ne-
Lefley, H. (1986) Evaluating the effects of cross-cultural train- glected influences on training effectiveness. Academy of
ing: Some research results. In H. Lefley & P. Pederson Management Review, 11, 736-749.
(Eds.), Cross-cuituraJ training for mental health profes- Oberg, K. (1960) Culture shock: Adjustment to new cultural
sionals (pp. 265-307). Springfield, IL: Thomas. environments. PracticaJ AnhropoJogis, 7, 177-182.
Manz, CO., & Sims, H. P. (1981) Vicarious learning: The O'Brien, G. E., Fiedler, F. E., & Hewett, T. (1970) The effects
influence of modeling on organizational behavior. Acad- of programmed culture training upon the performance of
emy of Management Review, 6, 105-113. volunteer medical teams. Human Relations, 24, 209-231.
McDaniel, C. O., McDaniel, N. C , & McDaniel, A. K. (1988) O'Brien, G. E., & Plooij, D. (1976) Development of cultural
Transferability of multicultural education from training to training manuals for medical workers with Pitjantjatjara
practice, /niernational Journal of Intercultural Relations, aboriginals. In G. E. Kearney & D. W. McElwain (Eds.),
12, 19-33. Aboriginai cognition (pp. 383-396). New Jersey: Humantic
McGroarty, M. (1984, March 6-11) Design and evaluation of Press.
cross-cultural workshops for ESL teachers and administra-
Oddou, G., & Mendenhall, M. (1984) Person perception in
tors. Paper presented at the annual meeting of Teachers of
cross-cultural settings: A review of cross-cultural and re-
English to Speakers of Other Languages, Houston. lated literature. International Journal of Intercultural Re-
Mendenhall, M., Dunbar, E., & Oddou, G. (1987) Expatriate lations, 8, 77-96.
selection, training, and career-pathing: A review and cri-
tique. Human fiesource Management, 26, 331-345. Porter, L. W., & Lawler, E. E. (1968) Managerial attitudes
and performance. Homewood, IL: Irwin.
Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. (1985) The dimensions of ex-
patriate acculturation. Academy of Management Review, PSI Associates, Inc. (1982) Peace corps stateside teacher
10, 39-47. training for volunteers in Liberia (Vol. 1) Evaluation report.
(Contract No. 82-042-1003) Washington, DC: Peace Corps.
Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. (1986) Acculturation profiles of
expatriate managers: Implications for cross-cultural train- Randolph, G., Landis, D., & Tzeng, O. C. (1977) The effects
ing programs. Columbia Journal of World Business, 21(4), of time and practice upon culture assimilator training. In-
73-79. ternational Journal of ntercultural Relations, 1, 105-119.
Miller, E. (1973) The international selection decision: A study Ratiu, 1. (1983) Thinking internationally: A comparison of
of managerial behavior in the selection decision process. how international executives leam. /nternaiionai Studies
Academy o/Management/ournai, 16, 234-252. of Management and Organization, 13, 139-150.
Misa, K. F., & Fdbricatore, J. M. (1979) Return on investment Roberts, K. H. (1970) On looking at an elephant: An evalu-
of overseas personnel. Financial Executive, 47(4), 42-46. ation of cross-cultural research related to organizations.
Mitchell, T. R., Dossett, D., Fiedler, F., & Triandis, H. (1972) Psychoiogicai Buiietin, 4(5), 327-350.
Cultural training: Validation evidence for the cultural as- Roberts, K. H., & Boyacigiller, N. A. (1984) Cross-national
similator. International Journal of Psychology, 7, 97-104. organizational research: The grasp of the blind men. In

135
B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organi- Swenson, L. L. (1980) Theories of learning. Belmont, CA:
zational behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 17-31). Greenwich, CT.: JAI Wadsworth.
Press.
Torbiom, I. (1982). Living abroad. New York: Wiley.
Ronen, S. (1986) Comparative and multinational manage-
Triandis, H. C , Vassiliou, V., Tanaka, Y., & Shanmugam,
ment. New York: Wiley.
A. (Eds.) (1972) The analysis of subjective culture. New
Runzheimer Executive Report (1984) Expatriation/ York: Wiley.
repatriation survey. (No. 31). Rochester, WI: Runzheimer
Associates, Inc. Tung, R. (1981) Selecting and training of personnel for over-
seas assignments. Columbia Journal of World Business,
Salisbury, L. (1971) College orientation program for Alaskan 16(1), 68-78.
natives: COPAN-education for survival. University of
Alaska at Fairbanks, Institute of Social, Economic, and Tung, R. (1982) Selection and training procedures of U.S.,
Government Research. European, & Japanese multinationals. California Man-
agement Review, 25(1), 57-71.
Schollhammer, H. (1975) Current research in international
and comparative management issues. Management In- Tung, R. (1984) Key to Japan's economic strength: Human
ternational Review, 13, 17-31. power. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Schwind, H. F. (1985) The state of the art in cross-cultural Vigushin, J. (1982) Helping campus personnel help foreign
management training. In R. Doktor (Ed.), /n(erna(ionaJ hu- students: A counseling simulation. Humanist Educator,
20(3), 134-142.
man resource development annual (Vol. 1, pp. 7-15). Al-
exandria, VA: American Society for Training and Devel- Vroom, V. (1964) Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
opment. Wanous, J. P. (1980) Organizational entry. Reading, MA:
Steers, R., &Mowday, R. (1981) Employee turnover and post- Addison-Wesley.
decision accommodation processes. In L. L. Cummings Weldon, D. E., Carlston, D. E., Rissman, A. K., Slobodin,
and B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behav- L., & Triandis, H. C. (1975) A laboratory test of effects of
ior (Vol. 3, pp. 237-249). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. culture assimilator training. Journal of Personality and So-
Steinkalk, E., & Taft, R. (1979) The effects of a planned inter- cial Psychology, 32, 300-310.
cultural experience on the attitudes and behaviors of the Worchel, S., & Mitchell, T. R. (1972) An evaluation of the
participants. International Journal of Intercultural Rela- effectiveness of the culture assimilator in Thailand and
tions, 3, 187-197. Greece. Journal of Applied Psychology, 56, 472-479.
Sterling, B. (1979) Problems in cross-cultural contact: A liter- Zeira, Y. (1975) Overlooked personnel problems in multina-
ature review. International Journal of Intercultural Rela- tional corporations. Columbia Journal of World Business
tions, 3, 269-313. 10(2), 96-103.

/. Siewarf Black (Ph.D., University of California, Irv-


ine) is Assistant Professor of Business Administration
in the Amos Tuck School of Business Administration at
Dartmouth College. Correspondence regarding this
article can be sent to him at the Tuck School of Busi-
ness Administration, Dartmouth College, Hanover,
NH 03755.
Mark Mendenhall (Ph.D., Brigham Young University)
is the J. Burton Frierson Chair of Excellence in Busi-
ness Leadership in the School of Business Administra-
tion at the University of Tennessee, Chattanooga.
The authors wish to thank Hal Gregersen for his help-
ful comments on an eariier version of this manuscript.

136
Copyright of Academy of Management Review is the property of Academy of Management and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like