You are on page 1of 1614

HANDBOOK OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

Fifth Edition
For Gardner Lindzey —
founding editor, scientist, and friend
Contents

PREFACE

CONTRIBUTORS

VOLUME 1
Part I: The Science of Social Psychology

1.⏐ HISTORY OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY: INSIGHTS, CHALLENGES,


AND CONTRIBUTIONS TO THEORY AND APPLICATION 3
Lee Ross, Mark Lepper, and Andrew Ward

2.⏐ THE ART OF LABORATORY EXPERIMENTATION 51


Timothy D. Wilson, Elliot Aronson, and Kevin Carlsmith

3.⏐ SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL METHODS OUTSIDE THE LABORATORY 82


Harry T. Reis and Samuel D. Gosling

4.⏐ DATA ANALYSIS IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY: RECENT AND


RECURRING ISSUES 115
Charles M. Judd and David A. Kenny

Part II: The Social Being

5.⏐ SOCIAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE 143


Matthew D. Lieberman

6.⏐ SOCIAL PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY AND EMBODIMENT 194


Jim Blascovich and Wendy Berry Mendes

7.⏐ AUTOMATICITY AND THE UNCONSCIOUS 228


Ap Dijksterhuis

8.⏐ MOTIVATION 268


John A. Bargh, Peter M. Gollwitzer, and Gabriele Oettingen

9.⏐ EMOTION 317


Dacher Keltner and Jennifer S. Lerner

ftoc.indd vii 12/29/09 12:02:30 PM


viii Contents

10.⏐ ATTITUDES 353


Mahzarin R. Banaji and Larisa Heiphetz

11.⏐ ATTITUDES AND PERSUASION: FROM BIOLOGY TO SOCIAL RESPONSES


TO PERSUASIVE INTENT 394
Dolores Albarracín and Patrick Vargas

12.⏐ PERCEIVING PEOPLE 428


C. Neil Macrae and Susanne Quadflieg

13.⏐ NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR 464


Nalini Ambady and Max Weisbuch

14.⏐ MIND PERCEPTION 498


Nicholas Epley and Adam Waytz

15.⏐ JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING 542


Thomas D. Gilovich and Dale W. Griffin

16.⏐ SELF AND IDENTITY 589


William B. Swann, Jr. and Jennifer K. Bosson

17.⏐ GENDER 629


Wendy Wood and Alice H. Eagly

18.⏐ PERSONALITY IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 668


David C. Funder and Lisa A. Fast

19.⏐ HEALTH 698


Shelley E. Taylor

20.⏐ EXPERIMENTAL EXISTENTIAL PSYCHOLOGY: COPING WITH


THE FACTS OF LIFE 724
Tom Pyszczynski, Jeff Greenberg, Sander Koole, and Sheldon Solomon

VOLUME 2
Part III: The Social World

21.⏐ EVOLUTIONARY SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 761


Steven L. Neuberg, Douglas T. Kenrick, and Mark Schaller

22.⏐ MORALITY 797


Jonathan Haidt and Selin Kesebir

23.⏐ AGGRESSION 833


Brad J. Bushman and L. Rowell Huesmann

24.⏐ AFFILIATION, ACCEPTANCE, AND BELONGING: THE PURSUIT


OF INTERPERSONAL CONNECTION 864
Mark R. Leary

ftoc.indd viii 12/29/09 12:02:31 PM


Contents ix

25.⏐ CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS 898


Margaret S. Clark and Edward P. Lemay, Jr.

26.⏐ INTERPERSONAL STRATIFICATION: STATUS, POWER, AND SUBORDINATION 941


Susan T. Fiske

27.⏐ SOCIAL CONFLICT: THE EMERGENCE AND CONSEQUENCES


OF STRUGGLE AND NEGOTIATION 983
Carsten K. W. De Dreu

28.⏐ INTERGROUP RELATIONS 1024


Vincent Yzerbyt and Stéphanie Demoulin

29.⏐ INTERGROUP BIAS 1084


John F. Dovidio and Samuel L. Gaertner

30.⏐ SOCIAL JUSTICE: HISTORY, THEORY, AND RESEARCH 1122


John T. Jost and Aaron C. Kay

31.⏐ INFLUENCE AND LEADERSHIP 1166


Michael A. Hogg

32.⏐ GROUP BEHAVIOR AND PERFORMANCE 1208


J. Richard Hackman and Nancy Katz

33.⏐ ORGANIZATIONAL PREFERENCES AND THEIR CONSEQUENCES 1252


Deborah H. Gruenfeld and Larissa Z. Tiedens

34.⏐ THE PSYCHOLOGICAL UNDERPINNINGS OF POLITICAL BEHAVIOR 1288


Jon A. Krosnick, Penny S. Visser, and Joshua Harder

35.⏐ SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND LAW 1343


Margaret Bull Kovera and Eugene Borgida

36.⏐ SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND LANGUAGE: WORDS, UTTERANCES,


AND CONVERSATIONS 1386
Thomas Holtgraves

37.⏐ CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 1423


Steven J. Heine

AUTHOR INDEX I-1

SUBJECT INDEX I-85

ftoc.indd ix 12/29/09 12:02:31 PM


ftoc.indd x 12/29/09 12:02:31 PM
Fifth Edition Preface

You hold in your hands (or on your screens) an array of co-editor); and his fourth edition appeared in 1998 (with
treasures. Since the first edition appeared in 1935, the Daniel Gilbert and Susan Fiske as his co-editors).
Handbook of Social Psychology has been the standard From the start, Gardner determined his topics and chose
reference work in the field, offering historical, integra- his authors democratically—by soliciting opinions from
tive, and penetrating surveys of the topics that constitute leaders in the field—which at once gave his Handbook
the discipline. For this we have the authors to thank. Each breadth, vision, and credibility. We have continued to use
chapter is written by the world’s foremost authorities on his method. Gardner ’s fingerprints are all over this fifth
the topic, and the list of contributors reads like an inter- edition in many other ways as well—in our philosophy
national who’s who in social psychology. This fifth edi- of representing the full field from neuron to nation, in our
tion serves a new generation of students and researchers, insistence that authors write for graduate students and not
reflecting the tremendous changes the field has experi- for one another, in our adherence to a prompt publication
enced in the last decade. Most authors in this edition are schedule, and we hope, in the continuing excellence of
new to the Handbook, as befits a vigorous, cutting-edge these volumes.
science. Many chapter topics remain from the previous Gardner ’s impact extended beyond his half century as
edition (although in most cases their content has changed the editor of the Handbook. He also shaped the field of
dramatically), and many others (social neuroscience, mind social psychology with his articles and books, although
perception, experimental existential psychology, morality, much of his influence occurred modestly, behind the
and interpersonal stratification) are new to this edition. scenes—advising, promoting, recommending, persuad-
This is the last edition of the Handbook that will bear ing. Neither of us will ever forget meeting with Gardner
the name of Gardner Lindzey, who passed away in 2008 15 years ago in a dark, smoke-filled restaurant in Boston,
at the age of 87. He helped organize this edition, but it is where he invited us to become part of this great tradition.
the first of the modern era to be completed without him. We are proud to carry forward his legacy, and we trust
Carl Murchison brought out the first official edition of the that future editors will do the same—without the smoke,
Handbook in 1935, but empirical work in social psychology perhaps, but with the same respect for the institution that
did not really explode until just after World War II, and it was Gardner ’s Handbook has become and with the same enthu-
then that Gardner founded the current Handbook series that siasm it inspires in all of us for the hub discipline of social
every social psychologist knows so well. His first edition psychology.
appeared in 1954; his second and third editions appeared STF, PRINCETON
in 1969 and 1985, respectively (with Elliot Aronson as his DTG, CAMBRIDGE

Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
PART I

The Science of Social Psychology


Chapter 1

History of Social Psychology: Insights,


Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and
Application
LEE ROSS, MARK LEPPER, AND ANDREW WARD

In his classic Handbook of Social Psychology chapter, problems in the real world. In short, this chapter represents
Jones (1985) offered a particularly comprehensive account a history, rather than the history of social psychology, an
of five decades of social psychology, beginning with the account of contributions, problems, insights, and events seen
late 1930s. His treatment of the contributions of Kurt through the particular interpretive lenses of its authors.
Lewin, whom he rightly identified as the most important The chapter includes five sections, each of which
shaper of modern experimental social psychology—and includes various subsections:
the groundbreaking work of Leon Festinger, whose dis-
crepancy reduction model (borrowed from Lewin’s • The first section discusses three major themes in our
tension-system concept) was applied to both pressures field’s approach to research: (a) the normative power
toward uniformity within groups and consonant versus dis- of the group, (b) the centrality of subjective meaning or
sonant cognitions of actors—remains essential reading for interpretation, and (c) an emphasis on impactful, and
aspiring researchers who want to understand what social often non-obvious, experimental demonstrations.
psychologists study, how they study it, and the “middle- • The second section examines some historical “dialec-
range” level of theorizing they find most comfortable. tics” in the evolution of particular topics and methods
Jones also offered balanced assessments of the most pro- in social psychology. It focuses on influences from psy-
vocative debates that had taken place within the field and chology in general, from social psychology in particular,
a clear-eyed account of the waxing and waning of specific and from real-world events and trends in U.S. society at
research programs (which he characterized as “bandwagons” large that have created opportunities and challenges, and
and “sinking ships”). Although we generally refrain from on occasion crises, for our field.
summarizing these debates and contributions, we do try to • The third section discusses four foundational insights or
build upon them—not only with some updating, but also with “pillars” that constitute cumulative lessons and continue
further consideration of the challenges confronting our disci- to guide contemporary analysis, research, and applica-
pline, and the various ways in which those challenges have tion: (a) “naïve realism” (i.e., the assumption of an iso-
been met. Any history of a field of study reflects particular morphism between what one “sees” through the prism
values and tastes. Ours include a fondness for studies that of one’s expectations, needs, and knowledge structures
employ consequential behavioral measures and an appre- and objective reality) and its social implications and
ciation of social psychology’s potential to speak to applied manifestations; (b) the importance of lay dispositionism

Author note: The authors gratefully acknowledge our many colleagues and graduate students who, over the years, have helped us to
understand and appreciate our field. Special thanks are owed to Benoit Monin, Barry Schwartz, and Greg Walton, who shared their
views with us about classic and contemporary contributions to social psychology, and to Phoebe Ellsworth and Dick Nisbett, whose
discussions with us about this chapter were only the latest in a long history of stimulating exchanges. Grant support to Lee Ross from the
National Science Foundation and the William and Flora Hewlett Foundation is also gratefully acknowledged. Finally, we wish to recog-
nize the invaluable assistance of our editors, Susan Fiske and Dan Gilbert. Their detailed feedback to an earlier draft helped sharpen our
focus and generally improve the chapter in a myriad of ways, both large and small.

3
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
4 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

(i.e., the general failure to appreciate the power of difficult to change them); and how, when, and why, they
situational forces and constraints in controlling social influence (or seemingly fail to influence) overt behavior
behavior); (c) the motivation or need to see oneself (see LaPiere, 1934). As a comprehensive and insightful
as consistent, rational, and moral; and (d) the impact overview of this topic, William McGuire’s 1969 Handbook
of expectations and beliefs that bias perceptions, chapter remains a classic, but students interested in more
interpretations, and reactions, and in so doing create recent reviews have many excellent sources to choose from
“self-fulfilling prophesies.” (e.g., Albarracín & Vargas, Banaji & Heiphetz, this volume;
• The fourth section reviews promising new topics Krosnick, Visser, & Harder, volume 2; Eagly & Chaiken,
and new approaches to topics of continuing interest, 1993; Fazio, 1990; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986).
including stereotyping and prejudice, cultural psychology, The third topic of continuing interest, which came to
limitations of standard economic models of decision dominate much of the field, is the study of social percep-
making, evolutionary psychology, implicit influences tion and self-perception. This topic includes the processes
on belief and behavior, terror management theory, posi- and biases that influence the assessments individual social
tive psychology, self-regulation, hedonic adaptation, actors make about each other and themselves (Jones, 1990;
close relationships, “virtual” interaction, and social cog- Gilbert, 1998). Today, cutting-edge work in this area is at
nitive neuroscience. least as much about social cognition as social perception
• The final section discusses examples of successful (see Fiske & Taylor, 2008; also Epley & Waytz and Macrae,
applications of social psychology to real-world prob- this volume), although work on visual perspective per se is
lems and offers some thoughts on the difficulties and making a bit of a comeback.
challenges faced by applied researchers, especially in As Jones (1985) noted, research on group processes and
“scaling up” small or mid-sized interventions. influences reached a new high during the Lewin and imme-
diate post-Lewin period, then dipped precipitously and
remained at a surprising low for decades (for reviews, see
CONTINUING THEMES IN CONTENT AND both Hackman and Hogg, volume 2). Today, while there is
METHODOLOGY still a relative paucity of work on groups per se, there has been
a dramatic increase in research on the workings of whole
Three Basic Content Areas cultures on the one hand and close personal relationships
on the other. The overall activity index for work on atti-
In discussing social psychology, lecturers and text- tudes has been more consistent. What has shifted over the
book authors commonly highlight three topics of central decades has been the aspect of the topic—measurement,
and continuing interest. One is the study of intra-group and structure and function, association with personality and/or
intergroup processes (what used to be called group dynam- ideology, persuasion techniques, effects on overt behavior
ics). This topic includes such concerns as how groups and vice versa, implicit versus explicit influences, etc.—pro-
“energize” behavior and diminish personal responsibility, ducing the most activity and interest. Social perception was
prompting individuals to actions they would never under- the slow starter (with initial work mainly on the degree and
take alone; how groups produce and maintain conformity determinants of accuracy in judging traits and emotions).
in public behavior and/or private beliefs; how groups treat But interest in this area accelerated with the flourishing of
in-group versus out-group members; what determines their attribution theory and the study of social perception pro-
productivity and quality of performance; and what pro- cesses and peaked in recent years as interest in potentially
cesses govern intergroup conflict and/or cooperation. This biasing influences (perceptual, cognitive, motivational, and
last topic, which Lewin pioneered and for which Morton decisional) gained prominence throughout psychology.
Deutsch assumed Lewin’s mantle of leadership, continues to More examples of specific research areas included under
have particular relevance in this age of ethnic conflict (see each of these topics at different times in our history are
Deutsch, 1977; Krauss & Deutsch, 1966; also Arrow, provided later in Table 1.2, along with a listing of some
Mnookin, Ross, Tversky, & Wilson, 1995; Pruitt & Carnevale, prominent social trends and events that contributed to shifts
1993; Pruitt & Rubin, 1986; Ross & Ward, 1995). in the focal research problems of our field. In addition, par-
A second topic that continues to play a large role in ticular problems, topics, and paradigms have sometimes
social psychology is the study of attitudes, opinions, and become “sinking ships” because a central question was
beliefs—how they are formed; what functions they serve answered to the satisfaction of researchers, because new
for the individual or group holding them; their various con- findings made the topic less instead of more interesting,
scious and non-conscious dimensions; how they cluster or just because no researcher came along with a new para-
together; how they can be changed (and why it is often so digm or question or an idea or insight provocative enough
Continuing Themes in Content and Methodology 5

to maintain the field’s interest in the face of promising new analysis not only of the processes by which the mem-
topics and approaches. Yet other topics and paradigms bers of the dominant group perceive and respond to the
declined for reasons that reflect shifts of emphasis in the members of the nondominant group, but also the conse-
field of psychology as a whole. quences felt, and responses made, by the latter (Dovidio &
The disappearance from flagship social psychology Gaertner and Yzerbyt & Demoulin, volume 2).
journals of research using laboratory animals to study
social phenomena is a case in point. Throughout the 1960s, Three Central Themes
there had been a steady stream of provocative articles on
affiliation, imitation, empathy, and cooperation. Zajonc Across these different research topics, three central “themes”
(1969) had explored both social facilitation and the effects in the research efforts of social psychologists can be dis-
of “mere exposure” on liking using a number of different cerned. The earliest and most obvious theme involves the
species. Lawrence and Festinger (1962) had even produced power of the group as a normative influence. A second
a fascinating monograph on “dissonance reduction” in lab- early and continuing theme—the centrality of subjective
oratory rats—i.e., evidence that animals that had to work meaning or interpretation—can now be seen in almost all
hard for meager or inconsistent extrinsic rewards behaved areas of psychology. However, it came to play a particu-
more persistently when those rewards were withdrawn larly important role in social psychology, wherein the classic
than those that had been more generously and consistently behaviorist formulation focusing on the links between
rewarded. But, for many years, the increasing cognitive (simple physical) stimuli and responses was transformed
ascendency throughout all of psychology discouraged most to deal with the way (more global) situations shape com-
researchers from doing such work of that sort. Recently, plex social behavior—a shift that required attention to the
however, there has been a resurgence of research investi- particular actors’ understandings or “definitions” of the situ-
gating dissonance, jealousy, the operation of hierarchies, ations they were confronting.
attraction and affiliation, nonverbal behavior, and other The third theme—an emphasis on “non-obvious” exper-
social psychological phenomena in man’s near relatives imental demonstrations—became influential somewhat
(Egan, Santos, & Bloom, 2007; Gosling & Mollaghan, later (in the 1960s), but may be the one that most distin-
2006; also, in this volume, chapters by Ambady; Epley & guishes the work of social psychologists from that of other
Waytz, and in volume 2, chapters by Fiske and Leary). social scientists. That is, researchers in all three content
Linking the topics of group processes, attitudes, and areas have frequently placed a premium on demonstra-
person perception is the study of interpersonal influence— tions that seemingly small manipulations could produce
the strategies individuals employ, successfully or unsuccess- surprisingly big effects, or that the specification of some
fully, to induce each other to comply with various requests non-obvious or subtle “mediator” could allow us to pre-
(which we discuss in a later section on “non-obvious” dict when a given effect would or would not be present.
effects) and the determinants of liking and attraction. Also apparent in many such demonstrations is the “situ-
Indeed, almost all “applied” undertakings at which social ationist” perspective that has been a major feature of our
psychologists have tried their hand demand attention to all field, and with that perspective the implicit suggestion that
three areas as well. This is certainly true of work on con- stable personal traits or dispositions matter less than lay
flict resolution, political psychology, health psychology, observers assume, or at least that they can be outweighed
industrial psychology, political psychology, environmental by particular features or manipulations of the immediate
psychology, educational psychology, and psychology and situation at hand.
law. In each area, both societal norms and the dynamics of Later we will discuss two other possible themes that
small groups are highly relevant, as are the processes of per- have gained increasing prominence in recent years. One
suasion and attitude change, as well as the perceptual and involves a focus on—one might even say an obsession
cognitive processes by which people evaluate each other with—the self. Not coincidentally, conceptions of the self,
and defer to or resist each other’s influence attempts. and more specifically the relationship between self and
But the “real-world” topic linking all three areas that has others, have been a particular focus in the “bandwagon”
received the most attention in social psychology is that of topic of East–West cultural differences. Research on this
racism and other forms of stereotyping and stigmatization topic has challenged our notions about the sources and nature
(including gender stereotyping and sexism)—a topic that of phenomena that we had once egocentrically, or rather ethno-
also brings together the study of underlying perceptual, cog- centrically, assumed to be “basic” and “universal.” The other
nitive, and motivational factors (Allport, 1954b; J. Jones, increasingly prominent, and again some critics might say
1997; Markus, 2008; Schneider, 2004). Work on this topic, obsessive, theme involves the exploration of cognitive and
reviewed later in this chapter, has increasingly involved motivational “biases” in perception, judgment, and decision
6 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

making, and, especially in the latter case, violations of the term “social influence” began to replace the more
specific normative standards (Luce & Raiffa, 1957). More restrictive “group influence.”
than any other, this topic has attracted the interest of col- Three later exceptions focusing on collective as well as
leagues in other fields and may suggest the most obvious individual level variables, however, are worth noting. Two
and direct implications for social policy and intervention. of these—intergroup relations (including the study of stig-
matization of racial minorities and other outgroups) and
Group Influence the study of cultural influences—will be discussed at sev-
The study of group influence dominated the early history eral points later in this chapter. The third topic involves
of our field. Indeed, as Allport described in his seminal estimates, judgments, and predictions by groups versus
1954 Handbook chapter, this theme was also central to our individuals—a topic with a very long history in the field,
pre-history, before the emphasis first on empiricism and going back at least to the 1920s and 1930s (reviewed by
then on experimentation distinguished the first generation Lorge, Fox, Davitz, and Brenner (1958)). The main mes-
of true social psychologists from the “armchair” social sage of this work, given renewed life by the publication of
philosophers whose observations and theories provided Surowiecki’s (2004) best-selling The Wisdom of Crowds,
the departure point for early research. One might even say involves the uncanny accuracy shown by the mean of large
that it was appreciation of the fact that group influences samples or by markets in making predictions, provided that
lead individuals to behave in ways not readily explicable in the responses being aggregated are made independently
terms of rational individual calculation, or the satisfaction rather than collaboratively, thus reflecting independent
of basic needs and drives, that provided a major impetus sources of information, and that relevant errors are random
for a separate discipline of social psychology. and uncorrelated.
Some of the landmarks worth noting include an early
Subjectivism
recognition of the importance of explicit and implicit
group norms and the relevance of reference groups, both The second longstanding theme, the emphasis on subjec-
as a source of such norms and as a basis for satisfaction or tive meaning, was sounded most emphatically, as Jones
dissatisfaction with one’s life circumstances. This recogni- noted, in our field’s resistance to behaviorist formulations
tion was followed by systematic theorizing and empirical that gave no place to “mentalistic” processes. Social psy-
investigation by Festinger, Schachter, and their collabora- chologists were skeptical about the sufficiency of classical
tors on pressures to uniformity and the moderating role theories of learning and operant conditioning to explain
of group cohesiveness. Next came the development of complex human behavior. Indeed, even if the temptation
social comparison theory, in which Festinger (1954) effec- to speculate about the subjective mental life of the rats,
tively moved the focus of analysis and research from the pigeons, and other non-sapiens so often used in the stud-
dynamics of the group to the perceptions, cognitions, and ies of conditioning and learning is wisely resisted, when
motivations of the individual. From there, it was but a it comes to human social behavior, most social psycholo-
short step to Schachter’s (1959) Psychology of Affiliation gists would insist that to understand, predict, and control
monograph, which added affective or emotional state to such behavior, one must be able to a recognize or deter-
the objects of social comparison and self-evaluation, and mine what stimulus the actors are attending to, and what
only a slightly longer step to the “cognitive” theories it means to those actors in light of their past experiences,
that left the study of social influence behind entirely and that current goals, and understandings about the world.
dominated research for two decades—namely, Festinger’s One also needs to know the actors’ interpretation of their
(1957) theory of cognitive dissonance and, to a lesser responses—what the actors intended to accomplish, and in
extent, Schachter and Singer’s (1962) “two-factor” theory some cases also how they believed those responses would
of emotion. be interpreted by others. Moreover, to predict the effects of
From that point on, while provocative work continued more complex events like “non-reinforcement” following
to be done on several aspects of group influence and group a history of prior reinforcement of the same action, one
functioning, including social exchange (Thibaut & Kelley, needs to know to what that non-reinforcement was “attrib-
1959), group versus individual risk tolerance (Wallach, uted” by the actor in question. Was it seen as reflecting a
Kogan & Bem, 1962), social facilitation (Zajonc, 1965), change in the state of the world or in the preferences of the
“groupthink” (Janis, 1972), de-individuation (Zimbardo, agent(s) of reinforcement, a “message” that more effort or
1970), jury deliberation (Hastie, Penrod, & Pennington, more refined responses are required, or simply a “chance”
1983), and other areas, the focus of the field remained event that should not influence future responding?
largely on processes within the mind of the individual For much of the history of our field, an appreciation of
perceiver, thinker, and decision maker. At the same time, the importance of subjective interpretation served mainly
Continuing Themes in Content and Methodology 7

as an impetus to attend carefully to research participants’ participants (again, experimental confederates) to make
appraisals or construals of the events they were experiencing the simple perceptual judgments they were called upon to
in the laboratory, and to make good use of post-experimental make; and to what did they think their own lone dissent on
interviews and questionnaires. The main exception was in the critical conformity trials would be attributed by those
the study of emotion, where the issue of appraisal became other participants?
central to theorizing (see Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). Only In any case, when Kelley (1967), in his seminal Attribution
in more recent work on framing and priming has the focus Theory opus, noted the links between Schachter’s work on
shifted from attempts to measure such processes to the use emotional labeling and Bem’s controversial “self-perception”
of subtle techniques to manipulate the subjective meaning account of key dissonance theory findings, social psychol-
of a given “objective” situation, and in so doing to alter ogists jumped on the bandwagon with an enthusiasm that
the way in which ordinary people respond to that situation. had not met earlier armchair philosophizing about causal
For example, Liberman, Samuels, and Ross (2004) gave inference by Heider (1958) and Ichheiser (1949). A flood
students an opportunity to play seven rounds of a standard of papers and chapters ensued (see Harvey, Ickes, & Kidd,
“Prisoner’s Dilemma” game that required them to opt for 1976, 1978; Jones et al., 1972; Weiner, 1974), including
“cooperation” or “defection.” The relevant payoff matrix Kelley’s own more reader-friendly 1973 paper spelling out
was held constant; what varied was the “name of the game” two basic principles—“discounting” and “covariation”—
mentioned by the experimenter and attached to that matrix. used by ordinary people seeking to understand why particu-
When told it was the Wall Street Game, two-thirds of the lar actors respond to particular objects or situations as they
students opted for defection on the first and subsequent do. Some of this work examined the reasons people give for
rounds of the game, and one-third opted for cooperation; their “voluntary” actions and decisions (Deci, 1975; Lepper &
by contrast when they were told it was the Community Greene, 1978), but most of it examined the causes to which
Game, these proportions were reversed. they attribute their successes and failures (Dweck, 1986,
Attention to subjective meaning also continues to guide 1999; Jones et al., 1972; Weiner, 1974).
and sharpen our appreciation of early social psychology Kelley’s theorizing also brought the once peripheral topic
classics. The experimenter’s explicit instructions not- of person perception to the fore. In an earlier paper, Jones
withstanding, did the youngsters in Triplett’s (1898) early and Davis (1965) had outlined processes by which observ-
co-action study (noted by Allport as the first real social ers of overt actions make inferences about the intentions of
psychology experiment) really regard their spool-winding the relevant actors—inferences that in turn allow observers
task as noncompetitive? In the reports of Sherif Robbers’ to determine the degree to which particular actions reflect
Cave studies, describing how competition between groups dispositions of the actor rather than situational demands
of preteen boys in a summer camp to win a valued prize and constraints. Kelley’s papers (1967, 1973), coming at
produced intergroup hostility (Sherif & Sherif, 1953; a time when cognitive psychology was beginning to assert its
Sherif, 1966), we are assured that the camp counselors “did dominance within psychology, and Bem’s articles (1965, 1967,
nothing” to encourage the aggressive acts that took place. 1972) offering a non-motivational account of key dissonance
Similarly, in Zimbardo’s (2007) account of the excesses of theory findings, almost immediately began to stimulate
the “guards” in his 1971 Stanford Prison Study, we are told new research. Ironically, given Kelley’s own emphasis on
that the authority figures in the “prison” neither suggested generally rational and successful attribution processes,
nor encouraged the humiliating treatment of “prisoners.” they also motivated younger researchers to shift the main
But what implicit “message,” one might ask, did the par- focus of attention from sensible attributional principles to
ticipants in these studies take from the lack of reaction on attributional biases and their consequences.
the part of authority figures when they first began to show Mislabeling of one’s own emotions became one focus
such behavior? of such research. For instance, Dutton and Aron (1974)
Indeed, some of the most famous studies in the showed how a walk on a swaying suspension bridge could
history of our field prompt related questions. To what spur romantic interest. Furthermore, the implications of mis-
did the participants in Milgram’s classic studies of labeled internal states, and especially unexplained arousal
destructive obedience attribute the experimenter’s bland or discontinuities and anomalies in mental experience,
reassurances and failures to act when they asked him check suggested fruitful areas of overlap between clinical and
on the well-being of the unfortunate “learner” (in actuality, social psychology (Maher, 1968; Zimbardo, 1999). But the
an experimental confederate) to whom they were adminis- major focus of attribution research centered on inferences
tering increasingly dangerous electric shocks every time he about one’s attitudes or tastes and the factors directing
erred? To what did subjects in Asch’s famous conformity and constraining one’s behavior (Nisbett & Valins, 1971;
studies attribute the unanimous inability of their fellow Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). In particular, investigators
8 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

recognized that Bem’s self-perception explanation for clas- no mere disciplinary idiosyncrasy. It reflects our recogni-
sic dissonance results demanded the additional assumption tion that all human beings are, in a sense, already intuitive
that actors in those studies, like observers, are making an psychologists (Ross, 1977)—observers and interpreters
attribution error, and more generally that erroneous per- of events who already know a great deal about how their
sonal and social inferences are common and consequential. fellow humans feel, think, and act, and in fact predicate
Inevitably, this focus on error led social psychologists to their own behavior on such knowledge. Indeed, many phe-
address other shortcomings in “lay psychology” (see Ross, nomena and problems that social psychologists investigate
1977; also Nisbett & Ross, 1980). arise from everyday failures in predicting and interpreting
While the study of attributional biases, and of inferential behavior, making the processes and biases that lead to such
strategies and biases more broadly, has become a distinct failures a matter of both practical and theoretical signifi-
undertaking in social psychology, the broader insight cance. Parallel failures of prediction and interpretation by
regarding subjective construal should not be lost. Greater practitioners and designers of intervention programs, and
understanding of the processes of “construal,” which is the the factors that may lead them to “miscalibrate” the relative
goal of virtually all work in social perception and social importance of various social and non-social influences that
cognition, is required if we are to understand, predict, and may determine the success of their programs, similarly
harness the “power of the situation.” The need to attend to become a topic of concern.
“the actor’s definition of the situation” is particularly Conventional accounts of human behavior offered in
important in meeting the challenge of effective “general psychology” typically reflect the conviction that
intervention—the topic to be addressed in the final sec- behavior is determined by preceding and attending percep-
tion of this chapter. That is, the targets of any intervention tions, thoughts, and feelings (the latter pair of which can be
will respond to their interpretations of the program and further distinguished or combined to create categories such
its consequences—both potential consequences and those as motives, emotions, tastes, preferences, goals, beliefs,
that actually take place—rather than those of the designers, expectations, and plans). These accounts also assign a critical
implementers, or funders of the intervention. role to the consequences of behavior, that is, positive or neg-
ative behavioral outcomes, unexpected as well as expected,
Non-Obviousness and Contrasting Methodological that produce learning, which in turn changes expectations
Approaches and shapes subsequent behavior.
The third theme—emphasis on “non-obviousness”—is of One set of non-obvious findings shows that the link-
newer vintage, and it too has been subject to some waxing ages among these events are bidirectional. Bruner and
and waning. To those outside our field, such an emphasis other New Look investigators showed that perceptions
might be regarded as an intellectually idiosyncratic prefer- not only shape motives and expectations; the latter also
ence. Contemporary physicists and chemists hardly worry shape the former. Similarly, Festinger and his followers
about whether their theories regarding the dimensions showed that while behavior is dictated by existing beliefs
postulated by “string theory” or the dynamics of hydro- and preferences, one’s behavior can alter those beliefs and
gen bonds are congruent or incongruent with everyday preferences, and that the relationship between richness
“lay physics” or “lay chemistry” (if for no other reason, of reward and consequences of reward can be opposite
because most people lack intuitions about those matters). to what one might expect. A second set of non-obvious
Perhaps most strikingly, our closer cousins in economics findings shows that social and/or situational context and
feel no need to apologize when their accounts of decision interpretation can be even more important than most
making seem largely to be a refinement of commonsense people recognize. The “ahistorical” Lewinian approach
principles of self-interest and their aggregated or “market” emphasized the impact of both immediate factors outside
consequences. On the contrary, they are reluctant to assign the mind and body of the actor and their contemporary
a role to influences on decision making that can not read- meaning to the actor. Subsequent investigators proceeded
ily be ascribed to self-interest; although, as we shall later to show just how powerfully determinative certain of
describe, many of them have succumbed to the allure of those external influences and mechanisms of interpreta-
work by psychologists on framing, reference points, loss tion, including relatively subtle ones, can be relative to
aversion, and other such influences (Kahneman & Tversky, some of the influences in the standard general psychology
1984; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974; also Gilovich, Griffin, & account (immediate needs and incentives, reinforcement
Kahneman, 2002). history, personal tastes and dispositions, even beliefs and
To those of us inside the field of social psychology, how- expectations).
ever, an emphasis on non-obviousness or “surprisingness” A taste for non-obviousness was already implicit in some
and the research strategies prompted by that emphasis are early research findings on reference groups and relative
Continuing Themes in Content and Methodology 9

deprivation. Newcomb (1943) reported how thoroughly at least large enough to cast doubt on the implicit theories
Bennington coeds in the 1930s had been weaned from the held by lay observers.
class-based political views of their well-to-do parents, once The dramatic effects shown in classic bystander inter-
they were exposed to the liberal-to-radical political norms vention studies by Latané and Darley (1968) and Darley and
of their new peers. Readers of Stouffer et al.’s American Batson (1973) are cases in point. The former showed inter-
Soldier volume (1949) learned that Black GIs stationed in vention rates of 75% for Columbia undergraduates seated
the South were more satisfied with military life than those alone when smoke began to waft into the room in which
stationed in the North, despite social and physical condi- they were completing a questionnaire but only 10% when
tions that objectively seemed much worse—because of how the undergraduate was seated beside two non-intervening
they were faring relative to the local non-soldiers of their confederates. Indeed, when three potential interveners
race to whom they were comparing themselves. But social saw the smoke, only 38% of cases produced even one
psychology’s emphasis on non-obviousness was first seen who sought help. The latter documented the huge effect
clearly in studies involving “channel factors”—that is, fac- (i.e., 10% vs. 63%) of “being late” versus “having time to
tors that facilitate the connections (we might say “clear spare” on the percentage of Princeton seminary students
the pathways” or even “grease the skids”) linking values (some of whom were on their way to tape a sermon on the
and intentions to behavior consistent with those values and Good Samaritan parable!) who stopped to assist someone
intentions. Here the violation of lay intuition lay not in lying in a doorway in apparent need.
the fact or direction of the relevant associations, but in the Studies of techniques for gaining compliance through
magnitude of those effects, relative to our initial intuitions induction of guilt, evoking of behavioral norms, taking
(Prentice & Miller, 1992), and the real-world responses advantage of the obligation people feel to reciprocate gifts,
that could be influenced. favors, or concessions, and similar time-tested tactics, offer
Cartwright (1949) reported that World War II bond many compelling examples of surprisingly large effects of
sales doubled when, instead of relying on media appeals small manipulations (Cialdini, 2007). In one particularly
exhorting workers to buy bonds at banks or post offices, noteworthy study, Freedman and Fraser (1966) showed
the government arranged for bonds to be sold in the work- that the percentage of homemakers who would agree to
place via face-to-face requests made by co-workers. Other a “big request” (to erect a large, crudely lettered “Drive
field studies showed how heavily the friendship choices of Carefully” sign on their front lawn) increased from 17%
MIT students in the “Westgate” complex depended not on to 76% when the request had been preceded by a much
shared tastes, interests, or views, but on the frequency with smaller request (that they merely place a 3-inch  3-inch
which physical proximity of homes and various unremark- sign with a related auto safety message in a window) made
able architectural factors such as the location of stairways by a different person, two weeks earlier. In another, Cialdini
and trash receptacles put people into casual everyday con- et al. (1975) showed that inducing refusal of a big request
tact (Festinger, Schachter, & Back, 1950; see also Whyte, (to assume a continuing obligation to counsel youths at a
1956, 1980). Indeed, one well-known and very early study juvenile detention center) could increase compliance with
(Bossard, 1932) showed that while love may depend on a smaller one (to serve as chaperone for one outing only)
many unfathomable mysteries of the heart, when it comes to from 17% to 50%.
marriage, mere propinquity plays no small role. In that study, Bearing in mind the lessons of the subjectivist tradi-
investigation of 5,000 marriage records revealed that tion should lead us to recognize that when seemingly small
one-third of marriages in Philadelphia took place between changes in the situation have large effects on behavior, it
people living within five blocks of each other. may be because those small changes significantly changed
While such findings attracted great interest, it was the the meaning of the situation for the actor. Consider, for
“demonstration experiment” that brought the pursuit of non- example, the finding that European countries requir-
obviousness and the situationist tradition together to most ing drivers to explicitly “opt out” of a program allowing
dramatic effect. The best known of these experiments consti- the harvesting of their organs if they are the victim of a
tute our field’s “crown jewels”—the experiments most often fatal accident recruit five to ten times as many potential
featured in our introductory texts and classroom lectures, donors as countries that require them to explicitly “opt in”
and the ones that we describe to strangers when we want to to such a program (Johnson & Goldstein, 2003). Superficial
convince them that we know a secret or two about human consideration might lead one to attribute this difference to
behavior. The object in these experiments was to show that laziness on the part of potential donors; but further analysis
some specific social context, subtle situational feature, or would lead one to recognize that the “opt-in” procedure
other theoretically relevant factor exerted enough influence conveys the message that participation in the program is a
to produce behavioral effects that seemed “too large”—or matter of altruism or of indifference as to the treatment of
10 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

one’s corpse, whereas the “opt-out” procedure conveys the relative to that of “personal moral values.” Nevertheless,
message that participation is normative and non-participation it clearly was the violation of their intuition that only cer-
reflects idiosyncratic rejection of a norm. tain “exceptional” kinds of people would obey, conform,
While between-condition comparisons can be com- or act brutally in the relevant situations that accounts for
pelling, the findings in the most dramatic (and contro- such surprise. The same is true for the violation of intuition
versial) demonstration experiments, such as Milgram’s that only certain kinds of people would fail to intervene
obedience experiments (1963; 1974), Zimbardo’s 1971 Stanford in the bystander intervention studies or agree to put up a
Prison experiment (see Zimbardo, 2007), or even the most huge “Drive Carefully” sign on their lawns in the Freedman
famous version of Asch’s conformity experiments (1951), and Fraser study.
that defied lay intuition, did not involve any such compari- It is also worth noting that the intuitive effect size
sons. Rather, they demonstrated that particular contexts could assessments that we routinely and automatically make
produce actions and/or failures to act, displays of seeming depend crucially upon the precise procedural details and
cruelty, mindless conformity or obedience, or buckling to results in each study. Thus, Milgram’s study is not just
authority that most of us would not expect from “normal about “obedience” in the abstract, but obedience in a par-
people.” Fully appreciating the implications of these stud- ticular, carefully scripted situation. Had Milgram chosen
ies, and the broader lesson about the power of the types as the learner’s punishment, instead of increasingly power-
of situational variables that social psychologists explore, ful electric shocks, increasingly stringent fines (from 1 to
reduces the surprise experienced upon learning that most of 2 to 3 cents . . .) or worse yet, “fines” involving mere hypo-
the low-level perpetrators of the horrors of the Holocaust, thetical points, it is unlikely that the studies would have
such as concentration camp guards and bureaucratic func- been published—much less that they would remain a clas-
tionaries, were ordinary people who lived unexceptional sic over half a century later. Nor would these studies have
lives both before and after their infamous deeds, rather had such a major impact had they shown only 10% obedi-
than self-selected psychopaths and sadists (Arendt, 1963; ence, even if that 10% were statistically significant com-
Goldhagen, 1996; Zimbardo, 2007). pared to some simple control condition.
The crucial point here is that even without benefit Another implication to be drawn from the aforementioned
of inferential statistics, random assignment, or control studies is that anyone observing the relevant behavior—even
groups, readers given only a single data point—that 65% of if he or she knew the specifics of the situation confronting
Milgram’s subjects fully obeyed the experimenter’s com- the actor, but especially if he or she did not, would make
mands to administer potentially life-threatening shocks to unwarranted dispositional inferences. In addition, any-
an innocent victim, that 70% of Asch’s subjects conformed one hearing of the behavior second-hand would be much
to a blatantly erroneous unanimous judgment offered by more likely to assume that it was something about the spe-
their peers, or that none of the pseudo-patients in Rosenhan’s cific actor rather than something about the specifics of the
(1973) classic study of psychiatric hospitals were ever rec- situation that accounted for that behavior (Gilovich, 1987).
ognized as “fakers”—respond to the apparent size and the Indeed, there is some danger that simply reading summaries
surprisingness of those effects. Note that this idea of “psy- of those studies could lead readers to draw too broad a situa-
chological” or “intuitive” effect size, which involves an tionist lesson. That is, they might overestimate the power of
evaluation of the findings in light of prior assumptions and conformity pressures, the degree of abandonment of respon-
expectations, is very different from (and often completely sibility in the “agentic state,” or the likelihood that the role
independent of) any of the measures of “statistical” effect of prison guard will prompt sadistic behavior “in general,”
size so in vogue these days. That these statistically unin- without recognizing some of the unique, and subtle, features
terpretable but psychologically powerful effects remain of the situations in question that made them so potent.
among the most widely cited in our field gives powerful As the field of social psychology evolved, a second
evidence of the importance of these more intuitive or psy- kind of non-obvious demonstration came into promi-
chological criteria. nence: studies focused on “process-relevant” measures
It should be noted that in none of the celebrated stud- or manipulations designed to demonstrate the critical
ies cited earlier did the investigators explicitly contrast the
relevant findings with lay predictions.1 Rather, in these
classic studies, the power of the situation, the channel 1 Milgram did report that, for one version of the study, psychia-
factor, the reference group, etc., was conveyed implicitly. trists failed to predict anything like the actual rates of obedience
Students and colleagues who were surprised upon read- to the bitter end that he obtained; and Bierbrauer (1979) showed
ing the results of these studies were not explicitly invited a similar failure on the part of research participants explicitly re-
to consider the power of the operative situational factors enacting the roles of “teacher” and “learner.”
Continuing Themes in Content and Methodology 11

(usually under-appreciated) role of various underlying cogni- also showed a surprising lack of interest in the conditions
tive or motivational mediators of response. This increasing under which their dependent measures, namely reported
emphasis on process, and de-emphasis on “mere demon- attitudes and beliefs, persisted over time and influenced
strations” of phenomena, was reflected in a progression of subsequent behavior. These gaps in research may be one
research strategies. The initial strategy involved the use reason why there was so little applied work in this tradition,
of internal analysis to show that phenomena and processes or alternatively the lack of interest in application may help
(for example, in group dynamics work, “pressures to uni- account for the existence of the relevant gaps. Ironically, as
formity”) become more apparent when measures of the we shall discuss later, researchers influenced by the hyper-
variables suggested by the relevant conceptual analysis cognitive tenets of self-perception theory made somewhat
and theory (notably, measures of group “cohesiveness”) more use of behavioral measures and showed more inclina-
suggested that they should. After Lewin’s death, the prime tion to do studies with clear applied implications.
strategy for the inheritors of his tradition increasingly There is another use of the 2  2 design that has become
came to involve direct manipulations of process-relevant particularly prevalent in the study of cultural differences,
variables in 2  2 designs of the sort that remain a standard but that was used much earlier to good effect by Stanley
practice in our discipline. In these designs, the objective Schachter in his once influential 1959 monograph, The
is to show that the phenomenon of interest is evident, or Psychology of Affiliation. This design “crosses” a person
perhaps dramatically evident, when, but only when, the or status variable (like birth order, or later, SES, race, or
investigator’s theory and conceptual analysis dictates that culture of origin) with the manipulation of a theoretically
it should be. relevant moderator or mediator variable (e.g., high versus
In this tradition, the ultimate achievement was a research low fear, or high versus low cultural salience). As in other
design that pitted the process of immediate interest against studies using a 2  2 design, the goal of the researcher is to
some other potential source of influence in a way that show that one of the two variables that are crossed “mat-
prompted erroneous or “opposite direction” expectations ters” only (or at least more) given one rather than the other
and predictions. The gasp of surprise from laypeople (and value of the second variable in the study’s design—in pre-
sometimes even researchers not armed with the correct cisely the manner predicted by the investigator’s theory
theory) when the data were presented, rather than mere and conceptual analysis.
acceptance of the study for publication in a leading journal, Put differently, as the foregoing discussion implies,
became the investigators’ reward. This art form reached social psychologists have long wrestled with a fundamental
its zenith in the Aronson and Mills (1959) study on the issue of self-definition and self-presentation: To what extent
effect of embarrassing versus innocuous “initiations” into is our field more akin to the physical sciences wherein the
a group on subsequent ratings of the group and its activi- goal is the development of increasingly powerful general
ties, and the Festinger and Carlsmith (1959) study of the theories and abstract statements of lawful relationships, and
effects of receiving large versus small cash payments on to what extent is the task we are engaged in more akin to
internalization of the counterattitudinal views expressed that of philosophers, and even dramatists, whose goal is
by the payment recipient. Aronson and Carlsmith’s (1963) to provide a compelling and accurate account of the behav-
study on the effects of harsh versus mild injunctions not ior of people in the context of society? Even the small
to play with a toy on the subsequent attractiveness of that sample of studies we have described thus far suggests that
forbidden toy, Lepper, Greene, and Nisbett’s (1973) study both characterizations can sometimes apply to our field.
on the undermining effects of extrinsic incentives on the Consider the extreme versions of two different meth-
intrinsic interest preschool children showed in novel art odological approaches portrayed in Table 1.1, based on
materials, and other dissonance and self-perception clas- Lepper (2009). The first approach involves formulating,
sics were in the same tradition. testing, and gradually refining general and abstract psycho-
Not sufficiently noted, or tested, in the dissonance the- logical theories, as textbook models of science describe.
ory studies described above was an implicit “tipping point” From this perspective, multiple conditions, explicit com-
hypothesis regarding the effects of “just enough” versus parisons, random assignment, and the like are crucial; and
“not quite enough” force/ incentive/ justification, etc., to details of procedure are considered largely secondary. This
produce behavior change (Lepper, 1983). In addition, while approach has generally disposed psychologists to investi-
dissonance researchers used elaborate manipulations, with gate underlying cognitive, perceptual, motivational, and
only a few exceptions (most notably those reported by social processes with, especially in more recent times, a
Zimbardo, 1969; also Marlowe, Frager, & Nuttall, 1965), heavy reliance on analyses that focus on observed correla-
they relied exclusively on rather mundane paper-and-pencil tions between outcome measures and measures of presumed
measures of dissonance reduction. The dissonance researchers mediators (see Baron & Kenny, 1986, which now has been
12 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

Table 1.1 Contrasting Models in Social Psychology Of course, these two models represent extreme cases.
“Textbook Model of Science” “Empirical Parables” Investigators whose concern was building and testing
• Theory-based hypothesis testing • Phenomenon-based
general theories often sought to show the world that their
demonstrations insights about theory allowed them to generate, and con-
• “Deductive” • “Inductive” firm, non-obvious hypotheses. This was certainly the case
• Explicit comparisons • Implicit comparisons
for the dissonance theorists (see Carlsmith, Ellsworth, &
• Method and results independent • Method and results
interdependent Aronson, 1976; Ellsworth, 2010). Indeed, most memorable
• Procedural details secondary • Procedural details critical studies in social psychology combine elements of both
• Statistical criteria of effect size • Intuitive criteria of effect size approaches, even studies that we remember mainly for
• Impartial and objective • Persuasive and subjective
• Presenting “the findings” • Telling “the story” findings that can be described in terms of simple situation-
ist influences. Thus, although the Darley and Batson study
(1973) tested three formal hypotheses about bystander
cited over 10,000 times). Unfortunately, according to some, intervention, it is most remembered for demonstrating that
it also has resulted in a greater premium being placed on the presence or absence of “extra time” could determine
studies that lend themselves to the confines of the labora- whether seminarians on their way to deliver a sermon on
tory, wherein self-report measures relevant to such medi- the Good Samaritan parable would stop to help an individ-
ators can readily be solicited, and to a decreased use of ual in obvious need of assistance. Likewise, although the
behavioral measures, especially in applied field studies famous “Bobo doll” studies conducted by Bandura, Ross,
(Baumeister, Vohs, & Funder, 2007; Cialdini, 2009).2 and Ross (1963) explicitly compared the effects of different
The second approach begins with laboratory demonstra- types of “models” and media on young children’s inclina-
tions (or “bottling”) of specific, provocative phenomena— tion to follow the aggressive example set by those models,
generally phenomena with familiar real-world analogues it too became famous not because of such comparisons but
(Abelson, Frey, & Gregg, 2004). Studies in this tradition because of the finding that “normal” middle-class children
tell a story, indeed offer a kind of “empirical parable” can readily be induced to imitate such aggressive behavior
designed to enlighten us—to hold up a kind of mirror themselves. Indeed, the same general argument applies to
that allows us to recognize something about the way we, and most of the well-known multiple-condition studies using
our neighbors, respond to particular contexts or in service “high impact” designs (Aronson & Carlsmith, 1968) and
of particular motives. In a sense, the investigator, like a consequential behavioral measures that we have described
playwright, stages a plausible brief drama (in which all above.3
roles save that of “subject” are scripted), one designed In this discussion of differing approaches, it is important
to prompt us to (re)consider our current theories about to bear in mind that in social psychology, theories, empiri-
some social behavior of consequence. Thus, procedural cal generalizations, and even accounts of phenomena are
details are critical to our evaluations of the study, and always underspecified with regard to domain of applicabil-
method and results interdependent. Readers feel impressed ity and stipulation of necessary and sufficient conditions.
and informed upon finding that their intuitions were dis- Accordingly, in both traditions, disconfirmed hypotheses
confirmed by actual results. Researchers following this or failed demonstrations are less informative than suc-
approach typically then proceed to test experimental varia- cesses. Furthermore, the design of influential experiments
tions designed to illuminate the factors most important in involves art and skill as well as correct scientific insights
producing the relevant phenomenon and those determining
or “moderating” its magnitude.
3 In the interest of historical accuracy, we should also note that
both Milgram and Asch—but not Zimbardo or Rosenhan—did
run different versions of the study and/or different conditions
2 Notwithstanding the analytic advantages of such analyses, within studies. Milgram, in fact, documented many factors (from
there is a potential problem of “over-claiming” that arises espe- the prestige of the setting to the physical remoteness of the
cially when the analyses make use of self-reports of cognitive or “teacher” vis-a-vis the “learner”) that “significantly” influenced
affective states. The limitations of such analysis in pinpointing rates of obedience; and Asch showed that whereas one variable
“underlying processes” and particularly in establishing causality (ease of discrimination task) mattered much less than one might
and should be abundantly clear in light of papers by Nisbett and imagine, another (unanimity of the confederates offering the
Wilson (1977); Wilson and Gilbert (2008); and others on the sta- wrong answer) was absolutely critical. But in each case it is the
tus of the types of self-reports that these analyses necessarily rely single one-condition effect that continues to be celebrated in our
upon (see also Spencer, Zanna, & Fong, 2005). textbooks and that most importantly challenges our intuitions.
Social History, New Challenges, and Dialectics of Social Research 13

and theories; and the impact on the field of many such set the stage for an interest in particular phenomena. Table 1.2
studies may have also reflected the investigators’ ability to notes a number of salient historical events and trends in
write persuasively and engagingly (Jordan & Zanna, 2007). American social history that we believe helped to shape
We note, too, that some classic paradigms in our field, most the agenda of American social psychologists over the past
notably both Asch’s conformity studies and Milgram’s obe- century. Thus, World War II and the Holocaust clearly stim-
dience research (which developed when Milgram was fol- ulated interest in ethnocentrism, aggression, propaganda,
lowing up on Asch’s most provocative findings), produced and group morale. McCarthyism gave ominous importance
results initially unanticipated by the investigator, who then to the study of conformity. The civil rights movement pro-
was quick to recognize the significance of those findings, vided the background for heightened interest in prejudice
and to alter the direction of his research and theorizing and racism, as well as social identity (Tajfel, 1974). Later,
accordingly. the influx of African Americans into the field helped to
shift our research focus from the mind and motives of the
perpetrators to the effects of racism on its targets.
The Vietnam War, arguably, was the impetus for increased
SOCIAL HISTORY, NEW CHALLENGES, AND
interest in problems of obedience and disobedience, sunk
DIALECTICS OF SOCIAL RESEARCH
costs, and, ultimately, minority influence (Moscovici, 1976);
and the current wars in Iraq and Afghanistan and the treat-
An ancient aphorism, as Ned Jones aptly noted, holds that
ment of suspected terrorists have clearly renewed that
social psychology is a field with a long past, but a short his-
interest. Post-Vietnam trends in American social history—the
tory. There are two major chapters in this story. The first,
inward focus of the “me” generation of the 1970s—surely
which marked the emergence more than a century ago of
played a role in our field’s increasing preoccupation with
psychology, under the leadership of Wundt, Helmholtz,
the self. In turn, both the focus on the self and misgivings
James, Hall, Cattell, Titchener, Brentano, Ebbinghaus, and
about that self-centeredness arguably played a key role in
others, as a distinct field of study, involved the shift from
the emergence of “positive” psychology. Finally, it is surely
philosophical speculation and analysis to reliance upon data.
no coincidence that cultural psychology in general, and
The second, as Jones described at length, involved the emer-
research on collectivist Asian cultures in particular, came
gence, just before and after WWII, under Lewin, Hovland,
to the forefront of our field during a period when the “eco-
Sherif, Asch, Festinger, and others, of social psychology as
nomic tigers” of East Asia (China, Japan, and Korea) began
a sub-discipline that relied on experiments in which investi-
to roar, and immigration from those nations reached new
gators directly manipulated social and situational factors of
heights.
theoretical relevance. Even within this more modern exper-
In considering the events listed in Table 1.2, the present
imental period, however, dramatic changes have occurred
authors are struck by some missed opportunities to evalu-
within social psychology—not only in the specific theories
ate the social and societal impact of “natural experiments”
and problems under study, but also in the goals and para-
wherein new technologies (television, personal computers,
digms that serve to define our field. Indeed, on balance,
credit cards) came to be widely adopted in some regions or
twentieth century social psychology seems to have been
countries before others. We write this chapter at a moment
driven as much by phenomena—both those demonstrated
when two events of historical import—the beginning of the
in the laboratory and those documented in mass media—as
Obama presidency and the worsening of the most difficult
by theories.
economic period since the Great Depression of the 1930s—
are very much on the minds of most Americans, including
Influences from Society at Large social psychologists. What impact will these events have
on what we study or on what we find when we investi-
The history and agenda of social psychology seems inex- gate racial stereotyping and “stereotype threat” or explore
tricably entwined with the history and priorities of the “risk aversion” and “loss aversion” in economic decision
American society in which it was developed. Indeed, in making? How will our research agenda, not to mention our
some instances, it is possible to identify specific events methods, be shaped by the increasing prominence of the
(e.g., the radio broadcast of the War of the Worlds, the Bay Internet and various new information-sharing and social-
of Pigs invasion of Cuba, the assassination of John Kennedy, networking technologies? Answers to these questions will
the desegregation of schools, the murder of Kitty Genovese, help shape the social psychology of the future.
or the 9/11 attacks on the United States) that have led to It should go without saying that Table 1.2 is replete
particular research programs. More typically, we see, in with oversimplifications and omissions, especially omis-
hindsight, the ways in which larger social trends or events sions involving societal changes that ebb and flow, such
14 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

Table 1.2 Historical Influences on Social Psychology


Attitudes and Attitude Social (and Self)
Era Context Groups and Group Dynamics Change Perception/Cognition
1930s Great Depression Frustration and aggression Prejudice Stereotyping
Fascism Authoritarianism: Lewin Adorno Attitudes vs. behaviors: LaPiere Empathy and accuracy
et al.
Conformity: Sherif
1940s World War II Norms and productivity: Lewin Propaganda The New Look—Perceptual biases:
Holocaust Relative deprivation: Stouffer Bruner et al.
Cooperation vs. competition: Sherif
1950s Suburbia Laboratory group dynamics: Prejudice: Allport Social comparison theory:
McCarthyism Cartwright & Zander, etc. Communication and persuasion: Festinger
Cold War Conformity: Asch Hovland, Janis, & Kelley Naïve psychology: Heider
Desegregation Affiliation: Schachter Dissonance theory: Festinger, Construal processes: Asch
Home television Game theory: Luce & Aronson
Raiffa,Thibaut & Kelley Balance and other consistency
Aggression: Bandura, etc. models: Heider, McGuire
1960s Civil Rights movement Obedience: Milgram Interactive persuasion models Two-factor model of emotion:
Vietnam War Other compliance studies Learning vs. yielding: McGuire Schachter
Recreational drugs Social facilitation: Zajonc Interpersonal attraction: Walster Self-perception theory: Bem
Intergenerational Bystander helping: Latané & & Berscheid Correspondent inferences: Jones
conflict Darley Ingratiation: Jones Attribution theory: Kelley
Deindividuation: Zimbardo
1970s “Me” generation Social loafing: Latané Stereotyping: Brewer, etc. Self-perception and attribution:
Black power Social identity: Tajfel Mere exposure: Zajonc Bem, Jones, Kelley
Rise of feminism Behavioral confirmation: Snyder Biased assimilation: Chapman Judgmental biases and heuristics:
More women working Minority influence: Moscovici & Chapman, Ross & Lepper, Kahneman & Tversky
Computers Social influence: Cialdini et al. Snyder et al. Self-determination: Deci, Lepper
Deep vs. superficial processing: Fundamental attribution error: Ross
Eagly & Chaiken, Petty & Salience, actors vs. observers:
Cacioppo Jones & Nisbett, Taylor
Social scripts: Abelson
Self-efficacy: Bandura
1980s Reaganism and Social traps, commons problem, Attitude priming: Fazio, Decision making: Kahneman &
Reaganomics Prisoner’s Dilemma: Dawes, Platt, Higgins Tversky
Personal computers etc. Psychophysiology: Cacioppo Intuitive scientist: Nisbett & Ross
End of Cold War Behavioral economics, ultimatum & Petty Self-concepts: Markus, Higgins
Breakup of USSR game, bargaining and negotiation Affect vs. cognition: Zajonc Self-affirmation: Steele
Evolution of cooperation Attitude automaticity: Zanna Automaticity: Gilbert
& Fazio Social cognition: Fiske & Taylor,
Helplessness vs. mastery Wyer & Srull
orientations: Dweck
1990s Democratization Cultural psychology: Markus & Collective representations: Priming unconscious processes:
Globalization Kitayama, Shweder, Nisbett Moscovici Bargh
Rise of East Asia Conflict resolution Stereotype threat: Steele “Ironic” processes: Wegner
Internet and cell Close relationships: Aron, Clark, Aversive racism: Dovidio & Social illusions: Taylor, etc.
phones Duck, etc. Gaertner Prospect theory: Kahneman &
Automatic vs. controlled Tversky
processing
2000s Reality television Terror management theory: Positive psychology: Diener, Self theories: Dweck
Fundamentalism Greenberg & Pyszczynski Seligman, Myers Further heuristics and biases:
9/11 and terrorism Virtual groups and relationships: Implicit attitudes: Banaji & Gilovich, etc.
Iraq and Abu Ghraib Blascovich Greenwald Affective forecasting: Wilson &
Economic meltdown Lucifer effect: Zimbardo Dynamics of race: Eberhardt, Gilbert
Markus, Richeson, Shelton Social neuroscience

as decreases and increases in overall economic prosperity and/ The most notable omission of all involves the dra-
or income inequality, unionization of the workforce, matic shifts in the demographic composition of American
availability of easy credit, rises and falls in religious society—not only the influx of immigrants from non-
observance, or the popularity of particular denominations. European countries, but the decline in the percentage of
Social History, New Challenges, and Dialectics of Social Research 15

traditional two-parent families and especially the increase targeted for work in social neuroscience (and, we antici-
in the number of elderly Americans and the increased pate, work related to energy conservation and the combat-
period of time between retirement and death. The intent ing of global warming). But too often the effects have
of our table is merely to provide some illustrations of the been negative, as in the cases of topics like interpersonal
manner in which changing external events and social trends attraction, gender studies, game theory, and many others
may influence the sorts of problems likely to become prom- that have at one time or another been made specifically
inent in the field in the years to follow. ineligible for NIH funding. Some of these ups and downs
It should be noted, as well, that this table can be mis- in funding have reflected the politics and ideology of
leading because it generally contains entries on particular particular administrations, or a willingness to cater to par-
topics only when they first emerged as a major research ticular constituencies. But in some cases the shutting off
topic. Thus, an analytic approach like Prospect Theory of funding was as baffling as it was ill advised. Consider
first came to prominence in the early 1990s, but its influ- for example, and with benefit of hindsight, the decision
ence has continued to grow in the ensuing decade. Interest to eschew funding of work on topics like the “commons
in non-conscious influences on thinking and perceiving dilemma” (Hardin, 1968)—a game-theory topic that
was strong between the two world wars, declined in the has proven to be all too relevant to our understanding of
period when psychoanalytic theory later lost its luster, but why sensible people get caught up in pyramid or “Ponzi”
was renewed when new research methods and a general schemes and fail to protect themselves against the vagaries
interest in sources of bias in judgment and decision mak- of stock market booms, bubbles, and busts.
ing came together. Likewise, the effects of external events Finally, Table 1.2 also does not include the considerable
are not always immediate, but may appear only a decade effects of the establishment of Institutional Review Boards
later. Terror management theory (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & and their accompanying regulations on research that may
Solomon, 1986) predates the 9/11 attacks but may have pose some risk of harm to participants. For example, the pub-
gained additional prominence as a result of those attacks. lication of Milgram’s first obedience studies was followed
Finally, we should note one other important class of quickly by the publication of ethical critiques of these studies,
influences not noted in Table 1.2 that have had a quite so that it became virtually impossible for others to replicate
direct impact on social psychology in the United States— (or fail to replicate) these studies within the United States
namely, changes in governmental policies concerning the (Blass, 2004; Miller, 1986). For thirty years, until Burger
funding of graduate education and of research grants in (2009) recognized the feasibility and value of replicating only
psychology. In the first instance, as Jones (1985) noted, the the initial stages of the Milgram paradigm (up to the point at
massive growth of our field (and many others) following which the “learner” first demanded to be released from the
WWII was in no small part a consequence of the provi- study, and beyond which the participants who continued to
sions of the G.I. Bill that provided funding for advanced give the next shock rarely stopped until the maximum shock
educational opportunities to veterans of that war. The fact level was reached), the only further studies on this topic were
that this bill incidentally ensured a vast overrepresentation conducted in countries without IRB review.
of men with shared military experiences entering the field Yet, in considering the role of politics in social psychol-
in the post-war period may also have had some important ogy, notwithstanding the claims of its critics, it would be
influences on the types of topics that were central to the wrong to characterize our field as one consistently dominated
field (e.g., conformity, social comparison, group productiv- by tenets of political liberalism. While many social psycholo-
ity, attitude change) and the ones that were largely ignored gists voiced liberal criticisms regarding past administrations’
(e.g., close relationships, interpersonal attraction, effects “trickle-down” economics, the conduct of war in Iraq, and the
of social support) during that era. Conversely, with later security measures taken in the name of fighting terrorism, an
universal funding of Ph.D. students, at least in most promi- earlier generation willingly helped in the design of wartime
nent psychology programs, the increasing influx of women propaganda (for example, the film series, Why We Fight) to
brought the latter topics to the fore, which in turn increased boost the morale of soldiers and increase civilian support
the attractiveness of the field for other women. for the war effort, and in the development of techniques to
Additionally, under some administrations, specific policy enhance worker productivity. Indeed, if any consistent ideo-
decisions concerning the eligibility of many areas of social logical strain exists in applied social psychology, it is one
psychology research or graduate training in social psychol- best characterized as a combination of reformism and prag-
ogy for federal funding through the NIH have also had matism—a combination that sometimes makes for strained
powerful and immediate effects on the types of problems relationships. In particular, social psychological expertise is
and paradigms likely to achieve prominence. These effects welcomed when we help to document need for intervention
can be positive, as in the case of recent increases in funding and suggest specific, theory-based, intervention strategies.
16 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

But our input is less welcome when we advocate formal experi- cognitive analyses of Bem, Jones, Kelley, and the other self-
mental evaluation designs rather than the subjective impressions perception and attribution theorists, the work of two Israeli
of participants to document program effectiveness. The con- psychologists, Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky, on
tributions and challenges of intervention work will receive “heuristics of judgment” (Kahneman & Tversky, 1972, 1973;
more extended discussion in the final section of this chapter. Tversky & Kahneman, 1973, 1974) began to make its influ-
ence felt. Within a decade, their papers in the judgment and
Influences from Psychology in General decision-making tradition were among the most frequently
cited by social psychologists, and their indirect influence on
While sociopolitical factors have helped to shape social
the content and direction of our field was even greater than
psychology, its concerns and methodologies have also been
could be discerned from any citation index.
influenced by changes taking place in other fields of psy-
The three heuristics they proposed—which involved the
chology. Interestingly, however, in the first decades of the
inferential use of cognitive salience or “availability,” simi-
modern experimental era in social psychology, it was social
larity or “representativeness,” and initial “anchors”—were
psychology’s relative independence from developments in
not irrational or unreasonable. On the contrary, in many
the mainstreams of the field that was most notable. While
judgment contexts they led to accurate or at least “good
many areas of psychology became steeped in psychoana-
enough” assessments. However, their use, especially at
lytic theory in the 1920s and remained so well past WWII,
the cost of ignoring more normative strategies or readily
social psychology remained largely unaffected by Freudian
accessible sources of accurate estimation, disposed users to
thought. Likewise, while general experimental psychology
make erroneous judgments of frequency, likelihood, and the
during those decades came under the sway of “behaviorism”
relative contribution of causal candidates. Indeed, they can
and various conditioning models, social psychology did
even lead us to commit striking violations of formal norma-
not succumb to the reductionism of Watson, Pavlov, Hull,
tive standards such as failure to give weight to probative
Skinner, and company. Instead, with only a few exceptions
“base-rate” information and, most dramatically, violation
(Lott & Lott, 1985; Staats, 1975; Staats & Staats, 1963), the
of the obvious logical truism that the intersection of two
field remained resolutely cognitive. Indeed, social psychol-
sets (e.g., feminist bank tellers) cannot be larger than one
ogy’s insistence on the central role of “meaning” attached
of the constituent sets (e.g., bank tellers). Social psycholo-
to potential stimuli, responses, reinforcers, and ultimately
gists were quick to recognize the significance of this work
more complex social contexts, constitutes one of its most
for understanding biases in the attribution process and other
powerful continuing legacies.4
shortcomings in the way laypeople perform other tasks of
Only decades later during the cognitive revolution—as
intuitive psychology such as prediction, inductive general-
the computer metaphor for mind replaced the array of prior
ization, and updating of theories and beliefs (Kahneman,
hydraulic models of needs, drives, and tension systems—
Slovic, & Tversky, 1982; Nisbett & Ross, 1980).
did social psychology change substantially as a function of
Slightly later, Kahneman and Tversky made a second
developments in other areas of psychology. In particular,
major contribution by exploring the effects of non-normative
soon after Lewinian equilibria, Festingerian pressures toward
influences not on inference and judgment but on decision
cognitive consistency, and the dynamic force-field models of
making. Their ground-breaking papers on Prospect Theory
Asch and Milgram had been supplanted by the more purely
(Kahneman & Tversky, 1979, 1984) had a profound effect
on young behavioral economists. In fact, their contributions,
along with those of Daniel Ariely, Robyn Dawes, Baruch
4 The ambitious and highly influential work on persuasion Fischhoff, Robert Frank, Tom Gilovich, Dale Griffin, Chip
by Carl Hovland and his Yale colleagues paid lip service to Health, George Loewenstein, Paul Slovic, Richard Thaler,
the behaviorist tradition in learning and memory. But their and other researchers whose work lay at the intersection of
systematic elaboration of factors (source, content, recipient, and social and cognitive psychology, created an important inter-
potential consequence of the persuasive message) actually made disciplinary field of study (Gilovich, Griffin, & Kahneman,
little use of the relevant theoretical machinery. Moreover, contem-
2002). New work in this field, described in more detail later
porary critics such as Solomon Asch (1952) were quick to point
in this chapter, has important applied implications both for
out that the putative source of a message changed the way that
message was understood or “construed”—in a famous example,
everyday decisions that people make about expenditures
that “rebellion” is a very different “object of judgment,” which of time and money and for the formulation and framing of
in turn leads to a very different degree of acceptance, when the social policy—implications recognized by the awarding of
quotation advocating it is attributed to its actual source, Thomas the 2002 Nobel Prize in economics to Daniel Kahneman
Jefferson, than when the same quotation is falsely attributed to (unfortunately, too late for it to be shared with his lifelong
the Communist leader V. I. Lenin. friend and collaborator, Amos Tversky, who died in 1996).
Social History, New Challenges, and Dialectics of Social Research 17

Contemporaneously, new approaches to social cognition have come from within the field, as researchers confronted
(Fiske & Taylor, 1984) came into prominence. Some research- a changing series of challenges regarding the field’s appro-
ers applied the methods and measures of cognitive psychology priate content and “mission.” As both Jones (1985) and
and cognitive science (e.g., reaction time and eye tracking) to the Allport (1954a) noted, the challenge in the field’s prehis-
study of social perception, social memory, and social information tory was the lack of separate identity, with distinct theories
processing (Hastie et al., 1980; Wyer & Carlson, 1979; Wyer & and methods—a challenge that was met most successfully
Srull, 1989). Others went even farther afield to borrow various by Lewin and his students with the emergence of distinct
brain-imaging techniques (fMRI, ERP, etc.) from the burgeoning methodologies, “middle-range” theories (most loosely
field of neuroscience, creating the new area of “social neurosci- adapted from field theory, with its emphasis on oppos-
ence” (Cacioppo et al., 2007; Cacioppo & Berntson, 1992) and the ing forces and equilibrium states), and a “can-do” spirit.
even more recent offshoot known as “social cognitive neurosci- Moreover, social psychology has remained eclectic and
ence” (both Blascovich & Mendes and Lieberman, this volume; interdisciplinary, taking “useful” theories and methods
Ochsner & Lieberman, 2001). Again, a more detailed discussion from wherever they could be found in a way that gave it
of these developments will appear later in this chapter. a clear identity (see Asch, 1952). But the ensuing decades
have lacked neither attacks from without and within the
Influences Within Social Psychology field nor soul-searching on the part of even its leaders.
However, there are also some enduring philosophical
Perhaps the most important influences that have shaped issues or polarities that have characterized intellectual
social psychology over the last eight decades, however, inquiry over the ages and across many disciplines. Are

Table 1.3 Prominent Research Traditions in Social Psychology


Big Issues and/or
Distinctive
Dominant Traditions Characteristics Methodologies
Group Dynamics Practicality Field experiments vs. correlations
1935-1950 Obviousness Bottling real-world phenomena
Scientism Cover stories, confederates, covert measures
Internal analysis to assess mediation
Yale School—Attitudes Rationality of persuasion Persuasion experiments—In the lab: Captive audience, high transparency
1950-1960 Obviousness Primarily self-report attitude measures
Festinger/Schachter Irrationality Complex experimental dramas in the lab
1955-1970 Non-obviousness 2  2 designs to assess mediation
“Frivolity” Counterintuitive predictions/findings/presentations
Ethical concerns Often opposite direction of effects
High Impact Era Good vs. evil Experimental dramas as above and
1965-1975 Ethical concerns Counterintuitive predictions—But “size,” not direction, of effects at issue
(Milgram, Cialdini, Often person  situation designs
Latané & Darley, etc.) Pure cases of demonstration experiments
Attribution and Self- Rationality vs. bias in Initially hypothetical paper-and-pencil manipulations
Perception Theories inferences and measures; concern with accuracy
1965-1990 Later, more behavioral measures; concern with biases and applications
Social Cognition High experimental precision Cognitive psychology methods/concepts
1970-1995 Low social relevance Reaction time/mental chronometry/imagery prototypes/multidimensional scaling, etc.
Hypothesis-testing, emphasis on precise theories
Explicit statistical analysis to assess mediation
Judgment and Decision Rationality of choice and Gedanken experiments with real data (some almost
Making inference did not need subjects)
1975-2000 Specific normative standards and claims
Non-conscious Processing Automaticity vs. Non-conscious priming/framing manipulations
1990-2005 deliberative control (eliminating problems of “demand”)
Implicit vs. explicit attitudes—Controlled vs. automatic processing
Cultural Social Universality vs. parochiality Cross-cultural experiments
Psychology Initially, exported American paradigms and main-effect findings
1990-2005 Later, culture  manipulation designs and interaction predictions
Social Neuroscience Localization in brain Brain-imaging techniques—EEG, fMRI, MEG, PET
2000– and other neuroscience techniques
18 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

people generally “good” or “evil,” rational or irrational, could bring important real-world problems and issues into
independent or communal, and so forth? Or, more reason- the laboratory where they could be systematically studied
ably, in what situations does one pole or another become and their causes investigated. These early experimental-
most powerful? Table 1.3 lists a progression of “domi- ists sought, in the terms of later commentators, to “bottle”
nant” research traditions and suggests some of the larger complex phenomena they had observed in the world (e.g.,
issues that the content or methodology of each tradition the effects of democratic versus autocratic leadership,
sought or failed to address. We trust that the reader will intergroup conflict, displays of racial prejudice, or success-
recognize their relevance when we describe some of the ful versus unsuccessful media appeals) in a form simple
shifts in research fashion and emphasis over the decades. enough to be amenable to experimental manipulation and
We also trust that it will be also apparent that whenever systematic study. As a result, the findings of such studies
our field becomes preoccupied with investigations focused were typically “scientific” demonstrations of phenomena
on one “pole” or the other—for example, human “ratio- that had already been observed and identified in the world
nality” versus “irrationality” or the essential “goodness” at large.
versus “evil” of human nature—there comes a point of Eventually, however, announcements that social psy-
diminishing returns whereby the payoffs for investigators chologists had shown that people are generally more
willing to explore the opposite pole become increasingly persuaded by communicators that they find trustworthy,
attractive. Moreover, events in the world outside academia expert, similar, and likable (Hovland and associates), that
may accelerate the speed and extent of “yin-to-yang” more cohesive groups usually have more powerful effects
movements in the field that might start for other reasons on their members (Lewin and colleagues), that people
(McGuire, 1973). typically compare themselves to others who are like them
Likewise, whenever one set of methods or paradigms, (Festinger), or that White Americans hold prejudiced atti-
with its various expedient compromises and choices in tudes about non-White Americans and “foreigners,” were
terms of balancing desire for control of variables against received less enthusiastically and, with a few rare excep-
that of capturing real-world complexity and consequential tions, had less of an impact on the world at large than
responses, comes to dominate our field for a period of time, the authors might have wished. Indeed, some researchers
the use of alternative procedures and designs (calling for working in that era can recall the skeptical, if not derisive,
different trade-offs and compromises) gains in value, and reactions (e.g., “So, they really pay you to do that kind of
attractiveness. And, typically, the results from such shifts research, huh?”) that sometimes greeted their efforts. Such
in methodology, like shifts in research emphasis, include reactions proved particularly galling in cases where their
findings that raise new issues and challenge the wisdom of listeners’ wisdom sometimes seemed largely the product of
the day. Recognizing that any analysis of shifting research what we now would call “hindsight bias,” emerging only
procedures and priorities necessarily entails oversimplifi- after the results in question had been described (Fischhoff,
cations, let us try to identify some of the salient “problems” 1975, 1982).
(which at times have escalated to the point of crisis) that In any discussion of obvious versus non-obvious
social psychology has faced over the past eight decades. findings, it is important to note that, notwithstanding
the conventions of statistical analysis, merely rejecting
The Problem of Obviousness the null hypothesis that a particular manipulation exerts
Perhaps the first major “crisis” faced by social psychologists absolutely no effect on a given outcome measure, or
in the post-war period concerned the seeming obviousness that particular factors are not significantly associated
of most experimental results. Whether discussing the early with particular outcomes, is rarely if ever the real goal
Lewinian research on group dynamics or the early efforts of of researchers when they report t-scores, F-ratios, cor-
Hovland and his associates investigating the determinants relation coefficients, and the like (Cohen, 1990). In fact,
of attitude change, there came a time in the mid-1950s at the unstated convention among most social psycholo-
which it was easy to parody the efforts of even the best gists has been to avoid using overly large sample sizes
and the brightest investigators as nothing more than prov- in a calculated effort to make effects of trivial magnitude
ing things “we knew all along,” or as some commentators reach statistical significance. Instead, the researchers
uncharitably put it, documenting “Bubba-psychology,” or generally seek to show that the manipulations or asso-
things our grandmothers already knew (without even a ciation in question also matter in terms of pragmatic and
“fancy college degree”) to be true. social significance (Abelson, 1995). One way to do this
In those earliest days of experimental social psychol- is demonstrate that a suitable manipulation and/or mea-
ogy, one central goal of investigators was to show that they sure allows us to influence and/or predict some outcome
Social History, New Challenges, and Dialectics of Social Research 19

that people care about. Another is to show that particular made explicit (and any potential hindsight claims to the
factors or influences are more powerful and important contrary made difficult) became so widespread that stu-
(or in some cases less so)—either in absolute terms or dents’ erroneous intuitions in the classroom were later
relative to other factors—than recognized by lay social cited by some in an (unsuccessful) attempt to rebut Bem’s
observers or even supposed experts.5 As noted earlier, alternative, self-perception interpretation of these disso-
this latter strategy was at the heart of most of the two- nance studies.
condition and one-condition demonstration experiments To some extent the fashion for counterintuitive dem-
discussed above. onstrations in the Festingerian mode waned for predict-
able reasons. Commonsense understandings of behavior
The Appearance of Frivolity are, on the whole, reasonably accurate, at least in terms
After a decade of frustration trying to convince the outside of the “direction” of predictions if not in the calibration
world of the significance and the novelty of their research, of effect sizes. The number of domains in which it is
it was hardly surprising that many investigators found possible to show genuinely counterintuitive between-
themselves drawn to theories and paradigms capable of condition differences or correlations is limited; and the
generating surprising or counterintuitive research findings. kind of training and talent required to do such studies is
In this tradition, as noted earlier, the goal of researchers in short supply. Furthermore, once a number of exam-
went beyond simply illustrating the larger-than-expected ples of specific counterintuitive findings in a particular
impact of particular factors and manipulations. The premium domain have been reported and explained, the intuitions
was on demonstrating something that not only defied the of one’s professional colleagues begin to change. What
expectations of the proverbial person in the street, but that had once been deemed surprising comes to be expected,
also came as a surprise to one’s fellow social psycholo- especially when more familiar examples of the princi-
gists. For a decade and a half, beginning in the mid-1950s, ples are brought to mind. (The idea that people try to jus-
this search for non-intuitive findings was refined into a tify their bad decisions to themselves as well as others,
high art form by the intellectual heirs of the Lewinian tra- that they particularly value things they worked hard for
dition (most notably Festinger, in his dissonance theory or sacrificed to obtain, or that they engage in other types
research, but also Schachter, Aronson, and other students of after-the-fact rationalizations hardly began with Leon
in that tradition). Festinger!)
In each of the major “insufficient justification” paradigms But there was another set of factors in the late 1960s
(e.g., Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959; Aronson & Mills, 1959; and early 1970s that made studies in this tradition lose
Aronson & Carlsmith, 1963), for instance, the clear intent favor. Critics both within and outside of the field increas-
of the investigators, evident even in the way that they ingly began to question the status of this sort of research as
described their studies, was to convince readers that there “serious” science. To their detractors, the dissonance
was good reason to expect results precisely opposite in researchers were seen as promoting cleverness for its own
direction from those about to be reported. Whether in jour- sake, at the expense of their young research participants
nal articles or in classroom presentations, readers or listen- (who were subjected to heavy deception and, in a sense,
ers were led to think about a study’s procedures in terms of made to look foolish). When Aronson and Carlsmith con-
some alternative model (usually a vague version of “rein- cluded their 1968 Handbook chapter on methodology by
forcement theory”) in a way that heightened the surprise urging readers to remember that research in social psy-
value of the actual results obtained. Indeed, the practice chology “is, and ought to be, fun,” their words captured
of asking students in a class to predict the outcome of dis- exactly what the critics decried. Against the backdrop of
sonance studies, so that their mistaken intuitions could be the Vietnam War and the widespread rebellion of college

5 While a more complete and nuanced discussion of this issue cannot be undertaken here, it should be understood that the relative impor-
tance of statistical effect size and the specifics of the outcome measure employed, as opposed to simple rejection of the null hypothesis,
obviously depends on the particulars of the research problem and mode of operationalization. Whether a manipulation of fear makes
people sit a mean of 6.9 inches or 9.6 inches closer together is less important than the demonstration that fear increases the desire for
closeness. By contrast, demonstrating that people are “significantly” influenced by perceived norms in a laboratory context that called
for them to sign a petition about water conservation is less noteworthy, and useful, than demonstrating that a manipulation of perceived
norms regarding lawn watering decreased water use in a given neighborhood by 20%—especially when the study includes evidence that
additional prosocial appeals were notably less effective.
20 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

students against established hierarchy, loftier goals and played a role in the rapid ascendance of new theories and
greater seriousness of purpose seemed called for.6 paradigms that required neither deception nor theatrical
It is easy, in retrospect, to defend the Festingerians and skills. These theories sought to explain phenomena initially
to celebrate their contributions to both theory and method- considered profoundly social, motivational, and non-
ology. William McGuire’s (1967) wry comment at the time conscious in more exclusively individual, cognitive, and
(borrowed from H. L. Mencken) that the critics resembled conscious terms. Perhaps most prominent was the rapid
the Puritans of old who seemed to be worried that “some- spread of interest, noted earlier, in Bem’s (1965, 1967)
where, somehow, someone might be enjoying himself” highly “behavioristic” formulation of self-perception the-
(p. 137) does not seem far off the mark. But the curious ory (which constituted a kind of “hostile takeover” of the
distaste of the dissonance researchers for application, dissonance theory enterprise) and in Kelley’s (1967, 1972)
despite their Lewinian heritage, merits some comment, rather abstract attribution theory. Interest in these models
especially since many of the most promising young and related work by Jones on “correspondent inference”
social psychologists of the time eventually moved from (Jones & Davis, 1965) and Ross (1977) on “the fundamen-
mainstream research into “do-gooder” kinds of applied tal attribution error” eventually led to the more general
work—promoting energy conservation, fostering good exploration of errors and biases in lay psychology.
health habits regarding food and exercise, combating Such concerns also dovetailed with increased attention
prejudice, serving disadvantaged students, and trying at this time to the ethics of social research and the devel-
to promote other socially desirable (to liberals) types of opment of Institutional Review Boards. Some researchers
behavior change. The failure of Lewin’s immediate intel- objected to specific research paradigms and programs in
lectual descendants to apply dissonance theory, with its social psychology, especially Milgram’s studies of obedi-
obvious debt to Lewinian tension-system theorizing, at a ence to authority (Baumrind, 1964; Miller, 1986; Orne &
time when a crucial issue of the day—racial integration of Holland, 1968; Rubin & Moore, 1971). Some objected
schools and neighborhoods—constituted an immense exer- to any research that used complex deceptions (Kelman,
cise in forced compliance, and hoped-for attitude change, 1968). In the process, various other methodologies, many
is especially puzzling. (Only later did several prominent focused on various forms of role-playing, were champi-
Lewinians—Elliot Aronson and Phil Zimbardo, along with oned as potential alternatives to deception research. The
Morton Deutsch and Hal Gerard—turn their attention to net result, however, was the virtual elimination of further
the analysis of important social issues like school integra- research on obedience to authority and other ethically sus-
tion, social isolation, alienation, and violence.) pect paradigms. While such review processes are necessary,
In any case, for young researchers committed to theory and researchers must take responsibility for the well-being
building and basic research, disenchantment with para- of participants in their research, overly zealous IRBs may
digms involving complex deceptions and laboratory dramas well have had a chilling effect on researchers who would
seek to employ manipulations and measures that have real
psychological stakes for the participants—one requisite
6A very different questioning of the intellectual “seriousness” of first step on the rocky road from laboratory exploration
the experimental tradition more generally came from Kenneth to field demonstration and then to significant real-world
Gergen (1973), Rom Harré (1979), and other European scholars application.
who viewed the field’s limitations through the lens of decon-
A Seeming Lack of Social Relevance
structionism. Many of their observations and criticisms were
cogent (and consistent with our characterization of landmark As social psychology was embracing attribution theory
social psychology experiments as empirical parables subject to and beginning to explore biases in inference and judgment,
interpretation, indeed changing interpretation, in light of social experimental psychology was well into its own “cognitive
and scientific developments). However, this critique, as Gergen revolution.” With these developments in cognitive psychol-
(1996) later acknowledged, ultimately, did little to change meth-
ogy came an array of new tools, methods, and measures,
odology or even the way in which findings are characterized.
which quickly migrated into social psychology, culminating
This lack of impact may be attributed in part to the failure of the
in the development of the broad new field of social cognition
methods of inquiry the critics favored to produce much in the way
of new, interesting, and impactful findings. But it also may reflect (Fiske & Taylor, 1984; Wyer & Carlson, 1979). The power
the continuing, indeed ever increasing, capacity of mainstream of these new methods was undeniable, and social cognition
theories and approaches to offer useful insights regarding con- became a major focus of the field. However, this emphasis
temporary social issues and problems and to prompt successful on cognitive processes, and the particular research para-
interventions of the sort to be described in the concluding section digms and measures employed, came at a cost. With some
of this chapter. notable exceptions, such as the work of Bob Zajonc (1980)
Social History, New Challenges, and Dialectics of Social Research 21

on the “primacy of affect,” processes involving motivation soon had social psychologists writing about loss aversion,
and emotion were, for some time, largely neglected. In reference points, and especially “framing.” Demonstrations
fact, behavior itself (beyond checking points on question- that the attractiveness of an option or gamble depended not
naire scales or pushing buttons) and strategies for changing only on its objective features but also on its description
behavior ceased to be a primary concern. (e.g., its “framing” in terms of prospective losses relative
During this period, the research agenda of much of the to one reference point rather than prospective gains with
field, and the findings that were winning places in our respect to another reference point) were particularly wel-
leading journals, seemed far removed from the practical comed in light of the field’s subjectivist tradition.
applications and real-world problems that had so occupied
The Question of Parochialism
the attention of researchers in the early days of the field.
To some critics, the topics being investigated seemed too In 1991, during an era of unprecedented globalization and the
narrow and too esoteric to interest anyone besides other breathtaking rise of the Asian tigers (China, Japan, and Korea),
academic social psychologists. The fact that many of the Hazel Markus and Shinobu Kitiyama published a paper
research paradigms in common use involved paper-and- that challenged the assumption that what most social psy-
pencil assessments regarding hypothetical social stimuli chologists in the Western world were studying was the way
and social situations, rather than consequential behavioral people think and act in social contexts—as opposed to the
responses to “real” stimuli and situations, was a cause way people in the West think and act, and the demands and
for concern. Still others worried that some of the distinc- constraints of Western social contexts. In so doing, they
tively “social” aspects of social psychology could not be not only made us more aware of the smug parochialism
addressed by paradigms adopted from cognitive psychol- that characterized much of our work (a criticism voiced
ogy, whereby individual subjects sat alone in front of a by Gergen, 1973, and others almost two decades earlier),
computer—that such subjects were being left, as critics they also laid out fertile terrain for new research and for
had earlier said of E. C. Tolman’s rats, “lost in thought.” reconsideration of some of the most firmly established
Even in areas of psychology where information-processing ideas in dissonance theory, attribution theory, and the study
models seemed appropriate and of obvious applied rele- of inference, judgment, and decision making. Their paper,
vance, the products of investigators’ increasingly skilled which remains one of the most heavily cited in our field,
labors were not bearing fruit of a sort likely to prove appe- came at a propitious time in another respect as well; for it
tizing to those outside the field. Perhaps inevitably, the coincided with and helped to prompt the rapid influx of
evidence-based answer to many pressing applied questions young researchers from East Asian cultures into American
proved to be “It all depends; it’s actually very compli- universities and social psychology programs.
cated . . . .” For instance, while once it had seemed that There had, of course, been previous efforts in “cross-
the generally positive effects of communicator credibility cultural” research, including attempts to replicate famous
on persuasion provided a solid building block for success- findings, and to compare the behavior of people from dif-
ful persuasion, it became increasingly apparent that the ferent societies in familiar research paradigms involving
effects of even this most obvious variable depended on its conformity, obedience to authority, bystander intervention,
interaction with a host of other variables, including per- and even dissonance reduction following free choice or
ceived motives of the communicator, discrepancy of the forced compliance. The central distinction that Markus and
message from the initial position of the audience, timing Kitiyama made between independent and interdependent
of the information about the communicator’s credibility, cultures had even been anticipated in the extensive pro-
the personal relevance of the issue to the audience, and so gram of research conducted by Harry Triandis on individu-
forth (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993; McGuire, 1969; Petty & alist versus collectivist societies (Triandis, 1968). But the
Cacioppo, 1986). specific targets of their research, especially their focus on
Two notable exceptions to this trend were mentioned situational versus dispositional attribution and their chal-
earlier and will be discussed at more length later. One, iron- lenge to the notion that dispositionism was a property of
ically, involved developments that began with attribution basic cognitive, perceptual, and motivational processes—
theory—a highly abstract theory, or rather a set of questions and the idea that the “fundamental” attribution error was
about which to theorize, which, to the surprise of many not all that fundamental—could not be ignored once the
who initially found it formulaic and even a bit boring, later research evidence began to mount.
became extremely important in several applied domains.
The Issue of Negativity
The other exception involved the meteoric rise to promi-
nence of work on judgment and decision making, wherein One of the more heartening developments over the past
the tenets of Kahneman and Tversky’s Prospect Theory decade or so has been the emergence of an empirical
22 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

subdiscipline that has been labeled “positive psychol- sentiments, and actions as coherent and consistent with a
ogy” (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). While, as is positive view of the self; and (4) the impact of expectations
the case with most such “new” developments, one can and beliefs on social outcomes—in particular, the role of
cite many antecedents—from the humanistic psychology confirmation biases and self-fulfilling prophesies.
of Abraham Maslow and others to B. F. Skinner’s contro- Each insight on this short list has, of course, prompted
versial fictional 1948 account of a behaviorist utopia in a great deal of provocative research and theorizing. But
Walden Two—this development has been dramatic. The our choices also reflect our judgments about the particular
emergence of positive psychology was to some extent a value of these insights in analyzing and addressing con-
natural reaction to the endless stream of distressing devel- temporary social issues and problems. Colleagues with dif-
opments outside psychology that were discussed earlier. It ferent tastes, priorities, and backgrounds might well offer
was also a reaction not only to demonstrations of nega- a somewhat different list—one that included, for instance,
tive behaviors in some of the demonstration classics that appreciation of the extent to which the ways that people
we noted earlier, but to the drumbeat of findings regarding feel, think, and act have been shaped by evolutionary pro-
biases and failings of rationality in judgment and decision cesses, the importance of non-conscious processes, or the
making, to say nothing of the continuing focus on labora- “primacy” of affect (or motivational goals). We would have
tory and real-world studies of racism, sexism, justifications no quarrel with such inclusions. Indeed, in our later review
of inequality, and the like. of important recent contributions to theory and research,
Finally, it may be that empirically trained psychologists we will discuss each of these insights.
tired of the stream of hard-cover and paperback books in
the “Human Potential” movement that, despite their lack Naïve Realism and Its Consequences
of intellectual or scientific rigor, and generally despite the
complete absence of data, outsold even the most influen- Individuals’ understandings of their everyday interactions
tial works in mainstream social psychology. Certainly, the with each other, and of their experiences with objects and
singular efforts of a few individuals (most notably Martin events, inevitably begins with their tacit assumption of
Seligman) who persuaded younger colleagues to focus isomorphism between their subjective perceptions—and
their work on happiness, creativity, altruism, morality, and even their judgments, inferences, and affective reactions—
other ennobling topics, and secured funding for empirical and some objective reality. Although typically unexamined
research and conferences, provided a vital catalyst. In any and underappreciated, such naïve realism seems fundamen-
case, this area, discussed in more detail later in this chapter, tal and universal—almost certainly a product of very basic
has clearly been a “bandwagon” not only in terms of journal sensory and cognitive processes (Griffin & Ross, 1991).
publications but also as a source of books (Gilbert, 2006; Any introductory philosophy class will inevitably
Haidt, 2006; Keltner, 2009; Lyubomirsky, 2007) combin- challenge students to appreciate the extent to which their
ing solid empirical work with lively real-world observa- knowledge of the objective world is at best indirect and
tions in a way that has attracted popular interest. mediated, that perceived properties of objects are products
of the interaction between mind and matter—and that, as
Einstein noted, “reality” as we experience it through our
FOUR INSIGHTS UNDERLYING RESEARCH particular sensory system is, in an important sense, an
AND APPLICATION “illusion.” Indeed, any college physics class will oblige
students to think about matter, energy, space, and even the
There are four hard-won insights about human perception, passage of time in ways that challenge ordinary experience,
thinking, motivation, and behavior that we believe represent to say nothing of theories at the cutting edge of modern
important, indeed foundational, contributions of social psy- physics (such as the aforementioned “string theory,” which
chology. These involve (1) the existence and implications postulates many dimensions beyond the standard four of
of the epistemic stance of “naïve realism” or the assump- a Newtonian or Einsteinian world) that utterly defy such
tion of isomorphism between subjective experience and experience. But neither classroom lessons nor mathematics
objective reality; (2) the existence and implications of “lay (nor electron microscopes, fMRI magnets, or other scien-
dispositionism,” or more generally, the tendency of social tific instruments) change our subjective experience of reality.
actors and observers to attribute actions and outcomes to Nor do they prevent us from continuing to assume that the
the attributes of the actor (or entity that is moving or chang- perceptions guiding our everyday actions are essentially
ing) rather than the field of forces influencing the relevant faithful reflections of reality.
actor or entity; (3) the existence and implications of social Such an assumption of isomorphism no doubt is highly
actors’ inclination to see their own beliefs, assessments, functional in helping us navigate through the physical
Four Insights Underlying Research and Application 23

world. When it comes to social dealings, however, there is dispositional inferences about other actors (another central
a cost to that assumption—one of obvious concern to social insight to be discussed next), especially those who respond
psychologists. In analyzing and interpreting the words and differently from them, and therefore differently from what
deeds of their peers, even the most skilled “intuitive psy- seems warranted by the demands and constraints of the situ-
chologists” fail to appreciate the extent to which they, no ation (Ross & Ward, 1996). Closely related is the tendency
less than their peers, see actions, events, and even facts for actors and observers to offer divergent attributions for
through the lenses of their own experience, the received the same actions and outcomes (Jones & Nisbett, 1972)—
wisdom of their group, and the often-distorting influence with observers attributing actions and outcomes to inferred
of their personal needs, hopes, and fears. The early clas- stable dispositions that the actors themselves attribute to
sic study by Hastorf and Cantril (1954), on the differing situational factors, including specific goals and obstacles or
perceptions of Dartmouth and Princeton students watching constraints blocking the achievement of those goals. Other
the same football game through the prisms of their rival phenomena in which naïve realism can be expected to play
partisanships, reflected a radical view of the “constructive” a role include overconfident social predictions (Dunning,
nature of perception that anticipated later discussions of Griffin, Milojkovic, & Ross, 1990) and the failure to give
naïve realism. So did the following, very modern sounding, assessments and judgments by one’s peers as much weight
quote from Ichheiser (1949): as one’s own.
We earlier noted social psychology’s core message that
We tend to resolve our perplexity arising out of the experience people respond to subjective rather than objective reality.
that other people see the world differently than we see it our- Indeed, the determinants of social perception and social
selves by declaring that those others, in consequence of some cognition, and the various biases distorting these processes
basic intellectual and moral defect are unable to see things “as
have preoccupied investigators from at least the era of Asch,
they really are” and to react to them in a “normal way.” We thus
Heider, and Ichheiser. Here we may also note evidence that
imply of course that things are in fact as we see them, and that
our ways are the normal ways. (p. 39, emphasis added)
most laypeople recognize many of the biases we study, but
they consistently believe that those biases apply to oth-
Likewise, the following even older, but no less penetrat- ers more than to themselves. In fact, the amount of bias
ing quote from Ben Franklin (1787, quoted in Copeland, they attribute to a given individual proves a direct function
Lamm, & McKenna, 1999): of the amount of disagreement between that individual and
themselves (Pronin, Gilovich, & Ross, 2004). This essen-
Most men, indeed, as well as most sections in religion, think tial insight regarding the nature and impact of naïve realism
themselves in possession of all truth, and that [to the extent is of particular applied significance in understanding and
that] others differ from them, it is so far error. addressing interpersonal and intergroup conflict. Even
friends and intimate partners are apt to feel in contexts of
The most obvious consequence of naïve realism is that disagreement that the problem is the other person’s lack
people expect other reasonable and objective perceivers to of objectively and, in cases of “hurt feelings,” either the
share their views and understandings—if not immediately, insensitivity of the person who has hurt them or the hyper-
then at least after they have enlightened those others about sensitivity of the person accusing them of the relevant
the way things really are. Hence, one should expect false abuse. When problems escalate to the point where some
consensus and related egocentric biases in social percep- third party is consulted, both parties proceed in the vain
tion (see Ross, Greene, & House, 1977; also review by hope and expectation that the counselor will see that their
Marks & Miller, 1987). Moreover, people will attribute view is the reasonable one, and that it is their partner who
bias and other malignant influences when others, including needs to be “set straight.”
third-party mediators or those providing media coverage Most disputants truly feel that their side is “right,” that
on issues of great concern to them, fail to share their “take” the other side is “wrong” (knowingly wrong and insincere,
on issues and events. One dramatic example of this phenom- or perhaps even worse, sincere but deluded and blinded by
enon was provided by Vallone, Ross, and Lepper (1985), their biases). Partisans also sincerely feel that “objective”
who showed pro-Israeli and anti-Israeli viewers samples of third parties should take their side. Naïve realism has rele-
the actual media coverage of the attack by Falangist gunmen vance as well for issues involving friction between different
on the Sabra and Chatilla refugee camps and found that both cultures or subcultures (wherein each makes invidious com-
groups of partisan viewers overwhelmingly saw that coverage parisons between “our” ways of looking at the world and
as slanted in favor of the “other side.” deciding what is natural, good, moral, and enlightened, ver-
Another phenomenon arising at least partially from naïve sus “their” ways). The same invidious “us” versus “them”
realism is the tendency for people to make unwarranted comparisons arise with respect to matters of race, gender,
24 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

and class as well. It is rarely possible to get people to rec- Lay dispositionism obviously also relates obliquely to
ognize fully the degree to which their own perceptions the question of “nature versus nurture,” and not so obliquely
and assessments are subject to “top-down” influences and to debates about the relative power of situational versus
biases of the same sort, and to the same degree, as those dispositional factors—debates prompted in part by the
that influence others from different backgrounds. relative paucity of findings showing larger-than-expected
Academics in the humanities and social sciences may predictive power of familiar, trait-like, personality variables.
pay lip service to such recognition in their writing and theo- As alluded to earlier, the debate is intractable, indeed mean-
rizing, but it does not prevent them from opining about mat- ingless, when it is discussed in the abstract, rather than
ters of public concern with unwarranted certainty, and too with respect to particular personal and situational factors.
often with scorn for those who disagree. Moreover, when Indeed, a review of our situationist classics reveals that
people recognize that they are seeing issues and events most of these studies involved a limited number of very
through the prism of their personal experiences or group specific sources of influence, most notably, channel fac-
identity, they insist that those influences are a source of tors and sources of actual and/or inferred group norms and
enlightenment, while regarding such influences on others as standards.
a source of distortion. In the context of intergroup conflict, The much larger set of studies potentially relevant to our
perhaps the best one can hope for is a willingness to attribute present discussion are demonstrations that, in the labora-
disagreements to sources of bias that are “normal”—that tory and in everyday life, the behavior of a given individual
is, products of motivational and cognitive processes such with respect to willingness to take risks or delay gratifi-
as wishful thinking and biased assimilation of information cation or to display cooperativeness or altruism (or many
that are shared by all people—rather than attributing them other dimensions that we typically think of as personal-
to unique or uniquely strong pathologies of the “other.” ity, temperament, or character) is apt to vary and appear
“inconsistent” across settings. Some of that variability
is random, or at least a product of factors that we cannot
Lay Dispositionism and Its Consequences
discern. Some simply reflects the influences of situational
A second insight intimately linked to the situationist tra- pressures or constraints in any given situation that make
dition, one with profound applied implications, is that people in general more or less likely to behave in ways
laypeople (and often even experts) generally fail to fully that we characterize with terms such as honesty, or selfish-
appreciate the power of the field of external or “situational” ness, or for that matter liberalism or conservatism. Some
forces that can compel or constrain the choices that actors of the variability reflects the influence not only of specific
make. And even when experience and education gives us situational pressures and constraints but also of factors that
such appreciation in one behavioral domain, this insight is determine the perceptual salience of particular stimulus
rarely generalized to new domains. Thus even a hard-won features, or the cognitive availability of specific associa-
appreciation of the lessons of Asch, Milgram, Zimbardo, tions, schemas, or personal goals at a given point in time
and company may not prevent an overly skeptical view of for the relevant actor.
so-called “false confessions” (Kassin, 2008), which, along Why does experience not educate the layperson to the
with erroneous but confident recollections and eye-witness impact of situational factors relative to that of stable per-
testimony, are a major source of wrongful convictions sonal attributes? The answer to this question is that much
(Dunning & Stern, 1994; Loftus, 2003; Wells, Memon, & of the time the people we know and encounter in every-
Penrod, 2006; Wells & Olson, 2003; Wright & Loftus, day experience do confirm our expectations about their
2008). We insist that we could never be induced by artful behavior, even expectations based on our past experiences
interrogators to confess to a crime that we did not com- (Swann & Bosson, this volume). But such confirmations
mit (which may be correct) and that those who do offer do not necessarily indicate broad, stable, individual dif-
false confessions must be dim-witted, weak, or plagued by ferences; and even where such differences seem evident,
a guilty conscience (which generally is not correct). Such they may themselves reflect the impact of situational fac-
misguided emphasis on the “dispositions” of the relevant tors. First, person and situation are inevitably confounded
actors, as Lewin (1931) observed long ago, represents a in the real world, unless a clever researcher finds a way
failing of lay psychology, in somewhat the same way that to “unconfound” them (Mischel, 1968, 1973), because
“Aristotelian” conceptions regarding the movement of many situational factors (e.g., role and relational demands,
objects (as opposed to Newtonian physics, which recognizes reputational concerns, commitments, and the costs versus
the role of forces between objects) erroneously attributed benefits of honoring or not honoring those commitments)
such movement to inherent properties of those objects rather persist over time, at least in the actor’s dealings with partic-
than the forces acting on and between them. ular people, and may even persist across diverse contexts.
Four Insights Underlying Research and Application 25

Second, once people choose situations (as when they under- political leaders whose earlier histories included violence
take a program of study or enter a career), those choices and terrorism. One such leader (David Ervine, a Northern
in turn guide and constrain their behavior—for example, Ireland “Loyalist”), in an address at Stanford University,
by obliging them to make commitments to other people. explained that, in his case, it was a matter of “51% vs.
Third, as Lewin observed, actors are a part of their own life 49%”—that this “change” involved not a transformation of
space; other people respond to physical and social charac- character but a kind of “tipping point” whereby the futil-
teristics of the actor in ways that in turn influence the actor ity and costs of violence became marginally more obvi-
(Gilbert & Jones, 1986). Finally, both personal factors and ous and the prospects for securing social justice through
situational factors create “cumulative consequences” in a normal politics became marginally brighter. He then added
way that magnifies the effects of either and produces stabil- the striking comment that when he was only 51% certain
ity over time (Caspi, Bem, & Elder, 1989; Caspi, Elder, & about the decision to embrace bombing as a tactic, he was
Bem, 1987, 1988). still 100% a “bomber,” and now that he is only 51% certain
In his 2008 best-seller Outliers, Malcolm Gladwell about the prospects for change through peaceful means, he
describes several compelling cases reflecting the cumu- is 100% a politician and peace activist.
lative consequences of situational factors. Some of these Although the tendency for laypeople to underestimate
relate to culture or subculture and therefore to values, aspi- the role of situational determinants is ubiquitous (at least in
rations, and reference groups, but some involve situational Western countries), that tendency is not invariant. In fact,
advantages or disadvantages that are essentially matters research has revealed counterexamples whereby the influ-
of coincidence (for example, the predominance of pro- ence lay psychologists assign to certain extrinsic factors—
fessional hockey players whose birthdate guaranteed that i.e., salient, tangible rewards and penalties, and self-serving
they would be older and physically more mature than most motives more generally—is exaggerated, and the role of more
of their cohort in youth hockey programs, which in turn intrinsic motives is underestimated (Miller, 1999). Indeed,
meant that they would get to play more, get more attention Lepper, Greene, and Nisbett (1973) documented something
from coaches, derive greater self-esteem from that domain, akin to such misattribution in self-perception in a study with
and thus practice harder and advance to higher levels of obvious implications for parents and teachers. The investiga-
competition). Particular traits, such as patience, persis- tors showed that the amount of time that nursery school chil-
tence, and self-mastery, whether the product of relatively dren opted to play with “magic markers” depended on the
innate temperaments or parenting practices and other fea- presence or absence of a prior extrinsic motive (the prospect
tures of early social learning, can have similar cumulative of receiving a “Good Player” award). As predicted, children
consequences, as Walter Mischel documented in report- who previously had played with the markers anticipating the
ing the surprisingly high correlation between children’s relevant award later showed less interest in that activity than
nursery school performance in his famous delay of grati- children who previously had done so without any prospect of
fication task and subsequent measures of study habits and an award. However, no such undermining of intrinsic inter-
academic success (Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990). Work est occurred when the same award was presented without
by Bandura (1977a) on self-efficacy and Dweck (1986) prior mention, and thus represented a “bonus” rather than a
on personal theories regarding fixed or malleable abilities “bribe” (Lepper & Greene, 1978).
similarly suggests that individual differences at one point Counterexamples wherein the role of certain situational
in time, regardless of their origins, can lead to choices that pressures and constraints is overestimated by observers, and
effectively change the actor’s life-space and magnify the sometimes also the actors themselves, have significance for
impact of those personal characteristics. theory. They suggest the dispositionist bias is not inevitable
Although dispositionist bias is evident when laypeo- (or “fundamental,” in the sense of being irreducible) but is
ple are surprised by the “non-obvious” results of cleverly rather the product of many different determining factors that
crafted psychology experiments of the sort we highlighted may operate to different degrees in different contexts. Thus,
earlier, their inability to outguess wily experimenters is Quattrone (1982) proposed that people making attributions
obviously not the extent, or even an important aspect, typically begin by making a dispositional attribution, which
of the problem. The more important consequence is our they “adjust” (insufficiently) in light of what they know about
failure to realize the extent to which the same person (or situational pressures and constraints (Gilbert, Pelham, &
group) may behave very differently when the balance of Krull, 1988). Indeed, the lay dispositionism apparent in
the relevant situational forces and constraints (e.g., role most everyday interpretations of behavior seems almost
demands, reputational concerns, commitments, or incen- overdetermined—a reflection not only of cognitive and
tive structures) changes. This point has been driven home perceptual factors, but also of motivational, linguistic,
to social psychologists who work with community and cultural, and even ideological factors (Ross, 1977).
26 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

An early and seminal contribution to the attribution lit- Both lay dispositionism and the tendency for observers
erature by Jones and Nisbett (1972) reported both anecdotal to be less sensitive than actors to the role of situational
and experimental evidence showing that people may rec- pressures and constraints have important real-world impli-
ognize the influence of particular situational factors on self cations. Lack of appreciation of the impact of situational
more than on others, and thus be more willing to ascribe factors other than salient prospective extrinsic incentives
cross-situationally consistent dispositions to others than and disincentives does more than foster erroneous interper-
to self. In accounting for this self-other divergence in the sonal inferences and undue pessimism about the possibility
attribution process, Jones and Nisbett cited the importance of behavior change. It leads people to overlook factors that
of two factors. One of these, little investigated at the time, may prove important determinants of success or failure in
was a difference in the amount and richness of informa- efforts to produce such changes. This “miscalibration”
tion about past behavior. That is, actors obviously have regarding strategies for achieving change leads us to be less
more information about cross-situational variation in their effective than we could be, whether the behavior we seek to
actions and outcomes in different situations that seem- change is that of friends, family members, neighbors, co-
ingly tap the same disposition. More specifically, actors workers, or students. The success of programs directed at
are more aware that their actions have varied as a function citizens whose current practices are at the root of problems
of situational demands and constraints, short-term goals regarding public health, environmental protection, crime
and priorities, and even passing mood states. (Generally, prevention, or promotion of better intergroup relations
investigators either ignored such factors, tried to “control” similarly will depend on our increasing sophistication
for them by giving the observer additional information, or about the role of social and situational factors.
investigated attributions about responses to novel choices
or situations to which past behavior seemed irrelevant.) The Need to See the Self as Consistent, Rational,
The second factor they discussed, and the one that most and Moral
caught the attention of investigators at the time, was the
obvious actor-observer difference in “perspective” and The last three decades of American social psychology (and,
“focus of attention.” Investigators were quick to use video- not coincidentally American pop psychology and perhaps
replays (Storms, 1973), seating arrangements (Taylor & American society as well) have been heavily focused on
Fiske, 1975), and the simple act of looking in a mirror the self. Indeed, focus on the autonomous self seems to be
(Duvall & Wicklund, 1972) to demonstrate the effects on a distinguishing, even defining, characteristic of our every-
attributions of giving observers the perspectives of actors day psychology, at least in Western cultures. The study of
and vice versa, and more generally to show that many other the processes by which individuals come to know and make
manipulations that made features of the actor or the situa- inferences about themselves and evaluate themselves rela-
tion confronting the actor more salient could alter causal tive to others, and later of the biases—cognitive, perceptual,
attributions (McArthur & Post, 1977; Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff, & and motivational—affecting such processes, received a
Ruderman, 1978; Taylor & Koivumaki, 1976). More major impetus from the work of Bem (1967, 1972) and the
recent investigators, bucking the current trend to empha- attribution theorists. Work on self-schemas (Markus, 1977)
size cognitive factors over perceptual ones, have explored and more dynamic theories about capacities of the self (e.g.,
the effects of subtler and more refined perspective manipu- Bandura, 1977a, 1997; Dweck, 1999; Dweck & Leggett,
lations on people’s interpretations of and reactions to both 1988) have also been influential. Within the larger culture,
past and future events (Libby, Eibach, & Gilovich, 2005; popular self-help books, seminars, and parental education
Pronin & Ross, 2006).7 classes, particularly in the latter part of the twentieth cen-
tury, have stressed the need for each of us to “feel good”
about ourselves, and also the obligation we have to help our
7 In an important extension of this work on similarities and dif- children, students, and friends feel good about themselves,
ferences in actor versus observer attribution, Nisbett and Wilson even in the face of failures and less than praiseworthy
(1977) demonstrated that actors and observers generally are sub-
behavior. Indeed, countless studies and everyday experience
ject to the same sources of accuracy and error in the attribution
alike suggest that most people do succeed in developing
process—i.e., that both interpret the available information in light
and maintaining a generally positive view of self.
of their lay theories. While actors may sometimes have access to
private experiential content (thoughts and feelings, construals, Two recurrent themes underlie research in this area
goals, etc.) that gives them an advantage over observers, they do (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2007; Swann & Bosson, this
not have direct access to any “process” information that guaran- volume). The first involves the tendency for actors to see
tees the veridicality of their causal inferences (see also Nisbett & themselves as coherent, consistent, and rational, despite behav-
Ross, 1980, pp. 205-226). ioral evidence that seemingly challenges that view—that is,
Four Insights Underlying Research and Application 27

when their behavior shows changes across time and cir- rather than the product of cognitive distortions that happen
cumstances and especially when it varies as a function of to have self-serving affective consequences. Finally, it is
subtle situational forces and constraints whose determina- often difficult to know whether public assertions involv-
tive role is not apparent to them. Within Western cultures ing the taking of credit (or for that matter taking blame)
in particular, people place a value on cross-situational con- faithfully reflect the individuals’ private assessments or
sistency, and while they acknowledge that they respond are merely lip-service declarations designed to serve some
adaptively to changes and differences in circumstances, social motive. These complexities have discouraged recent
they generally have a strong sense of a consistent self and investigators from pursuing this issue—at least in terms of
do not subscribe to the notion of socially situated identi- any general, context-independent, tendency—and to use
ties. The second theme, which connects academic and pop Jones’s (1985) term, the disentangling of motivational ver-
psychology, involves the struggle to maintain and enhance sus cognitive determinants of attributional bias has become
self-esteem, to see the self not only as coherent and ratio- a sinking ship.
nal, but also as good, moral, competent, in control of one’s Of greater contemporary interest are research findings
fate, and in tune with both one’s own values and the norms showing that people engage in selective investment of ego.
and values of the people whose opinions one values. These findings, which have obvious and potentially disturb-
This striving to see one’s actions both as consistent ing implications regarding education and other vehicles for
across situations and as commendable or at least justifiable, overcoming social and educational disadvantage, suggest
can be characterized as “dissonance theory writ large,” that people “identify” with, care about, and invest energy
with the proviso (following Aronson, 1969) that actors are in bettering themselves, for dimensions of potential self-
motivated to see themselves as both coherent and esteem- esteem in which they have experienced, and/or expect to
worthy. People assimilate new information in light of their experience, success. Conversely, they “disidentify” from,
existing theories, beliefs, and larger social, religious, and and fail to invest effort in, or evaluate their worth in terms
political ideologies in a way that shields them from the of, dimensions of potential evaluation at which they have
need to acknowledge error, when such acknowledgment failed or expect failure (Crocker & Major, 1989; Dunning,
would be painful or dissonant. To some extent they also 2005; Kunda, 1987; Steele, 1988).
see and interpret events through the prism of their needs Although the processes described above have been the
and motives in a manner that similarly shields them from most frequent targets of research, they are by no means
discomfort. “Self-serving” or “ego-defensive” attributions the only ones that people employ in order to see them-
play a role here. That is, people can make “dispositional” selves as coherent, commendable, and in control of their
attributions for behavior that reflects well on the self and destiny, and to ward off potential threats to those percep-
make situational attributions or offer other exculpatory tions. Most often, people do this in obvious and even laudable
explanations for behavior that reflects badly on them (Fiske & ways—by trying to meet their group’s standards of behav-
Taylor, 2008, pp. 159–160). But, as Heider (1958) noted, ior and achievement, which they have adopted as their own.
the need to learn and respond adaptively sets limits on such Once again, however, it is less obvious processes that have
“wishful thinking” and self-deception. attracted the greatest research interest. One such process
The literature on self-serving attributions, however, has involves selective or strategic social comparison. People
become increasing complex. First, everyday life offers can compare themselves to others who are less well off
many instances in which people seem to blame themselves than themselves, or even to themselves at a time when they
for failures and unfortunate events under circumstances in were worse off. In a seminal early paper in health psychol-
which peers and even neutral observers offer more chari- ogy, Taylor (1983) described how cancer patients coped with
table attributions, and at least some laboratory studies their illness through such comparisons—contrasting their
offer similar evidence (e.g., Gilovich, Medvec, & Savitsky, symptoms, treatment options, and family circumstances
2000; Savitsky, Epley, & Gilovich, 2001). Second, taking with even more dire diagnoses, options, and circumstances.
credit for success and blaming failure on external factors By contrast, of course, Festinger and his colleagues argued
beyond one’s control may simply be rational; successes are strongly that, in our society at least, most comparisons are
intended outcomes that one plans for and works toward, made upward, in an invidious direction that may fuel effort
whereas failures are often the product of obstacles too and achievement, but often at the price of dissatisfaction.
daunting to overcome or unforeseeable circumstances Selective comparison also allows one to defend one’s
beyond one’s control. Third, even when an apparently self- sense of fairness and morality in the face of dubiously moral
serving pattern of personal attributions for success and or fair behavior, by comparing oneself to others whose
situational attributions for failure is found, it is difficult to behavior in the pertinent domain is equally or even more
determine when such attributions are motivated distortions, suspect (Bandura, 1999, Monin, 2007). Another process,
28 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

which both “haves” and “have-nots” can employ to reduce milk and honey” long ago suggested the impact of such
feelings of guilt, shame, and other threats to self-regard, biases on perception. So did the Hastorf and Cantril (1954)
is that of “system justification” (Jost & Banaji, 1994; study noted earlier on the conflicting perceptions and
Jost & Kay, this volume; Lerner, 1980; Pratto, Sidanius, recollections of Princeton and Dartmouth football fans.
Stallworth, & Malle, 1994). This process involves a range Such biases do more than confirm expectations and serve
of strategies, including the blaming of victims for their wishful thinking. They make it difficult for people to aban-
misfortunes or continuing disadvantages, the belief that don theories or beliefs that are inaccurate and that serve
society is ultimately or globally fair (or at least as fair as them badly, even in the face of evidence that unbiased
it could be without imposing odious restrictions), the pre- observers would find convincing. Lord, Ross, and Lepper
sumption that the benefits that one reaps within the system (1979) and Edwards and Smith (1996), for example, pro-
are worth the costs, or even the faith that privations in this vided a striking example of the capacity of objectively
life will be replaced by the glories of some existence after mixed evidence regarding the deterrent efficacy of capital
death. punishment to strengthen the conflicting views of opposing
Before leaving this topic, it is worth noting the link partisans.
between striving for a positive view of the self and naïve Indeed, studies of biased perception and assimilation
realism. Because people are subject to various “positive of information have been a staple in social and cognitive
illusions” (Taylor & Brown, 1988; see also Greenwald, psychology. What most intrigues those outside our field,
1980, on the “totalitarian ego”), they do not see themselves however, is the capacity of beliefs, expectations, and “proph-
as they appear to disinterested observers; and they do not esies” (Merton, 1948)—those one holds about oneself and
take kindly to those who challenge their illusions or who those held about one by others—to prompt actions or fail-
use different bases for assessment or different standards for ures to act that reinforce those convictions (see Snyder,
comparison. Conversely, when others seem less than wel- 1992). In some cases, the consequences of these processes
coming of one’s constructive criticisms, gentle warnings are benign or positive—individuals and groups motivated
about closed-mindedness or wishful thinking, or comments to confirm positive expectations, including “positive illu-
about related displays of self-serving or belief-preserving sions” (Taylor, 1989; Taylor & Armor, 1996; Taylor &
bias, the attribution is a censorious one. Brown, 1988) about themselves, tend to act accordingly;
We are reminded, here, of the wry and telling obser- and often they benefit directly or indirectly from doing so.
vation by comedian George Carlin with regard to driving: Similarly, self-confirming positive beliefs, such as the belief
“Ever notice that anyone going slower than you is an idiot that success is determined less by innate ability than by per-
and anyone going faster is a maniac?!” To Carlin’s insight, sistent effort or flexibility in approach (Weiner, 1974), or
which captures the essence of naïve realism as well as any that abilities and even general intelligence are themselves
experiment we know, we would add the observation that malleable rather than fixed (Bandura, 1997; Dweck, 1999;
people characteristically see those who are less honest than Nisbett, 2009)—are not mere “illusions”; they are impor-
themselves on their income tax returns as cheats and those tant and empowering insights.
who are more honest as naïve; by the same token, they see In other cases, however, belief-confirming biases and
others who want to move slower than they do in the direc- especially belief-perpetuating behaviors can have malignant
tion of any particular social reform as reactionaries and consequences. In particular, they can result in individual
those who advocate moving faster than they deem prudent and collective efforts to counteract or reduce the threat of
as unrealistic dreamers. negative beliefs and expectations in ways that have delete-
rious personal and social consequences and that may even
serve, paradoxically, to confirm those negative expecta-
Confirmation Biases: Impact of Expectations,
tions. The phenomenon of “self-handicapping” (Jones &
Beliefs, and Social Representations
Berglas, 1979) involves an all-too-familiar type of behav-
Our final social-psychological insight concerns the impact ioral confirmation—one designed (sometimes consciously,
of beliefs and expectations. More specifically, it involves sometimes perhaps without self-awareness) to dampen the
the impact of biases in perception and cognition that attributional threat of a negative outcome, but does so in
make one see, find evidence for, and even produce, what ways that makes that outcome more probable. (Thus self-
one expects to see or have occur (or, in some cases, what one handicapping constitutes yet another process that protects
hopes or fears to see or have occur). The biblical parable people’s ability to see themselves, and to be seen by others,
of the “scouts” who, depending on the state of mind they in positive terms.) The most obvious example of this phe-
brought to their task, brought back reports either of for- nomenon is provided in cases where actors withdraw effort
bidding terrain peopled with fearsome giants or a “land of and personal investment in the face of the threat of failure;
Four Insights Underlying Research and Application 29

but alcoholism, drug abuse, tardiness, belligerence toward in some fixed hierarchy. Even within our own society (and
supervisors or co-workers, refusal to conform to institu- some domains of psychology), we would still find disagree-
tional standards of dress, hygiene, speech, or other violations ments about the role of nature versus nurture in determining
of behavioral norms can also be seen as instances of not only capacities to achieve, but also the aspects of tem-
self-handicapping. perament, tastes, and motivation that play a role in what one
Finally, as Wegner, Schneider, Carter, and White (1987) strives for and succeeds or fails in achieving.
showed in a simple study with a result that is both non-obvi- The less obvious insight is that beliefs and expectations
ous yet obviously true, attempting to avoid a given action, can be fulfilled and perpetuated through processes other
feeling, or action sometimes increases rather than decreases than the powerful but mundane ones of deliberate goal-
its likelihood. The instruction “try not to think of a white setting, teaching and learning, and sustained goal-directed
bear” as Wegner et al. showed, ultimately increased the effort either by actors or those in a position to help (or thwart)
frequency of such ursine imaginings, as compared to those such effort. The keen-eyed sociologist Erving Goffman
reported by a group asked to think of a white bear from the (1959) offered a “dramaturgical analysis” of social inter-
outset. Trying hard to avoid physical tics, speech disfluen- action that emphasized the constraining effects of roles,
cies, slips of the tongue, and the like is apt to prove similarly role-based expectations, and audiences. The novelist Kurt
counterproductive, and certainly is not the optimal clini- Vonnegut (in Mother Night) warned that since we “are
cal strategy, when anxiety and excessive self-monitoring what we pretend to be” we should take great care in choos-
already are part of the problem (Wegner, 1994). ing what we pretend to be; and Shaw’s Pygmalion is the
When, early in his first term, as the Great Depression famed story of a working-class girl who is tutored about
tightened its grip on the United States, President Franklin upper class speech, manners, and dress so that she can, and
Roosevelt said “we have nothing to fear but fear itself,” does, pass as a “lady.”
he was mindful of more collective processes and conse- There is more than literary and anecdotal evidence to
quences. That is, fear of worsening economic conditions, attest to the existence and potency of behavioral confirma-
bank failures, and job losses leads to behaviors (unwill- tion. In one famous early study (Kelley, 1950), students
ingness to spend or invest and withdrawal of funds from encountered a guest lecturer who had previously been
banks) that bring about precisely thomase dire results, described to them with a list of traits that included either
which in turn further encourage the behaviors that are the adjective “warm” or “cold.” In another, even more
exacerbating the problem, in a classic “vicious circle.” famous, albeit controversial study featuring a manipula-
(The economic crisis that is gripping the United States and tion of expectations (Rosenthal & Jacobsen, 1968), teach-
most other countries at the time we are writing this chapter ers were led to expect that certain students in their classes
makes accounts of that cycle all too familiar.) Social psy- were about to show learning spurts. In both cases, the result
chologists are hardly unique in noting the power of percep- was a change in the ensuing student-teacher interactions
tions and expectations to shape reality. What they brought that confirmed the relevant expectations (although in the
to the topic were demonstrations pinpointing some of the latter case the phenomenon appears to have been largely
processes by which this occurs, and illustrating how it limited to students in one early grade).
might play a role in various social phenomena of concern. A third slightly later study (Miller, Brickman, & Bolen,
Few in our society would dispute the fact that one’s 1975), which remains less well known than it should be given
goals and plans, and one’s confidence or lack of confidence the clarity of its applied implications, involved the direct
about what one can accomplish, play a significant role in effects of labeling on youngsters who were labeled. This
determining what one exposes oneself to, seeks to learn study showed that whereas exhorting primary school
and perfect, and is willing to take risks or delay gratifica- students to refrain from a particular practice (e.g., class-
tion in order to achieve. Equally obvious is the impact of room littering) had only a modest and temporary effect on
other people’s plans for us and expectations about what we behavior (objectively undeserved), communications from
can accomplish, both on what opportunities we are given their teacher, the principal, and the school custodian that
and how we take advantage or fail to take advantage of students deserved recognition for already showing such
the opportunities that present themselves. To some extent, we behavior had a larger effect, and one that continued to be
become what we seek, and expect, and want to become; apparent in the period after the “nagging versus labeling”
to some extent, we also become what others want for us, manipulation had ended.
demand of us, and help us to become. However, it is worth In yet another study from that era (Snyder, Tanke, &
noting that acceptance of this seeming truism is by no Berscheid, 1977), young men who had been shown a
means universal. Members of many other societies would picture of either a very attractive or less attractive young
assign a larger role to destiny or God’s will or one’s place woman then engaged in a telephone conversation with
30 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

someone whom they believed to be that woman. The demonstration experiments. People do not always simply
result, again, was an interaction that reflected the effect of do what those interacting with them hope and expect;
the research participants’ expectations. The belief that they sometimes, especially in the case of negative expectations,
were talking to someone particularly attractive changed the they both work hard to disconfirm those expectations and
behavior of the young men, and that of the young women succeed. Consider the phenomenon of psychological reac-
(who had no inkling about the experimental manipulation tance (Brehm, 1966), whereby individuals resist attempts
that was influencing the young men) in ways discernable to push their choice in one direction by developing more
to raters who were blind to that manipulation. The young positive feelings about the choices they are being prompted
men came away from their experience with an impression to give up (see also Ward & Brenner, 2006). Moreover, in
of the person with whom they had been conversing that many contexts people who are adversaries or even just
was consistent both with their state of mind during the con- competitors see influence attempts in strategic terms (if
versation and the later ratings of the outside observers. they want me to do, buy, or accept X instead of Y, then my
A particularly pernicious example of self-fulfilling beliefs doing, buying, or accepting X instead of Y must be good
and expectations, and the one most studied by social psy- for them, and therefore not so good for me).
chologists, is that of stereotypes and other negative beliefs In fact, even when people are not in an antagonis-
about particular groups of people. Some of these effects are tic or suspicious frame of mind, they do not necessarily
obvious, although no less important for their obviousness. show the responses that are being sought—even by skilled
If it is widely believed that the members of some group experimental social psychologists—when those responses
disproportionately possess some virtue or vice relevant tap motivations and concerns more potent than a vague
to academic or on-the-job performance, one is likely (in inclination to follow where they are led. Such resistance
the absence of specific legal or social sanctions) to make and reactance on the part of research participants is per-
school admission or hiring decisions accordingly—and in haps something that journal reviewers, for whom com-
so doing to deprive or privilege group members in terms plaints about the possible role played by “Rosenthal” and
of opportunities to nurture their talents, acquire credentials, “Hawthorne” effects are almost reflexive, should keep in
or otherwise succeed or fail in accord with the beliefs and mind when they evaluate studies with consequential depen-
expectations that dictated their life chances. dent measures. (More than one of our colleagues has mused
We will discuss some less obvious effects of racism, that if research participants were as prone to go along with
sexism, and other types of stigmatizing belief systems demand characteristics and to show experimenter bias
later in this chapter. For now, let us consider just one clas- effects as reviewers seem to imagine, our collective files of
sic study (Word, Zanna, & Cooper, 1974) that nicely fits failed experiments would be notably thinner.)
our criterion as an empirical parable. The investigators In most real-world influence attempts (e.g., charitable
first showed that White Princeton students interviewing appeals, requests for favors, pleas for better pay or working
individuals they believed to be prospective job candidates conditions, ads trumpeting the merits of political candi-
unwittingly afforded differential treatment to White and dates, or calls for the cessation of violence), the hope and
Black candidates—seating themselves farther away from expectation of the party making that attempt is explicit.
the latter, showing more discomfort and awkwardness, and In this regard, and in recognition of the situationist tradition
ending the interview more quickly. In a second phase of in social psychology, we should remember that expectations
the project, the authors trained interviewers to treat new are not communicated only in words or gestures. The envi-
applicants, all of whom were White, the way either Black ronment itself “signals” norms and expectations that can
or White applicants had been treated in the earlier phase in turn influence behavior. The “broken windows” thesis
of the study. When videotapes of those interviews were offered by Wilson and Kelling (1982), and much lauded by
subsequently viewed by evaluators, those receiving the thoughtful conservative commentators, suggested that tol-
treatment previously afforded to Black applicants were erance for vandalism, graffiti, antisocial actions, and other
judged more negatively than those receiving the treatment “trivial” offenses against social order signals a permissive-
previously afforded to White applicants. Similar findings, ness that encourages more serious offenses. This thesis
incidentally, were shown much more recently in an analo- (which can be seen as a borrowing of the tipping point
gous study regarding the effects of the treatment received notion implicit in Lewin’s tension system formulation long
by homosexual versus heterosexual job applicants (Hebl, before Gladwell, 2000, popularized the phrase in the title of
Foster, Mannix, & Dovidio, 2002). his best-selling book) has recently been given a successful
As is so often the case in social psychology, the take- empirical test by Keizer, Lindenderg, and Steg (2008), who
away message from years of research is not as simple as demonstrated the phenomenon of “cross norm” violation,
one might assume from the description of a few classic that is, disorder and non-lawfulness in one domain (e.g.,
Social Psychology Today 31

graffiti or littering) resulting in increased violation (e.g., tres- and job market but also in the courts (see Sommers &
passing or even mail theft) in another. Ellsworth, 2001). But whereas Americans once freely
reported their reluctance to accept someone Black, Jewish,
Muslim, or Asian (or a member of some similarly dis-
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY TODAY criminated-against group) in their neighborhoods, social
clubs, or restaurants, previous measures no longer showed
Recent history in social psychology has seen contributions such overt public racism. Indeed, in contexts where liberal
to theory and research in three major areas: the study of norms on matters of race hold sway, people avoid words
stereotyping, prejudice, and racism; the study of culture and deeds that could even be misinterpreted as racist or
and cultural influences; and the study of cognitive and sexist, unless they have first established their “moral cre-
motivational influences beyond, or even contrary to, the dentials” as someone not subject to such biases (Monin &
rational self-interest model central to economics and other Miller, 2001). More subtle measures were developed, such
social sciences. Each area has witnessed upswings and as the modern or symbolic racism scales of Kinder, Sears,
downturns in the interest it has received, and each has seen and McConahay, in order to detect less blatant forms of
marked shifts in focus as new generations of researchers prejudice. This process continues, as today many items on
have come to the fore. the so-called Modern Racism Scale (McConahay, 1986)
are no longer endorsed even by respondents who continue
to harbor residues of prejudice (Dovidio & Gaertner and
Stereotyping and Prejudice
Yzerbyt & Demoulin, volume 2).
From the 1920s to the present, a major concern in social In the years since Jones’s (1985) chapter, this field has
psychology has been the nature and effects of stereotyp- been marked by two major undertakings. The first has
ing and prejudice; however, the focus of research has regu- involved the development of theory and measures recognizing
larly shifted. In the 1920s and 1930s, the main concern of ever more covert forms of stereotyping and prejudice—
leading researchers including Thurstone, Bogardus, Likert, including “implicit” attitudes that may be embedded in the
and others was the development of reliable and valid culture rather than solely in the minds of individuals (Banaji &
methods for measuring negative attitudes toward various Heiphetz, this volume). The second undertaking has
racial and ethnic groups. Then, in the 1930s and 1940s, involved a shift from the study of the holders of stereotypes
as Miller and his Yale colleagues sought to translate psy- and prejudices to the study of the effects on, and responses
choanalytic insights into learning theory models, studies of, their targets (Swann & Bosson, this volume; Fiske,
of the motivational bases for prejudice came to the fore in volume 2; Steele, 2010).
social psychology, and phenomena such as scapegoating Although controversy currently surrounds the putative
and displacement were explored empirically. At the same validity of tools designed to assess covert forms of ste-
time, and in much the same spirit, personality theorists reotyping (Blanton et al., 2009; Greenwald, Poehlman,
led by Adorno and colleagues explored ethnocentrism and Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009), such “implicit” measures may
authoritarianism. ultimately be shown to provide additional predictive valid-
Throughout that early period, as is the case today, social ity over and above that offered by the standard explicit mea-
scientists recognized that cognitive processes as well as sures (e.g., Jordan, Spencer, Zanna, Hoshino-Browne, &
motivational ones were a central feature in the phenomena Correll, 2003). The intriguing possibility also exists that
of concern. Walter Lippman, in his 1922 book on Public explicit and implicit measures assess related but distinct
Opinion, gave the term “stereotype” its present meaning, constructs, each providing its own window on behavior. In
and three decades later Gordon Allport, in The Nature of particular, implicit measures may do a serviceable job of
Prejudice (1954b), argued that stereotyping could be seen, predicting behaviors that are normally free of conscious
in part, as a natural, if undesirable, consequence of ubiqui- self-monitoring (e.g., opting to hand a pen to a member of a
tous and generally useful cognitive processes of categori- different race versus placing it on a table; Wilson, Lindsey, &
zation and discrimination (see Hamilton, 1981). Schooler, 2000), whereas explicit measures may do a bet-
As social norms regarding public discourse on matters ter job of predicting behaviors that are subject to such
of race, ethnicity, and gender began to change following monitoring and control (e.g., questionnaire ratings of an
the 1954 Supreme Court decision integrating the nation’s interaction partner of a different race; Dovidio, Kawakami,
public schools and the Civil Rights Act of 1964, and as the Johnson, Johnson, & Howard, 1997).
Civil Rights Movement gained momentum, attitude mea- Issues of level of awareness and monitoring can have
surement again became a focus of interest. Objective evi- profound real-world consequences. Eberhardt, Goff,
dence of discrimination remained not only in the economy Purdie, and Davies (2004) showed that priming police
32 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

officers with crime-related words increased the attention and the Department of Social Psychology at Columbia,
they directed to Black faces relative to White faces. Correll where they worked side by side with Clyde Kluckholm,
et al. (2007) conceptually replicated that result but then John Whiting, Margaret Mead, and other eminent schol-
demonstrated that trained police officers were less likely ars. Researchers in many places used the Human Relations
to exhibit racial bias in a simulated shooting context than Area Files (Whiting & Child, 1953) to empirically test
were community members—i.e., that implicit associations postulated relationships between cultural practices or cir-
notwithstanding, behaviors toward members of stigma- cumstances and features of modal personality (see also
tized groups that are deliberate and explicitly executed McClelland and colleagues’, 1961, extensive work linking
may, through self-monitoring and/or training, prove to be culture to achievement motivation). In short, the topic of
relatively unbiased by such associations. cultural differences, and anthropology and sociology more
In focusing on the effects of stereotyping and prejudice, generally, had long occupied a niche within social psychol-
perhaps the most important insights gained involve the cop- ogy. (Witness also the contents of three successive volumes
ing strategies of targeted or “stigmatized” individuals. In of Readings in Social Psychology, in 1947, 1952, and 1958,
innovative research, Crocker and Major (1989), proceeding sponsored by the Society for the Psychological Study of
from the surprising finding that members of stigmatized Social Issues, that were de rigueur for graduate students
groups often show no deficit in self-esteem relative to of the late 1950s and early 1960s.)
members of non-stigmatized groups, suggested that indi- Soon after, however, culture virtually disappeared from
viduals can use the fact of such stigmatization to maintain mainstream social psychology. In part, this turning away
self-esteem—notably, by attributing negative outcomes, from the study of culture may have reflected the Lewinian
behaviors, and life circumstances to injurious prejudice penchant for explaining social influences in terms of the
rather than to their own failures and inadequacies. immediate thoughts and feelings of the individuals who
Drawing on his work on self-affirmation (Steele, 1988), were interacting, which was ironic in light of Lewin’s own
Claude Steele similarly argued that in certain “loaded” emphasis on situational and normative influences. Despite
domains, notably those implicating intellectual achieve- efforts by Triandis (1968), and a few others who tested the
ment, stigmatized individuals can respond by “disidenti- “generalizability” of findings by trying to replicate classic
fying” from that domain. As a consequence, in academic American studies in other countries, interest in cultural dif-
domains (as is the case for stigmatized African American ferences within our field dropped close to zero. What cre-
students, and for women in the domain of mathematics), ated a sudden bandwagon was the ground-breaking work
they underachieve and narrow their career opportunities of Hazel Markus and Shinobu Kitayama (1991) and their
accordingly. In discussing the processes and consequences colleagues (e.g., Morris & Peng, 1994; Nisbett, 2003),
of stigmatization, Steele (1997) pointedly maintained that highlighting differences between Eastern and Western cul-
the relevant toxic social representations are “in the air,” not tures (Heine, volume 2).
just in the mind of targeted individuals. As such, individuals What made Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) article on
do not personally need to accept or endorse those representa- the “independent versus interdependent self ” so impact-
tions to suffer their effects. In the face of what Steele termed ful and a continuing impetus for new research? First and
“stereotype threat,” poor performance can result from the foremost, it offered an insight that was both powerful and
awareness that one risks confirming negative stereotypes— correct (Shweder & Bourne, 1984; Triandis, 1989; Weisz,
and from the resulting effects of anxiety and expenditure of Rothbaum, & Blackburn, 1984). It was also partially a
cognitive resources that could otherwise be devoted to the matter of timing, as it coincided both with demographic
task at hand (Schmader, Johns, & Forbes, 2008). changes in the United States and the rise of China, Japan,
and South Korea as economic powers and the emergence of
the self as a focal topic (Baumeister, 1999). Additionally,
The Role of Culture
it offered a strategy of proceeding from a nuanced analy-
A second major issue that has received an enormous amount sis of a presumed cultural difference rather than a simple
of recent attention is the topic of culture and cultural dif- comparison of the way people from different cultures
ferences. Following WWII, almost all students of social responded to research paradigms previously developed and
psychology were familiar with the work of Ruth Benedict, tested in the United States and other Western countries. But
Alfred Kroeber, and other anthropologists in the Franz the staying power of the topic additionally resulted from a
Boas tradition. Social psychologists were frequently bil- timely shift in methodology.
leted in interdisciplinary programs, such as the Institute for As interesting and enlightening as the comparison of
Human Relations at Yale, the Institute for Social Research U.S. and Asian responses was to U.S. college students
at Michigan, the Department of Social Relations at Harvard, and most American researchers, the lessons that such
Social Psychology Today 33

comparisons offered about other ways of thinking, feel- of homo economicus—the standard economic model of
ing, and relating to family member and peers were ones individuals as rational maximizers of utility that has domi-
that were already familiar to the average teenager in the nated political theory since the eighteenth-century writings
“other” culture (and certainly to the many Asian and Asian of Adam Smith and Jeremy Bentham (Tyler, Rasinski, &
American graduate students who entered our Ph.D. pro- Griffin, 1986). Although our critiques today borrow heav-
grams). What brought this work to the very center of our ily from modern work in the judgment and decision-mak-
field, and what made even flagship journal reviewers and ing tradition, it can be noted that social psychologists never
editors receptive, was a shift to 2  2 experimental designs fully succumbed to the appeal of rational decision-making
that allowed a sharpening of hypotheses and findings. models and functionalist approaches more generally. They
Researchers demonstrated that under one set of circum- never forgot that people (to borrow a biblical maxim) “do
stances the assumed cultural difference in, for example, not live by bread alone,” but are influenced by a variety of
dissonance reduction (Kitayama, Snibbe, Markus, & social motives and values that get in the way of maximiz-
Suzuki, 2004), attribution (Morris & Peng, 1994), self- ing material outcomes (Frank, Gilovich, & Regan, 1993;
determination (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999), or self-enhance- Schwartz, 1994; Vohs, Mead, & Goode, 2006).
ment (Sedikides, Gaertner, & Toguchi, 2003) appears, while Indeed, people not only leave tips in restaurants that
under another set of circumstances, it does not. they anticipate never again visiting, they sacrifice and even
In the study of cultural diversity, the distinction between die for beliefs that do not materially enrich them, or for that
the Western World and the Far East has received the lion’s matter their kin and descendants (Kahneman, Knetsch, &
share of attention (Brewer & Chen, 2007). But recent work Thaler, 1986; Miller, 1999; Tetlock, 2002). Moreover, the
on social class, subculture, and religious affiliations as extent to which prosocial values (which sophisticated econ-
moderator variables in helping us understand variability omists explain in terms of reputational concerns and other
within our society has also gained currency. In the case of non-obvious but essentially self-serving motives) influ-
social class, Markus and colleagues (Snibbe & Markus, ence behavior proves highly susceptible to situational and
2005; Stephens, Markus, & Townsend, 2007) have offered schema-salience manipulations, a finding further attesting
a compelling demonstration of differences between middle- to the limitations of purely economic models (Mikulincer,
class and working-class Americans in the specific domain Shaver, Gillath, & Nitzberg, 2005). Indeed, as the work
of personal agency and choice. Thus, they have shown that, of Frank, Gilovich, and Regan (1993) suggests, exposure
when making choices, working-class individuals place a pre- to the self-interested model common in economics tends to
mium on options that reflect similarity to others, whereas breed self-interested behavior. Thus, undergraduates
middle-class individuals are more likely to favor options majoring in economics displayed much greater rates of
that distinguish them from others. Exploring subcultures in defection in a standard Prisoner’s Dilemma game (72%)
the United States, Nisbett and Cohen and their colleagues than did nonmajors (47%).
(e.g., Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle, & Schwarz, 1996; Nisbett A critique of homo economicus, especially in its simple
& Cohen, 1996) have focused on a particular form of vio- formulation, has thus constituted a significant and continu-
lence, namely that representing “honor killings,” and other ing contribution of our field. (We not only insisted on having
responses to insults and affronts, that the authors show to a place for altruism, morality, social reputation, and self-
be especially characteristic of Southern U.S. states and of regard; we also recognized that behavior as well as beliefs
individuals who have migrated from the South. In such can be dictated by attempts at dissonance reduction and
research, which featured a combination of laboratory stud- rationalization, and that manipulations of small situ-
ies, survey data, and crime statistics, once again the goal ational factors and the priming of particular goals, frames,
has not been simply to demonstrate the existence of group or schemas that do not change objective costs or benefits
differences. Rather, it has been to illuminate how cultural can be highly impactful.) But above all, it is work in the
forces interact with particular contextual factors to prime prospect theory tradition of Kahneman and Tversky (e.g.,
specific values (e.g., choice, autonomy, honor)—values 1979, 1984) and others who have applied framing and
that are not unique to a particular culture or subculture but priming techniques to further challenge standard economic
that are understood and acted upon differently by different models and address practical issues of social policy inno-
societies and by different groups within those societies. vation, that proved most challenging and decisive. Unlike
the earlier pioneering work by Herbert Simon on bounded
Critique of Homo Economicus rationality, which demonstrated conditions under which a
person might reasonably decide not to search for or insist
A third topic with both a long history in social psychology on an optimal solution, Kahneman and Tversky’s work
and a more recent spiking of interest involves the limitations illustrated pervasive biases that challenged fundamental
34 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

assumptions of the traditional economic model of human that relate directly or indirectly to sexual selection, mating
rationality. strategies, and differential parental investment. One
More recent research by social psychologists on the animating insight for this work is the fact that throughout
effects of too much choice has further eroded the validity most of human history, the two sexes have faced somewhat
of models of decision making that assume rational outcome different adaptive challenges in the effort to produce and
maximization. Iyengar and Lepper (2000) found that when ensure the survival of offspring who will in turn reproduce.
given the opportunity to taste-test six jams, 30% of shop- The other is that the mechanisms for meeting these chal-
pers ultimately purchased a jar of jam. When given the lenges persist and continue to exert their influence (Buss,
chance to taste-test twenty-four jams, only 3% of shop- 1994; Neuberg, Schaller, & Kenrick, this volume).
pers subsequently made a purchase. The availability of an No topic has been more controversial than the role of bio-
extensive choice set appears to have actually “demotivated” logically based mechanisms of evolutionary origin versus
individuals from making a choice. social and cultural influences. Critics within social psychol-
Drawing on this research, Schwartz et al. (2002) sug- ogy (e.g., Eagly & Wood, 1999) complain that evolutionary
gested a distinction between “maximizers,” who, when psychologists give the latter influences too little weight, espe-
faced with large choice sets, experience behavioral paraly- cially in considering sex differences. Critics outside the field
sis as they vainly attempt to select the “best” option, and complain that in considering contemporary human practices
“satisficers” (Simon, 1955), who are prone to select the and preferences, “just-so” stories and teleological specula-
first option that passes some threshold of acceptability tion are too often a substitute for the more rigorous strategy
(and who, in surveys, report themselves to be happier and of cross-species comparisons and contrasts, with particular
less prone to depression than maximizers). Although there attention to species-specific behaviors that seem anomalous
are no doubt cultural and situational variations determining rather than obviously adaptive. Ironically, perhaps, the most
preferences for large versus smaller choice sets, the poten- persuasive rejoinder to critics of evolutionary approaches
tial implications of such work go beyond the concerns of has been provided by the very same research strategy that
shrewd marketing executives. Overwhelming citizens, cultural psychologist have used effectively—that is, distin-
in the name of free choice and personal responsibility, guishing between conditions under which a given phenom-
with enormous numbers of medical insurance, retirement enon should be present and when it should not.
investment, or health improvement plans may have the For example, Haselton et al. (2007) reported that col-
unfortunate unintended consequence of deterring them lege women are more likely to “dress to impress,” through
from making any decision at all (or at best of leading then self-grooming and ornamentation of attire, when in the fer-
to choose the most salient option). Clearly, research on bet- tile rather than the luteal phase of ovulation. Studies of this
ter ways to frame and present options to help people make sort are not without their methodological critics and do not
the choices that best serve them promises to be a growth rule out non-evolutionary explanations for the phenomena
industry for some time to come. in question, much less prove the more general contentions
of evolutionary psychology, but they do reflect increased
sophistication in theorizing and theory testing. Accordingly,
Promising New Directions
they make it more difficult for social psychologists to dis-
Many other topics and approaches are also stimulating new miss such contentions because they are unwelcome. (As in
and potentially important work. Most of them draw on one other “nature vs. nurture” debates, it is important to remem-
or more of the field’s four foundational insights discussed ber that the fact of genetic or other biological determina-
earlier. Space concerns compel us to offer only a sample of tion need not say anything about degree of modifiability or
illustrative examples: susceptibility to environmental influence.)
A final point that particularly resonates with social psy-
Evolutionary Psychology
chologists, indeed one that was discussed by Leon Festinger
The field of evolutionary psychology, with its singular (1983), concerns the contrast between conditions of the
ability to bring together structuralism and functionalism, ancestral environment and those of the present day. Although
represents the efforts of researchers in many disciplines, speculative, such analyses may speak to the persistence of
including biologists, sociologists, and anthropologists, cognitive and motivational biases that appear maladaptive
as well as psychologists from many areas of psychology. today. Perhaps most notable is the assumption that others
The most powerful evolutionary ideas relevant to social share one’s goals, tastes, and understandings of the world—
psychology (such as those pertaining to romantic attrac- which presumably was a lesser source of error and misun-
tion, close relationships, and social development) are ones derstanding in a world when individuals lived in small bands
Social Psychology Today 35

with peers who shared their experiences, and strangers with Of course, even the most ardent supporter of implicit
disparate histories, needs, and preferences were rare. processes would acknowledge its limitations. Questions
remain about the power, flexibility, and duration of prim-
Implicit Influences ing effects, and most demonstrations continue to involve
As seen in recent research on prejudice and stereotyping, responses such as recognition memory rather than com-
investigation of non-conscious priming effects has become plex behavior (Mitchell, 2006). Furthermore, as Bargh et
increasingly prevalent and provocative. While much of the al. (1996) acknowledge, to be effective, subliminal behav-
work has involved simple effects on recognition and recall, ioral primes must be compatible with an individual’s cur-
there are also some behavioral effects worth noting. In rent goals and priorities. In short, we have yet to enter the era
perhaps the best known of these studies, Bargh, Chen, and when fanciful claims about the effectiveness of “subliminal”
Burrows (1996) demonstrated that participants who per- advertisements, such as those in Vance Packard’s (1957)
formed a sentence unscrambling task featuring words such best-seller, The Hidden Persuaders, have become valid.
as Florida and bingo subsequently walked down a hallway
Terror Management Theory
more slowly than participants who had unscrambled words
with no such age-relevant connotations. In an equally pro- In a field often lacking grand, organizing theories, terror
vocative study, Dijksterhuis and van Knippenberg (1998) management theory (TMT) has bucked the trend, produc-
reported that respondents primed with a professor stereo- ing over 300 studies. Inspired by the work of anthropologist
type correctly answered more Trivial Pursuit items than Ernest Becker, the originators of the theory argue that
did those primed with a secretary stereotype. Finally, in a knowledge of the finality of life induces people to engage
study designed to challenge the formulations of conven- in practices designed to buffer themselves against the
tional economists, Kay, Wheeler, Bargh, and Ross (2004) anxiety associated with that knowledge (Greenberg et al.,
showed that players in an Ultimatum Game primed by the 1986). Such “mortality salience,” it is postulated, prompts
presence of business-relevant objects (such as a briefcase) defenses of one’s “cultural worldview” in a way that other
made less generous offers than players primed by the pres- threats to the self (e.g., worrisome events, physical pain,
ence of objects related more to academia (such as a back- social exclusion) do not (Greenberg, Solomon, & Arndt,
pack) than business. In these and many other similar tasks, 2007; Pyszczynski et al., this volume).
the vast majority of participants appear to evince no aware- In arguing, among other things, that the ultimate purpose
ness that their behavior has been influenced by the relevant of self-esteem is its capacity to serve as a buffer against
prime (Banaji & Heiphetz; Bargh et al.; Dijksterhuis, all in death anxiety, adherents of terror management theory both
this volume). highlight the strengths and reveal the potential limitations
A parallel development in the domain of person percep- of their approach. Although some limiting conditions have
tion concerns the rapidity (and often surprising accuracy) been cited (Jonas, Schimel, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski,
with which we form initial impressions (Ambady, Bernieri, 2002), the theory is offered as an explanation for everything
& Richeson, 2000)—impressions that appear to be largely from suntanning (Greenberg et al., 2007) to the terrorist
intuitive responses to nonverbal cues, such as body pos- attacks in the United States on 9/11/01 (Pyszczynski et al.,
ture, eye contact or gaze avoidance, and various spontane- 2006). But the long history emphasizing the contextual
ous expressions of emotion that have been shown to play nature of social psychological phenomena tends to cir-
an important role in social interactions (Ekman, 2007; cumscribe such grand theorizing. While there well may
Ellsworth, Carlsmith, & Henson, 1972). In many ways, this be times when self-esteem bolstering behavior is rooted in
research represents a natural extension of earlier work on concerns regarding death, efforts to enhance self-esteem
the priming, or enhanced accessibility, of emotion and cog- can serve other purposes as well—for example, to confirm
nitions. At the same time, a clear debt is owed to the seminal one’s sense of social belonging (Leary, Tambor, Terdal, &
work of Nisbett and Wilson (1977), who argued that higher- Downs, 1995) or, as Greenberg (2008) acknowledges, to
order cognitive processes leave no conscious substrate maximize one’s potentialities and competencies.
available to introspection. As in recent work investigat- Given the heralded reach of the theory, it is curious
ing cognitive processes more generally, implicit priming that, with a few exceptions (e.g., Jonas et al., 2002), TMT
entails a recognition of dual processes, whereby one set researchers have focused almost exclusively on negative
of responses (e.g., behavior in response to an unconscious behaviors (e.g., derogation of those who do not share one’s
prime) appears to occur relatively automatically, whereas a cultural worldview) as their “dependent variables.” To date,
second set (e.g., behavior in response to consciously per- the limited number of published TMT studies demonstrat-
ceived stimuli) typically requires deliberation. ing the “positive” consequences of mortality salience seem
36 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

to have required the introduction of additional manipula- self-regulation and control (Baumeister & Vohs, 2004) that
tions, such as the invocation of “counteracting” values, to have now been implicated in a wide range of applied prob-
achieve their results (see Niesta, Fritsche, & Jonas, 2008). lems, including educational achievement (Shoda, Mischel,
This despite the increases in generosity, commitment to & Peake, 1990) and health maintenance (Mann & Ward,
future generations, etc., that so often seem to come from 2007). Theories proposing two distinct motivational sys-
existential musings, recovery from serious illnesses, near- tems, one oriented toward approach and the other toward
death experiences, and the like. Assessing the nature and avoidance (Carver, Sutton, & Scheier, 2000; Gray, 1990),
scope of this provocative research topic may thus require have been particularly influential. In regulatory focus the-
researchers from additional areas—indeed, perhaps from the ory (Higgins, 1998), for example, the concern is how an
new tradition discussed next—to fill this gap in knowledge. individual’s desire to engage in promotion versus preven-
tion behaviors “fits” goal-relevant tasks in the environ-
Positive Psychology ment. Although such an orientation can theoretically be
Begun in the past decade, positive psychology represents either personality-based or situationally induced, it is the
an attempt to promote empirically based research focused interaction between a particular orientation and certain
on human growth, strengths, and virtues, as opposed to the environmental variables that has produced especially inter-
negative behaviors, judgmental shortcomings, and social esting applied findings. For example, Mann, Sherman,
pathologies emphasized in most research (Krueger & and Updegraff (2004) applied a framing manipulation to a
Funder, 2004; Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Specific health message about dental flossing, emphasizing either
topics of inquiry include happiness, optimism, creativity, gains to be had from enacting the behavior (e.g., healthy
hope, gratitude, forgiveness, love, and other aspects of well- gums, fresh breath) or losses to be suffered from neglecting
being (e.g., Gable & Haidt, 2005; Langer, 2002; Lyubomirsky, the behavior (e.g., unhealthy gums, bad breath). Individuals
King, & Diener, 2005). For example, whereas accounts who reported a motivational style consistent with promo-
abound as to the origin and function of negative emotions, tion behaviors flossed more in response to the gain-framed
Fredrickson’s (2001) “broaden and build” theory attempts than the loss-framed message; those with a style character-
to explain the putative roots and purpose of positive emo- istic of prevention behaviors showed the opposite pattern
tions, arguing that they serve to expand both thinking and of responses.
social resources. Similarly, Gable, Gonzaga, and Strachman A second prominent approach has been adopted by
(2006) showed that the way romantic couples respond (or Baumeister and colleagues, who argue that exertions of
fail to respond) to positive developments in each other’s self-control in one domain temporarily sap the capac-
lives offers a better predictor of the health and duration of ity to engage in self-control in another domain. In their
the relationship than their reactions to negative events. The studies, individuals who engage in “ego-depleting” tasks
study of beneficial health consequences of positive emo- (e.g., resisting chocolate chip cookies or making effort-
tions, relationships, and experiences (Keltner, 2009), not ful choices between consumer items) have been found to
just the negative consequences of fear, stress, anxiety, and desist more quickly in a second, unrelated self-control task
interpersonal conflicts, thus presents an exciting and pro- (Baumeister, Vohs, & Tice, 2007). Questions remain as to
ductive focus for applied research. the mechanisms underlying such diverse effects. One sug-
There is little likelihood that emphasis on the sunny side gestion is that such tasks temporarily reduce blood glucose
of human functioning will gain as large a share of research levels, resulting in subsequent impairments in self-regula-
activity as it does shelf space in the popular psychology tion (Gailliot & Baumeister, 2007). Another possibility is
section of our bookstores. Research on so-called negative that the initial task merely diminishes an individual’s moti-
topics is too important to our collective efforts to address vation to persevere at a subsequent task—a possibility sup-
important societal problems and generate insights relevant ported by the fact that financial incentives appear to restore
to normal human functioning (Ward, 2000). As is the case self-regulatory strength (Muraven & Slessareva, 2003).
with so many topics, the future of positive psychology as a Whatever the mechanism producing these lapses in self-
field of empirical research will no doubt depend upon the control, as Baumeister et al. (2007) acknowledge, additional
conjoint influence of societal priorities, funding availabil- research is needed to understand how best to counteract
ity, methodological advances, and above all the capacity such failures. Some promising results have been generated
of researchers to show and explain phenomena interesting by research on implementation intentions, whereby indi-
and important enough to attract our best young minds. viduals are encouraged to anticipate how best to shield goal
pursuits from distracting influences (Gollwitzer & Sheeran,
Self-Regulation 2006). Likewise, activating a “high-level” construal of
Attendant with the general increase in research on the an event, focusing on global, superordinate features,
self has been a resurgence of concern with processes of as opposed to “low-level” construals focusing more on
Social Psychology Today 37

specific, subordinate features, has also been found to facil- in the face of continual experiences of events not produc-
itate successful self-control (Fujita, Trope, Liberman, & ing the degree of positivity or negativity they had expected,
Levin-Sagi, 2006). people continue to show levels of hope and anticipation (or
fear and avoidance) that prove unwarranted.
Hedonic Adaptation More recently, Wilson and Gilbert (2008) have inves-
In an important early study on hedonic adaptation, Brickman, tigated the conditions under which hedonic or affective
Coates, and Janoff-Bulman (1978) had lottery winners, adaptation does or does not take place. They argue that the
paralyzed accident victims, and control subjects assess inability to understand and explain an emotion-produc-
their past, present, and future happiness. Although the ing event (as any academic who has had a manuscript turned
results have sometimes been mischaracterized, and some down by a journal editor without a clear explanation for the
methodological problems need to be acknowledged (e.g., rejection can attest) forestalls the adaptation process that
more than a third of the accident victims failed to respond otherwise would occur. Indeed, with respect to negative
to some questions), the results made the study a classic. events, the predictions of the theory nicely dovetail with
On average, the three groups did not differ in “how happy the finding that people who can make sense of traumatic
they expected to be in a couple of years,” and while acci- events in their lives seem to recover better from the events
dent victims rated their present happiness levels as some- than those who cannot (Davis, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Larson,
what lower than did lottery winners, the latter did not differ 1998; Pennebaker, 1997; see also Kross, Ayduk, & Mischel,
from control group respondents. Moreover, even accident 2005). Interestingly, the theory also would seem to predict
victims rated their present happiness levels as, on average, that positive events will lose some of their potency when they
above the midpoint on the scale. can be “explained away.”
One suspects that the processes involved for these latter
Close Relationships
individuals (and the surprise of observers who imagine that
they would be less able to achieve even a moderate level of Although the dynamics of interpersonal attraction have long
life satisfaction) go beyond simple adaptation in the usual been a topic of study in social psychology (e.g., Aronson &
sense of the term. For example, most of the time the focus Linder, 1965; Berscheid, Dion, Walster, & Walster, 1971;
of attention of paralyzed individuals is not on their handicap Walster, Aronson, Abrahams, & Rottman, 1966), research
but on the activity in which they are currently engaged. And exploring the long-term health of relationships between dat-
many of those activities—savoring a good meal, watching a ing and married couples has only recently come into vogue
compelling film, enjoying the companionship of friends and (Clark & Lemay, volume 2). Much of that work employs
loved ones—are no less pleasurable; indeed, for some indi- methodologies associated more with traditional personality
viduals, they may be more pleasurable, because of that hand- psychology than social psychology. Thus, in a typical study,
icap. Conversely, lottery winners spend most of their time couples might come to a laboratory for observations of their
engaged in the same types of day-to-day tasks as the rest of interaction style, and those measurements would then be cor-
us; and, unless their attention is focused on matters financial, related with responses from both members of the couple to
a fat wallet or bank account does not enhance (and in fact various self-report surveys and/or to information culled from
may diminish) their experience of those tasks. But questions diary entries concerning their relationship over time, revealing
about precise mechanisms aside, the Brickman et al. study associations between interaction style and relevant outcome
does provide compelling evidence of our tendency to adapt measures (Aron & Aron, 1994; McNulty, O’Mara, & Karney,
hedonically to both positive and negative circumstances. 2008). This methodology presents problems familiar to all cor-
Twenty years later, Gilbert and Wilson and their collabo- relational research, but it allows researchers to pose questions
rators published the first in a series of elegant studies show- and explore relational factors that could never be investigated
ing how individuals fail to anticipate the mechanisms and in studies probing the responses of undergraduate college
consequences of hedonic adaptation, and thus fare poorly students interacting for 30 minutes with strangers. Recent
in forecasting the affective experience that everyday events innovations also include an interesting hybrid methodology, a
are likely to produce (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & variant of “speed dating,” whereby researchers use correlational
Wheatley, 1998; Wilson & Gilbert, 2003). This work has methods to investigate “first-date” behavior between strangers
drawn on many sources, including Nisbett and Wilson’s seeking partners, who are brought together for a series of brief
(1977) studies on the limits of introspection; work link- initial encounters (Finkel & Eastwick, 2008).
ing overconfident predictions to underappreciation of the Despite the use of largely correlational methods, many
vagaries of subjective construal (Griffin, Dunning, & Ross, contemporary studies of close relationships invoke con-
1990), and research on memory biases (Fredrickson & cepts central to social cognition. For example, one influ-
Kahneman, 1993). Its implications are unmistakable: Even ential program of research by Murray, Holmes, and Griffin
38 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

(1996a, 1996b) has extended earlier work by Taylor and virtual environments, for example, raises questions about
Brown (1988) on positive illusions from the realm of the once seemingly clear boundary between imagination
individuals to that of couples, arguing that long-term rela- and “real life” (Blascovich et al., 2002); at the same time,
tional satisfaction is most likely when the partners hold researchers can now explore phenomena that could not
idealized views of each other. Rusbult et al. (2000) simi- readily be pursued in face-to-face laboratory interactions.
larly found greatest relational endurance and satisfaction The opportunity to present complex stimuli to research
among partners who perceived themselves as superior to participants and measure their behavioral responses, even
other couples. Although such correlational findings pre- as they remain immobile in order to permit the simulta-
clude causal conclusions, they parallel results from other neous recording of their brain activity, raises especially
domains suggesting that an inflated, even delusional exciting possibilities. And there now exist unprecedented
sense of optimism and positivity is not only adaptive, but opportunities for the recording and analysis of the ongoing
can also prove self-fulfilling (see Reed, Kemeny, Taylor, information-seeking and computer-based “social” interac-
Wang, & Visscher, 1994). tions that occur as people conduct their lives in today’s
“digital” society.
Virtual Interaction
Social Cognitive Neuroscience
With the advent of email, the Internet, and related innovations
in communication technology have come unprecedented Finally, like our colleagues elsewhere in psychology, social
opportunities for “virtual interaction” with individuals all psychologists are increasingly taking advantage both of
over the world. Social networking sites like MySpace, older technologies such as electroencephalography (EEG)
Facebook, and Twitter have allowed people, particularly and newer ones including positron emission tomogra-
young people, to post and exchange information not only phy (PET) scanning, and, especially, functional magnetic
with intimate friends but also a network of acquaintances and resonance imagining (fMRI), in order to study, in vivo,
in many cases strangers. Social psychologists have begun brain activity accompanying and underlying behavioral
to explore the implications of such virtual interactions and responses (Cacioppo et al., 2007; Lieberman, this volume).
related phenomena, examining, for example, egocentric per- Classic topics such as attitudes, attribution, stereotyping,
ceptions when using email (e.g., Kruger, Epley, Parker, & motivation, and prejudice can now be explored though the
Ng, 2005) and the tendency to anthropomorphize inanimate lenses of neuroscience. In some cases, findings from such
objects such as one’s computer (Epley, Waytz, & Cacioppo, studies have served to buttress results from other domains
2007; Harris & Fiske, 2008; Reis & Gosling, this volume). of inquiry. For example, Phelps et al. (2000) found that
With all its promise of uniting our global village, the amygdala activation, which has been implicated in fear
ubiquity of computer-based communication has predictably responses, correlated with racial evaluations assessed by
prompted concerns that such virtual interactions are not a implicit measurement tools including the IAT. In other
full substitute for actual human contact, in fact that exces- cases, neuroscientific evidence has been marshaled in sup-
sive use of the Internet may be associated with decreases port of theoretical conjectures originally derived from other
in well-being (Kraut et al., 1998; but see Gosling, Vazire, sources, such as the distinction between controlled and
Srivastava, & John, 2004), at least among those already automatic processes involved in attribution (Lieberman,
vulnerable to social isolation (Kraut et al., 2002). Such Gaunt, Gilbert, & Trope, 2002).
concerns, perhaps fueled also by highly publicized cases In what may be its most promising use, neuroscientific
of ostracized youngsters carrying out murderous rampages data has also been employed to test specific social psycho-
against their classmates, have prompted researchers to logical hypotheses. Harris and Fiske (2006), for example,
begin exploring the effects of social isolation and exclu- asked participants undergoing fMRI to look at photographs
sion, which have now been linked to aggression (Twenge, of individuals belonging to socially stereotyped groups
Baumeister, Tice, & Stucke, 2001) and which some liken (e.g., elderly, disabled, or wealthy individuals). Photos of
to physical pain (MacDonald & Leary, 2005). Indeed, in groups who were low on both the dimensions of warmth
one study, individuals undergoing a brain scan who were and competence (i.e., homeless people and drug addicts)
excluded by two other “virtual” interaction partners appeared uniquely failed to trigger activation of the medial prefrontal
to show activations in areas of the brain typically associated cortex, a structure implicated in social cognition. Instead,
with pain (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003). activation patterns in response to members of those groups
While advances in computer and communication tech- paralleled responses to depictions of objects rather than
nology create new social concerns, they also provide people—chilling evidence for the hypothesized dehuman-
researchers with new tools for the studying of social pro- ization of these groups (Vaes, Paladino, Castelli, Leyens, &
cesses. Exploitation of increasingly realistic and immersive Giovanazzi, 2003).
Successful Applications and the Challenge of “Scaling Up” 39

SUCCESSFUL APPLICATIONS AND THE sympathetic to the goals of a project generally advocate the
CHALLENGE OF “SCALING UP” use of formal evaluation designs, with appropriate control
and comparison groups, objective outcome measures, and
We conclude this chapter with some examples of applied exploration of the subjective experiences of those affected
or “intervention” research that demonstrate the relevance by the intervention.
of social psychologists’ work for practitioners, decision Another old Lewinian insight involves the strategy of
makers, and funders outside the field. Such examples achieving change by removing rather than adding forces
should serve as a source of pride to those in our field who to an existing tension system—that is, instead of relying
are still inspired by George Miller’s (1969) clarion call to on positive and negative incentives (which can add “ten-
“give psychology away.” Increasingly, such work reflects sion” to a system), it may be more useful to determine
not only the experience- and tradition-based wisdom of what impediments or barriers stand in the way of achieving
practitioners but also the application of mainstream theory, change, and then eliminate or at least reduce them. Barriers
old as well as new, in social psychology. involving unhelpful group norms and pressures were gener-
One longstanding insight relates to the folly of assum- ally the ones emphasized in the applied Lewinian tradition.
ing that well-designed interventions will inevitably produce However, the insight is a very general one. The first step in
positive outcomes. Unanticipated and undesirable conse- designing a program to produce change is to analyze the
quences of seemingly straightforward interventions may sources of individual and/or collective resistance to such
occur for many reasons, including the possibility (noted change; and this analysis should include not just “social”
prominently in Lewin’s field theory formulations) that and “psychological” barriers but also situational and struc-
the removal of previously operative forces and constraints tural factors. Thus, failure of unemployed youth to utilize
may reveal and alter the dynamics of existing institutions a job training program may reflect negative group norms
and relationships that had previously served important and past experiences, but it may also reflect unrecognized
functions. (See Ross & Nisbett, 1991, pp. 208–216, for financial costs or other disincentives, or even the lack of
accounts of the Cambridge-Somerville project that was convenient public transportation to the training site.
designed to serve Depression-era youth deemed to be at Some of the newer insights involve application of prin-
high risk for crime and delinquency but that proved, if ciples derived from dissonance, self-perception, attribution,
anything, to be counterproductive; also the Seattle-Denver and prospect theory. But others reflect advances in evalu-
“guaranteed income” pilot project, which increased rather ation methods. In particular, researchers have increasingly
than decreased rates of marital dissolution; and other dis- come to recognize the value of measures that directly or
appointing intervention results.) indirectly assess the processes assumed to produce change.
At the same time, experience teaches us that interven- We are referring here less to the use of complex statisti-
tion success can sometimes prove easier to achieve than cal analyses to tease apart a number of different possible
expected because of so-called “Hawthorne” effects (Mayo, mediating variables than to the simpler task of finding out
1933, 1945; Roethlisberger, 1941)8 or other factors that whether an intervention did in fact change some factor
make participants in intervention experiments strive to or process that the program designers thought in need of
produce results welcome to the experimenter (Rosenzweig, address, and whether that change in process was associ-
1933). In fact, unpredictability of outcomes is one of the ated with positive outcomes. For example, in evaluating
reasons why even those social psychologists who are most a program designed to improve academic performance
by increasing students’ sense of “belonging” (Walton &
Cohen, 2007), it is important not only to determine whether
students’ grades improved but also to determine both
8 It should be noted that later critics, such as Bramel and Friend whether the intervention did in fact increase the students’
(1981) challenged and indeed “debunked” traditional secondary sense of belonging and whether the students whose grades
accounts of increased worker productivity as a simple response to improved the most were those whose sense of belonging
attention and awareness of being in a study. These critics pointed
increased the most, and vice versa.
out, among other things, that the Depression-era participants in
Throughout this chapter, we have documented both
the study were made aware that increased productivity in response
to the relevant changes in working conditions, even unwelcome
early and contemporary instances of “mainstream” labo-
ones, was a not only a goal but perhaps a requirement if they were ratory-based social psychological research with important
to retain their jobs and later achieve better conditions. But the implications for potential intervention. The past 40 years
larger point—that the targets of research actively interpret their have also seen the growth of a tradition in social psychol-
circumstances and respond to what they believe will be the con- ogy with a more explicit and extensive emphasis on applied
sequences of their actions— is uncontestable. work—one recognized with the publication of the first issue
40 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

of the Journal of Applied Social Psychology in 1971 and Ward, & Mann, 2007). The finding by Dal Cin et al. (2006)
the subsequent opening of a number of graduate programs that self-reported condom use in response to a safe-sex mes-
specifically devoted to training students in applied meth- sage was doubled when that message was accompanied by
odology (Schneider, Gruman, & Coutts, 2005). A related a “reminder” bracelet is particularly noteworthy. It is also
enterprise involves the significant contributions made by reminiscent of the much earlier finding by Leventhal,
social psychologists to the study of topics relevant to the Singer, and Jones (1965) that combining a persuasive com-
legal system. As discussed earlier, their investigations have munication with a simple “channel factor” manipulation
exposed the potential unreliability of memories for events, (e.g., providing students with a campus map with the uni-
eyewitness identifications of criminal suspects, and even versity health center circled and asking them to find a con-
confessions offered by those in police custody. They have venient time to visit it) produced an eightfold increase in
also shed light on the group dynamics of jury delibera- tetanus inoculations over the very low rate achieved with
tions and the cognitive and motivational biases that reveal the communication alone. Important advances have also
themselves when citizens are called to render verdicts, been made in the areas of smoking reduction (Strahan et
including, notably, decisions in potential capital punish- al., 2002; Westling, Mann, & Ward, 2006), and combat-
ment cases (for a review of this large and important area of ing underage drinking. In the latter case, Schroeder and
applied research, see Kovera & Borgida, volume 2). Prentice (1998) produced a 40% reduction in alcohol con-
We can also cite instances in which mainstream theorists sumption among research participants simply by counter-
skilled in the art of the laboratory experiment have ventured ing the prevailing pluralistic ignorance on the Princeton
outside the bounds of academia and engaged in direct inter- campus about the relevant social norms.
ventions in applied settings—particularly in the domains of In more recent work in the realm of education, Claude
health, education, and environmental conservation. Many of Steele and his associates applied the valuable lessons
these social psychologists have followed the Lewinian tradition learned in research on both self-affirmation and stereo-
of bringing rigorous theory to bear on significant real-world type threat to design the “21st Century Program” at the
problems. For example, Aronson and his colleagues have University of Michigan. The program, which included a
employed dissonance-inducing “hypocrisy” techniques to challenging curriculum modeled after the pioneering work
both enhance safer sexual practices among young adults and of University of California, Berkeley, mathematics profes-
promote water conservation (Dickerson, Thibodeau, Aronson, sor Uri Triesman, admitted ethnic majority and minority
& Miller, 1992; Stone, Aronson, Crain, Winslow, & Fried, students in an “honorific” fashion, avoiding the stigmatiz-
1994). In these interventions, participants were induced to ing labels associated with most remediation programs, and
make public statements strongly endorsing behaviors (e.g., explicitly affirming to students their potential for success.
using condoms, taking short showers) just after having been The promising results of this intervention and related ones
made aware that their own behavior in such regards has been are described by Steele (2010). Others, working within
less than exemplary. Participants thus motivated to reduce the this same theoretical framework, have shown the power
resulting state of dissonance subsequently engaged in more of short-term interventions affirming students’ sense of
of the behavior that they had “hypocritically” advocated. self-worth (Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, & Master, 2006), or, as
Aronson also pioneered the jigsaw classroom technique, alluded to earlier, belongingness (Walton & Cohen, 2007)
whereby students cooperatively provide each other with to enhance school achievement.
pieces of information they require to finish their projects These and many other successful interventions, including
(Aronson, Blaney, Stephin, Sikes, & Snapp, 1978; Aronson & those by particularly skilled and dedicated classroom teach-
Patnoe, 1997)—a technique shown to reduce racial ten- ers and administrators whose use of sound social psycholog-
sion and enhance scholastic performance (Aronson, 1990). ical principles (as well as sound educational principles and a
Similar techniques have been advocated to reduce the threat lot of hard work) might be more instinctive and less formal,
of school violence at the hands of socially rejected students add to a growing body of social and developmental psychol-
(Aronson, 2000). ogy literature that suggests that academic performance, and
Other social psychologists well known for both their indeed intelligence itself, is anything but fixed (J. Aronson,
experimental and applied work have made important con- Fried, & Good, 2002; Diamond, Barnett, Thomas, & Munro,
tributions in the field of health (Taylor, 2008). Salovey, 2007; Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, & Perrig, 2008; Rueda,
Rothman, and colleagues have successfully used framing Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, & Posner, 2005). In one
techniques to tailor messages to enhance health-promoting powerful illustration, Blackwell, Trzesniewsk, and Dweck
behaviors (Banks et al. 1995, Detweiler, Bedell, Salovey, (2007) showed that persuading junior high school students
Pronin, & Rothman, 1999; Rothman & Salovey, 1997; that intelligence is malleable was associated with a reversal
Rothman, Salovey, Antone, & Keough, 1993; see also Parent, of the downward trajectory in grades shown by their peers
Successful Applications and the Challenge of “Scaling Up” 41

over the same time period (see Schwartz, 1997; see also few could dispute that a half century of work in social psy-
Nisbett (2009) for an illuminating discussion of successes chology and related fields has armed us with an impressive
and disappointments in preschool, primary school, and high- collection of potential tools for changing behavior. Some
school intervention projects, with a particular emphasis on of these intervention tools entail changes in social and situ-
contributions by social psychologists). ational pressures and constraints; others entail changes in
The conclusion supported by such research efforts is perceptions or “definitions” of situations and/or interven-
worth underscoring. We now have solid “existence proofs” tion efforts. Finally, as many have documented, positive
of the possibility of helping disadvantaged students greatly intervention outcomes are not only possible but a matter
narrow the achievement gap separating them from more of proven fact.
advantaged students through wise educational and social Even in the domain of education, as we have just noted,
psychological interventions. At the same time, it must be successful small-scale interventions with disadvantaged
acknowledged that many of the hopeful claims of educators students who face a daunting array of personal and situ-
about ways to close the gap through better physical plants, ational challenges have been demonstrated. These success
smaller class sizes, more stringent education requirements stories are important because of the specific lessons they
for teachers, or greater racial, ethnic, and socioeconomic offer practitioners both about techniques of instruction
diversity, have not been supported by the data from rigor- and about the social and social psychological factors that
ous evaluation research (although in the case of class size, are necessary to make educational efforts more fruitful for
the issue is really one of cost-effectiveness, since the ben- more students. But they perhaps are even more important
efits, though small, seem to be well-documented). insofar as they challenge both conservative pessimism
In a number of settings, Robert Cialdini and his col- about the educability of such children and the equally pes-
laborators have harnessed the power of classic social- simistic radical claim that real progress—in the absence of
psychological techniques, most notably the use of “social huge “structural” change in society—is impossible.
proof ” or messages about group norms, to address environ- What remains to be proven is our capacity to move from
mental concerns. For example, in one study, in which the successful small, one-site interventions to achieve similar
outcome measure was reuse of hotel towels that are oth- success in the kind of district-wide, city-wide, statewide or
erwise replaced daily, they showed that pairing a standard nationwide programs that would really make a difference in
pro-environmental message with norm information (i.e., American society. Some of the challenges of such “scaling
“75% of guests participate in our new resource savings up” are financial and logistic (finding and training additional
program”) proved significantly more effective than other skilled, dedicated teachers and aides to implement new
potentially persuasive messages that omitted such informa- practices); some are political (gaining cooperation of teach-
tion (Goldstein, Cialdini, & Griskevicius, 2008). Cialdini ers’ unions and administrators reluctant to accept the input
and company have similarly used influence techniques of “outsiders”), and some are psychological (replicating
involving norms and norm violations to reduce littering the sense of mission on the part of program providers, and the
(Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990) and power consump- sense of specialness, belonging, and privilege on the part
tion (Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini, Goldstein, & Griskevicius, of students). Some relate to features of small programs that
2007), and they have also pointed out that admonishing necessarily could not be replicated in system-wide contexts
messages about the putative dire consequences stemming (for example, the ability to exclude especially problematic
from the high frequency of norm violation can have the per- students or uncooperative parents, or the requirement that
verse effect of making such violations seem more socially parents and students alike invest substantial amounts of
acceptable. time and energy to get into, and to stay in, the program).
The success of these types of small and medium scale Research to determine which of these features matter most
intervention projects, and increasing recognition by and devising ways to replicate these features in large-scale
researchers and government officials alike of three impor- programs or, where necessary, to compensate for their
tant facts, set the stage for the scaling-up problem to which absence, remains a vital applied undertaking.
we now turn. First, inside or outside of academia, and While these challenges are daunting, we should remem-
regardless of general political conviction, few would dis- ber that we have some lessons from the history of applied
agree that individual and collective problems of behavior research and the evaluation of natural experiments result-
lie at the heart of the most difficult social issues burdening ing from changes in social policy to guide us. For exam-
our society (specifically, the interrelated complex of factors ple, the real lessons of the Hawthorne effect studies (and
at the root of minority and underclass academic under- studies of demand characteristics and confirmation biases
achievement, homelessness, high unemployment, absent more generally), those of studies attesting to the effects
fathers, drug abuse, high incarceration rates, etc.). Second, of behavioral commitment, and above all the consistent
42 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

message from studies on the importance of perceived in ways that serve the greater good. Norms, of course,
norms, remind us of the unique factors that may be at play are not communicated solely through policies and practices.
when dedicated professionals apply their skills in small- As social psychologists have long recognized, the media
scale or medium scale intervention studies. Some of these can play a role as well. It is said that when Clark Gable
factors may be difficult to duplicate on the massive scale removed his shirt to reveal a bare chest in the romantic
required to make a difference in society at large, although comedy It Happened One Night, canny undershirt manu-
they should not be ignored; others (for example, creat- facturers recognized that trouble lay ahead.
ing a sense of belongingness, making use of mentors and Today, all over the world, telenovenas and radio daytime
role models, and clearly communicating to students the dramas are being used by human rights and women’s rights
message that success is indeed achievable for them, that activists, who make effective use of fictional role models
abilities are malleable, and that persistent effort pays off) to motivate and guide women to protect their own health,
readily can be addressed. safety, and dignity, and that of their children (Bandura,
Moreover, lessons from the laboratory and observa- 2006; Rogers et al., 1999). This intervention has its roots
tion of real-world events also help us appreciate factors in work that was begun over half a century ago by Albert
that may make it easier, in fact necessary, to intervene on Bandura (Bandura & Huston, 1961) showing the ways in
a large scale rather than a small one—factors involving which children learn from, and imitate, the positive and
not only economies of scale but also channel factors, the negative behavior of social “models,” and it skillfully
communication of social norms or “default” choices (as in applies the social learning theory principles that Bandura
the “opt-in” vs. “opt-out” studies), and the effects of mass developed and shared with the world (Bandura, 1977b). In
media role-models, which we have been investigating for a society where so many problems remain, and in a world
many decades. A striking example relevant not to educa- beset by so much conflict and so much need, the challenge
tion but to environmentalism makes this point. Consider to younger researchers to learn from, and imitate the suc-
the enormous increase over the last two or three decades cessful models of intervention reviewed here could not be
in the practice of recycling various materials that once clearer.
clogged our refuse dumps and (if burned or buried) were Robert F. Kennedy, who in many ways represented a
a source of ground, air, and water pollution. Thirty years standard-bearer for his generation’s interventionist spirit,
ago, this practice, at least in urban and suburban America, eloquently posed this challenge in two famous quotations.
was largely restricted to a small, socially conscious, largely The first, distinctly Lewinian in its plea to look beyond
liberal elite who duly separated the relevant materials and conventional models of change and identify and address
took them in their cars to neighborhood or district recy- barriers to societal transformation, was paraphrased from
cling centers, from which they were transported to larger the Irish playwright George Bernard Shaw: “There are
centers for processing. Today, in many towns and cities, those who look at things the way they are, and ask why.
placing recyclable products into a single large container I dream of things that never were, and ask why not.” The
(left, not incidentally, in public view), which is emptied second offers a boost to the collective self-efficacy of our
in large trucks on trash-collection days, is a standard field: “Few will have the greatness to bend history itself;
practice rather than an expression of social and political but each of us can work to change a small portion of events,
values. Indeed, not recycling constitutes a deviant act, an and in the total of all those acts will be written the history
act at odds with civic virtue, something akin to not shovel- of this generation.” We can think of no more appropriate
ing snow or raking the leaves in front of one’s home, or message with which to end this chapter.
(in some communities at least) not joining the PTA when
asked to do so on the first day of school.
The virtuous cycle is clear—changes in practice make REFERENCES
compliance easier, ease increases the rate of compliance,
the increased rate of compliance make non-compliance Abelson, R. P. (1995). Statistics as principled argument. Hillsdale,
deviant and therefore unacceptable to self and others. As NJ: Erlbaum.
Festinger and Bem could tell us, something is undoubtedly Abelson, R. P., Frey, K. P., & Gregg, A. P. (2004). Experiments with
lost in this virtuous cycle. When compliance becomes easy people: Revelations from social psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
and expected, the actions in question no longer become a Allport, G. W. (1954a). The historical background of modern social psy-
chology. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (1st ed.,
motive for internalization of values or a cue for personal Vol. 1, pp. 3–56). Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
identity. But society as a whole benefits—and social psy- Allport, G. W. (1954b). The nature of prejudice. Oxford, UK: Addison-Wesley.
chologists in the Lewinian tradition can shift their energies Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histol-
to other strategies for transforming social beliefs and practices ogy of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the
References 43

behavioral stream. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable
social psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 201–271). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
Arendt, H. (1963). Eichmann in Jerusalem: A report on the banality of statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
evil. New York: Viking Press. 51, 1173–1182.
Aron, A. P., & Aron, E. N. (1994). Love. In A. H. Weber & J. H. Harvey Baumeister, R. F. (Ed.). (1999). The self in social psychology. Philadelphia:
(Eds.), Perspectives in close relationships (pp. 131–152). Needham Psychology Press.
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (Eds.). (2004). Handbook of self-regula-
Aronson, E. (1969). A theory of cognitive dissonance. In L. Berkowitz tion: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford.
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 1–34). Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as the
New York: Academic Press. science of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to
Aronson, E. (1990). Applying social psychology to desegregation and energy actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 396–403.
conservation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 118–132.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of
Aronson, E. (2000). Nobody left to hate: Teaching compassion after self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351–355.
Columbine. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Baumrind, D. (1964). Some thoughts on ethics of research: After reading
Aronson, E., Blaney, N., Stephin, C., Sikes, J., & Snapp, M. (1978). The Milgram’s “Behavioral Study of Obedience.” American Psychologist,
jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 19, 421–423.
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1963). Effect of the severity of threat
Bem, D. J. (1965). An experimental analysis of self-persuasion. Journal of
on the devaluation of forbidden behavior. Journal of Abnormal and
Experimental Social Psychology, 1, 199–218.
Social Psychology, 66, 584–588.
Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cogni-
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1968). Experimentation in social psychol-
tive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183–200.
ogy. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology
(2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 1–79). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York:
Aronson, E., & Linder, D. (1965). Gain and loss of esteem as determi-
Academic Press.
nants of interpersonal attractiveness. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 1, 156–171. Berscheid, E., Dion, K., Walster, E., & Walster, G. W. (1971). Physical
attractiveness and dating choice: A test of the matching hypothesis.
Aronson, E., & Mills, J. (1959). The effect of severity of initiation on liking
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 173–189.
for a group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 177–181.
Bierbrauer, G. (1979). Why did he do it? Attribution of obedience and
Aronson, E., & Patnoe, S. (1997). The jigsaw classroom: Building coopera-
the phenomenon of dispositional bias. European Journal of Social
tion in the classroom (2nd ed.). New York: Addison Wesley Longman.
Psychology, 9, 67–84.
Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. M. (2007). Social psychology
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit
(6th ed.). Garden City, NJ: Prentice Hall.
theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent tran-
Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereo- sition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development,
type threat on African American college students by shaping theories of 78, 246–263.
intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125.
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Klick, J., Mellers, B., Mitchell, G., & Tetlock, P. E.
Arrow, K., Mnookin, R. H., Ross, L., Tversky, A., & Wilson, R. (Eds.). (2009). Strong claims and weak evidence: Reassessing the predictive
(1995). Barriers to conflict resolution. New York: W. W. Norton. validity of the IAT. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 567–582.
Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and Blascovich, J., Loomis, J., Beall, A., Swinth, K., Hoyt, C., & Bailenson, J.
distortion of judgments. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership and (2002). Immersive virtual environment technology as a research tool
men (pp. 177–190). Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press. for social psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 103–125.
Asch, S. E. (1952). Social psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Blass, T. (2004). The man who shocked the world: The life and legacy of
Bandura, A. (1977a). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral Stanley Milgram. New York: Basic Books.
change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. Bossard, J. H. S. (1932). Residential propinquity as a factor in marriage
Bandura, A. (1977b). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: selection. American Journal of Sociology, 38, 219–224.
Prentice-Hall. Bramel, D., & Friend, R. (1981). Hawthorne, the myth of the docile worker,
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: and class bias in psychology. American Psychologist, 36, 867–878.
W. H. Freeman. Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. Oxford, UK:
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhuman- Academic Press.
ities. Personality and Social Psychology Review 3, 193–209. Brewer, M. B., & Chen, Y. (2007). Where (who) are collectives in collec-
Bandura, A. (2006). Going global with social cognitive theory: From prospect tivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individualism and collectiv-
to paydirt. In S. I. Donaldson, D. E. Berger, & K. Pezdek (Eds.), Applied ism. Psychological Review, 114, 133–151.
psychology: New frontiers and rewarding careers. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners
Bandura, A., & Huston, A. C. (1961). Identification as a process of incidental and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and
learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 311–318. Social Psychology, 36, 917–927.
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963). Imitation of film-mediated aggres- Burger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would people still obey today?
sive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 3–11. American Psychologist, 64, 1–11.
Banks, S. M., Salovey, P., Greener, S., Rothman, A. J., Moyer, A., Beauvais, J., Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating.
et al. (1995). The effects of message framing on mammography utiliza- New York: Basic Books.
tion. Health Psychology, 14, 178–184. Cacioppo, J. T., Amaral, D. G., Blanchard, J. J., Cameron, J. L., Carter, C. S.,
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social Crews, D., et al. (2007). Social neuroscience: Progress and implica-
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on tions for mental health. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2,
action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244. 99–123.
44 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1992). Social psychological contri- Deutsch, M. (1977). The resolution of conflict: Constructive and destructive
butions to the decade of the brain: Doctrine of multilevel analysis. processes. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
American Psychologist, 47, 1019–1028. Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool
Cartwright, D. (1949). Some principles of mass persuasion: Selected find- program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387–1388.
ings of research on the sale of U.S. war bonds. Human Relations, 2, Dickerson, C. A., Thibodeau, R., Aronson, E., & Miller, D. (1992). Using
253–267. cognitive dissonance to encourage water conservation. Journal of
Carlsmith, J. M., Ellsworth, P. C., & Aronson, E. (1976). Methods of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 841–854.
research in social psychology. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). The relation between per-
Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and ception and behavior, or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Journal
personality: Emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 865–877.
Psychology Bulletin, 26, 741–751. Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A.
Caspi, A., Bem, D. J., & Elder, G. H. (1989). Continuities and consequences (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes.
of interactional styles across the life course. Journal of Personality, 57, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510–540.
375–406. Dunning, D. (2005). Self-insight: Roadblocks and detours on the path to
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1987). Moving against the world: knowing thyself. New York: Psychology Press.
Life-course patterns of explosive children. Developmental Psychology, Dunning, D., Griffin, D. W., Milojkovic, J. D., & Ross, L. (1990). The
23, 308–313. overconfidence effect in social prediction. Journal of Personality and
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1988). Moving away from the world: Social Psychology, 58, 568–581.
Life-course patterns of shy children. Developmental Psychology, 24, Dunning, D., & Stern, L. B. (1994). Distinguishing accurate from inaccu-
824–831. rate eyewitness identifications via inquiries about decision processes.
Cialdini, R. B. (2007). Influence: The psychology of persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 818–835.
New York: HarperCollins.
Dutton, D. G., & Aron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual
Cialdini, R. B. (2009). We have to break up. Perspectives on psychological attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality and
science, 4, 5–6. Social Psychology 30, 510–517.
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness.
normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering Oxford, England: Academic Press.
in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
Dweck, C. A. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American
1015–1026.
Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048.
Cialdini, R. B., Vincent, J. E., Lewis. S. K., Catalan, J., Wheeler, D., &
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality,
Darby, B. L. (1975). Reciprocal concessions procedure for inducing
and development. Philadelphia, PA: The Psychology Press.
compliance: The door-in-the-face technique. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 31, 206–215. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to
motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.
Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B. F., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult,
aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An “experimental ethnog- Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort
raphy.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 945–960. Worth, TX: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in
racial achievement gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science, human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American
313, 1307–1310. Psychologist, 54, 408–423.
Cohen, J. (1990). Things I have learned (so far). American Psychologist, Eberhardt, J. L., Goff, P. A., Purdie, V. J., & Davies, P. G. (2004). Seeing
45, 1304–1312. black: Race, crime, and visual processing. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 87, 876–893.
Copeland, L., Lamm, L. W., & McKenna, S. J. (Eds.). (1999). The world’s
great speeches: Fourth enlarged (1999) edition. Mineola, NY: Dover Edwards, K., & Smith, E. E. (1996). A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation
Publications. of arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 5–24.
Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., Wittenbrink, B., Sadler, M. S., & Keesee, T. Egan, L. C., Santos, L. R., & Bloom, P. (2007). The origins of cognitive
(2007). Across the thin blue line: Police officers and racial bias in the dissonance: Evidence from children and monkeys. Psychological
decision to shoot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, Science, 18, 978–983.
1006–1023. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejec-
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self- tion hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292.
protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608–630. Ekman, P. (2007). Emotions revealed (2nd ed.). New York: Holt.
Dal Cin, S., MacDonald, T. K., Fong, G. T., Zanna, M. P., & Elton-Marshall, Ellsworth, P. C. (2010). The rise and fall of the high-impact experiment.
T. E. (2006). Remembering the message: The use of a reminder cue In M. H. Gonzales, C. Tavris, & J. Aronson (Eds.), The scientist and
to increase condom use following a safer sex intervention. Health the humanist: A festschrift in honor of Elliot Aronson. New York:
Psychology, 25, 438–443. Psychology Press.
Darley, J. M., & Batson, C. D. (1973). “From Jerusalem to Jericho”: Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Henson, A. (1972). The stare as a
A study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behavior. stimulus to flight in human subjects: A series of field experiments.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 100–108. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 302–311.
Davis, C. G., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Larson, J. (1998). Making sense of Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-
loss and benefiting from the experience: Two construals of meaning. factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 561–574. 864–886.
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum. Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior:
Detweiler, J. B., Bedell, B. T., Salovey, P., Pronin, E., & Rothman, A. J. (1999). The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
Message framing and sunscreen use: Gain-framed messages motivate Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109).
beach-goers. Health Psychology, 18, 189–196. New York: Academic Press.
References 45

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Gilovich, T. (1987). Secondhand information and social judgment. Journal
Relations, 7, 117–140. of Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 59–74.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.). (2002). Heuristics and
Stanford University Press. biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. New York: Cambridge
Festinger, L. (1983). The human legacy. New York: Columbia University University Press.
Press. Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky, K. (2000). The spotlight effect
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced in social judgment: An egocentric bias in estimates of the salience of
compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203–210. one’s own actions and appearance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78, 211–222.
Festinger, L., Schachter, S., & Back, K. (1950). Social pressures in infor-
mal groups: A study of human factors in housing. New York: Harper. Gladwell, M. (2000). The tipping point: How little things can make a big
difference. New York: Little, Brown and Company.
Finkel, E. J., & Eastwick, P. W. (2008). Speed-dating. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 17, 193–197. Gladwell, M. (2008). Outliers: The story of success. New York: Little,
Brown and Company.
Fischoff, B. (1975). Hindsight is not equal to foresight: The effect of outcome
knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City,
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288–299. NY: Doubleday.
Fischhoff, B. (1982). For those condemned to study the past: Reflections Goldhagen, D. J. (1996). Hitler’s willing executioners: Ordinary Germans
on historical judgment. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky and the Holocaust. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
(Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a
Cambridge University Press. viewpoint: Using social norms to motivate environmental conservation
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition. New York: Random in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 472–482.
House. Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2008). Social cognition: From brains to cul- goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. In M. P.
ture. Boston: McGraw-Hill. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp.
Frank, R. H., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (1993). Does studying economics 69–119). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.
inhibit cooperation? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 7, 159–171. Gosling, S. D., & Mollaghan, D. M. (2006). Animal research in social psy-
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psy- chology: A bridge to functional genomics and other unique research
chology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American opportunities. In P. A. M. van Lange (Ed.), Bridging social psychol-
Psychologist, 56, 218–226. ogy: Benefits of transdisciplinary approaches (pp. 123–128). Mahwah
NJ: Erlbaum.
Fredrickson, B. L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retro-
spective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we
Social Psychology, 65, 45–55. trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions
about internet questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59, 93–104.
Freedman, J. L., & Fraser, S. C. (1966). Compliance without pressure:
The foot-in-the-door technique. Journal of Personality and Social Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition.
Psychology, 4, 195–202. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269–288.
Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and Greenberg, J. (2008). Understanding the vital human quest for self-esteem.
self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 351–367. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 48–55.
Gable, S. L., Gonzaga, G. C., & Strachman, A. (2006). Will you be there for Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes and con-
me when things go right? Supportive responses to positive event dis- sequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror management theory. In
closures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 904–917. R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public and private self (pp. 189–212). New
York: Springer-Verlag.
Gable, S. L., & Haidt, J. (2005). What (and why) is positive psychology?
Review of General Psychology, 9, 103–110. Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Arndt, J. (2007). A basic but uniquely human
motivation: Terror management. In J. Shah & W. Gardner (Eds.),
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of will-
Handbook of motivation science (pp. 114–134). New York: Guilford.
power: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 11, 303–327. Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of
Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618.
and Social Psychology, 26, 309–320. Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R.
Gergen, K. J. (1996). Social psychology as social construction: The emerging (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III.
vision. In C. McGarty & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The message of social psychol- Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social
ogy: Perspectives on mind in society (113–128). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Psychology, 97, 17–41.
Gilbert, D. T. (1998). Ordinary personology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T., Fiske, & Griffin, D. W., Dunning, D., & Ross, L. (1990). The role of construal pro-
G. Lindzey, (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, cesses in overconfident predictions about the self and others. Journal
pp. 89–150). New York: McGraw Hill. of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1128–1139.
Gilbert, D. T. (2006). Stumbling on happiness. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Griffin, D. W., & Ross, L. (1991). Subjective construal, social inference,
and human misunderstanding. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraint:
experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 319–359). New York:
Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and
Academic Press.
Social Psychology, 50, 269–280.
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busy- Haidt, J. (2006). The happiness hypothesis. New York: Basic Books.
ness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Hamilton, D. L. (Ed.). (1981). Cognitive processes in stereotyping and
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733–740. intergroup behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons. Science, 162,
P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective fore- 1243–1248.
casting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617–638. Harré, R. (1979). Social being. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
46 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1972). The actor and the observer: Divergent
low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological perceptions of the causes of the behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse,
Science, 17, 847–853. H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution:
Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2008). The brooms in Fantasia: Neural corre- Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 79–94). Morristown, NJ:
lates of anthropomorphizing objects. Social Cognition, 26, 210–223. General Learning Press.
Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W. J., & Kidd, R. F. (Eds.). (1976). New directions in Jones, J. M. (1997). Prejudice and racism (2nd ed.). New York:
attribution research (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McGraw-Hill.
Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W. J., & Kidd, R. F. (Eds.) (1978). New directions in Jordan, C. H., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Hoshino-Browne, E., & Correll, J.
attribution research (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (2003). Secure and defensive high self-esteem. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 85, 969–978.
Haselton, M. G., Mortezaie, M., Pillsworth, E. G., Bleske-Recheck, A. E., &
Frederick, D. A. (2007). Ovulation and human female ornamentation: Jordan, C. H., & Zanna, M. P. (2007). Not all experiments are created
Near ovulation, women dress to impress. Hormones and Behavior, 51, equal: On conducting and reporting persuasive experiments. In R. J.
40–45. Sternberg, D. Halpern, & H. L. Roediger (Eds.), Critical thinking in
psychology (pp. 160–176). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hastie, R., Ostrom. T. M., Ebbesen, E. B., Wyer, R. S., Hamilton, D. L., &
Carlston, D. E. (Eds.). (1980). Person memory: The cognitive basis of Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system jus-
social perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. tification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of
Social Psychology, 33, 1–27.
Hastie, R., Penrod, S.D., & Pennington, N. (1983). Inside the jury.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. (1986). Fairness as a constraint
on profit seeking: Entitlements in the market. American Economic
Hastorf, A., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case study. Journal
Review, 76, 728–741.
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under
Hebl, M. R., Foster, J. B., Mannix, L. M., & Dovidio, J. F. (2002). Formal
uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University
and interpersonal discrimination: A field study of bias toward homo-
Press.
sexual applicants. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
815–825. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment
of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430–454.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York:
Wiley. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction.
Psychological Review, 80, 237–251.
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a
motivational principle. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of deci-
social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 1–46). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. sions under risk. Econometrica, 47, 313–327.
Ichheiser, G. (1949). Misunderstandings in human relations: A study in Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames.
false social perception. American Journal of Sociology (Supplement), American Psychologist, 39, 341–350.
55, 1–70. Kassin, S. M. (2008). False confessions: Causes, consequences, and impli-
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the role of choice: cations for reform. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17,
A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality 249–253.
and Social Psychology, 76, 349–366. Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004). Material prim-
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: Can ing: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal
one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Social and competitive behavioral choice. Organizational Behavior and
Psychology, 79, 995–1006. Human Decision Processes 95, 83–96.
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving Keizer, K., Lindenderg, S., & Steg, L. (2008). The spreading of disorder.
fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the Science, 322, 1681–1685.
National Academy of Sciences, 105, 6829–6833. Kelley, H. H. (1950). The warm-cold variable in first impressions of per-
Janis, I. L. (1972). Victims of groupthink. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. sons. Journal of Personality, 18, 431–439.
Johnson, E. J., & Goldstein, D. (2003). Do defaults save lives? Science, Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine
302, 1338–1339. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192–241).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska.
Jonas, E., Schimel, J., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2002). The
Scrooge effect: Evidence that mortality salience increases prosocial Kelley, H. H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. In E.
attitudes and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, &
28, 1342–1353. B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior
(pp. 151–174). Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Jones, E. E. (1985). Major developments in social psychology during the
past five decades. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American
psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 47–108). New York: Random House. Psychologist, 28, 107–128.
Jones, E. E. (1990). Interpersonal perception. New York: W. H. Freeman. Kelman, H. C. (1968). A time to speak: On human values and social
research. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. (1978). Control of attributions about the self
through self-handicapping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and the Keltner, D. (2009). Born to be good: The science of a meaningful life.
role of underachievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, New York: W. W. Norton.
4, 200–206. Kitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R., & Suzuki, T. (2004). Is there
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965) From acts to dispositions: The attri- any “free” choice?: Self and dissonance in two cultures. Psychological
bution process in social psychology. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances Science, 15, 527–533.
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219–266). New York: Krauss, R. M., & Deutsch, M. (1966). Communication in interpersonal bar-
Academic Press. gaining. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 572–577.
Jones, E. E., Kanouse, D. E., Kelley, H. H., Nisbett, R. E., Valins, S., & Kraut, R., Kiesler, S., Boneva, B., Cummings, J. N., Helgeson, V., &
Weiner, D. (Eds.). (1972). Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behav- Crawford, A. M. (2002). Internet paradox revisited. Journal of Social
ior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Issues, 58, 49–74.
References 47

Kraut, R., Patterson, M., Lundmark, V., Kiesler, S., Mukopadhyay, T., & Loftus, E.F. (2003) Make-believe memories. American Psychologist, 58,
Scherlis, W. (1998). Internet paradox: A social technology that 864–873.
reduces social involvement and psychological well-being? American Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and
Psychologist, 53, 1017–1031. attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently con-
Kross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking “why” does not sidered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
hurt: Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative 2098–2109.
emotions. Psychological Science, 16, 709–715. Lorge, I., Fox, D., Davitz, J., & Brenner, M. (1958). A survey of studies
Krueger, J. I., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Towards a balanced social psy- contrasting the quality of group perfomance and individual perfor-
chology: Causes, consequences, and cures for the problem-seeking mance, 1920–1957. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 337–372.
approach to social behavior and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Lott, B., & Lott, A. J. (1985). Learning theory in contemporary social psy-
Sciences, 27, 313–327. chology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social
Kruger, J., Epley, N., Parker, J., & Ng, Z. (2005). Egocentrism over e-mail: psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 109–135). New York: Random House.
Can we communicate as well as we think? Journal of Personality and
Luce, R. D., & Raiffa, H. (1957). Games and decisions: Introduction and
Social Psychology, 89, 925–936.
critical survey. New York: Wiley.
Kunda, Z. (1987). Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evalu-
Lyubomirsky, S. (2007). The how of happiness: A scientific guide to get-
ation of causal theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
ting the life you want. New York: Penguin Press.
53, 636–647.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent
Langer, E. (2002). Well-being: Mindfulness versus positive evaluation.
positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin,
In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology
131, 803–855.
(pp. 214–230). New York: Oxford University Press.
LaPiere, R. (1934). Attitudes vs. actions. Social Forces, 13, 230–237. MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt?
The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander interven-
Bulletin, 131, 202–223.
tion in emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
10, 215–221. Maher, B. (1968). Principles of psychopathology: An experimental
approach. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Lawrence, D. H., & Festinger, L. (1962). Deterrents and reinforce-
ments: The psychology of insufficient reward. Stanford, CA: Stanford Mann, T., Sherman, D., & Updegraff, J. (2004). Dispositional motivations
University Press. and message framing: A test of the congruency hypothesis in college
students. Health Psychology, 23, 330–334.
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-
esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal Mann, T., & Ward, A. (2007). Attention, self-control, and health behaviors.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518–530. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 280–283.
Lepper, M. R. (1983). Social control processes and the internaliza- Marks, G., & Miller, N. (1987). Ten years of research on the false-consensus
tion of social values: An attributional perspective. In E. T. Higgins, effect: An empirical and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin,102,
D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social 72–90.
development (pp. 294–330). New York: Cambridge University Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the
Press. self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63–78.
Lepper, M. R. (2009). Two worlds of social psychology. Unpublished man- Markus, H. R. (2008). Pride, prejudice, and ambivalence: Toward a unified
uscript, Stanford University. theory of race and ethnicity. American Psychologist, 63, 651–670.
Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (Eds.). (1978). The hidden costs of reward. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98,
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining chil- 224–253.
dren’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the “overjus-
Marlowe, D., Frager, R., & Nuttall, R. L. (1965). Commitment to action
tification” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
taking as a consequence of cognitive dissonance. Journal of Personality
28, 129–137.
and Social Psychology, 2, 864–868.
Lerner, M. (1980). The belief in a just world. New York: Plenum Press.
Mayo, E. (1933). The human problems of an industrial civilization. New
Leventhal, H., Singer, R., & Jones, S. (1965). Effects of fear and specificity York: Macmillan.
of recommendation upon attitudes and behavior. Journal of Personality
Mayo, E. (1945). The social problems of an industrial civilization.
and Social Psychology, 2, 20–29.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lewin, K. (1931). The conflict between Aristotelian and Galileian modes of
thought in contemporary psychology. Journal of General Psychology, McArthur, L. Z., & Post, D. L. (1977). Figural emphasis and person per-
5, 141–177. ception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 520–535.
Libby, L. K., Eibach, R. P., & Gilovich, T. (2005). Here’s looking at me: McClelland, D.C. (1961) The achieving society. Princeton: Van Nostrand.
The effect of memory perspective on assessments of personal change. McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the modern
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 50–62. racism scale. In J. F. Dovidio, & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimi-
Liberman, V., Samuels, S. M., & Ross, L. (2004). The name of the game: nation, and racism (pp. 91–125). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Predictive power of reputations versus situational labels in determining McGuire, W. J. (1967). Some impending reorientations in social psy-
Prisoner’s Dilemma game moves. Personality and Social Psychology chology: Some thoughts provoked by Kenneth Ring. Journal of
Bulletin, 30, 1175–1185. Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 124–139.
Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflexion McGuire, W. J. (1969). Attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey &
and reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attribu- E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 3,
tional inference. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental pp. 136–314). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 199–249). San Diego, CA: Academic
McGuire, W. J. (1973). The yin and yang of progress in social psychol-
Press.
ogy: Seven koan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26,
Lippmann, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace. 446–456.
48 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

McNulty, J. K., O’Mara, E. M., & Karney, B. R. (2008). Benevolent cogni- Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of social cog-
tions as a strategy of relationship maintenance: “Don’t sweat the small nitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56, 717–734.
stuff” . . . But it is not all small stuff. Journal of Personality and Social Orne, M. T., & Holland, C. H. (1968). On the ecological validity of labora-
Psychology, 94, 631–646. tory deceptions. International Journal of Psychiatry, 6, 282–293.
Merton, R.K. (1948). The self-fulfilling prophecy. Antioch Review, 8, Packard, V. (1957). The hidden persuaders. New York: McKay Co.
193–210.
Parent, S., Ward, A., & Mann. T. (2007). Health information processed
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., & Nitzberg, R. A. (2005).
under limited attention: Is it better to be “hot” or “cool”? Health
Attachment, caregiving, and altruism: Boosting attachment security
Psychology, 26, 159–164.
increases compassion and helping. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 89, 817–839. Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Opening up: The healing power of expressing
emotions (Rev. ed.). New York: Guilford.
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persua-
and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378.
sion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York:
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority. New York: Harper & Row. Springer-Verlag.
Miller, A. G. (1986). The obedience experiments: A case study of contro- Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama,
versy in social science. New York: Praeger. E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., et al. (2000). Performance on indirect
Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of
54, 1053–1060. Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738.
Miller, G. A. (1969). Psychology as a means of promoting human welfare. Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social
American Psychologist, 24, 1063–1075. dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and
Miller, R. L., Brickman, P., & Bolen, D. (1975). Attribution versus persua- political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,
sion as a means for modifying behavior. Journal of Personality and 741–763.
Social Psychology, 31, 430–441. Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1992). When small effects are impressive.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 160–164.
Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the eye of
of personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252–283. the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others.
Psychological Review, 111, 781–799.
Mitchell, D. B. (2006). Nonconscious priming after 17 years: Invulnerable
Pronin, E., & Ross, L. (2006). Temporal differences in trait self ascription:
implicit memory? Psychological Science, 17, 925–929.
When the self is seen as an other. Journal of Personality and Social
Monin, B. (2007). Holier than me? Threatening social comparison in the Psychology, 90, 197–209.
moral domain. International Review of Social Psychology, 20, 53–68.
Pruitt, D. G., & Carnevale, P. J. (1993). Negotiation in social conflict.
Monin, B., & Miller, D.T. (2001). Moral credentials and the expression of Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 33–43.
Pruitt, D. G., & Rubin, J. Z. (1986). Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate,
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese and settlement. New York: Random House.
attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and
Pyszczynski, T., Abdollahi, A., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., &
Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.
Weise, D. (2006). Mortality salience, martyrdom, and military might:
Moscovici, S. (1976). Social influence and social change. London: The great Satan versus the axis of evil. Personality and Social
Academic Press. Psychology Bulletin, 32, 525–537.
Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanism of self-control failure: Quattrone, G. A. (1982). Overattribution and unit formation: When behav-
Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology ior engulfs the person. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Bulletin, 29, 894–906. 42, 593–607.
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996a). The benefits of Reed, G. M., Kemeny, M. E., Taylor, S. E., Wang, H. J., & Visscher, B. R.
positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in (1994). Realistic acceptance as a predictor of decreased survival time
close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, in gay men with AIDS. Health Psychology, 13, 299–307.
79–98. Roethlisberger, F. J. (1941). Management and morale. Cambridge, MA:
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996b). The self-fulfilling Harvard University Press.
nature of positive illusions in romantic relationships: Love is not blind, Rogers, E. M., Vaughn, P. W., Swalehe, R. M., Rao, N., Svenkerud, P., &
but prescient. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, Sood, S. (1999). Effects of an entertainment-education radio soap
1155–1180. opera on family planning in Tanzania. Studies in Family Planning, 30,
Newcomb, T. M. (1943). Personality and social change. New York: Dryden. 1193–1211.
Niesta, D., Fritsche, I., & Jonas, E. (2008). Mortality salience and its effects Rosenhan, D. L. (1973). On being sane in insane places. Science, 179,
on peace processes: A review. Social Psychology, 39, 48–58. 250–258.
Nisbett, R. E. (2009). Intelligence and how to get it: Why schools and Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom:
cultures count. New York: W. W. Norton. Teacher expectation and pupils’ intellectual development. New York:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of
violence in the South. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Rosenzweig, S. (1933). The experimental situation as a psychological
problem. Psychological Review, 40, 337–354.
Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and short-
comings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings:
Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
Nisbett, R. E., & Valins, S. (1971). Perceiving the causes of one’s own
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–240). New York:
behavior. New York: General Learning Press.
Academic Press.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Ross, L., Greene D., & House, P. (1977). The false consensus effect: An
Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal
231–259. of Experimental Social Psychology 13, 279–301.
References 49

Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (1953). Groups in harmony and tension. New
Perspectives of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. York: Harper.
Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1995). Psychological barriers to dispute resolu- Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent
tion. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay
(Vol. 27, pp. 255–304). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental
Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1996). Naive realism in everyday life: Implications Psychology, 26, 978–986.
for social conflict and misunderstanding. In E. S. Reed, E. Turiel, & Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the person
T. Brown (Eds.), Values and knowledge (pp. 103–135). Hillsdale, vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture
NJ: Erlbaum. theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 158–199). Cambridge,
Rothman, A. J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate England: Cambridge University Press.
healthy behavior: The role of message framing. Psychological Bulletin, Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly
121, 3–19. Journal of Economics, 59, 99–118.
Rothman, A. J., Salovey, P., Antone, C., & Keough, K. (1993). The influ- Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in
ence of message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 408–433. Snibbe, A. C., & Markus, H. R. (2005). You can’t always get what you want:
Rubin, Z., & Moore, J. C., Jr. (1971). Assessment of subjects’ suspicions. Educational attainment, agency, and choice. Journal of Personality and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 163–170. Social Psychology, 88, 703–720.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Snyder, M. (1992). Motivational foundations of behavioral confirmation.
Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National (Vol. 25., pp. 67–114). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Academy of Sciences, 102, 14931–14936. Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and
Rusbult, C. E., Van Lange, P. A. M., Wildschut, T., Yovetich, N. A., & interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereo-
Verette, J. (2000). Perceived superiority in close relationships: Why it types. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656–666.
exists and persists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, Sommers, S. R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2001). White juror bias: An investiga-
521–545. tion of prejudice against Black defendants in the American courtroom.
Savitsky, K., Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Do others judge us as Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 201–229.
harshly as we think? Overestimating the impact of our failures, short- Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal
comings, and mishaps. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational
81, 44–56. analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford and Social Psychology, 89, 845–851.
University Press. Staats, A. W. (1975). Social behaviorism. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physi- Staats, A. W., & Staats, C. K. (1963). Complex human behavior. New
ological determinants of emotional state, Psychological Review, 69, York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
379–399. Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). San Diego, CA: Academic
115, 336–356. Press.
Schneider, D. J. (2004). The psychology of stereotyping. New York: Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual
Guilford. identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
Schneider, F. W., Gruman, J., & Coutts, L. M. (2005). Applied social Steele, C. M. (2010). Whistling Vivaldi. New York: W. W. Norton.
psychology: Understanding and addressing social and practical Stephens, N. M., Markus, H. R., & Townsend, S. S. M. (2007). Choice as
problems. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. an act of meaning: The case of social class. Journal of Personality and
Schroeder, C. M., & Prentice, D. A. (1998). Exposing pluralistic ignorance Social Psychology, 93, 814–830.
to reduce alcohol use among college students. Journal of Applied Stone, J., Aronson, E., Crain, A. L., Winslow, M. P., & Fried, C. B.
Social Psychology, 28, 2150–2180. (1994). Inducing hypocrisy as a means of encouraging young adults
Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. to use condoms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20,
(2007). The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of 116–128.
social norms. Psychological Science, 18, 429–434. Storms, M. D. (1973). Videotape and the attribution process: Reversing
Schwartz, B. (1994). The costs of living: How market freedom erodes the actors’ and observers’ points of view. Journal of Personality and Social
best things in life. New York: W. W. Norton. Psychology, 27, 165–175.
Schwartz, B. (1997). Psychology, idea technology, and ideology. Stouffer, S. A., Suchman, E. A., DeVinney, L. C., Star, S. A., & Williams,
Psychological Science, 8, 21–27. R. M., Jr. (1949). The American soldier: Vol. 1. Adjustment during
Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., & army life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Lehman, D. R. (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a Strahan, E. J., White, K., Fong, G. T., Fabrigar, L. R, Zanna, M. P., &
matter of choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, Cameron, A. J. R. (2002). Enhancing the effectiveness of message
1178–1197. labels on tobacco packaging: A social psychological perspective.
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhance- Tobacco Control, 11, 183–190.
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 60–79. Surowiecki, J. (2004). The wisdom of crowds. New York: Doubleday.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: Tajfel, H. (1974). Social identity and intergroup behaviour. Social Science
An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5–14. Information, 13, 65–93.
Sherif, M. (1966). In common predicament: Social psychology of inter- Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cogni-
group conflict and cooperation. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. tive adaptation. American Psychologist, 38, 1161–1173.
50 History of Social Psychology: Insights, Challenges, and Contributions to Theory and Application

Taylor, S. E. (1989). Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and the Ward, A., & Brenner, L. (2006). Accentuate the negative: The positive effects
healthy mind. New York: Basic Books. of negative acknowledgment. Psychological Science, 17, 959–962.
Taylor, S. E. (2008). Health psychology (7th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological
Taylor, S. E., & Armor, D. A. (1996). Positive illusions and coping with Review, 101, 34–52.
adversity. Journal of Personality, 64, 873–898. Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987).
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psy- Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and
chological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.
193–210. Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribution theory.
Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1975). Point of view and perceptions of cau- Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
sality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 439–445. Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, F. M., & Blackburn, T. C. (1984). Standing out
Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N. L., & Ruderman, A. J. (1978). and standing in: The psychology of control in America and Japan.
Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. American Psychologist, 39, 955–969.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 778–793. Wells, G. L., Memon, A., & Penrod, S. D. (2006). Eyewitness Evidence:
Taylor, S. E., & Koivumaki, J. H. (1976). The perception of self and others: Improving its Probative Value. Psychological Science in the Public
Acquaintanceship, affect, and actor-observer differences. Journal of Interest, 7, 45–75.
Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 403–408. Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review
Tetlock, P. E. (2002). Social functionalist frameworks for judgment and of Psychology, 54, 277–295.
choice: Intuitive politicians, theologians, and prosecutors. Psychological Westling, E. H., Mann, T., & Ward, A. (2006). The self-control of smoking: When
Review, 109, 451–471. does narrowed attention help? Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. 36, 2115–2133.
New York: Wiley. Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and personality.
Triandis, H. C. (1968). Some cross-cultural studies of cognitive consis- New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
tency. In R. P. Abelson, E. Aronson, W. J. McGuire, T. M. Newcomb,
Whyte, W. H. (1956). The organization man. New York: Doubleday.
M. J. Rosenberg, & P. H. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Theories of cognitive
consistency: A sourcebook (pp. 723–730). Chicago, IL: Rand Whyte, W. H. (1980). The social life of small urban spaces. New York:
McNally. Project for Public Spaces.
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural Wilson, J. Q., & Kelling, G. L. (1982). Broken windows. Atlantic Monthly,
contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–520. 249, 29–38.
Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competi- Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. P. Zanna
tion. American Journal of Psychology, 9, 507–533. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 345–
411). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging
frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232. Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining away: A model of affec-
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: tive adaptation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 370–386.
Heuristics and biases. Science 185, 1124–1131. Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual atti-
Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). If tudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.
you can’t join them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on aggres- Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal media-
sive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, tion of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of
1058–1069. Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 109–120.
Tyler, T. R., Rasinski, K. A., & Griffin, E. (1986). Alternative images of Wright, D. B., & Loftus, E. F. (2008). Eyewitness memory. In G. Cohen &
the citizen: Implications for public policy. American Psychologist, 41, M. A. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the real world (3rd ed., pp. 91–105).
970–978. New York: Psychology Press.
Vaes, J., Paladino, M. P., Castelli, L., Leyens, J., & Giovanazzi, A. (2003). Wyer, R. S., & Carlson, D. (1979). Social cognition, inference, and attri-
On the behavioral consequences of infrahumanization: The implicit bution. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
role of uniquely human emotions in intergroup relations. Journal of Wyer, R. S., & Srull. T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1016–1034. context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vallone, R. P., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1985). The hostile media phe- Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274.
nomenon: Biased perception and perceptions of media bias in coverage
of the Beirut massacre. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Zajonc, R. B. (1969). Animal social psychology. New York: Wiley.
49, 577–585. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences.
Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological con- American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.
sequences of money. Science, 314, 1154–1156. Zimbardo, P. G. (1969). The cognitive control of motivation. Glenview, IL:
Wallach, M., Kogan, N., & Bem. D. (1962). Group influence on indi- Scott, Foresman.
vidual risk taking. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, Zimbardo, P. G. (1970). The human choice: Individuation, reason, and
75–86. order vs. deindividuation, impulse and chaos. In W. J. Arnold &
Walster, E., Aronson, V., Abrahams, D., & Rottman, L. (1966). Importance D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 17, pp.
of physical attractiveness in dating behavior. Journal of Personality 237–307). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
and Social Psychology, 4, 508–516. Zimbardo, P. G. (1999). Discontinuity theory: Cognitive and social
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2007). A question of belonging: Race, searches for rationality and normality may lead to madness. In
social fit, and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
Psychology, 92, 82–96. (Vol. 31, pp. 345–486). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ward, A. (2000). Why the bias to study biases? Psycoloquy, 11, Tue Nov Zimbardo, P. G. (2007). The Lucifer effect: Understanding how good
14, 2000. people turn evil. New York: Random House.
Chapter 2

The Art of Laboratory Experimentation


TIMOTHY D. WILSON, ELLIOT ARONSON, AND KEVIN CARLSMITH

If you are reading this chapter you may well be a graduate about social phenomena, as described in another chapter
student in social psychology, perhaps at the beginning of in this volume (Reis & Gosling). In this chapter we hope
your graduate career. If so, congratulations and welcome to convey something about the approach that has been the
to the field! We assume that your graduate school advi- workhorse of social psychological research, the laboratory
sors have already taught you the secret Social Psychology experiment.
Handshake that will identify you as a member of our spe- We have two main goals in this chapter. First, we
cial guild. What, they forgot? No worries—in this chapter will discuss why the laboratory experiment is often the
we will teach you the handshake, metaphorically speaking. method of choice. What are its advantages and disadvan-
It is the methods we use that define our guild, and once you tages? Why is it used so frequently when it has some clear
learn about these methods and begin to use them yourself, drawbacks? This is a timely question, because it is our
you won’t need a special handshake. impression that the use of the lab experiment has become
To be a successful social psychologist you need good less frequent in many areas of psychology, including social
ideas, of course—astute predictions about how people will psychology. One reason for this is that social psychologists
behave and brilliant explanations about why they behave that have ventured into areas in which it is more difficult to do
way. But in some ways that’s the easy part. Transforming experiments, such as the study of culture, close relation-
your ideas into hypotheses that can be tested with an ele- ships, and the areas of the brain that correspond to social
gant, tightly controlled experiment is the real challenge. As cognition and behavior (social neuroscience). Another
with any challenge, it can be both frustrating and great fun. reason is that sophisticated statistical techniques (e.g.,
As Leon Festinger once said, it is like solving a difficult structural equation modeling) are now available, allow-
puzzle: ing more precise tests of the relationships between vari-
ables in correlational designs. Although we welcome
I love games. I think I could be very happy being a chess player these advances, we fear that the unique power and value
or dealing with some other kinds of games. But I grew up of the experimental method sometimes gets lost in the
in the Depression. It didn’t seem one could survive on chess,
enthusiasm generated by new topics and techniques.
and science is also a game. You have very strict ground rules
In the first part of the chapter we will discuss the advantages
in science, and your ideas have to check out with the empirical
world. That’s very tou—gh and also very fascinating. (quoted
of experiments in general terms.
in D. Cohen, 1977, p. 133) The second part of the chapter is more of a “how-to”
manual describing, in some detail, how to conduct an
In this chapter we hope to convey some of this fascina- experiment. It is our hope that, during the first part of the
tion with a particular kind of scientific game, namely the chapter, we will have convinced the reader of the continued
laboratory experiment. This is not the only method avail- value of experiments; then, in the second part, we hope to
able to social psychologists, of course. We can and do use provide detailed instructions about “how to do it” for those
correlational designs and conduct our research in other set- new to this method. We hasten to add that the best way to
tings (e.g., in the field). These approaches have provided learn to do an experiment is to do so under the guidance
us with some of our richest and most fascinating data of an expert, experienced researcher. Experimentation is

Address correspondence to Dr. Timothy D. Wilson, Department of Psychology, 102 Gilmer Hall, University of Virginia, Charlottesville,
Virginia 22904-4400. Phone: 434-924-0674, fax: 434-982-4766, e-mail: twilson@virginia.edu.

51

CH02.indd 49 9/2/09 8:09:50 PM


52 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

very much a trade, like plumbing or carpentry or direct- your friend asks. Ah, you say, this was the point of the
ing a play; the best way to learn to do it is by apprenticing word completion task. It just so happened that five of
oneself to a master. Nonetheless, just as it helps to read the word fragments on the cards people saw could be
manuals explaining how to fix a leaky faucet or stage a completed to form words that were consistent with
production of Hamlet, our “how to do an experiment” American college students’ stereotypes about Asians. For
manual might prove to be a helpful adjunct to a hands-on example, the fragment S_Y could be completed to make
apprenticeship. the word “SHY,” and the fragment “POLI_E” could be
completed to form the word “POLITE.” The measure
of stereotype activation was the number of times people
WHY DO LABORATORY EXPERIMENTS? completed the fragments with the words that reflected
the Asian stereotype.
You have probably had something like the following expe- The results were as predicted, you proudly tell your
rience (nearly every social psychologist has): You are at a friend: People who were not busy and saw the Asian woman
party and someone asks you what you do. The first thing you generated the most stereotypic words. People who were
explain is that you are not THAT kind of psychologist; cognitively busy did not generate any more stereotypical
you aren’t Dr. Phil and you are not analyzing everyone in words for the Asian as opposed to the Caucasian woman.
the room. OK, your questioner gets that, and understands Even better, you (actually, Gilbert & Hixon) did a second
that you are a research psychologist who focuses on, say, study that distinguished between the activation and the
stereotyping and prejudice. “But how do you study that?” application of a stereotype, and found that the people’s rat-
your friend asks. Now comes the hard part——explaining ings of the Asian woman’s personality were most stereo-
why you do the kind of studies you do. typic when they were not busy while viewing the videotape
Suppose, for example, that you had just published a (allowing their stereotypes to be activated) but cognitively
well-known study of stereotype activation by Gilbert and busy while listening to the assistant describe her typical
Hixon (1991). “Here’s an example of one of my studies,” day (allowing the stereotype to be applied to the woman
you tell your friend. The participants were white college with no inhibition).
students who watched a videotape of a woman holding up Assuming that your friend has not left to go talk with
a series of cards with word fragments on them, such as the literature professor across the room, she is likely to
P_ST. The participants’ job was to make as many words have several questions. “People are being discriminated
from these fragments as they could within 15 seconds, against every day on the basis of their race or gender or sexual
such as POST or PAST. Now, unbeknownst to the partici- preference and wars are being fought over ethnic identity,”
pants, there were two versions of the videotape. In one the she says. “On the other hand, an African American has
woman holding up the cards was Caucasian, whereas in been elected president for the first time in the history of
the other she was Asian. This was one of the independent the United States. With such rich and important material
variables, you explain to your friend, looking around for to study in everyday life, why on earth are you doing a
some chalk and a blackboard. “That’s the variable that the lab study in which college students watch videotapes and
researcher varies to see if it has an effect on some other complete word fragments?”
variable of interest (the dependent variable).” Your friend Good question. Even to seasoned social psychologists,
nods, so you continue. The other independent variable, you lab studies sometimes seem far removed from the prob-
explain, was how “cognitively busy” or distracted people lems that inspired them. Most social psychologists would
were while watching the videotape. People in the “busy” agree that the perfect study would be one that was conducted
condition were asked to remember an eight-digit number, in a naturalistic setting, with a diverse sample of participants,
which made it difficult for them to think carefully about what that revealed the nature and causes of an important social
they were doing. People in the “nonbusy” condition were not psychological phenomenon (such as stereotyping and prej-
asked to remember any numbers. udice). Unfortunately, such a study is like a Platonic ideal
The hypothesis was that people who had to remember the that can rarely be achieved. Experimentation almost always
eight-digit number would not have the cognitive resources involves a trade-off between competing goals: the desire to
to activate their stereotype of Asians, and thus would study a real problem in its natural context, on the one hand,
judge the Asian woman no differently than the Caucasian and the desire to have enough control over the setting to be
women. Not busy participants, however, would have the able to learn something about that problem on the other.
resources to call to mind their stereotype, and thus would There are several important methodological points to be
judge the Asian woman differently than the Caucasian made here, beginning with the distinction between correla-
woman. “But how did you measure stereotype activation?” tional and experimental studies.

CH02.indd 50 9/2/09 8:09:51 PM


Why Do Laboratory Experiments? 53

Correlational Versus Experimental Studies building might differ in numerous ways from students
who took the classes in the quiet building. Maybe some
One of the points of the Gilbert and Hixon (1991) study departments offer classes in one building but not the other,
was to examine whether the amount of cognitive resources and maybe students interested in some subjects have more
people have influences the activation of their stereotypes. stereotypic views of Asians than other students do. If so,
Like most social psychological questions this is a causal the differences in ratings of the Asian instructors might
hypothesis, namely that one psychological variable (cogni- reflect these differences in endorsement of the stereotype
tive busyness) will have an interesting effect on another and have nothing to do with cognitive busyness. Further,
(stereotype activation). In order to test causal hypotheses, there is no way of knowing whether the instructors who
the researcher needs to have enough control over the situ- teach in the different buildings have similar personalities.
ation to manipulate the independent variable (in this case, Perhaps the Asian instructors teaching in the noisy building
cognitive busyness) while keeping everything else con- really were more shy than the Asian instructors teaching in
stant. Although that is sometimes possible to do in field the quiet building. In short, there is simply no way of tell-
studies, it is much easier to accomplish in the laboratory. ing whether students’ ratings of the Asian instructors in
To illustrate this point, think about ways in which we the different buildings were due to (a) differences in their
could test Gilbert and Hixon’s hypotheses about stereotype level of cognitive busyness; (b) the fact that different stu-
activation in a more realistic setting. It wouldn’t be easy, but dents took classes in the different buildings, and these
maybe we could pull off a study such as the following: At a students differed in their endorsement of the Asian ste-
large state university, we attend the first day of classes that reotype; or (c) the fact that different instructors taught in
are taught by graduate student teaching assistants—some the different buildings, and these instructors had different
of whom happen to be Caucasian and some of whom hap- personalities.
pen to be Asian. We take advantage of the fact that some One of the great advantages of an experiment is the
of the classes are held in a building that is being renovated, ability to ensure that the stimuli in experimental conditions
such that the high-pitched whine of power saws and the are similar. The fact that Gilbert and Hixon showed all
explosions of nail guns intrude into the classrooms. We can participants the same videotape of an Asian or Caucasian
assume that students in these rooms are cognitively busy, woman solved one of the problems with our hypothetical
because the noise makes it difficult to pay close attention correlational study: personality differences between the
to the instructor. Other classes are held in buildings in instructors of the courses. The fact that people who were
which there is no construction noise, and these students nonbusy showed more evidence of stereotyping than peo-
are assumed to be relatively “nonbusy.” At the end of each ple who were busy cannot be attributed to differences in
class we ask the students to rate their instructor on various the personality of the Asian women they saw on the video-
trait dimensions, including some that are part of the Asian tape, because participants in both conditions saw the same
stereotype (e.g., shyness). Suppose that the results of this woman.
study were the same as Gilbert and Hixon’s: Students in the But how do we know that this difference was not due
“nonbusy” (quiet) classrooms rate Asian instructors more to the fact that the students in the nonbusy condition hap-
stereotypically than students in the “busy” (noisy) class- pened to be more prejudiced toward Asians? Gilbert and
rooms (e.g., they think the instructors are more shy). There Hixon (1991) solved this problem with the most impor-
is no difference between busy and nonbusy students in their tant advantage of experimental designs: The ability to randomly
ratings of Caucasian instructors. assign people to conditions. Unlike the correlational study,
To many readers, this study probably seems to have people did not “self-select” themselves into the busy or
some definite advantages over the one conducted by nonbusy condition (i.e., by deciding which courses to take).
Gilbert and Hixon (1991). The measure of stereotyping— Everyone had an equal chance of being in either condition,
students’ ratings of their TA—seems a lot more realistic which means that people who were especially prejudiced
and important than the word fragments people complete against Asians were as likely to end up in one condition
after watching a videotape in a psychology experiment. as the other. Random assignment is the great equalizer: As
But, whereas the study would be interesting and possibly long as the sample size is sufficiently large, researchers can
worth doing, it would have definite limitations. It would be relatively certain that differences in the personalities or
demonstrate a correlation between cognitive busyness and backgrounds of their participants are distributed evenly
stereotypic ratings of Asians (at least as these variables across conditions. Any differences that are observed, then,
were measured in this study), but there would be no evi- are likely to be due to the independent variable encoun-
dence of a causal relationship between these variables. tered in the experiment, such as their different levels of
For example, students who took the classes in the noisy cognitive busyness.

CH02.indd 51 9/2/09 8:09:51 PM


54 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

Our discussion of the limits of correlational designs—and dent can we be from the correlational study? Kids who eat
the advantage of experiments—is no different from that in breakfast probably differ in countless ways from kids who
any introductory course in statistics or research methodol- do not. They may come from more functional families, get
ogy. As straightforward and obvious as these points may more sleep—or, for that matter, have parents or teachers
seem, however, they are often overlooked, by both lay who are more likely to help them with their homework!
people and professional researchers. To understand why, The experimental study, despite its flaw, rules out every
consider the following two (fictitious) investigations of single one of these alternative explanations except for one.
the same problem. In the first, a team of researchers finds Admittedly, this is a serious flaw; the researchers did err by
that school performance in a group of inner-city children confounding breakfast eating with extra attention from the
is related to the frequency with which they eat breakfast teachers. But the fact remains that the correlational study
in the morning. The more often the kids eat breakfast, leaves the door open to the same confound, and dozens or
the better their school performance, with a highly signifi- hundreds of others besides. If the goal is to reduce uncer-
cant correlation of .30 (this means that the relationship tainty about causality, surely the correlational study is
between eating breakfast and school performance is mod- much more flawed than the experimental one. Why, then,
erately strong and highly unlikely to be due to chance). does it seem like more can be learned from the correlational
As far as you can tell the researchers used good measures study? One reason is that the correlational study was done
and the study was well conducted. What do you think of well, by the standards of correlational designs, whereas
this finding? Does it make you more confident that pro- the experimental study was done poorly, by the stan-
grams that provide free breakfasts for underprivileged dards of experimental designs. Our point is that the same
children are having positive effects on their academic per- standard should be applied to both types of studies: How
formance? If you were reviewing a report of this study for much do they reduce uncertainty about causality?
a journal, how likely would you be to recommend publi- The ability to determine relationships between vari-
cation? Most of us, we suspect, would find this to be an ables in correlational designs has improved, we should
interesting and well-conducted study that should be in the add, with the advent of sophisticated statistical techniques
literature. such as structural equation modeling. These methods
Now consider this study: A team of researchers conducts allow researchers to test complex relationships between
an experiment with a group of inner-city children. Half of several variables and are useful techniques for distin-
the kids are randomly assigned to a condition in which they guishing between competing models. We do not have
receive free breakfasts at school every morning, whereas the space to review all of the pros and cons of structural
the other half are in a control group that does not receive equation modeling (for excellent reviews see Kenny, this vol-
this intervention. Unfortunately, the researchers introduced ume and Reis, 1982). Our point is that as useful as this
a confound into their design: While the kids in the first technique is, it cannot, in the absence of experimental
group eat their breakfast, teachers also read to them and manipulations with random assignment, determine causal
help them with their homework. After a few months, the relationships. One reason for this is obvious but sometimes
researchers assess the kids’ school performance, and find overlooked: It is impossible to measure all variables in a
that those in the breakfast condition are doing significantly correlational design, and the researchers might have omitted
better than those in the control condition. The measure of one or more crucial causal variables. Thus, although there
academic performance is the same as in the previous study may be a direct path two variables in a structural model,
and the magnitude of the effect is the same. What do you one can never be sure whether this is because one variable
think of this experiment? How likely would you be to rec- really causes the other or whether there are unmeasured
ommend that it be published? The confound in the design, variables that are the true causes and happen to correlate
we would guess, is likely to be apparent and appalling to highly with the measured variables. The only way to defi-
most of us. Is it eating breakfast that improved the kids’ nitely rule out such alternative explanations is to use exper-
performance or the reading and extra attention from the imental designs, in which people are randomly assigned to
teachers? Many of us would feel that the design of this different experimental conditions.
study is so flawed that it should not be published.
But let’s compare the two studies more carefully. The Validity and Realism in Experiments
key question is how confident we can be that eating break-
fast causes improved academic performance. The flaw in We hope we have convinced the reader of the great advan-
the experiment is that we cannot be sure whether eating tage of the experiment—its ability to answer causal
breakfast or extra attention from a teacher or both were questions. Some, however, might still be a little uncom-
responsible for the improved performance. But how confi- fortable with our conclusions, in that there is one way in

CH02.indd 52 9/2/09 8:09:52 PM


Why Do Laboratory Experiments? 55

which experiments are often inferior to observational and ratory experimentation in social psychology is criticized
correlational studies: They are typically done in the “arti- as being “the study of the psychology of the college soph-
ficial” confines of a psychology laboratory and involve omore,” what is being called into question is the exter-
behaviors (e.g., forming words from word fragments, nal validity of the findings. Because so many laboratory
remembering eight-digit numbers) that seem to have lit- experiments are conducted with college students as partici-
tle to do with the kinds of things people do in everyday pants, the truth of the causal relationships we observe may
life. This is, perhaps, the most common objection to social be limited to that particular population (Sears, 1986). If it
psychological experiments—they seem “artificial” and happens that college students—with their youth, above-
“unrealistic.” How can we generalize from such artificial average intelligence, and nonrepresentative socioeconomic
situations to everyday life? backgrounds—respond differently to our experimental
treatment conditions than other types of people, then the
Types of Validity external (but not internal) validity of our findings would
Campbell and his colleagues (Campbell, 1957; Campbell & be low.
Stanley, 1963; Cook & Campbell, 1979) distinguished The issue is actually a little more subtle. No one would
among different types of validity. In Campbell’s taxonomy, seriously deny that Princeton students might respond dif-
the interpretation of research results may be assessed with ferently to a particular experimental treatment than would
respect to at least three different kinds of validity—internal a sample of 50-year-old working-class immigrants or col-
validity, external validity, and construct validity. lege students in another culture. External validity refers to
the extent to which a particular causal relationship is robust
Internal Validity Basically, internal validity refers to across populations, cultures, and settings. Thus, if we
the confidence with which we can draw cause-and-effect were interested in the effects of lowered self-esteem on
conclusions from our research results. To what extent are aggression, we might have to use different techniques to
we certain that the independent variable, or treatment, lower self-esteem in the two populations. Being informed
manipulated by the experimenter is the sole source or cause that one has failed a quiz about the history of Ivy League
of systematic variation in the dependent variable? Threats football is likely to lower self-esteem more for Princeton
to the internal validity of research results arise when the sophomores than for working-class immigrants. But if we
conditions under which an experiment is conducted pro- can find another technique of lowering self-esteem among
duce systematic sources of variance that are irrelevant to the that second sample, we still must ask whether this lowered
treatment variable and not under control of the researcher. self-esteem will have the same effects on aggression in
The internal validity of a study is questioned, for instance, both samples.
if groups of participants exposed to different experimental External validity is related to settings as well as to
conditions are not assigned randomly and are different from participant populations. How do we know whether the
each other in some important ways other than the inde- results we find in one situation (e.g., a psychology labo-
pendent variable (as in our hypothetical breakfast-eating ratory) will generalize to another situation (e.g., everyday
study). life)? For example, Milgram’s (1974) initial studies of
As we have seen, it is usually much easier to maintain obedience were conducted in a research laboratory at Yale
high internal validity in a laboratory experiment, because the University, and a legitimate question is the extent to which
researcher has more control over extraneous variables that his findings would generalize to other settings. Because partici-
might compromise the design. Even when internal validity pants were drawn from outside the university and because
is high, however, there may be questions about the validity of many had no previous experience with college, the pres-
interpretations of causal effects obtained in any given tige and respect associated with a research laboratory at
study. It is here that the distinction between external valid- Yale may have made the participants more susceptible to
ity and construct validity becomes relevant. the demands for compliance that the experiment entailed
than they would have been in other settings. To address this
External Validity This term refers to the robustness issue Milgram undertook a replication of his experiment
of a phenomenon: the extent to which a causal relation- in a very different physical setting. Moving the research
ship, once identified in a particular setting with particular operation to a “seedy” office in the industrial town of
research participants, can safely be generalized to other Bridgeport, Connecticut, adopting a fictitious identity as
times, places, and people. Threats to external validity arise a psychological research firm, Milgram hoped to mini-
from potential interaction effects between the treatment mize the reputational factors inherent in the Yale setting.
variable of interest and the context in which it is delivered In comparison with data obtained in the original study, the
or the type of participant population involved. When labo- Bridgeport replication resulted in slightly lower but still

CH02.indd 53 9/2/09 8:09:53 PM


56 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

dramatic rates of compliance to the experimenter. Thus, be able to assess whether the results of that study are
setting could be identified as a contributing but not crucial internally valid.
factor to the basic findings of the research. Issues involving construct validity and external validity,
on the other hand, are more complicated. Researchers do
Construct Validity To question the external validity of the best they can in devising independent and dependent
a particular finding is not to deny that a cause and effect variables that capture the conceptual variables perfectly.
relationship has been demonstrated in the given research But how can external validity be maximized? How can
study, but rather to express doubt that the same effect could researchers increase the likelihood that the results of the
be demonstrated under different circumstances or with study are generalizable across people and settings? One
different participants. Similarly, concerns with construct way is to make the setting as realistic as possible, which is,
validity do not challenge the fact of an empirical relation- after all, one point of field research: to increase the extent
ship between an experimentally manipulated variable and to which the findings can be applied to everyday life, by
the dependent measure, but rather question how that fact conducting the study in real-life settings. The issue of real-
is to be interpreted in conceptual terms. Construct valid- ism however, is not this straightforward. There are several
ity refers to the correct identification of the nature of the different types of realism with different implications.
independent and dependent variables and the underlying
Mundane Realism Versus Experimental Realism
relationship between them. To what extent do the operations
Versus Psychological Realism
and measures embodied in the experimental procedures of
a particular study reflect the theoretical concepts that gave Aronson and Carlsmith (1968) distinguished between two
rise to the research in the first place? Threats to construct ways in which an experiment can be said to be realistic.
validity derive from errors of measurement, misspecifica- In one sense, an experiment is realistic if the situation is
tion of research operations, and, in general, the complexity involving to the participants, if they are forced to take it
of experimental treatments and measures. One of the most seriously, if it has impact on them. This kind of realism
difficult parts of experimental design is constructing a con- they called experimental realism. In another sense, the term
crete independent variable (e.g., memorizing an eight-digit “realism” can refer to the extent to which events occur-
number) that is a good instantiation of the conceptual vari- ring in the research setting are likely to occur in the normal
able (cognitive busyness). This is essentially an issue of course of the participants’ lives, that is, in the “real world.”
construct validity: How well does the independent variable They called this type of realism mundane realism. The fact
capture the conceptual variable? that an event is similar to events that occur in the real world
The same issue holds for the dependent variable. When does not endow it with importance. Many events that occur
we devise an elaborate rationale for inducing our partici- in the real world are boring and unimportant in the lives of
pants to express their attitudes toward the experiment or the actors or observers. Thus, it is possible to put a partici-
toward some social object in the form of ratings on a pant to sleep if an experimental event is high on mundane
structured questionnaire, how can we be sure that these realism but remains low on experimental realism.
responses reflect the effect variable of conceptual interest Mundane realism and experimental realism are not
rather than (or in addition to) the myriad of other complex polar concepts; a particular technique may be high on both
decision rules our participants may bring to bear in mak- mundane realism and experimental realism, low on both, or
ing such ratings? And how do we know that the functional high on one and low on the other. Perhaps the difference
relationships observed between treatment and effect, under between experimental and mundane realism can be clari-
a particular set of operations, represent the conceptual fied by citing a couple of examples. Let us first consider
processes of interest? Asch’s (1951) experiment on perceptual judgment. Here
We can now see that the experimenter is faced with a the participants were asked to judge the length of lines
daunting task: designing a study that is well-controlled and then were confronted with unanimous judgments
(high in internal validity), includes independent and depen- by a group of peers that contradicted their own percep-
dent variables that are good reflections of the conceptual tions. For most participants this experiment seems to have
variables of interest (high in construct validity), and is contained a good deal of experimental realism. Whether
generalizable to other settings and people (high in external participants yielded to group pressure or stood firm, the
validity). Internal validity may be considered a property of vast majority underwent a rather difficult experience that
a single experimental study. With sufficient knowledge caused them to squirm, sweat, and exhibit other signs of
of the conditions under which an experiment has been tension and discomfort. They were involved, upset, and
conducted, of the procedures associated with assignment deeply concerned about the evidence being presented to
of participants, and of experimenter behavior, we should them. We may assume that they were reacting to a situation

CH02.indd 54 9/2/09 8:09:53 PM


Why Do Laboratory Experiments? 57

that was as “real” for them as any of their ordinary expe- There is some overlap between experimental and psy-
riences. However, the experiment was hardly realistic in chological realism, in that many of the psychological
the mundane sense. Recall that the participants were judg- processes of interest to psychologists are ones that occur
ing a very clear physical event. In everyday life it is rare when people are reacting to impactful events in their envi-
to find oneself in a situation where the direct and unam- ronments. Thus, the situations in everyday life in which
biguous evidence of one’s senses is contradicted by the cognitive dissonance, prejudice, or aggression occur are
unanimous judgments of one’s peers. Although the judging usually ones in which people are quite engaged. Thus,
of lines is perhaps not important or realistic in the mundane when studying these phenomena, it is imperative to devise
sense, one cannot deny the impact of having one’s sensory experimental settings that are equally impactful. Such
input contradicted by a unanimous majority. studies would be high in both experimental and psycho-
On the other hand, consider an experiment by Walster, logical realism (although not necessarily high in mundane
Aronson, and Abrahams (1966) that, although high on realism). Increasingly, however, social psychologists have
mundane realism, was low indeed on experimental real- become interested in psychological processes that occur
ism. In this experiment, participants read a newspaper article when people are not actively engaged or motivated to pro-
about the prosecution of criminal suspects in Portugal. In cess information carefully. Examples include the study of
the article, various statements were attributed to a pros- automatic processing (as in the Gilbert & Hixon study),
ecuting attorney or to a convicted criminal. The article peripheral or heuristic processing of persuasive messages
was embedded in a real newspaper and hence, the partici- (Chaiken, 1987, Petty & Cacioppo, 1986), or “mindless-
pants were doing something they frequently do—reading ness” (Langer, 1989). To study these phenomena it is
facts in a newspaper. Thus the experiment had a great deal important to devise experimental settings that are high in
of mundane realism. However, nothing was happening to psychological realism but low in experimental realism.
the participant. Very few U.S. college students are seriously
External Validity: Is it Always a Goal?
affected by reading a rather pallid article about a remote
situation in a foreign country. The procedure did not have a Before leaving this topic it is important to make one more
high degree of experimental realism. point about external validity and generalizability. It is often
Aronson, Wilson, and Akert (1994) introduced a third assumed that all studies should be as high as possible in
type of realism that they termed psychological realism. This external validity, in the sense that we should be able to
is the extent to which the psychological processes that occur generalize the results as much as possible across popula-
in an experiment are the same as psychological processes that tions and settings and time. Sometimes, however, the goal
occur in everyday life. It may be that an experiment is noth- of the researcher is different. Mook (1983) published a
ing like what people encounter in everyday life (low in mundane provocative article entitled, “In defense of external invalid-
realism) and fails to have much of an impact on people (low ity,” in which he argued that the goal of many experiments
in experimental realism). It could still be high in psycho- is to test a theory, not to establish external validity. Theory-
logical realism, however, if the psychological processes that testing can take a variety of forms, some of which have
occur are similar those that occur in everyday life. Consider little to do with how much the results can be generalized.
the Gilbert and Hixon (1991) study we described earlier. This For example, a researcher might construct a situation in
study was low in mundane realism; in everyday life people which a specific set of results should occur if one theory is
rarely, if ever, watch a videotape of a woman holding up cards correct, but not if another is correct. This situation may be
with word fragments on them and think of words to complete completely unlike any that people encounter in everyday
the fragments. It was also relatively low in experimental real- life, and yet, the study can provide an interesting test of the
ism, in that the study was not very impactful or engaging. two theories.
Watching the woman was probably of mild interest, but surely Mook (1983) gives the example of Harlow’s clas-
the study was less impactful than the Milgram or Asch stud- sic study of emotional development in rhesus monkeys
ies. The study was high in psychological realism, however, (Harlow & Zimmerman, 1958). Infant monkeys were
to the extent that the psychological processes of stereotype separated from their mothers and placed in cages with
activation and application were the same as those that occur wire-covered contraptions that resembled adult monkeys.
in everyday life. It is common to encounter a member of a Some of the wire monkeys were covered with terry cloth
group and for a stereotype of that group to come to mind auto- and were warmed with a light bulb, whereas others were
matically. To the extent that this psychological process is bare and uninviting. Nourishment (in the form of a baby
the same as what occurred in Gilbert and Hixon’s (1991) bottle) was sometimes available from one type of mon-
study, they succeeded in devising a situation that was high key and sometimes from the other. Harlow found that the
in psychological realism. monkeys clung to the terry cloth “mother,” regardless of

CH02.indd 55 9/2/09 8:09:54 PM


58 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

whether it contained the bottle of milk. These results were at prison camps) followed the orders of authority figures to
damaging to drive-reduction theories that argued that the the point of committing horrific acts. Thus, if we were to
monkeys should prefer nourishment over emotional com- conclude that the psychological processes Milgram uncov-
fort. Was this study high in external validity? Clearly not. ered never occur in everyday life, we could justifiably dis-
There was no attempt to randomly select the monkeys miss his findings. The fact that these processes appear to
from those reared in the wild, or to simulate conditions be similar to those that occurred at some of humankind’s
that monkeys encounter in real-life settings. Nonetheless, darkest moments—such as the Holocaust—is what makes
if theories of drive reduction that were prevalent at the time his results so compelling.
were correct, the monkeys should have preferred the nour- We are essentially reiterating the importance of psycho-
ishment, regardless of which “monkey” it came from. The logical realism in experimentation. To test a theory it may
researchers succeeded in devising a situation in which a be necessary to construct a situation that is extremely arti-
specific set of actions should have occurred if a particular ficial and low in mundane realism. As long as it triggers
theory was right—even though the situation was not one the same psychological processes as occur outside of the
that would be found in everyday life. The purpose of this laboratory, however, it can be generalized to those real-life
experiment, then, was to disprove an accepted theory rather situations in which the same psychological processes occur.
than to establish external validity. Of course, as discussed earlier, claims about psychological
Mook also points out that some experiments are valuable realism cannot be taken completely on faith; only by repli-
because they answer questions about “what can happen,” cating a study in a variety of settings can external validity
even if they say little about “what does happen” in every- be firmly established.
day life. Consider Milgram’s experiments on obedience to
authority. As we’ve seen, there was little attempt to simu- Problem-Oriented versus Process-Oriented
late any kind of real-life setting in these studies; outside of Research: Studying the Phenomenon Versus
psychology experiments, people are never asked to deliver Studying the Process
electric shocks to a stranger who is performing poorly on
a memory test. The results were very informative, how- One of your mythical friend’s objections to the Gilbert
ever, because it was so surprising that people would act the and Hixon experiment was that it didn’t really study the
way they did under any circumstances. This is sometimes phenomena that inspired it, stereotyping and prejudice.
referred to as “proof of principle.” The fact that people can Seeing how people complete the word fragment S_Y is far
be made to harm a complete stranger, because an authority removed from ethnic conflict or housing discrimination.
figure tells them to, is fascinating (and frightening) despite We have already addressed this criticism to some extent:
the artificiality of the setting. Although it is true that people do not complete word frag-
Mook’s (1983) position is persuasive, and we heartily ments in everyday life (the study was low in mundane realism),
agree that the goal of many experiments is to test a theory, the study may well have captured the processes by which
rather than to establish external validity. Nonetheless, we stereotypes are activated in everyday life—that is, the study
believe that even if external validity is not the main goal of was high in psychological realism. But there is another
study, it should never be completely forgotten. The impor- answer to this criticism, which is related to our discussion
tance of a theory, after all, depends on its applicability to of Mook’s (1983) defense of external invalidity—whether
everyday life. The reason Harlow’s study is so important the researcher’s goal is to study a phenomenon that he or
is because the theories it addresses—drive-reduction and she wants to understand and possibly change, such as preju-
emotional attachment—are so relevant to everyday life. The dice, or to study the underlying mechanisms responsible for
theories apply to humans as well as monkeys, and to many the phenomenon. This distinction may seem a little odd, in
situations beyond cages and wire mothers. It is precisely that it probably seems that these goals are interdependent—
because the theories are generalizable (i.e., applicable to and they are. To understand and change a phenomenon, it
many populations and settings) that a test of those theories is necessary to understand the mechanisms that cause it.
is important. Thus, a specific study might test a theory in How can we reduce prejudice, example, without under-
an artificial setting that is low in external validity, but why standing the psychological mechanisms that lead to stereo-
would we conduct such a study if we didn’t believe that type activation? In practice, however, there is a distinction
the theory was generalizable? Similarly, Milgram’s results to be made between research that focuses on the problem
are so compelling because we can generate important, real- itself and research that focuses on mechanisms.
life examples of times when similar processes occurred. Part of this distinction involves still another one: the
Indeed, the inspiration for Milgram’s study was the Holocaust, difference between basic and applied research. With basic
in which seemingly normal individuals (e.g., guards research, investigators try to find the best answer to the

CH02.indd 56 9/2/09 8:09:54 PM


Why Do Laboratory Experiments? 59

question of why people behave the way they do, purely for Like our earlier distinctions, we hasten to add that this
reasons of intellectual curiosity. No direct attempt is made one is not entirely clear-cut. Sometimes research is both
to solve a specific social or psychological problem. In problem- and process-oriented; it explores a problem and
contrast, the goal in applied research is to solve a specific the mechanisms responsible for it simultaneously. Often,
problem. Rather than investigating questions for their own however, the focus of research on a particular problem
sake, constructing theories about why people do what they changes as research on it progresses. As noted by Zanna
do, the aim is to find ways of alleviating such problems and Fazio (1982), initial investigations of a problem tend
as racism, sexual violence, and the spread of AIDS. Thus, to explore “is” questions: What is the phenomenon? Does
the basic researcher is more likely to be interested in the it exist? These studies are, in our terms, very much prob-
mechanisms underlying an interesting phenomenon than lem-oriented; they establish the existence of a particular
the applied researcher. If applied researchers find some- phenomenon (e.g., whether there is a stereotype based on
thing that works they might not be as concerned with why. physical attractiveness). When this question is answered,
In medicine, for example, there are many examples of cures researchers typically move on to questions that have more
that work for unknown reasons, such as the effects of aspi- to do with the underlying mechanisms, namely studies
rin on body temperature. exploring variables that moderate or mediate the effect.
The distinction between problem-oriented and pro- Interestingly, these process-oriented studies sometimes do
cess-oriented research, however, involves more than the not study the original problem at all, focusing instead on
distinction between applied and basic research. To illustrate general mechanisms that produce many different effects
this, consider two basic researchers who are equally interested (as in our example of basic research on categorization
in understanding the causes of prejudice and racism. (As and impression formation that do not study interactions
with many social psychological topics this is, of course, between people of different races, but which are quite rel-
an eminently applied one as well, in that the research- evant to stereotyping and prejudice).
ers are interested in finding ways of reducing prejudice.)
The Basic Dilemma of the Social Psychologist
One researcher conducts a field study in which members
of different races interact under different conditions (e.g., It should be clear by now that the perfect social psychol-
cooperative vs. competitive settings), to study the condi- ogy study would be experimental instead of correlational,
tions under which reductions in prejudicial behavior occur. be extremely high in psychological realism, and study the
The other conducts a laboratory experiment on automatic psychological processes underlying an important phenom-
processing and categorization, or the way in which people enon. Ideally, the study would be conducted in a natural-
categorize the physical and social world immediately, spon- istic setting in which participants were randomly assigned
taneously, and involuntarily. The stimulus materials, how- to experimental conditions and all extraneous variables
ever, have nothing to do with race per se; in fact, the issue were controlled. Unfortunately, it is next to impossible
of race never comes up in this experiment. Participants to design an experiment that meets all of these demands.
judge a white stimulus person, under conditions thought Indeed, almost no study ever has. One of the few excep-
to trigger automatic evaluations and conditions thought to tions, perhaps, is the Lepper, Greene, and Nisbett (1973)
trigger more controlled, thoughtful evaluations (e.g. Bargh, classic study of the overjustification effect, which was
1989; Brewer, 1988; Uleman, 1989). conducted in a naturalistic setting (a preschool) in which
Which study is a better investigation of prejudice and participants (3- and 4-year old children) were randomly
racism? Most people, we suspect, would say the former assigned to various conditions of rewards or no rewards
study. What does the second study have to do with preju- for drawing with felt-tip pens, and the dependent vari-
dice? How can you possibly study racism, one might argue, able was how much the kids played with the pens 2 weeks
without looking at behavior and attitudes of one race toward later during a normal classroom activity. (An interesting
another? Herein lies our point: For researchers interested in social psychological parlor game is to see if you can come
process and mechanisms, the study of a phenomenon (such up with any other studies that meet all of the conditions
as prejudice) can involve the study of basic, psychological we have laid out for the Platonic Social Psychological
processes that are several steps removed from the phenom- Experiment—there are not many.) Aronson and Carlsmith
enon itself. In our view both types of studies are important: (1968) called this the basic dilemma of the experi-
Those that study the phenomenon (e.g., racism) itself and mental social psychologist. On the one hand, we want
work backward to try to discover its causes, and those that maximal control over the independent variable, to main-
study the basic mechanisms of human perception, cognition, tain internal validity. But, by maximizing internal validity,
motivation, emotion, and behavior, and then work forward to we often reduce external validity (e.g., by conducting our
apply these concepts to important problems (e.g., racism). study in the lab instead of the field).

CH02.indd 57 9/2/09 8:09:55 PM


60 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

Programmatic Research speak for themselves. Failures to replicate are ambiguous,


however, and therefore require supplementary information.
A solution to the basic dilemma of the social psycholo-
For these reasons, good programmatic research involves
gist is to not try to “do it all” in one experiment. Instead, a
replication with systematic differences in procedures and
programmatic series of studies can be conducted in which
operations so that differences in results are potentially inter-
different experimental procedures are used, in different set-
pretable. In many cases, including exact replication along
tings, to explore the same conceptual relationship. It is in
with conceptual variations are useful. Suppose, for exam-
this realm of conceptual replication with different scenar-
ple, that Jones, a hypothetical psychologist at the University of
ios that the interplay between lab and field experimentation
Illinois, produces a specific experimental result using Illinois
is most clear. However, in considering these interrelation-
undergraduates as participants. In addition, suppose that
ships, the tradeoff mentioned earlier between control and
Smith, at Yale University, feels that these results were not a
impact in different experimental settings becomes espe-
function of the conceptual variable proposed by Jones but
cially salient. In order to be defensible, weaknesses in one
rather were a function of some artifact in the procedure.
aspect of experimental design must be offset by strengths
Smith then repeats Jones’s procedure in all respects save
or advantages in other features, or the whole research effort
one: She changes the operations in order to eliminate this
is called into question. This dictum is particularly applicable
artifact. She fails to replicate and concludes that this dem-
to field experiments in which inevitable increases in cost and
onstrates that Jones’s results were artifactual. This is only
effort are frequently accompanied by decreases in preci-
one of many possible conclusions. Smith’s failure to rep-
sion and control that can be justified only if there are
licate has several possible causes and is therefore uninter-
corresponding gains in construct validity, impact, or the
pretable. It may be a function of a change in experimenter,
generalizability of findings.
a different participant population (Yale students may be
Multiple Instantiations of the Independent Variable different on many dimensions from Illinois students), or
Essentially, there are two properties that we demand of countless minor variations in the procedure such as tone
a series of experiments before we are convinced that we of voice. Most of this ambiguity could be eliminated by a
understand what the conceptual interpretation should be. balanced design that includes an “exact” replication of the
First, we ask for a number of empirical techniques that dif- conditions run by the original experimenter. That is, sup-
fer in as many ways as possible, having in common only pose Smith’s design had included a repeat of Jones’s con-
our basic conceptual variable. If all these techniques yield ditions with the suspected artifact left in, and her results
the same result, then we become more and more convinced approximated those of Jones’s experiment. If, as part of
that the underlying variable that all techniques have in com- the design, Smith changed the experiment slightly and
mon is, in fact, the variable that is producing the results. For produced no differences, or differences in the opposite
example, the construct of cognitive dissonance (Festinger, direction, one could then be sure that this result was not
1957) has been operationalized in a wide variety of ways in merely a function of incidental differences like the experi-
both laboratory and field studies, including having people menter or the participant population but must be a function
read lists of obscene words, write counter-attitudinal essays, of the change in the procedure. If she failed even to repli-
eat unpleasant foods, and make a difficult choice between cate Jones’s basic experiment, the results would be much
which horse to bet on at a racetrack. harder to interpret, because the different results could be
due to any number of factors (different experimenters, dif-
Multiple Instantiations of the Dependent Variable
ferent population of participants, etc.).
Second, we must show that a particular empirical realization
of our independent variable produces a large number of dif-
Non-Systematic Replications In many situations it is
ferent outcomes, all theoretically tied to the independent vari-
difficult to selectively and systematically modify the par-
able. Again, we point to research on cognitive dissonance, in
ticular operational definition of the independent variable
which a wide array of dependent variables has been used. For
without changing the entire experimental setting. This is most
example, asking people to engage in unpleasant activities,
dramatically true when conceptual replication involves a shift
under conditions of high perceived choice, has been found to
from laboratory setting to field setting. The potential com-
influence their attitudes, their galvanic skin response while
plementary aspects of different research paradigms are best
receiving electric shocks, and how hungry they are.
exemplified when operations of independent and dependent
Systematic Replications When it comes to interpretation, variables in laboratory procedures are significantly modi-
there is a fundamental asymmetry between positive and fied to take advantage of field settings so as to embed them
negative results of replications. If proper techniques have appropriately in this altered context. Such modifications
been employed to preclude bias, successful replications often involve fundamental rethinking about the conceptual

CH02.indd 58 9/2/09 8:09:55 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 61

variables; it is “back to square one,” with attendant costs admitted to a group will find the group more attractive than
in time and effort. If the result is a successful conceptual they would if they were admitted to that group with little
replication, the effort has paid off handsomely in enhanced or no initiation. To test this hypothesis, they conducted
validity for our theoretical constructs. But what if the rep- the following experiment. Sixty-three college women were
lication fails to confirm our original findings? In this case, recruited as volunteers to participate in a series of group
the multitude of procedural differences that could have discussions on the psychology of sex. This format was a
increased our confidence (with a successful replication) ruse in order to provide a setting wherein people could
now contributes to the ambiguity. be made to go through either mild or severe initiations in
Now that we have discussed the value of laboratory order to gain membership in a group.
experiments, we turn to a discussion of how to conduct Each participant was tested individually. When a partici-
one. In discussing the nuts and bolts of experimentation pant arrived at the laboratory, ostensibly to meet with her
we will not lose sight of the important questions about group, the experimenter explained to her that he was inter-
the advantages and disadvantages of experiments and will ested in studying the “dynamics of the group discussion
return to these issues frequently. process” and that, accordingly, he had arranged these discus-
sion groups for the purpose of investigating these dynamics,
which included such phenomena as the flow of communica-
PLANNING AND CONDUCTING A tions, who speaks to whom, and so forth. He explained that
LABORATORY EXPERIMENT he had chosen as a topic “The Psychology of Sex” in order
to attract many volunteers, as many college people were
The best way to describe how to conduct an experiment is interested in the topic. He then went on to say that, much to
to take a real study and dissect it carefully, examining how his dismay, he subsequently discovered that this topic pre-
it was done and why it was done that way. We have chosen sented one great disadvantage; namely, that many volunteers,
for illustrative purposes a classic laboratory experiment because of shyness, found it more difficult to participate in a
by Aronson and Mills (1959). We use this experiment for discussion about sex than in a discussion about a more neu-
several reasons. First, it illustrates clearly both the advan- tral topic. He explained that his study would be impaired if
tages and the challenges of attempting to do experimen- a group member failed to participate freely. He then asked
tal research in social psychology; we did not select it as the participant if she felt able to discuss this topic freely.
a Platonic ideal. Second, we discuss it as an example of Each participant invariably replied in the affirmative.
basic, process-oriented research that is applicable to many The instructions were used to set the stage for the initia-
different phenomena, including the one near the beginning tion that followed. The participants were randomly assigned
of this chapter—prejudice. At first glance this might be dif- to one of three experimental conditions: a severe-initiation
ficult to see, in that the Aronson and Mills (1959) study condition, a mild-initiation condition, or a no-initiation con-
investigated the effects of the severity of an initiation on lik- dition. The participants in the no-initiation condition were
ing for a discussion group—a topic that seems far removed told, at this point, that they could now join a discussion
from the kinds of prejudice and racism we see around us group. It was not that easy for the participants in the other
today. Indeed, some aspects of the Aronson and Mills two conditions. The experimenter told these participants
study might even seem old-fashioned; it was, after all, con- that he had to be absolutely certain that they could discuss
ducted over 50 years ago. Nonetheless, it deals with basic sex frankly before admitting them to a group. Accordingly,
issues that are as fresh and important today as they were in he said that he had recently developed a test that he would
1959: What happens when people invest time and effort in now use as a “screening device” to eliminate those students
something, such as joining a social group, that turns out to who would be unable to engage in such a discussion without
be much less enjoyable than they thought it would be? Can excessive embarrassment. In the severe-initiation condition,
the psychological processes that are triggered add to our the test consisted of having people recite (to the male exper-
understanding of why people in contemporary society tend imenter) a list of 12 obscene words and two vivid descrip-
to be attached to their own groups to an extreme degree, tions of sexual activity from contemporary novels. In the
and why they derogate members of other groups? The fact mild-initiation condition, the women were merely required
is that a laboratory experiment—even one conducted five to recite words related to sex that were not obscene.
decades ago—does have a lot to say about a variety of cur- Each of the participants was then allowed to “sit in” on
rent real-world phenomena, including prejudice, because it a group discussion that she was told was being carried on
illuminates basic, psychological processes. by members of the group she had just joined. This group
Aronson and Mills set out to test the hypothesis that was described as one that had been meeting for several
individuals who undergo a severe initiation in order to be weeks; the participant was told that she would be replacing

CH02.indd 59 9/2/09 8:09:56 PM


62 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

a group member who was leaving because of a scheduling discussion of experimental versus correlational methods
conflict. and laboratory versus field research: the ability to control
To provide everyone with an identical stimulus, the extraneous variables and to randomly assign people to the
experimenter had them listen to the same tape-recorded different conditions. They could have opted to study real
group discussion. At the same time, the investigators felt groups, such as fraternities and sororities, measuring the
it would be more involving for participants if they didn’t severity of their initiations and the attractiveness of each
feel that they were just listening to a tape recording but group to its members. Although such a study would have
were made to believe that this was a live-group discus- some advantages, we trust its disadvantages are by now
sion. In order to accomplish this and to justify the lack clear: the inability to determine causality. Because of the
of visual contact necessitated by the tape recording, the inability to control extraneous variables (i.e., the actual
experimenter explained that people found that they could attractiveness of the different fraternities and sororities),
talk more freely if they were not being looked at; therefore, and the inability to randomly assign people to condition (i.e.,
each participant was in a separate cubicle, talking through to groups with mild or severe initiations), there would be
a microphone and listening in on headphones. Since this no way of knowing whether severe initiations caused more
explanation was consistent with the other aspects of the attraction to the group. For example, it may be that desir-
cover story, all the participants found it convincing. able fraternities are inundated with applicants; because
Needless to say, it was important to discourage partici- of this, they set up severe initiations to discourage people
pants from trying to “talk back” to the tape, since by doing from applying. Once word gets around, only those who
so they would soon discover that no one was responding to are highly motivated to join those particular fraternities
their comments. In order to accomplish this, the experimenter are willing to subject themselves to severe initiations. If
explained that it would be better if she did not try to partici- this were the case, it is not the severity of the initiation
pate in the first meeting, since she would not be as prepared that caused people to find the fraternities attractive; rather,
as the other members who had done some preliminary read- it is the attractiveness of the fraternities that produced the
ings on the topic. He then disconnected her microphone. severity of the initiation!
At the close of the taped discussion, the experimenter
returned and explained that after each session all members Choosing the Type of Experiment to Perform
were asked to rate the worth of that particular discussion
and the performance of the other participants. He then Let us assume that you are a novice researcher with a ter-
presented each participant with a list of rating scales. The rific idea for an experiment. The first decision you would
results confirmed the hypothesis. The women in the severe- want to make is whether to design your experiment for
initiation condition found the group much more attractive the laboratory or the field. It is our position that all experi-
than did the women in the mild-initiation or the no-initia- ments should be conducted in a variety of settings. Thus,
tion conditions. we advocate that, ideally, all experimentally researchable
At first glance, this procedure has some serious prob- hypotheses should be tested in both the laboratory and
lems. As with the Gilbert and Hixon (1991) study we dis- the field. As we have mentioned, each approach has its
cussed earlier, the experimenters constructed an elaborate advantages and disadvantages, although it is often easier to
scenario bearing little relation to the “real-life” situations maintain internal validity in the laboratory. So, let’s suppose
in which they were interested. The “group” which people that you decide to start there. The next decision you must
found attractive was, in fact, nothing more than a few voices make is how to ensure that psychological realism is high in
coming in over a set of earphones. The participant was your study. Often this determines whether the experiment is
not allowed to see her fellow group members nor was she to be an impact or a judgment type. In impact experiments
allowed to interact with them verbally. This situation is a far people are active participants in an unfolding series of
cry from group interaction as we know it outside the labora- events and have to react to those events as they occur. Often,
tory. In addition, reciting a list of obscene words is undoubt- these events have a substantial impact on their self-views,
edly a much milder form of initiation to a group than most and people thus become deeply involved in the experiment.
actual initiation experiences outside the laboratory (e.g., a In judgment experiments participants are more like passive
college fraternity or into the Marine Corps). Moreover, the observers; they are asked to recognize, recall, classify, or
use of deception raises serious ethical problems as well as evaluate stimulus materials presented by the experimenter.
more pragmatic ones such as whether the deception was Little direct impact on participants is intended, except inso-
successful. far as the stimulus materials capture people’s attention and
The reasons why Aronson and Mills (1959) opted to do elicit meaningful judgmental responses. Which type of
a laboratory experiment should be clear from our earlier study should you do?

CH02.indd 60 9/2/09 8:09:57 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 63

As we mentioned, it depends on the psychological phe- create experimental manipulations, or to even dynamically
nomenon you are studying. A researcher who was interested react to respondents’ answers.
in the effects of sexual arousal on persuasibility would be in At this point in time, Internet-based research has primarily
the domain of the impact study. It would be absurd to con- focused on judgment-type experiments. However, there have
duct an experiment on the effects of sexual arousal without been several instances in which researchers have designed
doing something aimed at affecting the degree of sexual impact type experiments that occur over the Internet. For
arousal among some of the participants. On the other hand, example, Kip Williams successfully ported a laboratory
some hypotheses are judgmental in nature. For example, as paradigm for ostracism (Williams & Sommer, 1997) into the
we saw, Gilbert and Hixon (1991) hypothesized that ste- online realm (Williams, Cheung, & Choi, 2000). Participants
reotypes are more likely to be activated when people are in original paradigm sat in a waiting room with two confed-
not cognitively busy. They pointed out that interacting with erates who, by design, initiated a three-way game of catch
a member of a stereotyped group can itself make people with a rubber ball. After a predetermined amount of time, the
“busy,” in that people have to think about their own actions confederates excluded the participant and only tossed the ball
and the impressions they are making at the same time they to each other. Excluded participants found the experience to
are forming an impression of the other person. Thus, to see be intensely unpleasant and revealed marked cognitive defi-
whether stereotypes are more likely to be triggered when cits as a result. In the online version of this game, partici-
people are not cognitively busy, it was important to have pants control their own computer avatar and play “catch”
people judge a member of a stereotyped group but not to with other players. As in the real-life game, programmed
interact with this person—in short, to make it more of a confederates eventually exclude the participant. Williams et
judgment than an impact study. They accomplished this al. (2000) report that although the psychological impact of
by showing people a videotape of an Asian or Caucasian the ostracism is less pronounced, it is nonetheless undeni-
woman, instead of having them actually meet and interact ably real in the sense of having “experimental realism.”
with the woman. Researchers have also begun to explore the potential of
The point is that researchers should tailor their method immersive virtual reality experiences that promise to allow
to their hypothesis. Judgment experiments are usually researchers to control the laboratory experiment with even
easier to do, because they require a less elaborate “setting more precision (e.g., Blascovich et al., 2002). When these
of the stage” to involve the participants in an impactful sit- techniques are coupled with Internet capability, it promises
uation. If researchers are interested in what happens when to increase both the impact of laboratory studies as well as
a person’s self-concept is engaged by a series of events that the range of experimental milieus and subject populations.
happen to that person, however, there is no substitute for
the impact experiment. The Four Stages of Laboratory Experimentation
Experimentation and the Internet The process of planning a laboratory experiment consists
Recent developments in Internet applications have made of four basic stages: (l) setting the stage for the experiment,
it possible to order groceries, adjust a thermostat, and pro- (2) constructing the independent variable, (3) measuring the
gram a television recorder from one’s home, office, or wil- dependent variable, and (4) planning the post-experimental
derness retreat. It is thus not surprising that researchers have follow-up. In this section we will suggest ways of develop-
also found innovative methods to leverage this medium for ing a sensible and practical modus operandi for each of
psychological research. Our take on these innovations is those stages. We will be looking at both the impact experi-
that although they usefully expand the researcher’s tool- ment and the judgment experiment. It should be mentioned
box, the fundamental principles of experimentation that at the outset that the four phases listed above apply to both
we are laying out in this chapter remain unchanged. The types of laboratory experiment. Almost without excep-
mechanics are different, of course, but the underlying prin- tion, however, the impact experiment is more complex and
ciples are not. involves a wider scope of planning than does the judgment
Researchers have been using desktop computers for experiment. Accordingly, much of our discussion will be
decades to conduct experiments, of course, and the addition devoted to the high-impact type of study, not because we
of Internet connectivity enhances those capabilities. The consider such experiments as necessarily more important
most obvious and common application involves opinion but because we consider them more complex.
surveys that can be easily administered to large groups of
Setting the Stage
people with more demographic diversity than is typically
found within college populations. This approach has the In designing any laboratory experiment, a great deal of
added bonus of being able to easily modify the surveys to ingenuity and invention must be directed toward the context,

CH02.indd 61 9/2/09 8:09:57 PM


64 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

or stage, for the manipulation of the independent variable. rationale for both the presentation of the stimulus and the
Because of the fact that our participants tend to be intel- collection of the data.
ligent, adult, curious humans, the setting must make sense Providing a convincing rationale for the experiment is
to them. It not only must be consistent with the procedures almost always essential, since participants attempt to make
for presenting the independent variables and measuring sense of the situation and to decipher the reasons for the
their impact but also can and should enhance that impact experiment. A good cover story is one that embraces all
and help to justify the collection of the data. the necessary aspects of the experiment in a plausible
Many experiments involve deception; if deception is manner and thus eliminates speculation from a participant
used, the setting must include a sensible, internally con- about what the experimenter really has in mind. It also
sistent pretext or rationale for the research as well as a should capture the attention of the participants so that they
context that both supports and enhances the collection of remain alert and responsive to the experimental events.
the data and reduces the possibility of detection. This false This is not meant facetiously; if a cover story strikes the
rationale is often referred to as a cover story. participants as being a trivial or silly reason for conducting
In a judgment experiment, the cover story is typically an experiment, they may simply tune out. If the partici-
less elaborate and more straightforward than in an impact pants are not attending to the independent variable, it will
experiment. Although deception is frequently used in a judg- have little impact on them.
ment experiment, it is usually minimal and aimed primarily The setting may be a relatively simple one, or it may
at increasing the interest of the participants and providing involve an elaborate scenario, depending on the demands
a credible rationale for the data collection procedures and of the situation. Obviously, the experimenter should set the
judgment task. For example, Aronson, Willerman, and stage as simply as possible. If a simple setting succeeds
Floyd (1966) performed a judgment experiment to test the in providing a plausible cover story and in capturing the
hypothesis that the attractiveness of a highly competent per- attention of the participants, there is no need for greater
son would be enhanced if that person committed a clumsy elaboration. A more elaborate setting is sometimes neces-
blunder—because the clumsy blunder would tend to human- sary, especially in a high-impact experiment. For example,
ize the person. To provide an adequate test of the hypothesis, suppose researchers want to make people fearful. They
it was necessary to expose people to one of four experimen- might achieve this goal by simply telling the participants
tal conditions: (l) a highly competent person who commits that they will receive a strong electric shock. Yet the
a clumsy blunder, (2) a highly competent person who does chances of arousing strong fear are enhanced if one has
not commit a clumsy blunder, (3) a relatively incompetent set the stage with a trifle more embellishment. This can
person who commits a clumsy blunder, or (4) a relatively be done by providing a medical atmosphere, inventing a
incompetent person who does not. medical rationale for the experiment, having the experi-
What would be a reasonable context that would jus- menter appear in a white laboratory coat, and allowing
tify exposing people to one of these stimulus persons and the participant to view a formidable, scary-looking electri-
inducing them to rate the attractiveness of that person? cal apparatus as in Schachter’s (1959) experiments on the
The experimenters simply informed the participants (who effects of anxiety on the desire to affiliate with others. One
were students at the University of Minnesota) that their might go even further by providing the participant with a
help was needed in selecting students to represent the mild sample shock and implying that the actual shocks will
university on the College Bowl, a television program pit- be much greater.
ting college students from various universities against one The point we are making is that in a well-designed
another in a test of knowledge. They told the participants experiment, the cover story is an intricate and tightly woven
that they could evaluate the general knowledge of the can- tapestry. With this in mind, let us take another look at the
didates objectively, but that this was only one criterion for Aronson and Mills (1959) experiment. Here we shall indi-
selection. Another criterion was judgments from the par- cate how each aspect of the setting enhanced the impact
ticipants concerning how much they liked the candidates. and/or plausibility of the independent and dependent
The experimenter then presented the participant with an variables and contributed to the control of the experiment.
audio tape recording of a male stimulus person being inter- The major challenge presented by the hypothesis was to
viewed. This stimulus person answered a number of ques- justify an initiation for admission to a group. This was
tions either brilliantly or not so brilliantly and either did solved, first, by devising the format of a sex discussion, and
or did not clumsily spill a cup of coffee all over himself. second, by inventing the cover story that the experimenters
The participant then rated the stimulus person on a series were interested in studying the dynamics of the discus-
of scales. The cover story in this experiment was simple sion process. Combining these two aspects of the setting,
and straightforward and did succeed in providing a credible the experimenter could then, third, mention that because

CH02.indd 62 9/2/09 8:09:58 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 65

shyness about sex distorts the discussion process, it was, as this into a concrete experimental operation. One of the
fourth, necessary to eliminate those people who were shy most important parts of experimental design is to devise a
about sexual matters by, fifth, presenting the participants procedure that “captures” the conceptual variable perfectly
with an embarrassment test. without influencing any other factors. If we have our par-
All five aspects of the setting led directly to the manipu- ticipants recite a list of obscene words and then listen to a
lation of the independent variable in a manner that made boring group discussion, how can we be sure that this is,
good sense to the participants, thereby allaying any suspi- in fact, an empirical realization of our conceptual variable?
cions. Moreover, this setting allowed the experimenter to Sometimes this is very difficult, and after an experiment
use a tape-recorded group discussion (for the sake of control) is done, the researcher realizes that whereas participants
and at the same time to maintain the fiction that it was an in Conditions A and B were thought to differ only in one
ongoing group discussion (for the sake of impact). conceptual variable (the amount of cognitive dissonance
This fiction of an already formed group served another people experienced), they also differed in some other way.
function in addition to that of enhancing the involvement Controversy over the correct interpretation of the results
of the participants. It also allowed the experimenter to obtained in the Aronson and Mills initiation experiment
explain to the participant that all the other members had discussed earlier provides an example of this problem.
been recruited before the initiation was made a require- The complex social situation used by Aronson and Mills
ment for admission. This procedure eliminated a possible has many potential interpretations, including the possi-
confounding variable, namely, that participants might bility that reading obscene materials generated a state of
like the group better in the severe-initiation condition sexual arousal that carried over to reactions to the group
because of the feeling that they had shared a common discussion. If that were the case, it could be that transfer of
harrowing experience. arousal, rather than effort justification, accounted for the
Finally, because of the manner in which the stage had higher attraction to the group.
been set, the dependent variable (the evaluation of the A replication of the initiation experiment by Gerard and
group) seemed a very reasonable request. In many experi- Mathewson (1966) ruled out this interpretation. Their experi-
mental contexts, obtaining a rating of attractiveness tends ment was constructed so as to differ from the Aronson and
to arouse suspicion. In this context, however, it was not jar- Mills study in many respects. For example, Gerard and
ring to the participant to be told that each member stated her Mathewson used electric shocks instead of the reading of
opinion of each discussion session, and therefore it did not obscene words as their empirical realization of severe ini-
surprise the participant when she was asked for her frank tiation (and the dissonance it produced); the shocks were
evaluation of the proceedings of the meeting. Ultimately, justified as a test of “emotionality” rather than as a test of
the success of a setting in integrating the various aspects embarrassment; the tape recording concerned a group dis-
of the experiment is an empirical question: Do the partici- cussion of cheating rather than of sex; and the measure of
pants find it plausible? In the Aronson-Mills experiment attractiveness of the group differed slightly. Thus, sexual
only one of 64 participants expressed any suspicions about arousal was eliminated as a concomitant of the experimen-
the true nature of the experiment. tal procedures. The results confirmed the original findings:
The testing of some hypotheses is more difficult than People who underwent painful electric shocks in order to
others because of their very nature. But none is impossible; become members of a dull group found that group to be
with sufficient patience and ingenuity a reasonable context more attractive than did people who underwent mild shocks.
can be constructed to integrate the independent and dependent Such a confirmation of the basic initiation effect under quite
variables regardless of the problems inherent in the hypothesis. different experimental operations supports, at least indirectly,
the idea that it was cognitive dissonance produced by a
Constructing the Independent Variable severe initiation, and not some other conceptual variable, that
One of the most important and difficult parts of experi- was responsible for the results. A considerable amount of
mental design is constructing an independent variable that research in social psychology has been motivated by similar
manipulates only what you want it to manipulate. The controversies over the valid interpretation of results obtained
experimenter begins with what we will call the concep- with complex experimental procedures.
tual variable, which is a theoretically important variable
that he or she thinks will have a causal effect on people’s The Issue of Standardization Ideally, the empirical real-
responses. In the Aronson and Mills study, for example, ization of an independent variable is forceful enough to
the conceptual variable might be thought of as cognitive have maximum impact and clear enough to generate the
dissonance caused by an embarrassing initiation. There are intended interpretation in all participants. This section has
many ways to translate an abstract conceptual variable such sought to establish some important general guidelines. There

CH02.indd 63 9/2/09 8:09:58 PM


66 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

is, however, one crucial, yet frequently misunderstood, chological experience of participants. Of course, by allow-
point: It is extremely important for all participants to be in ing the experimenter to depart from a standardized script,
the same psychological state as a result of the manipula- one may increase the possibility of introducing a systematic
tion of the independent variable. This does not necessarily bias. But if proper techniques are employed to eliminate bias,
mean that all participants should be exposed to the identical this ceases to be a problem. In particular, if the experimenter
independent variable. This does mean that the experiment- who is giving the instructions is unaware of the participant’s
er’s skill and wisdom should be used to make sure that all experimental condition, there is no way in which variations
participants arrive at a similar understanding of the instruc- in the presentation can systematically bias the results.
tions (or the implications of the “event” manipulation). To We return now to a discussion of independent vari-
achieve this goal, the experimenter should take consider- ables and how they should be administered. Recall that
able latitude in delivering the instructions or experimental the essence of an experiment is the random assignment
manipulation. This is a tricky issue and is one that may of participants to experimental conditions. For this rea-
raise doubts in the minds of many investigators. Our point son, it should be obvious that any characteristics that the
is this: In their zeal for standardization, many experiment- participants bring to the experiment cannot be regarded as
ers make an effort to have all instructions to the participants independent variables in the context of a true experiment.
tape-recorded, printed, or computerized, to ensure that all It is not infrequent to find an “experiment” purporting to
participants are exposed to identical stimuli. Such an effort assess the effects of a participant variable (like level of
is admirable, but in practice it ignores the fact that people self-esteem, for example) on some behavior in a specific situ-
are different, and as a consequence, the same instructions ation. It should be clear that although such a procedure may
do not mean the same thing to all participants. More pro- produce interesting results, it is not an experiment because
saic, yet more important, participants differ greatly in their the variable was not randomly assigned. Nonrandom
ability to understand instructions. For example, one of the assignment of participants to experimental conditions is
most common mistakes the novice experimenter makes is not confined to the use of personality measures in lieu of
to present the instructions too succinctly; consequently, a experimental treatments. It usually takes place in more
large percentage of the participants fail to understand what subtle ways. One of the most common occurs when the
is going on in an experiment (especially one as complicated experimenter is forced to perform an “internal analysis” in
as most social psychological experiments are), a good deal order to make sense out of his or her data.
of redundancy is necessary. The term “internal analysis” refers to the following situ-
A brief analogy to drug trials will make our point. When ation. Suppose that an experimenter has carried out a true
a clinician tests the efficacy of an anti-anxiety medication, experiment, randomly assigning participants to different
she administers a precise quantity of the drug to ensure that treatment conditions. Unfortunately, the treatments do not
each participant develops the same concentration of the produce any measurable differences on the dependent vari-
drug in his or her bloodstream. The 250-pound linebacker able. In addition, suppose that the experimenter has had
will be administered a significantly higher dose than the the foresight to include an independent measure of the
90-pound dancer; but in the end both participants have effectiveness of the experimental treatment. Such “manipu-
the identical therapeutic dose of the drug in their system. lation checks” are always useful in providing information
In social psychological experiments it may sometimes be about the extent to which the experimental treatment had
necessary to “titrate” the independent variable in a similar its intended effect on each individual participant. Now,
manner; one must focus on the outcome of the manipula- if the manipulation check shows no differences between
tion rather than the input of the manipulation. experimental treatments, the experimenter may still hope
We anticipate that many experimenters will disagree with to salvage his or her hypothesis. That is, the manipula-
us, suggesting that standardization is the hallmark of an tion check shows that for some reason the treatments were
experiment. We agree, but exactly what is it that should be unsuccessful in creating the internal states in the partici-
standardized? What the experimenter says, or what the par- pants that they were designed to produce. Since they were
ticipant understands? We feel that the more variability there unsuccessful, one would not expect to see differences on
is in the participants’ comprehension of the experimental the dependent variable. In this case, the experimenter may
operations, the more likely it will be that the changes caused analyze the data on the basis of the responses of the partici-
by the independent variable will be obscured. This discussion pants to the manipulation check, resorting participants into
again echoes the constant tension in experimentation between “treatment” according to their responses to the manipula-
control and impact. However, in this case we would argue tion check. This is an internal analysis.
that by giving up rigid conformity to a script, one in fact can For example, Schachter (1959) attempted to alter the
gain increased impact and decreased variability in the psy- amount of anxiety experienced by his participants by varying

CH02.indd 64 9/2/09 8:09:59 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 67

the description of the task in which the participants were to basis. In a between-subject design people are randomly
engage. However, in some of the studies, many participants assigned to different levels of the independent variable,
who had been given the treatment designed to produce low- as in the Aronson and Mills study, in which different
anxiety actually reported higher anxiety levels than some groups of people received different levels of initiation. In
who had been given the treatment designed to produce high a within-subjects design all participants receive all levels
anxiety. From the results of an internal analysis of these data, of the independent variable. For example, in the literature
it does seem that anxiety is related to the dependent vari- on detecting deception, participants are typically shown a
able. Again, these data can be useful and provocative, but videotape of another person and are asked to judge whether
since the effect was not due to the manipulated variable, no that person is lying or telling the truth. A number of factors
causal statement can be made. Although many of the “highly have been manipulated to see how easy it is to tell whether
anxious” participants were made anxious by the “high-anxi- the person is lying, such as whether the person on the tape
ety” manipulation, many were highly anxious on their own. is saying something good or bad about another person and
Because people who become anxious easily may be differ- whether the person had the opportunity to think about and
ent from those who do not, we are dealing with an individual plan the lie before delivering it (e.g. DePaulo, Lanier, &
difference variable. This means that we can no longer claim Davis, 1983). These factors are often manipulated on a
random assignment—and, in effect, we no longer have an within-subjects basis. In the DePaulo et al. (1983) study,
experiment. for example, participants watched people make four state-
Another situation in which the treatments are assigned ments: a planned lie, a spontaneous lie, a planned true
nonrandomly occurs when the participants assign them- statement, and a spontaneous true statement. The partici-
selves to the experimental conditions. That is, in certain pants did not know which statement was which, of course;
experimental situations the participant, in effect, is given their job was to guess how truthful each statement was. As
a choice of two procedures in which to engage. The it turned out, people were able to detect lies at better than
experimenter then compares the subsequent behavior of chance levels, but spontaneous lies were no easier to detect
participants who choose one alternative with those who than planned lies.
choose the other. For example, Carlsmith, Wilson, and Within-subject designs are often preferred, because fewer
Gilbert (2008) examined the emotional consequences of participants are required to achieve sufficient statistical power.
exacting revenge. They created a situation in which the Imagine that DePaulo et al. (1983) had used a between-
participant could readily punish an offending person, and subjects design, such that four separate groups of participants
then measured participants’ affect shortly thereafter. One saw statements that were either planned lies, unplanned lies,
approach would have been to create two groups—punishers planned truthful statements, or unplanned truthful state-
and non-punishers—by allowing the participants to decide ments. They probably would have had to include at least 15
for themselves whether to punish. Such a procedure, how- people per condition, for a total of 60 participants. By using
ever, would have confounded the act of punishment with a within-design in which every participant was run in each of
participant variables. For example, it could be that people the four conditions, fewer people were needed (there were
who entered the experiment in a grouchy mood would be only 24 people who judged the statements in this study).
more likely to punish and more likely to report negative One reason fewer participants are needed is because
affect. Thus, in order to maintain the experimental nature each participant serves as his or her own control; each
of the design, it was necessary to create a true control person’s responses in one condition are compared to that
group that had no opportunity to punish and to create a same person’s responses in the other conditions. This con-
punishment group in which every participant opted to pun- trols for any number of individual difference variables
ish, although technically they had the option not to. The that are treated as error variance in a between-subjects
problem of free choice is a particularly sticky one because, design. Suppose, for example, that one participant has a
if the hypothesis involves the effects of choice, it is obvi- very suspicious view of the world and thinks that people
ously important to give the participant a perception of clear are lying most of the time. Another participant is very
choice. Yet this perception must remain nothing more than trusting and thinks that people seldom lie. Suppose further
a perception, for as soon as the participant takes advan- that a between-subjects design was used, and the distrust-
tage of it, we are beset with the problems of nonrandom ful and trusting people are randomly assigned to different
assignment. conditions. In this design, it would be difficult to separate
the effects of the independent variable (e.g., whether the
Between- versus Within-Subject Designs Another deci- person on the tape was lying or telling the truth) from
sion facing the experimenter is whether to manipulate the how suspicious participants’ are in general. With random
independent variable on a between-subject or within-subject assignment, of course, individual differences tend to cancel

CH02.indd 65 9/2/09 8:09:59 PM


68 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

out across condition; the number of suspicious versus spent their entire lives in a social environment and—because
trusting people should be roughly the same in all condi- of their intelligence and curiosity—they have formed their
tions. Nonetheless the “noise” produced by personality own theories and hypotheses about precisely the behaviors
differences makes it difficult to detect the “signal” of the we are trying to investigate. That is to say, everyone in the
effects of the independent variable, and a large number of world, including the participants in our experiments, is a
participants often have to be run to detect the signal. In a social psychological theorist.
within-subjects design, this problem is solved by running In a nutshell, the challenge (and the excitement) of
every person in every condition. The suspicious person’s doing experiments in social psychology lies in the quest to
responses to the lies are compared to his or her responses find a way to circumvent or neutralize the theories that the
to the non-lies, thereby “canceling out” his or her tendency to participants walk in with so that we can discover their true
rate everyone as deceptive. behavior under specifiable conditions, rather than being
If a within-subject design is used it is important, of left to ponder behavior that reflects nothing more than
course, to vary the order of the experimental conditions, to how the subjects think they should behave in a contrived
make sure that the effects of the independent variable are attempt to confirm their own theory.
not confounded with the order in which people receive the One special form of participant awareness is closely
different manipulations. This is referred to as “counterbal- related to the idea of “demand characteristics” as described
ancing,” whereby participants are randomly assigned to get by Orne (1962). The term refers to features introduced into
the manipulations in different orders. In the DePaulo et al. a research setting by virtue of the fact that it is a research
(1983) study, for example, the presentation of the decep- study and that the participants know that they are part of
tive versus nondeceptive statements and planned versus it. As aware participants, they are motivated to make sense
unplanned statements was counterbalanced, such that dif- of the experimental situation, to avoid negative evaluation
ferent participants saw the statements in different orders. from the experimenter, and perhaps even to cooperate in a
In many social psychological experiments within-subject way intended to help the experimenter confirm the research
designs are not feasible, because it would not make sense hypothesis (Sigall, Aronson, & Van Hoose, 1970). Such
to participants to evaluate the same stimulus more than motivational states could make participants responsive to
once under slightly different conditions. For example, any cues—intended or unintended—in the research situation
in the experiment by Aronson, Willerman, and Floyd, once that suggest what they are supposed to do to appear normal
a participant was exposed to a tape recording of a compe- or “to make the study come out right.” It is for this reason
tent person spilling coffee, it would have been ludicrous that experimenters frequently employ deception, elaborate
to present that same participant with an otherwise identi- cover stories, and the like, in an attempt to keep participants
cal tape of a competent person who doesn’t spill coffee. unaware of the experimental manipulations in play.
Who would believe that there are two people in the world Another aspect of the problem of demand character-
who are identical in all ways except for their coffee-spilling istics and participant awareness is the possibility that the
behavior? By the same token, in the vast majority of impact experimenter’s own behavior provides inadvertent cues that
experiments, the nature of the impactful manipulation pre- influence the responses of the participants. In our experi-
cludes utilization of the same participants in more than one ence novice researchers often dismiss this possibility; they
condition. For example, in the Aronson and Mills experi- smile knowingly and say, “Of course I wouldn’t act in such
ment, once the experimenters put a participant through a a way to bias people’s responses.” Decades of research on
severe initiation in order to join a group and then asked her expectancy effects, however, show that the transmission of
to rate the attractiveness of that group, it would have been expectations from researchers to participants is subtle and
silly to ask her to start all over and go through a mild initia- unintentional, and that this transmission can have dramatic
tion. Thus, within-subjects designs are preferable if at all effects on participants’ behavior. It can occur even between
possible, but in many studies—especially impact experi- a human experimenter and an animal participant; in one
ments—they are not feasible. study, for example, rats learned a maze quickly when the
experimenter thought they were good learners and slowly
Avoiding Participant Awareness Biases It is arguably when the experimenter thought they were poor learners
more challenging to perform a meaningful experiment in (Rosenthal, 1994; Rosenthal & Lawson, 1964).
social psychology than in any other scientific discipline Therefore, steps must be taken to avoid this transmission
for one simple and powerful reason: In social psychology, of the experimenter’s hypotheses to the research participants.
we are testing our theories and hypotheses on adult human One way of doing so is to keep the experimenter unaware
beings who are almost always intelligent, curious, and expe- of the hypothesis of the research. The idea here is that if
rienced. They are experienced in the sense that they have the experimenter does not know the hypothesis, he or she

CH02.indd 66 9/2/09 8:10:00 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 69

cannot transmit the hypothesis to the research participants. which recorded instructions each participant receives (e.g.
In our judgment, however, this technique is inadequate. One Harackiewicz, Manderlink, & Sansone, 1984).
characteristic of good researchers is that they are hypothesis- In other studies, however—particularly high impact
forming organisms. Indeed, as we mentioned earlier, this ones—the experimenter must deliver the independent vari-
is one characteristic of all intelligent humans. Thus, if not able in person, making it more difficult for him or her to
told the hypothesis, the research assistant, like a participant, be unaware of participant’s experimental condition. In the
attempts to discover one. Moreover, keeping the assistant Aronson and Mills experiment, for example, people’s con-
in the dark reduces the value of the educational experi- dition was determined by which list of words they had to
ence. Since many experimenters are graduate students, full read aloud to the experimenter. The experimenter could
participation in an experiment is the most effective way have given people a list and asked them to read the words
of learning experimentation. Any technique involving the to themselves, but this obviously would have reduced the
experimenter’s ignorance of the hypothesis or a reduction impact of the manipulation considerably. In studies such
in contact with the supervisor is a disservice to him or her. as these, where it is necessary for the experimenter to
A more reasonable solution involves allowing the experi- “deliver” the independent variable, several steps can still be
menters to know the true hypothesis but keeping them taken to avoid demand characteristics, participant aware-
ignorant of the specific experimental condition of each ness biases, and experimenter expectancy effects. First, the
participant. This is typically referred to as a “double-blind” experimenter should be kept ignorant of people’s condition
study in which both the experimenter and participant are until the precise moment of crucial difference in manipula-
unaware of the experimental condition. In theory, this is a tions. That is, in most studies, the experimenter need not
simple and complete solution to the problem and should be know what condition the participant is in until the crucial
employed whenever possible. manipulation occurs. When the choice point is reached, a
In a study by Wilson et al. (1993), for example, the inde- randomizing device can be used, and the remainder of the
pendent variable was whether people were asked to think experiment is, of course, not carried out in ignorance. For
about why they felt the way they did about some art posters, example, in the Aronson and Mills study, it would have
to examine the effects of introspection on attitude change been easy to delay assignment of participant to condition
and satisfaction with consumer choices. Participants were until the point of initiation; by reaching into a pocket and
told that the purpose of the study was to examine the dif- randomly pulling out one of three slips of paper, the exper-
ferent types of visual effects that people like in pictures imenter could determine whether the participant would
and drawings and that they would be asked to evalu- recite the obscene words, the mild words, or no words at
ate some posters. The critical manipulation was whether all. Thus, all the pre-manipulation instructions would be
people wrote down why they felt the way they did about unbiased.
each poster (the reasons condition) or wrote why they had This is only a partial solution because the experimenter
chosen their major (the control condition). To assign peo- loses his or her ignorance midway through the experiment.
ple to condition randomly, the experimenter simply gave However, if the experimenter left the room immediately
them a questionnaire from a randomly ordered stack. To after the recitation and a different experimenter (unaware
make sure the experimenter did not know whether it was of the participant’s experimental condition) collected the
the reasons or control questionnaire, an opaque cover sheet data, this solution would approach completeness. The use of
was stapled to each one. The experimenter left the room multiple experimenters, each ignorant of some part of the
while the participant completed the questionnaire, and thus experiment, offers a solution that is frequently viable. For
throughout the experiment was unaware whether the par- example, Wilson and Lassiter (1982) were interested in
ticipant was in the reasons or control condition. whether prohibiting people from engaging in unattractive
In other types of experiments, the experimental manipu- activities would increase the appeal of those activities; that
lations cannot be delivered simply by having people read is, whether the Aronson and Carlsmith (1963) “forbidden
written instructions, making it more difficult to keep the toy” effect would apply when the prohibited activity was
experimenter unaware of condition. In studies on intrinsic undesirable at the outset. The participants were preschool
motivation, for example, the critical manipulation is the level children who were seen individually. In one condition the
of reward people believe they will get for performing a task. experimenter showed the child five toys and said that he
This could be conveyed in written form, but there is a risk or she could play with any of them but a plastic motorcycle,
that participants will not read the questionnaire carefully which was known to be unattractive to the children. In the
enough, missing the crucial information about the reward. control condition the children were allowed to play with
A frequently used solution to this problem is to tape record all five toys. As we have discussed, the experimenter ran-
the instructions, and to keep the experimenter unaware of domly assigned people to condition at the last possible

CH02.indd 67 9/2/09 8:10:00 PM


70 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

moment, namely after he had shown the children all the bulb was burned out. He searched for another for a while,
toys and demonstrated how they worked. unsuccessfully, and then told participants that they would
To assess children’s subsequent interest in the toys, the have to go on with the study without seeing the rest of the
children were seen again a week later and given two of slides. By staging this “accident,” the researchers ensured
the toys to play with—the plastic motorcycle and another, that people were not suspicious about why they saw only
attractive toy. At this session the same experimenter could positive or negative behaviors a second time.
not be used, however, because he was no longer unaware Another way to make the independent variable seem
of the child’s experimental condition. Further, his presence like a spontaneous, unrelated event is to have a confederate,
might cause children to base their choice on factors other apparently a fellow participant, introduce the manipula-
than their liking; for example, they might be concerned tion. For example, Schachter and Singer (1962) attempted
that he still did not want them to play with the motorcycle. to manipulate euphoria by having a confederate waltz
Thus, a different experimenter (unaware of the child’s con- around the room shooting rubber bands, play with hula
dition) was used, and the children were not told that this hoops, and practice hook shots into the wastebasket with
session was part of the same study as the first session. As wadded paper. Presumably, this behavior was interpreted
predicted, the children who were prohibited from playing by the participant as a spontaneous, unique event unrelated
with the motorcycle in the first session played with it sig- to the intentions of the experimenter. A third method is to
nificantly more at the second session than did people in the use the whole experimental session as the independent
control condition. variable and to measure the dependent variable at some
Returning to the more general issue of demand charac- later time. For example, in the Wilson and Lassiter (1984)
teristics, it should be clear that the most effective type of study mentioned earlier, the independent variable (whether
deception in an impact experiment involves the creation people were constrained from playing with an unattractive
of an independent variable as an event that appears not to toy) was introduced at one session, and the dependent vari-
be part of the experiment at all. Creating such an indepen- able (how long people played with the toy) was assessed
dent variable not only guarantees that the participant will at another session a week later. It is unlikely that the par-
not try to interpret the researcher’s intention but also that ticipants realized that what happened in the first study was
the manipulation has an impact on the participant. Several the independent variable of interest. Even within the same
classes of techniques have been used successfully to pres- experimental session it is possible to convince people that
ent the independent variable as an event unrelated to the they are taking part in separate, unrelated experiments.
experiment. Perhaps the most effective is the “accident” A common ruse is the “multiple study” cover story, in
or “whoops” manipulation, in which the independent vari- which people are told that for reasons of convenience sev-
able is presented as part of what appears to be an accident eral unrelated mini-experiments are being conducted at the
or unforeseen circumstance. Wilson, Hodges, and LaFleur same session. This ruse is commonly employed in prim-
(1995) used a variation on this procedure to influence ing experiments, in which it is very important that people
people’s memory for behaviors performed by a target per- not connect the independent variable (the priming of a
son. These researchers showed people a list of positive and semantic category) with the dependent variable (ratings of
negative behaviors the target person had performed a target person whose standing on that category is ambigu-
and then wanted to make sure that people found it easi- ous). Higgins, Rholes, and Jones (1977), for example, had
est to remember either the positive or negative behaviors. people memorize words related to adventurousness or
They did so by simply showing people either the positive recklessness as part of an initial “Study 1” concerned with
or negative behaviors a second time. The danger of this perception and memory, and then had people rate a stimu-
procedure, however, is that it would be obvious to people lus person, whose behavior was ambiguous as to whether
that the researchers were trying to influence their memory. it was adventurous or reckless, as part of a “Study 2” on
If Wilson et al. had said, “OK, now we are going to show impression formation.
you only the positive (negative) behaviors again,” partici-
pants would undoubtedly have wondered why and possibly Optimizing the Impact of the Independent Variable As
figured out that the point was to influence their memory we mentioned, one problem with keeping experimenters
for these behaviors. To avoid this problem, Wilson et al. unaware of condition, by delivering the independent vari-
told people that they would see all of the behaviors again able in written form, is that the impact of the independent
on slides. After only positive (or negative) ones had been variable will be reduced. One of the most common mistakes
shown, it just so happened that the slide projector malfunc- the novice experimenter makes is to present instructions too
tioned. The projector suddenly went dark, and after examining briefly; consequently, a large percentage of the participants
it with some frustration, the experimenter declared that the fail to understand some important aspects of the instructions.

CH02.indd 68 9/2/09 8:10:01 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 71

To ensure that all participants understand what is going on manipulate. For example, in the Wilson et al. (1995) study,
in an experiment (especially one as complicated as most the point was to see what happens when people analyze the
social psychological experiments), the instructions should reasons for their impressions of a person and either posi-
be repeated in different ways. tive or negative thoughts about that person are most acces-
More important than simple redundancy, however, is sible in memory. The hypotheses of the study could only be
ensuring the instructions are expressed precisely so that tested if the manipulation of people’s memory succeeded
each participant fully understands them and the events that in making positive or negative thoughts more accessible.
occur in the experiment. This can be accomplished by a The ability to play with a design so that the manipulations
combination of written and verbal instructions, in which change the right variables is a skill similar to that of a tal-
the experimenter repeats or paraphrases key parts of the ented director who knows exactly how to alter the staging
instructions until satisfied that the participant is completely of a play to maximize its impact on the audience. This is
clear about all of them. Although the point seems obvi- where some of the most important work in experimental
ous, it has been our experience that many experiments fail design occurs, but it is rarely reported in published articles,
precisely because the instructions were never made clear because it would not be very informative or interesting to
enough to become understandable to all the participants. begin the methods section by saying, “We will first tell you
In the well-designed impact experiment, there is less about all the ways of manipulating the independent vari-
likely to be a question about whether the participant able that didn’t work. The first mistake we made was…”
is paying attention to the relevant stimulus conditions. It is another matter, however, if the manipulation works
Nonetheless the experimenter should be as certain as pos- as intended but does not influence the dependent variable
sible that the complex bundle of stimuli constituting the in the predicted manner. Another reason that a manipula-
independent variable produce the intended phenomenolog- tion can fail to have an effect is because the researcher’s
ical experience in the participants. For this purpose, there hypothesis is wrong. The manipulation might work exactly
is no substitute for the thorough pretesting of the manipu- as intended (as indicated, for example, on a manipula-
lation. During the pretesting, the experimenter can conduct tion check), but have a different effect on the dependent
long, probing interviews with the participant after the test variable than predicted. This is informative, because it
run of the experiment is completed or, better yet, after the suggests that the hypothesis might be wrong. The catch is
manipulation of the independent variable. that it is often difficult to tell whether an experiment is not
One of the most frequently misunderstood aspects of working because the manipulation is ineffective or because
experimentation is the amount of pretesting that is often the hypothesis is wrong. The answer to this question often
required to make sure that the independent variable is hav- becomes clear only after extensive tinkering and restaging
ing the desired impact. When students read published of the experimental situation.
experiments in psychological journals, they often have the Once it becomes clear that the manipulation is working
impression that the researchers had an idea, designed a as intended but the hypothesis is off the mark, a second
study, collected the data in a few weeks, analyzed the data, talent comes into play: The ability to learn from one’s mis-
and presto, found exactly what they predicted. Little do they takes. Some of the most famous findings in social psychology
know that in most cases the experiment was preceded by a did not come from reading the literature and deducing new
good deal of pretesting, whereby different versions of the hypotheses, or from “aha” insights while taking a shower.
independent variable were “tried out.” For example, in the Rather, they came about from the discovery that one’s
Wilson, Hodges, and LaFleur (1995) study mentioned earlier, hypotheses were wrong and the data suggest a very dif-
in which the researchers staged a malfunction of a slide pro- ferent hypothesis—one that is quite interesting and worth
jector, a good deal of pretesting was required to “fine tune” pursuing faithfully.
this manipulation. Different versions of the manipulation
were tried before one was found that worked convincingly. Choosing the Number of Independent Variables We
This might seem to be misleading, in that the research- have been talking thus far of the independent variable in
ers ended up reporting only the version of the indepen- the social psychological experiment as if it were a simple
dent variable that had the desired effect. It is important to two-level variation on a single dimension. Yet many, if not
note, however, that there are two meanings of the phrase most, experiments involve procedures that simultaneously
“desired effect”: (a) whether the researchers manipulated manipulate two or more variables. Once one has taken the
what they intended to manipulate and (b) whether the inde- time and trouble of setting up a laboratory experiment,
pendent variable had the predicted effect on the dependent recruiting participants, and training research assistants, it
variable. An experiment cannot test a hypothesis unless the seems only efficient to take the occasion to assess the effects
independent variable manipulates what it is supposed to of more than one experimental treatment.

CH02.indd 69 9/2/09 8:10:01 PM


72 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

There are no pat answers to the question of how many the ideal measure of interpersonal attraction is affiliative
independent variables can or should be manipulated at behaviors between two individuals. If you pick up a copy
one time, but our own rule is that an experiment should of a recent social psychological journal, however, you will
be only as complex as is required for important relation- find that measures of actual behavior are hard to come by
ships to emerge in an interpretable manner. Sometimes it is (Baumeister, Vohs, & Funder, 2007; de la Haye, 1991). The
essential to vary more than one factor because the phenom- dependent measures are more likely to be such things as
enon of interest appears in the form of an interaction. Petty, questionnaire ratings of people’s thoughts, attitudes, emo-
Cacioppo, and Goldman (1981), for example, hypothesized tions, and moods; their recall of past events; the speed with
that the way in which people process information in a per- which they can respond to various types of questions; or,
suasive communication depends on the personal relevance as we saw in the Gilbert and Hixon (1991) study, the ways
of the topic. When the topic was highly relevant, people in which people complete word fragments.
were predicted to be most influenced by the strength of There are four main reasons why social psychologists
the arguments in the communication, whereas when it was often measure things other than actual behavior. The first
low in relevance, people were predicted to be most influ- is convenience: It is much easier to give people a question-
enced by the expertise of the source of the communication. naire on which they indicate how much they like a target
To test this hypothesis the authors had to manipulate (a) the person, for example, then to observe and code their actual
personal relevance of the topic, (b) the strength of the argu- behavior toward the target person. Of course, convenience
ments in the message, and (c) the expertise of the source of is no excuse for doing poor science, and the assumption
the message. Only by including each of these independent that questionnaire responses are good proxies for actual
variables could the authors test their hypothesis, which was behavior should not be taken on faith. In the early years of
confirmed in the form of a three-way interaction. attitude research, for example, it was assumed that people’s
questionnaire ratings of their attitudes were good indicators
Measuring the Dependent Variable of how they would actually behavior toward the attitude
The basic decision facing the researcher in planning the object. It soon became apparent that this was often not the
measurement of dependent variables is whether to rely on case (e.g., Wicker, 1969), and many researchers devoted
participants’ self-reports or observations by others as the their energies to discovering when questionnaire measures
means of assessing a person’s responses to the experimen- of attitudes predict behavior and when they do not. A large
tal situation. Actually, it is not that simple, for it is possible literature on attitude-behavior consistency was the result,
to imagine a continuum ranging from behaviors of great and it is now clear that self-reported attitudes predict behav-
importance and consequence for the participant down to ior quite well under some circumstances but not others (e.g.,
the most trivial paper-and-pencil measures about which the Fazio, 1990; Wilson, Dunn, Kraft, & Lisle, 1989).
participant has no interest. At one extreme the experimenter Needless to say, there are some situations in which
could measure the extent to which participants actually per- obtaining a direct measure of the behavior of interest is
form a great deal of tedious labor for a fellow student (as a not simply inconvenient, it is virtually impossible. For
reflection of, say, their liking for that student). At the other example, Aronson and his students conducted a series of
extreme one could ask them to circle a number on a scale laboratory experiments aimed at convincing sexually active
entitled “How much did you like that other person who par- teenagers to use condoms as a way of preventing AIDS
ticipated in the experiment?” Close to the behavioral end and other sexually transmitted diseases (Aronson, Fried, &
of the continuum would be a measure of the participant’s Stone, 1991). The ideal behavioral dependent variable is
commitment to perform a particular action without actually obvious: whether the participants in the experimental con-
performing it. We call this a “behavioroid” measure. dition actually used condoms during sexual intercourse to
It is a fair assumption to say that most social psychologists a greater extent than participants in the control condition.
care the most about social behavior: how people treat each Think about it for a moment: How would you collect those
other and how they respond to the social world. The goal is data? Even experimental social psychologists feel obliged
not to explain and predict which number people will circle to stop short of climbing into bed with their subjects in order
on a scale or which button on a computer they will press, but to observe their sexual behavior directly. Aronson and his
people’s actual behavior toward another person or the envi- students were forced to use proxies. In some of their stud-
ronment. Thus, the first choice of a dependent measure in a ies, they used self-report as a proxy. In others, in addition
social psychological experiment is usually overt behavior. to self-report, they set up a situation where, at the close of
The ideal measure of prejudice is the way in which mem- the experiment, the experimenter while leaving the room,
bers of different groups treat each other, the ideal measure indicated that the participants, if they wanted, could pur-
of attitude change is behavior toward an attitude object, and chase condoms (at a bargain price) by helping themselves

CH02.indd 70 9/2/09 8:10:02 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 73

from huge a pile of condoms on the table and leaving the we can observe the extent to which the participant makes
appropriate sum of money. Although the participants had an effort to join a group to which the confederate belongs.
no way of suspecting that their behavior was being moni- This latter behavior seems to be a reflection of liking, but
tored, as soon as they left the laboratory, the experimenter it may reflect other things instead. For example, it may be
returned and re-counted the condoms on the table to ascer- that some participants in the “insult” condition want to join
tain exactly how many they had purchased. Admittedly, the group in order to prove to the confederate that they are
the number of condoms purchased is not quite as direct a not stupid. Or it may be that some want an opportunity to
measure as the actual use of condoms, but especially given see the insulting person again so that they can return the
the fact that this measure was consistent with self-report favor. Neither of these behaviors reflects liking, and conse-
measures, it seems like a reasonable proxy. quently, may produce results different from those produced
A second reason behavioral measures are sometimes by the questionnaire measure.
avoided has to do with our earlier distinction between Fourth, some psychological states are best measured by
problem-oriented and process-oriented research. If the self-report instruments—that is, by asking people—than
research is problem-oriented, then the dependent mea- by observing their behavior. In recent years, for example,
sures should correspond as closely to that phenomenon there has been a considerable amount of interest in human
(e.g., prejudice, consumer behavior, condom use) as happiness, such as what causes it, how well people can
possible. If it is process-oriented, however, the goal is predict it, and whether it can be changed (e.g., Diener &
to understand the mediating processes responsible for Biswas-Diener, 2008; Gilbert, 2006; Wilson & Gilbert,
a phenomenon, and the dependent measures are often 2003). Researchers have conducted a great deal of psycho-
designed to tap these processes, not the phenomena they metric work on how best to measure how happy people
produce. For example, to understand when people will are, and it turns out that the most valid and reliable way is
act in a prejudiced manner toward a member of a social to ask them (Andrews & Robinson, 1991; Diener, 1994;
group, it is important to know when their stereotype of Fordyce, 1988). Thus, in some cases self-report instru-
that group is activated. As we saw earlier, Gilbert and ments are the best measure of the phenomenon researchers
Hixon (1991) addressed this question by showing peo- are trying to assess.
ple a videotape of a woman holding up cards with word Nonetheless, it is important to note some limitations of
fragments on them and asking people to complete the frag- questionnaire measures. Most fundamentally, people may
ments to make as many words as they could. The main not know the answer to the questions they are asked. This
dependent measure was the number of times people com- is especially true of “why” questions, whereby people are
pleted the fragments with words that were consistent with asked to report the reasons for their behavior and attitudes.
Caucasians’ stereotypes of Asians to see if this differed Rather than reporting accurately, people might be rely-
according to whether the woman on the tape was Asian ing on cultural or idiosyncratic theories about the causes
and whether people were under cognitive load. Note that of their responses that are not always correct (Nisbett &
the researchers never measured people’s behavior toward Wilson, 1977; Wilson, 2002).
Asians—participants never interacted with anyone except
the experimenter. How, then, can this be an experiment on Disguising the Measure Even if people know the answer
stereotyping and prejudice? It is by studying some of the to a question, they may not answer truthfully. As previ-
psychological processes (stereotype activation) hypoth- ously mentioned, people might distort their responses due
esized to mediate prejudicial behavior. to self-presentational concerns or because they have fig-
A third reason why nonbehavioral measures are often ured out the hypothesis and want to tell the experiment-
used is that, in many situations, that they can be a more pre- ers what they want to hear. It is thus often important to
cise measure of intervening processes than overt behavior. disguise the fact that a particular collection of data is actu-
Behavior is often complex and multidetermined, making ally the measurement of the dependent variable. This presents
it difficult to know the exact psychological processes that problems very similar to those involved in attempting to dis-
produced it. For example, suppose in an experiment a con- guise the independent variable, as discussed in the earlier
federate (posing as a fellow participant) either praises the section on guarding against demand characteristics. Again,
participant, implying that he or she is brilliant, or insults there are several classes of solutions that can be applied to
the participant, implying that he or she is stupid. Suppose the problem of disguising the dependent variable.
our dependent variable is how much the participant likes One approach is to measure the dependent variable in a
the confederate. We can measure it by handing participants setting that participants believe is totally removed from the
a rating scale and asking them to rate their liking for the remainder of the experiment. For example, in research on
confederate, from +5 to –5. Or, on a more behavioral level, intrinsic motivation it is common to assess people’s interest

CH02.indd 71 9/2/09 8:10:02 PM


74 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

in an activity by observing how much time they spend minimize such self-presentational concerns, however, as
on that activity during a “free time” period. Participants much as possible. As it happened, Wilson et al.’s predic-
believe that this time period is not part of the experiment tions were confirmed: People who analyzed reasons chose
and do not know that they are being observed. Lepper, different types of posters than people who did not.
Greene, and Nisbett (1973), for instance, measured chil- A similar approach is to tell participants that the depen-
dren’s interest in a set of felt-tip pens by unobtrusively dent variable is part of a different study than the one in
observing how much time they spent playing with the pens which the independent variable was administered. As men-
during a free-play period of their preschool class. tioned earlier the “multiple study” cover story can be used,
Another example of how the dependent measure can in which participants think they are taking part in separate
be disguised comes from the Wilson et al. (1993) study studies (e.g., Higgins et al., 1977).
mentioned earlier, in which people either analyzed why If the independent and dependent variables are included
they liked some posters or did not. One hypothesis of this in the same study, steps are often taken to disguise the
study was that people who analyzed reasons would change purpose of the dependent measure. For example, there is
their minds about which posters they preferred the most, a family of techniques for measuring a dependent variable
and would thus choose different types of posters to take that is parallel to the “whoops” procedure for manipulat-
home than people in the control condition. To test this ing an independent variable. The most common member
hypothesis the experimenter told people, at the end of the of this family involves claiming that the pretest data were
study, that as a reward for their participation, they could lost so that a second set of measures must be collected. In
choose one poster to take home. Asking people to make attitude-change experiments, the most typical solution is to
their choice in front of the experimenter would have been embed the key items in a lengthy questionnaire that is
problematic, because self-presentational biases might have given to the participant. One may have some qualms about
come into play, whereby people chose a poster on the basis the extent to which this always disguises the measurement
of how this made them look to the experimenter, rather from the participant, yet it has been used effectively in
than on the basis of which one they really liked the best some instances.
(Baumeister, 1982; DePaulo, 1992; Schlenker, 1980). The
posters were of different types; some were reproductions Dependent Measures that are Uncontrollable All of the
of classic paintings, whereas others were more contempo- above ways of disguising the dependent measure make the
rary, humorous posters. Participants might have thought, assumption that if people knew what was being measured,
“I would prefer one of the humorous posters but this might they might alter their responses. The prototypical example of
make me look shallow and inane, so I will go ahead and such a measure is the questionnaire response; if people are
take the one by Monet.” asked on a 7-point scale whether they would help someone
To minimize self-presentational biases, Wilson et al. in an emergency, they might indicate how they would like to
took the following steps to make the choice of poster as respond, or how they think they should respond, instead of
private as possible: After telling the participant that she according to how they really would respond. There is another
could choose a poster to take home, the experimenter said way of avoiding this problem: Use dependent measures that
that she had to go get the explanation sheet describing the by their very nature are uncontrollable, such that people could
purpose of the study. She told the participant to pick out not alter their responses even if they wanted to—obviating
a poster from bins that contained several copies of each the need to disguise the measure. Controllability is a matter
poster, and then left the room. The participant expected the of degree, of course; it is more difficult to control one’s heart
experimenter to return shortly, and thus may still have been rate than one’s response on a 7-point scale, but even heart
concerned that the experimenter would see which poster rate can be controlled to some degree (e.g., by holding one’s
she chose. To minimize such a concern, the researchers breath). Social psychologists have broadened their arsenal
placed multiple copies of each poster in each bin. Further, of dependent measures considerably in recent years, and for
all the posters were rolled up so that only the reverse, blank present purposes it is interesting to note that many of these
side was showing, making it impossible (in the minds of measures are more difficult for people to control than ques-
the participants) for the experimenter to tell which poster tionnaire responses, and less susceptible to demand charac-
she had chosen. (After the participant had left, the experi- teristics or self-presentational concerns. Examples include
menter was able to tell which poster people chose by counting measures of physiological and neurological responses, as
the number left in each bin.) It is possible that despite these well as the virtual explosion of measures of automatic cog-
rather elaborate precautions, some participants were still nitive and affective responses (e.g. Bargh, 1990; de Houwer,
motivated to choose posters that would make them look in press; Gilbert, 1991; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Nosek,
good rather than ones they really liked. It is important to Greenwald, & Banaji, 2007; Wegner, 1994).

CH02.indd 72 9/2/09 8:10:03 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 75

An obvious advantage of such measures is that they are with imaging studies. Critics point out that many of the
difficult for people to control. Another is that they may tap published results could have been obtained with more tra-
psychological constructs that are distinct from what peo- ditional dependent measures and could have been obtained
ple are able to report. We do not have the space here to for a mere fraction of the cost. We have no doubt that much
review the vast literatures on implicit measures and uncon- will be learned from social neuroscience, particularly as new
scious mental processes (see the chapters in this volume technologies are developed (e.g., that permit brain scans dur-
by Banaji and Dijksterhuis). Suffice it to say that this is an ing social interactions). But we hope that researchers and
exciting time for social psychological theory and methods; funding agencies will conduct cost-benefit analyses of the
new frontiers are opening as researchers develop new tools value of such studies, and not lose sight of the advantages
and methods to tap psychological processes. of experimental studies that “merely” include self-report and
One of these frontiers is social neuroscience, whereby behavioral measures.
researchers measure the neural correlates of social psy-
The Postexperimental Follow-up
chological processes, typically using functional Magnetic
Resonance Imaging (fMRI) to measure blood flow in The experiment does not end when the data have been col-
the brain and electroencephalography (EEG) to measure lected. Rather, the prudent experimenter will want to remain
event-related potentials (ERP). These measures can be with the participants to talk and listen in order to accomplish
quite useful in identifying the regions of the brain that are three important goals: (a) to ensure that the participants are
active during a particular task, and thus to infer some of in a good and healthy frame of mind; (b) to be certain that
the underlying social psychological processes that drive the participants understand the experimental procedures, the
a given behavior. For example, Greene et al. (2001; hypotheses, and their own performance so that they gain a
2004) demonstrated that different sorts of moral dilem- valuable educational experience as a result of having par-
mas activate different regions of the brain, suggesting a ticipated; (c) to avail themselves of the participant’s unique
dual process model of moral reasoning. They found that skill as a valuable consultant in the research enterprise; that
“impersonal moral dilemmas” elicited utilitarian responses is, only the participants know for certain whether the instruc-
(judging personal moral violations to be acceptable when tions were clear, whether the independent variable had the
they serve a greater good), which were associated with brain intended impact on them, and so on; (d) to probe for any
activation patterns involving the abstract reasoning centers suspicion on the part of the participants, such as whether
of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. By contrast, “personal they believed the cover story.
moral dilemmas” elicited quite different responses that It is impossible to overstate the importance of the post-
focused on fairness and appropriateness rather than “great- experimental follow-up. The experimenter should never
est good” considerations. These latter cases generally drew conduct it in a casual or cavalier manner. Rather, the exper-
quick decisions and involved heightened brain activity in imenter should probe gently and sensitively to be certain
the emotion and social cognition areas (specifically, the that all of the above goals are accomplished. This is espe-
medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate/precuneus, cially and most obviously true if any deception has been
and superior temporal sulcus/temporoparietal junction). employed. In this case, the experimenter needs to learn if
As exciting as some of these advances have been, the deception was effective or if the participant was suspi-
we would be remiss not to point out that the value of cious in a way that could invalidate the data based on his
brain-imaging studies to date are incremental rather than or her performance in the experiment. Even more impor-
revolutionary. Indeed, many of the technical requirements of tant, where deception was used, the experimenter must
current fMRI and ERP studies directly contradict our advice reveal the true nature of the experiment and the reasons
on previous pages. For example, participants in fMRI stud- why deception was necessary. Again, this cannot be done
ies are necessarily alone and confined within a rather large lightly. People do not enjoy learning that they have behaved
magnet. They cannot have meaningful and authentic social in a naive or gullible manner. The experimenter not only
interactions, and are often acutely aware of the psychologi- must be sensitive to the feelings and dignity of the partici-
cal variables that are being measured. Current technology pants but also should communicate this care and concern
limits these studies to judgment-type experiments, and typi- to them. We have found that people are most receptive to
cally they rely on wholly within-participant designs. At their experimenters who are open in describing their own dis-
best these studies can clarify the region of the brain involved comfort with the deceptive aspects of the procedure. Then,
in judgments; at their worst they merely identify the neural in explaining why the deception was necessary, the experi-
correlates of behavior without yielding new psychologi- menter not only is sharing his or her dilemma as an ear-
cal insight regarding the behavior of humans. This debate nest researcher (who is seeking the truth through the use
is exacerbated by the tremendous fiscal cost associated of deception) but also is contributing to the participants’

CH02.indd 73 9/2/09 8:10:04 PM


76 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

educational experience by exploring the process as well as discovered whether the participants misunderstood the
the content of social psychological experimentation. instructions or whether any responded erroneously. If no
Although it is important to provide people with a com- suspicions have been voiced, the experimenter should
plete understanding of the experimental procedures, this is continue: “Do you think there may have been more to the
not the best way to begin the postexperimental session. In experiment than meets the eye?” This question is virtually
order to maximize the value of the participants as consul- a giveaway. Even if the participants had not previously sus-
tants, it is first necessary to explore with each the impact of pected anything, some will probably begin to suspect that the
the experimental events. The value of this sequence should experimenter was concealing something. In our experience,
be obvious. If we tell the participants what we expected to we have found that many people will take this opportunity
happen before finding out what the participants experienced, to say that they did feel that the experiment, as described,
they may have a tendency to protect us from the realization appeared too simple (or something of that order). This is
that our procedures were pallid, misguided, or worthless. desirable; whether the participants were deeply suspicious
Moreover, if deception was used, the experimenter, before or not, the question allows them an opportunity to indicate
revealing the deception, should ascertain whether the par- that they are not the kind of person who is easily fooled. The
ticipant was suspicious and whether particular suspicions experimenter should then explore the nature of the suspi-
were of such a nature as to invalidate the results. cion and how it may have affected the participant’s behavior.
This should not be done abruptly. It is best to explore From the participant’s answers to this question, the experi-
the feelings and experiences of the participants in a gentle menter can make a judgment as to how close a participant’s
and gradual manner. Why the need for gradualness? Why suspicions were to the actual purpose of the experiment and,
not simply ask people if they suspected that they were the consequently, whether the data are admissible. Obviously,
victims of a hoax? Subjects may not be responsive to an the criteria for inclusion should be both rigorous and rigid
abrupt procedure for a variety of reasons. First, if a given and should be set down before the experiment begins; the
person did see through the experiment, he or she may be decision should be made without knowledge of the partici-
reluctant to admit it out of a misplaced desire to be help- pants’ responses on the dependent variable.
ful to the experimenter. Second, as mentioned previously, The experimenter should then continue with the debrief-
since most of us do not feel good about appearing gull- ing process by saying something like this: “You are on the
ible, some participants may be reluctant to admit that right track, we were interested in exploring some issues
they can be easily fooled. Consequently, if participants that we didn’t discuss with you in advance. One of our
are told pointedly about the deception, they might imply major concerns in this study is…” The experimenter should
that they suspected it all along, in order to save face. Thus, then describe the problem under investigation, specifying
such an abrupt procedure may falsely inflate the number why it is important and explaining clearly exactly how the
of suspicious participants and may, consequently, lead the deception took place and why it was necessary. Again,
experimenter to abandon a perfectly viable procedure. experimenters should be generous in sharing their own dis-
Moreover, as mentioned previously, abruptly telling peo- comfort with the participant. They should make absolutely
ple that they have been deceived is a harsh technique that certain that the participant fully understands these factors
can add unnecessarily to their discomfort and, therefore, before the postexperimental session is terminated.
should be avoided. It is often useful to enlist the participant’s aid in
The best way to begin a postexperimental interview is improving the experiment. Often the participant can pro-
to ask the participants if they have any questions. If they do vide valuable hints regarding where the weaknesses in
not, the experimenter should ask if the entire experiment the manipulation occurred and which one of these caused
was perfectly clear—the purpose of the experiment as well competing reactions to the one the experimenter intended.
as each aspect of the procedure. The participants should These interviews can and should, of course, be continued
then be told that people react to things in different ways, during the time the experiment is actually being run, but it
and it would be helpful if they would comment on how the is usually during pretesting that the most valuable informa-
experiment affected them, why they responded as they did, tion is obtained.
and how they felt at the time, for example. Then each par- Finally, regardless of whether deception is used, the
ticipant should be asked specifically whether there was any experimenter must attempt to convince the participants
aspect of the procedure that he or she found odd, confusing, not to discuss the experiment with other people until it is
or disturbing. completed. This is a serious problem because even a few
By this time, if deception has been used and any partici- sophisticated participants can invalidate an experiment.
pants have any suspicions, they are almost certain to have Moreover, it is not a simple matter to swear participants to
revealed them. Moreover, the experimenter should have secrecy; some have friends who may subsequently volunteer

CH02.indd 74 9/2/09 8:10:04 PM


Planning and Conducting a Laboratory Experiment 77

for the experiment and who are almost certain to press their right to privacy. We will not dwell on the historical
them for information. Perhaps the best way to reduce antecedents of these values or on the philosophical intrica-
such communication is to describe graphically the colos- cies of the ethical dilemma posed by the conflict of these
sal waste of effort that would result from experimenting values. It suffices to say that the dilemma is a real one and
with people who have foreknowledge about the procedure cannot be dismissed either by making pious statements
or hypothesis of the experiment and who thus can rehearse about the importance of not violating a person’s feelings
their responses in advance. The experimenter should also of dignity or by glibly pledging allegiance to the cause of
explain the damage that can be done to the scientific enter- science. It is a problem every social psychologist must face
prise by including data from such participants. It often squarely, not just once, but each time he or she constructs
helps to provide participants with some easy but unreveal- and conducts an experiment, since it is impossible to delin-
ing answers for their friends who ask about the study (e.g., eate a specific set of rules and regulations governing all
“it was about social perception”). If we experimenters are experiments. In each instance the researcher must decide
sincere and honest in our dealings with the participants on a course of action after giving careful consideration
during the postexperimental session, we can be reasonably to the importance of the experiment and the extent of the
confident that few will break faith. potential injury to the dignity of the participants.
To check on the efficacy of this procedure, Aronson (1966) Obviously, some experimental techniques present more
enlisted the aid of three undergraduates who each approached problems than others. In general, experiments that employ
three acquaintances who had recently participated in one of deception cause concern because of the fact that lying, in
his experiments. The confederates explained that they had and of itself, is problematic. Similarly, procedures that
signed up for that experiment, had noticed the friend’s name cause pain, embarrassment, guilt, or other intense feel-
on the sign-up sheet, and wondered what the experiment was ings present obvious ethical problems. In addition, any
all about. The experimenter had previously assured these procedure that enables the participants to confront some
confederates that their friends would remain anonymous. aspect of themselves that may not be pleasant or positive
The results were encouraging. In spite of considerable urging is of deep ethical concern. For example, many of Asch’s
and cajoling on the part of the confederates, none of the for- (1951) participants learned that they could conform in
mer participants revealed the true purpose of the experiment; the face of implicit group pressure; many of Aronson
two of them went as far as providing the confederates with a and Mettee’s (1968) participants learned that they would
replay of the cover story, but nothing else. cheat at a game of cards; and many of Milgram’s (1974)
What if the participant has been forewarned before participants learned that they could be pressured to obey an
entering the experimental room? That is, suppose a partici- authority even when such obedience involved (apparently)
pant does find out about the experiment from a friend who inflicting severe pain on another human being. Even more
participated previously. Chances are the participant will imposing are the findings of the Stanford prison study in
not volunteer this information to the experimenter before which college students learned that, even in the absence of
the experiment. Once again, we as experimenters must direct explicit commands, they would behave cruelly and
appeal to the cooperativeness of the participant, emphasiz- even sadistically toward fellow students (Haney, Banks, &
ing how much the experiment will be compromised if peo- Zimbardo, 1973).
ple knew about it in advance. We cannot overemphasize It can be argued that such procedures are therapeutic or
the importance of this procedure as a safeguard against the educational for the participants. Indeed, many of the par-
artifactual confirmation of an erroneous hypothesis because ticipants in these experiments have made this point. But
of the misplaced cooperativeness of the participant. If the this does not, in and of itself, justify the procedure primar-
participants are indeed cooperative, they will undoubtedly ily because the experimenter could not possibly know in
cooperate with the experimenter in this regard also and will advance that it would be therapeutic for all participants.
respond to a direct plea of the sort described. Moreover, it is arrogant for the scientist to decide that he
or she will provide people with a therapeutic experience
Ethical Concerns in Experimentation without their explicit permission.
Experimental social psychologists have been deeply con- The use of deception, when combined with the possi-
cerned about the ethics of experimentation for a great bility of “self-discovery,” presents the experimenter with
many years precisely because our field is constructed on a special kind of ethical problem. In a deception experi-
an ethical dilemma. Basically, the dilemma is formed by ment it is impossible, by definition, to attain informed
a conflict between two sets of values to which most social consent from the participants in advance of the experiment.
psychologists subscribe: a belief in the value of free sci- For example, how could Milgram or Asch have attained
entific inquiry and a belief in the dignity of humans and informed consent from their participants without revealing

CH02.indd 75 9/2/09 8:10:05 PM


78 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

aspects of the procedure that would have invalidated any from a series of experiments on social dilemmas by Dawes
results they obtained? An experimenter cannot even reveal and his students (Dawes, McTavish, & Shaklee, 1977). In
in advance that the purpose of an experiment is the study these experiments, typically, the participant must make a
of conformity or obedience without influencing the partici- decision between cooperating with several other people or
pant to behave in ways that are no longer “pure.” Moreover, “defecting.” The contingencies are such that if all partici-
we doubt that the experimenter can reveal that deception pants choose to cooperate, they all profit financially; how-
might be used without triggering vigilance and, therefore, ever, if one or more defect, defection has a high payoff and
adulterating the participant’s response to the independent cooperation produces little payoff. Each person’s response is
variable. anonymous and remains so. The nature of the decision and
A number of guidelines have been developed to protect its consequences is fully explained to the participants at the
the welfare of research participants. In 1973 the American outset of the experiment. No deception is involved.
Psychological Association (APA) published a set of guide- Twenty-four hours after one experimental session, an
lines for the conduct of research involving human par- elderly man (who had been the sole defector in his group
ticipants, which have since been revised and updated a and had won $19) telephoned the experimenter trying to
number of times. It behooves all investigators to read these return his winnings so that it could be divided among the
guidelines carefully before conducting research (American other participants (who, because they chose to cooperate,
Psychological Association, 2002). Further, as stated in the had each earned only $1). In the course of the conversation,
guidelines, ethical decisions should not be made alone. he revealed that he felt miserable about his greedy behavior
Researchers may not always be in the best position to and that he had not slept all night. After a similar experi-
judge whether their procedures are ethically permissible. ment, a woman who had cooperated while others defected
Because of this fact, all research using human subjects revealed that she felt terribly gullible and had learned that
that is funded by the federal government, or conducted at people were not as trustworthy as she had thought. In order
colleges and universities, must receive approval from an to alleviate this kind of stress, Dawes went on to develop
Institutional Review Board (IRB). This is a panel of sci- an elaborate and sensitive follow-up procedure.
entists and nonscientists who judge whether the risks to We repeat that these experiments were selected for
participants outweigh the potential gains of the research. discussion precisely because their important and powerful
It is not uncommon for an IRB to ask researchers to revise impact could not have been easily anticipated. We are
their procedures to minimize risks to participants. intentionally not focusing on experiments that present
It is worth noting that there have been some empirical clear and obvious problems like the well-known obedi-
investigations of the impact of deception experiments on ence study (Milgram, 1974), or the Stanford prison study
participants. These studies have generally found that people (Haney et al., 1973). We have purposely selected an exper-
do not object to the kinds of mild discomfort and decep- iment that involves no deception and is well within the
tions typically used in social psychological research (e.g., bounds of ethical codes. Our point is simple but important.
Christensen, 1988; Sharpe, Adair, & Roese, 1992; Smith No code of ethics can anticipate all problems, especially
& Richardson, 1983). If mild deception is used, and time is those created through participants discovering something
spent after the study discussing the deception with partici- unpleasant about themselves or others in the course of
pants and explaining why it was necessary, the evidence is an experiment. However, we believe a sensitive postex-
that people will not be adversely affected. Nonetheless, the perimental interview conducted by a sincere and caring
decision as to whether to use deception in a study should not experimenter not only instructs and informs, but also
be taken lightly, and alternative procedures should always be provides important insights and helps reduce feelings of
considered. guilt or discomfort generated by such self-discovery (see
As we noted in our discussion of the postexperimental Holmes, 1976a, 1976b; Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975).
interview, it is critical to explain to participants in a deception
study, at its conclusion, the true nature of the procedures and
the reasons for them. We strongly recommend, however, that CONCLUDING COMMENTS
a thorough explanation of the experiment be provided regard-
less of whether deception or stressful procedures are involved. We hope that this chapter has helped explain why labora-
The major reason for this recommendation is that we cannot tory experiments are often the method of choice for social
always predict the impact of a procedure; occasionally, even psychologists and has provided useful tips about how to
procedures that appear to be completely benign can have a conduct experiments. We want to emphasize, however,
powerful impact on some participants. An interesting exam- that social psychology cannot live by lab experimentation
ple of such an unexpectedly powerful negative impact comes alone and that we must use multiple methods if we are to

CH02.indd 76 9/2/09 8:10:05 PM


Concluding Comments 79

advance theory and find solutions to important problems. his colleagues were able to enhance their understanding
Basic, process-oriented experimental research may isolate of the underlying dynamic of this cooperative behavior by
important causal processes, but convincing demonstrations closer scrutiny under controlled laboratory conditions. For
that those processes operate in applied settings are essen- example, in one such laboratory experiment, they showed
tial before theory can be converted into practice. that, in a competitive situation, individuals make situational
The research literature on self-affirmation and stereotype self-attributions for failure and dispositional self-attributions
threat provides a particularly good example of how a syn- for success, while making the reverse attributions to their
thesis between field and laboratory experiments can work opponent. However, in a cooperative structure, individuals
at its best. Research in these areas began with laboratory gave their partners the same benefit of the doubt that they
experiments conducted with college student participants, gave to themselves, that is, dispositional attributions for suc-
showing that (a) people can deal with threats in one domain cess and situational attributions for failure (Stephan, Presser,
by affirming themselves in another (Steele, 1988), and that Kennedy, & Aronson, 1978).
(b) targets of prejudice perform poorly under conditions of Field experimentation in applied settings often provides
stereotype threat, in which they are concerned that their per- an opportunity for impact and involvement of research partic-
formance will confirm a negative stereotype of their group ipants that vastly exceeds any ever achieved in the laboratory.
(Steele & Aronson, 1995). Based on these ideas, Cohen, However, the focus of such research also tends to be more
Garcia, Apfel, and Master (2006) developed an intervention limited than the general tests of theory underlying most labo-
to improve the academic performance of African American ratory research efforts, because they are forced to deal only
middle school students. Some students were randomly with variables found in the particular applied context under
assigned to a self-affirmation condition in which they chose study. If the distinctive contribution of experimental social
values that were important to them and wrote about these psychology to the general body of knowledge is ever to be
values for 15 minutes. Students in the control condition realized, an optimal integration of theory-oriented laboratory
wrote about why the values might be important for some- research with applied field experimentation will be required.
one else. This simple intervention had remarkable effects: At present we are concerned because the alternative
Thoughts about race became less accessible to the students research modes in social psychology seem, for the most
in the self-affirmation condition and the students achieved part, to be functioning in isolation from each other. What
higher grades during the remainder of the academic term. is needed now is a new attempt at synthesis; that is, to con-
We dare say that the idea that such a “miniscule” interven- struct a more limited (and perhaps closer to the original)
tion could have such dramatic effects would never have version of the Lewinian model of the interplay between
occurred to researchers without the prior laboratory experi- laboratory and field research. Such a synthesis will require
ments on self-affirmation and stereotype threat. a concern with discovering more specifiable relationships
Another good example of the creative interplay between rather than with attempts to find sweeping general theories
laboratory and field experimentation is the work of Aronson of human social behavior. It will require an emphasis on
and his colleagues on the effects of cooperative learning assessing the relative importance of several variables, which
(Aronson & Bridgeman, 1979; Aronson & Osherow, 1980; all influence an aspect of multiply-determined behavior,
Aronson, Stephan, Sikes, Blaney, & Snapp, 1978). The rather than on testing to see if a particular variable has a
research began as an experimental intervention in response “significant” impact. And it will require a sensitivity to the
to a crisis in the Austin (Texas) school system following interaction between research design and research setting
its desegregation. Aronson and his colleagues observed and the benefits of multiple methodologies. We have great
the dynamics of the classroom and diagnosed that a major faith in our fellow social psychologists’ ability to meet these
cause of the existing tension was the competitive atmo- challenges. Indeed, many are already deeply immersed in
sphere that exacerbated the usual problems brought about research programs that are increasing our understanding of
by desegregation. They then changed the atmosphere of basic social psychological processes and having an impact
existing classrooms by restructuring the learning environ- on real world problems. We hope this chapter inspires a
ment so that some students were teaching one another in new generation of social psychologists to do the same.
small, interdependent “jigsaw” groups, while others con-
tinued to study in more traditional classrooms.
The results of this and subsequent field experiments
REFERENCES
showed that the cooperative classroom atmosphere decreased
negative stereotyping, increased cross-ethnic liking, increased
American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of psy-
self-esteem, improved classroom performance, and increa- chologists and code of conduct. Retrieved July 30, 2009 from: http://
sed empathic role taking. At the same time, Aronson and www.apa.org/ethics/.

CH02.indd 77 9/2/09 8:10:06 PM


80 The Art of Laboratory Experimentation

Andrews, F. M., & Robinson, J. P. (1991). Measures of subjective well- Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna,
being. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario
Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (Vol. 1, Symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
pp. 61–114). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Christensen, L. (1988). Deception in psychological research: When is
Aronson, E. (1966). Avoidance of inter-subject communication. its use justified? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14,
Psychological Reports, 19, 238. 664–675.
Aronson, E., & Bridgeman, D. (1979). Jigsaw groups and the desegre- Cohen, D. (1977). Psychologists on psychology. New York: Taplinger.
gated classroom: in pursuit of common goals. Personality and Social
Cohen, G., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial
Psychology Bulletin, 5, 438–446.
achievement gap: a social-psychological intervention. Science, 313,
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1963). Effect of the severity of threat on 1307–1310.
the devaluation of forbidden behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experiments: design and
Psychology, 66, 583–588.
analysis issues for field settings. Shokie, IL: Rand McNally.
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1968). Experimentation in social psychol-
ogy. In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social Dawes, R. B., McTavish, J., & Shaklee, H. (1977). Behavior, communication,
psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 1–79). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. and assumptions about other people’s behavior in a common dilemmas
situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 1–11.
Aronson, E., Fried, C. B., & Stone, J. (1991). Overcoming denial and in-
creasing the intention to use condoms through the induction of hypo- de la Haye, A. (1991). Problems and procedures: A typology of paradigms
crisy. American Journal of Public Health, 81, 1636 – 1637. in interpersonal cognition. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 11,
279–304.
Aronson, E., & Mettee, D. (1968). Dishonest behavior as a function of
differential levels of induced self-esteem. Journal of Personality and de Houwer, J. (in press). What are implicit measures and why are we using
Social Psychology, 9, 121–127. them? In R. Wiers & A. Stacy (Eds.), Handbook of implicit cognition
Aronson, E., & Mills, J. (1959). The effect of severity of initiation on liking for and addiction. New York: Sage.
a group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 177–181. DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation.
Aronson, E., & Osherow, N. (1980). Cooperation, prosocial behavior, and Psychological Bulletin, 111, 203–243.
academic performance: experiments in the desegregated classroom. DePaulo, B. M., Lanier, K., & Davis, T. (1983). Detecting the deceit of
Applied Social Psychology Annual, 1, 163–196. the motivated liar. JourÍnal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,
Aronson, E., Stephan, C., Sikes, J., Blaney, N., & Snapp, M. (1978). The 1096–1103.
jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportu-
Aronson, E., Willerman, B., & Floyd, J. (1966). The effect of a pratfall on nities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157.
increasing interpersonal attractiveness. Psychonomic Science, 4, 227 Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). The science of optimal happiness.
– 228. Boston: Blackwell Publishing.
Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. M. (1994). Social psychology: The Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior:
heart and the mind. New York: HarperCollins. The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
Asch, S. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and dis- Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109).
tortion of judgment. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership, and San Diego: Academic Press.
men (pp. 177 – 190). Pittsburgh: Carnegie Press.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA:
Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic Stanford University Press.
influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman & J. A.
Fordyce, M. W. (1988). A review of research on the happiness measures:
Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 3 – 51). New York: Guilford.
A sixty second index of happiness and mental health. Social Indicators
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto - motives: Preconscious determinants of social Research, 20, 355–381.
interaction. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of
Gerard, H. B., & Mathewson, G. C. (1966). The effects of severity of ini-
motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 93 – 130). New York: Guilford.
tiation on liking for a group: A replication. Journal of Experimental
Baumeister, R. F. (1982). A self-presentational view of social phenomena. Social Psychology, 2, 278–287.
Psychological Bulletin, 91, 3–26.
Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist,
Baumeister, R., Vohs, K., & Funder, D. (2007). Psychology as the science 46, 107–119.
of self - reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to actual
behavior?. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 396 – 403. Gilbert, D. T. (2006). Stumbling on happiness. New York; Knopf.
Blascovich, J., Loomis, J., Beall, A., Swinth, K., Hoyt, C., & Bailenson, Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation
J. (2002). Immersive virtual environment technology as a research tool and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and
for social psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 103 – 125. Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D.
T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral
1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. judgment. Neuron, 44, 389–400.
Campbell, D. T. (1957). Factors relevant to validity of experiments in social Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen,
settings. Psychology Bulletin, 54, 297–312. J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi - judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108.
experimental designs for research. In N. L. Gage (Ed.), Handbook of Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes,
research on teaching (pp. 171 – 246). Chicago: Rand McNally. self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.
Carlsmith, K. M., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T (2008). The paradoxical Haney, C., Banks, C., & Zimbardo, P. (1973). Interpersonal dynamics in a
consequences of revenge. Journal of Personality and Social Psycho- simulated prison. International Journal of Criminology and Penology,
logy, 95, 1316 – 1324. 1, 69–97.

CH02.indd 78 9/2/09 8:10:06 PM


References 81

Harackiewicz, J. M., Manderlink, G., & Sansone, C. (1984). Rewarding Sharpe, D., Adair, J. G., & Roese, N. J. (1992). Twenty years of decep-
pinball wizardry: Effects of evaluation and cue value on intrinsic inter- tion research: A decline in subjects’ trust? Personality and Social
est. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 287–300. Psychology Bulletin, 18, 585–590.
Harlow, H. F., & Zimmerman, R. R. (1958). The development of affec- Sigall, H., Aronson, E., & Van Hoose, T. (1970). The cooperative subject
tional responses in infant monkeys. Proceedings of the American myth or reality? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 1–10.
Philosophic Society, 102, 501–509.
Smith, S. S., & Richardson, D. (1983). Amelioration of deception and harm
Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility in psychological research: The important role of debriefing. Journal of
and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1075–1082.
13, 141–154.
Steele, C. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity
Holmes, D. S. (1976a). Debriefing after psychological experiments: I. of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy-
Effectiveness of postdeception dehoaxing. American Psychologist, 31, chology: Vol. 21. Social psychological studies of the self: Perspectives
858–867. and programs (pp. 261–302). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Holmes, D. S. (1976b). Debriefing after psychological experiments: II. Steele, C., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test
Effectiveness of postexperimental desensitizing. American performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychologist, 31, 868–875. Psychology, 69, 797–811.
Langer, E. J. (1989). Minding matters: The consequences of mindlessness-
Stephan, C., Presser, N. R., Kennedy, J. C., & Aronson, E. (1978).
mindfulness. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
Attributions to success and failure after cooperative or competitive
psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 137–174). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
interaction. European Journal of Social Psychology, 8, 269–274.
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s
Uleman, J. S. (1989). A framework for thinking intentionally about unin-
intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the overjustification
tended thoughts. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended
hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.
thought (pp. 425–449). New York: Guilford.
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view.
New York: Harper & Row. Walster, E., Aronson, E., & Abrahams, D. (1966). On increasing the per-
suasiveness of a low prestige communicator. Journal of Experimental
Mook, D. G. (1983). In defense of external invalidity. American
Social Psychology, 2, 325–342.
Psychologist, 38, 379–388.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
Review 101, 34–52.
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The Implicit Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship between
Association Test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review verbal and overt behavioral responses to attitude objects. Journal of
(pp. 265–292). In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Automatic processes in social Social Issues, 25, 41–78.
thinking and behavior. New York: Psychology Press. Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). CyberOstracism:
Orne, M. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experi- Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and
ment. American Psychologist, 17, 776–783. Social Psychology, 79, 748–762.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persua- Williams, K. D., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). Social ostracism by one’s
sion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: coworkers: Does rejection lead to loafing or compensation? Personality
Springer-Verlag. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 693–706.
Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive
as a determinant of argument-based persuasion. Journal of Personality unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
and Social Psychology, 41, 847–855. Wilson, T. D., Dunn, D. S., Kraft, D., & Lisle, D. J. (1989). Introspection,
Reis, H. T. (1982). An introduction to the use of structural equations: attitude change, and attitude-behavior consistency: The disruptive
Prospects and problems. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality effects of explaining why we feel the way we do. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
and social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 255–287). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205).
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Interpersonal expectancy effects: A 30-year perspec- Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
tive. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 176–179. Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. P.
Rosenthal, R., & Lawson, R. (1964). A longitudinal study of the effects of Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp.
experimenter bias on the operant learning of laboratory rats. Journal of 345–411). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Psychiatric Research, 2, 61–72. Wilson, T. D., Hodges, S. D., & LaFleur, S. J. (1995). Effects of introspect-
Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self percep- ing about reasons: Inferring attitudes from accessible thoughts. Journal
tion and social perception: Biased attributional processes in the debriefing of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 16–28.
paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 880–892. Wilson, T. D., & Lassiter, D. (1982). Increasing intrinsic interest with the
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation: experimental studies of use of superfluous extrinsic constraints. Journal of Personality and
the sources of gregariousness. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Social Psychology, 42, 811–819.
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social and physiological Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D., Schooler, J., Hodges, S. D., Klaaren, K. J., &
determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399. LaFleur, S. J. (1993). Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice
Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management. Monterey, CA: satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 331–339.
Brooks-Cole. Zanna, M. P., & Fazio, R. H. (1982). The attitude-behavior relation: Moving
Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a toward a third generation of research. In M. P. Zanna, E. T. Higgins,
narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Consistency in social behavior: The Ontario
of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530. Symposium (Vol. 2, pp. 283–301). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

CH02.indd 79 9/2/09 8:10:07 PM


Chapter 3

Social Psychological Methods Outside the


Laboratory
HARRY T. REIS AND SAMUEL D. GOSLING

When Kurt Lewin ushered in the modern era of experimen- influences and differentiate causal mechanisms from one
tal social psychology, he did so with the strong belief that another (Smith, 2000), and easy access to undergraduate
the scientific psychology of the time seemed to be trying samples. These advantages were a great part of the reason
“increasingly to stay away from a too close relation to life” why social psychology, which had been more non-experi-
(1951, p. 169). Lewin primarily intended to keep experi- mental than experimental in its early days, evolved into
mental social psychology close to life by urging researchers a predominantly experimental science during the 1930s
to maintain an active interest in applications of theory to and 1940s (House, 1977; Jones, 1985), a considerable and
social problems, but he also felt that, beyond research with enduring legacy.
experimentally created laboratory groups, the field But these advantages may also have a cost, in terms of
increasing distance from Lewin’s “close relation to life.”
shall have also to develop research techniques that will per-
Laboratory settings by definition remove research par-
mit us to do real experiments within existing “natural” social
groups. In my opinion, the practical and theoretical impor-
ticipants from their natural contexts and place them in an
tance of these types of experiments is of the first magnitude. artificial environment in which nearly all aspects of the
(1951, pp. 164–165) setting, including physical features, goals, other persons
involved, and even the possibility of getting up and doing
By this Lewin meant that social psychological research something else, are determined by an external entity (i.e.,
needed to keep its theoretical feet firmly grounded in real- the experimenter). Natural habitats, in contrast, are marked
world contexts, problems, and social relations. by far greater diversity and clutter of the physical and
In the more than half-century of research and theoriz- social environment, the necessity of choosing for oneself
ing that followed, social psychology’s remarkable progress what task to pursue and how to engage it, and the option
has derived in large measure from laboratory research. For of changing settings and tasks. Ironically, social-psy-
example, Sears (1986) reported that 78% of the social- chological research has provided ample testimony of the
psychological research published in 1985 in the field’s top importance of context for understanding behavior.
journals was conducted in the laboratory. Rozin (2001) The good news is that social psychology can have it
similarly concluded that nearly all of the articles published both ways. As is discussed below, researchers have come
in the first two sections of volume 66 (1994) of the Journal to realize that validity is not an “either-or” proposition
of Personality and Social Psychology (JPSP) were situ- but rather the result of complementary methods targeting
ated in the laboratory or used questionnaires. No doubt this the same theories, processes, and concepts. Just as social
emphasis reflects the many benefits of laboratory (typi- psychologists have used stagecraft to import some of the
cally, although not exclusively experimental) research, richness of natural settings into the laboratory, recent meth-
including experimental control over variables, contexts and odological advances have made possible with non-labora-
procedures, which allows researchers to control extraneous tory methods some of the same precision and control that

For their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this article, we gratefully thank Matthias Mehl, Peter Caprariello, Michael Maniaci,
Shannon Smith, and the editors.
Direct correspondence to Harry T. Reis, Ph.D., Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, Box 270266, University of
Rochester, Rochester, New York 14627; voice: (585) 275-8697; fax: (585) 273-1100; e-mail: reis@psych.rochester.edu.

82
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
What Is Meant By Non-Laboratory Research? 83

heretofore was possible only in the laboratory. Moreover, discussions of these methods are available elsewhere.
many of these advances allow non-laboratory research to Readers interesting in learning more about survey meth-
ask more complex questions or to obtain far more detailed odology may consult the chapter by Schwarz, Groves, and
responses than the typical laboratory experiment. As a Schumann in the fourth edition of this Handbook (1998),
result, non-laboratory methods represent a far more power- Krosnick and Fabrigar (in press), Groves et al. (2004), or
ful tool for social psychological research and theory than Visser, Krosnick, and Lavrakas (2000). Fuller descrip-
they have previously. Adding them to a research program tion of observational methods (which are applied both
may also make the results of research more interesting and in the laboratory and in non-laboratory settings such as
relevant, as Cialdini (2009) suggests. work sites, homes, and schools) may be found in Weick’s
The distinction between laboratory and non-laboratory (1985) chapter in the third edition of this Handbook, or in
research is sometimes conflated with sampling. Although Bakeman (2000), Bakeman and Gottman (1997), Kerig and
undergraduate and non-undergraduate samples are stud- Lindahl (2001), and McGrath and Altermatt (2000). Other
ied in both kinds of settings, in actuality the vast majority of non-laboratory methods used by social psychologists that
laboratory studies rely on undergraduate samples, whereas we do not discuss include archival methods (Simonton,
non-laboratory studies are more likely to use non-student, 2003; Webb, Campbell, Schwartz, & Sechrest, 2000),
adult samples. Eighty-three percent of the studies in Sears’s computer simulations (Hastie & Stasser, 2000), interviews
(1986) review used samples composed of students. (Bartholomew, Henderson, & Marcia, 2000), and partici-
Reviews of the 1988 volume of JPSP, the 1996 volume pant observation in the field.
of the Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, and the
2002 volume of JPSP put these estimates at 80%, 85%,
and 85%, respectively (Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava, & WHAT IS MEANT BY NON-LABORATORY
John, 2004; Sherman, Buddie, Dragan, End, & Finney, RESEARCH?
1999; West, Newsom, & Fenaughty, 1992). Another study
reported that 91.9% of studies of prejudice and stigma pub- We are tempted to define the term non-laboratory research
lished in the field’s top three journals from 1990 to 2005 as all research conducted elsewhere than in a laboratory
relied on undergraduate samples, and even in two expressly suite, room, or cubicle. Laboratories are spaces specially
applied journals (Journal of Applied Social Psychology, equipped for research that permit experimenters to control
Basic and Applied Social Psychology), 73.6% of studies nearly all facets of the participant’s experience, includ-
were based on research with undergraduates (Henry, 2008). ing the physical (e.g., ambient sound and temperature,
Laboratory studies use undergraduate samples because it is furniture, visual cues) and social environment (e.g., other per-
difficult and expensive to recruit nonstudent participants sons), as well as the possibility of distraction by external
to come to the lab. With non-laboratory studies, research- circumstances (e.g., cell phones). Conducting non-labo-
ers usually have little reason to prioritize nonstudent ratory research necessarily involves sacrificing this high
samples. level of control over extraneous factors for the benefits
This chapter reviews some of the more important, discussed below. Researchers often design non-labora-
popular, and timely methods for conducting social psychologi- tory studies to observe social-psychological phenomena in
cal research outside of the laboratory. The chapter begins their natural context, reflecting the belief that the setting in
with a review of the purpose of non-laboratory methods, which a behavior occurs must be a fundamental part of any
emphasizing how they have been used in social psychol- theoretical account of that behavior (Weick, 1985). (This
ogy, as well as the kinds of insights that they can and can- belief is of course entirely consistent with the rationale for
not provide. Included in this section is a review of how laboratory research, because settings would not need to
laboratory and non-laboratory methods complement each be controlled if they were not influential.) In contrast, the
other in a research program. We then describe in some laboratory setting is likely to engender certain expectations
detail five methods that have become influential tools in and scripts (e.g., serious purpose, scientific legitimacy, the
social psychology and give every indication of continued possibility of deception, the importance of attentiveness),
value: field experiments, Internet methods, diary methods, which may affect behavior (Shulman & Berman, 1975).
ambulatory monitoring, and trace measures. The chapter Non-laboratory research also tends to constrain participant
concludes with a brief commentary on the future of non- behavior less, in the sense that the setting offers many more
laboratory methods in social psychology. alternative activities (e.g., participants can choose what to
We do not review two broad and common classes of do, when, where, and with whom) and distractions, so that
non-laboratory methods, survey research and observa- self-direction and spontaneous selection among activities
tional methods, for space reasons and because excellent is greater. In a laboratory study, participants usually can
84 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

do little else but complete the tasks assigned to them by psychology, both historically and in contemporary
researchers. research. A few examples may illustrate this role, as
Laboratory and non-laboratory settings differ in well as highlight the diversity of methods included in
various ways, some of them more influential than others. this general category.
Administering a questionnaire in a classroom versus Field studies (including experiments, quasi-experiments,
a laboratory cubicle may not make much difference, and nonexperimental designs) include the famous Robbers
whereas conducting a field experiment on the effects of Cave research, conducted in 1954, in which observations
affectionate smiles on attraction at a social mixer versus of early adolescent boys attending a summer camp led to
a laboratory room may matter more. Non-laboratory and findings about ingroup cooperation and outgroup competi-
laboratory contexts differ in three general ways: the physi- tion that spawned one of social psychology’s most enduring
cal environment, the goals likely to be activated by the research areas, intergroup conflict (Sherif, Harvey, White,
setting and their correspondence with the behaviors being Hood, & Sherif, 1961). The development of cognitive dis-
studied, and the degree to which the setting is natural and sonance theory was influenced in an important way by
appropriate for the research question. Of these, we see the When Prophecy Fails, a field study in which researchers
latter two factors as more significant for social-psycho- infiltrated a prophetic group of doom-sayers predicting the
logical research. That is, because behavior reflects personal end of the world (Festinger, Riecken, & Schachter, 1956).
goals and concerns, and is embedded in naturally occurring Many important studies of bystander intervention in the
contexts, non-laboratory research can complement labora- 1960s and 1970s took place in natural settings, such as
tory studies best when it highlights such influences. grocery stores, streets, the New York City subways, and
It is important to note that the setting in which a study is Jones Beach (Moriarty, 1975). Important studies examin-
conducted is independent of whether a study is experimen- ing if, when, and how intergroup contact reduces prejudice
tal or non-experimental (see Figure 3.1). Studies conducted and discrimination have been conducted with actual con-
outside of the laboratory can possess all of the features of a flicting groups (Amir, 1969), and real-world classrooms
true experiment—random assignment to conditions, manip- have been used to study the effects of cooperative learning
ulation of the treatment conditions—as in the case of field structures (the so-called Jigsaw Classroom) on intergroup
experiments and randomized clinical trials (Wilson et al., this relations and academic achievement (Aronson, 2004;
Handbook), just as studies conducted in a laboratory space Johnson, Johnson, & Smith, 2007). Some of the earliest
can have an experimental or correlational design. Some stud- studies of self-disclosure processes were conducted with
ies include both laboratory and non-laboratory components, Navy recruits in boot camp training for service on subma-
such as when measurements obtained in the laboratory are rines and other isolated yet intensely interactive settings
used to help explain behaviors observed in non-laboratory (e.g., Altman & Haythorn, 1965). Pioneering studies of
settings. Also, although non-laboratory research may pos- the acquaintance process observed the development (and
sess less of the tight control over setting and procedure that is non-development) of friendships among new students
typically associated with laboratory research, systematic, at Bennington College and the University of Michigan
carefully designed methods are still essential. (Newcomb, 1961). More contemporary examples of field
Their relative infrequency notwithstanding, non-lab- research in social psychology include studies of personal
oratory studies have played an important role in social living and working spaces (Gosling, Ko, Mannarelli, &
Morris, 2004), investigations of attachment processes
within the Israeli military (Davidovitz, Mikulincer, Shaver,
Izsak, & Popper, 2007), and Sherman and Kim’s (2005)
Design studies of self-affirmation among student members of
sports teams. The Internet has also created many new pos-
Experiment Non-Experiment
sibilities for research.
Ambulatory monitoring (including diary methods) has
Lab grown rapidly in recent years, no doubt due to technological
Setting
advances that make such procedures more accessible and cost-
effective while providing better, more detailed information.
Field Among the most popular of these methods are Experience
Sampling (Hektner, Schmidt, & Csikszentmihalyi,
2007) and Ecological Momentary Assessment (Stone &
Shiffman, 1994), both defined later in this chapter, which
Figure 3.1 Designs and Settings Are Orthogonal. have been used extensively to study affect, cognition,
Why Study Social Psychological Processes Outside the Laboratory? 85

health symptoms, health-related behavior, social interac- laboratory settings tend to be preferable for conducting the
tion, and activity in everyday life. Ambulatory assessment most carefully controlled studies, because manipulations
procedures have also been used in social psychological can be crafted to precisely test some theoretical principle,
research to collect random samples of the acoustic envi- controlling for the “noise” of the real world and ruling out
ronment (Pennebaker, Mehl, & Niederhoffer, 2003); to alternative explanations and potential artifacts (even those
obtain detailed reports of physiological states, particularly that may be confounded with the key independent variable
heart rate variability and other cardiovascular measures in natural experience). Nevertheless, the value of non-lab-
(Hawkley, Burleson, Berntson, & Cacioppo, 2003), as they oratory research goes well beyond showing that the same
relate to what the person is doing or experiencing; to char- processes are also evident in the real world. As Brewer
acterize sleep (Ajilore, Stickgold, Rittenhouse, & Hobson, notes, “the kind of systematic, programmatic research that
1995); and to quantify person-to-person proximity for accompanies the search for external validity inevitably
social network analyses (Pentland, 2007). contributes to the refinement and elaboration of theory as
well” (2000, p. 13).
Brewer (2000) described the three Rs of how exter-
WHY STUDY SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL nal validity contributes to the development of theory and
PROCESSES OUTSIDE THE LABORATORY? knowledge:

Debate over the relative priority that should be given to 1. Robustness, or whether a finding is replicated in
internal and external validity is not new. Many commen- different settings, with different samples, or in dif-
tators have bemoaned the seeming low priority given to ferent historical or cultural circumstances. Although
generalizability (e.g., Helmreich, 1975; McGuire, 1967; researchers sometimes couch replications of this sort
Ring, 1967; Silverman, 1971; see Henry, 2008, for a more in checklist terms (“yes it did” or “no it didn’t”), it is more
recent version). Various replies have been provided, the informative to think about replications in terms of
most commonly cited of which argue that the purpose of their ability to identify boundary conditions for an
laboratory experiments is to evaluate theories, regardless effect and other moderating variables, which in turn
of the applicability of those theories—in other words, to may contribute to fuller understanding of the scope,
determine “what can happen” as opposed to “what context, and mechanism for a phenomenon. For exam-
does happen” (e.g., Aronson, Wilson, & Brewer, 1998; ple, most social psychologists believe that behavior
Berkowitz & Donnerstein, 1982; Mook, 1983). Perhaps is a function of Person ⫻ Environment interactions
reflecting the effectiveness of these replies, social psychol- (Funder, 2006), and such interactions are more likely
ogists are usually taught that internal validity has higher to be revealed in studies with heterogeneous popula-
priority than external validity—that it is more important to tions. Similarly, ever since Barker (1968), most social
be certain about concluding that an independent variable is psychologists have believed that settings affect behav-
the true source of changes in a dependent variable than it ior, yet in laboratory studies, although setting variables
is to know that research findings can be generalized to other may be controlled (perhaps as part of “lab lore”), they
settings and samples. Too often, however, in our opinion, are not systematically investigated. The situational
the lesser priority of external validity is confused with low variable held constant in one program of research may
priority, which fosters a certain irony. Social psychology be the focal variable of another research program.
generally seeks principles to describe social behavior that Replications, in other words, help identify modera-
hold across persons, settings, and (sometimes) cultures tor variables that are essential to the full specifica-
(Cook & Groom, 2004). How do we know this to be so tion of a theory and its component processes. Because
without research that establishes the point? Non-laboratory laboratory studies typically isolate the variables in
methods are well suited to demonstrating external validity. question from influence by settings, individual differ-
As most discussions of methodology point out, no sin- ences, and other contextual factors in order to identify
gle study can maximize all types of validity (e.g., Brewer, cause-and-effect associations, they tend to privilege
2000; Smith & Mackie, 2000). All methods have their main effects (Cook & Groom, 2004).
advantages and drawbacks, which is why methodological 2. Representativeness, or do the conditions or processes
pluralism—using multiple and varied paradigms, operations, actually occur in the real world? This differs from
and measures to triangulate on the same concepts—has robustness because an effect might be highly replicable,
long been advocated as a feature of research programs but unlike anything that occurs in normal circum-
(Campbell, 1957; Campbell & Fiske, 1959), if more in stances. Brunswik (1956) pointed out the importance
principle than in practice. There is little reason to doubt that of representativeness, in noting that generalizability to
86 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

the real-world depended on random sampling of both A somewhat different way of conceptualizing the rela-
participants and contexts. Nonetheless, the biological tive advantage of non-laboratory research concerns the
and physical sciences commonly use unrepresenta- issue of closeness to real-world concerns (closely related
tive conditions to test theory (e.g., the behavior of to, but not the same as, the distinction between mundane
electrons in a vacuum may illuminate a proposed realism, or, the extent to which the events of an experiment
mechanism), and they may have similar value for social- resemble real-world events, and experimental realism, or,
psychological theory (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). For the extent to which experimental events are involving; see
example, examining the effects of mere exposure with Wilson et al., this Handbook). Weick (1985) posed a series
variably mixed combinations of familiar and unfamiliar of intriguing questions about which situations get “closer”
stimuli may not resemble circumstances that naturally to the human condition: A study of how one tells a newly
occur, but they allow researchers to compare explana- acquainted stranger in the laboratory that she is about to
tions based on fluency and repetitiveness (Dechêne, receive a mildly painful electric shock or a study of how a
Stahl, Hansen, & Wänke, under review). Nonetheless, coroner announces death to next of kin. Or, anticipation of
understanding when and how processes apply to natural putting one’s hand in a bucket of ice water in a controlled
social behavior necessarily provides a foundation for laboratory room or learning how to work on high steel in
theory development, just as descriptive taxonomies of a 21-story building. Distance, Weick argued, may encour-
species provide a foundation for biological research age ambiguity and detachment from the motives, wishes,
(Kelley, 1992). Furthermore, identification of the fears, and concerns that drive behavior in the real world.
circumstances under which a phenomenon occurs To be sure, laboratory studies can be intensely involving,
in the real world may suggest important clues about but often they are not (Baumeister, Vohs, & Funder, 2007),
covariates, mechanisms, and limiting conditions (e.g., especially in light of the restrictions that Research Ethics
as has been shown in repeated efforts to apply the Boards increasingly demand, which make it difficult for
contact hypothesis to actual intergroup conflicts researchers to engage participants in a way that activates
[Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006]). Representativeness is strong personal involvement. If the setting is chosen
also important for translations and application of basic properly, such involvement is readily accessible in non-
research. laboratory studies—for example, the same undergraduate
3. Relevance, or can the findings be used to modify student who is only mildly concerned about having per-
behavior in the real world? Of course not all research formed poorly on a laboratory task of mental arithmetic
(laboratory or non-laboratory) is intended to test may be substantially more engaged in the outcome of her
intervention-related hypotheses, but to the extent that the- calculus midterm examination. Similarly, recent speed-
ories can be used to modify behavior, their theoretical dating research has yielded results that differ from more
basis is strengthened. This principle underlies Lewin’s traditional laboratory studies of initial romantic interac-
(1951) belief in the value of “action research” for tions (Finkel & Eastwick, 2008). Non-laboratory studies,
theory development, as well as the more general claim in other words, may bring research questions “closer”
that psychological theories are useful if they can be to involving, personally meaningful motives, defenses,
used to predict and control behavior. Because non- affects, and thought processes.
laboratory applications do not isolate the effects of a Just how effectively non-laboratory studies accomplish
given manipulation from the simultaneous effects of these goals depends, of course, on how the research is
other processes in the natural environment, they help designed and conducted. Non-laboratory studies need to be
identify the relative strength of a given manipulation systematic, coherent, and controlled for the impact of errors
in context, as well as its sensitivity to interference by and artifacts; a flawed field study contributes no more than
moderating variables. (It is easy to imagine circum- a poorly designed laboratory experiment. No individual
stances in which a manipulation might produce effects study can simultaneously minimize all threats to internal
of considerable effect size under the tightly controlled validity by experimental control, nor all possible limits on
conditions of the laboratory, yet be ineffectual in the generalizability by going outside the laboratory. Validity, in
real world.) Haslam and McGarty (2004) suggest an the broadest sense, depends on matching protocols, designs,
inverse relationship between relevance and sensitiv- and methods to questions, so that, across a program of
ity: The more relevant a given issue to participants, research, all reasonable alternative explanations are ruled
the less sensitive (i.e., modifiable) their behavior may out and boundary conditions are established. Thus, as with
be. For example, in most cases it would be easier to laboratory research, the ultimate rationale for conducting
modify lawn care behavior than sexual behavior, even non-laboratory research is to advance the depth, accuracy,
though the same general theory may apply. and usefulness of social-psychological knowledge.
Field Experiments 87

FIELD EXPERIMENTS the independent (attire) and dependent (giving a coin to the
accomplice) variables. It is unlikely that participants sus-
As mentioned earlier, experiments, quasi-experiments, pected that they were in an experiment or that their response
and non-experimental (correlational) designs can be to the attire was under scrutiny. Had the same experiment
enacted in field settings. The principles that distinguish been conducted in the laboratory, participants might well
these designs from one another are the same, regardless have been more attentive to these possibilities. (Of course,
of whether the research is conducted in the field or in the in the laboratory, it would be easier to manipulate per-
laboratory; consequently, readers are referred to the chap- ceived authority in a way that kept confederates unaware
ter by Wilson et al. (this Handbook) for discussion of the of conditions, so that their behavior could not have varied
basic principles of experimentation. It bears noting that a systematically across conditions.) Additionally, participants
great deal of field research is non-experimental in nature— cannot walk away muttering “sorry” in the laboratory, as
for example, simple observational studies in which the they can in real life.
behavior of persons in natural habitats is observed. We do The inability to gain control over extraneous circum-
not discuss those methods here; for simplicity, we use the stances that might have influenced the findings is the chief
term “field experiments” to refer to field experiments and disadvantage of field experiments. In Bushman’s simple
field quasi-experiments, although we intend no conceptual experiment, these seem unlikely. But consider a field
confusion between the terms. experiment conducted by Josephson (1987), in which
Researchers conduct field experiments for several rea- second- and third-grade boys were frustrated before or
sons. The desire to maximize external validity is cardinal after watching violent or nonviolent television programs
among them, as discussed earlier. Another reason is the in school, then observed playing floor hockey with other
desire to observe phenomena in their natural contexts, children. Because of random assignment to conditions,
without controlling for other influences, so that processes we can be confident that the conditions were responsible
can be studied within the full circumstances in which they for observed differences in aggressiveness but various
are most likely to occur (Reis, 1983). This principle refers uncontrolled factors may also have been influential: How
to whether the conditions in research are representative closely did the boys attend to the programs? Did the adults
of the typical conditions in which that effect commonly present respond to the boys in ways that facilitated or
occurs.1 A third advantage of field experiments is that most inhibited aggression? Were there cues in the school that
often, participants are not aware of being in a psychology influenced their responses? Did interaction among the
experiment, thereby minimizing demand characteristics children alter their responses? Questions of this sort are
(cues that suggest to research participants the behaviors central to identifying the mechanism responsible for an
that researchers expect of them), suspicion, and other effect, and it is likely that these factors could have been
reactive effects that may occur in the laboratory context. controlled better in a laboratory experiment.
A final reason is that some researchers simply find field Researchers more commonly conduct quasi-experiments
settings “more interesting” (Salovey & Williams-Piehota, in field than in laboratory settings, and because partici-
2004), although, we hasten to add, for other researchers the pants in quasi-experiments are not randomly assigned to
same sentiment may apply to laboratory research. conditions, threats to internal validity tend to be greater.
Consider a study conducted by Bushman (1988). In this For example, had the boys in Josephson’s study not been
study, a female confederate approached pedestrians and randomly assigned to conditions, but instead had one
instructed them to give change to an accomplice stand- classroom been assigned to watch violent programs and
ing next to an expired parking meter. To investigate the another classroom to watch nonviolent programs, other
effects of perceived authority on compliance, the confeder- factors (e.g., pre-existing differences between the class-
ate wore one of three outfits: a uniform, business clothes rooms, other classroom events during the study interval)
(to imply status but not authority), or sloppy clothes that might plausibly have caused the observed differences. For
made her appear to be a panhandler. The uniform condition this reason, quasi-experiments involve pre-manipulation
induced greater compliance than the other two conditions, and post-manipulation assessments, and typically include
which did not differ significantly from each other. This set- as many other design elements as possible to address these
ting is a natural one for this kind of request and for both threats to internal validity (Cook & Campbell, 1979; West,
Biesanz & Pitts, 2000).
Field experiments often alter the typical balance between
1Although this is sometimes referred to as ecological validity, mundane and experimental realism. As originally defined
Hammond (1998) points out that this term represents a misleading by Aronson and Carlsmith (1968), mundane realism is high
application of what Brunswik, who originated the term, meant. when a research protocol resembles events likely to occur
88 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

in normal activity. Experimental realism, in contrast, is high need not be the case, however. Studies conducted in or near
when participants find research involving and engrossing so specialized settings (e.g., football stadia, bridal shows, sin-
that they are interested, attentive, and motivated to take their gles’ bars, farmers’ markets, or on Wall Street or Telegraph
task seriously. Laboratory research generally puts higher Avenue in Berkeley) may also be unrepresentative, in the
priority on experimental than mundane realism, for reasons sense of providing a non-random sample of persons. Aside
explained by Wilson et al. (this Handbook). Field research almost from the possibility that an effect operates differently
by definition maximizes mundane realism, because partici- in one nonrandom sample than in another, nonrandom
pants are encountered in their normal activity, although per- samples may possess restricted range on key variables,
haps ironically, experimental realism may not be high. For which can attenuate results and obscure potential modera-
example, persuasive messages or a request for help delivered tors (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). Moreover, in
casually and ineffectually by a stranger in a coffee shop may quasi-experimental and correlational field studies, the fac-
be dismissed in a cursory manner, with little or no thought or tors that lead participants to one or another condition of a
concern. Or, distracted passers-by may not even notice events study may introduce the possibility of substantial alterna-
staged to take place on a busy street corner, in which many tive explanations. For example, a study of participants at a
stimuli compete for attention. Researchers should not assume Democratic or Republican presidential rally would need to
experimental realism in field settings; establishing it requires contend with the fact that there are likely many differences
as much (and perhaps more) care as it does in the laboratory. between these groups beyond the candidate supported.
Of course, some studies are designed to examine processes It also can be difficult to randomly assign participants
that operate with minimal engagement (e.g., automaticity), to conditions in field settings. Participants might be more
and in this circumstance low experimental realism may be unwilling to take part in an effortful, costly, or unpleas-
appropriate. ant condition of an experiment than in a less effortful, less
Sometimes, significant real-world events lead research- costly, or more pleasant condition, a potential threat to non-
ers into the field, either because that event creates a natu- equivalence of groups and hence internal validity (West
ral manipulation for what has been studied in the lab (e.g., et al., 2000). This can be particularly vexing for interven-
Zucker, Manosevitz, and Lanyon’s 1968 study of affiliation tion studies, in which demanding interventions (e.g., for
and birth order during the November 1965 New York City smoking cessation) may foster greater attrition in treatment
blackout) or because the event is so inherently compelling groups than in wait-list control groups. Or sometimes, the
that a research response is called for (e.g., responses to 9/11; lesser degree of control that inheres in field settings may
Silver, 2004). Such studies most commonly survey responses allow participants to undermine random assignment. For
to the events, but quasi-experiments and experiments are also example, teachers might be randomly assigned to run some
feasible. For example, one group of researchers conducted lin- classrooms in a very cold and controlling manner but others
guistic analyses of data collected by an online journaling ser- in a warmer, more supportive way. Nonetheless, when in
vice for two months before and after the 9/11 attacks (Cohn, the classroom and faced with instructional demands and
Mehl, & Pennebaker, 2004). In another example, researchers other distractions, teachers may behave as they see fit,
used archived letters to the editors of local newspapers to ignoring, misinterpreting, or contradicting the conditions
study coping responses over time to the Mount St. Helen’s to which they were assigned.2 Of course, researchers
volcano eruptions (Pennebaker & Newtson, 1983). Pre-data can and do take steps to monitor and content with these
for natural events may also be available fortuitously; in one potential problems; our point is that in field experiments,
instance the researchers had been conducting a short-term lon- participants may make choices that interfere with well-
gitudinal study of falling in love when the 1989 Loma Prieta designed experimental plans.
earthquakes hit the San Francisco Bay Area (Aron, Paris, & Finally, field experiments may suffer from uninten-
Aron, 1995). Events such as these often allow researchers to tional experimenter bias in the selection of participants
tell a gripping story, but because it is usually impossible to and their assignment to conditions. In laboratory studies,
control key independent variables or to collect pre-event data experimenters typically do not choose participants, and
retrospectively, threats to internal validity may be substantial. they assign participants to conditions either before arrival
Below we briefly discuss several issues for researchers or without possible bias (e.g., by a computer program).
planning non-laboratory studies to consider. In field studies, however, experimenters sometimes chose

Sampling and Random Assignment


2Of course, this may also be a factor in laboratory experiments,
Field research is often conducted to obtain samples that but because the experimenter has greater control over what tran-
are more representative than undergraduate samples. This spires, it is less likely.
Field Experiments 89

whom to approach in a public setting (e.g., “the next person to wear school-identifying clothes as a function of foot-
walking alone to turn the corner”) or which condition to ball victories and losses, supporting the idea of “basking in
assign a participant. In principle experimenters have no reflected glory.” In another example, seating choices of bus
discretion over these decisions but in practice experiment- commuters in Singapore display ingroup preferences as a
ers are sometimes tempted to skip a potential participant function of sex and ethnicity, but not age (Sriram, 2002).
who looks uncooperative or unfriendly, or to assign an And, in a study of Judo participants in the 2004 Athens
unattractive person to a condition that would require less Olympics (Matsumoto & Willingham, 2006), photogra-
interaction (which would be equally problematic in the phers took action shots at several points. Gold and bronze
lab). It is important to obviate such biases. medal winners were more likely to display Duchenne
(spontaneous, genuine) smiles than silver medal winners,
supporting the role of counterfactual thinking in emotional
Choice of Outcome Measures
experience. Behaviors characteristic of attachment (e.g.,
Because field studies are often designed so that “the sci- clinging, crying, hugging, holding hands) demonstrably
entist’s intervention is not detectable by the subject and occur among adults separating at airports (Fraley & Shaver,
the naturalness of the situation is not violated” (Webb, 1998). A final example comes from the test of a hypothesis
Campbell, Schwartz, Sechrest, & Grove, 1981, p. 143), about the role of concealed ovulation in human mating.
they often use unobtrusive, non-reactive behavioral out- Professional lap dancers earned significantly greater tips
come measures. Although this tendency is not inviolate— while ovulating, but showed no similar increase if using
field studies often rely on self-report, and lab experiments oral contraceptives (Miller, Tybur & Jordan, 2007). All
may use unobtrusive measures (see, for example, Ickes’s these indicators are passive.
1983 Unstructured Interaction Paradigm, in which par- Although field experiments involve active interven-
ticipants’ spontaneous interactions are videotaped without tion by researchers in creating the conditions being studied,
their awareness)—field studies invite researchers to develop they often use outcome measures for which participants are
and use outcome measures that index the processes under unaware of being observed. Field experiments have been
investigation without raising participants’ awareness that prominent in the bystander intervention literature, where
researchers are scrutinizing their behavior. Non-experiments the outcome is whether a helping intervention occurred. For
have the further goal to avoid altering or modifying partici- example, in Piliavin, Rodin, and Piliavin’s (1969) classic
pants’ behavior from what they would otherwise do. These experiment, a confederate feigning drunken behavior was less
settings create a need to balance creativity and relevance likely to receive help on a New York City subway train than
(Does the construct actually apply in this setting?) against a confederate feigning illness. Other studies have used the
validity (Does the measure assess the process it purports to lost letter technique, in which fully addressed letters, varying
assess?) and sensitivity (Does the proposed measure vary according to the independent variable of interest (e.g., a return
systematically and in measurable increments corresponding address of the Communist Party or the American Red Cross)
to the predictor variable?). Because field research tends to are left in public places, to be found and mailed by passers-
involve more variability in settings and samples than labo- by, if they are so inclined (Milgram, Mann, & Harter, 1965).
ratory experimentation, measure development may take Other well-known field experiments in social psychology
relatively more time and effort. include manipulations of choice and responsibility in a sam-
A brief and non-representative sampling of measures ple of elderly nursing home residents, which, in an 18-month
used in field studies illustrates the kind of creativity that follow-up, were shown to have beneficially affected mortal-
characterizes successful field research. We distinguish ity rates (Langer & Rodin, 1976; Rodin & Langer, 1977).
passive observation—in which data collection exerts no In still other studies, the number of available alternative
meaningful effect on the behaviors being assessed—from choices influenced purchases of gourmet jams or chocolate
active observation—in which participants respond to (Iyengar & Lepper, 2000), drivers behind a stopped vehicle
some sort of circumstance or manipulation created by the at a green light honked sooner if the stopped vehicle had a
experimenter. (This is slightly different than the distinc- gun rack and an aggressive bumper sticker (Turner, Layton &
tion between non-reactive and reactive assessment, which Simons, 1975), men were no more likely to pay a return visit
refers to whether participants are required to respond to to a prostitute if she had played “hard to get” than if she had
something or whether the data are already available.) not (Walster, Walster, & Lambert, 1971), and, when French
Classic examples of passive observation include Triplett’s music was being played in a supermarket, French wine
(1898) observation that bicycle racers tended to race faster outsold German wine but when German music was being
when in the presence of other racers than when alone and played, German wine outsold French wine (North &
Cialdini et al.’s (1976) tally of the tendency of students’ Hargreaves, 1997).
90 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

A Special Ethical Consideration in Field was simply a new medium for delivering conventional
Experiments methods, most often surveys, to new populations in a cost-
effective manner. For example, in 1996 one early study
Informed consent is a core principle of modern ethical regula- used an online form to collect pet owners’ ratings of their
tions concerning the use of human participants in research. pets’ personalities (Gosling & Bonnenburg, 1998). Around
Even if participants in a laboratory study are not fully informed the same time, Ulf-Dietrich Reips and John Krantz sepa-
as a study commences, by their presence they have given con- rately began using the Internet to deliver experiments to
sent, almost always explicitly, to participating in a study. This research participants (Musch & Reips, 2000). By today’s
consent is based on an implicit and often explicit “contract” standards these early studies were rather rudimentary, and
that expresses the participant’s willingness to be observed under the samples were biased towards educated, technically
experimentally created conditions, in return for the experiment- savvy users. However, the studies hinted at the potential
er’s promise to protect his or her welfare and privacy. No such offered by the Internet. They showed, for example, that
contract exists in field research. As described earlier, a prime Internet studies could rapidly access large numbers of
rationale for field research is to examine natural behavior participants, many of whom were beyond the convenient
when people are unaware of being scrutinized. In many cases, reach of conventional methods, and they could do so at a
asking potential participants in a field experiment to provide fraction of the cost and without the laborious error-prone
informed consent prior to a study would likely (and perhaps data entry associated with traditional methods. So if social
dramatically) reduce external validity. psychologists were concerned about the critique of rely-
Some commentators have argued for this reason that field ing too heavily on convenience samples of college students
experiments should be proscribed, but most Research Ethics (e.g., Sears, 1986), the Internet offered a ready solution.
committees allow some latitude. Regulations and their inter- It was not long before large-scale projects began to
pretation vary from one institution to another, although capitalize on the opportunities afforded by Web research,
some generalizations are possible. Consent can typically using Internet technology to improve the efficiency and
be bypassed in studies that are solely observational and that accuracy with which traditional forms of data could be
involve anonymous, public behavior (e.g., pedestrian walk- collected. In addition to reductions in data-entry errors,
ing patterns). When interventions are involved and consent the Web allowed researchers to collect data around the
would interfere with external validity, researchers must take world without the delays of land-based mail. Moreover,
more than the usual amount of caution to ensure that partici- the validity of protocols could be checked instantly, the
pants will not be harmed, distressed, annoyed, or embarrassed. data stored automatically, and feedback delivered instan-
Practically, this means that field studies are typically limited to taneously to participants. This last benefit quickly proved
be less invasive than laboratory studies. (We suspect that few to be particularly important because feedback served as a
contemporary ethics committees would permit an experiment major incentive for participation (Reips, 2000). By pro-
such as Piliavin et al.’s 1969 subway study, described earlier, viding personalized automated feedback, investigators
because obtaining informed consent prior to the manipula- were able to collect data from hundreds of thousands of
tion would render that study uninteresting.) Researchers can participants, samples previously unheard of in psychologi-
and should ask participants for consent and fully debrief them cal research. For example, since 1998 the Project Implicit
afterwards in most field experiments. Although post-hoc con- website has collected several million tests of implicit atti-
sent shows some degree of respect for participants’ privacy, it tudes, feelings, and cognitions from all over the world
does not avert problems brought on by distress, embarrassment, (http://projectimplicit.net/generalinfo.php).
or unwanted invasions of privacy. After-the-fact consenting may The role of the Internet in psychological research has
even alert participants that the situation just encountered was an continued to expand as quickly as the growth of the Internet
experiment rather than a natural occurrence, potentially increas- itself. An idea of the breadth of topics already covered by
ing negativity. Researchers and ethics committees therefore pay Internet research is conveyed by sampling the chapters of a
special attention to consent issues in field experiments. volume summarizing recent trends in Internet psychology
Aronson et al. (1998) provide lengthier discussion of (Joinson, McKenna, Postmes, & Reips, 2007): In addition
these issues. to well-studied areas of investigation, such as social iden-
tity theory, computer-mediated communication, and virtual
communities, the volume also includes chapters on topics as
INTERNET RESEARCH diverse as deception and misrepresentation, online attitude
change and persuasion, Internet addiction, online relation-
In the late 1990s psychologists and other social scientists ships, privacy and trust, health and leisure use of the Internet,
began using the Internet for research. At first the Internet and the psychology of interactive websites.
Internet Research 91

In recent years, the Internet has lived up to its promise of which stimulus is presented next), and presenting rich
allowing researchers to access populations and phenomena media (e.g., sounds and videos [Krantz, 2001; Krantz &
that would be difficult to study using conventional meth- Williams, in press]). Moreover, some methods that for-
ods. For example, to obtain access to white supremacists’ merly involved cumbersome procedures, like sorting tasks,
attitudes about advocating violence toward Blacks, one can be straightforwardly implemented online. For example,
group of researchers visited online chat rooms associated “drag-and-drop” objects can easily be used to complete
with supremacist groups (Glaser, Dixit, & Green, 2002). ranking tasks, magnitude scaling, preference-point maps,
The researchers posed as neophytes, allowing them to and various other grouping or sorting tasks (Neubarth, in
conduct semi-structured interviews concerning the factors press; http://hpolsurveys.com/enhance.htm). As a result of
(threat type, threat level) most likely to elicit advocacy of these benefits, more and more researchers are doing basic
violence. The anonymity afforded to both researchers and experiments (e.g., on priming) via the Internet, sometimes
participants by the chat-room context and the easy access delivering the studies no further than to a room on their
to a small, hard-to-reach group of individuals resulted in a own campus.
dataset that would have been difficult to gather with con- Many researchers have gone beyond merely using the
ventional methods. Another study took advantage of the Internet as a convenient and flexible way to deliver standard
Internet to contact and survey a sample of people suffering surveys, stimuli, and experiments to participants. Studies
from sexsomnia, a medical condition in which individu- range from those that use the technological capabilities
als engage in sexual activity during their sleep (Mangan & of computers connected to the Internet to gain access to
Reips, 2007); the embarrassment and shame experienced new venues and populations (e.g., the White supremacists
by sufferers meant that little was known about the condi- noted earlier) to those that focus on behavioral phenomena
tion. Yet, the reach and anonymity afforded by the Internet spawned by the Internet (e.g., Internet messaging, online
allowed the researchers to sample more than five times as social networking, large-scale music sharing).
many sexsomnia sufferers than had been reached in all pre- The Internet provides opportunities to study phenom-
vious studies combined from 20 years of research. ena unconstrained by the physical and practical parameters
of the offline world. For example, personal websites can be
used to examine identity claims that are hard to isolate in
Domains of Web Research
real-world contexts (Marcus, Machilek, & Schütz, 2006;
In the first decade of the new millennium, Internet stud- Vazire & Gosling, 2004). Specifically, by exploiting the
ies have proliferated, addressing a broad array of social unique characteristics of personal websites and compar-
psychological topics. To illustrate the scope of poten- ing personal websites with other contexts in which identity
tial research strategies we next provide a non-exhaustive claims are made, the effects of deliberate self-expression
review of Internet-based studies. can be isolated from the effects of inadvertent expression,
The most basic class of Internet research—sometimes which are confounded in most offline contexts of social
referred to as “translational methods” (Skitka & Sargis, perception. For example, a snowboard leaning against a
2006)—uses Internet technology to improve the effective- bedroom wall may indeed reflect the occupant’s past snow-
ness with which traditional forms of data can be collected. boarding behavior (i.e., behavioral residue), but her deci-
One prominent example of this approach is Project Implicit’s sion to leave it out rather than stow it in a closet could also
large-scale administration of the Implicit Association Test reflect a deliberate statement directed to others about her
(see Banaji & Heiphetz, this volume), which is designed to lifestyle and preferences (i.e., an identity claim). In a phys-
measure the strength of automatic associations between men- ical room, one cannot tell whether the snowboard owes its
tal representations of various concepts (e.g., having implicit presence to its role as behavioral residue, as an identity
negative feelings toward the elderly compared to the young). claim, or both. In contrast, most elements of a personal
And there have been many other successful attempts to website have been placed there deliberately for others to
measure attitudes, values, self-views, and any other entity see and the information on the sites can be rapidly saved
that could formerly be measured with computers or paper- and coded. (It is even possible to obtain records of past
and-pencil instruments. Such studies are administered via websites via www.archive.org, which is collecting them
computer, allowing them to take advantage of features asso- for the historical record). In a similar vein, the options of
ciated with the medium, such as providing participants with decorating and furnishing virtual spaces (e.g., in Second
immediate feedback, automatically checking for errors (e.g., Life) are not subject to the practical, physical, and financial
missing responses), screening for invalid protocols (e.g., due constraints associated with real-world spaces. The vir-
to acquiescent responding), implementing adaptive test- tual world provides many more possibilities than those
ing (e.g., where the response to one stimulus determines afforded by real life for experimenting with one’s physical
92 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

representation (e.g., choosing avatars or game characters and the distinction between online and offline life is
of a different sex, race, body type, and species). becoming increasingly blurred; where, for example, is the
In addition to being a domain in which to construct new line between speaking face-to-face, talking on the phone,
studies to collect data, the Internet already contains rich and chatting via text or IM? With so much of contempo-
pre-existing deposits of psychologically relevant data that rary social life played out online even those interactions
vigilant researchers can harvest. For example, one study that do not extend to offline contexts should be of interest
replicated findings derived from self-reported music pref- to social psychologists because the laws of human behav-
erences (which might be subject to self-reporting biases) ior are likely to apply regardless of whether interactions
with analyses of music libraries, which were accessible via are conducted on or offline.
a music-swapping website (Rentfrow & Gosling, 2003). By some estimates almost 600 million people world-
The millions of pages of text that are created online wide have profiles on online social networking sites, such
everyday provide another enormous source of pre- as MySpace and Facebook (http://www.comscore.com/
existing data. These pages offer opportunistic investigators press/release.asp?press=2396), making them an intrigu-
an abundance of research possibilities. For example, as ing domain of inquiry. Which psychological needs are met
noted earlier, one project examining social psychological by these sites? Which social psychological processes are
reactions to traumas analyzed the diaries of over a thousand operative? One early study of Facebook behavior exam-
U.S. users of an online journaling service spanning a period ined how cues left by social partners on one’s online net-
of four months, starting two months prior to the September working profile can affect observers’ impressions of the
11th attacks (Cohn et al., 2004). Linguistic analyses of profile owner (Walther, van der Heide, Kim, Westerman,
the journal entries revealed pronounced psychological & Tong, 2008). The investigators examined the effects on
changes in response to the attacks. In the short term, par- profile owners of the attractiveness of people leaving “wall
ticipants expressed more negative emotions, were more postings” (public notes left by friends on a person’s profile
cognitively and socially engaged, and wrote with greater page). Results suggested that the attractiveness of profile
psychological distance. After two weeks, their moods and owners’ friends affected ratings of their own attractive-
social referencing returned to baseline, and their use of ness in an assimilative pattern, such that people with wall
cognitive-analytic words dropped below baseline. Over posts left by attractive friends were themselves viewed as
the next six weeks, social referencing decreased, and more attractive than people with posts left by less attrac-
psychological distancing remained elevated relative to tive friends.
baseline. The effects were stronger for individuals highly A large range of applications, such as online social net-
preoccupied with September 11th but even participants works, online role-playing games, and meeting software
who hardly wrote about the events showed comparable allow people to create online virtual representations of
language changes. As noted by the authors this study themselves (e.g., as game characters or avatars in virtual
bypassed many of the methodological obstacles of trauma worlds). The advent of these representations creates whole
research and provided a fine-grained analysis of the time- new worlds for social psychological inquiry. For example,
line of human coping with upheaval. how are impressions formed and how are identities
Another creative project used a German online auction created in immersive virtual worlds such as those found in
site to examine ethnic discrimination (Shohat & Musch, games like EverQuest, World of Warcraft, and in the vir-
2003). The apparent ethnicity of sellers was manipulated tual social network Second Life (Yee, Bailenson, Urbanek,
by varying their last names. Analyses indicated that sellers Chang, & Merget, 2007)? And what are the connections
with Turkish names took longer to receive winning bids between real people and their virtual representations? As
than did those with German names. Given that so many more interactions and relationships become entirely vir-
interactions are now conducted online, and that many of tual, it is important for researchers to examine the causes
them leave a trace, savvy researchers should be ready to and consequences of the new social phenomena emerging
pounce on opportunities as they arise. in this domain.
An increasing number of studies focus on Internet The popularity of social networking sites and online
behaviors as worthwhile social psychological phenomena multi-player videogames will almost certainly be super-
in their own right, not simply because they are more seded by new yet-to-be-invented online behaviors. Our
convenient than studies done in the physical world. Some point applies regardless: The online world is a legitimate
of these behaviors are extensions of offline behaviors venue in which to examine a plethora of social psychologi-
but others are unique to the online world. With mobile cal behavior. Examples of online phenomena of potential
Web access, Internet behaviors are becoming ever more interest to social psychologists include online message
integrated into the milieu of modern-day social interactions boards and chat rooms, Internet messaging (IM), virtual
Internet Research 93

worlds (e.g., Second Life), online support groups (e.g., The central problems of Internet studies stem
for rare conditions), online multi-player video games from the physical disconnect between researcher and
(e.g., World of Warcraft), online social networks (e.g., participant, resulting in a potential lack of control over the
Facebook), Internet dating (e.g., eHarmony), online auc- assessment or experimental setting. Researchers are not
tion sites (e.g., eBay), blogs, and an ever-growing list of physically present when Internet studies are conducted
others. so they cannot easily assess participants’ alertness and
attentiveness. However, several methods have been
developed to detect the degree to which participants are
Overcoming Skepticism
attending to the experimental materials and following
Initial papers based on Internet research were greeted instructions properly (Johnson, 2005; Oppenheimer,
with a healthy dose of skepticism. Quite reasonably, jour- Meyvis, & Davidenko, 2008). For example, the Instructional
nal editors and reviewers had a number of concerns about Manipulation Check (IMC) measures whether participants
method artifacts and sampling issues. The major fears are reading the instructions. The IMC works by embedding
about Internet data can be summarized in terms of six a question within the experimental materials that is similar
concerns: (1) that Internet samples are not demographi- to the other questions in length and response format but
cally diverse; (2) that Internet samples are maladjusted, that asks participants to ignore the standard response for-
socially isolated, or depressed; (3) that Internet data do not mat and instead provide confirmation that they have read
generalize across presentation formats; (4) that Internet the instruction (Oppenheimer et al., 2008).
participants are unmotivated; (5) that Internet data are Another potential problem with Internet studies is that
compromised by the anonymity of the participants; and (6) researchers cannot easily answer questions from partici-
that Internet-based findings differ from those obtained with pants about the procedure. Because they are not directly
other methods. These concerns were addressed in a study observing research participants, researchers cannot be
comparing a large Internet sample with a year’s worth of aware of possible distractions, such as eating, drinking,
conventional samples published in JPSP (Gosling et al., television, music, conversations with friends, and the
2004). Analyses suggested that, compared to conventional perusal of other websites. Internet users, especially young
samples, Internet samples are more diverse with respect Internet users, are notorious for multitasking while logged
to gender, socioeconomic status, geographic region, and on, which could adversely affect the quality of Internet-
age. Moreover, Internet findings generalize across presen- based data. In the case of ability testing, with all of the
tation formats, are not adversely affected by non-serious information on the Internet at their disposal, it is difficult to
or repeat responders, and are generally consistent with keep participants from cheating. The extent to which these
findings from traditional methods. Similar conclusions distractions and other available sources of information
have been reached by other reviews addressing the valid- affect the findings of Internet studies is not known; how-
ity of Internet research (e.g., Krantz & Dalal, 2000). As a ever, research on Internet data versus real-life samples has
result of these reviews and as Internet research has become allayed many concerns about data quality by showing that
more widespread, much of the skepticism has evaporated. the Internet samples are generally not inferior to conven-
Nonetheless, it is important to keep in mind the advan- tional samples from a psychometric standpoint (Gosling
tages and disadvantages associated with Internet-based et al., 2004; Luce et al., 2007). Evidence is accumulating
methods. for their validity (Birnbaum, 2004; Krantz & Dalal, 2000).
As noted earlier, one advantage of Internet research is its
ability to reach samples beyond the reach of conventional
Advantages and Disadvantages of Internet-Based
methods. Internet-based samples tend to be more diverse
Methods
and considerably more representative than the convenience
As described earlier, Internet methods afford many advantages samples of college students commonly used in psychology
to social science researchers. The most important of these research (Birnbaum, 2004; Gosling et al., 2004; Skitka &
include the improved efficiency and accuracy with which Sargis, 2006) but these samples are still not representative
traditional forms of data (e.g., surveys, informant reports, of the general population (Lebo, 2000; Lenhart, 2000).
reaction-time experiments) can be collected, the possibility of Participation in Internet-based research is restricted to
instantly checking the validity of protocols and providing par- people who have access to the Internet, know how to use
ticipants with immediate feedback, the ability to reach large a Web browser, and, in some kinds of research, have a
and diverse samples from around the world, and the oppor- functioning email address or instant messaging capability.
tunity to integrate various media (e.g., sounds, photographs, People who are computer phobic, those who cannot afford
videos) into studies (Gosling & Johnson, in press). a computer and Internet service and have no public access,
94 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

and those who are uninterested in learning how to browse php; Reips & Neuhaus, 2002) especially useful. WEXTOR
the Web will be excluded from Internet research. Evidence is a free Web-based tool that allows researchers to quickly
is mixed regarding the extent to which this sampling bias design and visualize a large variety of Web experiments
affects the generalizability of Internet findings (Reips & in a guided step-by-step process. It dynamically creates
Krantz, in press). Generally, and as in all research, inves- the customized Web pages needed for an experimental
tigators need to be cautious in making claims regarding procedure that will run on any platform and it delivers
the generalizability of their findings; to guide the scope of a print-ready display of the experimental design. Using
their generalizations, researchers should collect and report an example of a 2 2 factorial design, Reips and Krantz
information about the demographics of their samples. (in press) provide a useful and accessible step-by-step
Finally, learning to construct Web pages, write program description of how WEXTOR can be used to build a Web
scripts, manage computer data bases, and engage in all of experiment; advice is also provided on how to monitor,
the other activities involved in starting up online research manage, and reduce dropout rates (i.e., attrition). Ulf-
can be time consuming. Entire new sets of skills must Dietrich Reips also maintains iScience.eu, a free and
be acquired, practiced, and polished. Fortunately, a large up-to-date portal to many of the services useful for gener-
number of resources are available for aspiring Internet ating and editing experiments, recruiting participants, and
researchers, which we summarize next. archiving studies.
Many decisions face researchers undertaking studies on
the Internet, and many potential pitfalls await the inexperi-
The Basics of Internet Research
enced or unwary investigator. Before undertaking Internet
The huge variety of possible topics, experimental designs, experiments, new researchers should draw on the numer-
and implementation options make it impossible to provide ous lessons already learned (e.g., Gosling & Johnson, in
much here in the way of specific advice on creating online press; Reips, 2000, 2002a, 2002b, 2002c). A prudent first
experiments. Fortunately, a number of general books for step would be to consult Reips (2002a), which summarizes
investigators taking their first steps into the domain of expertise gleaned from the early years of Internet-based
Internet research are available (Birnbaum, 2001; Fraley, experimental research and presents recommendations
2004; Gosling & Johnson, in press), along with work- on the ideal circumstances for conducting a study on the
shops (e.g., by Michael Birnbaum or John E. Williams), Internet, what precautions have to be undertaken in Web
and websites (e.g., iscience.eu; Project Implicit; websm. experimental design, which techniques have proven useful
org). Birnbaum (in press) provides a particularly useful in Web experimenting, which frequent errors and miscon-
introduction to the basic decisions that anyone planning to ceptions need to be avoided, and what should be reported.
conduct an experiment online needs to make. These deci- Reips’s article concludes with a useful list of sixteen stan-
sions range from deciding what kind of server makes most dards for Internet-based experimenting.
sense to choosing the appropriate client side (e.g., PHP,
Perl) and server side (e.g., Java, JavaScript) programs
and will be guided by design requirements. For exam- DIARY METHODS
ple, JavaScript can be particularly useful for designs that
require randomizing the order of materials or the assign- Diary methods, also known as event sampling, have become
ment of participants to conditions, or adding checks for increasingly popular and influential during the past three
unreasonable or missing responses. decades. A recent PsycInfo search revealed more than
For researchers who do not want to program the websites 1,200 published papers using or describing these methods.
themselves, several options exist, including survey websites Although there is some flexibility in what counts as diary
(e.g., via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk; Survey Monkey), methods, they generally include measures for self-reporting
websites for creating experiments (e.g., WEXTOR), col- behavior, affect, and cognition in everyday life, collected
laborative opportunities with existing research groups (e.g., repeatedly over a number of days, either once daily (so-
Project Implicit), and government-funded projects such as called daily diaries) or sampled several times during the
Time-sharing Experiments for the Social Sciences (TESS), day. The most popular of these latter sampling protocols are
which offers researchers opportunities to test their experi- the Experience Sampling Method (ESM; Csikszentmihalyi,
mental ideas on large, diverse, randomly selected subject Larson, & Prescott, 1977) and Ecological Momentary
populations via the Internet. Assessment (EMA; Stone & Shiffman, 1994; Shiffman,
Researchers who do not have the time or resources Stone, & Hufford, 2008). Another type of diary protocol is
to program their own experiments from scratch will find based on the occurrence of particular events, such as social
WEXTOR (http://psych-wextor.unizh.ch/wextor/en/index. interactions, sexual activity, or cigarette smoking.
Diary Methods 95

Diary protocols are designed to “capture life as it is monitored (e.g., Nelson, 1977); industrial psychologists’
lived” (Bolger, Davis & Rafaeli, 2003, p. 580)—that is, to use of self-reports of work-related activity, as an adjunct to
provide data about experience within its natural, spontane- observation by outside observers (e.g., Burns, 1954);
ous context (Reis, 1994). By documenting the “particulars and checklist approaches to the study of life-event stress,
of life,” researchers have a powerful tool for investigating popularized by Holmes and Rahe (1967). It was not until
social, psychological, and physiological processes within the seminal work of Csikszentmihalyi and colleagues
ordinary, everyday interaction. Key to the diary approach (Csikszentmihalyi, Larson, & Prescott, 1977), who devel-
is an appreciation for “the importance of the contexts in oped the ESM, that the field began to develop and apply
which these processes unfold” (Bolger et al., 2003, p. 580) systematic methods for studying everyday experience that
as a central element in the operation and impact of social could be adapted to diverse phenomena, questions, and cir-
psychological processes. As the accessibility and popular- cumstances. Csikszentmihalyi and his colleagues wanted to
ity of diary methods have grown, the kinds of questions that know more about the contexts in which flow (a mental state
they can address have evolved in range and complexity. in which people are fully and energetically immersed in
Researchers have used diary methods to study a diverse whatever they are doing) emerges, as well as its behavioral,
range of phenomena and processes in social-personality affective, and cognitive correlates, and they felt that retro-
psychology. Topics for which diary studies have become spective accounts were too inaccurate. Hence they decided
commonplace include affect (e.g., Conner & Barrett, 2005; to use pagers to randomly signal research participants, ask-
Larsen, 1987; Sbarra & Emery, 2005), social interaction ing them to report on their experiences at the moment of
(e.g., Reis & Wheeler, 1991), marital and family interaction the signal. At around the same time, Wheeler and Nezlek
(e.g., Larson, Richards, & Perry-Jenkins, 1994; Story & (1977) created the Rochester Interaction Record (RIR), a
Repetti, 2006), stress (e.g., Almeida, 2005), physical systematic method for recording the details of social inter-
symptoms (e.g., Stone, Broderick, Porter, & Kaell, 1997), actions as they occur. More recently, Stone and Shiffman
subjective well-being and mental health (e.g., Oishi, (1994) offered a similar method, EMA, which can incorpo-
Schimmack, & Diener, 2001), and nearly every trait in rate physiological measures. Other sampling frameworks,
the personality lexicon (e.g., Bolger & Zuckerman, 1995; notably including daily diary methods, in which respon-
Fleeson, 2004; Suls, Martin, & David, 1998). Other areas dents provide data once daily for a prescribed period of
in which diary studies are less common but increasingly time, can be considered adaptations of these methods,
useful include sex (e.g., Birnbaum, Reis, & Mikulincer, although, as described below, the longer interval of a report
2006; Burleson, Trevathan, & Todd, 2007), self-esteem and differing sampling schedule represents an important
(e.g., Murray, Griffin, Rose, & Bellavia, 2003), self- conceptual difference.
regulation (e.g., Wood, Quinn, & Kashy, 2002), intergroup
relations (e.g., Pemberton, Insko, & Schopler, 1996), social The Rationale for Diary Research
comparison processes (e.g., Wheeler & Miyake, 1992),
social cognition (e.g., Skowronski, Betz, Thompson, & Diary studies have two main rationales, one conceptual
Shannon, 1991), attitudes (e.g., Conner, Perugini, O’Gorman, and one methodological. The conceptual rationale is to
Ayres, & Prestwich, 2007), motivation (e.g., Patrick, Knee, capture information about daily life experiences, as they
Canevello, & Lonsbary, 2007; Woike, 1995), and culture occur within the stream of ongoing, natural activity, and
and the self (e.g., Nezlek, Kafetsios, & Smith, 2008). For as they reflect the influence of context. Key is the idea that
further details, we refer readers to surveys of diary meth- ordinary, spontaneous behavior, or what Reis and Wheeler
ods used in social-psychological (Reis & Gable, 2000), called the “recurrent ‘little experiences’ of everyday life
psychopathology (deVries, 1992), and health psychology that fill most of our waking time and occupy the vast
(Stone, Shiffman, Atienza, & Nebeling, 2007) research. majority of our conscious attention” (1991, p. 340) can
contribute to social-psychological knowledge. Two kinds
of information fit under this heading. The first concerns
A Brief History of Diary Methods
basic facts: What happens when, where, and with whom
Wheeler and Reis (1991) trace interest in the self-record- else present. For example, diary studies can identify activ-
ing of everyday life events to four distinct historical ity patterns, such as the relative distribution of studying,
trends in social science research: time-budget studies, socializing, and TV watching among adolescents. The sec-
which date back to the early 1900s (e.g., Bevans, 1913); ond refers to the subjective phenomenology of daily life:
the need in behaviorist therapies to have patients keep to examine “fluctuations in the stream of consciousness
track of the behaviors being modified, such as smoking or and the links between the external context and the con-
marital conflict, so that treatment effectiveness could be tents of the mind” (Hektner, Schmidt, & Csikszentmihalyi,
96 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

2007, p. 6). Under this heading one might examine reports diary methods have documented how people spend their
of affect and cognition, such as mood, focus of attention, time (Robinson & Godbey, 1997), with whom they social-
self-evaluations, feelings of social connection, thoughts, ize (Reis & Wheeler, 1991), what they eat (Glanz, Murphy,
worries, or wishes. Both kinds of information can be Moylan, Evensen, & Curb, 2006), and how often they feel
obtained with open-ended responses or with checklists and bored (Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1977). Behavior descrip-
rating scales, although the latter is much more common in tion is an important, under-recognized and under-practiced
published research. component of theory development in social-personality
A more methodological rationale concerns the “dra- psychology (Rozin, 2001, 2009). More than a half-century
matic reduction” (Bolger et al., 2003, p. 580) in the effects ago, Solomon Asch argued, “Before we inquire into origins
of retrospection, the result of minimizing the time between and functional relations, it is necessary to know the thing
an event and its description. Traditional survey meth- we are trying to explain” (1952, p. 65). McClelland (1957)
ods suffer from various well-researched biases, such as made a similar argument for personality theory, suggest-
recency (more recent events are more likely to influence cur- ing that behavioral frequencies may be the best place for
rent judgments), salience (moments of peak intensity and personality theorizing to begin. As Reis and Gable com-
distinctive or personally relevant events tend to be more mented, “to carve nature at its joints, one must first locate
influential), recall (the greater the time between an event and its those joints” (2000, p. 192).
recollection, the greater the potential distortion), state of mind Nonetheless, social-personality psychologists are most
(current states may influence recall of prior states), and aggre- likely to apply diary methods for testing theory-driven
gation (people find it difficult to summarize multiple events; hypotheses in three different ways. First, diary methods
see Reis & Gable, 2000; Hufford, 2007; Schwarz, Groves, & can be used to evaluate alternative mechanisms thought to
Schuman, 1998; Stone et al., 2000, for reviews). Diary meth- underlie an effect. For example, comparing three poten-
ods are intended to reduce these biases as well as errors attributable tial explanations for the observed correlation between
to difficulty and to heuristic processing. This is a particularly trait neuroticism and distress, Bolger and Schilling (1991)
central rationale for diary methods that require instantaneous reported the best support for the tendency of persons
reporting of what is going on at the moment that a signal is high in neuroticism to react more strongly to stressful
received (e.g., EMA, ESM). These biases are more likely to circumstances. Second, diary studies can be used to distin-
affect diary methods that cover longer periods (e.g., daily diaries guish competing predictions. An example is Wheeler and
or methods that ask for reports of events since the prior report), Miyake’s (1992) contrast of two plausible effects of mood
although the extent of such effects, which depends on the time on subsequent social comparison: cognitive priming, which
gap, the questions being asked, and the nature of the events, is predicts comparing upward, to better-off persons, and self-
likely to be less than with traditional surveys. enhancement, which predicts downward comparison, to
Diary methods also have certain advantages over observa- less-fortunate others. Upward comparison was better sup-
tional methods that sample a narrower range of behavior, such ported. Third, diary methods are particularly well suited to
as laboratory observations of dyadic interaction. Although identifying conditions under which effects vary in strength
laboratory observations provide a videotaped record that, with or relevance (moderators). For example, solitary drinking
considerable time and effort, can be coded from an independent is more likely on days with negative interpersonal experi-
and semi-objective perspective, the structured context of being ences, whereas social drinking is more likely on days with
observed by experts in a restrictive setting may elicit behavior positive interpersonal experiences (Mohr et al., 2001).
that is unrepresentative of more natural, unstructured settings Looking at the types of questions that diary methods can
(Reis, 1994). (For example, participants in a laboratory obser- address in a somewhat different way, Bolger et al. (2003)
vation typically cannot get up and turn on the TV, as they can described three types of research questions to which diary
during real-life conflicts.) Furthermore, observational studies studies are suited: aggregating over time, modeling the time
rarely provide information about behavior in more than one or course, and examining within-person processes. The first
two contexts, whereas diary studies can be informative about asks about persons over time and context and involves
multiple and diverse contexts, a key consideration for studies aggregating individual responses over multiple reports.
seeking to identify contextual determinants of behavior. Typically, this approach is meant to improve over methods
that ask respondents to summarize their experience dur-
ing a timespan (e.g., “How much have you socialized with
Types of Questions For Which Diary Methods Are
others during the past two weeks?”), which are subject to
Well Suited
retrospection, selection, and aggregation biases. Although
Perhaps understandably, given their history, diary meth- diary designs sometimes seem like overkill for questions
ods have had appeal for descriptive research. For example, of this sort—asking people to repeatedly report a range of
Diary Methods 97

experiences for the purpose of arriving at a single summary methods are ideal for studying P ⫻ E effects of the sort
score—the substantial increase in data quality provides first theorized by Lewin and since then endorsed, at
more than adequate justification for the effort. The oppor- least in the abstract, by nearly all social and personality
tunity for “data mining”—sorting through large amounts psychologists (Fleeson, 2004; Funder, 2006).
of data to ask more refined, more detailed, or alternative Diary designs also have the important advantage of
questions—is an additional tangible benefit. unconfounding between-person and within-person ques-
Modeling the time course allows researchers to explore tions. Consider the hypothesis that perceived discrimination is
temporal and/or cyclical patterns in phenomena. Well- associated with lower effort in achievement settings. This
known among these patterns are diurnal (Clark, Watson, & might be studied by characterizing a person’s experiences
Leeka, 1989) and weekly (Reis, Sheldon, Gable, with discrimination (e.g., with a questionnaire) and relating
Roscoe, & Ryan, 2000; Stone, Hedges, Neale, & Satin, those scores to measures of achievement-related effort. An
1985) cycles of affect, such that positive affect tends to be alternative study might sample moments in a person’s life,
higher, and negative affect lower, in the early evenings and assessing ongoing covariation between perceived discrimi-
on weekends, respectively. Diary designs are also amena- nation and achievement-related effort. Although seemingly
ble to identifying more complex trends (e.g., repeated “up similar, these two hypothetical studies address indepen-
and down” cycles, such as might be shown in a sine wave dent questions. The former study asks a personological
[Walls & Schafer, 2006]), longer intervals (e.g., seasons question: Do persons who tend to perceive discrimination
or years), dynamic models, or so-called “broken stick” or also exert differential effort in achievement-related set-
step-function models, in which the pattern of an outcome tings? The latter study asks an experiential question: When
variable is discontinuous before and after a particular point discrimination occurs, do people respond with differential
(e.g., following a major life event, such as September 11th, effort? Numerous theorists (e.g., Epstein, 1983; Gable &
unemployment, or divorce). Analyses of this sort have Reis, 1999) have noted that these questions, and hence the
been rare in social psychology. nature of the processes that would explain their answers,
The most widespread use of diary designs in social differ fundamentally. In a more general way, Campbell
psychology falls into Bolger et al.’s (2003) third category, and Molenaar (in press) argue that much of psychological
examining within-person processes. Such studies inves- science erroneously assumes that intra-individual variation
tigate “the antecedents, correlates, and consequences of in response to time or context follows the same rules and
daily experiences” (Bolger et al., 2003, p. 586) as well mechanisms as inter-individual variation. They discuss
as, potentially, the processes underlying their operation. what they see as a major reorientation in the field toward
For example, studies have shown that high work stress is “person-specific paradigms,” capable of distinguishing
likely to lead to family conflict (Repetti, 1989) and that these different levels of explanation. Diary methods are a
invisible support tends to yield better adjustment to powerful tool for any such reorientation.
stressors than visible support does (Bolger, Zuckerman,
& Kessler, 2000). Many researchers construe this use Design and Methodological Issues in Diary
of diary methods as the non-experimental equivalent of Research
experimentation, inasmuch as the association between
specified independent and dependent variables can be Like any research paradigm, diary methods require that
assessed. However, there is an obvious and important researchers make choices guided by conceptual and practi-
difference: Experiments involving random assignment cal concerns. Diary methods are flexible and can be tailored
of participants to conditions permit causal inference, to the needs of an investigation. At the same time, planning
whereas diary studies do not (although data analyses and conducting research requires addressing inherent prac-
can rule out some alternative explanations, as described tical issues and limitations. Below we review some of the
below). Conversely, a major advantage of diary meth- more important (and in some cases contentious) issues that
ods is their ability to examine Person ⫻ Environment have arisen in current practice. More detailed information is
(P ⫻ E) interactions, or whether situational effects available in Christensen, Barrett, Bliss-Moreau, Lebo, and
vary systematically for different kinds of persons. For Kaschub (2003), Conner, Barrett, Tugade, and Tennen (2007),
example, low self-esteem persons respond to perceived Reis and Gable (2000), or Christensen’s website, http://
relationship threats by distancing from their partners psychiatry.uchc.edu/faculty/files/conner/ESM.htm. Hektner
whereas high self-esteem persons respond to the same et al. (2007) describe practical issues in the ESM in more
kind of threats by moving closer (Murray et al., 2003). detail, and Piasecki, Hufford, Solhan, and Trull (2007)
By allowing researchers to track individual differences in describe the application of diary methods in clinical
response to variability in the natural environment, diary assessment. For a proposed list of methodological
98 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

information to include in journal reports, see Stone and Signal-contingent protocols are limited in their ability
Shiffman (2007). to capture rare, occasional, or fleeting events, for which
event-contingent protocols are better suited. When events
Designs are rare or short-lived, random signals are unlikely to sam-
Choice among reporting protocols is generally based on ple a sufficient number, especially when researchers wish to
two considerations: The research question and the relative compare different subtypes of those events. Thus, instruc-
frequency of the key phenomena. Wheeler and Reis (1991) tions may ask participants to record their experiences
described three major protocols: interval-, signal-, and event- whenever a target event occurs. Examples include social
contingent (see also Reis & Gable, 2000). Interval and interactions lasting 10 minutes or longer (Wheeler & Reis,
signal are preferred choices when researchers are inter- 1991), conflicts among adolescents (Jensen-Campbell &
ested in “phenomena as they unfold over time” (Bolger Graziano, 2000), ostracism (Williams, 2001), sex
et al., 2003, p. 588) or when the phenomena occur often. (Birnbaum et al., 2006), smoking (Moghaddam & Ferguson,
Studies that focus on specific events, especially rare events 2007), alcohol consumption (Mohr et al., 2001), altruistic
(e.g., major life events, drug use), are more likely to use thoughts and behavior (Ferguson & Chandler, 2005), social
event-contingent protocols. comparisons (Wheeler & Miyake, 1992), prejudice and
Interval-contingent methods require reports at regular, discrimination (Swim, Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001),
predetermined times, so that the gap between successive and stressful events (Buunk & Peeters, 1994). To avoid
reports is relatively constant. The most popular such sched- bias, event-contingent protocols must unambiguously
ule is the daily diary, in which participants report on their define the types of events to be reported, and participants
experiences once a day, typically in the evening, before must do so when those events occur. Alternatively, a recent
bedtime. This daily cycle is consistent with the importance technological innovation uses sensors embedded within
of the day as a natural interval for organizing life activities, a recording device to detect certain events (for example,
as well as circadian rhythms underlying certain biologi- an accelerometer to monitor activity levels) and signal the
cal and psychological processes (e.g., DeYoung, Hasher, respondent to provide a report (Choudhury et al., 2008).
Djikic, Criger, & Peterson, 2007; Hasler, Mehl, Bootzin, &
Delivery Systems
Vazire, 2008). Other research has collected reports at sev-
eral fixed times during the day, such as thrice-daily (noon, The earliest ESM studies, reflecting that era’s tech-
dinnertime, and bedtime; Larsen & Kasimatis, 1991). nology, used pagers to prompt participants to com-
Fixed intervals are particularly valuable for studies of plete paper-and-pencil records. Since then, advances in
time-sequences and temporal cycles, in which repetitive, microprocessing technology have enabled many more
constant, or precisely timed intervals are helpful or essen- sophisticated systems for collecting diary data. One of
tial (e.g., day-of-the-week effects or the time-bound impact the earliest developments relied on digital watches, which
of activities, such as meals or afterschool activities). could be preprogrammed to deliver on schedule a week
Signal-contingent protocols prompt participants to or more’s worth of signals, although paper-and-pencil
report their experiences at the moment of receiving a reports were still required (Delespaul, 1992). A more
signal, usually delivered by pagers, cell phones, or prepro- important advance came from Personal Digital Assistants
grammed devices (e.g., palmtop computers, watches). As (PDAs, such as palmtop computers), which allowed
a rule, signaling schedules are random and unpredictable researchers to signal participants, collect responses, and
within predetermined blocks of time, so that a fixed num- branch to different question sets depending on what the
ber of prompts are sent each day (often, but not necessarily, participant is doing at the time (Barrett & Barrett, 2001).
around 10). Randomness is key: Because the data presum- Several websites provide or describe programming for
ably represent a random sampling of daily experiences, such devices, at least one of which, developed by Lisa
researchers receive non-selective, unbiased estimates of Feldman Barrett and Daniel Barrett with National Science
the distribution and quality of daily activities, affects, and Foundation support, is free (www2.bc.edu/˜barretli/esp).
cognitions. Non-random signals might be skewed toward par- Relative to paper-and-pencil, PDAs offer the advantage
ticular kinds of experiences, and predictability would allow of verifying the time of the participant’s response (which
participants to alter their activities shortly before signal. can then be compared to the schedule to assess fidelity,
Signal-contingent methods also typically demand that as described below) and can also record the reaction time
participants report their experiences right at the moment or duration of responding for particular questions. On the
of signal, with little or no delay, to support the claim other hand, PDAs are costly, breakable, stealable, and can
that they represent “real-time data capture” (Stone be difficult, inconvenient, or intrusive for some partici-
et al., 2007) without functional retrospection bias. pants (e.g., with elderly or less tech-savvy samples) and in
Diary Methods 99

some circumstances (e.g., when participants are in classes closings3 (Stone, Shiffman, Schwartz, Broderick, &
or meetings). Some researchers have created specialized Hufford, 2002). About 94% of the electronic diaries were
or proprietary devices that can be programmed to accom- compliant with the reporting schedule, but only 11% of the
modate particular circumstances (e.g., Invivodata Inc). paper responses. A further problem was that the vast major-
A relatively recent and promising development uses cell ity of the paper-condition participants claimed (apparently
phones in this manner (Collins, Kashdan, & Gollnisch, falsely) that they had been compliant. Chief among the fac-
2003). Downloadable software for using cell phones to tors that may contribute to noncompliance is hoarding: the
conduct context-aware experience sampling can be found tendency to complete multiple records at one time, such as
at http://myexperience.sourceforge.net. shortly before collection by researchers.
The regularity of interval-contingent protocols permits Most published research either cannot or does not verify
use of dedicated Internet sites for data collection. These compliance, although diary researchers would likely agree
tend to be appropriate when participants have easy Internet that noncompliance varies across studies, contexts, and per-
access (e.g., college students), and the scheduled timing of sons. Nevertheless, subsequent studies have suggested that
reports is consistent with this access (e.g., end of the day, the problem of noncompliance may not be as pandemic as
when participants are at home). Internet data collection Stone et al. indicate. For example, three studies reported
verifies the time of reporting and has the further advantage by Gaertner, Elsner, Pollmann-Dahmen, Radbruch, and
of allowing researchers to monitor compliance in real time, Sabatowski (2004) indicate that noncompliance is far less
so that noncomplying participants can be contacted imme- common than Stone et al. report, a conclusion similar to
diately. A conceptually similar low-tech approach involves that of Tennen, Affleck, Coyne, Larsen, and DeLongis,
a telephone call to participants at each prearranged time who state, “in six separate studies, we found almost no
and having an interviewer ask questions and record answers evidence of hoarding” (2006, p. 115). To social psycholo-
(Wethington & Almeida, in press). Verification of the time gists, a more important question than the frequency of
of report is particularly important when researchers wish noncompliance is the question of impact. In this regard,
to synchronize diary reports with other ambulatory mea- Green, Rafaeli, Bolger, Shrout, and Reis (2006) conducted
sures, such as physiological data. For example, one study extensive analyses, concluding that paper diaries (which
examined covariation in cardiac function and emotional could not be verified) and electronic diaries (which could
experience at randomly selected moments over 3 days be verified) yielded psychometrically equivalent data and
(Lane, Reis, Peterson, Zareba, & Moss, 2008). findings. A similar conclusion follows from another study
The benefits of electronic data collection methods not- comparing electronic and paper pain diaries (Gaertner
withstanding, many researchers (including us) still see an et al., 2004).
appropriate role for paper-and-pencil diaries. When elec- Perhaps more than with most methods, we see fidelity
tronic methods are not needed to deliver random signals as a matter of participant motivation: Diaries are often bur-
or varying protocols, and when the need to verify com- densome, and they require that participants regularly and
pliance is either not great or otherwise achievable (see reliably invest a significant amount of time and attention
below), paper-and-pencil diaries (e.g., in booklet form) are to describing their experiences. Client motivation affects
convenient, easy, accessible, user-friendly, and minimally patient compliance with self-reporting protocols in behav-
burdensome, all significant advantages when people are ior therapy research (Korotitsch & Nelson-Gray, 1999).
asked to report repeatedly in a personal way on their activi- For this reason, many diary researchers emphasize the
ties, thoughts, and feelings. We therefore recommend that importance of developing a collaborative, trusting rela-
the choice of delivery systems take into account both the tionship with participants. It is unlikely that the fact of
needs of the research and the likely experience of partici- monitoring or the method of administration—e.g., using
pants when using that system. a PDA that records time of response—will resolve most
issues of noncompliance. For example, if participants are
Fidelity busy, have misplaced their PDA, become reactive to the
Because the rationale for diary studies depends on timely suggestion that they cannot be trusted, feel that the protocol
reporting, controversy exists about whether participants can is difficult or unpleasant, or do not feel enough commit-
be trusted to comply with these schedules in the absence ment to the research project to prioritize timely recording,
of verification. This controversy was made prominent in noncompliance rates may be high. (Simply eliminating
an important study comparing compliance among partici-
pants using an electronic diary, which overtly recorded
the time of response, with paper diaries contained in 3 An unfortunate confound in this study is that there were also
a logbook that surreptitiously recorded openings and other differences between conditions.
100 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

persons or reports exceeding some compliance criterion marital conflicts for 15 days alter spouses’ behavior on
may introduce nonrandomness, a potentially important a videotaped conflict-resolution task (Merrilees, Goeke-
problem.) Furthermore, even near-immediate reports are Morey, & Cummings, 2008). Similarly, momentary reports
not free of memory-related distortion (Takarangi, Garry, of mood collected several times a day did not enhance later
& Loftus, 2006). For these reasons, researchers should recollection of those moods (Thomas & Diener, 1990).
take steps to minimize the motivation and opportunity And, although a sample of treated alcoholics claimed
for noncompliance rather than emphasizing monitoring. becoming more aware of their drinking patterns after taking
When objective verification is desired, PDAs or Internet part in a signal-contingent protocol, few actual differences
sites routinely record time of response. Compliance can were observed (Litt, Cooney, & Morse, 1998).
be monitored with paper diaries, such as with a portable These reassuring findings notwithstanding, the poten-
secure (unalterable) time-stamping device or, for daily dia- tial for reactivity problems suggests the need for caution
ries, by requiring that data be handed in or mailed each in designing protocols, minimizing factors that may
day. Postmarks might also be used as an admittedly imper- adversely affect participants’ willingness to be thoughtful
fect variant of the bogus pipeline (a technique for reducing and specific (e.g., asking too many similar questions; insensi-
response bias whereby research participants are led to tivity to interference with normal activities; running studies
believe that researchers have access to their true feelings for unnecessarily lengthy periods). Analyses should also
or attitudes) for encouraging and monitoring compliance routinely examine data for signs of response stereotypy
(Tennen et al., 2006). or carelessness, or for changes in the nature and pattern
of responses from early and late records (e.g., comparing
Reactivity week-1 and week-2 means and variances in a two-week
Researchers sometimes worry that the process of diary study; see Green et al., 2006, for examples). Finally,
diary record-keeping may alter participants’ experiences we concur with others (e.g., Bolger et al., 2003; Gable &
and reports. Hypothetically, any of several effects are Reis, 2000; Rafaeli, 2009) who have called for further
feasible. Self-monitoring might enhance awareness of research into reactivity effects. Such research would have
personal behavior—for example, eating or work habits— methodological benefits, and would shed light on the role
motivating participants to pursue change. Self-awareness of self-monitoring and awareness in everyday experience.
may reduce the intensity of affective states (Silvia, 2002)
Data Analytic Considerations
and introspection about traumatic events may facilitate
healthy cognitive reorganization (Pennebaker, 1997). Diary data represent an analytic challenge for two reasons.
Habituation or response decay over time might lead to Statistically, diary data are nested within individuals (and
stereotyped, non-thoughtful responding. Knowledge about a often individuals are nested within higher-order units, such
phenomenon—for example, which circumstances seem to be as couples, classrooms, or work groups), so that repeated
associated with memory loss—might develop as participants observations are not independent (Kenny, Kashy, & Bolger,
reflect on their personal experiences with it. Anticipation of 1998). Furthermore, as with most time-series data, vari-
a diary report might even cause participants to modify their ables in one report are likely to be correlated with prior
behavior. For example, asking people about their intent to reports, creating autocorrelation, which must be consid-
engage in certain behaviors increased the frequency of those ered in data analyses. Conceptually, because researchers
behaviors in three nondiary studies (Levav & Fitzsimons, using diary methods are usually interested in something
2006). Similarly, participants might avoid undesirable or more than a count of the total number of stressful events
illegal activities, or circumstances that will be effortful to or the mean level of intimacy across all social interactions,
describe, lest they have to inform researchers about those at the least simple aggregation underutilizes effortfully
activities. collected, potentially informative data.
Although little research has investigated these possi- Multilevel models have become the standard method of
bilities, what research there is suggests minimal problems. analysis, allowing researchers to examine both between-
Some studies report little effect of repeated responding person and within-person processes (that is, variation
(e.g., Hufford, Shields, Shiffman, Paty, & Balabanis, 2002), within person as a function of time or conditions), as well
whereas other studies have found small effects as a func- as their interaction. Although under certain circumstances
tion of the number of required reports (Mahoney, Moura & multilevel analyses can be conducted with traditional
Wade, 1973) or the obtrusiveness of the recording process methods (e.g., repeated-measures analysis of variance),
(Kirby, Fowler & Wade, 1991). The process of recording maximum-likelihood estimation is more common.
healthy habits had no discernible effects on enactment Maximum-likelihood methods (e.g., Hierarchical Linear
of those habits (Conti, 2000), nor did keeping diaries of Modeling [Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992]) provide more
Diary Methods 101

Yt⫺1 Yt Yt⫺1 Yt variable must be durable enough to persist from reports at


t–1 to reports at t. This seems more likely in designs where
assessments are separated by relatively small intervals (e. g.,
Xt⫺1 ESM, EMA). In the common daily diary designs, prospec-
Xt
tive prediction requires that effects endure from one day
Prospective Prediction Contemporaneous Change to the next, a relatively tenuous assumption for many
phenomena, given that a full day’s worth of activity, as
Figure 3.2 Two Kinds of Temporal Comparisons in Diary
Designs. well as the restorative effects of sleep, intervene. It follows
furthermore that in the absence of intervening events, the
contemporaneous change model provides a more accurate
estimate of the association between outcome and predictor.
accurate estimation of population values, especially when For this reason, contemporary change models are prefer-
the number of records varies from one person to the next able in certain instances, their greater inferential ambiguity
and when random effects are considered more appropriate notwithstanding. The choice of analytic models, therefore,
than the usual fixed effects. Excellent discussions of these should be based on the researcher’s goals.
analytic methods are available elsewhere (e.g., Bolger Although social psychologists have been quick to adopt
et al., 2003; Nezlek, 2003; Schwartz & Stone, 1998; Walls diary methods for examining processes within persons,
& Schafer, 2006; West & Hepworth, 1991), so we do not they have been slow to use these methods for investigat-
discuss them here. ing more complex temporal patterns. For example, one
Given the possibility of carryover from one report to might use spectral analysis to examine the periodicity
the next, researchers often analyze a given criterion vari- (frequency and amplitude of repetitive cycles) of various
able by controlling for the prior report’s value of that vari- phenomena, such as mood, over the day (Larsen, 1987)
able—for example, by examining today’s affect controlling or week (e.g., the day-of-the-week effect; Reis et al.,
for yesterday’s affect. This is commonly done in either of 2000), or in response to major life events (e.g., bereave-
two ways, as shown in Figure 3.2, and their implications ment). Investigating the natural life cycle of phenomena
differ significantly, although the choice is rarely explicit. such as conflict, instances of ostracism or discrimination,
The first method, prospective prediction, involves analyz- affective forecasts, or persuasive appeals, and accounting
ing the outcome variable on a given day t as a function of for variability in these cycles as a function of situational
the predictor and outcome on the prior day, t–1. The sec- factors and individual differences is a fertile opportunity for
ond method, contemporaneous change, looks at covariation expanding social psychological knowledge. Another type
between outcome and predictor on a given day t controlling of analysis exploits the repeated sampling of diary designs
for the outcome on the prior day t–1. The major rationale for by using temporal models to specify processes that con-
prospective prediction concerns inferences about causal prior- tribute to continuity and discontinuity in social behavior
ity. By predicting outcomes from both prior-day variables, over time. In this regard, Fraley and Roberts (2005) pro-
reverse causality—that the outcome is causally responsible pose different statistical models that contribute to longitudi-
for the predictor—is rendered implausible. In other words, nal stability—that is, to a high test-retest correlation—in
and similar to the logic of prospective prediction in longi- personality characteristics over the life course. These mod-
tudinal studies, because the partialled predictor at time t–1 els can also be used to better understand stability in social-
shares no common variance with the outcome yet tempo- psychological phenomena over shorter intervals.
rally precedes the outcome at time t, it plausibly exerts a
Diary Research with Couples and Families
causal effect on the outcome. For example, this method
has been used to establish that daily events are more likely Diary methods, especially daily diaries, have become par-
to be causally responsible for daily affect than the reverse ticularly popular among researchers who study couples and
(Gable, Reis, & Elliot, 2000). On the other hand, because families. All of the advantages of diary methods discussed
in the contemporaneous change model outcome and predic- earlier apply to couples and families; additionally, diary
tor are assessed simultaneously, causal priority cannot be methods allow researchers to study interactive processes
ascertained. However, controlling for the prior t–1 outcome (e.g., family conflict, intimacy) as they unfold in interde-
variable removes carryover effects so that whatever asso- pendent social units and also to identify contextual and
ciations are obtained result from that moment or interval, dispositional factors that moderate their impact (e.g., work
rather than prior moments or intervals. stress, self-esteem). For example, one partner’s feelings of
Although prospective prediction has clear advantages, vulnerability may engender behavior that contributes to
there is a potential downside: The effects of the predictor the other partner’s dissatisfaction with the relationship, a
102 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

process that is exacerbated when the vulnerable partner is technology, ambulatory methods have become exponen-
high in rejection sensitivity or low in self-esteem (Downey, tially more useful and adaptable to research. In this sec-
Freitas, Michaelis, & Khouri, 1998; Murray et al., 2003). tion we briefly review the application of these methods to
Conducting diary research with couples and families social psychology. Fahrenberg and Myrtek (2001) provide
generally necessitates that partners do not discuss their a more general review. It bears noting that most researchers
responses and that they keep their reports confidential from include momentary self-report methods, such as ESM and
each other. Confidentiality is important because partners EMA, under the general heading of ambulatory assessment.
might well be reluctant to report certain behaviors (e.g., This chapter has not followed that convention because
violence, infidelity, sources of dissatisfaction) if there was ESM and EMA are used primarily for self-reports, whereas
even a slim chance that their partners might see their reports. the methods reviewed in this section are non-self-report.
Privacy can be difficult to ensure with standard delivery Non-self-report ambulatory methods can also be combined
systems, so that dedicated systems are preferred (e.g., cell with ESM and EMA, as several examples below show.
phones or PDAs that do not store responses locally or that The main rationale for ambulatory assessment is the
are password-protected). The former is particularly impor- same as that discussed earlier for diary methods: To pro-
tant when comparisons of partners’ perspectives are of vide detailed data about variables of interest within their
interest, as in the example of studies that examine the rela- natural, spontaneous context. By applying this approach to
tive impact on daily affect and relationship well-being of behavioral (i.e., not self-reported) data, researchers capitalize
shared and differing perspectives about everyday couple on the advantages of non-laboratory assessment—
interaction (Gable, Reis, & Downey, 2003). At the same external validity, repeated, contextually sensitive data—
time, couple and family researchers coordinate reporting while avoiding the pitfalls of self-reports (Stone et al.,
schedules so that all parties provide reports at the same 2000). Ambulatory measures are particularly useful for
time or following the same events. Otherwise, one would assessing processes that operate outside of awareness,
not know if divergence reflected differing perspectives on which cannot be self-reported. Currently available ambu-
the same interaction or whether different interactions were latory methods include tools for assessing physiological
being described. processes, location and activity, speech, and features of the
Couple and family data require special methods of ambient environment. How this is done varies markedly.
analysis to manage interdependence (Kenny, Kashy, Ambulatory measures can be obtrusive (e.g., blood pressure
& Cook, 2006), and multilevel analyses of diary data monitors) or unobtrusive (e.g., sound recording devices,
are no exception. The couple/family adds an additional motion sensors), and they can be self-contained (e.g.,
level of nesting to such analyses (repeated reports are PDAs) or telemetric (devices that transmit data remotely,
nested within individuals, whose responses are nested such as by using mobile phone technology). As modern
within the couple or family), and some researchers pre- technology has expanded the range of what is possible,
fer to analyze these data as three-level hierarchical mod- more and more sophisticated gadgets and gizmos have been
els. Nevertheless, there are both practical and statistical designed with significant potential for behavioral science
reasons to consider using two-level models, using a tech- research (Goodwin, Velicer, & Intille, 2008). Application of
nique introduced by Raudenbush, Brennan, and Barnett these tools in theory-oriented research programs has been
(1995) and recently described by Laurenceau and Bolger variable, with some gaining immediate favor and others
(2005). This method takes advantage of the fact that part- awaiting adoption. This variability reflects several practi-
ners are distinguishable—for example, one is husband and cal considerations—cost, ease of use, adaptability to par-
one is wife—so that predictor variables representing both ticular circumstances, involvement of social psychologists
of them can be included in the same level of analysis. in development and dissemination—as well as a more

4 Many researchers include ESM and EMA in the general cat-


AMBULATORY ASSESSMENT
egory of ambulatory assessment methods, because many of the
same methodological and conceptual principles mentioned here
The term ambulatory assessment refers to the use of
also apply to ESM and EMA. We do not follow that convention
mechanical or electronic devices to record information for two reasons. First, common practice in social psychology
about an individual’s activity, circumstances, or states uses ESM and EMA in much the same manner as other diary
within ordinary daily life.4 First developed for medical methods. Second, the data collected with ESM and EMA are self-
purposes—specifically, monitoring of blood pressure and reports of experiences, thoughts, and feelings, much like diary
cardiac function over the course of normal activity—with data, whereas we limit the discussion of ambulatory assessment
the increasing complexity and miniaturization of digital methods to direct recording of non-self-report data.
Ambulatory Assessment 103

fundamental question: Researchers need to imagine how a hours. Using a sample of individuals with Long QT syndrome
new method can enhance the informativeness of their work. (a genetically based disorder that puts affected individuals at
In some instances, technological advances offer relatively risk for sudden cardiac death), Lane et al. (2008) found that
small potential for theoretical advances, whereas in other positive emotion was associated with shortening of the Q-T
instances, these advances may have potential to dramati- interval, lessening the risk of cardiac events.
cally improve the quality and relevance of findings. In still
other instances, a new device may open an entirely new Electronic Recording of the Acoustic Environment
area to social-psychological research.
Below we describe four examples—two established, two Observational researchers sometimes fantasize about
novel—with particular relevance to social psychology. implanting recording devices on the person of a research
participant so as to obtain an objective account of everything
they do. The Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR),
Ambulatory Cardiovascular Monitoring
developed by Pennebaker and colleagues (Pennebaker,
Ambulatory blood pressure monitoring for medical pur- Mehl, Crow, Dabbs, & Price, 2001), represents a first, less
poses is now commonplace, as research showed that megalomaniacal step in that direction. The EAR is a por-
blood pressure recordings taken in the individual’s normal table audio recorder that unobtrusively switches on and off
environment were better indicators of cardiovascular risk at random or regular intervals, providing samples of the
than office-based assessments (e.g., White, Schulman, acoustic environment as participants go about their daily
McCabe, Holley, & Dey, 1989). Cardiovascular reactivity activities. Participants cannot tell when the device is record-
has long been considered an important marker of stress, and ing, allowing researchers an opportunity to unobtrusively
more particularly of whether stressful circumstances are observe even relatively subtle sounds. For example, in sev-
appraised as threatening or challenging (Blascovich, 2000). eral studies, the EAR switched on for 30 seconds every
Combining these two principles suggests that ambulatory 12.5 minutes, yielding about 70 samples per person per day,
cardiovascular monitors would provide better indications which contain about 35 minutes worth of recordings (Mehl,
of the impact of social-personality factors on cardiovas- Vazire, Ramirez-Espinosa, Slatcher, & Pennebaker, 2007).
cular function than laboratory assessments. For example, Most commonly, researchers have used the EAR to sample
trait negative affects (depression, anger, neuroticism) pre- spoken language (i.e., verbal content and linguistic styles,
dict higher blood pressure in daily life (Ewart & Kolodner, which can be transcribed and analyzed via manual content
1994; Raikkonen, Matthews, Flory, Owens, & Gump, analysis or text-analysis software), but it can also be used to
1999). In a related vein, lonely people tend to be higher describe the acoustic environment in other ways; for exam-
than non-lonely persons in total peripheral resistance, a ple, what sorts of interactions or activities are taking place
physiological indication of threat responses (Hawkley (Mehl & Pennebaker, 2003a). An added benefit is that EAR
et al., 2003). transcriptions are easily archived for subsequent reanalysis
If ambulatory measurements are combined with event as new hypotheses emerge.
records (such as daily diaries), within-person changes to The EAR has been used in social psychology in several
social-psychologically relevant events can be assessed. ways. One analysis reported simple word frequencies from
Thus research has shown that the association between six studies, concluding that the popular stereotype that
trait negative affectivity and blood pressure elevation women are more talkative than men is unfounded (Mehl
is stronger in the classroom than in other settings et al., 2007). Men and women both used about 16,000
(Ewart & Kolodner, 1994), and that New York City traffic words per day—with large individual differences but no
enforcement officers experience higher blood pressure than evidence of a sex difference (the least talkative person used
baseline when engaging in unpleasant communications 695 words, the most talkative 47,016 words). A study fortu-
with the public (Brondolo, Karlin, Alexander, Bobrow, & itously begun just before the events of September 11, 2001,
Schwartz, 1999). Similarly, momentary negative moods found that a relative preponderance of dyadic over group
elevated both systolic and diastolic blood pressure among interactions fostered success coping (Mehl & Pennebaker,
optimists to approximately the same levels characteristic 2003b). The EAR has also been used to obtain a measure
of chronically negative people (Raikkonnen et al., 1999). of the frequency of different behaviors (e.g., talking on the
Ambulatory cardiovascular monitors have become telephone), which were then used as objective criteria for
increasingly sophisticated and are now capable of recording comparing the relative accuracy of self-ratings and other-
more than blood pressure and vascular resistance. For ratings of behavior (Vazire & Mehl, 2008). (Close others
example, Holter monitors can continuously record cardiac were often as accurate as the self, although these two
activity (much like an office ECG) for periods as long as 24 perspectives were often independent.)
104 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

Activity Monitoring when entered, might trigger requests for self-reports of


thought or affect. Intille (2007) refers to this as context-
Accelerometers are small devices used for detecting sensitive EMA. Another intriguing possibility would use
acceleration and changes in gravity-related forces (recent location sensors to keep track of social network mem-
wireless versions are called wockets). They are prob- bers’ proximity to one another. Proximity creates oppor-
ably most familiar to social psychologists in iPhones tunities for interaction, a venerable topic in interpersonal
and iPods, but researchers can also use them for sensing attraction research, but as yet no studies have examined
movement and activity patterns. Some researchers use systematically how physical presence leads to interaction.
accelerometers to provide objective accounts of sedentari- For example, family members might each carry with them
ness. For example, TV-watching was inversely related to a small badge containing a sensor that would continuously
general activity levels in one study (Hager, 2006), and in transmit location information to a central computer. These
another, autonomous motivation for exercise predicted the records could be combined to describe proximity among
frequency of moderate-intensity exercise (Standage, family members, co-workers, friends, or caregivers and
Sebire, & Loney, 2008). Accelerometers are also popular care recipients. Continuous real-time records of this sort
in sleep research. For example, the Actigraph is a rela- are ideally suited for data-intensive analytic methods,
tively inexpensive wristwatch-like sensor that identifies such as dynamic modeling of social influence processes
and stores objective information about physical motion, (Mason, Conrey, & Smith, 2007).
yielding data that is highly correlated with more expen-
sive and intrusive sleep lab polysomnography (deSouza
et al., 2003). Accelerometer readings can also be used with TRACE MEASURES
activity recognition algorithms to identify unique motion-
activity patterns, such as walking, eating, working on a Some social behaviors, attitudes, cognitions, and emotions
computer, gesturing, and talking on the telephone, which, leave physical traces in their wake. Bumper stickers on
once identified, might generate a signal to participants to cars, political buttons pinned to overcoats, and posters of
record event-contingent ratings about their thoughts and icons pinned to bedroom walls are all used to convey ele-
feelings (Choudhury et al., 2008). ments of attitudes, values, and identity to others. The fact
Although accelerometers are rare in social-personality that some phenomena leave residue in the physical envi-
psychology, they seem useful for examining hypotheses ronment raises the possibility of assessing psychological
about the relationship between activation level and mood, phenomena by examining the physical traces they
or about movement and activity patterns associated with produce.
individual differences, for describing patterned responses Perhaps the most ambitious and wide-reaching effort
to social stimuli (e.g., freezing or fleeing a fearful stimu- to understand behavior from physical traces was William
lus, orienting one’s body toward or away from a potential Rathje’s garbage project. Rathje reasoned that just as
interaction partner), or for determining whether social archeologists use ancient refuse to learn about the behavior
interaction partners synchronize their movement. They of people who lived many millennia ago, he too could
are also likely to be helpful in applications of social psy- use modern-day garbage to get insight into contemporary
chological theories to health, where objective accounts of behavior. Thus, in 1973 he founded the garbage project
activity levels are desired. at the University of Arizona with the goal of using refuse
to understand contemporary patterns of consumption. In
contrast to traditional studies, which had relied on ques-
Location Mapping
tionnaires, surveys, government documents, or industry
Global positioning systems (GPS) have become highly records, the garbage project was grounded in hands-on
precise, capable of identifying a person’s location within sorting of quantifiable bits and pieces of garbage. Instead
a foot or so. Moreover, this technology is readily avail- of self-reports, the “garbologists” made inferences about
able. Pentland notes that “the majority of adults already consumer behavior directly from the material realities peo-
carry a microphone and location sensor in the form of a ple left outside their houses. The investigators often found
mobile phone, and that these sensors are packaged with discrepancies between the answers given in self-reports
computational horsepower similar to that found in desk- and those provided by their refuse analyses. For example,
top computers” (2007, p. 59). Location can be informative in one study, “front door” interview data suggested beer
for social-psychological research. For example, location consumption occurred in only 15% of the homes and was
readings might be used to identify behavior settings of no higher than eight cans per week, whereas “back door”
research interest, such as schools, work, or nature, which, garbage analyses revealed that beer was consumed in 77%
Trace Measures 105

of the homes, with 54% of them exceeding the supposed on the windowsill (e.g., a twig from a tree once planted
maximum of eight cans (Rathje & Hughes, 1975). In addi- with an uncle who has since passed away), and pho-
tion to fresh sorts of garbage bags left outside houses, the tos of family on the refrigerator (e.g., images of an
garbage project researchers also examined other sources, absent grandparent to evoke feelings of belonging and
such as “core samples” drilled out from deep inside security).
landfills. • Identity Claims. One of the ways in which people make
At a very broad level, all trace measures rely on the pro- spaces their own is by adorning them with “identity
cesses of either erosion or accretion (Webb et al., 1981). claims”—deliberate symbolic statements about how
A classic and widely cited example of erosion came from they would like to be regarded (Baumeister, 1982;
staff at the Chicago Museum of Science and Industry, who Swann, 1987; Swann, Rentfrow, & Guinn, 2003).
noticed that the floor tiles in front of the hatching chicks Posters, awards, photos, trinkets, and other memen-
exhibit had to be replaced more frequently than those in tos are often displayed in the service of making such
front of other exhibits, providing an index of the relative statements. Such signals can be split into two broad
popularity of different exhibits. Staying in the museum categories: Self-directed identity claims are symbolic
context, Webb et al. suggest that accretion measures too statements made by occupants for their own benefit,
could be used to track the popularity of exhibits with glass intended to reinforce their self-views (e.g., displaying
fronts by counting the numbers of nose-prints on the glass, a fountain pen awarded in a high-school science fair).
even making estimates of the ages of the viewers from the Other-directed identity claims are symbolic statements
heights of the prints. (e.g., displaying a poster of Malcolm X) about attitudes
Building on this tradition, the personal environments and values made to others about how one would like to
that individuals craft around themselves, such as offices and be regarded.
bedrooms, could be rich with information about the occu- Identity claims consist of things individuals do
pants (Gosling et al., 2002). It seems likely, for example, deliberately to their spaces, even if the occupants do
that the pictures a person selects to hang on her walls, the not direct conscious attention to the psychological goals
books she chooses to read, and the way she arranges underlying their actions; thus, even if taping a humorous
the items that fill the space around her all reflect aspects article from the satirical newspaper, The Onion, to one’s
of her attitudes, behaviors, values, and self-views. Three office door is driven by the goal of projecting a quirky
different mechanisms can be delineated by which people nerdy cynical persona to others, it is likely that the
can have an impact on the environments around them and, occupant will experience the motive as “I just thought it
in turn, how physical environments can serve as reposito- was funny.” Of course, some identity claims are made
ries of individual expression (Gosling et al., 2002; Gosling, deliberately, but that does not mean they are disingenu-
Gaddis, & Vazire, 2008). Broadly, people alter their spaces ous; self-verification theory suggests that people make
for three reasons: They want to affect how they think and many of these claims not to create a false impression
feel, they want to broadcast information about themselves, but to induce others to see them as they genuinely see
and they inadvertently affect their spaces in the course of themselves (Swann, this volume; Swann et al., 2002).
their everyday behaviors. Nonetheless, it is still possible that some claims are made
with the explicit intention of fooling others (e.g., falsely
• Thought and Feeling Regulators. Personal environments claiming to admire a rock band with street credibility by
are the contexts for a wide range of activities, ranging wearing the band’s logo on a t-shirt). Of course, there
from relaxing and reminiscing to working and playing. are numerous obstacles to pulling off a successful ruse
The effectiveness with which these activities can be ac- (Gosling, 2008).
complished may be affected by the physical and ambient • Behavioral Residue. Many behaviors leave some kind
qualities of the space. It can be hard to relax with a lot of of discernible residue in their wake. Given that large
noise around and it is difficult to concentrate when sur- quantities of behavior occur in personal environments, it
rounded by distractions. Specific memories, thoughts, is reasonable to suppose that these environments might
and emotions can be evoked by mementos and photos accumulate a fair amount of residue. Interior behavioral
of people, pets, and places. As a result, many items residue refers to the physical traces in an environment
within an environment owe their presence to their abil- of activities conducted within that space (e.g., an
ity to affect the feelings and thoughts of the occupant. organized desk). Exterior behavioral residue refers to
Elements used to regulate emotions and thoughts could remnants of past activities and material preparations
include the music on an iPod (e.g., upbeat music to get for activities that will take place elsewhere (e.g., a
a person pumped up for a night on the town), keepsakes snowboard).
106 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

The elements in people’s spaces are psychologically and neat appearance), and other malleable elements of
interesting phenomena in their own right but they can also appearance (e.g., in female targets, plucked eyebrows and
be used to measure occupants’ behaviors, attitudes, values, cleavage showing; Vazire et al., 2008). In other words,
goals, and self-views. For example, cohabiting couples physical appearance often holds clues to an individual’s
may use jointly acquired objects to signal things to oth- attitudes, values, intentions, behaviors, and self-views.
ers about their couple identity (e.g., prominently displayed One study of the links between human territoriality and
honeymoon photos) or to remind themselves of special aggression relied on trace measures of territoriality found
moments together (e.g., pebble from a beach where they on cars (Szlemko, Benfield, Bell, Deffenbacher, & Troup,
had their first kiss); as a result, these objects may reflect 2008). Starting with a definition of territoriality as a set
the couples’ relationship closeness, commitment, and of behaviors and cognitions that a person exhibits based
dyadic adjustment (Arriaga, Goodfriend, & Lohmann, on perceived ownership of space, bumper stickers, window
2004; Lohmann, Arriaga, & Goodfriend, 2003). To date, only decals, and other forms personalization served as an index
a few measures of physical spaces have been developed of territoriality. As predicted, drivers of cars with territo-
(Gosling, Craik, Martin, & Pryor, 2005a, 2005b). As a riality markers scored higher on tests of driver aggression
result, environmental evidence of social psychological and lower on the use of constructive expressions of anger
behaviors has remained largely untapped despite interest in behind the wheel.
the topic in the 1960s and 1970s. As with all methods, trace measures have their own
Nonetheless, the potential value of trace measures advantages and disadvantages. One drawback is that it may
to social psychologists is great, especially given that the be difficult to know who was responsible for a particular
environmental manifestations of attitudes, values, and self- trace or whether the action presumed to be responsible for
views extend well beyond physical environments. Many the trace actually caused it. For example, it may be diffi-
kinds of environments other than physical spaces (and the cult to tell whether the current or previous owner placed a
possessions that fill them) could furnish information about bumper sticker on a car. As with any measure, the onus is
people. Just as people craft their physical spaces, they on the researcher to establish its construct validity. Thus,
also select and mould their auditory and social environ- the study of territoriality markers in cars validated the car
ments (Mehl, Gosling, & Pennebaker, 2006; Rentfrow & owners’ reports of bumper stickers, window decals, and
Gosling, 2003, 2006). Just as people physically dwell so on with codings by judges made from photographs of
in houses and offices, they dwell virtually in online participants’ cars; the investigators also demonstrated that
environments like virtual worlds, personal websites, and the presence of markers showed expected patterns of cor-
social-networking portals (e.g., Facebook.com; Back, relations with other variables such as the condition of the
Schmukle, & Egloff, 2008; Vazire & Gosling, 2004). Just vehicle and the owner’s attachment to it (Szlemko et al.,
as people leave traces of their actions, intentions, and 2008).
values in their permanent spaces, they also leave traces Past research can be used to validate trace measures.
in other immediate surroundings such as their cars (e.g., For example, research on “social snacks” supports the idea
dings in the door, unpaid scrunched-up parking tickets in that pictures of loved ones kept in one’s wallet or sitting on
the foot well, bumper stickers) or clothing (e.g., muddy one’s desk are used as emotion regulation strategies buffer-
running shoes, mismatched socks, a t-shirt or button with ing the pain of social isolation. In one study, participants
a rock band or political icon on it; Alpers & Gerdes, 2006; were assigned to bring to the lab either a photo of a friend
Vazire, Naumann, Rentfrow, & Gosling, 2008). or a photo of a favorite celebrity (Gardner, Pickett, &
Thus, many environments may be used to obtain infor- Knowles, 2005). Participants put the photos on the desks in
mation about people. Gosling et al.’s (2002) model was front of them and were then asked to recall in vivid detail
developed in the context of two studies of physical envi- an experience of being rejected by other people. Unlike the
ronments but it can easily be applied more widely. For people who had a picture of a celebrity in front of them,
example, the mechanisms linking individuals to their the people who were looking at a friend’s image did not
environments can be applied to physical appearance— experience a drop in mood.
hairstyle and clothing can reflect identity claims, clothing The validity of behavioral residue may also vary within
and accessories can provide evidence of past or anticipated a category. For example, some music genres are more
behaviors, or even levels of sexual motivation (Haselton, tightly associated than other genres with particular values,
Mortezaie, Pillsworth, Bleske-Rechek, & Frederick, self-views, preferences, and activities. For example, the
2007). In the domain of personality, narcissism can be stereotype that contemporary religious music fans place
expressed in terms of the kinds of clothes that people wear high importance on values like forgiveness, inner harmony,
(e.g., expensive, stylish), their condition (e.g., organized love, and salvation shows some accuracy, but the stereotype
Conclusion 107

that rap fans place low importance on values like a world laboratory, emphasizing what it offers for the field while at
of beauty, inner harmony, intellect, and wisdom has lit- the same time acknowledging its limitations. Whatever one’s
tle accuracy (Rentfrow & Gosling, 2007). Such findings preferences for working inside or outside of the laboratory,
would inform researchers who use music-preference infor- we hope it is apparent that we see non-laboratory studies as
mation (e.g., from iPods, CD collections) as indicators of neither more nor less desirable than laboratory work. Just
values held by participants. as an artist or a craftsperson uses different tools to carry
Findings that converge across methods are particularly out different parts of a creative work, laboratory and non-
valuable because they both underscore the robustness of laboratory settings can provide social psychologists with dif-
the findings and cross-validate the methods. One study ferent, and if used appropriately, complementary tools for our
found converging evidence for the psychological underpin- creative work. Both are intended to give researchers useful
nings of political orientation by gathering data based on instruments for testing important theories and hypotheses
self-views, behavioral codings of social interactions, and about social behavior. And more important than the par-
records of behavioral residue (Carney, Jost, Gosling, & ticular methods outlined here, we hope this chapter will
Potter, 2008). In particular, liberals’ tendencies to be open- serve to stimulate researchers to remain vigilant for new
minded, creative, and interested in novelty seeking was opportunities to examine social psychological phenomena
reflected in high self-ratings on openness, in their tendency in their natural habitats.
to smile and to be expressive and engaged in social inter- Most commentators agree in principle that the most
actions, and for their bedrooms to contain a wide variety valid theories and findings are those that have been tested
of books (including books on travel and feminism), music with multiple methods in diverse settings, as noted in the
(including world and classical genres), art supplies, and introduction to this chapter. Nevertheless, current social
cultural memorabilia. Conservatives’ need for order psychological practice (as, we hasten to note, in many
and conventionality was reflected in high self-ratings on other disciplines) often falls short of this lofty standard.
conscientiousness and low ratings on openness, in their Instead, researchers tend to stick with an established para-
tendency to be detached and disengaged in social inter- digm, more often conducted in the laboratory with college
actions, and for their bedrooms to contain organizational students than anywhere else. Extending a laboratory para-
items (e.g., event calendars), conventional décor (e.g., digm to non-laboratory settings may sometimes require
sports paraphernalia, American flags), and be generally greater effort and time than conducting an additional labo-
neat, clean, and organized. ratory replication but researchers who step outside the
One major advantage of studying behavioral residue laboratory are often rewarded with increased validity and
rather than behavior itself is that it overcomes some of the generalizability of their findings.
significant practical challenges associated with observ- Kurt Lewin’s call for action-oriented, real-world-
ing behavior in natural settings (Barker, 1968; Barker & relevant research, now well-aged more than a half-century,
Wright, 1951; Craik, 2000; Gosling, John, Craik, & Robins, still inspires many social psychologists. Were Lewin still
1998; Hektner et al., 2007; Mehl et al., 2006). Moreover, alive, we think he would be even more enthusiastic today
whereas self-reports of behavior may underestimate actual about the prospects for conducting rigorous, theoreti-
behavioral occurrences, the existence of behavioral residue cally informative and practically useful research outside
(e.g., a beer can in the trash) is usually a good sign that the of the laboratory. As we have tried to illustrate, the tools
behavior actually occurred. A final major benefit of resi- available for such research are far more sophisticated
due is the advantage of aggregation. A single behavior is today than they were in Lewin’s era. The Internet affords
less reliable than a behavioral trend and physical spaces unparalleled access to large and diverse samples and data-
reflect behavioral trends (Epstein, 1983). For example, bases. Advances in computerization, miniaturization, and
whereas even a generally organized person may occasion- cellular technology have spawned devices capable of pro-
ally fail to return a CD to its case and file it in the right slot, viding extensively detailed accounts of behavior, from
it is unlikely that such a person would have a chaotic CD internal biological events to subjective states and affects
collection, because a disorganized collection of CDs is the to descriptions of the person’s environment. Statistical
result of repeatedly engaging in similar actions. methods to take advantage of these data and yield finer
insights are becoming ever more sophisticated. In other
words, advancing technology has made the non-laboratory
CONCLUSION environment an increasingly viable and fertile site for the
generation of social-psychological knowledge. There is
In this chapter we have tried to describe the rationale for little doubt that these technological advances will continue
conducting social-psychological research outside of the and likely accelerate. As they do they will enhance our
108 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

prospects for asking and answering interesting and impor- Baumeister, R. F. (1982). A self-presentational view of social phenomena.
Psychological Bulletin, 91, 3–26.
tant questions in social psychology and beyond.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as the
science of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to
actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 396–403.
Berkowitz, L., & Donnerstein, E. (1982). External validity is more than
REFERENCES skin deep: Some answers to criticisms of laboratory experiments.
American Psychologist, 37, 245–257.
Ajilore, O., Stickgold, R., Rittenhouse, C. D., & Hobson, J. A. (1995). Bevans, G. (1913). How working men spend their time. New York:
Nightcap: Laboratory and home-based evaluation of a portable sleep Columbia University Press.
monitor. Psychophysiology, 32, 92–98. Birnbaum, G. E., Reis, H. T., & Mikulincer, M. (2006). When sex is more
Almeida, D. M. (2005). Resilience and vulnerability to daily stressors than just sex: Attachment orientations, sexual experience, and relationship
assessed via diary methods. Current Directions in Psychological quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 929–943.
Science, 14, 64–68. Birnbaum, M. H. (2001). Introduction to behavioral research on the
Alpers, G. W., & Gerdes, A. B. M. (2006). Another look at “look-alikes”: Internet. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Can judges match belongings with their owners? Journal of Individual Birnbaum, M. H. (2004). Human research and data collection via the
Differences, 27, 38–41. Internet. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 803–832.
Altman, I., & Haythorn, W. W. (1965). Interpersonal exchange in isolation. Birnbaum, M. H. (in press). An overview of major techniques of web-based
Sociometry, 28, 411–426. research. In S. D. Gosling & J. A. Johnson (Eds.), Advanced methods
Amir, Y. (1969). Contact hypothesis in ethnic relations. Psychological for behavioral research on the Internet. Washington, DC: American
Bulletin, 71, 319–342. Psychological Association.
Aron, A., Paris, M., & Aron, E. N. (1995). Falling in love: Prospective Blascovich, J. (2000). Psychophysiological methods. In H. T. Reis &
studies of self-concept change. Journal of Personality and Social C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and person-
Psychology, 69, 1102–1112. ality psychology (pp. 117–137). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Aronson, E. (2004). Reducing hostility and building compassion: Lessons Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life
from the jigsaw classroom. In A. G. Miller (Ed.), The social psychol- as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579–616.
ogy of good and evil (pp. 469–488). New York: Guilford Press. Bolger, N. & Schilling, E. A. (1991). Personality and the problems of
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1968). Experimentation in social psychol- everyday life: The role of neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to
ogy. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psy- daily stressors. Journal of Personality, 59, 355–386.
chology (2nd ed., vol. 1, pp. 1–79). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Bolger, N., & Zuckerman, A. (1995). A framework for studying personal-
Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Brewer, M. B. (1998). Experimentation in ity in the stress process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), 69, 890–902.
The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., vol. 1, pp. 99–142). Bolger, N., Zuckerman, A., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Invisible support and
New York: McGraw-Hill. adjustment to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Arriaga, X. B., Goodfriend, W., & Lohmann, A. (2004). Beyond the indi- 79, 953–961.
vidual: Concomitants of closeness in the social and physical environ- Brewer, M. B. (2000). Research design and issues of validity. In H. T. Reis
ment. In D. J. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and & C. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social psychol-
intimacy (pp. 287–303). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. ogy, (pp. 3–16). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Asch, S. E. (1952). Social psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall. Brondolo, E., Karlin, W., Alexander, K., Bobrow, A., & Schwartz, J. (1999).
Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C. & Egloff, B. (2008). How extraverted is Workday communication and ambulatory blood pressure: Implications
honey.bunny77@hotmail.de? Inferring personality traits from email for the reactivity hypothesis. Psychophysiology, 36, 86–94.
addresses. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1116–1122. Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psy-
Bakeman, R. (2000). Behavioral observation and coding. In H. T. Reis & chological experiments. (2nd ed.). Berkeley: University of California
C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and per- Press.
sonality psychology (pp. 138–159). New York: Cambridge University Bryk, A. S. & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models.
Press. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Observing interaction: An Burleson, M., Trevathan, W. R., & Todd, M. (2007). In the mood for love,
introduction to sequential analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge or vice versa? Understanding the relations among physical affection,
University Press. sexual activity, mood, and stress in the daily lives of mid-aged women.
Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological psychology: Concepts and methods for Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 357–368.
studying the environment of human behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford Burns, T. (1954). The directions of activity and communication in a depart-
University Press. mental executive group: A quantitative study in a British engineering
Barker, R. G., & Wright, H. F. (1951). One boy’s day. A specimen record factory with a self-recording technique. Human Relations, 7, 73–97.
of behavior. New York: Harper & Row. Bushman, B. J. (1988). The effects of apparel on compliance: A field
Barrett, L. F., & Barrett, D. J. (2001). An introduction to computerized experiment with a female authority figure. Personality and Social
experience sampling in psychology. Social Science Computer Review, Psychology Bulletin, 14, 459–467.
19, 175–185. Buunk, B. P., & Peeters, M. C. W. (1994). Stress at work, social support
Bartholomew, K., Henderson, A. J. Z., & Marcia, J. E. (2000). Coded semi- and companionship: Towards an event-contingent recording approach.
structured interviews in social psychological research. In H. T. Reis Work & Stress, 8, 177–190.
& C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and Campbell, C. G., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (in press). The new person-
personality psychology (pp. 286–312). New York: Cambridge specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological
University Press. Science.
References 109

Campbell, D. T. (1957). Factors relevant to the validity of experiments in Dechêne, A., Stahl, C., Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (under review). Mix me a
social settings. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 297–312. list: Repeated presentation may not be sufficient for the Truth Effect
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant vali- and the Mere Exposure Effect. Manuscript under review.
dation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, Delespaul, P. A. E. G. (1992). Technical note: Devices and time-sampling
56, 81–105. procedures. In M. W. de Vries (Ed.), The experience of psychopathol-
Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives ogy: Investigating mental disorders in their natural settings (pp. 363–
of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, 373). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology, 29, 807–840. deSouza, L., Benedito-Silva A. A., Nogueira Pires, M. L., Poyares, D.,
Choudhury, T., Consolvo, S., Harrison, B., Hightower, J., LaMarca, A., Tufik, S., & Calil, M. H. (2003). Further validation of actigraphy for
LeGrand, L., et al. (2008). The mobile sensing platform: An embedded sleep studies. Sleep, 26, 81–85.
activity recognition system. IEEE Pervasive Computing, 7, 32–41. deVries, M. W. (1992). The experience of psychopathology in natural set-
Christensen, T. C., Barrett, L. F., Bliss-Moreau, E., Lebo, K., & Kaschub, tings: Introduction and illustration of variables. In M. W. deVries (Ed.),
C. (2003). A practical guide to experience-sampling procedures. The experience of psychopathology: Investigating mental disorders
Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 53–78. in their natural settings (pp. 3–26). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Cialdini, R. B. (2009). We have to break up. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 4, 5–6. DeYoung, C. G., Hasher, L., Djikic, M., Criger, B., & Peterson, J. B. (2007).
Morning people are stable people: Circadian rhythm and the higher-
Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thorne, A., Walker, M. R., Freeman, S., &
order factors of the Big Five. Personality and Individual Differences,
Sloan, L. R. (1976). Basking in reflected glory: Three (football) field
43, 267–276.
studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 366–375.
Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-
Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Leeka, J. (1989). Diurnal variation in the posi-
fulfilling prophecy in close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and
tive affects. Motivation and Emotion, 13, 205–234.
rejection by romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple Psychology, 75, 545–560.
regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.).
Epstein, S. (1983). Aggregation and beyond: Some basic issues on the pre-
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
diction of behavior. Journal of Personality, 51, 360–392.
Cohn, M. A., Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Linguistic indica-
tors of psychological change after September 11, 2001. Psychological Ewart, C., & Kolodner, K. B. (1994). Negative affect, gender, and expres-
Science, 15, 687–693. sive style predict elevated ambulatory blood pressure in adolescents.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 596–605.
Collins, R. L., Kashdan, T. B., & Gollnisch, G. (2003). The feasibility
of using cellular phones to collect ecological momentary assessment Fahrenberg, J., & Myrtek, M. (Eds.) (2001). Progress in ambulatory
data: Application to alcohol consumption. Experimental and Clinical assessment: Computer-assisted psychological and psychophysiologi-
Psychopharmacology, 11, 73–78. cal methods in monitoring and field studies. Kirkland, WA: Hogrefe
and Huber.
Conner, M. T., Perugini, M., O’Gorman, R., Ayres, K., & Prestwich, A.
(2007). Relations between implicit and explicit measures of attitudes Ferguson, E., & Chandler, S. (2005). A stage model of blood donor behav-
and measures of behavior: Evidence of moderation by individual dif- iour: Assessing volunteer behaviour. Journal of Health Psychology,
ference variables. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 10, 359–372.
1727–1740. Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy
Conner, T., & Barrett, L. F. (2005). Implicit self-attitudes predict spontane- fails. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
ous affect in daily life. Emotion, 5, 476–488. Finkel, E. J., & Eastwick, P. W. (2008). Speed-dating. Current Directions
Conner, T., Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Tennen, H. (2007). Idiographic in Psychological Science, 17, 193–197.
personality: The theory and practice of experience sampling. In Fleeson, W. (2004). Moving personality beyond the person–situation
R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. Kreuger (Eds.), Handbook of research debate: The challenge and the opportunity of within-person variability.
methods in personality psychology (pp.79–98). New York: Guilford. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 83–87.
Conti, R. (2000). Self-recording of healthy behavior. Unpublished manu- Fraley, R. C. (2004). How to conduct behavioral research over the Internet:
script, Colgate University. A beginner ’s guide to HTML and CGI/Perl. New York: Guilford.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design & Fraley, R. C., & Roberts, B. W. (2005). Patterns of continuity: A dynamic
analysis issues for field settings. Chicago: Rand-McNally. model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in
Cook, T. D., & Groom, C. (2004). The methodological assumptions of psychological constructs across the life course. Psychological Review,
social psychology: The mutual dependence of substantive theory and 112, 60–74.
method choice. In C. Sansone, C. Morf, & A. T. Panter (Eds.), The
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Airport separations: A naturalistic
Sage handbook of methods in social psychology (pp. 19–44). Thousand
study of adult attachment dynamics in separating couples. Journal of
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1198–1212.
Craik, K. H. (2000). The lived day of an individual: A person-environ-
Funder, D. C. (2006). Towards a resolution of the personality triad: Persons,
ment perspective. In W. B. Walsh, K. H. Craik, & R. H. Price (Eds.),
situations, and behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 21–34.
Person–environment psychology: New directions and perspectives
(pp. 233–266). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Gable, S. L., & Reis, H. T. (1999). Now and then, them and us, this and
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R. W. & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of that: Studying relationships across time, partner, context, and person.
adolescent activity and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Personal Relationships, 6, 415–432.
6, 281–294. Gable, S. L., & Reis, H. T. (2001). Appetitive and aversive social interaction. In
Davidovitz, R., Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Izsak, R., & Popper, M. J. H. Harvey & A. E. Wenzel (Eds.), Close romantic relationship mainte-
(2007). Leaders as attachment figures: Leaders’ attachment orienta- nance and enhancement (pp. 169–194). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
tions predict leadership-related mental representations and followers’ Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Downey, G. (2003). He said, she said: A quasi-
performance and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social signal detection analysis of daily interactions between close relation-
Psychology, 93, 632–650. ship partners. Psychological Science, 14, 100–105.
110 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Behavioral activation Haslam, S. A. & McGarty, C. (2004). In C. Sansone, C. Morf, & A. T. Panter
and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social (Eds.), The Sage handbook of methods in social psychology (pp. 237–
Psychology, 78, 1135–1149. 264). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gaertner, J., Elsner, F., Pollmann-Dahmen, K., Radbruch, L., & Hasler, B., Mehl, M. R., Bootzin, R., & Vazire, S. (2008). Preliminary evi-
Sabatowski, R. (2004). Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 28, dence of diurnal rhythms in everyday behaviors associated with posi-
259–267. tive affect. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1537–1546.
Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., & Knowles, M. L. (2005). Social “snacking” Hastie, R., & Stasser, G. (2000). Computer simulation methods for social
and social “shielding”: The satisfaction of belonging needs through the psychology. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research
use of social symbols and the social self. In K. Williams, J. Forgas, & methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 85–114). New York:
W. von Hippel (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism,social exclusion, Cambridge University Press.
rejection, and bullying. New York: Psychology Press. Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T.
Glanz, K., Murphy, S., Moylan, J., Evensen, D., & Curb, J. D. (2006). (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity, psycho-
Improving dietary self-monitoring and adherence with hand-held com- social context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social
puters: A pilot study. American Journal of Health Promotion, 20, Psychology, 85, 105–120.
165–170. Hektner, J. M., Schmidt, J. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Experience
Glaser, J., Dixit, J., & Green, D. P. (2002) Studying hate crime with the sampling method: Measuring the quality of everyday life. Thousand
Internet: What makes racists advocate racial violence? Journal of Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Social Issues, 58, 177–193. Helmreich, R. (1975). Applied social psychology: The unfulfilled promise.
Goodwin, M. S., Velicer, W. F., & Intille, S. S. (2008). Telemetric moni- Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1, 548–560.
toring in the behavior sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 40, Henry, P. J. (2008). College sophomores in the laboratory redux: Influences
328–341. of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of the nature of prej-
Gosling, S. D. (2008). Snoop: What your stuff says about you. New York: udice. Psychological Inquiry, 19, 49–71.
Basic books. Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The Social Readjustment Rating
Gosling, S. D., & Bonnenburg, A. V. (1998). An integrative approach to Scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213–218.
personality research in anthrozoology: Ratings of six species of pets House, J. S. (1977). The three faces of social psychology. Sociometry, 40,
and their owners. Anthrozoös, 11, 184–156. 161–177.
Gosling, S. D., Craik, K. H., Martin, N. R., & Pryor, M. R. (2005a). Material Hufford, M. R. (2007). Special methodological challenges and opportuni-
attributes of Personal Living Spaces. Home Cultures, 2, 51–88. ties in Ecological Momentary Assessment. In A. S. Stone, S. Shiffman,
Gosling, S. D., Craik, K. H., Martin, N. R., & Pryor, M. R. (2005b). The A. A. Atienza, & L. Nebeling (Eds.), The science of real-time data
Personal Living Space Cue Inventory: An analysis and evaluation. capture (pp. 54–75). New York: Oxford University Press.
Environment and Behavior, 37, 683–705. Hufford, M. R., Shields, A. L., Shiffman, S., Paty, J., & Balabanis, M.
Gosling, S. D., Gaddis, S., & Vazire, S. (2008). First impressions based on the (2002). Reactivity to ecological momentary assessment: An exam-
environments we create and inhabit. In N. Ambady, & J. J. Skowronski ple using undergraduate problem drinkers. Psychology of Addictive
(Eds.), First Impressions (pp. 334–356). New York: Guilford. Behaviors, 16, 205–211.
Gosling, S. D., John, O. P., Craik, K. H., & Robins, R. W. (1998). Do peo- Ickes, W. (1983). A basic paradigm for the study of unstructured dyadic
ple know how they behave? Self-reported act frequencies compared interaction. New Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral
with on-line codings by observers. Journal of Personality and Social Science, 15, 5–21.
Psychology, 74, 1337–1349. Intille, S. S. (2007). Technological innovations enabling automatic, con-
Gosling. S. D., & Johnson, J. A. (in press) Advanced methods for text-sensitive ecological momentary assessment. In A. S. Stone, S.
behavioral research on the Internet. Washington, DC: American Shiffman, A. A. Atienza, & L. Nebeling (Eds.), The science of real-
Psychological Association. time data capture (pp. 308–337). New York: Oxford University Press.
Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A room Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000), When choice is demotivating: Can
with a cue: Judgments of personality based on offices and bedrooms. one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Social
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379–398. Psychology, 79, 995–1006.
Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Graziano, W. G. (2000). Beyond the school
trust Web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions yard: Relationships as moderators of daily interpersonal conflict.
about Internet questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59, 93–104. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 923–935.
Green, A. S., Rafaeli, E., Bolger, N., Shrout, P. E., & Reis, H. T. (2006). Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Smith, K. (2007). The state of coopera-
Paper or plastic? Data equivalence in paper and electronic diaries. tive learning in postsecondary and professional settings. Educational
Psychological Methods, 11, 87–105. Psychology Review, 19, 15–29.
Groves, R. M., Fowler, F. J., Jr., Couper, M. P., Lepkowski, J. M., Johnson, J. A. (2005). Ascertaining the validity of web-based personality
Singer, E., & Tourangeau, R. (2004). Survey methodology. New York: inventories. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 103–129.
Wiley-Interscience. Joinson, A. N., McKenna, K., Postmes, T., & Reips, U.-D. (Eds.) (2007).
Hager, R. L. (2006). Television viewing and physical activity in children. The Oxford handbook of Internet psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford
Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 656–661. University Press.
Hammond, K. R. (1998). Ecological validity: Then and now. Retrieved Jones, E. E. (1985). Major developments in social psychology during the
February 11, 2009, from http://www.albany.edu/cpr/brunswik/notes/ past five decades. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook
essay2.html of social psychology (3rd ed., vol. 1, pp. 47–107). New York: Random
Haselton, M. G., Mortezaie, M., Pillsworth, E. G., Bleske-Rechek, A., & House.
Frederick, D. A. (2007). Ovulatory shifts in human female orna- Josephson, W. L. (1987). Television violence and children’s aggression:
mentation: Near ovulation, women dress to impress. Hormones and Testing the priming, social script, and disinhibition predictions. Journal
Behavior, 51, 40–45. of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 882–890.
References 111

Kelley, H. H. (1992). Common-sense psychology and scientific psychol- Luce, K. H., Winzelberg, A. J., Das, S., Osborne, M. I., Bryson, S. W., &
ogy. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 1–23. Taylor, C. B. (2007). Reliability of self-report: Paper versus online
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social administration. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1384–1389.
psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The Mahoney, M. J., Moura, N. G., & Wade, T. C. (1973). Relative efficacy of
handbook of social psychology (vol. 1, 4th ed., pp. 233–265). self-reward, self-punishment, and self-monitoring techniques for weight
New York: McGraw-Hill. loss. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40, 404–407.
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Cook, W. L. (2006). Dyadic data analysis. Mangan, M., & Reips, U.-D. (2007). Sleep, sex, and the Web: Surveying
New York: Guilford. the difficult-to-reach clinical population suffering from sexsomnia.
Kerig, P. K., & Lindahl, K. M. (Eds.). (2001). Family observational coding Behavior Research Methods, 39, 233–236.
systems: Resources for systemic research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Marcus, B., Machilek, F., & Schütz, A. (2006). Personality in cyberspace:
Kirby, K. C., Fowler, S. A., & Baer, D. M. (1991). Reactivity in self-record- Personal websites as media for personality expressions and impres-
ing: Obtrusiveness of recording procedure and peer comments. Journal sions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 1014–1031.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 487–498. Mason, W. A., Conrey, F. R., & Smith, E. R. (2007). Situating social influ-
ence processes: Dynamic, multidirectional flows of influence within
Korotitsch, W. J., & Nelson-Gray, R. O. (1999). An overview of self-
social networks. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11,
monitoring research in assessment and treatment. Psychological
279–300.
Assessment, 11, 415–425.
Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2006). The thrill of victory and the
Krantz, J. H. (2001). Stimulus delivery on the Web: What can be presented
agony of defeat: Spontaneous expressions of medal winners of the 2004
when calibration isn’t possible? In U.-D. Reips & M. Bosnjak (Eds.),
Athens Olympic games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Dimensions of Internet science (pp. 113–130). Lengerich, Germany:
91, 568–581.
Pabst Science Publishers.
McClelland, D. C. (1957). Toward a science of personality psychology. In
Krantz, J. H., & Dalal, R. (2000). Validity of Web-based psychological
H. P. David & H. von Bracken (Eds.), Perspective in personality theory
research. In M. H. Birnbaum (Ed.), Psychological experiments on the
(pp. 355–382). New York: Basic Books.
Internet (pp. 35–60). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
McGrath, J. E. & Altermatt, T. W. (2000). Observation and analysis
Krantz, J. H., & Williams, J. E. (in press). Using media for Web-based
of group interaction over time: Some methodological and strategic
research. In S. D. Gosling & J. A. Johnson (Eds.), Advanced methods
choices. In M. Hogg & R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell’s handbook of
for behavioral research on the Internet. Washington, DC: American
social psychology: Vol. 3. Group processes (pp. 525–556). London:
Psychological Association.
Blackwell Publishers.
Krosnick, J. A., & Fabrigar, L. R. (in press). The handbook of question- McGuire, W. J. (1967). Some impending reorientations in social psy-
naire design. New York: Oxford University Press. chology: Some thoughts provoked by Kenneth Ring. Journal of
Lane, R., Reis, H. T., Peterson, D., Zareba, W., & Moss, A. (2008). Emotion Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 124–139.
in Long QT Syndrome. Manuscript in preparation. Mehl, M. R., Gosling, S. D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2006). Personality in
Langer, E. J., & Rodin, J. (1976). The effects of choice and enhanced per- its natural habitat: Manifestations and implicit folk theories of person-
sonal responsibility for the aged: A field experiment in an institutional ality in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90,
setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 191–198. 862–877.
Larsen, R. J. (1987). The stability of mood variability: A spectral analytic Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2003a). The sounds of social life: A
approach to daily mood assessments. Journal of Personality and Social psychometric analysis of students’ daily social environments and natu-
Psychology, 52, 1195–1204. ral conversations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
Larsen, R. J., & Kasimatis, M. (1991). Day-to-day physical symptoms: 857–870.
Individual differences in the occurrence, duration, and emotional Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2003b). The social dynamics of a cul-
concomitants of minor daily illnesses. Journal of Personality, 59, tural upheaval: Social interactions surrounding September 11, 2001.
387–423. Psychological Science, 14, 579–585.
Larson, R. W., Richards, M. H., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (1994). Divergent Mehl, M. R., Pennebaker, J. W., Crow, D. M., Dabbs, J., & Price, J. H.
worlds: The daily emotional experience of mothers and fathers in (2001). The Electronically Activated Recorded (EAR): A device for
the domestic and public spheres. Journal of Personality and Social sampling naturalistic daily activities and conversations. Behavior
Psychology, 67, 1034–1046. Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 33, 517–523.
Laurenceau, J.-P., & Bolger, N. (2005). Using diary methods to study mari- Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Ramirez-Esparza, N., Slatcher, R. B., &
tal and family processes. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 86–97. Pennebaker, J. W. (2007). Are women really more talkative than men?
Science, 317, 82.
Lebo, H. (2000). The UCLA Internet report: Surveying the digital future.
Retrieved July 1, 2004, from http://www.ccp.ucla.edu/UCLAInternet- Merrilees, C. E., Goeke-Morey, M., & Cummings, E. M. (2008). Do event-
Report-2000.pdf. contingent diaries about marital conflict change marital interactions?
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 253–262.
Lenhart, A. (2000). Who’s not online: 57% of those without Internet
access say they do not plan to log on. Retrieved July 1, 2004, from Milgram, S., Mann, L., & Harter, S. (1965). The lost-letter technique: A
http://www.pewinternet.org/reports/toc.asp?Report=21 tool of social research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 29, 437.
Levav, J., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2006). When questions change behavior: The Miller, G., Tybur, J. M., & Jordan, B. D. (2007). Ovulatory cycle effects
role of ease of representation. Psychological Science, 17, 207–213. on tip earnings by lap dancers: Economic evidence for human estrus?
Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 375–381.
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Harper.
Moghaddam, N. G., & Ferguson, E. (2007). Smoking, mood regulation,
Litt, M. D., Cooney, N. L., & Morse, P. (1998). Ecological momentary and personality: An event-sampling exploration of potential models
assessment (EMA) with treated alcoholics: Methodological problems and moderation. Journal of Personality, 75, 451–478.
and potential solutions. Health Psychology, 17, 48–52. Mohr, C. D., Armeli, S., Tennen, H., Carney, M. A., Affleck, G., &
Lohmann, A., Arriaga, X. B., & Goodfriend, W. (2003). Close relationships Hromi, A. (2001). Daily interpersonal experiences, context, and alcohol
and placemaking: Do objects in a couple’s home reflect couplehood? consumption: Crying in your beer and toasting good times. Journal of
Personal Relationships, 10, 439–451. Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 489–500.
112 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

Mook, D. G. (1983). In defense of external invalidity. American Piliavin, I. M., Rodin, J., & Piliavin, J. A. (1969). Good Samaritanism:
Psychologist, 38, 379–387. An underground phenomenon? Journal of Personality and Social
Moriarty, T. (1975). Crime, commitment, and the responsive bystander: Psychology, 13, 289–299.
Two field experiments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Rafaeli, E. (2009). Daily diary methods. In H. T. Reis & S. Sprecher (Eds.),
31, 370–376. Encyclopedia of human relationships. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Murray, S. L., Griffin, D. W., Rose, P., & Bellavia, G. M. (2003). Calibrating Raikkonen, K., Matthews, K. A., Flory, J. D., Owens, J. F., & Gump, B. B.
the sociometer: The relational contingencies of self-esteem. Journal of (1999). Effects of optimism, pessimism, and trait anxiety on ambulatory
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 63–84. blood pressure and mood during everyday life. Journal of Personality
Musch, J., & Reips, U.-D. (2000). A brief history of Web experimenting. and Social Psychology, 76, 104–113.
In M. H. Birnbaum (Ed.), Psychological experiments on the Internet Rathje, W. L., & Hughes, W. W. (1975). The garbage project as a nonreac-
(pp. 61–88). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. tive approach: Garbage in…garbage out? In H. W. Sinaiko, & L. A.
Nelson. R. O. (1977). Assessment and therapeutic functions of self-moni- Broedling (Eds.) Perspectives on attitude assessment: Surveys and
toring. In M. Hersen, R. M. Eisler & P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in their alternatives. Washington: Smithsonian Institution.
behavior modification (vol. 5, pp. 273–297). New York: Academic Raudenbush, S. W., Brennan, R. T., & Barnett, R. C. (1995). A multivariate
Press. hierarchical model for studying psychological change within married
Neubarth, W. (in press). Drag and drop: Using moveable objects to let par- couples. Journal of Family Psychology, 9, 161–174.
ticipants measure and rank items. In S. D. Gosling and J. A. Johnson Reips, U.-D. (2000). The Web Experiment Method: Advantages, disadvan-
(Eds.), Advanced methods for behavioral research on the Internet. tages, and solutions. In M. H. Birnbaum (Ed.), Psychological experi-
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. ments on the Internet (pp. 89–118). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Newcomb, T. M. (1961). The acquaintance process. New York: Holt, Reips, U.-D. (2002a). Standards for Internet-based experimenting.
Rinehart & Winston. Experimental Psychology, 49, 243–256.
Nezlek, J. B. (2003). Using multilevel random coefficient modeling to Reips, U.-D. (2002b). Internet-based psychological experimenting: Five
analyze social interaction diary data. Journal of Social and Personal dos and five don’ts. Social Science Computer Review, 20, 241–249.
Relationships, 20, 437–469. Reips, U.-D. (2002c). Theory and techniques of Web experimenting. In B.
Nezlek, J. B., Kafetsios, K., & Smith, C. V. (2008). Emotions in everyday Batinic, U.-D. Reips, & M. Bosnjak (Eds.), Online social sciences (pp.
social encounters: Correspondence between culture and self-construal. 229–250). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 366–372. Reips, U.-D., & Krantz, J. (in press). Experimental data. In S. D. Gosling
North, A. C. & Hargreaves, D. J. (1997). In-store music affects product & J. A. Johnson (Eds.), Advanced methods for behavioral research on
choice. Nature, 390, 132. the Internet. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (2001). Pleasures and subjective Reips, U.-D., & Neuhaus, C. (2002). WEXTOR: A Web-based tool for gen-
well-being. European Journal of Personality, 15, 153–167. erating and visualizing experimental designs and procedures. Behavior
Oppenheimer, D. M., Meyvis, T., & Davidenko, N. (2008). Instructional Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 234–240.
manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical Reis, H. T. (Ed.). (1983). Naturalistic approaches to studying social inter-
power. Manuscript under review. action. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Patrick, H., Knee, C. R., Canevello, A., & Lonsbary, C. (2007). The role Reis, H. T. (1994). Domains of experience: Investigating relationship
of need fulfillment in relationship functioning and well-being: A self- processes from three perspectives. In R. Erber & R. Gilmour (Eds.),
determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Theoretical frameworks for personal relationships (pp. 87–110).
Psychology, 92, 434–457. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pemberton, M. B., Insko, C. A., & Schopler, J. (1996). Memory for and Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2000). Event-sampling and other methods
experience of differential competitive behavior of individuals and groups. for studying everyday experience. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.),
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 953–966. Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology
Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a thera- (pp. 190–222). New York: Cambridge University Press.
peutic process. Psychological Science, 8, 162–166. Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., & Ryan, R. M. (2000).
Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological Daily well-being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness.
aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 419–435.
of Psychology, 54, 547. Reis, H. T., & Wheeler, L. (1991). Studying social interaction with the
Pennebaker, J. W., & Newtson, D. (1983). Observation of a unique event: Rochester Interaction Record. Advances in experimental social psy-
The psychological impact of the Mount Saint Helens Volcano. New chology (Vol. 24, pp. 269–318). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral Science, 15, Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2003). The do re mi’s of everyday life:
93–109. The structure and personality correlates of music preferences. Journal
Pentland, A. (2007). Automatic mapping and modeling of human networks. of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1236–1256.
Physica A, 378, 59–67. Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2006). Message in a ballad: Personality judg-
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup ments based on music preferences. Psychological Science, 17, 236–242.
contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2007). The content and validity of music-genre
751–783. stereotypes among college students. Psychology of Music, 35, 306–326.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1996). Addressing disturbing and disturbed Repetti, R. L. (1989). Effects of daily workload on subsequent behavior
consumer behavior: Is it necessary to change the way we conduct during marital interaction: The roles of social withdrawal and spouse
behavioral science? Journal of Marketing Research, 33, 1–8. support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 651–659.
Piasecki, T. M., Hufford, M. R., Solhan, M., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Ring, K. (1967). Experimental social psychology: Some sober ques-
Assessing clients in their natural environments with electronic dia- tions about some frivolous values. Journal of Experimental Social
ries: Rationale, benefits, limitations, and barriers. Psychological Psychology, 3, 113–123.
Assessment, 19, 25–43.
References 113

Robinson, J. P., & Godbey, G. (1997). Time for life: The surprising ways Sriram, N. (2002). The role of gender, ethnicity, and age in intergroup
Americans use their time. University Park: The Pennsylvania State behavior in a naturalistic setting. Applied Psychology: An International
University Press. Review, 51, 251–265.
Rodin, J., & Langer, E. J. (1977). Long-term effects of a control-relevant Standage, M., Sebire, S. J., & Loney, T. (2008). Does exercise motivation
intervention with the institutionalized aged. Journal of Personality and predict engagement in objectively assessed bouts of moderate-intensity
Social Psychology, 35, 897–902. exercise?: A self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Sport &
Rozin, P. (2001). Social psychology and science: Some lessons from Exercise Psychology, 30, 337–352.
Solomon Asch. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 2–14. Stone, A. A., Broderick, J. E., Porter, L. S., & Kaell, A. T. (1997). The
Rozin, P. (2009). What kind of empirical research should we publish, fund, experience of rheumatoid arthritis pain and fatigue: Examining momen-
and reward? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 435–439. tary reports and correlates over one week. Arthritis Care & Research,
10, 185–193.
Salovey, P., & Williams-Piehota, P. (2004). Field experiments in social
psychology: Message framing and the promotion of health protective Stone, A. A., Hedges, S. M., Neale, J. M., & Satin, M. S. (1985). Prospective
behaviors. American Behavioral Scientist, 47, 488–505. and cross-sectional mood reports offer no evidence of a “blue Monday”
phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49,
Sbarra, D. A., & Emery, R. E. (2005). The emotional sequelae of non-
129–134.
marital relationship dissolution: Analysis of change and intraindividual
variability over time. Personal Relationships, 12, 213–232. Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (1994). Ecological momentary assessment
(EMA) in behavioral medicine. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16,
Schwartz, J. E., & Stone, A. A. (1998). Strategies for analyzing ecological
199–202.
momentary assessment data. Health Psychology, 17, 6–16.
Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (2007). Capturing momentary self-report
Schwarz, N., Groves, R. M., & Schuman, H. (1998). Survey methods.
data: A proposal for reporting guidelines. In A. A. Stone, S. Shiffman,
In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook
A. A. Atienza, & L. Nebeling (Eds.), The science of real-time data
of social psychology (Vol. 1, 4th ed., pp. 143–179). New York:
capture (pp. 371–386). New York: Oxford University Press.
McGraw-Hill.
Stone, A. A., Shiffman, S., Atienza, A. A., & Nebeling, L. (Eds.). (2007).
Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the lab: Influences of a narrow
The science of real-time data capture. New York: Oxford University
data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of
Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530.
Stone, A. A., Shiffman, S., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Hufford, M.
Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C. W. (1961).
R. (2002). Patient non-compliance with paper diaries. British Medical
Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The Robbers Cave experiment.
Journal, 324, 1193–1194.
Norman, OK: University Book Exchange.
Stone, A. A.,Turkkan, J. S., Bachrach, C. A., Jobe, J. B., Kurtzman, H. S., &
Sherman, D. K., & Kim, H. S. (2005). Is there an “I” in “Team”? The
Cain, V. S. (Eds.). (2000). The science of self-report: Implications for
role of the self in group-serving judgments. Journal of Personality and
research and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Social Psychology, 88, 108–120.
Story, L. B., & Repetti, R. (2006). Daily occupational stressors and marital
Sherman, R. C., Buddie, A. M., Dragan, K. L., End, C. M., & Finney, L. J.
behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 690–700.
(1999). Twenty years of PSPB: Trends in content, design and analysis.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 177–187. Suls, J., Martin, R., & David, J. P. (1998). Person-environment fit and its limits:
Agreeableness, neuroticism, and emotional reactivity to interpersonal con-
Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., & Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological Momentary
flict. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 88–98.
Assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 1–32.
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet.
Shohat, M., & Musch, J. (2003). Online auctions as a research tool: a
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1038–1051.
field experiment on ethnic discrimination. Swiss Journal of Social
Psychology, 62, 139–145. Swann, W. B., Jr., Rentfrow, P. J., & Guinn, J. (2003). Self-verification:
The search for coherence. In M. Leary & J. Tagney (Eds.), Handbook
Shulman, A. D., & Berman, H. J. (1975). Role expectations about subjects
of self and identity. New York: Guilford Press.
and experimenters in psychological research. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 32, 368–380. Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson, M. J. (2001). Everyday
sexism: Evidence for its incidence, nature, and psychological impact
Silver, R. C. (2004). Conducting research after the 9/11 terrorist attacks:
from three daily diary studies. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 31–53.
Challenges and results. Families, Systems & Health, 22, 47–51.
Szlemko, W. J., Benfield, J. A., Bell, P. A., Deffenbacher, J. L., & Troup, L.
Silverman, I. (1971). Crisis in social psychology: The relevance of rel-
(2008). Territorial markings as predictors of drive aggression and road
evance. American Psychologist, 26, 583–584.
rage. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38, 1665–1688.
Silvia, P. J. (2002). Self-awareness and the regulation of emotional inten-
Takarangi, M. K., Garry, M., & Loftus, E. F. (2006). Dear diary, is plas-
sity. Self and Identity, 1, 3–10.
tic better than paper? I can’t remember: Comment on Green, Rafaeli,
Simonton, D. K. (2003). Qualitative and quantitative analyses of historical Bolger, Shrout, and Reis (2006). Psychological Methods, 11, 119–22.
data. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 617–640.
Tennen, H., Affleck, G., Coyne, J. C., Larsen, R. J., & DeLongis, A. (2006).
Skitka, L. J., & Sargis, E. G. (2006). The Internet as psychological labora- Paper and plastic in daily diary research: Comment on Green, Rafaeli,
tory. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 529–555. Bolger, Shrout, and Reis (2006). Psychological Methods, 11, 112–118.
Skowronski, J. J., Betz, A. L., Thompson, C. P., & Shannon, L. (1991). Thomas, D., & Diener, E. (1990). Memory accuracy in the recall of emo-
Social memory in everyday life: Recall of self-events and other-events. tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 291–297.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 831–843.
Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competi-
Smith, E. R. (2000). Research design. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), tion. American Journal of Psychology, 9, 507–533.
Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology
Turner, C. W., Layton, J. F., & Simons, L. S. (1975). Naturalistic stud-
(pp. 17–39). New York: Cambridge University Press.
ies of aggressive behavior: Aggressive stimuli, victim visibility, and
Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2000). Social psychology (2nd ed.). horn honking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31,
Philadelphia: Psychology Press. 1098–1107.
114 Social Psychological Methods Outside the Laboratory

Vazire, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2004). e-perceptions: Personality impres- methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 40–84). New York:
sions based on personal websites. Journal of Personality and Social Cambridge University Press.
Psychology, 87, 123–132. West, S. G., & Hepworth, J. T. (1991). Statistical issues in the study of tem-
Vazire, S., & Mehl, M. R. (2008). Knowing me, knowing you: The accu- poral data: Daily experiences. Journal of Personality, 59, 609–662.
racy and unique predictive validity of self-ratings and other-ratings West, S. G., Newsom, J. T., & Fenaughty, A. M. (1992). Publication trends
of daily behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, in JPSP: Stability and change in topics, methods, and theories across two
1202–1216. decades. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 473–484.
Vazire, S., Naumann, L. P., Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Portrait Wethington, E., & Almeida, D. M. (in press). Assessing daily well-being
of a narcissist: Manifestations of narcissism in physical appearance. via telephone diaries. In R. Belli, F. P. Stafford, & D. F. Alwin (Eds.),
Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1439–1447. Using calendar and diary methods in life events research. Thousand
Visser, P. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Lavrakas, P. J. (2000). Survey research. Oaks, CA: Sage.
In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods Wheeler, L., & Miyake, K. (1992). Social comparison in everyday life.
in social and personality psychology (pp. 223–252). New York: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 760–773.
Cambridge University Press.
Wheeler, L., & Nezlek, J. (1977). Sex differences in social participation.
Walls, T. A., Schafer, J. L. (2006) (Eds.). Models for intensive longitudinal Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 742–754.
data. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Wheeler, L., & Reis, H. T. (1991). Self-recording of everyday life events:
Walster, E., Walster, G. W., & Lambert, P. (1971). Playing hard-to-get: Origins, types, and uses. Journal of Personality, 59, 339–354.
A field study. Reported in Walster, E., Walster, G. W., Piliavin, J., &
White, W. B., Schulman, P., McCabe, E. J., & Dey, H. M. (1989). Average
Schmidt, L. (1973). “Playing hard to get”: Understanding an elusive
daily blood pressure, not office blood pressure, determines cardiac
phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26,
function in patients with hypertension. JAMA, 261, 873–877.
113–121.
Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York:
Walther, J. B., van der Heide, B., Kim, S-Y., Westerman, D., & Tong, S. T.
Guilford.
(2008). The role of friends’ appearance and behavior on evaluations
of individuals on Facebook: Are we known by the company we keep? Woike, B. A. (1995). Most-memorable experiences: Evidence for a link between
Human Communication Research, 34, 28–49. implicit and explicit motives and social cognitive processes in everyday
life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1081–1091.
Webb, E. J., Campbell, D. T., Schwartz, R. D., & Sechrest, L. (2000).
Unobtrusive measures (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Wood, W., Quinn, J. M., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Habits in everyday life:
Webb, E. J., Campbell, D. T., Schwartz, R. D., Sechrest, L., & Grove, J. B. Thought, emotion, and action. Journal of Personality and Social
(1981). Nonreactive measures in the social sciences (2nd ed.). Boston: Psychology, 83, 1281–1297.
Houghton Mifflin. Yee, N., Bailenson, J. N., Urbanek, M., Chang, F., & Merget, D. (2007).
Weick, K. E. (1985). Systematic observational methods. In G. Lindzey & E. The unbearable likeness of being digital: The persistence of nonver-
Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (3rd ed., pp. bal social norms in online virtual environments. CyberPsychology and
567–634). New York: Random House. Behavior, 10, 115–121.
West, S. G., Biesanz, J. C., & Pitts, S. C. (2000). Causal inference and Zucker, R. A., Manosevitz, M., & Lanyon, R. I. (1968). Birth order, anxi-
generalization in field settings: Experimental and quasi-experimental ety, and affiliation during a crisis. Journal of Personality and Social
designs. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research Psychology, 8, 354–359.
Chapter 4

Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent


and Recurring Issues
CHARLES M. JUDD AND DAVID A. KENNY

INTRODUCTION analysis, handle missing data, and establish the equivalence


of measures.
In 1969, John Tukey described data analysis as detective It may seem that there is a tension between the roles of
work. The scientist has a case to solve—trying to understand the data analyst: as detective and as advocate or data analy-
reality—and the data are the clues that are used to solve sis as exploratory and as confirmatory. But in fact these roles
that case. Statistical methods are the tools that can be used are easily reconcilable. Data analysis presumes a causal the-
to understand the meaning of those clues. Unfortunately, ory, and one explores one’s data in making the case for that
all too often social psychologists treat data analysis and causal theory. One must be willing to be surprised along
statistics as more of an onerous, obligatory ritual that they the way and open to modifications to one’s guiding causal
must endure rather than as an opportunity to dig, discover, theory.
and be enlightened. One of the goals of this chapter is show Two aspects of a causal theory and their implications for
how data analysis can be used to help us better understand data analysis deserve elaboration before we plunge into the
the social-psychological reality that interests us. details of this chapter. The first concerns the nature of what
Tukey (1980) also argued that data analysis is often we call invariance assumptions. The second, and related,
confirmatory as well as exploratory: The researcher has a aspect is the assumptions that are made about error. We
theory of reality and tries to determine whether the data turn to each of these briefly and then we provide a road-
conform to that theory. Similarly, Robert Abelson, in his map for the remaining sections of the chapter.
wonderful book Statistics as Principled Argument (1995),
argues that the data analyst is an advocate and uses statis- Assumptions About Invariance
tics in order to argue for a causal theory. Most data analysis
in social psychology is confirmatory and most of the theo- A key aspect of a causal law is the assumption of invari-
ries that we seek to confirm are causal theories. Successful ance. Invariance means that the causal law is assumed
data analysis in social psychology very often requires that the to apply across some domain. Social psychologists rou-
analyst have an explicit causal theory that guides the analy- tinely wish to argue for invariance across persons: We
sis. Social psychologists are most familiar with having a routinely examine whether results generalize across people
causal theory underlying their analyses when they analyze in our studies and worry about generalizing causal theo-
the results of randomized experiments in order to look at the ries across people in general. Thus, a central concern in
causal effects of independent variables. They are also famil- contemporary social psychology has been whether results
iar with causal modeling in the context of structural equation are truly invariant across people from different cultures.
modeling of non-experimental data. However, these are However, there are other domains across which we can
not the only contexts in which causal thinking guides data and should worry about invariance. Sometimes invariance
analysis. We shall see that having the right causal theory might be assumed to be across time rather than across
is essential if we are to conduct mediation and moderation persons. For instance, although we might assume that stress

We thank Anne Black, Niall Bolger, Randi Garcia, John Graham, Deborah A. Kashy, David MacKinnon, Dominique Muller, Kris
Preacher, Eliot Smith, Amanda Snook, Tessa West, and Vincent Yzerbyt who provided much appreciated comments on an earlier version
of this chapter.

115
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
116 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

generally causes negative mood, we may further acknowl- one distribution of errors, but then there may be a few
edge that the strength of the causal relationship may be weird cases sampled from another distribution of errors.
different for different persons. If we are to allow for a These can “grab” the model unless one recognizes that
causal effect to vary across persons, we may assume that they really come from a different error distribution.
within each person this relationship is invariant across time Probably most perniciously, in a great deal of social
and situations. Alternatively, we might also want to assume psychological data there is more than one source of errors,
invariance of a causal effect across specific stimulus items commonly called levels. Most obviously, repeated mea-
that are used in studies. For instance, in studies involving sures taken from the same person involve errors both at the
implicit measures, such as evaluative priming procedures person level and at the level of individual observations over
(Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995), we gener- time within a person. But this kind of multiple-level struc-
ally have many trials and we routinely assume invariance ture characterizes many other sorts of data as well, often
across these. Making appropriate assumptions about where unbeknownst to the researcher. When people are in dyads
invariance is expected and where it is not is critical for or groups, there are errors both at the person and group
conducting the proper statistical analyses. levels and these are very likely to have different variances.
Invariance is crucial to research design and data A model that assumes only one source of error would lead
analysis because it points to where it is that one obtains to serious problems of statistical inference.
replications. If the causal model is the same for all persons,
we can obtain replications by studying more people. If, Chapter Overview
however, the causal relationship varies across persons but
is invariant over time, we can obtain more replications by Our focus in this chapter is on various issues in data
measuring more times for each person. analysis in social psychology that we believe cause most
researchers problems. We do not intend to comprehensively
survey how data analysis proceeds and to summarize ana-
Assumptions About Errors
lytic methods.1 Rather, our goal is to point to ongoing
Once we have chosen a causal theory and determined issues in data analysis that we feel deserve close attention.
the domain to which it applies (i.e., the invariance), we The chapter is divided into five main sections. In the first,
then employ statistical models to analyze our data. Judd, we deal with issues in the assessment of mediation. The
McClelland, and Ryan (2008) have described data as hav- second is devoted to moderation of causal effects. The third is
ing two parts. One part is the model we build and that we devoted to issues in repeated measures analyses and multi-
understand; the other part is what is left over and is the part level modeling. The fourth discusses problems of missing
we do not understand: data and how these might be handled. And the final section
is devoted to the general issue of invariance. These various
Data  Model  Error sections can be read discretely.

By “Model,” they include what social psychologists


MEDIATION
focus on most: means, mean differences, and regression
coefficients. We social psychologists are keen on under-
Since Baron and Kenny’s (1986) influential paper on medi-
standing the Model part of data analysis, but we often
ation and moderation, mediational analysis has largely
forget about Error. If the theory of Error is wrong, the Model
become de rigueur in many disciplines, including social
may be misleading and probably misunderstood. Consider
psychology. For instance, 34% of papers published in the
three ways in which we might have the wrong model of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology in 2004
Error: non-normality, outliers, and multiple levels.
employ some form of mediational analysis. As a result
We are all taught about the assumption that errors are
there is a large and flourishing literature devoted to the
normally distributed. If the distribution of the errors is
not normal, then our estimate of the error variance and con-
sequently our measures of uncertainty of parameter estimates 1There are many excellent comprehensive guides. For the basics
(e.g., differences between means) would be incorrect.
on the general linear model, analysis of variance, and regression
Outliers are anomalous observations that may drasti- procedures, integrated guides are provided by Cohen, Cohen,
cally affect both the estimates of the Model and Error West, and Aiken (2003), Judd, McClelland, and Ryan (2008),
(McClelland, 2000). Their presence requires that a differ- and Maxwell and Delaney (2003). A recent guide to effectively
ent model of error needs to be applied to them. In other reporting and interpreting social psychological results is Kashy,
words, most observations may have errors that come from Donnellan, Ackerman, and Russell (2009).
Mediation 117

conduct of mediational analysis, including a recent book The subsequent literature on the conduct of mediation has
devoted to the subject (MacKinnon, 2008). Our purpose largely focused on two issues: (1) the necessity of each of
is not to thoroughly review this literature. Rather, what these four conditions and (2) the most powerful approaches
we wish to do is raise some concerns about this literature to test for mediation.
and the ways in which mediational analyses are routinely The first Baron and Kenny condition, that there be an
conducted. overall or total effect of the independent variable (c苷 0),
As articulated by Baron and Kenny (1986) (and Judd arose because of the conviction that there must be such an
& Kenny, 1981 previously), a mediational model is a effect for the question of mediation to even make sense:
causal model in which an independent variable of interest “If there is no effect of the independent variable, there is
is presumed to affect some outcome variable because, at nothing to mediate.” There is now, however, an emerging
least in part, the independent variable affects a mediator consensus (see Kenny, Kashy, & Bolger, 1998; MacKinnon,
that in turn affects the outcome. This causal model is laid Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002; Shrout & Bolger,
out in Figure 4.1, with the designation of effects (typically, 2002) that this first condition is unnecessary. Note that if the
but not always, least-squares regression coefficients) given total effect (c) is in fact zero, then by necessity the indirect
in the terminology that has become standard in the litera- effect (ab) would have the opposite sign from the residual
ture on mediation. In the top causal model, c is the estimate direct effect (c), a condition defining what is known as
of the total effect of the independent variable (X ) on the suppression (MacKinnon, Krull, & Lockwood, 2000). In
outcome (Y ). In the bottom causal model, a is the estimate other words, there may be an effect of the independent
of the effect of the independent variable on the mediator variable that emerges only when the mediator is controlled
(M ) and b and c are estimates of two partial effects, the and this effect goes in the opposite direction from the
first is the effect of the mediator on the outcome controlling mediated effect.
for the independent variable and the second is the effect of An example can help illustrate suppression and why it
the independent variable on the outcome, controlling for sometimes ought to be considered a special case of media-
the mediator. The fundamental equality that underlies cur- tion. A developmental psychologist may believe that
rent approaches to mediation is that c  c  ab. In words, children who are more intelligent (X ) ought to show more
the total effect of the independent variable on the outcome emotional stability (Y ). However, it happens that the sim-
(c) can be partitioned into two parts, the direct effect (c) ple relation between these two (c) is close to zero. She has
and the indirect effect via the mediator (ab). Solving for ab also measured the extent to which children are bullied by
yields the expression of c  c  ab, which in words says other children. The degree of being bullied is the M vari-
that the reduction in the effect of X on Y is the amount of able. Intelligence results in being bullied more (i.e., a > 0).
mediation. Additionally, being bullied leads to less emotional stability
Baron and Kenny (1986) argued that four conditions (i.e., b < 0). Hence, the indirect effect of intelligence on
must hold to demonstrate mediation: stability via being bullied is negative (i.e., ab < 0). Once
one controls for the bullying variable the residual direct
1. c should differ from zero. positive effects of intelligence on emotional stability
2. a should differ from zero. emerges (i.e., c > 0).
3. b should differ from zero. Following from these considerations and also from the
4. Ideally, c should be zero, or at least smaller in fact that the indirect effect (ab) may differ significantly
absolute value than c. from zero even if one of its components does not, the bulk
of the literature has concluded that ultimately there is only
a single condition that must be met to statistically establish
mediation: ab  c  c 苷 0. For many years this condition
c was widely tested using what is called the Sobel test (Sobel,
X Y
1982); there are now more powerful approaches, largely
M involving resampling procedures (MacKinnon, Fairchild, &
a b Fritz, 2007; MacKinnon et al., 2002; Preacher & Hayes,
2004, 2008; Shrout & Bolger, 2002) to determine if ab is
c statistically different from zero.
X Y
Although in our opinion the reconsideration of the Baron
Figure 4.1 Mediation Models Where X Is the Causal Variable, and Kenny (1986) four conditions represents a substantial and
M the Mediator, and Y the Outcome (Top Model without the appropriate clarification and refinement, the conclusion that
Mediator and Lower Model with the Mediator). only a single condition must hold to demonstrate mediation,
118 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

i.e., ab  c  c 苷 0, has led to the unfortunate conclusion Height, i.e., b 苷 0. Accordingly, we would certainly
among many social psychologists that there is a statistical expect the “indirect effect” of Height on Weight via
“test” of mediation and all that one needs to do to argue for Gender (i.e., ab) to differ significantly from zero. So the
mediation is to have that “test” be statistically significant. statistical “test of mediation” would likely be found to be
This is far from true. The “test” simply amounts to a test of statistically significant, but surely we would not want to
whether a partial slope is different from an unpartialed or argue that the causal assumptions built into “mediational”
zero-order slope. And these two slopes may certainly differ model hold because the test was significant. Obviously,
in many contexts where the mediational causal model is Height does not cause Weight in part because it causes
totally implausible (see the next paragraph for an example). Gender!
In fact, with any three variables, so long as there is some
correlation among all possible pairs and regardless of the Critical Assumptions of the Mediation Model
true causal model that is responsible for those correlations,
the zero-order slope of the first variable regressed on the The mediational models in Figure 4.1 are causal models
second would almost always be different from the partial with very strong assumptions that are not critically exam-
slope of the first regressed on the second controlling for ined simply by estimating model parameters and “testing”
the third. Surely such a condition by itself cannot be used mediation. In Figure 4.2, we have drawn the model with all of
to argue for the set of causal assumptions that underlies the the paths, covariances, and variances that are assumed to be
mediational model. What the statistics do is estimate and zero. First, in mediational models we assume that the causal
test the indirect path if the model is true. However, the directions are correct. That is, it is not the case that Y causes
model might be wrong in many ways and the statistics M (bMY  0) or X (bXY  0) or that M causes X (bXM  0).
never inform us about that. Second, we must assume that the causal variables X and M
An example can illustrate the argument. Height, Weight, have no measurement error2 (σEX2  σEM2  0). Third, we
and Gender are all correlated with each other. Suppose one must assume that there are no unmeasured common causes
estimated a “mediational” model in which Height is the of any of the pairs of variables (σWU  σWV  σ UV  0).
X variable, Gender is the M variable, and Weight is the Y Violation of any of these eight assumptions may substantially
variable. Undoubtedly, in this model c would be greater bias the estimates of mediation.
than zero (i.e., taller people do weigh more). Also we could A proper mediation analysis requires more than the esti-
predict Gender from Height to some extent, i.e., a苷 0, and mation of the parameters in the mediation model and a media-
surely Gender is related to Weight even controlling for tional “test”; rather, it requires an adequate rationale for that

M
True 1
M 1
1
EX EM

1
a b
U
X

W V
1

1 1
X Figure 4.2 Mediation Model with Specification Error:
Y
True
Reverse Causal Effects, Omitted Variables, and Measurement
c Error in X and M.

2 The reader might think that we need to assume that the outcome, Y, has no measurement error. This need not be assumed because we
can include measurement error in Y in its disturbance variable.
Moderation 119

model and for assuming that all eight of the “extra” param- to improve the power of tests of the indirect effect in a
eters are zero. Design consideration can be used. For instance, mediational model. While we applaud these developments,
if X were a manipulated experimental variable, then five of we strongly believe that this literature has insufficiently
the parameters can safely be assumed to be zero (bXY  bXM emphasized the very difficult assumptions that underlie
 σ EX2  σ WU  σ WV  0). This model, with a manipulated the mediational model. This deficiency has led to many
X, is given in Figure 4.3. We can relax the assumption of zero papers that employ a mediation test without any discus-
error variance in the mediator by the use of multiple indica- sion of the strong assumptions that are implicitly being
tors. We can lessen the chance that Y causes M by measuring made. Establishing the causal assumptions of mediation is
M before Y, although it still may be the case that covariation not something that is easily done. Those assumptions are
between M and Y may be due to a prior value of Y that causes complex and no statistical treatment of the data, by itself,
M and the observed Y (Cole & Maxwell, 2003). can render those assumptions plausible. Ultimately, theo-
The most difficult and problematic assumption is that there retical and empirical arguments need to be made for the
are no common causes of M and Y, besides X. Most typically, plausibility of a mediating process. And these arguments
both M and Y are measured variables that are likely to have undoubtedly involve much more than the estimation of a
many common causes over and above X. If these exist and simple mediational model and a “test” of mediation. At the
are not controlled, then the estimates of the model would be least and even in a situation where X has been experimen-
biased, almost always in a direction that leads to an overesti- tally manipulated, it seems crucial to demonstrate that the
mation of the indirect effect and hence of mediation. partial path between the mediator and the outcome really
There are three ways of dealing with this most serious represents a causal effect, be it by further experimentation
problem. First one could attempt to specify, measure, and or in some other way.
control likely common causes of both M and Y. Second, one Let us be clear. We are not arguing that mediational analy-
could conduct further work in which M is experimentally sis should not be done. Rather, we are arguing that the dif-
manipulated (Spencer, Zanna, & Fong, 2005). The presence ficult and important issues in mediational analysis are not
of an effect of manipulated M on Y certainly helps make the statistical. The most difficult issue is in establishing the ade-
argument for mediation, although comparing the effect of an quacy of the theory underlying the mediational model and
experimental manipulation of M to the estimated effect of a the design considerations that have been used to buttress that
measured M can be difficult because of issues of construct theory. We should expect to see an extended discussion of
validity: Is manipulated M the same construct as measured these issues whenever mediational analyses are presented.
M? Third, if one can assume that c is zero (i.e., there is “com-
plete” mediation), then one can estimate a mediational model
that includes and estimates rUV.3 Of course, this assumption MODERATION
of complete mediation is also a very difficult one to make.
As we said at the start of this section, the recent litera- Although moderation has been given much less attention
ture on mediation has largely concentrated on procedures than mediation, examining moderation is just as essential

U
1 1
EM M 1
M
True

a b
V

c Y Figure 4.3 Mediation Model with a Manipulated X


X
Variable and Specification Error.

3 The basic mediational model, the lower model in Figure 4.1, is said to be just-identified or saturated. If the causal model were overi-

dentified because we believed we had complete mediation (i.e., path c  0), then it would be possible to estimate one additional covari-
ance in the model, otherwise assumed to be zero.
120 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

to theory development. In essence, moderation examines people very strong arguments or rather weak arguments.
claims about the invariance of causal relationships across We might then expect that there is considerable variability
different domains, be they persons, situations, or cultures. in both the attitudes themselves and the strength of those
We might examine, for instance, whether the impact of a attitudes as a function of our manipulations. In field set-
persuasive message on attitude change depends on the extent tings, however, the actual attitudes and the strength of
to which participants are motivated to attend to the message. those attitudes are likely to vary much more continuously,
We might ask whether the degree to which one expresses with the majority of people expressing neither particularly
less liking of an outgroup than an ingroup depends on the strong nor particularly weak attitudes.
situational constraints against showing outgroup hostility. A key factor that affects the power of the test of the
And we might ask whether the factors that give rise to par- interaction, i.e., testing the partial slope of the product, is
ticularly high levels of self-esteem depend on one’s culture. the residual variance of the product, having partialed out the
The basic data-analytic approach for testing modera- component variables. Although the variances of the compo-
tion is well worked out (Aiken & West, 1991; Judd et al., nent variables may be less in field settings than in laboratory
2008); however, the practice of testing moderation in the settings, the variance of the partialed product variable would
literature has problems to which we wish to call attention. be much less in field settings. Accordingly, with continu-
We start with some common problems in the testing of ously measured independent variables, rather than experi-
moderation, problems that have been articulated in the lit- mentally manipulated ones, the power to test for moderation
erature but that nevertheless persist in the common conduct is often exceedingly low. Very often to have adequate
of moderation analysis. We then turn to a more perplexing power, one needs sample sizes in the high hundreds. For
issue that has not been dealt with elsewhere. And finally, instance, Aiken and West (1991) argue that if A and F are
we briefly discuss models that integrate this section with two normally distributed and uncorrelated variables and the
the previous section on mediation; specifically, we discuss effect of partialed product is typical, one needs 392 cases to
mediated moderation and moderated mediation. have an 80 percent chance to detect moderation! Because of
this low power, replication of effects across different opera-
Continuing Problems tions of the variables can be important. We urge researchers
to report (and editors to allow them to report) estimates for
We start with the simplest model of moderation in which all tests of moderators (and not just statistical decisions, i.e.,
the effect of attitudes (A) on behavior (B) is assumed to “not significant”) in reporting moderator effects.
be moderated by some factor (F) such as attitude strength, A second issue is the interpretation of the various para-
familiarity, or direct experience. The test of moderation meter estimates in the above linear model. Although the
involves testing the partial effect of a product predictor meaning of the effect of the product term is not fun-
(A*F) in a linear model where B is regressed on A, F, and damentally affected by the scaling of A and F, the
the product: meanings of coefficients for A and F dramatically change
when they are rescaled. The parameter estimate for A
B = b0 +b1A +b2 F +b3 (A * F ) + e
estimates the simple A effect when F equals zero and the
parameter estimate for F estimates the simple F effect when
In this model, the b’s are simply unstandardized partial A equals zero. Thus, a change in the zero point of one of
slopes or partial regression coefficients. Importantly, the these variables changes the parameter estimate associated
interaction is not tested by a simple regression with only with the other. Moreover, if zero is not meaningful for
the product as the predictor. Rather, one must test the par- either A or F, then the simple effects are meaningless.
tial slope of the product controlling for its two components To illustrate, suppose that A varied from 1 to 7, with
(A and F) (Cohen, 1978). higher numbers indicating a more favorable attitude. And
One key issue in the testing of interactions is that of F also varied from 1 to 7, again with higher numbers indi-
statistical power. McClelland and Judd (1993) compared cating greater attitude strength. While b3 in the above
the typical power for tests of moderation in experimental model estimates the effect of the A by F interaction, b1
situations, where the A and F variables are manipulated at in the above model estimates not the “main” effect of A,
relatively extreme values, and in field settings in which the but the simple effect of A when and only when F equals 0,
variables are measured and may show considerably less an impossible value. A similar interpretation applies to b2.
variability. For instance in the laboratory, we may give One suggestion to make the effects of components inter-
people positive or negative attitudes toward some hypo- pretable is to center (i.e., subtract the grand mean (Aiken &
thetical issue (e.g., senior comprehensive exams) and then West, 1991)) for the two component variables (A and F) prior
manipulate the strength of those attitudes by either giving to computing the product predictor and then these centered
Moderation 121

variables (along with their product) be used as the predictors in moderation analysis neither A nor B is manipulated, and
in the model. What centering does is make the zero value for the causal direction may be unclear. Most researchers pre-
A and F interpretable. Note, however, that sometimes center- sume that the direction of causation does not matter in tests
ing may be unnecessary. If A already has a meaningful value of moderation. After all, regressing variable A on variable
at zero (e.g., neutrality), then centering may be less informa- B produces the exact same standardized regression coef-
tive. Generally, we recommend that researchers always scale ficient as regressing B on A. So it might be supposed that
their variables so that zero is a meaningful value. So instead of it is immaterial whether A is presumed to cause B or vice
using a 1 to 7 scale for attitude, we use a 3.5 to 3.5 scale, versa. However, as we shall see, the causal direction can
where 0 would represent a neutral attitude. Of course, the matter a great deal for tests of moderation.
grand mean may sometimes be the most meaningful value, in Consider the statistics from an artificial dataset in
which case our recommendation is equivalent to the centering Table 4.1. We give the variances for A and B and their cor-
recommendation. relation for two levels of the moderator variable F (assumed
Frequently, one or both of the component variables of an to be dichotomous). Again, by way of illustration we mea-
interaction may be categorical; such variables may be either sure participants’ attitudes, their behavior intentions, and
experimental manipulations or measured variables (e.g., finally we measure F, a dichotomous variable that captures
gender). All too often, because the rationale underlying the whether participants regard the attitude issue as important
interpretation of slopes of component variables in interac- or not. Note that the correlation is exactly the same at the
tive models is not fully understood, researchers use coding two levels of the moderator. However, note that there is an
conventions for these categorical variables where zero is not asymmetry in the variances. For F  –1, the variance of A
a meaningful value. For instance, if one coded a categori- is greater than the variance of B. The pattern reverses for
cal variable using a 1 versus 2 coding system and included F  1. It is this asymmetry that produces the “strange” result
this variable, a continuous predictor, and the product of these that the moderator effect reverses as a function of whether
two variables in a moderation model, then the slope of the one examines the effect of the A*F interaction on B or the
continuous predictor estimates the simple slope of that pre- effect of the B*F interaction on A. When the analysis is
dictor at a meaningless level of the categorical predictor, i.e., done presuming that A causes B, the relationship is stronger
when it equals zero. Even in the case of dummy coding, where when F  1 than when F  –1; however, when the analy-
zero is a meaningful value, researchers all too often interpret sis is done presuming that B causes A, the within-group
the slope of the continuous predictor as the “main effect” unstandardized regression coefficient is stronger when
of that variable and fail to realize that it is the simple effect F  –1 than when F  1. As this example illustrates, chang-
for that variable for the condition coded as 0 on the dummy ing the assumptions about the direction of causation can
code. The lesson is that for both categorical and continuous have a dramatic effect on the estimated moderator effect.
predictors in a model involving product predictors, we need to Mathematically, the effect of A on B equals bBA  rAB ( s s )
B

and the effect of B on A equals bAB  rAB ( s s ) , where sA and


A

be aware that slopes of component predictors estimate simple A


B

effects at the zero level of the other component predictors. sB equal the standard deviations of A and B and rAB equals
Accordingly, one needs to think about what the zero points their correlation. From these two expressions for the partial
2 2
of those variables are, even in the case of categorical predic- slopes, it follows that b b  s s . Because the size of s s
AB
BA
A
2
B
A
2
B

tors. In our own research with categorical variables, we gener- can vary dramatically across levels of the moderator, the
ally prefer to use contrast codes or effects codes rather than effect of the moderator may reverse direction depending
dummy codes precisely for this reason.

Table 4.1 Hypothetical Effects of Moderator Variable Illustrating


Examining Moderation When the Direction of How the Direction of Causation Can Affect the Degree of Moderation
Causation Is Unknown (F Moderating the A:B Relationship)

A thorny and largely unrecognized issue4 in the examina- F  1 F1


tion of moderation concerns the direction of the causal sA2  4 sA2  1
21 sB2  4
relationship between A and B, assumed to be moderated sB
by F. If A were a manipulated variable, then it would be rAB  .5 rAB  .5
reasonable to assume that A causes B. However, very often A→B
bBA  0.25 bBA  1.00
B→A
4We especially want to thank Irving Kirsh who originally pointed
bAB  1.00 bAB  0.25
out this problem to us.
122 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

on which variable, A or B, is treated as the dependent vari- For more complicated problems, one might consider the
able and which is treated as the predictor. O’Brien test (1979).
What should researchers do to test for moderation given The second type of moderation is that the causal effect
this ambiguity? We believe first that the researcher needs is stronger in one group than the other. This is the type
to consider in depth the plausible direction of causation. of moderation that is almost always assumed and typically
Sometimes the direction of causation is known by the design the only one that it is estimated and tested, using the test
of the research. For instance if one variable is manipulated of the product term in multiple regression. This test, of
or if one variable is measured prior to another,5 the causal course, presumes homogeneity of variance.
direction may be known. Other times, theory can be used Third, the unexplained variance in B may vary across
to justify the direction of causation. For instance, in our groups. As we just said, in the typical test of moderation,
attitude–behavior example, many—though not all—would testing the product term in multiple regression, the unex-
argue that attitudes are more likely to cause behaviors than plained variance in the two groups is assumed to be equal.
the other way around. That said, the researcher needs to But this assumption is seldom tested. We think it would
explicitly provide a rationale for the choice of cause and be beneficial to test this assumption when conducting a
effect and a rationale why causation does not flow in the moderator analysis. If one found less unexplained variance in
other direction. Without such a rationale, the results from one group, then it could be argued that the moderator may be
the moderation analysis are dubious, especially if there are inhibiting the causal effects of other unmeasured variables on
unequal variances. B. This would seem to be the least plausible of the three expla-
If the direction of causation is known, say from A to B, nations but when it does occur, it can be very interesting.
there are potentially three different parameters that could If the direction of causation is unknown, we suggest
be moderated by F: the following two-step procedure. First, we test if both
of the variances (the variances of the both B and A) are
a. the variance of the causal variable or sA2 , equal across the levels of F. As we have argued, often
b. the strength of the causal effect or bBA, and moderators generate or inhibit variance. If the variances do
not differ, then the standard approach to testing for mod-
c. the unexplained variance of the outcome or sB2 | A .
eration, i.e., testing the partial effect of the product, can
be used, and it does not matter which variable is treated as
Let us consider each of these in turn.
the outcome variable. In the presence of equal variances, a
First, very often what a moderator variable does is
statistically significant moderator effect would also mean
generate variance in the causal variable. That is, modera-
that the correlations between A and B across levels of the
tors are variables that are suspected to activate or inhibit a
moderator would also be unequal.
causal process. They may activate the process by expand-
Our discussion in this section has focused on a categori-
ing the range of the causal variable, and they may inhibit
cal moderator with just two levels, because the problems
the process by restricting the range of that variable. For
are easier to illustrate in this case. However, what we have
instance, consider the effect of attitudes on behavior and
said also applies to continuous moderators. When the direc-
the moderation of that causal effect by direct experience
tion of causation is unknown and when the homogeneity of
(Fazio & Towles-Schwen, 1999). Most likely direct expe-
variance assumption is violated, such that either B or A
rience with an attitude object leads to a wider range of
becomes more variable at higher levels of the moderator,
attitudes toward that object. We note that even if there is no
then different patterns of moderation may ensue depending
moderation in the sense of different regression slopes, the
upon whether B or A is treated as the dependent variable in
correlation between A and B would tend to be greater for
the analysis of moderation.
the group with the larger variance in the causal variable,
Once again, we emphasize the importance of a causal the-
assuming that the two groups have the same residual vari-
ory on which data analysis is based. Given a strong causal theory,
ance in the outcome variable. The standard test of a differ-
there is no ambiguity about how moderation is to be assessed.
ence in variance is the F test of homogeneity of variance.

Mediated Moderation

5Measuring Y after X of course does not by itself guarantee that


The final issue to be discussed in this section involves an
X causes Y. There are likely third variables that affect them both integration of the topic of the previous section, mediation,
and that are likely responsible for the shared variance between with our current focus on moderation. Assuming that one
X and Y. Longitudinal data can help point to the causal direction has a causal theory that predicts moderation of the effect
but they are no guarantee. of A on B as a function of some variable F, one might also
Moderation 123

have a causal theory in mind about the mediating process expect that the moderation of the overall effect of A on B by
that accounts for that moderation. Such a theory posits what F results from the moderation by F of the indirect effect of
has been called mediated moderation (Edwards & Lambert, A on B via M. And there are two alternatives here, indicated
2007; Muller, Judd, & Yzerbyt, 2005; Preacher, Rucker, & by the two places in which F moderates the two components
Hayes, 2007; Wegener & Fabrigar, 2000) and in this sec- of the indirect effect in Figure 4.4. First, there may be mod-
tion, we present a brief overview of such. eration of the causal connection between A and the mediator,
To illustrate mediated moderation and its alternative M (called path a in the mediation section). Second, the effect
forms, we continue to use the attitude–behavior example, of M on B may also be moderated by F (called path b). As
assuming that the causal direction is known, from A to B. we will illustrate, the variable that may moderate these two
Let us further consider two potential moderators of this different indirect effects may be different. Thus, we use F to
relationship: attitude strength (Krosnick & Petty, 1995) indicate moderators in general rather than a specific variable.
and behavioral control (Ajzen & Madden, 1986). Stronger Additionally, the residual direct effect of A on B (called path c)
attitudes should strengthen the attitude–behavior link. may also be moderated, as indicated in Figure 4.4.
Additionally, attitudes should lead to consistent behaviors In terms of the two potential moderators of the attitude–
only when one has the ability or control to implement the behavior relationship that we have used in the example, it
behavior. Finally, for this example, we consider behavioral certainly seems reasonable that attitude strength would be
intentions as the variable that mediates the effects of atti- a moderator of the first link: The link between attitudes and
tudes on behavior: Attitudes result in consistent behavioral the intention to behave in attitude consistent ways should
intentions that then translate into actual behaviors. be stronger for those whose attitudes are more important
The causal models of Figure 4.4 underlie mediated mod- and stronger. And it seems likely that behavioral control
eration and make clear the various ways in which it can would be a moderator of the second link: Intentions would
be produced. In the model at the top of the figure, there is a translate into behaviors only to the extent that the behavior
causal effect of the attitude (A) on behavior (B) and this over- is under volitional control.
all causal effect is moderated by F, either attitude strength or Muller et al. (2005) lay out the basic analytic models for
behavioral control (or both). This overall moderation is indi- examining mediated moderation. In essence, these models
cated by the arrow from F in the model to the causal path estimate and test whether there is significant moderation of
linking A to B. In the model at the bottom of the figure, we the overall effect of A and B, then whether the moderator
have added the mediator, M, which in the example is behav- affects the component of the indirect effect from A to the
ioral intentions. We remind the reader of the very strong mediator and/or the component of the indirect effect from
assumptions that underlie the demonstration of mediation, the mediator to B. These models are straightforward exten-
discussed in the earlier section of this chapter. Given the sions of the basic mediational models except that the mod-
overall moderation and fact that the causal effect of A on B eration of the various effects on those models is estimated.
is assumed to be mediated by M, then one might reasonably An important question is whether, in the presence of the
overall moderation of the treatment effect, that moderation
is fully accounted for by the moderation of the indirect
effect via the mediator, either on the effect from A to M
c
A B or on the effect from M to B. If the moderation of these
indirect effects is not found, then one can conclude that
the overall moderation is not in fact mediated, i.e., there
F
persists moderation of the residual effect of A on B, as
indicated by the effect of F on the A–B effect in the model
at the bottom of Figure 4.4.
F F Besides mediated moderation, there is also moderated
a M b mediation. The distinction is that in mediated moderation
one starts with overall moderation of the treatment effect
and then asks whether that moderation is mediated. On the
c other hand, in the case of moderated mediation, one starts
A B
with an overall hypothesis about mediation and then exam-
ines whether that mediation, i.e., the indirect effect, varies
F as a function of some moderator. At a deep level, mediated
moderation and moderated mediation are analytically the
Figure 4.4 Mediated Moderation Models. same. However, there is a conceptual difference in that in
124 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

mediated moderation one starts with overall moderation, introduction, such dependencies mean there are errors at
whereas with moderated mediation one starts with overall different levels (the individual observation level and the
mediation. level at which observations are linked) and these errors
Regardless of whether one prefers conceptually to think would each have their own variance.
about moderation that is mediated (i.e., mediated mod- Social psychologists are routinely trained in the analy-
eration) or mediation that is moderated (i.e., moderated sis of data that are linked because they come from the same
mediation), the fundamental expectation is that both sug- participants who are repeatedly observed in different experi-
gest that the indirect effect via a mediator is moderated. mental conditions (independent variables that vary “within
And what is perhaps a more interesting distinction from subjects”). Thus, “repeated measures” ANOVA or “mixed”
a theoretical point of view is where the moderation of the ANOVA procedures (where some independent variables
indirect effect occurs. Is it that the effect of A on M is mod- vary within subjects and others vary between them) are
erated (as for example attitude strength moderates the atti- procedures with which most social psychologists are quite
tude–behavioral intention link) or that the effect of M on B familiar. We refer to these in general as RANOVA. Although
is moderated (as in our example behavioral control might widely used and appropriate under the right circumstances,
be expected to moderate the behavioral intention–behavior RANOVA has a series of requirements that may not be met.
link)? Or does a moderator affect both components of the First, it requires that the independent variables that vary within
indirect effect? These distinctions are likely the most inter- participants have discrete categorical levels. And it is assumed
esting from a theoretical point of view. that each participant has a single score under each combina-
tion of levels of those independent variables, with no missing
data. Finally, it is assumed, when coupled with the homoge-
EXTENDING THE ANALYSIS neity of variance assumption, that all the correlations of the
OF REPEATED MEASURES repeated measures across participants are the same, i.e., that
there is an equal degree of dependence across all measures
Most social psychologists have been trained to do experiments taken on the same participants. As we shall discuss, these
and, as such, are thoroughly conversant with the analysis of assumptions are unnecessarily restrictive and social psycholo-
experimental data using factorial analysis of variance pro- gists should expand their horizons beyond RANOVA. In this
cedures. Additionally, due to the pioneering work of Jacob section, we initially describe RANOVA, then show how it can
Cohen and collaborators (Cohen, 1968; Cohen & Cohen, be reconceptualized so that the limitations discussed above
1975; Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003), there is now wide- can be overcome.
spread recognition that the analysis of variance is simply
a special case of the general linear model. Accordingly, the Conventional RANOVA
false dichotomy between analysis of variance and multiple
regression procedures has largely been eliminated, and social We begin with a simple design that can be handled by
psychologists have taken to heart the admonition not to force RANOVA, and we show later how our approach can be gen-
continuous independent variables into discrete categorical eralized to handle cases that RANOVA cannot handle. For
ones (via median splits, for instance) simply to fit them into illustrative purposes, assume we are interested in the role
their analysis of variance programs (MacCallum, Zhang, of issue involvement in persuasion (Petty & Cacioppo,
Preacher, & Rucker, 2002). 1984). Participants are asked to read about two issues
Additionally, there is now widespread recognition (e.g., instituting comprehensive senior exams and requir-
that serious bias results when the general linear model is ing senior theses from all students). For each issue, they
applied without modification to data that exhibit depen- are given a persuasive communication but the strength of
dencies (Kenny & Judd, 1986). Dependencies potentially the arguments in those communications varies between
exist whenever individual observations in a dataset can be participants. Half of the participants receive strong argu-
linked in some way to other observations in that dataset. For ments in favor of both issues and half receive relatively
instance, if we measure the same participant multiple times, weak arguments. Additionally, the personal relevance of
those observations are likely dependent upon each other the issues varies within participants, such that one issue is
because they come from the same person. Dependencies described as potentially being implemented on campus in
can also arise when data are collected from both members the next year while the other issue is described as poten-
of a pair, from members of small groups, from families, tially being implemented on a different campus. Which
and from any other sort of groupings that link individual specific issue is the high or low relevance issue is of course
observations to each other. As we briefly discussed in the counterbalanced, as well as their order and, to simplify our
Extending the Analysis of Repeated Measures 125

considerations, we presume that there are no effects of issue following, we first demonstrate the analysis using the com-
and order. After reading the communications on each issue, posite sum and difference scores and then we extend it.
participants are asked to indicate how much in favor of the If we assume that variation in each Y is partially
issue they are. Accordingly, this is a two-factor experimen- due to which A condition a participant is in (contrast
tal design, varying personal relevance within participants coded as X i  ½ if A1 and X i  ½ if A2 ), then we
and argument strength between them. Designs like this are have the following models for each observed depen-
ubiquitous in experimental social psychology. dent variable:
The between-subject factor, argument strength (A) has
levels A1 (strong arguments) and A2 (weak arguments). The Y1i  b10  b1X Xi  e1i
within-subject factor relevance (R) also has two levels R1 Y2 i  b20  b2 X Xi  e2 i
(high relevance issue) and R2 (low relevance issue). Assume
a single dependent variable measured in each condition (atti-
The intercepts ( b 10and b 20) in these models are the
tude toward each issue), designated as Y1i and Y2i, measured
unweighted means of the two variables, averaging
for R1 and R2 respectively. The standard RANOVA of the across levels of A , and the slopes ( b 1X and b 2X) equal
data from such a design partitions the variation in these scores the difference in the means of each variable between the
into two components, one between-subjects, where the main two levels of A . From the two above equations we do
effect of A is tested, and the other within-subjects, where the not get the usual RANOVA tests of the main effects
R main effect and the A  R interaction are tested. The result- of A , R , and the A  R interaction. To obtain these,
ing RANOVA source table is given in Table 4.2. Note that we need to create the two composite variables, the
the two portions of the table have different error terms, one sum of the two Y variables, Y Si Y 1i Y 2i and their dif-
at the level of the individual observation and one at the level ference, Y Di Y 1i Y 2i. Given the above models, the
of the participant, and their relative sizes are indicative of the definitional models for the two composite variables,
degree of dependence in the data induced by participants, as Y Si and Y Di are:
indexed by the intraclass correlation (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979).
YSi  (b10  b20 )  (b1X  b2 X ) Xi  (e1i  e2 i )
RANOVA Through the Analysis of Sums and YDi  (b10  b20 )  (b1X  b2 X ) Xi  (e1i  e2 i )
Differences

Most social psychologists are familiar with the RANOVA Accordingly, if one estimates coefficients for the following
decomposition, which we have just described. What is less two equations:
well known is that the exact same analysis is identically
YSi  bS 0  bSX Xi  eSi
conducted through regression procedures, analyzing two
composite variables computed as the sum of each partici- YDi  bD 0  bDX Xi  eDi
pant’s two Y scores and as their difference (Judd et al., 2008).
This is conceptually important because once we realize that the slope in the first model estimates the mean dif-
RANOVA can be equivalently done by analyzing these two ference between the levels of A in the sum of the two
composite variables, then it is a short further step to incor- Y variables; its associated t statistic, when squared,
porate continuously varying independent variables. In the exactly equals the F statistic from the RANOVA test-
ing the main effect of factor A (i.e., are attitudes on
both issues more favorable with stronger arguments
Table 4.2 A 2  2 Repeated Measures ANOVA Source
Table (Factor A Between-subjects, Factor R Within-subjects) than weaker ones?). The estimated intercept from the
second model estimates the mean difference between
Source DF
the R conditions, on average across levels of A, and
Between Subjects its associated t statistic, once squared, is identical to the F
A 1 statistic for the main effect of the within-subject factor R
Error between n2
Total between n1 in the RANOVA (i.e., are attitudes more or less favor-
able on the high relevance issue than on the low relevance
Within Subjects
R 1 issue?). And finally, the estimated slope of Xi in the sec-
AR 1 ond model estimates the degree to which the mean
Error within n2 difference between the levels of R differs as a function
Total within n
of factor A. Its associated t statistic, again once squared,
126 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

is identical to the F statistic for the A  R interaction in Y1i  b10  b1Z Z1i  b1 X Xi  e1i
the RANOVA (i.e., does the effect of strong versus weak
Y2 i  b20  b2 Z Z2 i  b2 X Xi  e2 i
arguments depend on issue relevance?). Thus, by com-
puting and analyzing the sum and difference we can
obtain the very same information as in RANOVA. Notice we are assuming that there are no cross-issue
effects, i.e., that reading time for one communication is
related to the attitude on only that issue and not to the atti-
Continuous Predictors in Repeated tude on the other issue, once reading time on that other
Measures Data issue is controlled. If we make the additional assumption
Reconceptualizing RANOVA in terms of the sum and dif- that the effects of the covariate, reading time, in each rele-
ference has the advantage of pointing to ways in which vance condition are the same, i.e., b1Z  b2Z  bZ (no reading
data that exhibit dependencies may be analyzed more flex- time by relevance interaction), a reasonable assumption in
ibly. First, it easily allows the inclusion of between-subject such an experimental paradigm, then it can be shown that
independent variables that vary continuously. For instance, the models for the analysis of the sums and differences of
maybe the between-subject variable is an individual differ- the Y’s, YSi and YDi, respectively, are as follows:7
ence measure such as the need for cognition (Cacioppo &
Petty, 1982). Rather than using a dichotomous variable, Xi, YSi  (b10  b20 )  (bZ )( Z1i  Z2 i )
to predict the sum and difference, we can use such a con-  (b1X + b2 X ) Xi  (e1i  e2 i )
tinuously varying predictor. Assuming that it has been YDi  (b10  b20 )  (bZ )( Z1i  Z2 i )
centered (Aiken & West, 1991),6 the interpretations for the  (b1 X  b2 X ) Xi  (e1i  e2 i )
various regression coefficients that we have already given
continue to hold, paralleling those for the RANOVA, albeit Accordingly, one might estimate the following two
with a continuous between-subject independent variable. models:
This reconceptualization also makes possible the YSi  bS 0  bSZ ZSUMi  bSX Xi  eSi
straightforward inclusion of continuously measured covari-
ates that vary both between and within subjects. Imagine, YDi  bD 0  bDZ ZDIFi  bDX Xi  eDi
for example that we also measured how long each partici-
where ZSiZ1iZ2i and ZDiZ1iZ2i.
pant spent reading each of the two persuasive communications.
We might want to analyze the attitude scores on each issue In the first of these models the estimated slope for Xi
controlling for the reading time measures. As before, the estimates the mean difference between the two A condi-
attitude scores, Y1i and Y2i, are the primary dependent vari- tions, controlling for or adjusting for mean reading time
ables of interest. The two reading time scores, the covariates, for each participant (i.e., is there an attitude difference as
we designate as Z1i and Z2i. Standard RANOVA proce- a function of strong versus weak arguments, on average
dures do not readily incorporate such covariates. across both attitude issues, once the average reading time
We can model each of the two Y variables as a func- for the two issues is controlled?). In the second model, the
tion of its respective Z variable and as a function of the intercept now estimates the predicted difference in the two
between-subjects factor A, coded as before by Xi: Y scores, Y1iY2i, between the two levels of the within-
subject factor R when the difference in reading times,
ZDiZ1iZ2i, equals zero. In essence, we are now asking
whether attitudes are more favorable for the higher rele-
6 In the earlier section on moderation, we discussed the impor- vance issue once the reading times for the high versus low
tance of having variables that are components of the interaction
product have zero points that are meaningful. Sometimes this is
7 Itis empirically unlikely that the two cross-condition effects
best accomplished by centering the variables, but there are also
other possible meaningful values that might be used. In the pres- would be exactly zero and that the effects of reading time will be
ent context, it is important that the independent variable have exactly equivalent in each relevant condition. If neither of these
a meaningful zero value because the intercept in the difference assumptions holds, then the sum and difference Y scores need
model equals the predicted difference as a function of the within- to be modeled as a function of both the sum of the Z’s and their
subject variable when the predictor equals zero. Fundamentally, difference. This complicates these models but it does not change
the issue here is the same as the issue discussed in the section on the fundamental interpretation of the coefficients in these models
moderation, because the intercept in the difference model esti- that we give below. A full exposition of these more complicated
mates the simple effect of the within-subject factor when the pre- models, not making the simplifying assumptions we make here,
dictor variable equals zero. is given in Judd, Kenny, and McClelland (2001).
Extending the Analysis of Repeated Measures 127

relevance issues are made equivalent.8 And the estimated Table 4.3 A 2  9 Repeated Measures ANOVA Source
Table (Factor A Between-subjects, Factor T Within-subjects)
slope for the Xi predictor in the second model continues to
estimate the A  R interaction, but this time holding con- Source df
stant the difference between the two reading times: Does Between Subjects
argument strength affect attitudes more for the high rel- A 1
evance issue even when we control for the linear effect of Error between n–2
Total between n–1
reading time? Finally, we can also interpret the coefficients
for the two Z variables in both models. In the sum model, Within Subjects
T 9
we are asking whether mean reading times are related to AT 9
mean attitude scores, averaging across the two relevance Error within 9(n – 2)
conditions, within levels of the between-subject A factor. Total within 9n
And in the difference model, the estimated slope for the Z
difference variable examines whether reading time differ-
ences as a function of relevance are predictive of attitude
differences. If we were to specify particular within-subject differ-
The example that we have just explored incorporates ences in which we were most interested, we could test each
continuous predictor variables that vary between partici- one of these individually, allowing different error terms for
pants, either measured once or measured at each level of a each. Suppose, for instance, in the present example, that
dichotomous within-subject independent variable. We can we were only interested in linear change in the Yki’s over
now extend our within-subject independent variable so that time. Assuming that these are taken from 10 adjacent days,
it becomes possible to treat it as continuously measured as we could compute a difference among them using contrast
well. We begin this extension by thinking first about the weights for the linear orthogonal polynomial for 10 obser-
analysis of a within-subject factor that is in fact categori- vations: 4.5, 3.5, 2.5, 1.5, 0.5, 0.5, 1.5, 2.5, 3.5,
cal, but that is an ordered categorical one having many levels, and 4.5 (i.e., time centered). We call these values of Tk.
such as time (T). Each participant is measured once each We now have one linear difference score for each partici-
day on some outcome variable for a total of 10 days. So pant, YLDi (equaling kTkYki ) and both this difference vari-
we now have 10 Y scores from each participant, Yki where able and the sum of the Yki scores, YSi ( ⌺kYki), could be
k represents time and i represents participant. Each par- regressed on the Xi variable that contrast codes the levels of
ticipant is, as before, under one of the two levels of the factor A:
between-subject factor A. So, for instance, we might be
YSi  bS 0  bS1 Xi  eSi
examining persuasion on a single issue over time (10 days)
as a function of argument strength (factor A). YLDi  bLD 0  bLD1 Xi  eLDi
Again, we could conduct a traditional RANOVA on the
resulting data, resulting in the source table contained in As before, the slope in the first model estimates the main
Table 4.3. Note that the degrees of freedom for both the T effect of factor A, collapsing across all ten levels of factor
main effect and the A  T interaction equal 9 and the error T, time. The intercept in the second model now estimates
term for testing these two effects has 9(n – 2) degrees of the linear main effect of T, examining linear changes on
freedom. If there are more than two levels of the repeated average in the Yki scores over time. And the slope in the
measures, as there are here, we encounter the likely viola- second equation estimates the degree to which those linear
tion of the assumption that all pairs of observations mani- changes over time differ for the two levels of factor A, i.e.,
fest the same degree of dependence.9 the linear time by A interaction.
The conceptual step from this analysis to one that
involves within-subject predictors that vary continuously
8 Note that the zero value of this Z difference score is theoretically comes with the realization that the linear time difference
meaningful—it represents the situation where the two reading
that we just discussed YLDi, as well as the composite sum
scores are equal to each other. Therefore, we are not centering
variable, YSi, emerge equivalently if we were to compute a
this predictor because it already has a meaningful zero point.
9 In actuality, the assumption is that each possible difference we separate regression equation for each participant, regress-
might compute among pairs of the S has the same variability across ing his or her 10 attitude scores, Yki, on a predictor variable
participants and thus that we can use a pooled or combined error that codes time, using the exact same values for that predic-
term in the within-subject portion of the source table. We also note tor that went into computing the linear polynomial differ-
that we can, should we wish to, conduct a multivariate analysis of ence, i.e., Tk. To understand these individual regressions,
variance, which does not make this restrictive assumption. imagine the data for a generic participant. There are two
128 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

variables for this participant, Yki and a variable called Tki. of Xi in the first model remains what it was in the model
We estimate a linear regression model for this participant where YSi was the criterion (i.e., the main effect of fac-
across his or her 10 observations, treating Yki as the criterion tor A—strong versus weak arguments); the interpretation
and Tki as the predictor. It can be shown that the resulting of the intercept in the second model remains what it was
intercept from this regression equals that participant’s mean in the model where YLDi was the criterion (i.e., the linear
Yki score, Y 10 , and the slope equals that participant’s YLDi
Si time difference in attitudes on average across the two lev-
score divided by constant, Y ∑ T . Thus each participant’s
LDi
2 els of factor A); finally, the interpretation of the slope in
k k
slope and intercept from their within-subject regression the second model also remains what it was in the model
model are perfectly correlated with their sum and dif- where YLDi was the criterion (i.e., the linear time by factor
ference scores, respectively, and these (the intercept and A interaction: Are the linear changes over time in attitudes
slope) could be used as the dependent measures in the different as a function of argument strength?).
between-subject regression models that were used before.
In general, for each participant, we estimate the follow-
Multilevel Modeling
ing model
So far, we have broadened traditional RANOVA by using
Yki  a0 i  a1i Tki  eki
sums and differences. We have shown that the sum and dif-
ference parallel an intercept and a slope that is estimated
In this model, a0i represents each participant’s intercept for each participant. There is an even more general esti-
(the mean of all 10 attitude scores for that participant), mation strategy of repeated measures data, called mul-
a1i is each participant’s slope (the difference in their pre- tilevel modeling or MLM. This method—which has also
dicted Yki scores as a linear function of time), and eki been called hierarchical linear modeling, mixed models,
is a residual or error in predicting each participant’s and random coefficient modeling—permits the analysis of
individual Yki scores as a linear function of time. We data that involve continuously varying predictors within
have intentionally used different letters to represent subjects, that relaxes the assumption that each participant
the regression coefficients in this model from the ear- is measured once and only once under each level of the
lier models in order to differentiate the levels at which within-subject independent variable, and that can handle
each model holds. This model is estimated at the level of random missing data (MCAR, see the later section of this
each individual participant, estimated across his or her chapter on missing data). Moreover, MLM is more flex-
observations; the earlier models were estimated across ible and more efficient than the computation of sums and
participants. Note also in this model that the intercept differences. We assume that each participant has multiple
and slope have i subscripts, thus indicating that these are observations, Yki, and that these have associated with them
estimated for each participant and, accordingly, vary values on various predictor variables that also vary within
across them. This model is commonly referred as the subjects, e.g., T1ki, T2ki. These can be measured continu-
level-1 growth-curve equation in the multilevel model- ously or categorically and may interact with each other.
ing literature. “Growth curve” here simply means that Individuals can vary in how often they are measured and
we are examining each individual’s change over time, the levels of the various predictor variables under which they
admittedly in this model simply the linear change over are measured. Then models are estimated at two levels, one at
time. Nonlinear time effects could be added to the model the level of the individual, across that individual’s k obser-
to make it more truly a growth “curve” model. vations, estimating that individual’s intercept and slopes.
As we have already explained, each participant’s esti- Then at the second level, across participants, models are
mated intercept and slope are completely redundant with estimated in which these intercepts and slopes are mod-
his or her YSi and YLDi composite variables. Accordingly, eled as a function of predictor variables that vary across
these estimates can be modeled as a function of the them. These person-level predictors can be categorical or
between-subject predictor: continuous.
Although this very general multilevel approach to the
a0 i  b00  b01 Xi  u0 i
analysis of dependent data may be done in a piecemeal
a1i  b10  b11 Xi  u1i fashion, using standard multiple regression procedures, it
is optimally done using MLM. This approach uses algo-
In these models, we have used u’s to indicate the errors or rithms that estimate the models at both levels simultaneously
residuals. These two equations are called the level-2 model and that optimally weight the individual observations
in multilevel modeling. The interpretation of the slope (Kenny et al., 1998). Unlike the piecemeal approach of
Extending the Analysis of Repeated Measures 129

sums and difference that uses least squares, MLM typically While those who use RANOVA can benefit from learn-
uses restricted maximum likelihood. Details on how to do ing MLM, multilevel modelers can benefit from concepts
this estimation are now available in many different sources from RANOVA. As an example, we return to a growth
(e.g., Hox, 2002; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002; Snijders & curve model and consider first the test of the slope. As we
Bosker, 1999). have shown above, in RANOVA the slope is computed for
To make clear the estimation at both levels simultane- each participant, and we can compute the average slope
ously as done by MLM, consider as before the case where and test its significance using the variance in slopes as
we have two levels, person being level 2 and time being the error term. In RANOVA, we can treat non-linearity
level 1. The model for each person, called the level-1 as an error term for the variance in slopes. In MLM, the
model, is as follows: researcher has a choice to treat the slope as either fixed
or random. Treating the slope as random implies that
Yki  a0 i  a1iTki  eki the slope interacts with person. If the slope parameter is
mistakenly treated as fixed when it is random, there is an
where a0i represents each participant’s intercept and a1i is increased probability of making a Type I error in the test
each participant’s slope. We can treat each random vari- of the slope. Because the test of variance of the slopes is
able in their level-2 equations: a test of moderation, it has low power. Thus, very often,
we may falsely not reject the null hypothesis of no slope
a0 i  b00  b01 X i  u0 i variance (i.e., make a Type II error), which will lead us to
a1i  b10  b11 X i  u1i falsely reject the null hypothesis that the average slope is
zero (i.e., make a Type I error). The most conservative test
Substituting these level-2 equations into the level-1 of the average slope is to presume that slopes are random.
model yields the combined equation for person i at obser- With MLM, the researcher has a great deal of flexibility
vation k: in testing assumptions about the correlational structure of
the errors. Unfortunately, many MLM researchers do not
Yki  (b00  b01 Xi  u0 i ) (b10  b11 Xi  u1i )Tki  eki avail themselves of that flexibility. We suggest the follow-
 b00  b01 Xi  b10 Tki  b11 Xi Tki  Tki u1i  u0 i  eki ing strategy. One might estimate the growth curve model
above. Then one compares the fit of two models.10 One is
It is this latter model that is actually estimated by MLM the standard growth curve model with random slopes and
procedures, but it often helps to think of the model as two intercepts. The other is the model in which slope and inter-
submodels, a level-1 and a level-2 submodel. Note that in cept are fixed, but the error structure can take on any form.
many ways this is a standard moderation model. The esti- The later model, in which the all the error variances and all
mated b00 is the predicted value of Yki when Xi and Tki are the covariances are free, is the more general model. What
zero; note if Xi and Tki are centered then b00 is like a grand we would hope to show is that the simpler model, the stan-
mean. The estimate b01 represents the effect of Xi, the level- dard growth model, fits as well as the more complicated
2 predictor, when Tki is zero; the estimate b10 is the effect of model.
Tki, the level-1 predictor, when Xi is zero; and the estimate A final point is that traditionally MLM treats time points
b11 represents the interaction of Tki and Xi. as nested within persons, whereas RANOVA treats time as
Importantly, in this model there is not one random error crossed with person. What is lost by treating the design
term but three, u1i, u0i and eki these can have different vari- as nested is the main effect of time. Note in Table 4.3,
ances and two of them, u1i and u0i, may be correlated with there is the main effect of time that has 9 df. Traditional
each other. Thus, returning to a central point made in the MLM estimates only one effect of time, the linear one. If
introduction to this chapter, these models allow multiple there were other effects of time, which there often are, the
random errors for individual observations, at the various model would be misspecified. It is possible to adapt MLM
levels under which those observations are nested. Thus, to the analysis of crossed designs (commonly called cross-
the inherent limitations in assuming a single set of errors classified in the MLM literature) and such an approach
are avoided. Moreover the interpretation of these variances should be considered instead of the more traditional nested
can be important. Consider the variance in the slopes of
effect of Tki on Yi. It tells us whether the effect of Tki var-
ies by participant, i.e., a participant by Tki interaction. In 10 Agrowth-curve model may not appear to be a nested under a
essence, the variance tells us whether there is some type of model in which the error variance-covariance matrix is free, but
moderation of the Tki effect across participants. as discussed by Singer and Willet (2003), it is.
130 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

approach. One advantage of using the crossed approach is Consider a model is which all three variables, the out-
that it is possible to allow for the effect of another indepen- come Yik, the mediator Mik, and independent variable Xik, all
dent variable (e.g., the effect of stress on mood) to interact vary over time within individuals. For instance, we might
with time as well as person. be interested in exercise behavior and subjective well-
Our goal in this section is to encourage social psy- being. The treatment (amount of exercise) varies within
chologists to think about the analysis of data that exhibit individuals over days. We assume that everyone exercises
dependencies in ways that considerably extend the typi- at least some each day. We also measure the outcome, sub-
cal RANOVA models that we all have learned well. At the jective well-being each day. Additionally, we might have
very least, we would hope that researchers expand their a mediator that is measured each day, such as mood while
analyses of repeated measures data by analyzing sum and exercising. In this case, we can estimate the mediational
difference scores and thereby incorporating continuously model slopes within each participant: ci would be the esti-
varying independent variables as between-subject predic- mated overall effect of exercise on subjective well-being
tors in such models. For even more flexibility, we strongly for the ith participant; ai would be the effect of exercise on
encourage readers to expand their analyses of data that mood for that participant; bi would be the partial effect
exhibit dependencies, or that exhibit a multilevel struc- of mood on subjective well-being controlling for exercise;
ture, by using multilevel modeling procedures. The fact and c´i would be the partial effect of exercise on subjective
that these procedures allow errors to be at multiple lev- well-being controlling for mood. Because these effects
els represents a very significant analytic advance. Again, in the mediational model, ci, ai, bi, and c´,i are random
we recommend that interested readers consult more thor- variables that may vary across individuals, they may also
ough treatments of multilevel models (e.g., Bickel, 2007; covary. As Bauer et al. (2006) and Kenny et al. (2003) have
Hox, 2002; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002; Snijders & Bosker, shown, the fundamental equality that underlies mediational
1999). models (c  c  ab) does not then hold because of the cova-
riance between ai and bi: People who show a larger effect of
Mediation in Repeated Measures exercise on mood while exercising may also show a larger
and Multilevel Data effect of mood while exercising on well-being. Bauer et al.
(2006) have developed a way to estimate this covariance.
With repeated measures data, in which each participant is If Xi varies only between individuals (e.g., an intervention
measured once in each condition, and where there is some to increase exercise), and is accordingly a level-2 variable,
hypothesized mediator measured in each condition as well, whereas the other variables in the mediational model, Mik,
the approach we outlined earlier for the analysis of sum and Yik, vary across level-1 units as well, then bi would be a
and difference scores can be used to examine mediation. In random variable, varying across level-2 units, but the other
essence, one asks whether the difference in the dependent effects in the mediational model would not be. And finally,
variable between conditions is reduced once the within- if Yik is the only variable at level 1 while Xi and Mi vary
subject difference on the potential mediator is controlled. across only level-2 units, then the mediational equality can be
This approach to analyzing mediation in repeated measures expected to hold.
data is thoroughly explicated in Judd et al. (2001). Again, it is worth re-emphasizing the very strong assump-
When we have many repeated measures and we apply tions that underlie the assessment of mediation. While a
MLM, one can estimate mediational models with appro- multilevel mediational analysis can certainly be conducted,
priate level-1 and level-2 models. A number of recent the multilevel nature of the data in no way makes those
papers have considered such multilevel mediational analy- causal assumptions any easier to make. For instance, for
ses (Bauer, Preacher, & Gill, 2006; Kenny, Korchmaros, the exercise example, we would have to assume that being
& Bolger, 2003, Krull & MacKinnon, 1999). The compli- in a good mood does not cause people to exercise more or
cations in such multilevel mediation models derive from that those factors other than exercise that result in better
the fact that the various variables in the models may be daily moods do not also increase daily estimates of subjec-
measured at the same or different levels. If the variables in tive well-being.
the mediating model are measured at level 1 (at different
times within the same individual in the repeated measures
example we have been using), then the slopes that estimate MISSING DATA
components of the indirect effect and the direct effect may
be random variables that vary between persons. This then Our studies almost always have missing data, but we often
introduces complications because these different effects act as though they do not. In recent years, considerable the-
may also covary across persons. oretical and analytic progress has been made in the handling
Missing Data 131

of missing data. Most social psychologists are unfamiliar The second type is Missing at Random or MAR. The data
with these important developments. In this section, we pro- loss is non-random, but missingness is due to a variable that
vide a brief introduction. We begin with a typology of missing is measured. The name for this type may seem a little strange
data. We then discuss strategies for handling missing data. because we start by saying the loss is non-random. But in
Finally, we discuss viewing other issues as missing fact it is random once we control for that variable that causes
data issues. missingness. Imagine for our example that gender determines
We consider the following as a running example. A who shows up for the posttest and who does not. It does not
researcher studies 200 people who are randomly assigned determine it perfectly, rather it is the only variable that has
to a treatment and control group, a variable we designate a systematic effect on showing up for the posttest. Further,
as T. There is a set of covariates measured at the pretest, assume that gender is one of the X variables measured at the
which are designated here as X variables. There are miss- pretest. Thus, the variable that determines missingness is one
ing data: Ten percent of the pretest sample (20 people) do of the variables that are measured. When we run the logistic
not show up for the posttest, Y. This example is much sim- regression, we would find that gender would be a predictor
pler than most examples in that there are only missing data of missingness. We would then need to control for gender in
on a single variable, the outcome variable. Nonetheless, it subsequent analyses.
can illustrate the major points that we want to make. The third type is Missing Not at Random or MNAR,
which is sometimes also called Nonignorable Missing
Data. Here, the variable causing the missing data is either
Typology of Missing Data
not measured or is itself missing. An important special case
Probably the most important contribution to the theory of is when the variable on which observations are missing,
missing data is the Rubin (1987; Little & Rubin, 2002) Y, is the cause of whether data are missing. For instance, it
typology of missing data. Unfortunately, the terms of might be that people who would score low on the posttest,
this typology can be confusing, not only to practitioners Y, are less likely to show up for that testing. Alternatively,
but even to methodologists and statisticians. They are nev- if gender caused missingness and Y, but we did not mea-
ertheless critical to the understanding of missing data. The sure gender, then we would have MNAR.
typology focuses on the process or the causal mechanism A serious difficulty arises from the fact that MNAR and
by which the missing data were created. As is to be seen, MAR are not empirically distinguishable. For instance,
the focus is on the variable(s) that bring about or cause the presume that we have MNAR and there is an unmeasured
missing data. We need to introduce a variable that social variable, for example “motivation to change,” that deter-
psychologists do not often consider: missingness. For each mines missingness. Imagine also that this variable is corre-
variable in the data set, there is a variable that equals one lated with gender. If we ran a logistic regression, we would
if the case is missing and zero if it is not missing. For our find that gender predicts missingness, but gender is not the
running example, we need only consider the missingness variable that truly causes missingness, rather it is motiva-
of the outcome variable. Essentially, the different types of tion to change that causes missingness. We need to have the
missing data in this typology concentrate on the model correct model of missingness. If we do and if that model is
underlying the missingness variable(s), i.e., what causes a MAR, the results from the logistic regression would give
score on a variable to be missing. us the right answer. But if we do not, and the correct model
The first type in the Rubin typology is Missing Completely is MNAR, the logistic model can lead us astray and make
at Random or MCAR. Here the data are lost by a random us think that we have MAR.
process. For instance, suppose it were entirely random who
shows up for the posttest and who does not. Thus, the vari- Strategies for Handling Missing Data
able that determines missingness is random (i.e., indepen-
dent) with respect to the other variables in the model. We can We consider the following strategies: case deletion, imputa-
empirically evaluate MCAR by running a logistic regression tion, and estimation without imputation. Finally, we outline a
in which we predict showing up at the posttest versus not (our method with which most social psychologists are unfamiliar:
missingness variable) using the X variables and T as predic- multiple imputation.
tors. In principle, none of the X variables or T should predict
Case Deletion
missingness. Little (1988) provides a general method across
multiple missingness variables for testing whether the data are The old-fashioned way of handling missing data is to
consistent with the MCAR assumption and his procedure is delete missing cases. One common strategy is what is
now implemented in many computer packages (e.g., SPSS’s traditionally called listwise deletion. The rule is that if a
Missing Values). case has missing data on one or more variables, the case is
132 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

missing entirely. There are several limitations with listwise A third alternative for imputation, and better than the
deletion. First, it can result in the loss of too much data. last set, is to take the multiple regression or maximum
Particularly when the study involves combining many likelihood estimates of missing values, and add to them
sources of data (e.g., a multiwave longitudinal study), the a random score that would produce the correct estimate
listwise-deleted data set may be too small for a meaningful of variability. For instance, if we were using a multiple
statistical analysis. Second, if the loss of data is not MAR, regression equation to predict posttest scores, we would
the sample means and variances resulting from listwise obtain an estimate of mean square error or the variances
deletion can be highly unrepresentative.11 For instance for of the residuals. We would then take our regression esti-
our example, if men are more likely not to show up at the mate of a missing value and add to it a random normal
posttest than women and men score lower than women, score whose variance would equal the mean square error.
then the posttest mean would end up being inflated due to We refer to this method as stochastic imputation. This
listwise deletion. method produces unbiased estimates of means, covari-
ances, and variances. Note that this method would yield a
Imputation
somewhat different estimate each time it is performed. Thus,
With imputation, the researcher substitutes a value for the if the analysis were redone, there would be differences, hope-
missing case. Most social psychologists use mean substitu- fully small, in the results, because the imputed data change.
tion for data imputation. With mean substitution, we take
as the posttest value for the missing cases the mean of cases Multiple Imputation
for which we do not have missing data. Mean substitution, With stochastic imputation, as just discussed, a random num-
though widely used, is problematic for several reasons. ber is added to the score. Thus, one source of sampling error
First, it assumes, probably incorrectly, MCAR, because is the imputation itself and so the process of estimating
the assumption is that the missing cases are just like all the the missing case creates “error.” With multiple imputation,
non-missing cases on average. Second, mean substitution we estimate the sampling variance due to imputation. As the
results in greatly reduced variances. That is, the variance name suggests, we create several different data sets, each with
of the posttest is necessarily underestimated because all of different imputed values. We can then measure how much the
the missing cases have values on the posttest that equal results change due to imputation. Multiple imputation was
the posttest mean. This will typically result in inflated originally suggested by Rubin (1987) and many important
Type I error rates. In general, mean substitution provides contributions have been made by Schafer (1997). Graham et
unbiased estimates only for means, and only then when al. (2003) and Sinharay, Stern, and Russell (2001) have writ-
missingness is MCAR. Even under MCAR, mean substi- ten introductions to the method for psychologists.
tution gives biased estimates for all other parameters (e.g.,
There are four steps in multiple imputation:
variances and regression coefficients).
Step 1: We first impute the missing cases using a stochastic
A second alternative is to use multiple regression12 to esti-
method.
mate the missing values. This strategy, which assumes MAR,
Step 2: We do the statistical analysis using the imputed
involves using available data to predict the posttest via mul-
values and save the standard errors and estimates of
tiple regression. Then using the coefficients of the regression
relevant parameters.
equation, we can obtain predicted scores for the missing cases.
Step 3: We redo the first two steps m times. (How large m
This method is much better than mean substitution because it
must be depends on how much missing data there are.)
provides unbiased estimates of covariances. However, regres-
sion derived estimates suffer from the same problem as mean Step 4: We pool estimates and we pool the standard errors and we
add to the standard error the variability due to imputation.
substitution: The imputed values have too little variability.
We can obtain statistically better and more general estimates Obviously, multiple imputation is computationally inten-
of missing values using EM algorithm (Graham, Cumsille, & sive, but it is better than single imputation methods to handle
Elek-Fisk, 2003; Schafer, 1997) but this method too suffers from missing data that is MAR. Increasingly, multiple imputation
the problem of the imputed values having too little variability. is an option that is becoming available in software packages.
As discussed by Schafer and Graham (2002), it is becoming
11 However, listwise deletion tends to perform as well as the better a very useful method for the handling of missing data.
methods we describe later for estimating regression coefficients
(Graham & Donaldson, 1993).
12 We are assuming that all the variables with missing data are
Handling Missing Data Without Imputation
measured at the interval level of measurement. If not, the strategy It is possible to handle missing data without either listwise
would need to be modified. deletion or imputation. A simple example is what happens
Invariance of Effects 133

when one has a scale with multiple items and one or more, Similarly, some counterbalancing designs involve stra-
but not all, of the items are missing for any person. To form tegically chosen missing data. Consider a design in which
a scale score, we use the available items and compute an we counterbalance experimental condition with stimulus. We
average across them. The general principle of this approach might use what is called a Latin-square design to do this. Such
is that we do not impute the missing values at all. Rather, a design does not fully cross the stimuli with all experimental
we use all the available data to obtain estimates of the rel- conditions, rather the stimuli are ordered and then the order-
evant variables or parameters of the model. In this case, the ing is crossed with conditions. In such a study, we do not
relevant variable is the average score on the variables in create all possible combinations of stimuli and condition. We
the scale for all non-missing items. strategically create “missing data” because the model that we are
A second more complicated analysis is growth curve interested in does not require the gathering of such data.
modeling. Imagine 50 persons who are measured at 30 times, Missing data play a role in causal inference. Consider
but for some people one or more time points are missing. the quasi-experimental design known as the regression dis-
However, all persons have three or more time points. We continuity design (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). In
can then estimate a slope and intercept for each person this design, persons are assigned to the treatment or con-
without needing to impute any of the missing cases. Thus, trol conditions not randomly but based on their scores on
the slope and intercept for each person (the relevant param- some X variable: those above a certain value of X receive
eters) can be estimated, and there is no imputation of the the treatment and those below that value of X are in the
missing observations. control group. For instance, one might evaluate the effects
A third example is the use of Full Information Maximum of being on the dean’s list on subsequent academic perfor-
Likelihood (FIML; Arbuckle, 1996) estimation within mance. Prior GPA determines who is on the dean’s list and
Structural Equation Modeling. We could apply FIML to who is not, and there is a fixed cut-point for making this
our missing posttest data set. What this procedure does decision. In this design, we do not observe treated persons
is treat the data as if there were two “groups,” one with a who score below the cut-point and control persons who
posttest and one without. It would then use the informa- score above it. We do not observe them because treatment
tion in the two variance–covariance matrices with means to is deliberately confounded with X. But because we mea-
estimate the effects. Again, there is no imputation of raw sure X and because we assume that X has a certain form of
scores. Graham et al. (2003) highly recommend the use of relationship with the outcome, usually linear, we can con-
FIML for missing data and see it as effective as multiple trol for the confounding effects of X. With the regression
imputation. discontinuity design, we have missing data, but we make
causal assumptions about the relationship between X with
the treatment variable and the outcome.
Missing Data Viewed More Generally
In general, we think that social psychologists need to
Modern thinking about missing data has greatly extended be much more creative in how they deal with missing data.
the way we can think about missingness. That is, the usual This will require approaching missingness using modern
conception of missing data is that we had hoped to gather methods. Finally, thinking about missing data creatively
all the data, but somehow we failed to do so. The more can lead to new insights about design decisions that are
modern approach is to think that sometimes we gather all made in the conduct of research.
the data we want to, but there were data that we could have
gathered but we decided not to. We briefly discuss two
such applications of this new approach: design differences INVARIANCE OF EFFECTS
and causal inference.
Consider the difference between a between-subjects The final topic that we wish to explore in this chapter is
design in which participants are in one of two conditions the concept of invariance. Numerous previous parts of the
and a within-subjects design in which participants are in chapter are related to the issue of invariance; our goal in
both conditions. We could view the between-subjects this section is to bring that work into sharper focus by
design as a within-subjects design with missing data. In a defining exactly what we mean by invariance, exploring
randomized experiment, for instance, we might think that its various forms, and arguing for a deeper appreciation for
subjects in the experimental condition have missing data invariance assumptions that are routinely made, and occa-
in the control condition and those in the control condition sionally explored, in social psychological research.
have missing data in the experimental condition. Assuming At the core of inferential statistics is the question of
that subjects have been randomly assigned, then we can invariance. The invariance question is the following: Across
assume that the data are MCAR. what entities are we making generalizations? Alternatively,
134 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

the question is for what entities does the model apply, in the the time in social psychological research where we assume
sense that the model parameters are invariant across them? that our results can be generalized across stimulus materials,
Most typically, social psychologists worry about general- contexts, times, and cultures without explicitly thinking about
ization across participants, but in fact there are many other whether such generalization is actually appropriate. All too
types of entities across which we might wish to generalize. often the only sort of invariance that we expect and exam-
These include, in addition to participants, the situations, ine is invariance across participants. There are many other
the times, the variables, the experimental stimuli, and factors or types of entities across which invariance ought
any other factor across which one might like to general- to be examined.
ize some set of results. Lee Cronbach, in developing what Now that we have developed a general overview of what
he and others have called Generalizability Theory, referred we mean by invariance and the alternative ways in which it
to these various factors as “facets” across which gener- is typically dealt with, we want to turn to several particular
alizability might be sought (Cronbach, Gleser, Nanda, & recurrent issues in social psychological research where we
Rajaratnam, 1972; Shavelson & Webb, 1991). think invariance tends to be problematically assumed and
It is the parameter estimates that we obtain from a spe- generally not examined.
cific dataset that we would like to be able to generalize.
Those estimates derive from a dataset collected from par- Failing to Examine Invariance
ticular participants, in a particular context, at a particular
time, examining a particular set of measured variables. In this section, we consider three different situations in
Such parameter estimates include means, variances, struc- which social psychologists often assume invariance but fail
tural coefficients (i.e., slopes, including mean differences), to examine that assumption: invariance across items, con-
and measurement coefficients (i.e., factor loadings). federates, and situations.
In the conduct and reporting of research results, there First, whenever one uses multiple questionnaire items
are three ways in which we may deal with (or not deal with) and then collapses these into a single total scale score, one
the question of the invariance of our parameter estimates. is implicitly assuming invariance across the items. By this
First, there are many occasions when we have an explicit we mean that all of the items in that scale are assumed to
hypothesis that invariance does not hold and we explicitly measure the same thing and that the effects of the inde-
test for the absence of invariance (i.e., we hypothesize that pendent variable across those items are invariant. It is the
parameter estimates vary in a specific way and we examine case that researchers are somewhat sensitive to this sort
this hypothesis). For instance, we anticipate that two groups of invariance assumption, as they are likely to report the
of participants have different means. Or we anticipate growth reliability of the resulting scale scores. Additionally, they
(e.g., means change over time). Or we anticipate mod- might conduct a factor analysis to establish their unidi-
eration (i.e., that the slope linking an independent to a mensionality. The important issue, however, in terms of
dependent variable depends on particular participant invariance is whether other variables relate to each of the
groups or contexts). Or we anticipate that individual vari- items in the same way, which is to say that the effects of
ables have different degrees of reliability in different sam- the independent variables are invariant across items.
ples. The key feature is that there is a specific hypothesis Another situation in which invariance is typically not
that model parameters vary in a particular way and that examined occurs when social psychologists use confeder-
hypothesis is evaluated. ates. Consider the classic case in social psychology where
A second way in which the invariance question is dealt one or more experimental confederates might be used to
with is when we expect an invariance to hold and we examine behavioral manifestations of prejudice directed
explicitly base our conclusions on tests of that invariance. toward minority group members. One might use confed-
This occurs whenever we estimate a standard error of a erates, who are members of a minority group, to examine
parameter estimate across some set of entities (most typi- how comfortable participants are interacting with minor-
cally participants). For instance, in testing the null hypoth- ity group members. In the sense just described, confeder-
esis that a particular slope or effect differs from zero, we ates can be seen as “items” and the question of invariance
derive its standard error across some set of entities and test across them becomes important. Typically, researchers pay
that null hypothesis. Our conclusion, given that the hypoth- attention only to the random variation across participants
esis is rejected, is that we can generalize that slope across when they should also be paying attention to random vari-
the population of entities from which we have sampled. ation that may be due to the confederates. All too often
And the final way in which the invariance question is experimenters assume invariance across confederates who
dealt with is when we presume invariance, either explic- are sampled from the minority group, but they are unable
itly or implicitly, and never examine it. This happens all to test for that invariance because they have just a single
Invariance of Effects 135

confederate. And when they do have multiple confederates, to which the treatment, for instance, is different from the
they do not include confederate in the statistical model. It control) is invariant across studies. We think that is essen-
is crucial to demonstrate that effects are invariant across tial for meta-analysts to code for the variability of the inde-
confederates in this example. pendent variable and to examine whether that variability
It is not just items and confederates for which we assume moderates the treatment effect.
but fail to examine invariance. Ironically, we social psy-
chologists, who sometimes pride ourselves on making clear
Invariance Assumptions in Logistic Regression
the power of the situation, fail to fully examine invariance
across situations. Consider the following example: an inves- Logistic regression is increasingly used in social psy-
tigation of the factors that lead people to help others. An chology to examine the effects of independent variables
investigator has a model that victim need (N) and provider on dichotomous outcome variables. Let us consider a
resources (R) both affect helping (H). The investigator simple example. Suppose we conduct a foot-in-door study
writes a single scenario about a hypothetical case, describ- and find more compliance when a small prior request is
ing the hypothetical victim and provider. This scenario is complied with than when no such prior request is made.
then read by all participants who provide judgments of N, Suppose also we conduct the study in China and Japan and
R, and H. Then, a model is estimated to measure the effect we find that the logistic regression coefficient, B, is greater
of N and R on H using participant as the unit of analy- in China than in Japan. Following Allison (1999), logistic
sis. Note that both N and R are variables that are likely to regression coefficients are inherently standardized and it is
vary much more over situations than over participants. As problematic to compare these B values across groups. The
a result, one should be asking whether effects are invariant issue is that to compare the B’s, we need to make the assump-
across situations rather than across participants. A better tion that the error variance is the same (i.e., is invariant) in
research design would be the following: One writes many both groups. This is akin to the homogeneity of variance
scenarios. One gives these scenarios to participants13 and in multiple regression. However, note that if the outcome
then one can examine the effects of N and R on H at three were continuous, then we could evaluate the homogeneity
different levels of analysis: person, situation, and their of variance assumption by computing the error variance in
interaction. One can determine if the causal effects are the the two countries. With a dichotomous outcome, there is
same (i.e., are invariant) across these three levels. no way to check for that invariance. Thus the finding that
the foot-in-the-door effect is different in Japan and China
might be due to the fact that other variables affect com-
Meta-Analysis
pliance less in one country than the other. For instance, if
We very often fail to realize that invariance assumptions there were some unmeasured variable that equally affected
are at the heart of meta-analysis. Consider a meta-analy- compliance in both countries but that varied more in one
sis of 20 studies of an experimental variable that has two country than the other (e.g., the politeness of the request
levels. Because different outcome variables are used in for compliance), then there would be differences in the
each study, the meta-analyst computes a d value (mean error variance in the two countries that would go unno-
difference between conditions divided by the pooled ticed. Readers might consult Allison (1999) who proposes
within-groups standard deviation) for each study. While a solution to this problem.
we routinely compute d, doing so makes very strong
invariance assumptions. One assumption is that the error Cross-Cultural Modeling
variance in each study, relative to the mean difference, is
a constant. Almost certainly, if all studies used the same An important issue in invariance concerns what is known
outcome measure and if we had the raw data from the stud- as measurement or factorial invariance, when we use the
ies that were combined, we would find that the equal error “same” measures in multiple groups. It is then important
variance assumption is violated. Another assumption is to examine whether the latent factor structure underlying
that the variability of the independent variable (the degree those measures is the same, i.e., invariant, across groups
(Chen, 2008; Meredith, 1993; Reise, Widaman, & Pugh,
1993). Only with some sort of measurement invariance
is it possible to make any comparison between different
13 We do not necessarily have to give every participant every sce- groups. The usual approach to measurement invariance
nario. Because of participant time limitations, it might be better is to estimate a measurement model in each group and then
to randomly divide scenarios into sets and then each participant to examine whether the coefficients of that model (most
would read one set of scenarios. typically factor loadings) are similar across groups. This
136 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

is notoriously difficult to do when exploratory principal Another approach to the question of invariance, fol-
components or common factor analyses are done in mul- lowing Nesselroade (2007; see also Nesselroade, Gerstrof,
tiple groups because of rotational indeterminacy issues. Hardy, & Ram, 2007), is as follows. We allow for the mea-
A more appropriate approach is through the use of surement model (i.e., loadings) to vary across groups, but
multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis, where one we force the structural model to be invariant. In this case,
estimates the same factor model simultaneously in mul- we argue that the measures relate to theoretical constructs
tiple groups and then statistically evaluates measure- in different ways in each culture, but the constructs relate
ment invariance. In the following, we assume some in the same way to each other. To accomplish this analysis,
basic knowledge of confirmatory factor analysis and we do the following. First, we do not use a marker vari-
structural equation modeling. Invariance of a measure- able strategy in the measurement (i.e., fix one loading on
ment model has several different meanings. In the least each factor to one), but rather we allow all the loadings
restrictive form of measurement invariance, the same of the indicators of latent variables to be free. Next, we
confirmatory model is examined in each group without fix the variances of all latent exogenous variables to one.
any between-group constraints on factor loadings or any Next, we fix all the endogenous disturbance variances to
other estimates. Such a model implies that the measured one. Finally, we fix the paths in the structural model to be
variables map onto the same set of latent factors in the invariant across groups.14
same manner, although the specific magnitude of load-
ings is allowed to vary across groups. A more restrictive
Summary
model is one with loading invariance, constraining all
of the loadings in one group to equal the loadings in the A fundamental issue is knowing the appropriate domain
other groups. If supported, this form of measurement over which our results are generalizable. We are fairly
invariance suggests that the factors are equivalently skilled at determining if our results are generalizable over
defined in each group, in other words that the loadings participants. We encourage researchers to broaden their
of individual items on their latent factors are equivalent thinking and think about other sources of variability. We
in the different groups. find it ironic that a field that emphasizes the importance
If the loadings are invariant across groups, then one of situations over persons (Ross & Nisbett, 1991) focuses
can explore other forms of invariance. If there is no causal much more on the degree to which results are invariant
model within each group, the focus would be on the invari- across persons and much more infrequently attends to
ance of factor variances and covariances and of means. invariance across situations.
For variances and covariances, one constrains factor vari-
ances and covariance across groups. For the means, one
CONCLUSION
initially fixes the intercepts of the measured variables to
be the same across groups; one sets the factor means
Returning to the theme in the introduction, data analysis is
to zero in one group, and frees the factor means in the other
detective work. All too often we think that looking at our
groups. If these factor means are nonzero, then the means
data is “fishing” and therefore improper. Rather, “playing
are not invariant. This provides a strong test that the groups
with our data” helps us understand the social reality that
are equivalent in their true means on the latent variables.
generated our observations. We believe that too much of
Although it may frequently be the case that loading invari-
data analysis is “publication oriented.” That is, we do only
ance is found across groups, variance/covariance invariance
the analyses that we need to include in the tables and results
and mean invariance of the latent factors are much less likely.
sections for the paper that we are writing. We need to learn
And even more restrictive is complete invariance, where the
that some analyses are worth doing even if they cannot
error variances of the measured variables are assumed to be
ultimately be published. Let us give some examples.
equal across groups. If all of these invariances are met, then
Sam Smith has a dichotomous outcome and is told that
the conclusion is that the full mean and variance/covariance
he needs to run logistic regression, a method with which he
structure of the variables is equivalent in the groups, a most
unlikely result.
If there is a structural model that is estimated in both
groups, where one or more latent factors is assumed to 14 Still another method to relax invariance assumptions is mixture
affect other factors, then the typical focus in on whether modeling where a model’s parameters can be different for dif-
those structural effects are invariant across groups. Before ferent people. For example, Muthén and Muthén (2000) found
this can be meaningfully evaluated, measurement invari- different growth curve trajectories for different groups of people.
ance must be established. These models are also called latent class models.
References 137

is unfamiliar. Besides the logistic analysis, he might run an Sure enough there is an extreme outlier in her data. After
ordinary multiple regression analysis treating the outcome she removes the outlier, she finds that the mean difference
as a 0/1 variable. Such an analysis is statistically incorrect, disappears.
and editors and reviewers would rightly be very critical. Our point in these hypothetical examples is that much
Nonetheless, Sam might be encouraged to do the analysis, can be revealed through the probing of data in ways that
because it should generally be very similar to the better go beyond standard and conventional analyses. Like a good
logistic analysis. If Sam can more easily comprehend the detective, the good data analyst searches for clues to a
wrong analysis, he can better understand the right analysis. causal argument through his or her data, wherever they
By doing the wrong analysis, Sam can see if he actually can be found. And often they turn up in the most surpris-
understands the right analysis. ing places, uncovered in ways of looking at data that are
Anna Jones has persons in groups and must conduct a not what journal editors and statisticians expect. We do not
multilevel analysis. She predicts a cross-level interaction in mean by this to denigrate the expectations of journal edi-
that gender differences (a level 1 or person variable) interacts tors and statisticians. Rather, we simply want to encourage
with a group size (three vs. eight; a level 2 or group variable). the probing of data. Moreover, we realize ideally that Sam
She uses the computer program HLM for the very first time Smith should understand the details of logistic regression,
and finds what she thinks is that interaction and computes and that Anna Jones should be well-versed in the use of the
graphs the predicted means using HLM. Anna would likely computer program HLM, that Arielle Cohen should have
benefit from redoing the analysis, ignoring group, and treat- had an a priori theory of the causal ordering of her depen-
ing person as the unit. From such an inappropriate analysis, dent variables, that Rui Lopes should have initially con-
she would obtain a 2  2 table of means. The means from this ducted an extensive item analysis on his inventory, and that
wrong analysis should be very similar to those she computed Lola Anthony should have screened her data for outliers
from the HLM analysis. If not, then likely the HLM analysis before doing the t-test. However, the last time we checked,
was done incorrectly or misinterpreted. we do not live in such an ideal world where everyone has
Arielle Cohen has a 2  2 between-subjects design in all the skills they need to analyze their data completely and
which she predicts a cross-over interaction and she has two most appropriately. Exploring data is how one learns about
outcome variables, Y1 and Y2. She finds the results that she one’s data and, at the same time, perfects one’s skills in
has predicted on each outcome variable. On a lark, Arielle data analysis.
treats Y1 as a covariate in the Y2 analysis. She notices that We social psychologists conduct laboratory and field
the interaction for Y2 disappears. When she makes Y2 the experiments, conduct surveys, perform meta-analyses, and
covariate, the interaction for Y1 is weakened but is still present. probe archival data. To accomplish all of these analyses,
By fooling around with his data, Arielle has preliminary we must be familiar with a very wide range of data-analytic
evidence that Y1 might be mediating the interaction for the methods. We hope that this chapter has provided our col-
Y2 variable, consistent with mediated moderation. leagues with some guidance to make us better detectives in
Rui Lopes measures depression with a 21-item inven- understanding the clues that social reality has given us.
tory. He averages scores on the items to get a total score
for each individual. In the process of doing his analysis,
he happens to examine the correlations between the indi-
vidual 21 items scores and the average score. He notices REFERENCES
that these correlations look rather different for the men in
his sample than for the women. This leads him to wonder Abelson, R. P. (1995). Statistics as principled argument. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
if the construct has the same meaning for men and women.
Aguinas, H. (2003). Regression analysis for categorical moderators. New
He begins to consider whether he should have a different York: Guilford.
theory about depression for the two groups. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and inter-
Lola Anthony has a simple two-group study, and she preting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
does a t-test and finds a large and statistically significant Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior:
difference between the two groups. All is fine, but on a Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of
lark she does a median split on the outcome and does a Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453–474.

simple 2  2 chi square contingency table analysis of her Allison, P. D. (1999). Comparing logit and probit coefficients across
groups. Sociological Methods and Research, 28, 186–208.
data. Much to her surprise, she finds no difference in the two
Arbuckle, J. L. (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of
groups, either in terms of statistical significance or the pro-
incomplete data. In G. A. Marcoulides & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.),
portions. This result suggests a problem, so she looks at Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp.
a full scatter plot of the full data set for the two groups. 243–278). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
138 Data Analysis in Social Psychology: Recent and Recurring Issues

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator vari- Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (1986). Consequences of violating the independence
able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strate- assumption in analysis of variance. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 422–431.
gic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in
Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindsey (Eds.),
Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J., & Gil, K. M. (2006). Conceptualizing and Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 233–265). Boston:
testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multi- McGraw-Hill.
level models: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Kenny, D. A., Korchmaros, J. D., & Bolger, N. (2003). Lower level media-
Methods, 11, 142–163. tion in multilevel models. Psychological Methods, 8, 115–128.
Bickel, R. (2007). Multilevel analysis for applied research: It’s just regres- Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents and
sion! New York: Guilford. consequences. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Cacioppo, J. T. & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Krull, J. L., & MacKinnon, D. P. (1999). Multilevel mediation modeling in
Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–132. group-based intervention studies. Evaluation Review, 23, 418–444.
Chen, F. F. (2008). What happens if we compare chopsticks to forks? The Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multi-
impact of making inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural research. variate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1005–1018. Association, 83, 1198–1202.
Cohen, J. (1968). Multiple regression as a general data-analytic system. Little, R. J. A., & Rubin, D. B. (2002). Statistical analysis with missing
Psychological Bulletin, 70, 426–443. data (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Cohen, J. (1978). Partialed products are interactions; partialed powers are MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On
curve components. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 858–866. the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlation Methods, 7, 19–40.
analysis for the behavior sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Introduction to statistical mediation analysis.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple Englewood, NJ: Erlbaum.
regression/correlation analysis for the behavior sciences (3rd ed.). MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analy-
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. sis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593–614.
Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence
longitudinal data: Myths and tips in the use of structural equation mod- of the mediation, confounding, and suppression effect. Prevention
eling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 558–577. Science, 1, 173–181.
Cronbach, L. J., Gleser, G. C., Nanda, H., & Rajaratnam, M. (1972). The MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., &
dependability of behavioral measurements: Theory of generalizability Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test the significance of
for scores and profiles. New York: Wiley. the mediated effect. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.
Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for integrating modera- Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (2003). Designing experiments and
tion and mediation: A general analytical framework using moderated analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (2nd ed.). Mahwah,
path analysis. Psychological Methods, 12, 1–22. NJ: Erlbaum.
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). McClelland, G. H. (2000). Nasty data: Unruly, ill-mannered observations
Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial can ruin your analysis. In H. T. Reis & C. M Judd (Eds.), Handbook of
attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 393–412).
Psychology, 69, 1013–1027. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE model of McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detect-
attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual ing interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114,
process theories in social psychology (pp. 97–116). New York: 376–390.
Guilford. Meredith, W. (1993). Measurement invariance, factor analysis, and facto-
Graham, J. W., Cumsille, P. E., & Elek-Fisk, E. (2003). Methods for han- rial invariance. Psychometrika, 58, 525–543.
dling missing data. In I. B. Weiner (Editor-in-Chief) and J. A. Schinka & Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When moderation is
W. F. Velicer (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology: Vol. 2. Research meth- mediated and mediation is moderated. Journal of Personality and
ods in psychology (pp. 87–114). New York: Wiley. Social Psychology, 89, 852–863.
Graham, J. W., & Donaldson, S. I. (1993). Evaluating interventions Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. K. (2000). Integrating person-centered and
with differential attrition: The importance of nonresponse mecha- variable-centered analyses: Growth mixture modeling with latent tra-
nisms and use of followup data. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, jectory classes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24,
119–128. 882–891.
Hox, J. (2002). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Nesselroade, J. R. (2007). Factoring at the individual level: Some mat-
Erlbaum. ters for the second century of factor analysis. In R. Cudeck &
Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1981). Process analysis: Assessing mediation R. C. MacCallum (Eds.), Factor analysis at 100: Historical developments
in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review, 5, 602–619. and future directions (pp. 265–292). New York: Routledge.
Judd, C. M., Kenny, D. A., & McClelland, G. H. (2001). Estimating Nesselroade, J. R., Gerstorf, D., Hardy, S. A., & Ram, N. (2007). Idiographic
and testing mediation and moderation in within-participant designs. filters for psychological constructs. Measurement, 5, 217–235.
Psychological Methods, 6, 115–134. O’Brien, R. G. (1979). A general ANOVA method for robust test of additive
Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. H., & Ryan, C. S. (2008). Data analysis: A models for variance. Journal of the American Statistical Association,
model comparison approach (2nd ed.). New York: Taylor and Francis. 74, 877–880.
Kashy, D. A., Donnellan, M. B., Ackerman, R. A., & Russell, D. A. (2009). Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984) The effects of involvement on responses
Reporting research in PSPB: Practices, principles, and pragmatics. to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persua-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin., 35, 1131–1142. sion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 69–81.
References 139

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for esti- Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non-
mating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research experimental studies: New procedures and recommendations.
Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731. Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strate- Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assess-
gies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator ing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428.
models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879– 891. Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis:
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing mod- Modeling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University
erated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Press.
Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 185–227. Sinharay, S., Stern, H. S., & Russell, D. (2001). The use of multiple impu-
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: tation for the analysis of missing data: New approaches to missing data.
Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, Psychological Methods, 6, 317–329.
CA: Sage. Snijders, A. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction
Reise, S. P., Widaman, K. F., & Pugh, R. H. (1993). Confirmatory factor to basic and advance multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
analysis and item response theory: Two approaches for exploring mea- Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects
surement invariance. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 552–566. in structural models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological methodology
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives 1982 (pp. 290–312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal
Rubin, D. B. (1987) Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. chain: Why experiments are more effective than meditational analy-
New York: Wiley. ses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 89, 845–851.
Schafer, J. L. (1997) Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. London:
Tukey, J. W. (1969). Analyzing data: Sanctification or detective work.
Chapman & Hall.
American Psychologist, 24, 83–91.
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state
Tukey, J. W. (1980). We need both exploratory and confirmatory. American
of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147–177.
Statistician, 34, 23–25.
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and Wegener, D. T., & Fabrigar, L. R. (2000). Analysis and design for non-
quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: experimental data: Addressing causal and noncausal hypotheses. In
Houghton Mifflin. H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social
Shavelson, R. J., & Webb, N. M. (1991). Generalizability theory: A primer. and personality psychology (pp. 370–392). New York: Cambridge
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. University Press.
PART II

The Social Being


Chapter 5

Social Cognitive Neuroscience


MATTHEW D. LIEBERMAN

Who we are as humans has a lot to do with what happens have become leaders in the field, despite few having pub-
between our ears. What happens between our ears has a lot lished social cognitive neuroscience findings at that point.
to do with the social world we traverse, engage, and react There were introductory talks on social cognition and cog-
to. The former has been the province of neuroscience and nitive neuroscience by Neil Macrae and Jonathan Cohen,
the latter the province of social psychology for nearly a respectively, along with symposia on stereotyping (William
century. Recently, scientists have begun to study the social Cunningham, Jennifer Eberhardt, Matthew Lieberman,
mind by literally looking between the ears using the tools and Wendy Mendes), self-control (Todd Heatherton, Kevin
of neuroscience. Social cognitive neuroscience uses the tools Ochsner, and Cary Savage), emotion (Ralph Adolphs,
of neuroscience to study the mental mechanisms that cre- Turhan Canli, Elizabeth Phelps, and Stephanie Preston),
ate, frame, regulate, and respond to our experience of the imitation and social relations (Alan Fiske, Marco Iacoboni,
social world. On its worst days, social cognitive neurosci- David Perrett, and Andrew Whiten), and theory of mind
ence is phrenological, cataloguing countless brain regions (Chris Ashwin, Josep Call, Vittorio Gallese, and Kevin
involved in the vast array of social processes. On its best McCabe). If this meeting represented the first time that all
days, social cognitive neuroscience enhances our under- of the ingredients of social cognitive neuroscience were
standing of the social mind as well as any other method. mixed together in a single pot, the water was already boil-
The goals of this handbook chapter are to give an ing when the ingredients were tossed in. To appreciate
overview of the human history of this research area how the pot got this way, several historical strands must
(Section I), to summarize the techniques common to this be highlighted.
approach (Section II), to survey the functional neuro- In the early 1990s, John Cacioppo used the term “social
anatomy of social cognition (Section III), and to discuss neuroscience” (Cacioppo, 1994) to characterize how the
how brain research can make specific contributions to the social world affects the nervous system. Work in this area
social psychological enterprise (Section IV). A special was mostly health relevant (Berntson, Sarter, & Cacioppo,
note to social psychologists with little intrinsic interest in 1998; Kiecolt-Glaser & Glaser, 1989; Segerstrom, Taylor,
the brain trying to determine whether social cognitive neu- Kemeny, & Fahey, 1998) or animal research (Carter, 1998;
roscience is worth getting acquainted with: Go straight to Insel & Winslow, 1998; Panksepp, 1998) examining the
Section IV. impact of social factors on the autonomic, neuroendocrine,
and immune systems (Blascovich & Mendes, this volume).
In other words, early social neuroscience primarily focused
I. HISTORY on how the social world affects the peripheral nervous system
and other bodily systems. Although neurocognitive mecha-
The Oxford Dictionary of Psychology (Colman, 2006) nisms clearly fall under the umbrella of social neuroscience,
identifies a 2001 conference, held at the University of there were few investigations linking social processes with
California, Los Angeles, as a starting point for social cog- brain processes during the 1990s. Social cognitive neuro-
nitive neuroscience. This was the first formal meeting on science represented a new arm of social neuroscience that
social cognitive neuroscience, and many of the attendees primarily focused on the neurocognitive mechanisms of

I would like to thank Naomi Eisenberger and members of the UCLA Social Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory for various discussions
about the contents of this chapter.

143

CH05.indd 143 12/22/09 5:33:29 PM


144 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

everyday social cognition. Subsequently, the terms “social cognitive processes that were seemingly similar. These
cognitive neuroscience” and “social neuroscience” have lines of research are the precursors of social cognitive neu-
largely become synonymous because the domains and meth- roscience and served as inspiration for many who would
ods of study have merged. go on to work in this area.
Although social cognitive neuroscience reached its Finally, a great deal of human capital was spent bring-
boiling point around 2001, with numerous scientists begin- ing social cognitive neuroscience into existence. Influential
ning to use neuroscience methods to study social cogni- scientists already doing social neuroscience, such as John
tion, there were isolated programs of research focusing on Cacioppo and Ralph Adolphs, helped promote funding for
social cognitive neuroscience in the 1990s. Antonio and and publication of social cognitive neuroscience research.
Hannah Damasio’s work on the socioemotional changes in Established top-notch social psychologists including Todd
individuals with ventromedial prefrontal cortex (PFC; see Heatherton, Mahzarin Banaji, Neil Macrae, and Susan
Table 5.1 for a list of acronyms and neuroscience terms Fiske began conducting social cognitive neuroscience
used in this chapter) (Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & research and lent much-needed credibility to the fledg-
Anderson, 1994) sparked great interest in social cognitive ling area of research. Finally, Steve Breckler and Carolyn
neuroscience, affective neuroscience (Panksepp, 1998), Morf, program officers at the National Science Foundation
and neuroeconomics (Camerer, Loewenstein, & Prelec, (NSF) and National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH),
2005). Chris and Uta Frith began an extremely fruitful line respectively, had the vision to fund young scientists in this
of research on the neural bases of theory of mind in the area, before the area even existed.
mid-1990s (Fletcher et al., 1995), a topic that is founda- Stir all these ingredients together and drop in a generous
tional within social cognitive neuroscience. Stan Klein and helping of motivated graduate students and, voilà: social
John Kihlstrom examined self-knowledge by examining a cognitive neuroscience. In 2000, the term “social cognitive
patient with temporary amnesia, providing the best early neuroscience” first appeared in two papers (Lieberman,
example of how neuroscience could provide constraints on 2000; Ochsner & Schachter, 2000), and the first functional
social psychological theories (Klein, Loftus, & Kihlstrom, magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study examining a
1996). Research on the neural bases of face and biologi- traditional social psychology topic was published (Phelps
cal motion processing were relatively advanced in this et al., 2000). In 2001, the first review of social cogni-
period (McCarthy, Puce, Gore, & Allison, 1997), but not tive neuroscience was published (Ochsner & Lieberman,
yet in a way that resonated with traditional social psycho- 2001), although, in truth, the paucity of published research
logical questions. Finally, Cacioppo, Crites, and Gardner at that time made this review as much a promissory note as
(1996) examined the neural bases of attitudes and evalu- a progress report.
ative processing using event-related potentials (ERPs) In the decade since, social cognitive neuroscience has
and demonstrated important dissociations between social gone through an explosion. In 2001, a Google search for
“social cognitive neuroscience” returned 6 hits. In 2009,
the same search returned over 52,000 hits (see Figure 5.1).
Table 5.1 Acronyms and Jargon in Social Cognitive Neuroscience Similarly, the number of empirical social cognitive neuro-
PFC Prefrontal Cortex
science articles published each year has steadily increased
from 2000 through 2008 (see Figure 5.1). There have been
STS Superior Temporal Sulcus
numerous literature reviews of social cognitive neurosci-
TPJ Tempoparietal Junction
ence (Adolphs, 2001; Amodio & Frith, 2006; Bechara,
FFA Fusiform “Face” Area
2002; Blakemore, Winston, & Frith, 2004; Lieberman,
ACC Anterior Cingulate Cortex 2007a; Ochsner, 2004, 2007), not to mention a few cri-
Anterior Towards the front of the brain tiques (Cacioppo et al., 2003; Kihlstrom, 2006; Vul, Harris,
Posterior Towards the back of the brain Winkielman, & Pashler, 2009; Willingham & Dunn, 2003).
Rostral Towards the front of the brain There have been special issues on social cognitive neurosci-
Caudal Towards the back of the brain ence in several journals, including Journal of Personality
Dorsal Towards the top of the brain and Social Psychology (2003), Neuropsychologia (2003),
Ventral Towards the bottom of the brain Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (2004), Neuroimage
(2005), Brain Research (2006), New York Academy of
Superior Towards the top of the brain
Sciences (2007), Group Processes and Intergroup Relations
Inferior Towards the bottom of the brain
(2008), and Child Development (2009). Two new jour-
Lateral Away from the middle of the brain
nals were founded in 2006 to focus on this area of study:
Medial Towards the middle of the brain
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience (SCAN) and

CH05.indd 144 12/22/09 5:33:29 PM


Methods and Analysis 145

Google hits for SCN (cumulative)


60000
52600

42700
40000
Number of Hits

29500

21600
20000

3530

6 115 346 922


0
2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009
Year

Empirical SCN publications (per year) (Aug. 08)


200
196

150 162
Number of Publications

142

100
Figure 5.1 Growth of social cognitive neu-
94 roscience. The top panel displays the num-
92
ber of hits returned from a Google search of
“social cognitive neuroscience” on January 1
50 53 of each year from 2001–2009. The bottom
42 panel displays the number of social cogni-
33
20 tive neuroscience empirical articles published
0 each year from 2000–2009. Note that the 196
2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 articles indicated for 2008 were from January
Year through August.

Social Neuroscience. Several funding agencies have had to understand the tools themselves (this section) and the
special funding initiatives for social cognitive neurosci- techniques (next section) used to draw inferences about
ence; these agencies include the National Institute of social psychological processes in the brain. The primary
Mental Health, National Institute of Drug Addiction, tools used are neuroimaging techniques (fMRI, PET, ERP)
National Institute of Aging, and the National Institute of and lesion studies.
Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Finally, there have been
a series of social cognitive neuroscience preconferences Positron Emission Tomography
and small meetings, and now a yearly Social and Affective The earliest neuroimaging that focused on functional brain
Neuroscience (SAN) conference. In this decade, social localization was PET. In PET, the subject is either injected
cognitive neuroscience has gone from virtually nonex- with or inhales radioactive tracers that attach to biologi-
istent to having an increasingly firm foundation and the cally active molecules. Gamma rays from these tracers can
other accoutrements of a scientific discipline. then be detected with PET, allowing for the identification
of where the tracers are traveling in the brain during dif-
ferent kinds of mental activity. Typically, PET scans have
II. METHODS AND ANALYSIS
a temporal resolution of about a minute (i.e., one aggre-
gate data point per minute) and a spatial resolution of
Social Cognitive Neuroscience Methods
about a cubic centimeter. Apart from being the first form
Before jumping into a review of what has been learned with of functional neuroimaging of the whole brain, PET’s
the tools of social cognitive neuroscience it is important greatest advantage is that different kinds of molecules can

CH05.indd 145 12/22/09 5:33:30 PM


146 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

be tagged by tracers, thereby allowing studies to examine brain region and thus it is difficult to make inferences to a
not just blood flow in the brain but also the distribution of specific region.
neurochemical processes. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) allows for the
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is a nonin- creation of temporary lesions to a particular region of cor-
vasive neuroimaging technique that has replaced PET as the tex and thereby overcomes some of the limitations of lesion
dominant mode of functional neuroimaging largely because studies. TMS relies on electromagnetic pulses, which stim-
of its better temporal resolution (1 to 2 seconds) and spatial ulate the neurons in a small area of cortex. This is typi-
resolution (approximately 3 mm3). Most fMRI studies use cally done to excite the neurons until they stop operating
blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) fMRI to determine efficiently. Functionally speaking, this repetitive TMS will
which brain regions are more or less active during any psy- take a brain region offline for several minutes, allowing
chological task. BOLD fMRI works on the principle that researchers to determine which temporary lesions produce
the blood flowing to an active region is more oxygenated performance deficits on tasks of interest.
than blood elsewhere, and oxygenated blood has differ-
Neuroimaging Analyses
ent magnetic properties than deoxygenated blood: fMRI
can detect the spatial location of these different magnetic Most published social cognitive neuroscience research has
properties and reconstruct where blood was flowing to. used fMRI, and thus it is worth describing in more detail
A limitation of fMRI is that each condition of interest must how analyses are conducted with fMRI data (also see
typically be represented by several trials, which can lead Lazar, 2008). This section is provided with an eye toward
to habituation and contamination effects. Also, nearly the social psychologist who may want to know a bit more
all fMRI analyses are comparisons between experimental about the steps involved in inferring that “region X is
conditions within a subject, typically aggregated across more active during task A than during task B,” without
subjects. Between-group analyses are the exception, not having to mire through too much jargon.
the norm, and even these are between-group comparisons
Preprocessing
of within-subject comparisons. Various social psychologi-
cal findings become difficult to replicate with fMRI if sub- fMRI data are typically preprocessed before analyses
jects are exposed to all task conditions. are conducted. What this means is that various things are
done to the raw data that are obtained during scanning to
Event-Related Potentials make the information suitable for analysis. One can think
Event-related potentials (ERPs) are derived from an elec- of it a bit like statistically normalizing scales before com-
troencephalograph (EEG), which measures the summated bining them or applying log transformations to make a
electrical activity from neurons firing in the outer layers distribution more normal. In fMRI studies, realignment,
of the cortex. ERPs are the reliable responses that occur normalization, and smoothing are the standard components
time-locked to a stimulus or response, averaged over sev- to preprocessing. It should be noted that each of these steps
eral trials. The two primary advantages of the ERPs are introduces some noise to the signal while improving the
that they directly measure the brain’s electrical activity and signal in other ways. Assumptions go into how each of
have millisecond temporal resolution, allowing for exqui- these steps is performed, and the practical implementation
site measurement of time course. Two weaknesses of ERPs of these assumptions is never perfect.
are that only the outer cortex can be reliably assessed and Realignment is a process that corrects the brain images
the spatial resolution of ERPs is quite poor. to account for the motion of a subject’s head while in the
scanner. Small movements of a few millimeters in any
Lesions direction can alter whether the signal appears to be com-
By examining individuals with damage to different brain ing from one brain structure or another. Realignment uses
regions and observing the ensuing psychological deficits, structural features of the brain to determine how the brain
one can determine the contributions of the damaged regions has moved and then “puts the brain back” in the same space
to psychological function. The great advantage of lesion as the brain was in during a reference scan. When success-
studies over neuroimaging methods is that neuroimaging ful, realignment ensures that the amygdala, for instance,
only identifies regions active during psychological pro- shows up in the same place in the acquired brain images
cesses but cannot establish their causal relevance, whereas throughout the entire data collection.
lesion studies yield causal inferences. The main limitation Whereas realignment tries to ensure that an individual’s
of lesion studies is that damage is rarely limited to one brain maintains its own constant “coordinate space,” the

CH05.indd 146 12/22/09 5:33:30 PM


Methods and Analysis 147

goal of normalization is to put all of the different subjects’ articles that show yellow and orange “blobs” typically
realigned brain scans into a single coordinate space so represent the regions that cross some threshold (e.g., 10
that the brain structures line up across subjects. Brains contiguous voxels all with regression values of p < .001)
come in all shapes and sizes, and normalization essentially for consistency with the experimental regressor.
morphs different brains into a common space. Different These analyses yield the brain regions for a single subject
programs do this in different ways, and no method does that are sensitive to task demands. Our interest is usually
this perfectly. in generalizing to the population at large, so we combine
Spatial smoothing is the last key step in preprocessing. single-subject whole-brain analyses across subjects to
Smoothing involves averaging over adjacent “voxels” (i.e., determine which brain regions are reliably active across
three-dimensional [3D] pixels) in the brain images. This subjects. This is done by computing one sample t-test at
provides a number of benefits in terms of enhancing the each voxel, using the parameter estimates (i.e., regression
detection of certain kinds of signals, but this is done at coefficients) from each subject at the same voxel. If the
the expense of diminishing the likelihood of detecting other average parameter estimate from each subject in a particu-
kinds of signals. Usually this is a desirable trade-off, but it lar region is large enough, it will emerge as significant in
again demonstrates that the data analyzed in fMRI studies this random effects analysis.
are far from their raw state and represent a series of deci-
Region of Interest Analyses
sions and transformations that render the data more analyz-
able, while sometimes introducing problems when the data Neuroimaging studies commonly report the results of
do not conform to the assumptions behind the transforma- region of interest (ROI) analyses. Such analyses reflect the
tions. In many ways this differs little from the assumptions search within a specific region of the brain for significant
that are made in statistical analyses but are often untested in activations. ROI analyses can serve several different pur-
our behavioral studies (heteroscedasticity anyone?). poses in a study. One benefit of searching within a smaller
region of the brain is that it reduces the number of simul-
Whole-Brain Analyses taneous statistical tests and thus reduces the burden of cor-
The great majority of analyses reported in fMRI research recting for multiple comparisons. It also allows for a priori
are whole-brain analyses comparing brain activations under hypothesis testing by intentionally searching within brain
two task conditions across all of the voxels in the brain. regions thought to be relevant to the comparison. In some
For instance, imagine a study in which the subject spends ways, this is analogous to performing one-tailed rather than
alternating 30-second periods looking at pictures of ingroup two-tailed t-tests where a more lenient test can be performed
members and then outgroup members, for a total of 3 min- because a precise hypothesis is specified. One unfortunate
utes. Say we want to know which brain regions are dif- side effect of papers that rely solely on the ROI approach is
ferentially activated under these two conditions. The MRI that they can give the inadvertent impression that only the
scanner may collect a full brain volume (i.e., a set of images examined regions are involved in a process of interest.
taken at roughly the same time that, stacked together, cover Whatever the purpose of an ROI analysis, it is impor-
the entire brain) every 3 seconds, and thus there are a total tant to know exactly what kind of ROI analysis is being
of 60 volumes takes over the 3-minute scan. Each of the reported. There are at least two kinds of distinctions to
60 volumes represents a time point; thus, at each voxel in be drawn between different ROI analyses. First, an ROI
the brain there is a 60-point time series reflecting the rela- can be either functionally or anatomically defined. An
tive activation of each voxel. The statistical tools convolve anatomically defined ROI involves trying to find the true
a hypothetical BOLD response (i.e., a model of how the borders of a brain structure on the brain images. Functional
blood oxygenation typically rises and falls over time in an ROIs ignore anatomical boundaries and instead use some
active area) with the experimental design to create a hypo- existing pattern of activation to identify the ROI. For
thetical time series of what a brain region’s activity would instance, one might run a “localizer scan” (Saxe, Brett, &
look like if it were differentially sensitive to the two con- Kanwisher, 2006) to define an ROI using a task well known
ditions of the experiment. This hypothesized time series is to activate a particular brain structure and then examine
then regressed against the actual time series at every voxel what that ROI does in some new experimental condition.
in the brain to see which voxels in the brain show a pat- The second kind of distinction among ROI analyses
tern of activation consistent with the hypothesized pattern. concerns whether the ROI is treated as a “supervoxel” or
When several contiguous voxels from a brain region all a “search space.” Some ROI analyses treat the ROI as a
show the hypothesized pattern across time, it is generally space within which significant clusters of activation can
inferred that this region of the brain is more active under be detected. Other ROI analyses treat the ROI as a single
one condition than another. The brain images in published entity that is either significant as a whole or not.

CH05.indd 147 12/22/09 5:33:31 PM


148 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Each kind of ROI analysis described here is valid, and imagine that the true dynamics between some regions
there are more kinds that were not described. Nevertheless, involve time lags of up to a few seconds (e.g., 2 seconds
it is critical to know which kind of ROI is being used of prefrontal effort toward self-control might be needed
because each supports different kinds of inferences and has before downstream reductions in amygdala area are
different limitations. observed). A between-subjects connectivity analysis might
still capture this effect because it does not make assump-
Connectivity Analyses tions about the temporal dynamics, but a functional connec-
Researchers are increasingly interested in the relationships tivity analysis would probably miss the effect. Solutions to
between brain regions, rather than focusing on what each these problems, allowing for hypothesis-driven time lags,
brain region is doing independently. Connectivity analyses are being worked on (Formisano et al., 2002).
provide an estimate of the extent to which brain regions are
Regression Analyses
showing coordinated activity under particular task condi-
tions. Inverse connectivity is also of interest within social Because social psychologists are interested in how social
cognitive neuroscience because this indicates that two and personality factors interact to affect task behavior,
brain regions show a pattern consistent with one region social cognitive neuroscience commonly uses regression
regulating the other. These analyses do not establish cau- analyses in fMRI. Regression analyses are straightforward
sality, as they are entirely correlational; however, the cor- to run in most fMRI statistical packages. Here, a vector of
relations do point to the regions that are good candidates to regressor values, one value per subject, is entered into a
have causal effects. whole-brain comparison of two task conditions. The output
There are two main kinds of connectivity analyses will look like any whole-brain analysis with p-values for
that correspond roughly to between-subjects and within- each voxel, indicating the reliability for the correlation, and
subjects analyses. Between-subjects connectivity analyses brain maps displaying clusters of activation. For discussion
are much easier to conduct, but they are less likely to be able of the characterization of such analyses as “voodoo,” see
to provide strong evidence that brain regions are actually papers by Vul et al. (2009) and Lieberman, Berkman, and
working together or at odds with one another. Such anal- Wager (2009).
yses involve correlating a single estimate of activity for With use of this technique, any trait-level or self-
each subject in one brain region with a single estimate of report variable can be used to examine whether it is
activity for each subject in another brain region. What such associated with the pattern of activity across subjects.
analyses reveal is whether the extent to which a person Socioeconomic status, neuroticism, and rejection sensi-
activates brain region X more during task A than task B tivity are just a few of the trait variables whose relation
is associated with the activity in brain region Y during the to neural responses have been examined. One can also
same comparison of task A and B. For instance, is the mag- assess behavior that occurs after the scanning procedures
nitude of activity in a region of prefrontal cortex for each to examine the relation of that behavior to neural responses
subject during attempts at self-control, compared with a during a relevant task in the scanner. For instance, one
baseline task, inversely associated with the magnitude of could examine whether individual differences in automatic
amygdala activity across subjects as well. The limitation mimicry in a laboratory setting are associated with indi-
of this procedure is that a single average estimate of activity vidual differences in the magnitude of imitation-related
during the task is used (i.e., how much did a subject activate brain activity in an fMRI scanning session. The between-
the prefrontal region averaged across all self-control trials subjects connectivity analyses described earlier are actu-
of the task?) and thus it says nothing about the temporal ally just a special application of this kind of regression
dynamics of the brain regions. analysis.
The second type of connectivity, functional connectiv- One can also use physiological, behavioral, or self-
ity, addresses this issue by examining the extent to which report responses obtained during the scanning session itself
the time series of activation in two regions are correlated as a regressor at the single-subject level. Here, as with func-
with one another. Specifically, functional connectivity tional connectivity, the regressor of interest is correlated
assesses whether the time series of activation between with the time series of activity to determine whether the
brain regions X and Y are more strongly correlated under two are related. For instance, a study might involve the pre-
task A than under task B. This analysis must be carried sentation of 50 works of art and obtain the subject’s rating
out on each subject individually and then aggregated of desirability for each. These ratings can then be entered as
across subjects. The conceptual limitation of these anal- a regressor unfolding over time to determine, within a sub-
yses is that they typically assess only how brain regions ject, which brain regions have activity that rises and falls
are correlated at the same moment in time. One can easily with this psychological response.

CH05.indd 148 12/22/09 5:33:32 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 149

Reverse Inference It has been observed that autistic individuals, compared


Reverse inference refers to a particular difficulty in with healthy matched control subjects, show less amygdala
drawing psychological inferences from neuroimaging data activity when presented with emotional faces (Baron-
(Poldrack, 2006). Ideally, neural activations could serve Cohen et al., 1999; Pelphrey, Morris, McCarthy, & LaBar,
as markers that a particular psychological process has 2007). The initial inference drawn was that the amygdalae
occurred. If we could confidently assert that every time the of autistic persons were less sensitive to faces or the emo-
amygdala is activated some form of fear processing has tional content of faces. However, autistic individuals also
occurred, this would be a boon to social psychologists spend less time looking at the eyes of a face than do healthy
for whom the limitations of self-report and introspection individuals, and the eyes are extremely important for iden-
are well-known (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). Unfortunately, tifying emotional expressions (Adolphs et al., 2005). When
the amygdala is activated under numerous task conditions, eye gaze differences were accounted for, using eye-tracking
including, for instance, getting a reward. Without a one- equipment in the scanner, there were no remaining dif-
to-one correspondence between function and structure, ferences in amygdala responses of autistic versus control
reverse inferences become far less reliable (Ochsner, 2007). subjects (Dalton et al., 2005). Similarly, when a patient
In truth, reverse inference is a part of almost every with amygdala damage who was impaired at recognizing
study and will continue to be. It is only slightly different fear expressions (Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio,
outside of fMRI research. For instance, reaction times can 1994) was retested with instructions to attend to the eyes
vary for any number of reasons, and thus it is problem- of the target faces, the patient performed at normal levels
atic to assume that it necessarily reflects the number of (Adolphs et al., 2005). These results change the interpreta-
underlying operations or the difficulty of each operation. tion of the original findings, suggesting that the amygdala
Realistically, reverse inference will always be a potential directs eye gaze to important cues in the environment and
inferential problem, but several steps can be taken to mini- that autistic individuals and those with amygdala damage
mize the problem. are less likely to spontaneously do this. Another possibil-
First, a focus on networks of brain regions rather than ity is the amygdalae of autistic persons are hypersensitive,
a single brain region can help dramatically. For instance, rather than hyposensitive, to distressing social information
the dorsomedial PFC, posterior superior temporal sulcus and therefore look less at these stimuli. It is natural to think
(STS), and temporal poles are commonly coactivated that the extent to which a region of the brain responds to a
when subjects perform theory of mind or mentalizing stimulus presented in the scanner reflects that brain region’s
tasks (i.e., thinking about the psychological states and sensitivity to that class of stimuli. Knowing where the sub-
characteristics of another; Frith & Frith, 2003). Although jects are looking, what they are attending to, or what they
the temporal poles may be activated under various task are thinking about while processing the stimulus can lead to
conditions (e.g., semantic processing), there is little evi- very different interpretations.
dence that all three regions are coactivated under condi-
tions that do not involve mentalizing (Cabeza & Nyberg,
2000). Thus, the presence of any one of the three regions III. FUNCTIONAL NEUROANATOMY
may not be a valid marker for mentalizing, but the three
together may constitute a marker. Connectivity analyses Welcome to the “lite-brite” portion of the chapter. This sec-
can also suggest that these regions are working in concert tion reviews the known neural bases of social cognition,
with each other during a particular task, strengthening the self-processes, and processes specific to social interactions.
inference further. Localizer scans can help as well. If each “Lite-brite” is a pejorative term, based on a toy from the
subject performs an explicit mentalizing task prior to a 1960s, for studies that examine social psychological pro-
second task where we would like to surreptitiously assess cesses in the scanner and see what lights up. This is also
whether mentalizing is occurring, functionally defined referred to as brain mapping and has gotten something of
ROIs can be created for each subject in the particular a bad rap. Social psychologists have rightly pointed out
regions used for mentalizing. It is then possible to deter- that knowing where a process occurs in the brain does not
mine whether those same ROIs are activated during the in itself add one iota to psychological theories. But some-
subsequent task. times, such studies lead to other studies that do add an iota
or two to our theories. Sometimes, several brain mapping
Eye Movement Confounds
studies considered together can suggest new divisions and
Another consideration before leaving this section concerns commonalities between processes that might not have been
how eye movement may dramatically alter our interpreta- obvious from other behavioral and self-report methods (see
tion of neuroimaging (and for that matter, behavioral) data. Section IV).

CH05.indd 149 12/22/09 5:33:32 PM


150 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Social Perception and maximally activated by facial stimuli (Kanwisher,


McDermott, & Chun, 1997; McCarthy et al., 1997). This
Humans and other primates are sensitive to a wide array of
region has been dubbed the fusiform face area (FFA) by
nonverbal cues of social significance. We may not always
Kanwisher and colleagues (1997). A second, more posterior
reflect on the meaning of these cues, but ongoing social
region that also shows face selectivity has been called the
perception invariably influences our thoughts, feelings, and
occipital face area (OFA; Hoffman & Haxby, 2000).
behaviors. Basic capacities of social perception are taken for
A significant challenge to the equating of the FFA and
granted in many models of social cognition, yet it is these
OFA with face processing came from Gauthier, Skudlarski,
basic capacities that received the most attention in the early
Gore, and Anderson (2000). Gauthier argued that the FFA
days of social cognitive neuroscience. Cognitive neurosci-
is specialized for expert visual processing and that face
entists have extensively studied the neural bases of face and
processing is just one obvious application of this region’s
body perception, biological motion, action observation,
computations. Gauthier created fictional animals (“gree-
and emotion recognition. Each of these social perception
bles”) and found that increased perceptual experience
processes is reviewed in this section (see Figure 5.2).
with greebles led to increases in FFA activity. Similarly,
Face and Body Perception car and bird experts show significant activity in the FFA
and OFA to cars and birds, respectively (Gauthier et al.,
Face perception research has been a major topic for
2000). Kanwisher notes that across these studies, the FFA
neuroimaging research since the mid-1990s. The primary
still shows the greatest activation to faces (Grill-Spector,
question has been whether there are regions of the brain that
Knouf, & Kanwisher, 2004).
are tuned specifically for the processing of faces or whether
Another approach (Haxby et al., 2001) suggests that
faces are one of many entities decoded through a common set
although the FFA may be most attuned to faces, whereas
of perception processes. A number of neuroimaging studies
other regions of occipitotemporal cortex are more respon-
have converged on a region of the fusiform gyrus, which
sive to nonface objects, this is not the only metric that
links the occipital and temporal cortices, that is selectively
matters. Regardless of what class of object each of these
regions is most responsive to, the activity in each of
these regions still discriminates between the presence and
Social Perception absence of numerous kinds of stimuli. Thus, the FFA may
be most relevant to processing faces and yet still partici-
pate, along with other regions in a distributed network, in
the processing of various nonface stimuli.
Just as the FFA is particularly responsive to the presence
of faces, another region in occipital cortex, referred to as
the extrastriate body area (EBA), is more active when sub-
jects are presented with bodies than when shown faces or
other stimuli (Downing, Yuhong, Shuman, & Kanwisher,
2001). Interestingly, the response of the EBA is greater
when the head is occluded than when the head is visible
(Morris, Pelphrey, & McCarthy, 2006). The EBA is also
more active when subjects view bodies from a distance,
allocentrically, rather than from an egocentric perspective
typically associated with viewing one’s own body directly
(Chan, Peelen, & Downing, 2004).

1 posterior superior temporal Biological Motion


sulcus 5 amygdala
The fact that even infants have the ability to discriminate
2 fusiform “face” area 6 inferior parietal lobule
3 extrastriate “body” area 7 ventrolateral PFC – pars opercularis between biological motion (i.e., movements consistent
4 occipital “face” area 8 ventrolateral PFC – pars orbitalis with the biomechanics of biological organisms) and non-
biological motion (Fox & McDaniel, 1982) suggests that
Figure 5.2 The brain regions involved in social perception (face
the brain may have dedicated support for processing bio-
and body perception [2–4], biological motion perception [1], action
observation [6, 7], and emotion recognition [5, 8]). Numbers in logical motion. Like many aspects of nonverbal decoding,
brackets correspond to the regions in the figure reliably associated biological motion simply appears to us in perception as
with a particular aspect of social perception. qualitatively different from nonbiological motion.

CH05.indd 150 12/22/09 5:33:33 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 151

Biological motion is detectable from “point light 2008; Decety et al., 1997; Johnson-Frey et al., 2003; Lamm,
walkers” (Johansson, 1973) in which only a handful of Batson, & Decety, 2007; Lotze et al., 2006; Molnar-Szakacs,
points identifying a target’s joint locations are shown Kaplan, Greenfield, & Iacoboni, 2006; Pierno et al., 2009).
as the target moves. Several fMRI studies have shown that One study examining the effects of cognitive load on action
the posterior STS (see Figure 5.2) is more active dur- observation found that IPL and posterior STS activity were
ing the presentation of point light walkers than various unaffected by load but that ventrolateral PFC responses
control stimuli (Grèzes et al., 2001; Grossman & Blake, to action were absent during load (Chong et al., 2008).
2002; Vaina, Solomon, Chowdhury, Sinha, & Belliveau, In addition, the posterior STS and temporoparietal junction
2001). Additionally, increased posterior STS activity (TPJ) have been observed in some of these studies as well
to point light walker stimuli over a period of training is (Chong et al., 2008; Lamm, Batson, et al., 2007; Liljeström
associated with improvements in behavioral performance et al., 2008).
(Grossman, Blake, & Kim, 2004). Even sounds of people
Emotion Recognition
walking activate the posterior STS (Bidet-Caulet, Voisin,
Bertrand, & Fonlupt, 2005; Saarela & Hari, 2008). Finally, Recognizing the emotional displays of other people is one
lesions to this region produce deficits in processing point of the most frequent and important forms of nonverbal
light walkers (Saygin, 2007). Together, these findings sug- decoding performed by humans. Such displays provide
gest a strong link between this form of biological motion relatively automatic, prereflective access into the psycho-
detection and the posterior STS (cf. Noguchi, Kancoke, logical state of others, although it should be noted that
Kakigi, Tanabe, & Sadato, 2005). The inferior parietal lob- the bare perception of these displays does not necessarily
ule (IPL) and FFA have also been implicated in a subset of imply that those psychological states are being explicitly
point light walker studies (Grèzes et al., 2001; Grossman & represented or processed.
Blake, 2002; Grossman et al., 2004; Vaina et al., 2001). A number of brain regions have been implicated in
Processing the gaze direction of others has also the processing of emotional facial expressions; however, the
reliably activated the posterior STS, particularly in the vast majority of studies have focused on the amygdala.
right hemisphere (Cloutier, Turk, & Macrae, 2008; The amygdala has been a central focus of study in affective
Hoffman & Haxby, 2000; Hooker et al., 2003; Mosconi, neuroscience more generally, in part because of its clear
Mack, McCarthy, & Pelphrey, 2005; Pelphrey, Morris, & causal role in fear conditioning in rodents (LeDoux, Iwata,
McCarthy, 2005; Pelphrey, Singerman, Allison, & McCarthy, Cicchetti, & Reis, 1988) and its frequent activation in neu-
2003; Pelphrey, Viola, & McCarthy, 2004; Wicker, Perrett, roimaging studies of fearful faces (Morris et al., 1996).
Baron-Cohen, & Decety, 2003). Young children show this Since these early studies, it has become clear that the
effect (Mosconi et al., 2005), whereas individuals with amygdala can respond to both positively and negatively
lesions to the superior temporal region have gaze-process- valenced stimuli (Hamann, Ely, Hoffman, & Kilts, 2002),
ing deficits (Akiyama, Kato, Muramatsu, Saito, Nakachi, as long as they are high in arousal (Anderson, Christoff,
et al., 2006; Akiyama, Kato, Muramatsu, Saito, Umeda, et al., Panitz, De Rosa, & Gabrieli, 2003; Cunningham, Raye, &
2006). Hoffman and Haxby (2000) observed that the presen- Johnson, 2004), as well as various facial expressions
tation of faces showing different gaze cues could modulate (Fitzgerald, Angstadt, Jelsone, Nathan, & Luan Phan, 2006;
FFA or posterior STS activity depending on whether sub- van der Gaag, Minderaa, & Keysers, 2007). An increas-
jects were instructed to attend to the targets’ identity or ingly common view is that the amygdala serves as a detec-
gaze, respectively. Similar to the observation of walking, gaze tor of potential emotional significance of things in the
perception also modulates activity in the IPL (Hoffman & environment. Consistent with this view, the amygdala is
Haxby, 2000; Pelphrey et al., 2003). responsive to novelty, regardless of valence or arousal,
as new things may provide as yet unidentified reward or
Action Observation threat (Schwartz et al., 2003).
Action observation involves the perception of biologi- If the amygdala is part of the brain’s advance scout team
cal motion that implies a specific action is being enacted determining what is important to focus on and react to, one
intentionally. Most action observation studies have exam- would expect this region to operate very efficiently. There
ined the neural responses to “reaching to grasp” actions or is now converging evidence to suggest that the amygdala
other hand actions. These studies have commonly observed processes the emotional significance of perceptual stim-
increased activity in the left IPL and left posterior ventro- uli automatically. First, the amygdala responds to threat
lateral PFC (bleeding into the contiguous region of ventral stimuli presented subliminally (Morris, Öhman, & Dolan,
premotor cortex) during action observation compared with 1998; Whalen et al., 1998) or in binocular rivalry para-
control stimuli (Chong, Williams, Cunnington, & Mattingley, digms (Pasley, Mayes, & Schultz, 2004; Williams, Morris,

CH05.indd 151 12/22/09 5:33:34 PM


152 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

McGlone, Abbott, & Mattingley, 2004). Second, individuals prosody have been shown to activate the right superior
who have damage to visual pathways still produce activa- temporal gyrus or STS in a region anterior to the region
tion of the amygdala to emotional stimuli (Anders et al., commonly observed in studies of biological motion
2004; Hamm et al., 2003; Vuilleumier et al., 2002). Third, (Beaucousin et al., 2007; Wiethoff et al., 2007). When the
intracranial recordings of amygdala activity suggest that it emotional tone heard is explicitly labeled, there is still STS
responds to emotional stimuli within 200 ms of their pre- activity along with activity in right or bilateral ventrolateral
sentation (Krolak-Salmon, Hénaff, Vighetto, Bertrand, & PFC (Bach et al., 2008; Ethofer et al., 2006; Wildgruber,
Mauguière, 2004). Finally, amygdala activity to emotional Pihan, Ackermann, Erb, & Grodd, 2002; Wildgruber et al.,
stimuli is preserved under some forms of cognitive load 2005). Identifying emotion from bodies has been shown
(Anderson et al., 2003; Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver, & to activate the right posterior STS, right TPJ, EBA, amyg-
Dolan, 2001), although not always (Pessoa, McKenna, dala, and bilateral temporal pole each in one of three stud-
Gutierrez, & Ungerleider, 2002). ies (de Gelder, Snyder, Greve, Gerard, & Hadjikhani, 2004;
With respect to facial expressions, the role of the amyg- Grèzes, Pichon, & de Gelder, 2007; Peelen, Atkinson,
dala has been most clearly established in lesion studies by Andersson, & Vuilleumier, 2007), with only the bilateral
Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio, and Damasio (1995) demonstrat- ventrolateral PFC appearing in multiple studies. Finally,
ing that damage to the amygdala produces deficits in iden- one study has used a standardized test of nonverbal decod-
tifying emotional expressions, particularly fear. Lesions to ing ability, the Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity (Rosenthal,
the insula (Calder, Keane, Manes, Antoun, & Young, 2000), Hall, DiMatteo, Rogers, & Archer, 1979), and observed
basal ganglia (Calder, Keane, Lawrence, & Manes, 2004), posterior STS, left IPL, left TPJ, and bilateral ventrolateral
and ventromedial PFC (Heberlein, Padon, Gillihan, Farah, & PFC activity while labeling the emotional state of the tar-
Fellows, 2008) have also been shown to impair identifica- gets. Additionally, those self-reporting greater social skills
tion of one or more facial expressions. produced larger increases in right ventrolateral PFC, dor-
The FFA is also modulated by emotional expressions somedial PFC, and basal ganglia.
compared with neutral faces; however, a series of studies by
Vuilleumier and colleagues have demonstrated that this Social Inference
response is likely due to feedback from the amygdala after
the amygdala has already processed the facial expression. Social inference has been at the heart of social cognition
First, the pattern of activity in amygdala and FFA under for more than three decades. Social inference encompasses
dual-task conditions is more consistent with the amygdala’s a variety of processes invoked as we form representations
influence over FFA than visa versa (Vuilleumier et al., of the psychological states, traits, and preferences of oth-
2001; Vuilleumier, Mohr, Valenza, Wetzel, & Landis, 2003). ers. These inferences can be made using inferential alge-
Second, patients with amygdala lesions do not show bra (Jones & Harris, 1967), covariation analyses (Kelley,
greater FFA activity to emotional than nonemotional faces 1973), stereotype-based inferences (Ames, 2004; Fiske &
(Vuilleumier, Richardson, Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2004). Neuberg, 1990), or by projecting oneself onto the target
The right ventrolateral PFC is another region that is (Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). Some of these processes
commonly activated during emotion recognition. This occur automatically, whereas others occur slowly guided
activity may be specifically related to explicitly identi- by specific inferential intentions that require cognitive
fying an emotional expression (Lieberman et al., 2007; resources and effort (Gilbert, Pelham, & Krull, 1988).
Nomura et al., 2004), as this region is less often observed Despite social psychology’s focus on a deficit in the ability
during passive viewing of emotional faces and is typically of humans to make sense of other minds (Gilbert & Malone,
absent if attention is directed toward nonemotional aspects 1995), the vast majority of social cognitive neuroscience
of emotional faces. This parallels the finding of decreased studies of social inference have been inspired by the study of
activity in the ventrolateral PFC during action observation children developing the ability to make sense of other minds
under cognitive load, described earlier, and other similar (i.e., “mentalizing”). Wimmer and Perner (1983) first used
findings in the domains of visual self-recognition (Sugiura false-belief tests to determine when children begin to show
et al., 2000) and imitation (Lee, Josephs, Dolan, & basic mentalizing competence. These tasks are usually of the
Critchley, 2006), described later. following form: Person A knows that X is true (e.g., Sally
Although the lion’s share of emotion recognition has knows her marble is in the box on the left); while Person A is
focused on facial expressions, some studies have examined absent, things are changed such that X is no longer true (e.g.,
emotional prosody (i.e., tone of voice) as well as body posi- while Sally is out of the room, Anne moves the marble to the
tion and movements as indicators of emotional state. Passive box on the right). The subject is then asked what Person A
presentations of emotional compared with nonemotional now believes about X. The subject knows that X is no longer

CH05.indd 152 12/22/09 5:33:35 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 153

true, but Person A does not and therefore the subject should Frith, & Frith, 2003; Walter et al., 2004) or to determine
indicate that Person A believes X is true. Good performance what one’s own intention would be in particular situations
is thought to indicate that the child has a theory of other (Blakemore, den Ouden, Choudhury, & Frith, 2007; den
minds (i.e., theory of mind; Premack & Woodruff, 1978) and Ouden, Frith, & Blakemore, 2005).
that other minds can represent the world differently from our A final set of mentalizing studies has examined online
own. Most children master this basic mentalizing skill by age mentalizing as it occurs in the context of interaction with
three or four. other people (although not face-to-face). In three studies,
subjects played strategy games (e.g., prisoner ’s dilemma)
Mentalizing against a person or computer, under the assumption that
To isolate the neural correlates of mentalizing, several mentalizing should occur only when playing against a
researchers have used verbal stimulus materials, includ- person (Fukui, Murai, Shinozaki, 2006; Gallagher, Jack,
ing variants of the false-belief paradigm described earlier Roepstorff, & Frith, 2002; Rilling, Sanfey, Aronson,
(Gobbini, Koralek, Bryan, Montgomery, & Haxby, 2007; Nystrom, & Cohen, 2004). In a fourth study, subjects
Grèzes, Berthoz, & Passingham, 2006; Grèzes, Frith, & believed they were either collaborating on a task with the
Passingham, 2004; Mitchell, 2008; Perner & Aichhorn, experimenter or working alone (Gilbert et al., 2007). In a
2006; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe, Moran, Scholz, & fifth study, professional taxi drivers drove a simulated taxi
Gabrieli, 2006; Saxe, Schulz, & Jiang, 2006). Other ver- in a virtual reality environment in which they interacted
bal tasks have used short stories that require mentalizing to with numerous other characters whose mental states were
explain a target’s behavior, but do not specifically depend relevant (Spiers & Maguire, 2006).
on a false belief (Fletcher et al., 1995; Gallagher et al., 2000; In addition to these different methods for studying men-
Happé et al., 1996; Hynes, Baird, & Grafton, 2006; Saxe & talizing, there have been a handful of studies that have
Kanwisher, 2003; Völlm et al., 2005). Also, some verbal tasks reported on the neural bases of irony and idiom compre-
are used to assess the ability to infer other individual’s feel- hension (Lauro, Tettamanti, Cappa, & Papagano, 2008;
ings, rather than thoughts (Hynes et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory, Wakusawa et al., 2007; Wang, Lee, Sigman, & Dapretto,
Tibi-Elhanany, & Aharon-Peretz, 2006; Shamay-Tsoory & 2006a, 2006b). Comprehension of irony and idiom involves
Aharon-Peretz, 2007; Vollm et al., 2006). understanding of communicative intent and requires dis-
Other tasks induce mentalizing nonverbally. Several tinguishing literal from contextually suggested meanings.
studies have used animations of geometric shapes inspired Therefore, these tasks probably require similar, if not iden-
by the classic Heider and Simmel (1944) fighting triangles tical, processes as those used for mentalizing.
video (Castelli, Frith, Happé, & Frith, 2002; Gobbini et al., Across 45 tasks/studies,1 three regions were present in
2007; Moriguchi et al., 2006; Ohnishi et al., 2004; Schultz, more than half of the studies (see Table 5.2 and Figure 5.3).
Imamizu, Kawato, & Frith, 2004), which promote anthro- The dorsomedial PFC (Brodmann areas [BA] 8/9) was
pomorphism and mental state attributions to the shapes. reported in 91% of mentalizing tasks, whereas the TPJ and
Some nonverbal tasks require inferences to be drawn about temporal pole were reported in 59% and 52%, respectively.
mental states from a target’s eyes (Baron-Cohen et al., The posterior STS and precuneus were each observed in
1999; Platek, Keenan, Gallup, & Mohamed, 2004) or use 39% of studies, and the medial PFC (BA 10) was observed
nonverbal cartoons in which subjects choose a final panel in 33%. In approximately half of the studies reporting
based on their understanding of the target’s mental state temporal pole, posterior STS, and TPJ activations, these
from the earlier panels (Brunet, Sarfati, Hardy-Baylé, & activations were bilateral. In those studies in which
Decety, 2000; Gallagher et al., 2000). these regions were reported in only one hemisphere, only
A third type of mentalizing study examines judgments of the posterior STS was reliably lateralized, appearing in the
enduring psychological characteristics of others via impres- right hemisphere in 88% of these nonbilateral studies.
sion formation, for example, by asking what characteristics Table 5.2 also breaks down the activations by mentaliz-
the person has (Harris, Todorov, & Fiske, 2005; Heberlein & ing induction type for any method that has been used at least
Saxe, 2005; Mitchell, Banaji, & Macrae, 2005a, 2005b; four times (false belief, story, animation, impression forma-
Mitchell, Cloutier, Banaji, & Macrae, 2006), and via con- tion, intention inference, online mentalizing, and irony and
ceptual perspective-taking, for example, by asking how idiom comprehension). There are three notable conclusions.
the person would judge topic X (Ruby & Decety, 2003, First, the dorsomedial PFC is the only region that is reliably
2004). Inferences about momentary intentions have also
been examined in paradigms that require subjects to infer
the intentions of others (Ciaramidaro et al., 2007; German, 1
Studies including runs of more than one method are counted
Niehaus, Roarty, Giesbrecht, & Miller, 2004; Kampe, separately for each method’s results.

CH05.indd 153 12/22/09 5:33:36 PM


154 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Table 5.2 Activations from 45 Mentalizing Studies


DMPFC TPJ Temporal Pole pSTS Precuneus MPFC IFG VMPFC Fusiform Gyrus
False belief (n⫽8) 88% 88% 25% 25% 63% 25% 25% 0% 0%
Verbal stories (n⫽6) 83% 100% 67% 17% 33% 17% 17% 17% 0%
Animations (n⫽5) 100% 0% 100% 100% 20% 20% 40% 20% 80%
Impression formation (n⫽6) 100% 66% 33% 33% 66% 33% 33% 50% 0%
Intention inference (n⫽6) 100% 67% 67% 67% 67% 67% 33% 17% 17%
Online mentalizing (n⫽5) 100% 40% 40% 20% 20% 40% 0% 0% 0%
Irony & idioms (n⫽4) 75% 25% 75% 75% 0% 50% 25% 50% 0%
Total (n⫽45) 91% 59% 52% 39% 39% 33% 24% 15% 13%

Mentalizing Mentalizing

Mirror System Empathy

Figure 5.3 The brain regions involved in


social inference. The top row of images
displays the regions commonly activated in
mentalizing and theory of mind tasks. The
bottom left image displays the mirror sys-
1 dorsomedial PFC 6 ventrolateral PFC – pars opercularis and tem. The bottom right image displays brain
2 precuneus/posterior cingulate ventral premotor cortex regions identified in studies of empathy.
7 inferior parietal lobule
3 temporal junction Note: Anterior insula is displayed on the medial
8 dorsal anterior cingulate cortex wall for presentation purposes, but is actually
4 posterior superior temporal sulcus
9 anterior insula between the medial and lateral walls of the
5 temporal pole 10 medial PFC cortex.

activated by each mentalizing paradigm. Second, anima- These results suggest that the dorsomedial PFC may
tion-induced mentalizing consistently recruits the temporal play a central role in mentalizing in general, with other
pole and posterior STS, but not the TPJ; verbally induced subsets of regions being recruited for particular kinds of
mentalizing via false belief and other verbal stories consis- materials or task demands. There is at least some evidence
tently recruits the TPJ, but not the temporal pole and poste- to suggest that dorsomedial PFC activation is modulated
rior STS. This is consistent with the notion that the STS and by an explicit mentalizing goal and can be taken offline by
TP are involved in nonreflective social cognition, whereas cognitive load in dual-task paradigms. Conversely, the
the TPJ, as part of lateral parietal cortex, is involved in more posterior STS and temporal pole can be activated to
reflective aspects of social cognition (Satpute & Lieberman, mentalizing-relevant materials in the absence of a mentaliz-
2006; Liberman, 2009b). Finally, although fusiform gyrus ing goal and are still activated to the same degree with and
was observed in 13% of the studies overall, it was present in without cognitive load (den Ouden, U. Frith, C. Frith, &
80% of the animation-based studies. Blakemore, 2005; Mason, Banfield, & Macrae, 2004;

CH05.indd 154 12/22/09 5:33:36 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 155

Mitchell, Macrae, & Banaji, 2004; van Duynslaeger, van the results of studies looking at mentalizing in individuals
Overwalle, & Verstraeten, 2007). with amygdala lesions is mixed (Han, Jiang, Humphreys,
A number of lesion studies have also helped to identify Zhou, & Cai, 2005; Shaw et al., 2007; Stone, Baron-Cohen,
the regions that causally contribute to mentalizing. These Calder, Keane, & Young, 2003), a study by Shaw et al. (2004)
studies can be subdivided into those that focus on the pre- may help explain the amygdala’s role in mentalizing and
frontal cortex, TPJ, or amygdala. Several lesion studies why it does not appear in most neuroimaging studies. Shaw
have demonstrated prefrontal involvement in mentalizing; et al. (2004) compared 15 subjects with congenital amyg-
however, these studies do not provide much anatomical dala damage from birth or early childhood to 11 subjects
specificity (Channon & Crawford, 2000; Stone, Baron- with amygdala lesions that developed in adulthood. Early
Cohen, & Knight, 1998; Stuss, Gallup, & Alexander, 2001). damage was associated with a variety of mentalizing defi-
One study did find that left ventrolateral PFC was asso- cits, whereas late damage was not. Moreover, the subject’s
ciated with impairments in making personality judgments age at the time the lesion developed was strongly correlated
(Heberlein, Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio, 2004). with overall mentalizing performance. This suggests that
Bird, Castelli, Malik, and Husain (2004) reported a the amygdala may play a critical role in bridging between
case study of a patient with focal dorsomedial PFC and early and mature forms of mentalizing. More sophisticated
medial PFC damage. Despite the strong links in the fMRI forms of mentalizing may not specifically depend on the
literature between the dorsomedial PFC and mentalizing, amygdala; however, they may develop in the first place
this patient demonstrated no mentalizing impairments. It is only if simpler amygdala-based mentalizing skills are in
worth noting that developmental mentalizing studies have place to be built upon (see Machado, Snyder, Cherry,
consistently reported decreasing dorsomedial PFC activ- Lavenex, & Amaral, 2008).
ity with age, suggesting that it may play a greater causal Attempts are being made to determine the functional
role in adolescence than in adulthood (Blakemore et al., contributions of particular brain regions to mentalizing,
2007; Wang et al., 2006a; see also Pfeifer, Lieberman, & but most of the results are quite tentative at this point
Dapretto, 2007). This would be consistent with the dor- (Decety & Lamm, 2007; Gallagher & Frith, 2003; Saxe &
somedial PFC playing a controlled processing role in Wexler, 2005). The posterior STS responds to biological
mentalizing that may be less needed as elements of men- motion cues (e.g., gaze shifts, lip movements) that are likely
talizing are increasingly automated. to provide raw perceptual material for drawing inferences
In another case study, a patient with focal right ventrolateral about the mental states of others. Temporal poles are com-
PFC damage experienced mentalizing deficits under specific monly activated when seeing the faces or names of famil-
circumstances (Samson, Apperly, Kathirgamanathan, & iar people (Sugiura et al., 2006), and some have suggested
Humphreys, 2005). The patient was capable of reasoning that this region represents semantic information in the social
about a target’s false belief if the story was crafted to indi- domain (Lambon Ralph, Pobric, & Jefferies, 2009). Saxe and
cate that the target had a false belief without revealing colleagues have suggested that the TPJ is specifically respon-
what the true state of affairs was. In contrast, if the patient sible for belief-related cognition (Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003;
knew the true state of affairs, he consistently projected this Saxe & Wexler, 2005); however, others have suggested that
knowledge onto the target. Samson and colleagues inter- the TPJ is responsible for directing attention to salient cues
preted these findings as indicating an impaired ability to in the environment (Decety & Lamm, 2007; Mitchell, 2008)
inhibit one’s own perspective and knowledge, rather than a rather than having a specific role in mentalizing.
deficit in belief reasoning per se (see also Lamm, Nusbaum, Although the dorsomedial PFC is by far the most com-
Meltzoff, & Decety, 2007). This interpretation is supported by monly activated region during mentalizing, there is not yet
developmental findings that mentalizing abilities in children an agreed-upon account of its function (Amodio & Frith,
are correlated with inhibitory skill as well (Carlson & Moses, 2006; Saxe & Powell, 2006). One relatively unexplored
2001). Three studies examining left TPJ lesions (Apperly, idea suggests an analogy to working memory processes
Samson, Chiavaino, & Humphreys, 2004; Heberlein et al., where the dorsolateral PFC is thought to orchestrate work-
2004; Samson, Apperly, Chiavarino, & Humphreys, 2004) ing memory using various “slave” systems in the lateral
also demonstrated significant mentalizing impairments asso- parietal cortex (Baddeley, 2002) and elsewhere (Postle,
ciated with this region (cf. Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2006). 2006). In the context of mentalizing, the dorsomedial PFC
Finally, there has been an ongoing debate about the would orchestrate cognition about mental states with the
role of the amygdala in mentalizing. The amygdala fig- help of more simplistic slave systems in the TPJ, posterior
ured prominently in early theories of mentalizing (Baron- STS, and temporal poles. Such a model would be relatively
Cohen et al., 2000), but it was reported in only 2 of the 45 straightforward to test with modified working memory
neuroimaging studies of mentalizing reviewed. Although paradigms.

CH05.indd 155 12/22/09 5:33:38 PM


156 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Mentalizing About Similar Others Together, the ventral premotor cortex and anterior IPL
Even if the exact role of the dorsomedial PFC is not yet form, in monkeys, what has been called the mirror neuron
specified, a recent series of studies have helped clarify system (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004).
how the dorsomedial and medial PFCs differentially con- Although no human research has identified single
tribute to mentalizing. Mitchell and colleagues (Mitchell neurons in these regions that respond both when observ-
et al., 2005b; Mitchell, Macrae, & Banaji, 2006) have ing and when performing an action, there is compelling
demonstrated that the subjective similarity between a fMRI data to suggest that a homologous mirror system
target and oneself determines which PFC region is most exists in humans. Iacoboni and colleagues (1999) pro-
strongly associated with mentalizing. In most mental- vided the first evidence by having subjects observe and
izing studies, there is little basis for even evaluating the imitate finger tapping while in a scanner. They found
similarity of targets to oneself, and these studies reliably three brain regions that were active during both observa-
recruit the dorsomedial PFC. In Mitchell’s studies, to tion and imitation: left posterior ventrolateral PFC,2 right
the extent that targets are rated as dissimilar to the self, the anterior IPL, and right anterior intraparietal sulcus. The
dorsomedial PFC is again the region most activated by bilateral posterior ventrolateral PFC and bilateral anterior
mentalizing. However, to the extent that targets are rated IPL have been identified as the regions central to the
as similar to oneself, a more ventral region in the medial mirror system (Chaminade & Decety, 2002; Hamilton,
PFC is increasingly activated. Mitchell has suggested that Wolpert, Frith, & Grafton, 2006; Heiser, Iacoboni, Maeda,
for similar targets, subjects are projecting themselves onto Marcus, & Mazziotta, 2003; Urgesi, Moro, Candid, &
the other person to answer questions about the target. Self- Aglioti, 2006). One critical difference between the human
referential processing is strongly associated with medial and monkey mirror systems is that in monkeys, only hand
PFC activity (Lieberman, 2007), and thus this account actions that are observed in the presence of the object to
makes intuitive sense. be manipulated produce activity in the mirror neurons
Other qualitative distinctions might contribute to a (Gallese et al., 1996). In contrast, for humans a variety of
split between the contributions of the dorsomedial PFC hand actions that do not involve an object or involve an
and medial PFC in mentalizing. Mentalizing is typically occluded object still produce mirror system activity (Liu
equated with theory of mind processes in a broad fashion. et al., 2008; Montgomery, Isenberg, & Haxby, 2007). Also,
Yet people have a theory of “minds in general” as well as the human mirror system is active when observing goal-
theories of “specific minds.” It could be the case that the directed actions performed by robots whose action paths
dorsomedial PFC supports the general theory of mind, differ from human actions (Engel, Burke, Fiehler, Bien, &
including rules for understanding how the average person Rösler, in press; Engel, Burke, Fiehler, Bien, & Rösler, 2008;
is likely to experience and respond to different situations Gazzola, Rizzolatti, & Keysers, 2008; cf. Tai, Scherfler,
and events. In contrast, the medial PFC might support idio- Brooks, Sawamoto, & Castiello, 2004).
syncratic theories of specific minds, including our own Beyond the original studies of hand–object actions,
mind and those close to us. To this end, van Overwalle there have been a number of extensions regarding the
(2009) published a meta-analysis suggesting that mentaliz- classes of actions that activate the mirror system in humans.
ing about close others does reliably recruit the medial PFC Communicative hand gestures and mimed actions both acti-
(cf. Heatherton et al., 2006). From this perspective, the vate this system (Liu et al., 2008; Montgomery et al., 2007).
similarity findings from Mitchell and colleagues (2005b) Hearing actions activate the ventral premotor cortex (Kohler
may be a special case of applying a specific theory of et al., 2002). In addition, being touched or watching another
mind (i.e., one’s specific theory of one’s own mind) to a person being touched produces mirror-like effects in the IPL
similar other. (Keysers et al., 2004). A series of studies have also deter-
mined that the mirror system is activated during observation
Imitation and the Mirror Neuron System and imitation of facial expressions (Carr, Iacoboni, Dubeau,
In the early 1990s, Rizzolatti and colleagues (di Pellegrino, Mazziotta, & Lenzi, 2003; Hennenlotter et al., 2005;
Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese, & Rizzolatti, 1992; Gallese, Fadiga,
Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996) discovered a set of neurons
in the ventral premotor cortex in monkeys that was active 2
For the remainder of this chapter, “posterior ventrolateral PFC”
both when the monkey performed a goal-directed action is used to refer to the pars opercularis region of the inferior fron-
(e.g., grabbing a raisin) and when the monkey watched tal gyrus and the neighboring ventral premotor region commonly
someone else perform the same goal-directed action. Later found in imitation studies. “Ventrolateral PFC” is used to refer to
researchers observed similar effects in the anterior section mid-ventrolateral and anterior ventrolateral areas, including the
of the IPL (Gallese, Fogassi, Fadiga, & Rizzolatti, 2002). pars triangularis, pars orbitalis, and lateral BA 10.

CH05.indd 156 12/22/09 5:33:38 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 157

Lee et al., 2006; Leslie, Johnson-Frey, & Grafton, 2004; of these finding greater mirror system activity for unknown
Pfeifer, Iacoboni, Mazziotta, & Dapretto, 2008). than for known chords (Vogt et al., 2007). One resolution
Hennenlotter et al. (2009) found that amygdala activity in to these conflicting findings centers on the observer ’s goal.
response to angry faces was reduced in subjects after Botox In the studies in which only known actions activated the
injections into their foreheads; they also observed that the mirror system, subjects did not have the goal of subse-
reduction in amygdala activity correlated with the reduction quently performing these unknown actions. In the studies
in “frown muscle” activity. These results suggest that spon- in which unknown actions produced robust mirror system
taneous imitation of the observed facial expressions contrib- activity, subjects were required to subsequently perform
utes to the strength of one’s own limbic responses. Finally, these actions. Thus, having the explicit goal of learning to
somewhat counter to the notion that the same representation perform an action can bring the mirror system online even,
for action is activated both when seeing and when per- or perhaps especially, while observing unknown actions.
forming an action, performing actions that complement A final study relevant to the role of the mirror system in
an observed action activate the mirror system more than known and unknown actions scanned dancers before and
actually imitating the action (Newmann-Norlund, van after 5 days of training on particular dance routines (Cross,
Schie, van Zuijilen, & Bekkering, 2007). Kraemer, Hamilton, Kelley, & Grafton, 2009). Subjects
The discovery of mirror neurons in primates and the were scanned while watching several dance routines—
homologous mirror system in humans has produced enor- some that would be learned and others that would not. For
mous excitement within the scientific community and both kinds of dances, the mirror system was at its most active
beyond. This system is proposed to be at the root of our lan- before the training period. Those that were then practiced
guage abilities, the ability to learn through imitation, a basis for 5 days retained nearly the same level of activation in the
for social ingratiation through unconscious mimicking, mirror system, whereas the untrained dances produced far
and a mechanism critical to automatic nonverbal encod- less mirror system activity at the posttest. These data suggest
ing and decoding, mental state inference, and empathy. that having a preexisting action representation contributes
Faith in the significance of the mirror system has led some less to mirror system activity than the motivational rel-
to “predict that mirror neurons will do for psychology evance of the actions to oneself. The fact that food-grasping
what DNA did for biology. . . . They will provide a unify- behavior produces more mirror system activity in hungry
ing framework and help explain a host of mental abilities subjects than in satiated subjects is consistent with this
that have hitherto remained mysterious” (Motluck, 2001). motivational account (Chen, Meltzoff, & Decety, 2007).
In contrast, Gopnik (2007) has argued that much like the
Is Mirroring Automatic?
left-brain/right-brain notions that took root in popular
culture in the 1970s and still retain a myth-like status, the It is generally assumed that the mirror neuron system oper-
mirror neuron mania is promising much more than it has ates automatically, converting third-person observations of
delivered. Publications of articles in the mainstream media actions into embodied first-person experiences, and there-
with titles such as “Cells That Read Minds” (Blakeslee, fore into an understanding of the mental states (intentions,
2006) oversimplify the findings and give an inaccurate char- thoughts, feelings, desires) of others. Taking the automa-
acterization of what these neurons are known to be doing. ticity claim first (with the second claim addressed in the
Beyond responding both when an action is observed and next section), the best evidence for this comes from a study
performed, what are the functional properties of the mirror in which some subjects were instructed to explicitly focus
system? One open question is whether this is a system that on an actor ’s actions and intentions and other subjects
supports vicarious learning of new behaviors or is tuned to were instructed simply to watch the video clips (Iacoboni
respond to actions that are already well established in one’s et al., 2005). Similar levels of mirror system activity were
behavioral repertoire. Supporting the latter interpretation, found in both sets of subjects, leading to the conclusion
professional pianists show greater mirror system activity that mirror system activity is automatic. The difficulty with
when listening to music than do nonmusicians (Bangert this interpretation is that the subjects not receiving action
et al., 2006) and professional dancers show greater mir- observation instructions (1) were free to explicitly focus on
ror system activity when watching a dance performance in the intentions and actions in the clips and (2) viewed clips
their own style of dance than a performance from another in which there was little else to attend to but the actions.
tradition (Calvo-Merino, Glaser, Grèzes, Passingham, & Stronger tests of automaticity have thus far come down on
Haggard, 2005). In contrast, two studies have found that the side of the mirror system being relatively intentional
observation of unknown guitar chords, for which no motor and controlled. For instance, Lee and colleagues asked
representation already exists, produced mirror system subjects to look at emotionally expressive faces and to
activity (Buccino et al., 2004; Vogt et al., 2007), with one imitate the emotional expressions in one set of trials and

CH05.indd 157 12/22/09 5:33:39 PM


158 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

to make gender discriminations in the other set of trials a critical element that would lead the “other team” to cry
(Lee et al., 2006). In both types of trials, subjects were foul. On one hand, imitation studies that successfully
attending to the faces, but only the imitate trials produced recruit the mirror system do not ask subjects to draw infer-
mirror system activity. If the mirror system responds auto- ences about the mental states of the observed target or
matically, there should have been activity in both condi- check whether they have. On the other hand, mentalizing
tions. Another study found that when simulated biological studies, which almost always have an abstract detached
motion was viewed along with a task to determine whether quality to them, do not lend themselves to mirror system
the motion was biologically plausible, there was more involvement. Reading vignettes or watching abstract shapes
mirror system activity than if the same motion was observed move around are not the kinds of real-life experiences that
with an instruction to focus on the colors of the moving simulation theory focuses on.
elements (Engel et al., 2008). A third study used a work- Two studies have attempted to address these multiple
ing memory paradigm to examine neural responses when concerns in a single study. In a 2 ⫻ 2 study design by
one, two, or three actions had to be held in memory for Wheatley, Milleville, and Martin (2007), subjects were
several seconds; mirror system activity in this study was shown object animations. The researchers varied whether
found to increase linearly with the number of actions to be the animations looked like animate or inanimate entities
remembered (Engel et al., in press). This suggests that the and whether subjects were watching or imagining the
mirror system may operate as a working memory system movements. The mirror system was activated, and to a
for action, which is consistent with a controlled processing similar degree, during all four trial types. In contrast, the
account. Given the paucity of studies examining whether brain regions that selectively responded to animacy were
the mirror system functions automatically, the answer is almost all mentalizing regions and none were mirror sys-
not yet clear, but the evidence thus far does suggest that the tem regions, except for the posterior STS, which is the one
mirror system may not function automatically. region that sometimes appears in both networks. Judging
animacy is not the same as mental state inference, but it is
Mentalizing Versus Mirroring certainly a step in that direction.
Most studies examining individuals’ ability to infer the In another study, Spunt, Satpute, and Lieberman (in press)
contents of another ’s mind (i.e., mental state inference) presented subjects with video clips of an actor performing
have typically come from the theory of mind tradition simple everyday goal-directed behaviors (e.g., brushing his
(Wimmer & Perner, 1983). The mirror system is thought to teeth) but manipulated the subject’s inferential goal along
represent a neural substrate for a second way of understand- an action identification hierarchy (Vallacher & Wegner,
ing the mental states of others characterized by simulation 1987). On different trials, subjects were asked to think
theory (Goldman, 1989). According to simulation theory, about what the target was doing (medium action identi-
“we understand others’ thoughts by pretending to be in their fication level; “brushing his teeth”), how the target was
‘mental shoes’ and by using our own mind/body as a model performing the behavior (low action identification level;
of the minds of others” (Gallese, Ferrari, & Umiltà, 2002, “moving his arm”), or why the target was performing the
p. 36). In terms of the mirror system, this suggests that we behavior (high action identification level; “maintaining
understand the mental states that lead a person to perform a oral hygiene”). Critically higher levels of action identifica-
certain action because seeing this action activates the motor tion require a greater focus on the internal mental states
representations we possess for performing the same action. of the actor, and lower levels shift attention away from men-
This then allows us to use our own activated mental states to tal states and focus more on the external mechanics of the
understand the other individual’s mind. behavior. Similar to the results of the study by Wheatley
This is an appealing account of understanding others in et al. (2007), performing each of the identification tasks
an embodied way. The open question is whether the mirror activated the mirror system to the same degree, suggest-
system contributes to understanding others, and if so, in ing that differential needs for mental state inference did
what ways? Despite the claim that mirror neurons provide not differentially engage the mirror system. In contrast,
a unifying “basis of social cognition” (Gallese, Keysers, & multiple regions in the mentalizing network produced
Rizzolatti, 2004), studies of the mirror system almost never parametric increases in activity that tracked increases in
assess the social understanding supposedly obtained as a action identification level. Given that these were every-
result of mirror system activity, and studies that examine day kinds of behaviors that could or could not be used
social cognition overtly (i.e., mentalizing studies) rarely to draw inferences about the mental states of the actor,
report activity in the mirror system. depending on the subject’s goals, it is difficult to raise the
The limitation of previous studies to address this issue abstraction argument that applies to previous mentalizing
is that mentalizing and mirroring studies each leave out tasks.

CH05.indd 158 12/22/09 5:33:39 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 159

At this point, it appears that the mirror system is primarily as an empathic response. Similarly, empathy is not merely
involved in understanding observed behaviors externally being in the same emotional state as another person. If one
as behaviors (i.e., behavior identification) but may not be were to see another in pain and become so distressed that
involved in consciously understanding or representing the one began to ruminate on one’s own distress and how such
mental states of others. In contrast, the mentalizing net- painful episodes could be avoided by oneself in the future,
work thus far appears to be more central to mental state this also would not be an empathic response (Batson,
inference. Interestingly, at rest the mentalizing and mirror 1991). Along similar lines, having a positive emotional
system networks are negatively correlated with one another response to the sight of one’s favorite food being served to
(Fox et al., 2005). someone who despises that dish does not seem empathetic
either. Here, one would be focused on one’s own response
Empathy
rather than the other person’s.
Empathy has quickly become a major area of study within Thus, there are open questions as to what common brain
social cognitive neuroscience. One of the first studies in activations during the observation and experience of pain-
this area involved subjects being scanned while alternately ful stimulation means with respect to empathy. Because
receiving painful stimulation and observing their romantic self-reported empathic feelings have not been correlated
partner receiving painful stimulation (Singer et al., 2004). with neural responses in the more than four dozen fMRI
Analogous to the mirror system’s common response to per- studies of empathy, it is difficult to know whether subjects’
forming and observing an action, Singer and colleagues distress is related to feeling bad for the observed target or
found that the pain distress regions of the brain, the dor- if subjects are experiencing a self-focused type of distress.
sal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the anterior insula, One study has found that dorsal ACC and anterior insula
were activated while receiving and observing another activity is modulated by whether the target receiving pain-
receive painful stimulation. Eight studies have now almost ful stimulation has previously been observed treating
all shown the dorsal ACC and anterior insula to be active in others unfairly or not (Singer et al., 2006). If pain observa-
studies of empathy for physical pain (Botvinick et al., 2005; tion were only leading to self-focused distress, the moral
Morrison, Peelen, & Downing, 2007; Ochsner et al., 2008; assessment of the pain recipient would probably be of little
Singer et al., 2004, 2006), distressing loud noises (Lamm, consequence. Thus, this study provides some evidence that
Batson, et al., 2007), and disgusting odors (Jabbi, Swart, & the mirrored pain response may reflect empathic responses
Keysers, 2007; Wicker, Keysers et al., 2003) in which sub- rather than self-focused responses.
jects were both observers and receivers of the distressing Another issue is whether the dorsal ACC and ante-
experience. rior insula activations reported during visual observation
Although this is an extremely robust set of findings, it is of pain and distress generalizes to other kinds of empa-
unclear how they relate to the broader concept(s) of empathy. thy inductions (e.g., linguistic) and empathy inductions
More than any other domain in social cognitive neuro- focused on different domains of experience (e.g., sharing
science, there seems to be little agreement about what in another ’s success). Humans are capable of empathizing
empathy is and what psychological processes it involves. with an endless variety of experiences, but thus far pain has
Lamm, Batson, et al. (2007) recently defined empathy in been the primary experience examined. It is plausible that
terms of three components: “(1) an affective response to dorsal ACC and anterior insula activity is a consequence
another person, which some believe entails sharing that of already feeling empathic toward a person who now hap-
person’s emotional state; (2) a cognitive capacity to take pens to be in pain and that other brain regions would be
the perspective of the other person; and (3) some moni- activated if one watched an empathized-with person win
toring mechanisms that keep track of the origins (self vs. the lottery.
other) of the experienced feelings” (p. 42). This definition Although a number of studies have begun to address
gives a seat at the table to each of several different existing these issues, few conclusions have emerged, because there
approaches to empathy. has been little consensus across different studies. For instance,
Another way to arrive at the same definition is to con- when subjects are exposed to stories or scenarios meant
sider three things that empathy is not. Empathy is not a mere to induce empathic responses (Decety & Chaminade,
cognitive understanding of the emotional state of another 2003; Farrow et al., 2001; Shamay-Tsoory, Tomer, Berger,
without having any emotional reaction of one’s own. For Goldsher, & Aharon-Peretz, 2005), the dorsal ACC and ante-
instance, one could see a picture of Hitler wincing in pain rior insula are not commonly activated. Instead, mentaliz-
and be able to accurately indicate his level of pain without ing regions such as the dorsomedial PFC and temporal pole
necessarily feeling a similar emotional response of one’s tend to be activated along with amygdala. As mentioned,
own (Singer et al., 2006). We would not want to label this no study has obtained self-reported empathy to presented

CH05.indd 159 12/22/09 5:33:40 PM


160 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

stimuli that can be used to correlate with neural responses; (Ochsner et al., 2008; Singer et al., 2004; Wicker, Keysers,
however, several studies have assessed trait empathy and et al., 2003). Other studies manipulated the subjects’ per-
correlated this with neural responses. Here, the brain region spective to focus on a target’s experience or their own
most commonly associated with trait empathy is the medial experience (Jackson, Brunet, Meltzoff, & Decety 2006;
PFC (BA 10; Ranklin et al., 2006; Shamay-Tsoory, Tomer, Preston et al., 2007; Schulte-Rüther et al., 2007). Although
Berger, & Aharon-Peretz, 2003; Shamay-Tsoory, Lester no brain region was observed as being active in a majority
et al., 2005; Singer et al., 2004). Other regions, including the of these studies, some regions were more involved in self-
dorsal ACC, anterior insula, ventrolateral PFC (both mirror or other-focused attention. Specifically, the dorsal ACC,
system and non–mirror system areas), dorsomedial PFC, anterior insula, and posterior ventrolateral PFC were more
and ventral striatum, have each been identified in at least active only during self-focused or personal experience
two studies using trait empathy measures (Chakrabarti, conditions. In contrast, the ventromedial PFC, precuneus,
Bullmore, & Baron-Cohen, 2006; Kaplan & Iacoboni, posterior STS, TPJ, IPL, and amygdala tended to be more
2006; Pfeifer et al., 2008; Ranklin et al., 2006; Schulte- active during other-focused or target observation condi-
Rüther, Markowitsch, Fink, & Piefke, 2007; Singer et al., tions. In a connectivity analysis, Zaki, Ochsner, Hanelin,
2004; Shamay-Tsoory, Lester et al., 2005). Wager, and Mackey (2007) observed stronger connectivity
Perhaps most clarifying in light of the tripartite empathy between the dorsal ACC and medial PFC, posterior STS,
definition given previously are the two studies by Lamm and precuneus, and IPL during the observation, relative to the
colleagues (Lamm, Batson, et al., 2007; Lamm, Nussbaum, experience, of pain. This suggests a role for the mentaliz-
et al., 2007) that assessed trait emotional contagion, the ing network in empathy.
tendency to mirror what others are feeling. These studies Thus, the tentative conclusion that may be drawn at this
both found that activity in the dorsal ACC, anterior insula, point is that the dorsal ACC and anterior insula are acti-
and mirror system was related to trait emotional contagion, vated both when a person is observing and experiencing
suggesting that these regions may be specifically involved painful stimulation, potentially supporting an internal mir-
in the bottom-up emotion matching that often occurs with roring of another ’s affective response. In contrast, self and
empathy, rather than the top-down components of empathy social cognition regions, including the medial PFC, dorso-
(i.e., perspective taking and keeping focus on the other medial PFC, ventromedial PFC, and precuneus, may sup-
rather than on the self). port processes supporting focusing on and making sense of
One of these studies in particular helps bolster this inter- another ’s experience as it would feel for them.
pretation. Lamm, Nussbaum et al. (2007) had subjects view
Attributions of Morality and Trustworthiness
two sets of pictures that depicted needles going through
the skin of a person’s hand; however, for one set, subjects Although most neuroimaging studies examining the pro-
were informed that the “hand had already been numbed for cesses whereby the psychological states and traits of others
a biopsy.” The bottom-up visual inputs from both sets of are inferred have focused on this process generically, there
images appear painful, but top-down cognitive appraisal has been some work focusing on domain-specific attribu-
should drive very different empathy responses to the two tions. Two commonly studied domain-specific attributions
stimuli. Pain regions including the dorsal ACC, anterior are for morality and trustworthiness.
insula, and somatosensory cortex were strongly activated The most significant finding in the domain of morality
by both sets of pictures. In contrast, regions involved in judgments is that personal, relative to impersonal, moral
mentalizing (the medial PFC, dorsomedial PFC, ventrome- decisions recruit more regions associated with mentaliz-
dial PFC, and precuneus) and self-control (the right ven- ing and self-referential processing, including the medial
trolateral PFC) were differentially activated to the different PFC, precuneus, and TPJ (Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom,
sets of pictures. These regions may play a role in contex- Darley, & Cohen, 2001). Moral reasoning in general also
tualizing empathic responses to take account of what the invokes elements of the mentalizing and self-reference
experience of the other is likely to be, based on knowledge network, including the medial PFC, ventromedial PFC, TPJ,
of their situation (e.g., numbed hand) or personality (e.g., and posterior STS (Moll, de Oliveira-Souza, Bramati, &
masochist?). Grafman, 2002; Moll, de Oliveira-Souza, Eslinger et al.,
A number of studies have now examined what differs 2002). Several studies have now shown modulation of these
in the brain as one considers another ’s distress rather than regions as factors related to moral attributions are manipu-
one’s own. These studies are an important complement to lated, including the actor ’s intentions (Berthoz, Armony,
those that reveal the commonalities. Some of the studies Blair, & Dolan, 2002; Borg, Hynes, Horn, Grafton, & Sinnott-
that reported commonalities also reported what was greater Armstrong, 2006) and beliefs about the consequences of
during experiencing or observing something distressing the action (Young, Cushman, Hauser, & Saxe, 2007), actual

CH05.indd 160 12/22/09 5:33:40 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 161

consequences (Borg et al., 2006; Young et al., 2007), and Agency


whether an audience is present to the actions (Finger, Agency refers to the sense that one was causally responsible for
Marsh, Kamel, Mitchell, & Blair, 2006). Additionally, a particular behavior and forms one of the phenomenological
damage to the ventromedial and medial PFCs has been cores of selfhood. The neural correlates of agency have been
associated with impaired ability to make personal, but examined in two different ways. First, a number of studies
not impersonal, moral judgments (Ciaramelli, Muccioli, have compared the neural bases of freely chosen self-initi-
Ladavas, & di Pellegrino, 2007; Mendez, Anderson, & ated actions to externally triggered actions. Although these
Shapira, 2005). studies do not assess the experience of agency, they operate
In contrast, judgments of trustworthiness have been under the assumption that freely chosen actions are asso-
almost exclusively linked to amygdala activity across studies. ciated with a sense of agency and free will. Perhaps the
Adolphs, Tranel, and Damasio (1998) observed that most famous of these studies is Libet’s, which has been
patients with bilateral amygdala damage, relative to con- referred to time and again to argue that free will is an
trols, were heavily biased to rate faces as more trustworthy. illusion (Libet, Wright, & Gleason, 1982).
Similarly, an early fMRI study found that the amygdala In Libet’s study, subjects freely chose when to make a
was more active when the subject was presented with response and were asked to watch a clock and remember
untrustworthy faces than with trustworthy faces (Winston, the precise time when they formed the intention to respond.
Strange, O’Doherty, & Dolan, 2002). Interestingly, Engell, Intriguingly, Libet observed a neural response, the readiness
Haxby, and Todorov (2007) observed that amygdala potential thought to emanate from the supplementary motor
responses more closely tracked consensus judgments of area, a few hundred milliseconds prior to when subjects
trustworthiness for different faces than the subjects’ own claimed to have formed an intention. Libet argued that the
ratings for those faces. neural responses that would ultimately trigger a behavior
were causing an intention to be formed rather than an inten-
tion setting the motor response in motion (see also Fried
Self-Processes
et al., 1991). The neuroimaging studies that have followed
The self has been a central topic within social psychology for have commonly observed supplementary motor area activ-
decades, because many theories regarding the development, ity, along with the dorsal ACC, lateral PFC, medial PFC,
maintenance, and regulation of the self suggest that these and precuneus (Babiloni et al., 2008; Brass, Derrfuss, &
are profoundly social processes and because self-processes von Cramon, 2005; Brass, Zysset, & von Cramon, 2001;
continuously influence our social cognition and behavior. It C. Frith, Friston, Liddle, & Frackowiak, 1991; Hunter
is little surprise then that the self has been one of the most et al., 2003; Lau, Rogers, Haggard, & Passingham, 2004;
actively researched topics within social cognitive neurosci- Lau, Rogers, Ramnani, & Passingham, 2004). One study
ence. In the following sections, I discuss in turn the functional observed that supplementary motor area activity that occurs
neuroanatomy of agency, self-recognition, self-reflection and just prior to intention formation predicts the timing of self-
self-knowledge, and self-control (see Figure 5.4). reported intention formation, whereas activity in the medial

Self-Processes

Figure 5.4 The brain regions involved in


self-processes (agency processing [1, 3, 5, 7],
1 medial PFC 5 supplementry motor area self-recognition [6, 7], self-reflection [1–3],
2 dorsomedial PFC 6 ventrolateral PFC and self-control [4, 5, 6]). Numbers in brackets
3 precuneus/posterior cingulate 7 inferior parietal lobule correspond to the regions in the figure reliably
4 rostral ACC associated with a particular self process.

CH05.indd 161 12/22/09 5:33:41 PM


162 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

PFC and precuneus up to 10 seconds prior to intention behavior than do control subjects, with the effect increasing
formation predicts the timing of self-reported intention for- with symptom strength (Franck, O’Leary, Flaum, Hichwa, &
mation (Soon, Brass, Heinze, & Haynes, 2008). Andreasen, 2002; Ganesan, Hunter, & Spence, 2005;
These studies are not without limitations. Waiting for an Spence, Brooks, Hirsch, Liddle, & Grasby, 1997). Lastly,
extended time until one has an intention to press a button lesion-induced out-of-body experiences have been localized
is an artificial task that may well involve processes dis- to the IPL–TPJ region (Blanke, Landis, Spinelli, & Seeck,
tinct from those involved in intention–action connections 2004), with intracranial stimulation and TMS to this region
in more naturalistic settings. These studies do not merely producing out-of-body–like experiences (Blanke, Ortigue,
examine intention formation but rather intention formation Landis, & Seeck, 2002; Blanke et al., 2005). Together, these
while in the mind-set of reflecting on and detecting one’s results suggest that this region may code for the mismatch
own intention formation. This would seem to have all of between intention and action, with quiescence in this region
the usual issues with introspection (Nisbett & Wilson, resulting during normal personal agency.
1977). One can imagine monitoring one’s own intention Overall, these two experimental approaches suggest
formation, particularly in such an artificial task, to be more that forming an intention to act and assigning agency to
of a signal detection task than a direct read-off of one’s an observed behavior may depend on different neural sys-
own psychological states. When attending to one’s own tems. Forming an intention appears to rely largely on struc-
thoughts, a variety of fleeting thoughts are likely to occur, tures on the medial walls of the cortex, whereas evaluating
and the individual must decide which rise to the level of whether the behavior that results is one’s own involves a
full-blown intentions and which do not. In the study by lateral region of parietal cortex.
Soon et al. (2008), it is not hard to imagine that a partially
Self-Recognition
formed and vaguely conscious intention to press a button
occurs at one point but does not meet one’s threshold for The canonical test for whether an animal or human baby has
declaring that an intention has occurred. Nevertheless, self-awareness is the mirror self-recognition test (Gallup,
this subthreshold intention may set in motion a series of 1970). In this test, colored ink or powder is applied to the
psychological events that trigger the full-blown intention subject’s forehead while the subject is asleep. Once awake,
several seconds later. If the subthreshold intention is not the subject is placed in front of a mirror. If upon notic-
reported, its neural correlates would appear to predict the ing the colored patch in the mirror, the subject proceeds
subsequent above-threshold intention, thus subverting the to touch its own forehead where the color is, the subject is
apparent order of events. Consequently, it is unclear at this then said to have passed the mirror self-recognition test.
point whether neural events causally precede all inten- A number of neuroimaging studies have now established
tions or just those intentions that we reflectively recognize the network of brain regions involved in recognizing one-
as intentions. self from pictures. Nine of ten neuroimaging studies using
The second approach to the study of agency involves cre- “pictures of the self” observed increased right ventrolateral
ating discrepancies between one’s behavior and the visual PFC activity (Devue et al., 2007; Hodzic, Muckli, Singer, &
presentations of one’s behavior. Typically, these studies Stirn, 2009; Kaplan, Aziz-Zadeh, Uddin, & Iacoboni, 2008;
manipulate visual feedback such that one’s arm movements Morita et al., 2008; Platek et al., 2004, 2006; Suguira
appear to move in a different trajectory than intended, or a et al., 2000, 2005, 2008). About half of these also reported
delay is used such that one’s hand movements are seen a few increased right IPL activity (cf. Morita et al., 2008). One
hundred milliseconds after they are produced. Across these of these studies (Kaplan et al., 2008) found that identify-
studies, the most common finding is that the IPL, in the area ing the self from pictures or voice recordings activated the
of the TPJ, increases in activity as the mismatch between same region of right ventrolateral PFC. Additionally, TMS
produced and observed behavior increases (Blakemore, applied to right IPL was found to reduce subjects’ sensi-
Oakley, & Frith, 2003; Farrer, et al., 2003; Farrer et al., 2008; tivity to self–other distinctions (Uddin, Molnar-Szakacs,
Leube et al., 2003; Shimada, Hiraki, & Oda, 2005). Studies Zaidel, & Iacoboni, 2006). One study of note (Suguira
have used TMS applied to this area to disrupt agency judg- et al., 2000) compared active and passive responses to
ments (Preston & Newport, 2008; Tsakiris, Costantini, & self-images. In the conjunction of these two tasks, right
Haggard, 2008). Bilateral activity in this region has also IPL activity was observed, whereas right ventrolateral
been observed when hearing delayed playback of one’s own PFC activity was observed only in the comparison of the
voice (Hashimoto & Sakai, 2003). two tasks such that it was more active when subjects were
Similar to these findings, schizophrenic patients and those explicitly identifying their own faces. Thus, right IPL activ-
with related experiences of external control of one’s actions ity may be involved in lower-level visual processing of the
tend to produce greater right IPL activity during normal self, whereas right ventrolateral PFC activity may be more

CH05.indd 162 12/22/09 5:33:42 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 163

involved in intentional self-recognition. Interestingly, in East lateral PFC (Beer, John, & Knight, 2006; see also Beer,
Asian subjects, right ventrolateral PFC activity is more Heerey, Keltner, Scabini, & Knight, 2003). Similarly,
active for one’s own face relative to a coworker ’s face, if activity in the medial and ventromedial PFCs was greater
they are primed with an independent self-construal (Sui & when subjects successfully predicted whether they would
Han, 2007), whereas an interdependent self-construal be able to retrieve particular words from memory (Schnyer,
produces similarly strong activations for both faces in this Nicholls, & Verfaellie, 2005). Although there have been
region. only a few neuroscience investigations of self-insight,
these studies are particularly important because they link
Self-Reflection and Self-Knowledge neural processes to adaptive outcomes of self-reflection.
The ability to reflect on one’s current and past experiences, It is one thing to identify the medial PFC’s involvement
preferences, traits, and abilities is one of the signature when people try to reflect on themselves, but it is quite
achievements of the human brain. Although some other spe- another to determine that activating the medial PFC dur-
cies have shown evidence of rudimentary self-awareness, ing these attempts is associated with something useful and
perhaps as evidenced by the mirror self-recognition test, no accurate about oneself.
other species has such an overdeveloped self-awareness as to The great majority of self-reflection studies have
need aisle after aisle of self-help books. A few dozen neuro- focused on trait self-knowledge. In these studies, subjects
imaging and lesion studies have now examined the processes are typically asked to indicate whether trait words or
by which we focus our attention internally on ourselves. phrases are descriptive of themselves, are descriptive
Free-form reflection on the self has been found to of another person, or have some textual or semantic feature
produce activity in the medial PFC and the contiguous (Craik et al., 1999; D’Argembeau, Xue, Lu, Van der Linden, &
regions of the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex Bechara, 2008; Fossati et al., 2003, 2004; Gutchess,
(jointly referred to in this section as precuneusPCC) rela- Kensinger, & Schacter, 2007; Heatherton et al., 2006;
tive to control tasks; in addition, there is more activity in Johnson et al., 2002; Kelley et al., 2002; Kircher et al., 2002;
the medial PFC (BA 10) relative to free-form reflection Lou et al., 2004; Macrae, Moran, Heatherton, Banfield, &
on another individual (D’Argembeau et al., 2005; Farb Kelley, 2004; Moran, Macrae, Heatherton, Wyland, & Kelley,
et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2006; Kjaer, Nowak, & Lou, 2006; Pfeifer et al., 2007; Saxe, Moran, et al., 2006;
2002). The involvement of the medial PFC is of particular Schmitz & Johnson, 2006; Schmitz, Kawahara-Baccus, &
interest given that this is the only region of the prefrontal Johnson, 2004; Seger, Stone, & Keenan, 2004; Turner,
cortex known definitively to be disproportionately larger Simons, Gilbert, Frith, & Burgess, 2008; Vanderwal, Hunyadi,
in humans than in other primate species (Semendeferi, Grupe, Connors, & Schultz, 2008; Zhang et al., 2006; Zhu,
Schleicher, Zilles, Armstrong, & Van Hoesen, 2001). Trait Zhang, Fan, & Han, 2007). All but one of these studies
self-consciousness has also been specifically associated has shown increased medial PFC activity during self-
with medial PFC activity (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & judgments relative to either other-judgments or con-
Satpute, 2005). Similarly, explicitly attending to one’s pref- trol judgments, with precuneusPCC and dorsomedial PFC
erences, relative to a non–self-reflective control task, has activations also present in several studies. Two studies have
reliably been associated with medial PFC and dorsomedial found that the medial PFC is more active while judging
PFC activity (Goldberg, Harel, & Malach, 2006; Gusnard, positive self-traits than negative self-traits (Fossati et al.,
Akbudak, Shulman, & Raichle, 2001; Johnson et al., 2005; 2003, 2004), and a third found that the medial PFC was
Lane, Fink, Chau, & Dolan, 1997; Ochsner, Knierim, not sensitive to this distinction and that the subgenual
et al., 2004). Interestingly, mindfulness meditation train- ACC was activated by positive self-traits relative to nega-
ing that attempts to shift self-processing from linguistic tive self-traits (Moran et al., 2006). Multiple studies have
self-evaluation to a more experiential basic awareness has also linked the medial PFC to subsequent memory for self-
been shown to diminish this medial PFC activity (Farb relevant traits (Fossati et al., 2004; Macrae et al., 2004),
et al., 2007). Another study (Johnson et al., 2006) found which is consistent with the association of the medial PFC
that reflecting on the self with a promotion or prevention with autobiographical memory relative to episodic mem-
focus (Higgins, 1998) was associated with either increased ory more generally (Gilboa, 2004).
medial PFC or precuneusPCC activity, respectively. Given that self-knowledge and self-concepts change
Additionally, the medial and ventromedial PFCs have over time, it is important to determine the neural processes
both been associated with self-insight processes. For involved in the developmental and experience-driven
instance, patients with damage to these regions were less changes in these processes. One developmental fMRI
aware of whether their behavior constituted social trans- study (Pfeifer et al., 2007) found that the medial PFC was
gressions compared with patients with damage to the significantly more active in 9-year-old children than in

CH05.indd 163 12/22/09 5:33:42 PM


164 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

adults when making trait self-judgments. In contrast, the common; however, these activations are not as reliably
levels of medial PFC activity in young adults and older invoked by self-reflection processes as is medial PFC
adults were similar when making trait self-judgments activity. This is almost the mirror image of the pattern from
(Gutchess et al., 2007). A study on self-schemas compared mentalizing studies in which dorsomedial PFC activations
trait self-judgments in domains for which subjects were or were present in 91% of studies and medial PFC and precu-
were not self-schematic (i.e., had substantial experience) neusPCC activations were present in 33% and 39% of stud-
(Lieberman, Jarcho, & Satpute, 2004). Judgments made in ies, respectively.
the self-schematic domain produced greater activity in the Finally, classic theories of self-knowledge have pro-
ventromedial PFC, ventral striatum, amygdala, lateral tem- posed that self-concepts develop when individuals take
poral cortex, and precuneusPCC than judgments from the the perspective of others on themselves (Cooley, 1902;
nonschematic domain. In contrast, nonschematic judg- Mead, 1934). Reflected appraisals constitute one person’s
ments produced greater activity in the dorsomedial PFC assessment of what another person thinks of him or her.
and medial temporal lobe. These results suggest that sche- Three studies of adults have now examined the neural cor-
matics may recruit more automatic affective processes than relates of reflected appraisals of the self (“what I think you
nonschematics in making these judgments. think of me”) compared with direct appraisals of the self
Change over time has also been examined by asking (“what I think of me”), and each have found similar
subjects to take different temporal perspectives on the self. levels of medial PFC and dorsomedial PFC activity in
Studies comparing the present perspective of the self to the two forms of appraisals (D’Argembeau et al., 2007;
future (Ersner-Hershfield, Wimmer, & Knutson, in press) Ochsner et al., 2005; Pfeifer et al., 2009). One of these
and past (D’Argembeau et al., 2008) perspectives of the self studies (Pfeifer et al., 2009) focused primarily on ado-
have both observed greater medial PFC activity when indi- lescents, because this is a critical period of self-concept
viduals focus on the self as it is currently constituted rather development. The TPJ, a region that commonly appears
than on the self at other time points. These data are con- in mentalizing tasks, was strongly activated during
sistent with the notions that there is a greater identification reflected appraisals in adolescents and adults. Given
with the current self and that future and past selves may be that reflected appraisals involve mentalizing about the
treated in some ways as if they are altogether different indi- belief another person holds toward oneself, this is not a
viduals from oneself (Libby, Eibach, & Gilovich, 2005). surprising result. Perhaps more surprising was the strong
Other open questions include whether the medial PFC is activation of the TPJ during direct appraisals in adoles-
similarly active for self- and other-judgments and whether cents, but not in adults. This suggests the possibility that
the medial and dorsomedial PFCs are each involved in adolescents, but not adults, are spontaneously drawing
both self and social cognition. As to the first question, upon social sources of information when asked to gener-
some studies have reported greater medial PFC activity for ate direct appraisals. Consistent with this notion, a num-
self-judgments relative to other-judgments (Kelley et al., ber of regions involved in mentalizing about others were
2002; Lou et al., 2004), although some have not (Schmitz more active during direct appraisals in adolescents than
et al., 2004; Seger et al., 2004). One criticism of those that in adults, including the dorsomedial PFC, posterior STS,
have shown a difference is that in these studies the self is a and precuneusPCC.
far better known target than nonself targets (e.g., the queen
Self-Control
of Denmark or the president of the United States). One
study (Heatherton et al., 2006) specifically compared self- Self-control, or the ability to regulate, manipulate, or con-
judgments to judgments of a close friend and still found trol one’s prepotent thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, has
significantly greater medial PFC activity for self-versus been extensively examined using various tools of neuro-
other-judgments; however, others have found similar science. Explicit attempts at self-control across various
medial PFC activity for self-judgments and judgments of a domains commonly recruit a network of brain regions,
significant other or mother (Ochsner et al., 2005; Schmitz including the lateral PFC and the contiguous regions of
et al., 2004; Vanderwal et al., 2008). the dorsal ACC, presupplementary motor area (BA 6),
With respect to the relative involvement of the medial and posterior dorsomedial PFC (BA 8). It should be noted
PFC, dorsomedial PFC, and precuneusPCC, across all of the that the dorsal ACC is typically thought to serve a con-
self-reflection and self-knowledge studies, medial PFC flict detection function indicating the need for self-control,
activations were present in 94% of the studies, whereas whereas the lateral PFC is thought to be more involved in
dorsomedial PFC and precuneusPCC activations were pres- implementing control or inhibiting prepotent responses
ent in 53% and 63% of studies, respectively. Thus, acti- (MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, & Carter, 2000). Lesion data
vations of the dorsomedial PFC and precuneusPCC are support the latter claim regarding the lateral PFC (Aron,

CH05.indd 164 12/22/09 5:33:43 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 165

Robbins, & Poldrack, 2004), but they are less supportive subjects are led to believe that their pain or anxiety will be
of the former claim regarding the dorsal ACC (Fellows & alleviated by a pill or cream that is in fact pharmacologi-
Farah, 2005). cally inert. Although there is no instruction to intention-
Most relevant to social psychology are the more than ally regulate one’s pain or anxiety, subjects often report
30 neuroimaging studies of affect and emotion regula- less distress in placebo conditions. In the five neuroimag-
tion (Ochsner & Gross, 2005; for relevant cognitive ing studies (Kong et al., 2006; Lieberman, Jarcho, Berman
studies, see Goel & Dolan, 2003; Mitchell et al., 2007). et al., 2004; Petrovic et al., 2005; Wager et al., 2004, studies
These studies can be divided according to whether emo- 1 and 2) that have related neural responses to placebo-
tion regulation is the explicit goal of the task or whether related distress reductions, four have reported right ven-
emotion regulation occurs incidentally as a consequence trolateral PFC activity and two have reported activity in
of another process not intended to produce emotion regu- left ventrolateral PFC, right dorsolateral PFC, and rostral
lation effects. Explicit emotion regulation tasks include ACC. Five studies employed conflict resolution tasks in
reappraisal (Banks, Eddy, Angstadt, Nathan, & Luan Phan, which emotional cues must be ignored to successfully per-
2007; Beauregard, Levesque, & Bourgouin, 2001; Eippert form the task (Enger, Etkins, Gale, & Hirsch, 2008; Etkin,
et al., 2007; Goldin, McRae, Ramel, & Gross, 2007; Enger, Peraza, Kandel, & Hirsch, 2006; Felmingham
Harenski & Hamann, 2006; Herwig et al., 2007; Kim & et al., 2007; Most, Chun, Johnson, & Kiehl, 2006; Ochsner,
Hamann, 2007; Luan Phan et al., 2005; McRae, Ochsner, Hughes, Robertson, Cooper, & Gabrieli, in press). Here, the
Mauss, Gabrieli, & Gross, 2008; Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & regulation of emotional responses is secondary to the main
Gabrieli, 2002; Ochsner, Ray, et al., 2004; Schaefer task of making a fast categorical judgment about another
et al., 2003; Urry et al., 2006; Wager, Davidson, Hughes, stimulus; thus, regulation is secondary to the main task.
Lindquist, & Ochsner, 2008), suppression (Goldin et al., In all five of these studies, the rostral ACC was associ-
2007; Lee, Dolan, & Critchley, 2008; Ohira et al., 2006), ated with successful regulation of the emotional distracter.
detachment (Kalisch et al., 2005; Levesque et al., 2003), Lastly, four fMRI studies (Altshuler et al., 2005; Hariri,
and self-distraction (Kalisch, Wiech, Herrmann, & Bookheimer, & Mazziotta, 2000; Lieberman, Hariri, Jarcho,
Dolan, 2006). Eisenberger, & Bookheimer, 2005; Lieberman et al., 2007)
Across 19 neuroimaging studies, task conditions that have examined the neural basis of why putting feelings into
invoked explicit emotion regulation efforts were com- words can dampen emotional responses (Pennebaker &
monly associated with activations in right ventrolateral Beall, 1986). In these studies, subjects chose affective
PFC (63% of studies), left ventrolateral PFC (63% of stud- labels to characterize the negative emotional images. In
ies), the contiguous regions of the presupplementary motor each of these studies, right ventrolateral PFC was the pri-
area and posterior dorsomedial PFC (47%), and left dorso- mary brain region active during “affect labeling,” relative
lateral PFC (32% of studies). Approximately half of these to control conditions. In addition, in each of these studies,
studies also reported on frontal regions whose activity was right ventrolateral PFC activity was associated with dimin-
associated with regulatory success either in terms of self- ished amygdala responses to the negative stimuli. During
reported affect or limbic activity. Although there is not an these studies, emotion regulation was incidental; subjects
entirely consistent pattern among these analyses, right and were not trying to regulate their emotional responses.
left ventrolateral PFCs do appear more often than other Across all of the incidental emotion regulation studies, right
regions. ventrolateral PFC and rostral ACC activations were pres-
Most of these studies have examined the regulation ent in 57% and 50% of these studies, respectively. Across
of negative affect. Although a few studies have looked at both intentional and incidental emotion regulation studies,
regulation during the presentation of positively valenced right ventrolateral PFC activity was reported most often
images (Kim & Hamann, 2007; Ohira et al., 2006), it is (59%), followed by left ventrolateral PFC activity (41%).
unclear whether such images produce a similarly intense Although right ventrolateral PFC activity was equally
emotional response to the negative images typically likely to be present in intentional and incidental emotion
used. A study by Delgado, Gillis, and Phelps (2008) exam- regulation studies (63% vs. 57%), left ventrolateral PFC
ined reappraisal in the context of financial reward and activity was far more likely to be present in intentional than
observed increased left ventrolateral and left dorsolateral in incidental regulation studies (63% vs. 14%), as was the
PFC activity along with diminished ventral striatum activ- case for the contiguous regions of the presupplementary
ity during reappraisal. motor area and posterior dorsomedial PFC (47% vs. 0%).
More than a dozen studies have examined incidental In contrast, the rostral ACC was much more likely to
emotion regulation using affect-based conflict resolution, be invoked during incidental regulation studies (50%) than
placebo, and affect labeling paradigms. In placebo studies, in intentional regulation studies (5%).

CH05.indd 165 12/22/09 5:33:44 PM


166 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Social Interaction decision with the other player, it is considered irrational for
DM2 to return any money to DM1.
Trust, Cooperation, and Fairness To examine the neural correlates of trusting another
Building relationships of any kind and effectively work- person in a one-shot trust game (McCabe, Houser, Ryan,
ing with others depends on mutual trust, a willingness to Smith, & Trouard, 2001), in contrast to mere investing
cooperate, and a sense that rewards and responsibilities phenomena, subjects played some rounds with a human
are being distributed fairly. Using paradigms created by DM2 and some with a computer DM2. The researchers
behavioral economists, social cognitive neuroscientists observed that the medial PFC was more active for DM1
and neuroeconomists have been examining these different when DM1 decided to transfer the funds over to DM2. It is
social adhesives (see Figure 5.5). possible that the medial PFC represents the DM1’s feeling
Several fMRI studies have used variants of the “trust of similarity to DM2 (Mitchell, Macrae, & Banaji, 2006)
game” (Berg, Dickhaut, & McCabe, 1995) to examine the and thus DM1’s willingness to cooperate. In another type
neural processes invoked when deciding whether to trust a of cooperative game, Decety and colleagues also found the
stranger. In the trust game, there are two players: decision medial and ventromedial PFCs to be more active when a per-
maker 1 (DM1) and decision maker 2 (DM2). DM1, also son was being cooperative (Decety, Jackson, Sommerville,
called the investor, is given a sum of money (e.g., $10). This Chaminade, & Meltzoff, 2004). In their trust game study,
money can be kept or invested. If invested, the money is Delgado, Frank, and Phelps (2005) observed greater ven-
moved to DM2, also called the trustee. Any money received tral striatum and left TPJ activity in DM1 when that person
by the trustee is increased by a known and predetermined chose to trust. King-Casas and colleagues (2005) examined
factor (e.g., multiplied by 4). DM2 then decides how much multiple games played between the same DM1 and DM2 and
money to transfer back to DM1. In the case of mutual trust found that when a DM1 responded to DM2’s untrustworthy
and repeated games with the same individual, it would be in behavior by investing even more on the next round of the
both players’ interest for DM1 to invest the entire sum and game, rather than less, activity in the caudate in the dorsal
for DM2 to return half of the proceeds. However, if DM1 striatum of DM1 increased. Finally, Krueger and colleagues
does not trust DM2 to return a fair share, DM1 is less likely (2007) observed greater dorsomedial PFC, ventral striatum,
to invest as much of the initial endowment. Additionally, in and septal activity in DM1 when that person chose to trust.
a one-shot game where each player will make only a single Thus, although there is substantial variability across studies,

Fairness, Trust, & Helping Unfairness & Social Rejection

Unfairness & Social Rejection

Figure 5.5 The brain regions involved in


1 medial PFC
2 ventromedial PFC
social interactions. The top left image dis-
plays brain regions activated in studies of
3 ventral striatum
4 dorsal ACC fairness, trust, and helping. The top right
5 anterior insula and bottom right images display brain
6 ventrolateral PFC regions activated in studies of unfairness
and social rejection.
Note: Anterior insula is displayed on the lateral
wall for presentation purposes, but is actually
between the medial and lateral walls of the
cortex.

CH05.indd 166 12/22/09 5:33:44 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 167

these findings do suggest that different regions on the Other studies have used the “ultimatum game” (Fehr &
medial prefrontal wall (the dorsomedial PFC, medial PFC, Schmidt, 1999) to examine punishment for unfair treatment.
and ventromedial PFC) and in the striatum (dorsal and ven- In this game, DM1 is given an endowment (e.g., $10) and
tral) are more active during the decision to trust. makes a proposal for how DM1 and DM2 should split the
Brain-based oxytocin levels are also associated with endowment (e.g., DM1 will keep $7 and DM2 will receive $3).
DM1’s trust behavior in the trust game. In the first study to If DM2 accepts the proposal, both players receive what DM1
examine this, a DM1 receiving an intranasal dose of oxy- has proposed. If DM2 rejects the proposal, both players get
tocin transferred more money to DM2 than those who had nothing. At one time, economists supposedly argued that
received a placebo (Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak, Fischbacher, & DM2 should accept any nonzero offer, being better than zero,
Fehr, 2005). In another study (Baumgartner, Heinrichs, and thus DM1 should always offer one penny and DM2 should
Vonlanthen, Fischbacher, & Fehr, 2008), a dozen one- accept. In actual play, DM1 usually offers 30% to 50% and
shot trust games were played after an oxytocin or placebo DM2 will reject many of the offers lower than 30%. Sanfey
induction; however, subjects received feedback about game and colleagues published the first neuroimaging study of
dynamics after the first six games had been played. At this the ultimatum game and found that subjects in the DM2
point, subjects who were in the role of DM1 were informed role showed greater anterior insula activity to unfair offers
that in 50% of the prior games, DM2 had not transferred ($1 or $2 out of $10) than to fair offers, but only if DM1 was a
money back to them. Knowing that future betrayals were person, not a computer (Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom, &
likely, placebo DM1 subjects reduced their later transfers Cohen, 2003). Additionally, the magnitude of anterior insula
to DM2. In contrast, DM1’s who had received oxytocin activity was associated with the tendency to reject the offer.
actually increased their transfers to DM2 after receiving Given that anterior insula activity has been associated with
the feedback. These oxytocin findings make sense in light feelings of disgust, Sanfey suggested that this activity may
of the known role of oxytocin in social attachment and pair represent the sense of insult or injustice associated with an
bonding in animals (Insel & Shapiro, 1992). unfair offer. A second fMRI study of the ultimatum game
Two studies have examined the neural correlates asso- (Tabibnia, Satpute, & Lieberman, 2008) equated the mate-
ciated with finding out that another person has failed rial payoff of fair and unfair offers, comparing, for instance,
to reciprocate one’s own trusting behavior. One study a fair offer of $5 out of $10 to an unfair offer of $5 out
(Rilling, Dagenais, Goldsmith, Glenn, & Pagnoni, 2008) of $23. As in the study by Sanfey and colleagues, anterior
used the “prisoner ’s dilemma” game in which DM1’s and insula activity was associated with the tendency to reject
DM2’s financial outcomes are each dependent on both unfair offers.
their own and the other player ’s decision. If both DM1 and The study by Tabibnia et al. (2008) also examined the
DM2 choose to cooperate, they receive equitable outcomes psychological struggle that can occur when an offer is
that maximize their joint reward total. However, for each simultaneously unfair and financially desirable (e.g., $5
decision maker, given a particular decision by the other out of $23). Subjects who more frequently accepted these
player, defecting will produce a greater personal reward unfair but desirable offers showed increased activity in
than cooperating. Rilling found that if DM1’s cooperation right ventrolateral PFC, a region that’s been associated
was unreciprocated by DM2, DM1 produced greater insula with emotion regulation and self-control more generally,
and reduced ventral striatum activity. Similarly, in a trust and also showed a correlated decrease in anterior insula
game, Delgado and colleagues (2005) found that DM2’s activity. In contrast, two studies (Knoch, Pascual-Leone,
choice not to transfer funds back to DM1 led to reduced Meyer, Treyer, & Fehr, 2006; van’t Wout, Khan, Sanfey, &
ventral striatum activity in DM1. This might have been Aleman, 2005) observed less frequent rejection of unfair
due to the diminished financial reward associated with this offers when TMS was applied to right dorsolateral PFC,
outcome; however, Delgado also showed that this effect presumably reducing the contribution of this region to
was absent when DM2 was believed by DM1 to be of high decision processes during this task.
moral character. This suggests that the diminished ventral Two other studies using the ultimatum game have
striatum activity was at least in part due to social factors. identified causal neural mechanisms contributing to an
Finally, multiple studies have examined the decision enhanced tendency to reject unfair offers. In one of these
to punish those who exhibit unfair behavior. A trust game studies (Koenigs & Tranel, 2007), patients with damage to
study using PET (de Quervain et al., 2004) found that if the ventromedial PFC and right ventrolateral PFC were
DM1 was given the opportunity to punish DM2 when DM2 more likely to reject unfair offers. In the second study
did not transfer money back, DM1 showed increased activ- (Crockett, Clark, Tabibnia, Lieberman, & Robbins, 2008),
ity in the dorsal striatum, and the magnitude of this activity pharmacological reduction of serotonin levels also led to
was correlated with the size of the punishment delivered. more frequent rejection of unfair offers. Reduced serotonin

CH05.indd 167 12/22/09 5:33:45 PM


168 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

levels have been shown to diminish ventrolateral PFC or reject each of a series of propositions that would yield
activity during a motor inhibition task (Evers et al., 2005), positive, neutral, or negative financial outcomes for one-
and thus it is plausible that regulation of one’s sense of self and/or for different charities (money really went to
insult is less effective due to serotonergic depletion effects these charities in this study). Trials in which subjects could
on the ventrolateral PFC. gain money for themselves with no negative consequence
for the charity unsurprisingly led to increased ventral stria-
Social Rewards and Helping tum activity. What was surprising is that trials in which
An interesting finding that has emerged from fMRI studies the charity would gain while the subject would lose money
of two-person economic games described in the previous (i.e., a donation) led to a higher level of ventral striatum
section is that people show evidence of reward activation activity than receiving money oneself. Additionally, the
when they participate in good interactions involving trust- magnitude of ventral striatum activity during donation
ing and fair behavior, even when this treatment confers decisions was associated with the tendency to accept dona-
no additional financial benefit to them or even leads to a tion propositions during the task. All of these studies taken
loss. In a prisoner ’s dilemma study (Rilling et al., 2002), together suggest that enacting or being the recipient of
subjects showed greater ventral striatum activity dur- prosocial behavior activates the ventral striatum, a region
ing mutual cooperation than during any other combina- that has been commonly associated with reward responses
tion of responses. This is striking in light of the fact that to primary reinforcers and to nonsocial secondary rein-
mutual cooperation is not the most financially rewarding forcers such as money, drug cues for addicts, and erotic
outcome possible. This suggests that against their own images (Lieberman & Eisenberber, 2009).
financial interest, there is a hedonic benefit to participat-
Social Rejection
ing in a reciprocated trusting behavior. Similarly, Tabibnia
et al. (2008) observed that fair offers produced greater The study of social rejection and ostracism has been a
activity in the ventral striatum and ventromedial PFC than major area of social psychological research in the past
unfair offers that would yield the same material benefit. decade (Williams, 2007). Being excluded or rejected repre-
A number of behavioral studies have yielded results con- sents some of the most distressing experiences that people
sistent with those of the fMRI studies, suggesting that being have, and fear of rejection is a powerful motivator that may
treated fairly is rewarding above and beyond the material help explain a wide array of classic findings of conformity
benefits that fair treatment often brings (De Cremer & and obedience to authority (Williams, Bernieri, Faulkner,
Alberts, 2004; Tyler, 1991). It has been suggested that this Grahe, & Gada-Jain, 2000). Based initially on animal stud-
makes sense evolutionarily because fair treatment can be ies (Panksepp, Herman, Conner, Bishop, & Scott, 1978), it
considered a proxy for whether one is valued by others has been suggested that there may be an overlap in the way
in a group. From an evolutionary perspective, continued that the brain represents experiences of physical pain and
inclusion in social groups has been critical to receiving a social pain (i.e., the pain of social rejection, exclusion, or
share of needed resources and even to survival; thus, any isolation) (Eisenberger & Lieberman, 2004; MacDonald &
cue that one has met this inclusion criterion is likely to be Leary, 2005).
rewarding. In humans, the neural components of the physical “pain
Indeed, simple signs of social acceptance have been matrix” are fairly well understood, including the dorsal
associated with ventral striatum activity in a number of ACC, anterior insula, somatosensory cortex, and periaque-
recent studies. Izuma, Saito, and Sadato (2008) found ductal gray (Price, 2000). Of these regions, the dorsal ACC
that a person’s ventral striatum was similarly activated has been most reliably associated with the distress of phys-
by financial rewards and by being informed that others ical pain (Rainville, Duncan, Price, Carrier, & Bushnell,
view that person in a positive light. In a developmental 1998), in contrast to the somatosensory cortex, which
social neuroscience study (Scott, Dapretto, Ghahremani, has been primarily associated with the sensory aspects of
Poldrack, & Bookheimer, under review), children’s good physical pain (e.g., identifying where on the body the pain
performance on each trial of a task was rewarded by either is felt). For instance, after surgical lesioning of the dorsal
financial reward or a smiling female face with the words ACC for chronic pain, patients typically report that they
“that’s correct” next to it. Similar increases in ventral stria- can identify the location of a painful stimulus on their body
tum activity were observed whether the reward was finan- and how intense the stimulus is, but they also report that
cial or social. the pain no longer bothers them (Foltz & White, 1968).
Another set of studies has shown that helping behav- Finally, as described earlier, right ventrolateral PFC and
ior in the form of charitable giving also generates reward rostral ACC have both been associated with the regulation
activity. Moll and colleagues (2006) asked people to accept of physical pain distress.

CH05.indd 168 12/22/09 5:33:46 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 169

Eisenberger and colleagues have conducted a series of involved in cognitive and affective processes, respectively, is
neuroimaging studies that suggest that social pain processes largely a consequence of an influential review paper (Bush,
largely rely on this same physical pain network (Eisenberger, Luu, & Posner, 2000). In this study the researchers reviewed
Gable, & Lieberman, 2007; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & dozens of cognitive conflict studies and found that these
Williams, 2003; Masten, Telzer, & Eisenberger, under tended to activate the dorsal ACC, whereas a study of emo-
review; Way, Taylor, & Eisenberger, 2009). In these studies, tional conflict detection in an emotional Stroop paradigm and
subjects believe they are playing a simulated ball-tossing other clinical symptom provocation studies produced rostral
game on the Internet while they and two other subjects are ACC activity. First, it is important to note that this literature
all in MRI scanners. Once in the scanner, the subjects actu- review included no studies of physical pain. Even though the
ally play against computer players programmed to include dorsal ACC has been repeatedly associated with the emo-
the subject for a certain amount of time and then stop throw- tional distress of physical pain, this finding was not accounted
ing the ball to the subject for the remainder of the scan. for in their analysis. Second, subsequent emotional conflict
Self-reported social distress during this exclusion episode is monitoring has found activity in the dorsal ACC (Davis
associated with greater dorsal ACC activity, whereas lower et al., 2005; Ochsner, Hughes, Robertson, Cooper, & Gabrieli,
distress reports are associated with increased right ventro- 2009). Third, numerous neuroimaging studies have shown
lateral PFC activity (Eisenberger et al., 2003; Eisenberger, dorsal ACC activity associated with anxiety and other affective
Way, Taylor, Welch, & Lieberman, 2007). Other studies have processes (Ehrsson, Weich, Weiskopf, Dolan, & Passingham,
also observed increased dorsal ACC activity in response to 2007; McRae, Reiman, Fort, Chen, & Lane, 2008; Simmons
rejection-themed images (Kross, Egner, Ochsner, Hirsch, & et al., 2008; Straube, Mentzel, & Miltner, 2007). Fourth, neu-
Downey, 2007) and video clips of disapproving facial ropsychological lesion data are more supportive of the dorsal
movements (Burklund, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007). ACC’s role in pain distress than cognitive conflict monitor-
Additionally, dorsal ACC activity during exclusion in the ing, because dorsal ACC lesions are commonly found to
scanner correlates with daily experiences of social dis- diminish pain distress (Foltz & White, 1968), whereas cogni-
connection outside the scanner (Eisenberger, Gable et al., tive conflict monitoring is often spared (Baird et al., 2006;
2007). Thus, the distress of social pain in the dorsal ACC Fellows & Farah, 2005; Stuss, Floden, Alexander, Levine, &
and the regulation of social pain in right ventrolateral PFC Katz, 2001).
closely parallel the findings from the physical pain litera- One way to reconcile these notions of dorsal ACC func-
ture. The animal literature supports these findings as well, tion is to think of it functioning like an alarm (Eisenberger &
having shown that electrical stimulation of the dorsal ACC Lieberman, 2004). Consider the typical smoke alarm. To
increases and surgical lesions of ACC decrease distress work successfully, it must fuse two functions together. On
vocalizations associated with social isolation in nonhuman one hand, it must have a mechanism capable of detect-
mammals (MacLean & Newman, 1988; Smith, 1945). ing when a critical threshold for smoke particles has been
One criticism of these findings (Sommerville, met—a mechanism conceptually analogous to cognitive
Heatherton, & Kelley, 2006) focuses on the common view conflict monitoring. On the other hand, in order to notify
that the dorsal ACC is responsible for cognitive processes, people that there’s a fire, it must have a mechanism that
whereas the rostral ACC is responsible for corresponding can sound an audible alarm after the first mechanism has
affective processes. This viewpoint suggests that the social detected the smoke. This latter process resembles the func-
rejection findings may reflect a violation of cognitive tion that pain distress plays in our lives, experientially
expectations of inclusion and that the dorsal ACC is there- notifying us that some harm may come to us. From this
fore activated because of cognitive conflict monitoring. perspective, determining the function of the dorsal ACC
However, this perspective does not account for the activity may not be an either/or decision. Rather conflict monitor-
correlating with the self-reported distress of the experience ing and pain distress may reflect coordinated cognitive and
(Eisenberger et al., 2003). Furthermore, it does not account experiential components of a single alarm mechanism.
for the increased dorsal ACC activity in rejection-sensitive
Attachment and Close Relationships
individuals to cues of rejection (Burklund et al., 2007),
because these individuals expect rejection more and yet A number of imaging studies have begun to examine how
show more dorsal ACC activity in response to it. Finally, the brain responds to the people we love (spouse, partner,
the strong linkage between an opioid polymorphism and the child, parent). Across these studies, most have observed
dorsal ACC response to rejection is hard to square with a limbic activations (e.g., amygdala, striatum, dorsal ACC,
purely cognitive account (Way et al., 2009). insula), although some report widespread activity in the
It is worth considering where this critique comes from mentalizing network (Leibenluft, Gobbini, Harrison, &
historically. The belief that the dorsal and rostral ACCs are Haxby, 2004; Seifritz et al., 2003). Hearing a child crying

CH05.indd 169 12/22/09 5:33:46 PM


170 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

has been associated with dorsal ACC activity (Lorberbaum associated with self-reported yearning for the deceased,
et al., 2002; Seifritz et al., 2003), whereas seeing pictures suggesting that ventral striatum activity may reflect cur-
of one’s own child or infant tends to activate the amygdala, rent desires for connection with the deceased that typi-
dorsal ACC, anterior insula, and bilateral lateral PFCs cally abate over the course of several months of normal,
(Bartels & Zeki, 2004; Leibenluft et al., 2004; Minagawa- noncomplicated grief.
Kawai et al., 2008; Ranote et al., 2004). One study of
mothers viewing pictures of their infant has shown ven- Attitudes and Attitude Change
tral striatum activity (Strathearn, Li, Fonagy, & Montague,
2008), but this result has not yet been replicated. Only Attitudes are one of social psychology’s oldest constructs
one study has examined the interaction of viewing one’s (Thurstone, 1928). People’s attitudes are of great interest
own infant or another ’s, either in distress or not; this study because they are believed to predict an individual’s behav-
revealed strong dorsal ACC and dorsomedial PFC activ- ior in a variety of attitude-relevant situations. In contrast to
ity when mothers viewed their own infant in distress rela- our intuitions, self-reported attitudes are often poor indica-
tive to the other conditions (Noriuchi, Kikuchi, & Senoo, tors of subsequent behaviors. This has led researchers to
2008). A recent study using near-infrared spectroscopy examine the existence and predictive efficacy of implicit
(Minagawa-Kawai et al., 2008) has examined infants’ neu- attitudes (Fazio & Williams, 1986), to assess attitudes in
ral responses to their mother ’s face and observed greater the aggregate (Ajzen, 2001), and to identify the critical
medial PFC activity in response to their mother smiling role of behavioral intentions linking attitudes to behaviors
(relative to the mother not smiling and a stranger smiling or (Gollwitzer, 1999). To date, the neuroscience of attitudes
not smiling). has largely focused on the neural correlates of attitudinal
Viewing pictures of one’s romantic attachments has evaluation and the neural correlates of attitude change.
typically produced dorsal striatum activity (Aron et al.,
Attitudinal Evaluation
2005; Bartels & Zeki, 2000); however, one study that sub-
liminally primed the name of one’s loved one has reported Several studies have examined which brain regions are
increased ventral striatum activity (Ortigue, Bianchi- more active when expressing attitudinal evaluations (e.g.,
Demicheli, Hamilton, & Grafton, 2007). Similar to the how good is it?) compared with when control judgments
network associated with seeing cues associated with one’s are made (e.g., how symmetrical is it?). There is substan-
own child, adult attachment studies have observed rela- tial variability in the activations reported across studies of
tionships between anxious attachment style and activity in attitudinal evaluation. This may be a result of the diversity
the amygdala and dorsal ACC during relationship distress of attitude objects examined in different studies. The
or hostile feedback paradigms (Gillath, Bunge, Shaver, objects examined include geometric shapes (Jacobsen,
Wendelken, & Mikulincer, 2005; Lemche et al., 2006). In Slotkin, Westerveld, Mencl, & Pugh, 2006), paintings
addition, another study reported that avoidant attachment (Kawabata & Zeki, 2004), music (Brattico, Tervaniemi, &
was associated with diminished ventral striatum feed- Picton, 2003), social concepts (Cunningham et al., 2004),
back during supportive feedback from a stranger (Vrticka, unfamiliar faces (O’Doherty et al., 2003), famous names
Andersson, Grandjean, Sander, & Vuilleumier, 2008). (Cunningham, Johnson, Gatenby, Gore, & Banaji, 2003;
Finally, a few studies have examined grief responses by Zysset, Huber, Ferstl, & von Cramon, 2002), and cur-
prompting individuals to think about the recent loss of a rent political candidates (Kaplan, Freedman, & Iacoboni,
significant other (e.g., mother recently dying of cancer or 2007). The most frequently observed activations in these
a romantic relationship that recently ended). These studies studies occur in the bilateral ventrolateral PFC, along with
have typically observed greater activity in the dorsal ACC a host of mentalizing and self-referential brain regions,
and anterior insula, consistent with a social pain account including the medial PFC, dorsomedial PFC, posterior
of grief, and in the posterior cingulate (Gundel, O’Connor, cingulate, TPJ, and temporal pole.
Littrell, Fort, Lane, 2003; O’Connor et al. 2008). One When subjects report their evaluations, it is difficult to
study (O’Connor et al., 2008) examined the neural dif- know what psychological processes are occurring to gen-
ferences among individuals who were showing a normal erate this evaluation. For instance, evaluations are some-
level of recovery from grief compared with those with times constructed in the moment, and other times they are
complicated grief, which refers to a persistent grief that is retrieved from memory. Sometimes people feel comfort-
not following the normal recovery pattern. Complicated able expressing their attitudes, and other times they engage
grief was associated with increased ventral striatum activ- in effortful mental processes to shape the expression of an
ity, relative to noncomplicated grief, when responding attitude for public consumption. Evaluations also vary in
to cues related to the deceased. This activity was also valence and arousal, and therefore task materials that vary

CH05.indd 170 12/22/09 5:33:47 PM


Functional Neuroanatomy 171

on these dimensions across studies could produce different account suggests that choosing between evenly liked items
results as well. Some of these elements have been exam- is at odds with previously ranking them as similar and that
ined. With respect to valence, positive and negative atti- by “spreading the alternatives” in one’s updated rankings,
tudes have been associated with left and right lateral PFCs, the selected items comes to look as though it was an obvi-
respectively (Cunningham, Espinet, DeYoung, & Zelazo, ous choice all along. Of course, to outsiders, this looks like
2005). In contrast, attitudinal intensity or arousal has post hoc rationalization.
been associated with the amygdala and ventromedial PFC Several early accounts of cognitive dissonance pro-
(Cunningham et al., 2004). Self-reported efforts to control cesses suggested that dissonance reduction processes were
one’s evaluation have been associated with activity in the relatively explicit and slow, occurring over a long period
ventrolateral PFC, dorsolateral PFC, dorsal ACC, medial of time after the conflictual behavior occurred (Festinger,
PFC, and precuneus. In contrast, being exposed to liked or 1964; Hovland & Rosenberg, 1960; Steele, Spencer, &
disliked attitude objects without expressing an evaluation Lynch, 1993). According to this model, an individual must
has been associated with activity in the ventral striatum be consciously aware that he or she has engaged in counter-
(Aharon et al., 2001) and amygdala (Cunningham et al., attitudinal behavior, attribute the resulting dissonance feel-
2003), respectively, suggesting that these regions may play ings to this specific conflict, and then engage in effortful
a role in implicit attitudes. processing to change this attitude over time. Lieberman et al.
More recently, an area of research referred to as neu- (2001) compared attitude changes in amnesics and healthy
romarketing has begun examining branding effects that controls because it is unlikely that amnesics would recog-
bear a close relationship to attitude processes. The most nize that they have engaged in a behavior that conflicts with
significant of these studies recreated the Pepsi challenge a previously expressed attitude. Despite this impairment,
inside the scanner (McClure et al., 2004). In the classic amnesics showed as much attitude change as control sub-
advertising campaign from the 1970s, it was found that jects, suggesting that the conventional account of cognitive
despite overwhelming self-reported preference for Coke, dissonance effects relies too heavily on controlled process-
when each drink was tasted without labels, Pepsi was ing mechanisms being deployed slowly over time. Multiple
more often preferred. The implication is that Coke is pre- electroencephalograph (EEG) studies also suggest that dis-
ferred because of the brand association rather than its taste. sonance effects may occur more quickly than previously
In this study, subjects tasted Coke and Pepsi on a series assumed (Harmon-Jones, Gerdjikov, & Harmon-Jones,
of trials, but could see the brand labels on only some of 2006; Harmon-Jones, Harmon-Jones, Fearn, Sigelman, &
the trials. They observed that in the absence of labels, Johnson, 2008).
ventromedial PFC activity was associated with drink pref-
erence, consistent with this region’s common association Stereotyping and Intergroup Processes
with hedonic experience (Trepel, Fox, & Poldrack, 2005).
Perceiving Race
In contrast, when the brand labels were available, prefer-
ences were associated with dorsolateral PFC and hippo- Neuroscience research on stereotyping and related inter-
campal activity, suggesting a role for higher cognitive and group processes represents a microcosm of the larger
memory processes. social cognitive neuroscience landscape, including studies
of social perception, implicit attitudes, self-like process-
Attitude Change ing of others, and self-control. This is also one of the areas
The first neuroscience investigation of attitude change of social cognitive neuroscience where ERP studies vastly
explored cognitive dissonance processes in patients with outnumber fMRI studies (for review, see Amodio, 2008;
anterograde amnesia (Lieberman, Ochsner, Gilbert, & Bartholow & Dickter, 2007; Kubota & Ito, 2009).
Schacter, 2001). Cognitive dissonance reduction usually A number of fMRI studies have examined the percep-
refers to the change in attitudes or beliefs that occur when tion of Black and White faces. Across these studies, inverse
one has freely chosen to engage in a behavior that conflicts affective and perceptual effects have emerged. On one
with a previously held attitude or belief. For instance, in the hand, greater amygdala activity in response to Black ver-
free choice paradigm, an individual ranks his or her prefer- sus White faces (Lieberman et al., 2005; Ronquillo et al.,
ences for several items in a category (e.g., kitchen appli- 2007) suggests a possible negative evaluative response
ances; Brehm, 1956) and then chooses which of two closely to or greater emotional evocativeness of Black faces. In
ranked items he or she would like to own; the subject then contrast, greater activity in the FFA to ingroup versus out-
finally re-ranks all of the items. The classic finding is that group faces (Golby, Gabrieli, Chiao, & Eberhardt, 2001;
the selected item goes up in the re-rankings, whereas the Lieberman et al., 2005) has been interpreted as reflect-
unselected item goes down in the re-rankings. The dissonance ing greater perceptual expertise with ingroup faces.

CH05.indd 171 12/22/09 5:33:47 PM


172 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

These results are paralleled by ERP studies (Ito & Urland, guard against having biased thoughts, feelings, or behaviors
2003), which reveal some early components that are more toward outgroup members. Multiple fMRI studies have
responsive to outgroup faces (N100, P200) and another observed a network almost identical to those seen in other
early component that is more responsive to ingroup faces forms of self-control (ventrolateral PFC, dorsolateral PFC,
(N200), which has been linked to FFA activity (Allison dorsal ACC, supplementary motor area) more active in con-
et al., 1994). ditions in which subjects are exposed to Black faces under
Although these early ERP components are not modu- conditions where bias could be revealed (Cunningham,
lated by race-related encoding goals (Ito & Urland, 2005), Johnson et al., 2004; Richeson et al., 2003). In addition,
both race-based categorization and individuation goals work with ERPs (Amodio et al., 2004; cf. Bartholow
have been associated with diminished amygdala responses et al., 2005) has shown evidence of a fast response in the
to Black faces in fMRI studies. Wheeler and Fiske (2005) dorsal ACC, called the error-related negativity response,
observed diminished amygdala activity when subjects during the Weapons Identification Task (Payne, 2001) on
judged a target’s food preference. In contrast, Lieberman trials that reveal bias. Critically, the dorsal ACC response
et al. (2005) observed diminished amygdala activity during on a particular trial predicted greater controlled processing
the labeling of a target’s race, similar to the effects of affect during the subsequent trial. This suggests that this activa-
labeling. Along the same lines, a study of stigma (Krendl, tion is an internal indicator of potential bias and the need to
Macrae, Kelley, Fugelsang, & Heatherton, 2006) reported be more careful on ensuing trials.
less amygdala activity when subjects’ judgments were Naturally, there are situations in which individuals do
explicitly focused on the stigma, compared with when they not mind acting on the basis of ingroup favoritism. People
were not. In addition, amygdala responses to race have want members of their ingroups to succeed and obtain their
been modulated by skin darkness (Ronquillo et al., 2007) fair share of resources, at a minimum. One study observed
and the direction of a target’s eye gaze (Richeson, Todd, & neural responses associated with this ingroup bias in the
Trawalter, 2008). absence of pressure to be unbiased (Rilling, Dagenais,
et al., 2008). Subjects were separated into groups using a
Implicit Attitudes minimal group paradigm manipulation; they then played
Other neuroimaging studies have examined the relation- prisoner dilemma games with ingroup and outgroup mem-
ship between attitudes and amygdala responses to Black bers. Approximately one third of the subjects reported
faces, relative to White faces. Most notably, an early fMRI feeling differently when playing against an ingroup mem-
study (Phelps et al., 2000) observed that amygdala activity ber than an outgroup member. This subsample, but not
to Black faces was correlated with the strength of negative the sample as a whole, produced greater activity in the
implicit attitudes toward Blacks but was not correlated dorsomedial PFC and right TPJ, both regions in the men-
with an explicit measure of racism. Similarly, another study talizing network, when playing with an ingroup rather than
(Cunningham, Johnson et al., 2004) reported greater amyg- with an outgroup member. In other words, playing with an
dala activity to Black faces versus White faces only when ingroup player may have produced more mentalizing about
the faces were presented subliminally, suggesting potential the perspective of the other player.
self-regulation under supraliminal conditions. In this study,
Being the Target of Prejudice
implicit attitudes were associated with amygdala activity
during subliminal presentations but not during supralimi- Although the vast majority of intergroup studies, both
nal presentations. Somewhat surprisingly, a patient with behavioral and neuroimaging, have examined the percei-
amygdala damage showed normal implicit racial attitudes ver ’s side of bias, a handful have examined the reactions
(Phelps, Cannistraci, & Cunningham, 2003), although the of the targets of prejudice. In the behavioral literature, ste-
lesion was acquired in adulthood and other social processes reotype threat (Steele & Aronson, 1995) is the most widely
have been spared for amygdala lesions acquired in adult- used paradigm for examining the effect of stereotypes on
hood (Shaw et al., 2004). In contrast, patients with ventro- the target of those stereotypes. In these studies, subjects for
medial and medial PFC damage do not produce implicit whom a stereotype exists (e.g., females are bad at math)
attitude effects (Milne & Grafman, 2001). perform a stereotype-relevant task (e.g., a math test) that
either is characterized as measuring their ability or is char-
Controlling Bias acterized in nonability terms (e.g., it is a game). The stan-
Given that stereotype-based expectations can lead to sys- dard finding is that stereotype targets perform worse on
tematically biased behavior (Payne, 2001) and given that these tasks when they believe the task is diagnostic of their
most individuals are motivated to be or appear nonbiased, ability, and these results are explained in terms of anxiety
self-regulation processes are often brought online in order to over confirming the stereotype. In other words, if a female

CH05.indd 172 12/22/09 5:33:48 PM


How Social Cognitive Neuroscience Contributes to Social Psychology 173

is anxious or distracted, thinking that poor performance on light because of brain data? Do neuroscience methods
a math test will confirm negative math stereotypes about allow us to ask social psychological questions that have
women, this may limit the woman’s ability to focus on gone unanswered for years? If the answer to any of these
task, thus creating a self-fulfilling prophecy. questions is yes, then social psychology needs the tools of
Two fMRI studies have examined the neural correlates of neuroscience just as surely as it needed the tools of cogni-
stereotype threat (Krendl, Richeson, Kelley, & Heatherton, tive psychology a few decades ago.
2008; Wraga, Helt, Jacobs, & Sullivan, 2006; see also Can social cognitive neuroscience answer all of social
Masten et al., under review). In both studies, increased ste- psychology’s questions? Of course not. No method can.
reotype threat was associated with increased rostral ACC Neuroimaging is no more a panacea than reaction time mea-
activity. Given that this region has been associated both sures or introspective self-reports. Indeed, for most of the
with emotional experience and with the regulation of emo- interesting findings from the history of social psychology,
tions, it is difficult to interpret the significance of this com- neuroimaging would have been a far worse tool than those
mon activation from just these two studies. In one of the already used by social psychology. During a typical fMRI
studies (Wraga et al., 2006), increased activity in the rostral session, a person lays prone in the scanner wearing goggles
ACC was marginally associated with poorer task perfor- that allow the subject to see a video feed; the subject responds
mance. However, this could be explained either as distress during tasks almost exclusively with button boxes limited
interfering with task performance or as attention to regu- to a few buttons, and there are constant loud noises during
lating one’s distress interfering with task performance. An scanning. Finally, experimental trials from each condition of
ERP study (Forbes, Schmader, & Allen, 2008) found that interest often must be repeated dozens of times, meaning that
those in a stereotype threat condition who responded to the any task for which trial repetition will necessarily contami-
task by devaluing its significance produced smaller error- nate the psychological phenomenon is off limits. Despite
related negativity responses to their own errors, suggesting these limitations, there are specific ways in which neurosci-
less self-monitoring as a consequence of devaluing. ence can contribute to our social psychological enterprise
that should matter even to social psychologists uninterested
in the brain. The remainder of this section discusses some
IV. HOW SOCIAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE of the ways that social cognitive neuroscience can and has
CONTRIBUTES TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY contributed to the mission of social psychology.

Now that we have reviewed where dozens of social psy- Brain Mapping
chological processes occur in the brain, anyone would be
forgiven for believing that social cognitive neuroscience is Knowing where social psychological processes occur
little more than phrenology. Knowing that social processes in the brain does matter for at least a few reasons. First,
can be localized within the brain is not all that interesting. animal research and cognitive neuroscience have made
What is the alternative hypothesis? That they will be local- significant progress in figuring out the computations per-
ized in your elbow? A cognitive neuroscientist who has formed by particular brain regions. This knowledge can
taken a shine to the social side of things might respond that be drawn on to generate preliminary inferences about the
brain mapping is essential to understanding what differ- kinds of subprocesses subserving macrolevel social pro-
ent brain regions do. How can we really understand what cesses. That is, social processes usually encompass multiple
a brain region does if it is examined using only abstract component processes simultaneously or in rapid sequence,
decontextualized stimuli that cognitive psychologists typi- and identifying the involvement of brain regions with well-
cally use? A complete understanding of the brain will be characterized functions can help us identify which corre-
constituted only if the brain is studied while situated in all sponding psychological processes may contribute to the
its social psychological glory. total mental act. For instance, imagine that when individu-
A social psychologist would likely respond that it is all als watch one person greeting another person, a region of
fine and well that neuroscientists want to probe their favor- the lateral temporal cortex known to be primarily involved
ite brain regions using social psychological paradigms to with semantic processing (Noppeney & Price, 2004) was
figure out what those regions do. But what does that do for activated, compared with some control task. One might
social psychology? Is our social psychology improved at infer that watching this social episode is comprehensible to
all by looking at the brain? Are there social psychological us because we retrieve social scripts from semantic memory.
theories that should be updated in light of social cognitive Alternatively, imagine that watching this greeting activates
neuroscience data? Is there conventional wisdom in our the mirror system. This finding might suggest that people
field that needs to be reconsidered or looked at in a fresh understand social episodes through simulation rather than

CH05.indd 173 12/22/09 5:33:48 PM


174 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

semantic coding. Incidentally, it could be the case that both possibility that descriptions of social experience may be far
the mirror system and semantic processes are activated less metaphorical than once thought. Social rewards such
when observing the greeting. One of the advantages of neu- as positive social feedback or being treated with respect
roimaging over standard behavioral testing is that multiple (Izuma et al., 2008; Tabibnia et al., 2008) activate the ven-
systems can be interrogated simultaneously and often with- tral striatum in much the same way that winning money
out eliciting a behavioral response from subjects that would or eating chocolate does. The experience and regulation of
require particular instructions and an attentional set that social pain are associated with brain regions involved in the
might contaminate the natural attitude of the subject. experience and regulation of physical pain (Eisenberger &
Brain mapping discoveries are the beginning, not the Lieberman, 2004). The sense of insult in response to
end, of the process for social cognitive neuroscience. Once unfair treatment and the experience of disgust in response
the regions involved in a social process are identified, to sensory stimuli are both associated with activity in the
one can then more carefully interrogate those regions in anterior insula (Borg, Lieberman, & Kiehl, 2008; Calder
future studies that focus on hypothesis testing. As social et al., 2000; Hsu, Anen, & Quartz, 2008; Sanfey, Rilling,
psychologists, we are used to our everyday experiences Aronson, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2003; Wicker, Keysers,
serving as the anecdotal database from which we design et al., 2003). In each of these cases, the social phenom-
studies. Brain mapping studies are the way that social cog- enon seems less abstract and more embodied in light of
nitive neuroscientists create an anecdotal database. these linkages. In addition, these unexpected convergences
In many cases, it might be argued that brain mapping is have led to behavioral studies that would not have been done
telling us something we already know from other existing otherwise. For instance, behavioral studies have examined
behavioral research, and that is a fair criticism. However, the relationship between social and physical pain sensi-
we should ask ourselves what the value of a neuroimaging tivity (DeWall & Baumeister, 2006; Eisenberger, Jarcho,
study would have been, had it come first. Would it have Lieberman, & Naliboff, 2006), with one recent experiment
updated our social psychological theories just as the behav- finding that taking Tylenol reduced self-reported feelings
ioral research did? If so, it indicates that neuroimaging data of social rejection (DeWall et al., in press).
can constrain our theories (Kihlstrom, 2006); it is just a his- The linkage of social to physical pain changes our con-
torical accident that the behavioral study came first. Surely ceptual understanding of social rejection and the need for
in the future there will be times when the neuroimaging social connection. Maslow’s (1943) hierarchy of needs
study will come first and make significant contributions. orders our needs (from most basic to least basic) as bio-
logical, safety, belonging, esteem, and self-actualization.
In other words, biological and safety needs are critical to
Convergences
survival, and the rest are more or less gravy. However,
Although social cognitive neuroscience is still a young dis- deficits in social connection cause a form of pain just
cipline, one of its most exciting contributions is a series of as deficits in other survival needs cause a form of pain (e.g.,
findings in which two experiences that seem quite different hunger, thirst, cold). It seems that evolution has a special
from each other phenomenologically, or were thought to be painful place for deficits in basic survival needs, and social
only metaphorically related, actually rely on overlapping connection has made the cut. It has been speculated that
neural processes. The assumption is that if two processes because mammalian young are born relatively helpless,
rely on common brain regions, then they rely on common incapable of securing their own food, water, and shelter,
computational processes as well. It is exceedingly difficult continued social connection with their caregiver(s) is their
to demonstrate that two psychological events that feel dif- primary means of survival. Knowing that social rejection
ferent from each other share a great deal at the computa- activates the same pain processes as other survival need
tional level (Kosslyn, 1999). Yet such demonstrations are deficits allows us to think differently about social connec-
a critical component to advancing social psychological tion’s place in our hierarchy of needs (Baumeister & Leary,
theory. We group psychological phenomena into domains 1995; Lieberman & Eisenberger, 2009).
of study based on whether phenomena feel similar or meet
Using the Self to Understand Similar Others
some set of logical criteria; however, additional progress
would be made if psychological phenomena were grouped Other work has shown convergences within social cogni-
based on their deep structure. tion that have been hypothesized but never clearly dem-
onstrated. For instance, although it is not surprising that
Social Metaphors Are Not So Metaphorical people would use their knowledge of themselves to make
A number of social psychological phenomena have now sense of others, until recently there had been no hard evi-
been linked to nonsocial phenomena in ways that raise the dence one way or the other. Studies by Mitchell, Macrae,

CH05.indd 174 12/22/09 5:33:49 PM


How Social Cognitive Neuroscience Contributes to Social Psychology 175

and Banaji (2006) provide compelling evidence that we but these neuroimaging data suggest that adolescents are
do use ourselves to make sense of at least some people. doing something social when making direct self-appraisals.
Specifically, they showed that the same region of the medial What this something is requires further investigations.
PFC is active when making self-referential judgments To be sure, these convergences are open to multiple inter-
and judgments about a similar other but that this region pretations. They are new findings that need further interroga-
is not active when making judgments about a dissimilar tion. However, each suggests new conceptual understandings
other. Such findings open up a variety of opportunities to of social phenomena and may inspire a variety of behavioral
hypothesize about how targets will be differentially under- and neuroscience studies to follow up on these leads. New
stood and treated based on the relative contributions of the findings are rarely ends in themselves. However, each of
medial PFC or dorsomedial PFC (Harris et al., 2005). these findings is part of a social psychological conversation,
and suggests that neuroscience can indeed have a seat at the
Empathy table and even have something worth saying to social psy-
Knowing that experiencing physical pain and seeing oth- chologists now and then. It is also worth noting that in each
ers in physical pain recruit the same neural systems makes of the preceding examples, knowing which brain regions are
an important contribution to empathy research (Singer involved is relatively superfluous to the relevance of the find-
et al., 2004). When someone says, “I feel your pain,” we ings for social psychology. One need not have an interest in
can certainly quibble about whose pain they are feeling, but neuroanatomy to find an overlap in how the brain processes
for the first time there is evidence they are really feeling social and nonsocial rewards quite compelling. One need
someone’s pain rather than merely entertaining an abstract never know that the ventral striatum is the point of conver-
idea. This often-replicated overlap also provides an experi- gence for this to be relevant. The anatomy can be left to the
mental paradigm for testing various important aspects of anatomists, but the investigation of such overlaps provides a
empathy theories in the future because the modulation method for conceptual advances within social psychology.
of this overlap by situational and personality factors can
be easily assessed (Singer et al., 2006). Dissociations
Direct and Reflected Self-appraisals A basic tenet of all psychological research is that if two pro-
Social psychologists and sociologists have long hypoth- cesses or performances can be dissociated on some depen-
esized about the role that others’ evaluations of us have dent measure such as reaction time, then the processes are
on our own self-views (Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934). As distinct from one another. Neuroscience research is no dif-
compelling and influential as this symbolic interactionist ferent. When lesion studies observe that damage to region
account has been over the years, there has been surpris- A produces deficits in task X but not in task Y, compared
ingly little empirical evidence to support it. Behavioral with damage to region B, which produces deficits in task
research has focused on the overlap in the content of direct Y but spared performance in task X, this is taken as strong
and reflected self-appraisals. Neuroimaging, however, evidence that task X and Y rely on different psychological
allows us to examine the overlap in the structures support- processes. Similarly, when an fMRI study reveals that
ing different kinds of appraisals. It might be expected that different brain structures tend to be active during tasks
asking a 12-year-old boy what his best friend thinks of X and Y, this too suggests different psychological pro-
him would recruit brain regions known to be involved in cesses may be at work. In some cases, these differences
self-referential processing and also brain regions known to are quite relevant to social psychological theories.
be involved in mentalizing. Here, the adolescent is being
Social Cognition Is Special
asked to reflect on the mental state of another person and
to derive a self-evaluation from this. The fact that adoles- Perhaps the single best example of a neuroimaging study
cents recruit both of these systems when asked to make challenging the traditional understanding of a social psycho-
a direct appraisal of themselves (e.g., what do you think logical finding comes from Mitchell et al. (2004). In a clas-
of yourself?) is more surprising. This finding constitutes sic behavioral study, subjects read passages with the goal of
preliminary evidence of reflected appraisals being spon- either memorizing the material for later testing or forming
taneously generated even when they have not been asked a social impression of the target in the passage (Hamilton,
for (Pfeifer et al., 2009). Adults do not show broad acti- Katz, & Leirer, 1980). The surprising finding was that the
vation of the social cognition network when making self- impression formation goal led to better performance on a
referential judgments. Note that if asked to make a direct subsequent memory test, even though those with an impres-
self-appraisal, neither adolescent nor adult is likely to spon- sion formation goal did not know the test was coming and
taneously use reflected appraisal language in their replies, those in the memorization condition did. The generally

CH05.indd 175 12/22/09 5:33:49 PM


176 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

accepted explanation of these results was depth of process- neuroscience. The implication is obvious. When left to
ing (Craik & Tulving, 1975), such that social encoding was their own devices, people think about themselves and their
believed to be a deeper, more elaborative form of encoding social lives (D’Argembeau et al., 2005; Gusnard et al.,
than encoding with a memorization goal. 2001; Iacoboni et al., 2004; Mason et al., 2007; Wicker,
Mitchell and colleagues (2004) replicated this paradigm Ruby, Royet, & Fonlupt, 2003).
in the scanner and discovered what those earlier studies could Put a different way, the brain’s default focus is social.
not. Social and nonsocial encoding do not just differ quan- Only when something nonsocial, like a working memory
titatively on a depth of processing dimension. Rather, they task, requires it to direct its resources elsewhere does it
rely on qualitatively dissociable processes. Countless stud- momentarily stop focusing on the social. Social psycholo-
ies have shown that successful memorization (i.e., encod- gists might find this to be obvious, but to funding agen-
ing that leads to later retrieval success) is associated with cies, the media, and your grandparents this kind of finding
activity in left ventrolateral PFC and the medial temporal really helps to firm up the significance of what we study
lobes (Wagner et al., 1998). Mitchell found that activity in (the fact that the size of the prefrontal cortex across species
these regions did predict retrieval success in the memori- correlates the typical group size in each species is a good
zation condition but did not predict retrieval success in the one to throw out there too; Dunbar, 1998).
social encoding condition. Instead, retrieval success in the It should be noted that it was recently reported that anes-
social encoding condition was associated with activity in thetized unconscious monkeys still had increased activity in
the dorsomedial PFC. This finding strongly calls into ques- the default regions (Vincent et al., 2007; see also Fransson
tion the depth of processing account and instead suggests that et al., 2007). This raises a fascinating issue, one that should
there is something qualitatively different about social encod- be relevant to social psychologists (and not just impress
ing. Regardless of how one evaluates the significance of this their grandparents). Does the brain show these social cog-
problem, it is a clear case in which the inference from the nition activations at rest because this is what we choose to
behavioral data was wrong and the neuroimaging evidence think about in our spare time? Or is it the case that we tend
provided a clear and compelling case for distinct processes to focus on social and self-related thinking in our spare time
operating in social and nonsocial encoding. Upon learning because high baseline activity in these regions biases us, in
the results of this study, one must update one’s understand- a sense priming us, to think about these things? Has evolu-
ing of this phenomenon based on these neuroimaging data. tion progressed in such a way that it has proved adaptive to
have our spare thought biased toward processing and repro-
Social by Default cessing information about ourselves and the social world?
One of the most significant discoveries in the past decade
Automaticity and Control
of cognitive neuroscience research is the default network.
These regions are highly activated when a subject is at rest At the end of the 1990s, great attention was being
(i.e., when not being given any experimental task to perform) devoted to the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald,
(Raichle et al., 2001) and show highly coordinated activ- McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) as a method for assessing
ity with each other at rest (Fox et al., 2005). They become implicit attitudes. On one hand, large numbers of social
less active when cognitive tasks are performed (Greicius, psychologists were conducting Implicit Association Test
Krasnow, Reiss, & Menon, 2003; Shulman et al., 1997), studies because among implicit measures it was straight-
to the extent that the cognitive tasks are more demand- forward to use and produced strong experimental effects
ing (McKiernan, Kaufman, Kucera-Thompson, & Binder, with relatively modest sample sizes. On the other hand,
2003), but when active during cognitive tasks, they tend to be there was a great deal of controversy over what the Implicit
associated with producing errors (Boly et al., 2007; Li, Yan, Association Test measured and whether what it mea-
Bergquist, & Sinha, 2007; Weissman, Roberts, Visscher, & sured could legitimately be called implicit. At one point,
Woldorff, 2006). At rest, these regions produce activity that so the story goes, the Journal of Personality and Social
is inversely correlated with activity in brain regions support- Psychology had a moratorium on publishing any addi-
ing common cognitive tasks (Fox et al., 2005). tional Implicit Association Test papers until it was clear
What is striking is that this default network could easily that it really assessed implicit attitudes. When Phelps and
be mistaken for a self and social cognition network. All of colleagues (2000) reported that the strength of amygdala
the regions that are highly active at rest (dorsomedial PFC, responses to images of Black faces was strongly associated
medial PFC, ventromedial PFC, precuneus, TPJ in almost with Implicit Association Test scores but not with explicit
all studies, with fusiform gyrus and temporal poles also attitude scores, this was generally received as significantly
appearing with some frequency) are among the regions that strengthening the case that the Implicit Association Test
figure most prominently in this review of social cognitive truly measured implicit attitudes. The amygdala has long

CH05.indd 176 12/22/09 5:33:50 PM


How Social Cognitive Neuroscience Contributes to Social Psychology 177

been thought to primarily engage in automatic processes, Based on the neural deficits associated with each neuropsy-
given its phylogenetic history, its early position in the chological impairment, neuroimaging studies have shown
visual processing stream, its role in fear conditioning in that implicit learning is associated with basal ganglia acti-
rodents, and the fact that subliminal presentations of fear vations, whereas explicit learning has been associated
expressions activate this region. If the Implicit Association with medial temporal lobe activations (Lieberman, Chang,
Test scores, but not explicit attitudes, are associated with Chiao, Bookheimer, & Knowlton, 2004; Poldrack et al.,
amygdala responses, then there is a good chance the 2001). Moreover, these regions appear to be in competition
Implicit Association Test is measuring something implicit. such that if one region is relatively active during task perfor-
Thus, neuroimaging findings help distinguish implicit from mance, the other tends to be correspondingly deactivated.
explicit attitudes and clarify the interpretation of one of the In one particularly elegant study, Foerde, Knowlton, and
most commonly used social psychological instruments. Poldrack (2006) trained subjects on two tasks known to be
As with implicit and explicit attitudes, several dual- learnable using both implicit and explicit processes. For one
process models within social psychology (Chaiken & Trope, task, subjects were trained under cognitive load; the other
1999) posit some combination of automatic and controlled task was learned without cognitive load. When there was no
processes believed to share the work in various domains cognitive load task, thus facilitating explicit learning strat-
(e.g., persuasion, attribution, self-knowledge, empathy). egies, activity in the medial temporal lobe during training
Automatic processes are fast, resistant to interruption, was associated with performance accuracy at a follow-up
independent of conscious intention, or outside of aware- test session. When there was cognitive load during train-
ness, whereas controlled processes are slow, interruptible, ing, thus interfering with explicit learning strategies, activity
intention-driven, and accessible to awareness (Wegner & in the medial temporal lobe during training was associated
Bargh, 1998). There are several remaining important ques- with performance at test; instead, activity in the basal gan-
tions about dual-process models. For instance, are auto- glia was associated with later performance. Critically, the
maticity and control two ends of a spectrum in which the behavioral performances were equivalent in both condi-
same processes and representations are employed but with tions. In other words, behaviorally there was no evidence
differing levels of efficiency? Or are there distinct auto- that different underlying psychological processes were sup-
matic and controlled processes that differ qualitatively porting performance at test, but neuroimaging revealed that
and may be sensitive to different types of inputs, store infor- there were indeed different processes at work. These results
mation differently, and respond differently as a function strongly suggest that there are two separate processes that
of context? If there are separate processes, how many sets of operate at different times and in different contexts. Although
dual-processes exist? One scientist (Kruglanski et al., there may be a smooth transition in observable performance
2003) hyperbolically suggested that there might be 30 sets as learning and performance switch from being controlled to
of dual-processes based on the fact that a contemporary automatic, the underlying neural responses argue for quali-
volume on dual-process models (Chaiken & Trope, 1999) tatively distinct processes.
had 30 chapters, each putting forth a dual-process model Although social cognitive neuroscience research has
with only minimal connections made between the different rarely set out to compare automatic and controlled variants
models. Because experiences in different domains of social of social cognition, a number of studies have had conditions
psychology feel so different from one another and have that would at least roughly meet the criteria allowing for such
such different outcomes, it is hard to assess whether dual- a comparison. Lieberman (2007) reviewed the findings from
process models in these domains (e.g., persuasion and ste- several domains of social cognition. Six brain regions were
reotyping) rely on common processes. Similarly, because reliably invoked during controlled, but not automatic, forms
different underlying processing architectures can produce of social cognition; these regions included the lateral PFC,
the same behaviors, it can be difficult to identify which pro- lateral parietal cortex, medial PFC, dorsomedial PFC, precu-
cessing architectures are really at work (Gilbert, 1999). neus, and medial temporal lobe. Four regions were reliably
Neuroimaging has been quite informative in general invoked during automatic, but not controlled, forms of social
in helping to tease apart processes that are implicit, auto- cognition; these regions included the amygdala, ventrome-
matic, nonconscious, or reflexive from those that are dial PFC, lateral temporal cortex, and ventral striatum.
explicit, controlled, conscious, or reflective (Lieberman, These results suggest an answer to the first of the lin-
2009a; Satpute & Lieberman, 2006). For instance, explicit gering dual-process questions: Are there really separate
learning is impaired in anterograde amnesiacs but not in automatic and controlled social processes? The findings are
patients with Parkinson’s disease, whereas implicit learning more consistent with an account of separate automatic and
is impaired in patients with Parkinson’s disease but not in controlled processes, rather than an account wherein single
anterograde amnesiacs (Knowlton, Mangels, & Squire, 1996). processes are called automatic when they operate efficiently

CH05.indd 177 12/22/09 5:33:50 PM


178 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

and called controlled when they operate inefficiently. Rather, Additionally, by definition, automatic processes that can be
it appears that with training, the brain regions responsible triggered without one’s intentions (e.g., through subliminal
for automatic processes slowly develop computational algo- presentations) are believed to be immune to interruption
rithms to support task performance, and as these processes from conscious processing. Finally, the controlled process
come online, brain regions supporting controlled processing in question directs attention to the emotional aspects of the
are needed less and less. stimulus and thus is unlikely to reduce amygdala activity
These data also speak to the second lingering ques- through distraction effects.
tion of how many sets of dual-processes exist. Although Although difficult to explain from a social cognition per-
no definitive answer is available, the review (Lieberman, spective, from a cognitive neuroscience perspective, these
2007) found that brain regions involved in automatic or results are quite amenable to explanation. There are brain
controlled processes tended to each be involved in a regions that, independent of one another, show evidence
variety of automatic or controlled processes. For instance, of possessing the operating characteristics of automatic or
the ventromedial PFC has been associated with automatic controlled processes, as commonly defined. However, these
aspects of self-knowledge, decision making, emotional brain regions are also interconnected in a multitude of ways,
experience, and attitudes, whereas right ventrolateral PFC including functionally inhibitory connections. In other
has been associated with inhibitory control over behavior, words, the amygdala may process emotional inputs in an
thought, emotion, attitudes, and perspective (Cohen & automatic fashion that requires no controlled processing
Lieberman, in press). Thus, it appears that the same net- resources to operate; however, the amygdala may also be
works responsible for automatic and controlled processing anatomically linked with regions of the prefrontal cortex
in one social psychological domain may deal with auto- that can inhibit the amygdala’s functioning if they are acti-
matic and controlled processing in other domains as well. vated. Although such results do not in themselves demand
The phenomenologically different inputs in each social a rewriting of all the rules of automaticity and control, they
domain may produce different outputs but still make use of do suggest aspects that are worth reconsidering and testing
a shared dual-process architecture. This may help explain as this new channel of data becomes available.
phenomena such as ego depletion (Baumeister, Bratslavsky,
Internal and External Self-Focus
Muraven, & Tice, 1998), in which self-control efforts in one
domain undermine subsequent self-control efforts in another The mirror self-recognition test (Gallup, 1970) is used to
domain. From the perspective of the brain, the processing test whether a particular species possesses self-awareness.
resources from the same brain regions may be required for Consequently, it is rather surprising that the network of
both tasks, and thus the brain is not starting fresh when mov- brain regions involved in recognizing oneself in a picture
ing from one task to another. and the network of brain regions involved in reflecting on
Such neuroscience findings may also help update our one’s feelings, preferences, and traits are completely non-
understanding of the relationship between automatic- overlapping networks (Lieberman, 2007). External self-
ity and control more broadly. Similar to the implicit and focus (i.e., visual self-recognition) is reliably associated
explicit learning findings, in a number of the reviewed stud- with a lateral frontoparietal network in the right hemi-
ies (Lieberman, in press), increasing activity in controlled sphere, whereas internal self-focus (i.e., reflecting on one’s
processing regions was associated with decreased activity psychological characteristics) is reliably associated with a
in automatic processing regions such as the amygdala. For medial frontoparietal network. What’s more, the activity in
instance, looking at an emotional picture nonreflectively these two networks at rest tend to be inversely correlated
leads to reliable amygdala activity. However, labeling the with one another (Fox et al., 2005). This separation of the
emotional content of the same picture reflectively leads neural networks supporting internal and external self-focus
to reliable right ventrolateral PFC activity and correlated calls into question whether the mirror self-awareness test is
decreases in amygdala activity. From the typical view of actually an index of the ability to reflect on the psychologi-
automaticity, it is difficult to explain how amygdala activity cal aspects of oneself or is limited to an ability to recognize
in response to an emotional picture would be diminished by the physical manifestations of oneself, perhaps a precursor
the addition of a conscious reflective process. The amyg- to, rather than evidence of, true self-awareness.
dala response occurs when such pictures are presented sub- Potentially the greatest implication of this dissociation
liminally (Morris et al., 1998; Whalen et al., 1998), a gold is that it may help explain why nearly all human beings
standard for automaticity. Automatic processes are believed maintain some intuitive belief in mind–body dualism,
not to rely on the common pool of controlled processes even when rationally admitting that dualism is a nonstarter
resources; thus, conscious reflective processing should not logically (Lieberman, 2009a). Although the broad strokes
take away any resources that the amygdala needs to respond. of Descartes’ brand of dualism focused on the existence of

CH05.indd 178 12/22/09 5:33:51 PM


How Social Cognitive Neuroscience Contributes to Social Psychology 179

two strata—the material and the immaterial, the impact of to be biased as a result of the structure of information
dualism largely follows from imputing material and imma- processing and the information sample available for con-
terial aspects to each individual (i.e., mind and body). Part sideration (e.g., a person is aware of all the housework
of the reason that this discredited theory is so compelling done by oneself but only a portion done by a roommate).
is that everyone has experiences that feel like a struggle Because studies often provided positive evidence for their
between two aspects of the self. When we “drag ourselves position without providing evidence against the alternative
out of bed,” this fits nicely with the notion that there is a account, the debate eventually lost steam and was believed
mind that somehow forces the unwilling body out of bed. by many to be irresolvable (Tetlock & Levi, 1982). If
However, the clean division between the brain regions neuroimaging can assess the extent to which self-related
involved in internal self-focus (i.e., focusing on one’s or motivational processes are at work, it should be pos-
mind) and external self-focus (i.e., focusing on one’s body) sible to fruitfully revisit this debate. In all likelihood, both
suggests that mind–body dualism may be a particularly motivational and cognitive processes can contribute to
sticky notion because our brain cleaves our perceptions these effects, but neuroimaging might reveal individual
of ourselves into these components whether we ask our differences in the source of these biases across individuals,
brain to or not. Just as sights and sounds are automatically which in turn might relate to different psychological con-
processed by separate neural networks and give rise to sequences (e.g., resistance to being challenged).
irreducibly distinct sensations, perhaps the separate pro- In the 1990s, research on automatic goals, motives, and
cessing streams for reflecting on one’s own mind and body behavior was (and continues to be) enormously influen-
produce the irreducible experience of dualism. tial (Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001). The fact that priming
“impression” leads people to act as if they have an impres-
sion formation goal (Chartrand & Bargh, 1996), that
Future Questions
priming “succeed” can produce an array of motivational
Expected or unexpected convergences and dissociations in phenomena (Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, &
the brain regions responsible for particular social processes Trötschel, 2001), and that priming “elderly” can lead peo-
help group these processes into the appropriate psycholog- ple to walk more slowly (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996)
ical bins. Neuroscience techniques allow for other kinds are extraordinary findings. Nevertheless, it is unclear from
of insights and hypothesis testing as well, although at this these findings alone whether automatic and nonautomatic
point, very little of this work has been done. For instance, variants of these processes are in fact one and the same.
as cognitive neuroscientists refine their understanding of The assumption within this literature is that they are the
the basic computations performed by different regions, same, but this has remained an assumption. Neuroimaging
activation in different networks can serve as an indicator may be relatively uniquely positioned to address this
that certain psychological processes have been invoked question because it can clearly show whether two puta-
(c.f. Poldrack, 2006). This is not to suggest that we will tive processes are relying on common or distinct neural
be able to look at the brain and know whether someone is networks.
reading Haruki Murakami or Italo Calvino anytime soon, Automatic goals, motives, and behaviors fall into the
but we may be able to have some idea of whether a person broader category of phenomena that are real but seem a bit
is at least recruiting self-processes in a very general way, magical. There are other linkages that always seem a
which would be useful. bit magical as well, such as the functioning of placebo
Starting in the 1970s, a variety of self-serving or ego- effects, hypnosis, and the impact of social support on
centric biases were reported on. For instance, people who health (after controlling for specific health care provided
live together each tend to believe they are responsible for by supporters). In each of these cases, it’s hard to tell a
a disproportionate amount of the housework that gets done straightforward compelling story about why the phenom-
(Ross & Sicoly, 1975). Similarly, after being asked if they ena occur because each is at odds with our basic dualistic
would walk around wearing a giant sign saying “Eat at notions that beliefs can change beliefs and overt behavior
Joe’s” for a small payment, regardless of the choice they but beliefs cannot change low-level perceptual or physi-
made, subjects tended to believe most other people would ological responses (i.e., our more mechanistic processes).
make the same choice as they did (Ross et al., 1977). Rival In each case, neuroimaging data are starting to reveal
accounts of these self-serving biases (Greenwald, 1980; where in the brain the magic happens (Eisenberger, Taylor,
Nisbett & Ross, 1980) led to countless studies attempting Gable, Hilmert, & Lieberman, 2007; Kosslyn, Thompson,
to show whether these effects were due to motivational Costantini-Ferrando, Alpert, & Spiegel, 2000; Wager
processes intended to justify a person’s own behavior and et al., 2004), and this will allow for further interrogation of
positions or were due to cognitive processes that tended these brain regions and how their neurocognitive function

CH05.indd 179 12/22/09 5:33:52 PM


180 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

might produce the observed results. Neuroscience is hardly of cognitive psychology for its own purposes. Whether
a cure-all, but these are the kinds of problems for which social psychologists choose to embrace the methods of neu-
neuroscience methods may shed new light and prompt new roscience to pursue our mission is still an open question.
programs of behavioral research. Nevertheless, this alone will determine whether the next
review of social cognitive neuroscience, a decade from now,
will have a better balance between brain mapping studies
V. CONCLUSIONS AND THE NEXT DECADE and studies that move social psychology forward.

This chapter has provided a history of social cognitive neu-


roscience, the neural landmarks that have been laid down REFERENCES
for hypothesis testing in various domains of social psy-
chology, and an exploration of the specific ways in which Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinion
social cognitive neuroscience directly contributes to the in Neurobiology, 11, 231–239.
mission of social psychology. Given the number of pages Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., &
Damasio, A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition
devoted to each of these sections, there is no denying that
after amygdala damage. Nature, 433, 68–72.
the emphasis of the past decade has been on brain mapping
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1998). The human amygdala in
far more than hypothesis testing. This is not surprising, social judgment. Nature, 393, 470–471.
because for neuroimaging research, the hypothesis test- Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1994). Impaired
ing phase generally follows the brain mapping phase. But recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage
to the human amygdala. Nature, 372, 669–672.
a more significant factor in the relative balance between
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Fear and
brain mapping and hypothesis testing is that doing the
the human amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 5879–5891.
kind of social cognitive neuroscience studies that ask and Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., &
answer the questions of social psychology is hard, much Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI
harder than doing a brain mapping study to see what lights and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32, 537–551.
up. Indeed, making truly meaningful contributions to Ajzen, I. (2001). Nature and operation of attitudes. Annual Review of
Psychology, 52, 27–58.
social psychology using any methodology is hard because
Akiyama, T., Kato, M., Muramatsu, T., Saito, F., Nakachi, R., & Kashima, H.
our phenomena are counterintuitive, our subjects are mov- (2006). A deficit in discriminating gaze direction in a case with right
ing targets trying to figure out the purpose of our experi- superior temporal gyrus lesion. Neuropsychologia, 44, 161–170.
ments, and our experiments must recreate just the right Akiyama, T., Kato, M., Muramatsu, T., Saito, F., Umeda, S., & Kashima, H.
ecologically valid experiences within ethically acceptable (2006). Gaze but not arrows: A dissociative impairment after right
superior temporal gyrus damage. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1804–1810.
limits, while still assessing the appropriate dependent vari-
Allison, T., Ginter, H., McCarthy, G., Nobre, A. C., Puce, A., Luby, M.,
ables. All of this is made that much harder when subjects et al. (1994). Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. Journal of
are essentially lying in coffin-like confinement, unable to Neurophysiology, 71, 821–825.
move, unable to speak, and needing several repetitions of Altshuler, L., Bookheimer, S.,Townsend, J., Proenza, M.A., Eisenberger, N. I.,
each trial type to extract detectable signals from the noise. Sabb, F., et al. (2005). Blunted activation in orbitofrontal cortex
during mania: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
Social cognitive neuroscience studies that address Biological Psychiatry, 58, 763–769.
social psychological questions will only be carried out Ames, D. R. (2004). Inside the mind reader ’s tool kit: Projection and ste-
to the extent that social psychologists want to ask those reotyping in mental state inference. Journal of Personality and Social
questions and make a commitment to conducting social Psychology, 87, 340–353.
cognitive neuroscience studies, either on their own or with Amodio, D. M. (2008). The social neuroscience of intergroup relations.
European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 1–54.
collaborators. Cognitive neuroscientists who are interested
Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of the minds: The medial
in using social psychological paradigms to clarify what frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature, 7, 268–277.
different brain regions do have every right to do so. They Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., Devine, P. G., Curtin, J. J.,
are pursuing their intellectual passion and they should. Hartley, S. L., & Covert, A. E. (2004). Neural signals for the detection
of unintentional racial bias. Psychological Science, 15, 88–93.
There is no reason why they should suddenly care about
Anders, S., Birbaumer, N., Sadowski, B., Erb, M., Mader, I., Grodd, W.,
the enduring questions of social psychology anymore than et al. (2004). Parietal somatosensory association cortex mediates
social psychologists should suddenly care about the endur- affective blindsight. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 339–340.
ing issues in neuroscience. Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., De Rosa, E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E.
It is incumbent upon social psychologists to make use (2003). Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial
signals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5627–5633.
of neuroscience for their own ends. And this is nothing new
Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., Chiavaino, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004).
for social psychologists. In the 1970s, social psychology Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind:
reinvented itself in large measure by co-opting the methods Neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced lan-

CH05.indd 180 12/22/09 5:33:52 PM


References 181

guage and executive demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, Beaucousin, V., Lacheret, A., Turbelin, M.-R., Morel, M. I., Mazoyer, B., &
1773–1784. Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2007). fMRI study of emotional speech
Aron, A. R, Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., & Brown, L. L. comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 339–352.
(2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early- Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates
stage intense romantic love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 327–337. of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21,
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right RC165.
inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 170–177. Bechara, A. (2002). The neurology of social cognition. Brain, 125, 1673–1675.
Babiloni, C., Vecchio, F., Bares, M., Brazdil, M., Nestrasil, I., Eusebi, F., Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994).
et al. (2008). Functional coupling between anterior prefrontal cortex Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human
(BA10) and hand muscle contraction during intentional and imitative prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.
motor acts. NeuroImage, 39, 1314–1323.
Beer, J. S., Heerey, E. A., Keltner, D., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2003).
Bach, D. R., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., Herdener, M., Strik, W. K., & The regulatory function of self-conscious emotion: Insights from
Seifritz, E. (2008). The effect of appraisal level on processing of emo- patients with orbitofrontal damage. Journal of Personality and Social
tional prosody in meaningless speech. NeuroImage, 42, 919–927. Psychology, 85, 594–604.
Baddeley, A. D. (2002). Fractionating the central executive. In D. T. Stuss &
Beer, J. S., John, O. P., & Knight, R. T. (2006). Orbitofrontal cortex and
R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function. (pp. 246–260).
social behavior: Integrating self-monitoring and emotion cognition
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 871–879.
Baird, A., Dewar, B., Critchley, H., Gilbert, S. J., Dolan, R. J., & Cipolotti, L.
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social
(2006). Cognitive functioning after medial frontal lobe damage including
history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10, 122–142.
the ACC: A preliminary investigation. Brain and Cognition, 60, 166–175.
Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H., Berntson, G. G., Sarter, M., Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Anxiety and cardio-
et al. (2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in vascular reactivity: The basal forebrain cholinergic link. Behavioural
professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction. NeuroImage, Brain Research, 94, 225–248.
30, 917–926. Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An fMRI
Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J., & Luan Phan, K. study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of
(2007). Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. social norms. Brain, 125, 1696–1708.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 303–312. Bidet-Caulet, A., Voisin, J., Bertrand, O., & Fonlupt, P. (2005). Listening
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social to a walking human activates the temporal biological motion area.
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on NeuroImage, 28, 132–139.
action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244. Bird, C. M., Castelli, F., Malik, U., & Husain, M. (2004). The impact of
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trötschel, R. extensive medial frontal lobe damage on “theory of mind” and cogni-
(2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of tion. Brain, 127, 914–928.
behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, Blakemore, S.-J., den Ouden, H., Choudhury, S., & Frith, C. (2007).
1014–1027. Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S., intentions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 130–139.
Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. C. R. (2000). The amygdala theory of Blakemore, S.-J., Oakley, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Delusions of alien
autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 355–364. control in the normal brain. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1058–1067.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T.,
Blakemore S.-J, Winston, J, & Frith, U. (2004). Social cognitive neuro-
Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., et al. (1999). Social intelligence in
science: Where are we heading? Trends in Cognitive Science, 8,
the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of
216–222.
Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898.
Blakeslee, S. (2006, January 10). Cells that read minds. New York Times.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love.
Neuroreport, 11, 3829–3834. Blanke, O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L., & Seeck, M. (2004). Out-of-body expe-
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and rience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain, 127, 243–258.
romantic love. NeuroImage, 21, 1155–1166. Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michael, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P.,
Bartholow, B. D., & Dickter, C. L. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience Seeck, M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self pro-
of person perception: A selective review focused on the event-related cessing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction.
brain potential. In P. Winkielman & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Social The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 550–557.
neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., & Seeck, M. (2002). Stimulating illu-
of social behavior (pp. 376–400). New York: Guilford Press. sory own-body perceptions. Nature, 419, 269–270.
Bartholow, B. D., Pearson, M. A., Dickter, C. L., Sher, K. J., Fabiani, M., &, Boly, M., Balteau, E., Schnakers, C., Degueldre, C., Moonen, G., Luxen, A.,
Gratton, G. (2005). Strategic control and medial frontal negativity: et al. (2007). Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosen-
Beyond errors and response conflict. Psychophysiology, 42, 33–42. sory perception in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological Sciences, 104, 12187–12192.
answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Borg, J. S., Hynes, C., Horn, J. V., Grafton, S., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego (2006). Consequences, action, and intention as factors in moral judgments:
depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 803–817.
and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265. Borg, J. S., Lieberman, D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2008). Infection, incest, and
Baumeister, R. F. & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire iniquity: Investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality.
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1529–1546.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. Botvinick, M., Jha, A. P., Bylsma, L. M., Fabian, S. A., Solomon, P. E., &
Baumgartner, T., Heinrichs, M., Vonlanthen, A., Fischbacher, U., & Prkachin, K. M. (2005). Viewing facial expressions of pain engages
Fehr, E. (2008). Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. NeuroImage,
adaptations in humans. Neuron, 58, 639–650. 25, 312–319.

CH05.indd 181 12/22/09 5:33:53 PM


182 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). The inhibition of Chaminade, T., & Decety, J. (2002). Leader or follower? Involvement of
imitative and overlearned responses: A functional double dissociation. the IPL in agency. NeuroReport, 13, 1975–1978.
Neuropsychologia, 43, 89–98. Chan, A. W. Y., Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2004). The effect of view-
Brass, M., Zysset, S., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). The inhibition of imita- point on body representation in the extrastriate body area. NeuroReport,
tive response tendencies. NeuroImage, 14, 1416–1423. 15, 2407–2410.
Brattico, E., Tervaniemi, M., & Picton, T. W. (2003). Effects of brief Channon, S., & Crawford, S. (2000). The effects of anterior lesions on
discrimination-training on the auditory N1 wave. NeuroReport, 14, performance on a story comprehension test: Left anterior impairment
2489–2492. on a theory of mind-type task. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1006–1017.
Brehm, J. W. (1956). Post-decision changes in the desirability of alterna- Chartrand, T., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impression
tives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 384–389. formation and memorization goals: Nonconscious goal priming repro-
Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M.-C., & Decety, J. (2000). A PET duces effects of explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality and
investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. Social Psychology, 71, 464–478.
NeuroImage, 11, 157–166. Chen, Y., Meltzof, A. N., & Decety, J. (2007). Motivation modulates the
Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G. R., Zilles, K., Freund, H.-J., et al. activity of the human mirror-neuron system. Cerebral Cortex, 17,
(2004). Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: 1979–1986.
An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 42, 323–334. Chong, T. T.-J., Williams, M. A., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J. B.
Burklund, L. J., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). The face of (2008). Selective attention modulates inferior frontal gyrus activity
rejection: Rejection sensitivity moderates dorsal anterior cingulated activity during action observation. NeuroImage, 40, 298–307.
to disapproving facial expressions. Social Neuroscience, 2, 238–253. Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Ladavas, E., & di Pellegrino, G. (2007).
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influ- Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage
ences in anterior cingulated cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective
215–222. Neuroscience, 2, 84–92.
Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review Ciaramidaro, A., Adenzato, M., Enrici, I., Erk, S., Pia, L., Bara, B. G.,
of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, et al. (2007). The intentional network: How the brain reads varieties of
12, 1–47. intentions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3105–3113.
Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). Social neuroscience: Automatic, neuroendocrine, Cloutier, J., Turk, D. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Extracting variant and
and immune responses to stress. Psychophysiology, 31, 113–128. invariant information from faces: The neural substrates of gaze detec-
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Lorig, T. S., Norris, C. J., Rickett, E., & tion and sex categorization. Social Neuroscience, 3, 69–78.
Nusbaum, H. (2003). Just because you’re imaging the brain doesn’t Cohen, J. R., & Lieberman, M. D. (in press). The common neural basis of
mean you can stop using your head: A primer and set of first principles. exerting self-control in multiple domains. In Y. Trope, R. Hassin, &
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 650–661. K. N. Ochsner (Eds.), Self-control.
Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., Jr., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Attitudes to Colman, A. M. (2006). Oxford Dictionary of Psychology (2nd ed.).
the right: Evaluative processing is associated with lateralized late posi- New York: Oxford University Press.
tive event-related brain potentials. Personality and Social Psychology Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and social order. New York: Scribner’s.
Bulletin, 22, 1205–1219. Craik, F. I. M., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M., Stuss, D. T., Winocur, G.,
Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Lawrence, A. D., & Manes, F. (2004). Impaired Tulving, E., et al. (1999). In search of the self: A positron emission
recognition of anger following damage to the ventral striatum. Brain, tomography study. Psychological Science, 10, 26–34.
127, 1958–1969. Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention
Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N., & Young, A. W. (2000). of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. 104, 268–294.
Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1077–1078. Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., Tabibnia, G., Lieberman, M. D., & Robbins, T. W.
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2008). Serotonin modulates behavioral reactions to unfairness.
(2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study Science, 30, 1739.
with expert dancers. Cerebral cortex, 15, 1243–1249. Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J. M., Hamilton, A. F. C., Kelley, W. M., &
Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2005). Neuroeconomics: How Grafton, S. T. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to
neuroscience can inform economics. Journal of Economic Literature, physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 315–326.
18, 9–64. Cunningham, W. A., Espinet, S. D., DeYoung, C. G., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005).
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibi- Attitudes to the right and left: Frontal ERP asymmetries associated
tory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, with stimulus valence and processing goals. NeuroImage, 28, 827–834.
1032–1053. Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., &
Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. Banaji, M. R. (2003). Neural components of social evaluation. Journal
(2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neu- of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 639–649.
ral systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Gatenby, J. C.,
Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, 5497–5502. Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Separable neural components in the
Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15, 806–813.
love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779–818. Cunningham, W. A., Raye, C. L., & Johnson, M. K. (2004). Implicit and
Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional inten-
syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to sity, and control in the processing of attitudes. Journal of Cognitive
animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849. Neuroscience, 16, 1717–1729.
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social D’Argembeau, A., Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Del Fiore, G.,
psychology. New York: Guilford Press. Degueldre, C., et al. (2005). Self-referential reflective activity and its
Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Empathizing relationship with rest: A PET study. NeuroImage, 25, 616–624.
with basic emotions: Common and discrete neural substrates. Social D’Argembeau, A., Ruby, P., Collette, F., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E.,
Neuroscience, 1, 364–384. Luxen, A., et al. (2007). Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex

CH05.indd 182 12/22/09 5:33:54 PM


References 183

are associated with self-referential processing and perspective taking. Eisenberger, N. I., Gable, S. L., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). fMRI
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 935–944. responses relate to differences in real-world social experience.
D’Argembeau, A., Xue, G., Lu, Z.-L., Van der Linden, M., & Bechara, A. Emotion, 7, 745–754.
(2008). Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near Eisenberger, N. I., Jarcho, J. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Naliboff, B. D.
and far future. NeuroImage, 40, 398–407. (2006). An experimental study of shared sensitivity to physical pain
Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., and social rejection. Pain, 126, 132–138.
Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts:
the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, A common neural alarm system for physical pain and social pain.
8, 519–526. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 294–300.
Davis, K. D., Taylor, K. S., Hutchison, W. D., Dostrovsky, J. O., Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Satpute, A. B. (2005). Personality
McAndrews, M. P., Richter, E. O., & Lozano, A. M. (2005). Human from a controlled processing perspective: An fMRI study of neuroti-
anterior cingulate cortex neurons encode cognitive and emotional cism, extraversion, & self-consciousness. Cognitive, Affective, and
demands. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 8402–8406. Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 169–181.
Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sympathy. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejec-
Neuropsychologia, 41, 127–138. tion hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292.
Decety, J., Grèzes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Eisenberger, N. I., Taylor, S. E., Gable, S. L., Hilmert, C. J., & Lieberman, M. D.
et al. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions influence of (2007). Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendo-
action content and subject’s strategy. Brain, 120, 1763–1777. crine stress response. NeuroImage, 35, 1601–1612.
Decety, J., Jackson, P. L., Sommerville, J. A., Chaminade, T., & Meltzoff, A. N. Eisenberger, N. I., Way, B. M., Taylor, S. E., Welch, W. T., & Lieberman, M. D.
(2004). The neural bases of cooperation and competition: An fMRI (2007). Understanding genetic risk for aggression: Clues from the
investigation. NeuroImage, 23, 744–751. brain’s response to social exclusion. Biological Psychiatry, 61,
Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junc- 1100–1108.
tion in social interaction: How low-level computational processes con- Engel, A., Burke, M., Fiehler, K., Bien, S., & Rösler, F. (2008). What
tribute to meta-cognition. The Neuroscientist, 13, 580–593. activates the human neuron system during observation of artifi-
De Cremer, D., & Alberts, H. J. E. M. (2004). When procedural fairness cial movements: Bottom-up visual features or top-down intentions?
does not influence how positive I feel: The effects of voice and leader Neuropsychologia, 46, 2033–2042.
selection as a function of belongingness need. European Journal of Engel, A., Burke, M., Fiehler, K., Bien, S., & Rösler, F. (in press). How
Social Psychology, 34, 333–344. moving objects become animated: The human mirror neuron system
Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral assimilates non-biological movement patterns. Social Neuroscience.
character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness
Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1611–1618. decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala.
Delgado, M. R., Gillis, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Regulating the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1508–1519.
expectation of reward via cognitive strategies. Nature Neuroscience, Enger, T., Etkins, A., Gale, S., & Hirsch, J. (2008). Dissociable neural sys-
11, 880–881. tems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters.
den Ouden, H. E., Frith, U., Frith, C., & Blakemore, S.J. (2005). Thinking Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1475–1484.
about intentions. NeuroImage, 28, 787–796. Ethofer, T., Anders, S., Erb, M., Herbert, C., Wiethoff, S., Kissler, J.,
Devue, C., Collette, F., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., et al. (2006). Cerebral pathways in processing of affective prosody:
et al. (2007). Here I am: The cortical correlates of visual self-recognition. A dynamic causal modeling study. NeuroImage, 30, 580–587.
Brain Research, 1143, 169–182. Etkin, A., Enger, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., & Hirsch, J. (2006).
DeWall, C. N., MacDonald, G., Webster, G. D., Masten, C., Baumeister, R. F., Resolving emotional conflict: A role for the rostral anterior cingu-
Powell, C., et al. (in press). Tylenol reduces social pain: Behavioral and lated cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron, 51,
neural evidence. Psychological Science. 871–882.
DeWall, C. N., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Alone but feeling no pain: Evers, E. A., Cools, R., Clark, L., van der Veen, F. M., Jolles, J.,
Effects of social exclusion on physical pain tolerance and pain thresh- Sahakian, B. J., et al. (2005). Serotonergic modulation of prefrontal
old, affective forecasting, and interpersonal empathy. Journal of cortex during negative feedback in probabilistic reversal learning.
Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1–15. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 1138–1147.
di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D, Fatima, Z.,
(1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiology study. et al. (2007). Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation reveals
Experimental Brain Research, 91,176–80. distinct neural modes of self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affective
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior express- Neuroscience, 2, 313–322.
way. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 1–40. Farrer, C., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Frith, C. D., Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M.
Downing, P. E., Yuhong, J., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical (2003). Modulating the experience of agency: A positron emission
area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science, 293, tomography study. NeuroImage, 18, 324–333.
2470–2473. Farrer, C., Frey, S. H., Van Horn, J. D., Tunik, E., Turk, D., Inati, S., et al.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary (2008). The angular gyrus computes action awareness representations.
Anthropology, 6, 178–190. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 254–261.
Ehrsson, H. H., Weich, K., Weiskopf, N., Dolan, R. J., & Passingham, R. E. Farrow, T. F., Zheng, Y., Wilkinson, I. D., Spence, S. A., Deakin, J. F.,
(2007). Threatening a rubber hand that you feel is yours elicits a cortical Tarrier, N., et al. (2001). Investigating the functional anatomy of empa-
anxiety response. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, thy and forgiveness. NeuroReport, 12, 2433–2438.
104, 9828–9833. Fazio, R. H., & Williams, C. J. (1986). Attitude accessibility as a moderator
Eippert, F., Veit, R., Weiskopf, N., Erb, M., Birbaumer, N., & Anders, S. of the attitude-perception and attitude-behavior relations: An investi-
(2007). Regulation of emotional responses elicited by threat-related gation of the 1984 presidential election. Journal of Personality and
stimuli. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 409–423. Social Psychology, 51, 505–514.

CH05.indd 183 12/22/09 5:33:55 PM


184 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). Willed
cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114, 817–868. action and the prefrontal cortex in man: A study with PET. Proceedings
Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Is ACC necessary for cognitive con- of the Royal Society of London, 244, 241–246.
trol? Brain, 128, 788–796. Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of men-
Felmingham, K., Kemp, A., Williams, L., Das, P., Hughes, G., Peduto, A., talizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, B:
et al. (2007). Changes in anterior cingulate and amygdala after cogni- Biological Sciences, 358, 459–473.
tive behavior therapy of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Fukui, H., Murai, T., Shinozaki, J., Aso, T., Fukuyama, H., Hayashi, et al.
Science, 18, 127–129. (2006). The neural basis of social tactics: An fMRI study. NeuroImage,
32, 913–920.
Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press. Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of “theory of
mind.” Trends in Cognitive Psychology, 7, 77–83.
Finger, E. C., Marsh, A. A., Kamel, N., Mitchell, D. G. V., & Blair, J. R.
(2006). Caught in the act: The impact of audience on the neural Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., &
response to morally and socially inappropriate behavior. NeuroImage, Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An
33, 414–421. fMRI study of “theory of mind” in verbal and nonverbal tasks.
Neuropsychologia, 38, 11–21.
Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression forma-
tion, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of Gallagher, H. L., Jack, A. I., Roepstorff, A., & Frith, C. D. (2002).
information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances Imagining the intentional stance in a competitive game. NeuroImage,
in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 1–74. 16, 814–521.
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recog-
Fitzgerald, D. A., Angstadt, M., Jelsone, L. M., Nathan, P. J., & Luan Phan, K.
nition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.
(2006). Beyond threat: Amygdala reactivity across multiple expres-
sions of facial affect. NeuroImage, 30, 1441–1448. Gallese, V., Ferrari, P. F., & Umilità, M. A. (2002). The mirror matching
system: A shared manifold for intersubjectivity. Behavioral and Brain
Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J.,
Sciences, 25, 35–36.
Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A func-
tional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Action rep-
Cognition, 57, 109–128. resentation and the IPL. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common
mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance
Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Modulation of com- (Vol.19, pp. 247–266). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
peting memory systems by distraction. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 11778–11783. Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the
basis of social cognition. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
Foltz, E. L., & White, L. E. (1968). The role of rostral cingulotomy in 8, 396–403.
“pain” relief. International Journal of Neurology, 6, 353–373.
Gallup, G. G., Jr. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–87.
Forbes, C. E., Schmader, T., & Allen, J. J. B. (2008). The role of devalu-
Ganesan, V., Hunter, M. D., & Spence, S. A. (2005). Schneiderian first-
ing and discounting in performance monitoring: A neurophysiologi-
rank symptoms and right parietal hyperactivation: A replication using
cal study of minorities under threat. Social Cognitive and Affective
fMRI. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1545.
Neuroscience, 3, 253–261.
Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000).
Formisano, E., Linden, D. E. J., Di Salle, F., Trojano, L., Esposito, F., Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recog-
Sack, A. T., et al. (2002). Tracking the mind’s image in the brain I: Time- nition. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 191–197.
resolved fMRI during visuospatial mental imagery. Neuron, 35, 185–194.
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., & Keysers, C. (2007). The anthropomorphic
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S. J., Graham, S. J., Grady, C., Keightley, M. L., brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic
Craik, F., et al. (2003). In search of the emotional self: An fMRI study actions. NeuroImage, 35, 1674–1684.
using positive and negative emotional words. American Journal of
de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., & Hadjikhani, N. (2004).
Psychiatry, 160, 1938–1945.
Fear fosters flight: A mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S. J., Lepage, M., Graham, S. J., Grady, C., emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of the National
Keightley, M. L., et al. (2004). Distributed self in episodic memory: Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 16701–16706.
Neural correlates of successful retrieval of self-encoded positive and
German, T. P., Niehaus, J. L., Roarty, M. P., Giesbrecht, B., & Miller, M. B.
negative personality traits. NeuroImage, 22, 1596–1604.
(2004). Neural correlates of detecting pretense: Automatic engagement
Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by of the intentional stance under covert conditions. Journal of Cognitive
human infants. Science, 218, 486–487. Neuroscience, 16, 1805–1817.
Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., & Gilbert, D. T. (1999). What the mind’s not. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope
Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 3–11).
into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the New York: Guilford Press.
National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102, 9673–9678. Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias.
Franck, N., O’Leary, D. S., Flaum, M., Hichwa, R. D., & Andreasen, N. A. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38.
(2002). Cerebral blood flow changes associated with Schneiderian Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busy-
first-rank symptoms in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry ness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of
Clinical Neuroscience, 14, 277–282. Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733–740.
Fransson, P., Skiold, B., Horsch, S., Nordell, A., Blennow, M., Lagercrantz, H., Gilbert, S. J., Williamson, I. D. M., Dumontheil, I., Simons, J. S., Frith, C. D., &
et al. (2007). Resting-state networks in the infant brain. Proceedings Burgess, P. W. (2007). Distinct regions of medial rostral prefrontal cor-
of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 15531–15536. tex supporting social and nonsocial functions. Social Cognitive and
Fried, I., Katz, A., McCarthy, G., Sass, K. J., Williamson, P., Spencer, S. S., Affective Neuroscience, 2, 217–226.
et al. (1991). Functional organization of human supplementary motor Gilboa, A. 2004. Autobiographical and episodic memory—one and the
cortex studied by electrical stimulation. The Journal of Neuroscience, same? Evidence from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies.
11, 3656–3666. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1336–1349.

CH05.indd 184 12/22/09 5:33:56 PM


References 185

Gillath, O., Bunge, S. A., Shaver, P. R., Wendelken, C., & Mikulincer, M. Gutchess, A. H., Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Aging,
(2005). Attachment-style differences in the ability to suppress negative self-referencing, and medial prefrontal cortex. Social Neuroscience, 2,
thoughts: Exploring the neural correlates. NeuroImage, 28, 835–847. 117–133.
Gobbini, M. I., Koralek, A. C., Bryan, R. E., Montgomery, K. J., & Haxby, J. V. Hamilton, A. F. de C., Wolpert, D. M., Frith, U., & Grafton, S. T. (2006).
(2007). Two takes on the social brain: A comparison of theory of mind Where does your own action influence your perception of another per-
tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1803–1814. son’s action in the brain? NeuroImage, 29, 524–535.
Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Explaining modulation of reasoning by Hamilton, D. L., Katz, L. B., & Leirer, V. O. (1980). Cognitive represen-
belief. Cognition, 87, B11–B22. tations of personality impressions: Organizational processes in first
Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Chiao, J. Y., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2001). impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other- 39, 1050–1063.
race faces. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 845–850. Hamm, A., Weike, A. I., Schupp, H. T., Treig, T., Dressel, A., & Kessler, C.
Goldberg, I. I., Harel, M., & Malach, R. (2006). When the brain loses its (2003). Affective blindsight: Intact fear conditioning to a visual cue in
self: Prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron, a cortically blind patient. Brain, 126, 267–275.
50, 329–339. Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Hoffman, J. M., & Kilts, C. D. (2002). Ecstasy
Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The neural and agony: Activation of the human amygdala in positive and negative
bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Psychological Science, 13, 135–141.
emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 577–586. Han, S., Jiang, Y., Humphreys, G. W., Zhou, T., & Cai, P. (2005). Distinct
Goldman, A. I. (1989). Interpretation psychologized. Mind and Language, neural substrates for the perception of real and virtual visual worlds.
4, 161–185. NeuroImage, 24, 928–935.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of sim- Happé, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C.,
ple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503. et al. (1996). “Theory of mind” in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan
Gopnik, A. (2007). Cells that read minds? What the myth of mirror neurons study of Asperger syndrome. NeuroReport, 8, 197–201.
get wrong about the human brain. Slate. www.slate.com/id/2165123. Harenski, C. L., & Hamann, S. (2006). Neural correlates of regulating neg-
Retrieved September 4, 2009. ative emotions related to moral violations. NeuroImage, 30, 313–324.
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000). Modulating
(2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judg- emotional responses: Effects of a neurotical network on the limbic sys-
ment. Science, 293, 2105–2108. tem. NeuroReport, 11, 43–48.
Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of Harmon-Jones, E., Gerdjikov, T., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2006). The effect of
personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618. induced compliance on relative left frontal cortical activity: A test
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring of the action-based model of dissonance. European Journal of Social
individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association Psychology, 36, 1–11.
test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. Harmon-Jones, E., Harmon-Jones, C., Fearn, M., Sigelman, J. D., &
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional Johnson, P. (2008). Left frontal cortical activation and spreading of
connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default alternatives: Tests of the action-based model of dissonance. Journal
mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 1–15.
100, 252–258. Harris, L. T., Todorov, A., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Attributions on the brain:
Grèzes, J., Berthoz, S., & Passingham, R. E. (2006). Amygdala activation Neuro-imaging dispositional inferences, beyond theory of mind.
when one is the target of deceit: Did he lie to you or to someone else? NeuroImage, 28, 763–769.
NeuroImage, 30, 601–608. Haxby, J. V., Gobbini, M. I., Furey, M. L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J. L., &
Grèzes, J., Fonlupt, P., Bertenthal, B., Delon-Martin, C., Segebarth, C., & Pietrini, P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces
Decety, J. (2001). Does perception of biological motion rely on specific and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science, 293, 2425–2430.
brain regions? NeuroImage, 13, 775–785. Hashimoto, Y., & Sakai, K. L. (2003). Brain activations during conscious
Grèzes, J., Frith, C. D., & Passingham, R. E. (2004). Inferring false beliefs self-monitoring of speech production with delayed auditory feedback:
from the actions of oneself and others: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 20, 22–28.
21, 744–750. Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L., Macrae, C. N., Demos, K. E., Denny, B. T., &
Grèzes, J., Pichon, S., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Perceiving fear in dynamic Kelley, W. M. (2006). Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self
body expressions. NeuroImage, 35, 959–967. from close others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1,
Grill-Spector, K., Knouf, N., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). The fusiform face 18–25.
area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identifica- Heberlein, A. S., & Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2004). Cortical
tion. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 555–562. regions for judgments of emotions and personality traits from point-
Grossman, E. D., & Blake, R. (2002). Brain areas active during visual per- light walkers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1143–1158.
ception of biological motion. Neuron, 35, 1167–1175. Heberlein, A. S., Padon, A. A., Gillihan, S. J., Farah, M. J., & Fellows, L. K.
Grossman, E. D., Blake, R., & Kim, C. Y. (2004). Learning to see bio- (2008). Ventromedial frontal lobe plays a critical role in facial emotion
logical motion: Brain activity parallels behavior. Journal of Cognitive recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 721–733.
Neuroscience, 16, 1669–1679. Heberlein, A. S., & Saxe, R. R. (2005). Dissociation between emotion and
Gundel, H., O’Connor, M. F., Littrell, L., Fort, C., & Lane, R. D. (2003). personality judgments: Convergent evidence from functional neuroim-
Functional neuroanatomy of grief: An FMRI study. American Journal aging. NeuroImage, 28, 770–777.
of Psychiatry, 160, 1946–1953. Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.
Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2003).
to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. European Journal of
Academy of Sciences, USA, 98, 4259–4264. Neuroscience, 17, 1123–1128.

CH05.indd 185 12/22/09 5:33:57 PM


186 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Hennenlotter, A., Dresel, C., Castrop, F., Castrop Baumann, A. O., Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and
Wohlschläger, A. M., & Haslinger, B. (2009). The link between facial negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage, 34, 1744–1753.
feedback and neural activity within central circuitries of emotion-new Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2006). Empathy
insights from botulinum toxin-induced denervation of frown muscles. examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how
Cerebral Cortex, 19, 537–542 . I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia, 44, 752–761.
Hennenlotter, A., Schroeder, U., Erhard, P., Castrop, F., Haslinger, B., Jacobsen, L. K., Slotkin, T. A., Westerveld, M., Mencl, W. E., & Pugh., K. R.
Stoecker, D., et al. (2005). A common neural basis for receptive and (2006). Visuospatial memory deficits emerging during nicotine withdrawal
expressive communication of pleasant facial affect. NeuroImage, 26, in adolescents with prenatal exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31,
581–591. 1550–1561.
Herwig, U., Baumgartner, T., Kaffenberger, T., Bruhl, A., Kottlow, M., Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model
Schreiter-Gasser, U., et al. (2007). Modulation of anticipatory emo- for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.
tion and perception processing by cognitive control. NeuroImage, 37,
Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Touryan, S. R., Greene, E. J., &
652–662.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2006). Dissociating medial frontal and poste-
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a rior cingulated activity during self-reflection. Social Affective and
motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 56–64.
30, 1–46.
Johnson, S. C., Baxter, L. C., Wilder, L. S., Pipe, J. G., Heiserman, J. E., &
Hodzic, A., Muckli, L., Singer, W., & Stirn, A. (2009). Cortical responses Prigatano, G. P. (2002). Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain, 125,
to self and others. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 951–962. 1808–1814.
Hoffman, E. A., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze Johnson, S. C., Schmitz, T. W., Kawahara-Baccus, T. N., Rowley, H. A.,
and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Alexander, A. L., Lee, J., et al. (2005). The cerebral response during
Nature Neuroscience, 3, 80–84. subjective choice with and without self-reference. Journal of Cognitive
Hooker, C. I., Paller, K. A., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., Mesulam, M. M., & Neuroscience, 17, 1897–1906.
Reber, P. J. (2003). Brain networks for analyzing eye gaze. Cognitive
Johnson-Frey, S. H., Maloof, F. R., Newman-Norlund, R., Farrer, C., Inati, S., &
Brain Research, 17, 406–418.
Grafton, S. T. (2003). Actions or hand-object interactions? Human
Hovland, C. I., & Rosenberg, M. J. (1960). Summary and further theo- inferior frontal cortex and action observation. Neuron, 39, 1053–1058.
retical issues. In M. J. Rosenberg, C. I. Hovland, W. J. McGuire, R.
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of
P. Abelson, & J. W. Brehm (Eds.), Attitude organization and change:
Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24.
An analysis of consistency among attitude components (pp. 198–235).
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Critchley, H. D., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P.,
Oakley, D. A., et al. (2005). Anxiety reduction through detachment:
Hsu, M., Anen, C., & Quartz, S. R. (2008). The right and the good:
Subjective, physiological, and neural effects. Journal of Cognitive
Distributive justice and neural encoding of equity and efficiency.
Neuroscience, 17, 874–883.
Science. 320, 1092–1095.
Kalisch, R., Wiech, W., Herrmann, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Neural cor-
Hunter, M. D., Farrow, T. F. D., Papadakis, N. G., Wilkinson, I. D.,
relates of self-distraction from anxiety and a process model of cog-
Woodruff, P. W. R., & Spence, S. A. (2003). Approaching an eco-
nitive emotion regulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18,
logically valid functional anatomy of spontaneous “willed” action.
1266–1276.
NeuroImage, 20, 1264–1269.
Kampe, K. K. W., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John:” Signals
Hynes, C. A., Baird, A. A., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Differential role of the
conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain
orbital frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking.
regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. The
Neuropsychologia, 44, 374–383.
Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5258–5263.
Iacoboni, M., Lieberman, M. D., Knowlton, B. J., Molnar-Szakacs, I.,
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face
Moritz, M., Throop, C. J., & Fiske, A. P. (2004). Watching social inter-
area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face per-
actions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD
ception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–4311.
fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline. NeuroImage, 21,
1167–1173. Kaplan, J. T., Aziz-Zadeh, L., Uddin, L. Q., & Iacoboni, M. (2008). The
self across the senses: An fMRI study of self-face and self-voice recog-
Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., &
nition. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3, 218–223.
Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own
mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3, e79. Kaplan, J. T., Freedman, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2007). Us versus them:
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Political attitudes and party affiliation influence neural responses to
Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. faces of presidential candidates. Neuropsychologia, 45, 55–64.
Science, 286, 2526–2528. Kaplan, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror
Insel, T. R., & Shapiro, L. E. (1992). Oxytocin receptors and mater- neurons, intention, understanding, and cognitive empathy. Social
nal behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 652, Neuroscience, 1, 175–183.
122–141. Kawabata, H., & Zeki, S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty. Journal of
Insel, T. R., & Winslow, J. T. (1998). Serotonin and neuropeptides in affili- Neurophysiology, 91, 1699–1705.
ative behaviors. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 207–219. Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American
Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain: Psychologist, 28, 107–128.
Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of mul- Kelley, W. M. C., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., &
tiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI
Psychology, 85, 616–626. study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 785–794.
Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2005). The influence of processing objectives Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J.-L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V.
on the perception of faces: An ERP study of race and gender percep- (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and
tion. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 21–36. experience of touch. Neuron, 42, 335–346.
Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., & Sadato, N. (2008). Processing of social and Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K, & Glaser, R. (1989). Psychoneuroimmunology: Past,
monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron, 58, 284–295. present, and future. Health Psychology, 8, 677–682.

CH05.indd 186 12/22/09 5:33:57 PM


References 187

Kihlstrom, J. F. (2006). Does neuroscience constrain social-psychological Kubota, J. T., & Ito, T. A. (2009). You were always on my mind: How
theory? Dialogue, 21, 16–17. event-related potentials inform impression formation research.
Kim, S. H., & Hamann, S. (2007). Neural correlates of positive and nega- In T. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimi-
tive emotion regulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, nation (pp. 333–346). New York: Psychology Press.
776–798. Lambon Ralph, M. A., Pobric, G., & Jefferies, E. (2009). Conceptual
King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R., & knowledge is underpinned by the temporal pole bilaterally: Convergent
Montague, P. R. (2005). Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a evidence from rTMS. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 832–838.
two-person economic exchange. Science, 308, 78–83. Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrates of
Kircher, T. T. J., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E., Simmons, A., Bartels, M., & human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal.
David, A. S. (2002). The neural correlates of intentional and incidental Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 42–58.
self processing. Neuropsychologia, 40, 683–692. Lamm, C., Nusbaum, H. C., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2007). What are
Kjaer, T. W., Nowak, M., & Lou, H. C. (2002). Reflective self-awareness you feeling? Using functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess
and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofron- the modulation of sensory and affective responses during empathy for
tal core. NeuroImage, 17, 1080–1086. pain. PLoS ONE, 2, e1292.
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Self-Knowledge of an Lane, R. D., Fink, G. R., Chau, P. M.-L., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Neural
amnesic patient: Toward a neuropsychology of personality and social activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses.
psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 125, 250–260. NeuroReport, 8, 3969–3972.
Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., & Fehr, E. (2006). Lau, H. C., Rogers, R. D., Haggard, P., & Passingham, R. E. (2004a).
Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal Attention to intention. Science, 303, 1208–1210.
cortex. Science, 314, 829–832. Lau, H. C., Rogers, R. D., Ramnani, N., & Passingham, R. E. (2004b).
Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit Willed action and attention to the selection of action. NeuroImage, 21,
learning system in humans. Science, 273, 1399–1402. 1407–1415.
Koenigs, M., & Tranel, D. (2007). Irrational economic decision-making Lauro, L. J. R., Tettamanti, M., Cappa, S. F., & Papagano, C. (2008). Idiom
after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the ultimatum comprehension: A prefrontal task? Cerebral Cortex, 18, 162–170.
game. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 951–956. Lazar, N. A. (2008). The statistical analysis of functional MRI data.
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & New York: Springer.
Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action LeDoux, J. E., Iwata, J., Cicchetti, P., & Reis, D. J. (1988). Different projec-
representations in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848. tions of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral
Kong, J., Gollub, R. L., Rosman, I. S., Webb, J. M., Vangel, M. G., Kirsch, I., correlates of conditioned fear. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2517–2529.
et al. (2006). Brain activity associated with expectancy-enhanced pla- Lee, T.-W., Dolan, R. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2008). Controlling emotional
cebo analgesia as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. expression: Behavioral and neural correlates of nonimitative emo-
The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 381–388. tional responses. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 104–113.
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Lee, T.-W., Josephs, O., Dolan, R. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2006). Imitating
Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435, 673–676. expressions: Emotion-specific substrates in facial mimicry. Social
Kosslyn, S. M. (1999). If neuroimaging is the answer, what is the question? Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 122–135.
Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 354, 1283–1294.
Leibenluft, E., Gobbini, M. I., Harrison, T., & Haxby, J. V. (2004). Mothers’
Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Costantini-Ferrando, M. F., neural activation in response to pictures of their children and other chil-
Alpert, N. M., & Spiegel, D. (2000). Hypnotic visual illusion alters dren. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 225–232.
color processing in the brain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157,
Lemche, E., Giampietro, V. P., Surguladze, S. A., Amaro, E. J., Andrew, C. M.,
1279–1284.
et al. (2006). Human attachment security is mediated by the amyg-
Krendl, A. C., Macrae, C. N., Kelley, W. M., Fugelsang, J. A., & dala: Evidence from combined fMRI and physophysiological measures.
Heatherton, T. F. (2006). The good, the bad, and the ugly: An fMRI Human Brain Mapping, 27, 623–635.
investigation of the functional anatomic correlates of stigma. Social
Leslie, K. R., Johnson-Frey, S. H., & Grafton, S. T. (2004). Functional
Neuroscience, 1, 5–15.
imaging of face and hand imitation: Toward a motor theory of empathy.
Krendl, A. C., Richeson, J. A., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2008). NeuroImage, 21, 601–607.
The negative consequences of threat: A functional magnetic resonance
Leube, D. T., Knoblich, G., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Bartels, M., & Kircher, T. T. J.
imaging investigation of the neural mechanisms underlying women’s
(2003). The neural correlates of perceiving one’s own movements.
underperformance in math. Psychological Science, 19, 168–175.
NeuroImage, 20, 2084–2090.
Krolak-Salmon, P., Hénaff, M. A., Vighetto, A., Bertrand, O., & Mauguière, F.
Levesque, J., Eugene, F., Joanette, Y., Paquette, V., Mensour, B., et al.
(2004). Early amygdala reaction to fear spreading in occipital, tempo-
(2003). Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness.
ral, and frontal cortex: A depth electrode ERP study in human. Neuron,
Biological Psychiatry, 53, 502–510.
42, 665–676.
Li, C.-S. R., Yan, P., Bergquist, K. L., & Sinha, R. (2007). Greater activation
Kross, E., Egner, T., Ochsner, K., Hirsch, J., & Downey, G. (2007). Neural
of the “default” brain regions predicts stop signal errors. NeuroImage,
dynamics of rejection sensitivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
38, 640–648.
19, 945–956.
Krueger, F., McCabe, K., Moll, J., Kriegeskorte, N., Zahn, A., & Grafman, J. Libby, L. K., Eibach, R. P., & Gilovich, T. (2005) Here’s looking at me:
(2007). Neural correlates of trust. Proceedings of the National The effect of memory perspective on assessments of personal change.
Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 20084–20089. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 50–62.

Kruglanski, A. W., Chun, W. Y., Erb, H. P. Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & Libet, B., Wright, E. W., & Gleason, C. A. (1982). Readiness-potentials
Spiegel, S. (2003). A parametric unimodel of human judgment. In proceeding unrestricted “spontaneous” vs. pre-planned voluntary acts.
J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & W. von Hippel (Eds.) Social judgments: Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 54, 322–335.
Implicit and explicit processes (pp. 137–161). New York: Cambridge Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience
University Press. approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109–137.

CH05.indd 187 12/22/09 5:33:58 PM


188 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt?
processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289. The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological
Lieberman, M. D. (in press) Why symbolic processing of affect can Bulletin, 131, 202–223.
disrupt negative affect: Social cognitive and affective neurosci- Machado, C. J., Snyder, A. Z., Cherry, S. R., Lavenex, P., & Amaral, D. G.
ence investigations. In A. Todorov, S. T. Fiske, & D. Prentice (Eds.), (2008). Effects of neonatal amygdala or hippocampus lesions on rest-
Social neuroscience: Toward understanding the underpinnings of ing brain metabolism in the macaque monkey: A microPET imaging
the social mind. New York: Oxford University Press. study. NeuroImage, 39, 832–846.
Lieberman, M. D. (2009a). What makes big ideas sticky. In M. Brockman MacLean P. D., & Newman, J. D. (1988). Role of midline frontolimbic
(Ed.), What’s next: Dispatches on the future of science (pp. 90–103). cortex in production of the isolation call of squirrel monkeys. Brain
New York: Vintage Books. Research, 45, 111–123.
Lieberman, M. D. (2009b). What zombies can’t do: A social cogni- Macrae, C. N., Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., Banfield, J. F., & Kelley, W. M.
tive approach to the irreducibility of reflective consciousness. In (2004). Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cerebral
J. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual process and beyond Cortex, 14, 647–654.
(pp. 293–316). New York: Oxford University Press. Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological
Lieberman, M. D., Berkman, E. T., & Wager, T. D. (2009). Correlations in Review, 50, 370–396.
social neuroscience aren’t voodoo: A reply to Vul et al. Perspectives Mason, M. F., Banfield, J. F., Macrae, C. N. (2004). Thinking about
on Psychological Science, 4, 299–307. actions: The neural substrates of person knowledge. Cerebral Cortex,
Lieberman, M. D., Chang, G. Y., Chiao, J., Bookheimer, S. Y., & 14, 209–214.
Knowlton, B. J. (2004). An event-related fMRI study of artificial gram- Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., &
mar learning in a balanced chunk strength design. Journal of Cognitive Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and
Neuroscience, 16, 427–438. stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393–395.
Lieberman, M. D., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Pains and pleasures of Masten, C. L., Telzer, E. H., & Eisenberger, N. I. (under review). Being the
social life. Science, 323, 890–891. target of racial discrimination: An fMRI study.
Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S. M., McCabe, K., Houser, D., Ryan, L., Smith, V., & Trouard, T. (2001).
Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007). Putting feelings into words: Affect A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal
labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. exchange. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,
Psychological Science, 18, 421–428. 98, 11832–11835.
Lieberman, M. D., Hariri, A., Jarcho, J. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., & Allison, T. (1997). Face-specific
Bookheimer, S. Y. (2005). An fMRI investigation of race-related amyg- processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive
dala activity in African-American and Caucasian-American individuals. Neuroscience, 9, 605–610.
Nature Neuroscience, 8, 720–722. McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., &
Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M., Berman, S. B., Naliboff, B. D., Montague, P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for
Suyenobu, B. Y., Mandelkern, M., et al. (2004). The neural correlates culturally familiar drinks. Neuron, 44, 379–387.
of placebo effects: A disruption account. NeuroImage, 22, 447–455. McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder, J. R.
Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M., & Satpute, A. B. (2004). Evidence- (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced
based and intuition-based self-knowledge: An fMRI study. Journal of deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive
Social and Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 421–435. Neuroscience, 15, 394–408.
Lieberman, M. D., Ochsner, K. N., Gilbert, D. T., & Schacter, D. L. McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Gross, J. J. (2008).
(2001). Do amnesiacs exhibit cognitive dissonance reduction? The role Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive
of explicit memory and attention in attitude change. Psychological reappraisal. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 11, 143–162.
Science, 12, 135–140. McRae, K., Reiman, E. M., Fort, C. L., Chen, K., & Lane, R. D. (2008).
Liljeström, M., Tarkiainen, A., Parviainen, T., Kujala, J., Numminen, J., Asssociation between trait emotional awareness and dorsal anterior
Hiltunenm, J., et al. (2008). Perceiving and naming actions and objects. cingulate activity during emotion is arousal-dependent. NeuroImage,
NeuroImage, 41, 1132–1141. 41, 648–655.
Liu, F., Buccino, G., Duzzi, D., Benuzzi, F., Crisi, G., Baraldi, P., et al. Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society (C. W. Morris, Ed.). Chicago:
(2008). Neural substrates for observing and imagining non-object- University of Chicago Press.
directed actions. Social Neuroscience, 3, 261–275. Mendez, M. F., Anderson, E., & Shapira, J. S. (2005). An investigation
Lorberbaum, J. P., Newman, J. D., Horwitz, A. R., Dubno, J. R., of moral judgement in frontotemporal dementia. Cognitive Behavioral
Lydiard, R. B., et al. (2002). A potential role for thalamocingulate cir- Neurology, 18, 193–197.
cuitry in human maternal behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 431–445. Milne, E., & Grafman, J. (2001). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions
Lotze, M., Heymans, U., Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Erb, M., Flor, H., et al. in humans eliminate implicit gender stereotyping. Journal of
(2006). Differential cerebral activation during observation of expres- Neuroscience, 21, 1–6.
sive gestures and motor acts. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1787–1795. Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Matsuoka, S., Dan, I., Naoi, N., Nakamura, K., &
Lou, H. C., Luber, B., Crupainm, M., Keenan, J. P., Nowak, M., Kjare, T. W., Kojima, S. (2008). Prefrontal activation associated with social attach-
et al. (2004). Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. ment: Facial-emotion recognition in mothers and infants. Cerebral
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, Cortex, 19, 284–292.
6827–6832. Mitchell, J. P. (2008). Activity in the right temporo-parietal junction is not
Luan Phan, K., Fitzgerald, D. A., Nathan, P. J., Moore, G. L., Uhde, T. W., & selective for theory of mind. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 262–271.
Tancer, M. E. (2005). Neural substrates for voluntary suppression Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2005a). General and spe-
of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. cific contributions of the medial prefrontal cortex to knowledge about
Biological Psychiatry, 57, 210–219. mental states. NeuroImage, 28, 757–762.
MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2005b). The link between
Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingu- social cognition and self-referential thought in the medial prefrontal
lated cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–1838. cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1306–1315.

CH05.indd 188 12/22/09 5:33:59 PM


References 189

Mitchell, J. P., Cloutier, J., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2006). Medial Motluck, A. (2001). Read my mind. New Scientist, 169, 22.
prefrontal dissociations during processing of trait diagnostic and Newmann-Norlund, R. D., van Schie, H. T., van Zuijilen, M. J., &
nondiagnostic person information. Social Cognitive and Affective Bekkering, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system is more active during
Neuroscience, 1, 49–55. complementary compared with imitative action. Nature Neuroscience,
Mitchell, J. P., Heatherton, T. F., Kelley, W. M., Wyland, C. L., 10, 817–818.
Wegner, D. M., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Separating sustained from Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and short-
transient aspects of cognitive control during thought suppression. comings. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Psychological Science, 18, 292–297.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Encoding-specific reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.
effects of social cognition on the neural correlates of subsequent mem- Noguchi, Y., Kancoke, Y., Kakigi, R., Tanabe, H. C., & Sadato, N. (2005).
ory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 4912–4917. Role of the superior temporal region in human visual motion percep-
Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Dissociable medial tion. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1592–1601.
prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Nomura, M., Ohira, H., Haneda, K., Iidaka, T., Sadato, N., Okada, T.,
Neuron, 50, 655–663. et al. (2004). Functional association of the amygdala and ventral pre-
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2002). frontal cortex during cognitive evaluation of facial expressions primed
Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judg- by masked angry faces: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 21,
ments. NeuroImage, 16, 696–703. 352–363.
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., Mourao- Noppeney, U., & Price, C. J. (2004). Retrieval of abstract semantics.
Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., et al. (2002). The neural correlates of NeuroImage, 22, 164–170.
moral sensitivity: A functional magnetic resonance imaging inves- Noriuchi, M., Kikuchi, Y., & Senoo, A. (2008). The functional neuroanatomy
tigation of basic and moral emotions. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, of maternal love: Mother ’s response to infant’s attachment behaviors.
2730–2736. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 415–423.
Moll, J., Krueger, F., Zahn, R., Pardini, M., de Oliveira-Souza, R., & Ochsner, K. N. (2004). Current directions in social cognitive psychology.
Grafman, J. (2006). Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide deci- Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 254–258.
sion about charitable donation. Proceedings of the National Academy
Ochsner, K. N. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: Historical devel-
of Sciences, USA, 103, 15623–15628.
opment, core principles, and future promise. In Kruglanksi, A., &
Molnar-Szakacs, I., Kaplan, J., Greenfield, P. M., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Social Psychology: A Handbook of Basic
Observing complex action sequences: The role of the fronto-parietal Principles (2nd ed., pp. 39–66). New York: Guilford Press.
mirror neuron system. NeuroImage, 33, 923–935. Ochsner, K. N., Beer, J. S., Robertson, E. R., Cooper, J. C., Gabrieli, J. D. E.,
Montgomery, K. J., Isenberg, N., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Communicative Kihstrom, J. F. (2005). The neural correlates of direct and reflected
hand gestures and object-directed hand movements activated the mirror self-knowledge. NeuroImage, 28, 797–814.
neuron systems. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002).
114–122. Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emo-
Moran, J. M., Macrae, S. N., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L., & tion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1215–1229.
Kelley, W. M. (2006). Neuroanaotomical evidence for distinct cognitive Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion.
and affective components of self. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242–249.
18, 1586–1594.
Ochsner, K. N., Hughes, B. L., Robertson, E., Cooper, J. C., & Gabrieli, J.
Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Lane, R. D., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., (2009). Neural systems supporting the control of cognitive and affec-
et al. (2006). Impaired self-awareness and theory of mind: An fMRI tive conflict. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1842–1855.
study of mentalizing in alexithymia. NeuroImage, 32, 1472–1482.
Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D. H., Haneline, J., Ramachandran, T.,
Morita, T., Itakura, S., Saito, D. N., Nakashita, S., Harada, T., Kochiyama, T., Glover, G., et al. (2004). Reflecting upon feelings: Systems support-
et al. (2008). The role of the right prefrontal cortex in self-evaluation ing the attribution of emotion to self and other. Journal of Cognitive
of the face: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Social Neuroscience, 16, 1746–1772.
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 20, 342–355.
Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of social cog-
Morris, J. P., Pelphrey, K. A., & McCarthy, G. (2006). Occipitotemporal nitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56, 717–734.
activation evoked by the perception of human bodies is modu-
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S.,
lated by the presence or absence of the face. Neuropsychologia, 44,
Gabrieli, J. D. E., et al. (2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems
1919–1927.
supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion.
Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W., NeuroImage, 23, 483–499.
Calder, A. J., et al. (1996). A differential neural response in the human Ochshner, K. N., & Schacter, D. L. (2000). A social cognitive neuroscience
amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383, 812–815. approach to emotion and memory. In J. C. Borod (Ed.), The neuro-
Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious psychology of emotion (pp. 163–193). New York: Oxford University
emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393, 467–470. Press.
Morrison, I., Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2007). The sight of others’ Ochsner, K. N., Zaki, J., Hanelin, J., Ludlow, D. H., Knierim, K.,
pain modulates motor processing in human cingulate cortex. Cerebral Ramachandran, T., et al. (2008). Your pain or mine? Common and
Cortex, 17, 2214–2222. distinct neural systems supporting the perception of pain in self
Mosconi, M. W., Mack, P. B., McCarthy, G., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2005). and other. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3, 144–160.
Taking an “intentional stance” on eye-gaze shifts: A functional neu- O’Connor, M.-F., Wellisch, D. K., Stanton, A. L., Eisenberger, N. I.,
roimaging study of social perception in children. NeuroImage, 27, Irwin, M. R., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). Craving love? Enduring
247–252. grief activates brain’s reward center. NeuroImage, 42, 969–972.
Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Johnson, M. R., & Kiehl, K. A. (2006). O’Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M., &
Attentional modulation of the amygdala varies with personality. Dolan, R. J. (2003). Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal
NeuroImage, 31, 934–944. cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41, 147–155.

CH05.indd 189 12/22/09 5:33:59 PM


190 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Ohira, H., Nomura, M., Ichikawa, N., Isowa, T., Iidaka, T., Sato, A., et al. Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S.,
(2006). Associations of neural and physiological responses during vol- Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., et al. (2000). Performance on indirect
untary emotion suppression. NeuroImage, 29, 721–733. measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of
Ohnishi, T., Moriguchi, Y., Matsuda, H., Mori, T., Hirakata, M., Imabayashi, E., Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738.
et al. (2004). The neural network for the mirror system and mental- Pierno, A. C., Tubaldi, F., Turella, L., Grossi, P., Barachino, L., Gallo, P.,
izing in normally developed children: An fMRI study. NeuroReport, et al. (2009). Neurofunctional modulation of brain regions by the
15, 1482–1487. observation of pointing and grasping actions. Cerebral Cortex, 19,
367–374.
Ortigue, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Hamilton, A. F. de C., & Grafton, S. T.
(2007). The neural basis of love as a subliminal prime: An event-related Platek, S. M., Keenan, J. P., Gallup, G. G., Jr., & Mohamed, F. (2004).
functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. Cognitive
Neuroscience, 19, 1218–1230. Brain Research, 19, 114–122.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human Platek, S. M., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., Busch, S., Ruparel, K., Phend, N.,
and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press. et al. (2006). Neural substrates for functionally discriminating self-face
from personally familiar faces. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 91–98.
Panksepp, J., Herman, B., Conner, R., Bishop, P., & Scott, J. P. (1978). The
Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroim-
biology of social attachments: Opiates alleviate separation distress.
aging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 59–63.
Biological Psychiatry, 13, 607–618.
Poldrack, R. A., Clark, J., Pare-Blagoev, E. J., Shohamy, D., Creso Moyano, J.,
Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T. (2004). Subcortical discrimi- Myers, C., et al. (2001). Interactive memory systems in the human
nation of unperceived objects during binocular rivalry. Neuron, 42, brain. Nature, 414, 546–550.
163–172.
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and and brain. Neuroscience, 139, 23–38.
controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory
and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192.
of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
Peelen, M. P., Atkinson, A. P., Andersson, F., & Vuilleumier, P. (2007). Preston, A. D., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Stansfield, R. B.,
Emotional modulation of body-selective visual areas. Social Cognitive Mehta, S., et al. (2007). The neural substrates of cognitive empathy.
and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 274–283. Social Neuroscience, 2, 254–275.
Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Neural basis of eye Preston, C., & Newport, R. (2008). Misattribution of movement agency fol-
gaze processing deficits in autism. Brain, 128, 1038–1048. lowing right parietal TMS. Social Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 26–32.
Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., McCarthy, G., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Price, D. D. (2000). Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective
Perception of dynamic changes in facial affect and identity in autism. dimension of pain. Science, 288, 1769–1772.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 140–149. de Quervain, D. J.-F., Fischbacher, U., Treyer, V., Schellhammer, M.,
Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2003). Schnyder, U., Buck, A., et al. (2004). The neural basis of altruistic
Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: The influence of punishment. Science, 305, 1254–1258.
context. Neuropsychologia, 41, 156–170. Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J.,
Pelphrey, K. A., Viola, R. J., & McCarthy, G. (2004). When strangers pass: Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain func-
Processing of mutual and averted social gaze in the superior temporal tion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 676–682.
sulcus. Psychological Science, 15, 598–603. Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, B., & Bushnell, M. C.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: (1998). Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulated but not
Toward an understanding of inhibition of disease. Journal of Abnormal somatosensory cortex. Science, 277, 968–971.
Psychology, 95, 274–281. Ranklin, K. P., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Allison, S. C., Stanley, C. M., Glenn, S.,
Perner, J., & Aichhorn, M. (2006). Thinking of mental and other repre- et al. (2006). Structural anatomy of empathy in neurodegenerative dis-
sentations: The role of left and right temporo-parietal junction. Social ease. Brain, 129, 2945–2956.
Neuroscience, 1, 245–258. Ranote, S., Elliott, R., Abel, K. M., Mitchell, R., Deakin, J. F. W., &
Appleby, L. (2004). The neural basis of maternal responsiveness to
Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002).
infants: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 15, 1825–1829.
Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99, 11458–11463. Richeson, J. A., Baird, A. A., Gordon, H. L., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L.,
Trawalter, S., et al. (2003). An fMRI investigation of the impact of
Petrovic, P., Dietrich, T., Fransson, P., Andersson, J., Carlsson, K., & Igvar, M. interracial contact on executive function. Nature Neuroscience, 16,
(2005). Placebo in emotion processing—induced expectations of 1323–1328.
anxiety relief activate a generalized modulatory network. Neuron, 46,
Richeson, J. A., Todd, A. R., & Trawalter, S. (2008). Eye-gaze direc-
957–969.
tion modulates race-relate amygdala activity. Group Processes and
Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2008). Intergroup Relations, 11, 233–246.
Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal com-
Rilling, J. K., Dagenais, J. E., Goldsmith, D. R., Glenn, A. L., & Pagnoni, G.
petence in children. NeuroImage, 39, 2076–2085.
(2008). Social cognitive neural networks during in-group and out-
Pfeifer, J. H., Lieberman, M. D., & Dapretto, M. (2007). “I know you are group interactions. NeuroImage, 41, 1447–1461.
but what am I?!”: Neural bases of self- and social knowledge in chil- Rilling, J. K., Goldsmith, D. R., Glenn, A. L., Jairam, M. R., Elfenbein, H. A.,
dren and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1323–1337. Dangenais, J. E., et al. (2008). The neural correlates of the affective
Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., Borofsky, L. A., Dapretto, M., response to unreciprocated cooperation. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1256–1266.
Lieberman, M. D., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009). Neural correlates of Rilling, J. K., Gutman, D. A., Zeh, T. R., Pagnoni, G., Berns, G. S., &
direct and reflect self-appraisals in adolescents and adults. Child Kilts, C. D. (2002). A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron, 35,
Development, 80, 1016–1038. 395–405.
Phelps, E. A., Cannistraci, C. J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2003). Intact per- Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. A.
formance on an indirect measure of race bias following amygdala dam- (2004). The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal
age. Neuropsychologia, 41, 203–208. interactions. NeuroImage, 22, 1694–1703.

CH05.indd 190 12/22/09 5:34:00 PM


References 191

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Schnyer, D. M., Nicholls, L., & Verfaellie, M. (2005). The role of VMPC in
Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. metamemorial judgments of content retrievability. Journal of Cognitive
Ronquillo, J., Denson, T. F., Lickel, B., Lu, Z.-L., Nandy, A., & Maddox, K. B. Neuroscience, 17, 832–846.
(2007). The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity: Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2007).
An fMRI investigation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-
2, 39–44. face interactions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging approach
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. to empathy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1354–1372.
(1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS Test. Schultz, J., Imamizu, H., Kawato, M., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Activation
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. of the human superior temporal gyrus during observation of goal attri-
Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An bution by intention objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16,
egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal 1695–1705.
of Experimental Psychology, 13, 279–301. Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., Whalen, P. J.,
Ross, M., & Sicoly, F. (1975). Egocentric biases in availability and attribu- McMullin, K. G., et al. (2003). Differential amygdalar response to
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 322–337. novel versus newly familiar neutral faces: A functional MRI probe
Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2003). What you believe versus what you think developed for studying inhibited temperament. Biological Psychiatry,
they believe: A neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking. 53, 854–862.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2475–2480. Scott, A. A., Dapretto, M., Ghahremani, D., Poldrack, R. A., &
Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2004). How would you feel versus how do you Bookheimer, S. Y. (under review). Social learning deficits relate to
think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with decreased neural response to rewards.
social emotions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 988–999.
Seger, C. A., Stone, M., & Keenan, J. P. (2004). Cortical activations during
Saarela, M. V., & Hari, R. (2008). Listening to humans walking together judgments about the self and another person. Neuropsychologia, 42,
activates the social brain circuitry. Social Neuroscience, 3, 401–409. 1168–1177.
Samson, D., Apperley, I. A., Chiavarino, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004). Segerstrom, S. C., Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., & Fahey, J. L. (1998).
Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone Optimism is associated with mood, coping, and immune change in
else’s belief. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 499–500. response to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Kathirgamanathan, U., & Humphreys, G. W. 1646–1655.
(2005). Seeing it my way: A case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-
Seifritz, E., Esposito, F., Neuhoff, J. G., Luthi, A., Mustovic, H., Dammann, G.,
perspective. Brain, 128, 1102–1111.
et al. (2003). Differential sex-independent amygdala response to
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biological
(2003). The neural basis of decision-making in the ultimatum game. Psychiatry, 54, 1367–1375.
Science, 300, 1755–1758.
Semendeferi, K., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Armstrong, E., & Van
Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2006). Integrating automatic and
Hoesen, G. W. (2001). Evolution of the hominoid prefrontal cortex:
controlled processing into neurocognitive models of social cognition.
Imaging and quantitative analysis of area 10. American Journal of
Brain Research, 1079, 86-97.
Physical Anthropology, 114, 224–241.
Saxe, R., Brett, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2006). Divide and conquer:
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal
A defense of functional localizers. NeuroImage, 30, 1088–1096.
networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study.
Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people:
Neuropsychologia, 45, 3054–3067.
The role of temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” NeuroImage,
19, 1835–1842. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Lester, H., Chisin, R., Israel, O., Bar-Shalom, R.,
Peretz, A., et al. (2005). The neural correlates of understanding the oth-
Saxe, R., Moran, J. M., Scholz, J., & Gabrieli, J. (2006). Overlapping and
er ’s distress: A positron emission tomography investigation of accurate
non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection
empathy. NeuroImage, 27, 468–472.
in individual subjects. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
1, 229–234. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tibi-Elhanany, Y., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2006). The
Saxe, R., & Powell, L. J. (2006). It’s the thought that counts: Specific brain ventromedial prefrontal cortex is involved in understanding affec-
regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Science, tive but not cognitive theory of mind stories. Social Neuroscience,
17, 692–699. 1, 149–166.
Saxe, R., Schulz, L. E., & Jiang, Y. V. (2006). Reading minds versus fol- Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., & Aharon-Peretz, J.
lowing rules: Dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the (2003). Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain
brain. Social Neuroscience, 1, 284–298. damage: The role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal
Saxe, R., & Wexler, A. (2005). Making sense of another mind: The of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 324–337.
role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia, 43, Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., Goldsher, D., &
1391–1399. Aharon-Peretz, J. (2005). Impaired “affective theory of mind” is
Saygin, A. P. (2007). Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cognitive and
for biological motion perception. Brain, 130, 2452–2461. Behavioral Neurology, 18, 55–67.
Schaefer, A., Collette, F., Philippot, P., der Linden, M. V., Laureys, S., Shaw, P., Lawrence, E., Bramham, J., Brierley, B., Radbourne, C., &
Delfiore, G., et al. (2003). Neural correlates of “hot” and “cold” emo- David, A. S. (2007). A prospective study of the effects of anterior
tional processing: A multilevel approach to the functional anatomy of temporal lobectomy on emotion recognition and theory of mind.
emotion. NeuroImage, 18, 938–949. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2783–2790.
Schmitz, T. W., & Johnson, S. C. (2006). Self-appraisal decisions evoke Shaw, P., Lawrence, E. J., Radbourne, C., Bramham, J., Polkey, C. E., &
dissociated dorsal-ventral aMPFC networks. NeuroImage, 30, David, A. S. (2004). The impact of early and late damage to the human
1050–1058. amygdala on “theory of mind” reasoning. Brain, 127, 1535–1548.
Schmitz, T. W., Kawahara-Baccus, T. N., & Johnson, S. C. (2004). Shimada, S., Hiraki, K., & Oda, I. (2005). The parietal role in the sense of
Metacognitive evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal self-ownership with temporal discrepancy between visual and proprio-
cortex. NeuroImage, 22, 941–947. ceptive feedbacks. NeuroImage, 24, 1225–1232.

CH05.indd 191 12/22/09 5:34:00 PM


192 Social Cognitive Neuroscience

Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Sugiura, M., Watanabe, J., Maeda, Y., Matsue, Y., Fukuda, H., &
Raichle, M. E., et al. (1997). Common blood flow changes across Kawashima, R. (2005). Cortical mechanisms of visual self-recognition.
visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive NeuroImage, 24, 143–149.
Neuroscience, 9, 648–663. Sui, J., & Han, S. (2007). Self-construal priming modulates neural sub-
Simmons, A., Matthews, S. C., Feinstein, J. S., Hitchcock, C., Paulus, M. P., & strates of self-awareness. Psychological Science, 18, 861–866.
Stein, M. B. (2008). Anxiety vulnerability is associated with altered Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side
anterior cingulate response to an affective appraisal task. NeuroReport, of fairness: Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and dis-
19, 1033–1037. regarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R., & Frith, C. D. Science, 19, 339–347.
(2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory compo- Tai, Y. F., Scherfler, C., Brooks, C. D., Sawamoto, N., & Castiello, U.
nents of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162. (2004). The human premotor cortex is “mirror” only for biological
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & actions. Current Biology, 14, 117–120.
Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathetic neural responses are modulated by the Tetlock, P. E., & Levi, A. (1982). Attribution bias: On the inconclusiveness
perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466–469. of the cognition-motivation debate. Journal of Experimental Social
Smith, W. (1945). The functional significance of the rostral cingular cor- Psychology, 18, 68–88.
tex as revealed by its responses to electrical excitation. Journal of Thurstone, L. L. (1928). Attitudes can be measured. American Journal of
Neurophysiology, 8, 241–255. Sociology, 33, 529–554.
Sommerville, L. H., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2006). ACC Trepel, C., Fox, C. R., Poldrack, R. A. (2005). Prospect theory on the brain?
responds differentially to expectancy violation and social rejection. Toward a cognitive neuroscience of decision under risk. Cognitive
Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1007–1008. Brain Research, 23, 34–50.
Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H.-J., & Haynes, J.-D. (2008). Unconscious Tsakiris, M., Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The role of the right
determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, temporo-parietal junction in maintaining a coherent sense of one’s
11, 543–545. body. Neuropsychologia, 46, 3014–3018.
Spence, S. A., Brooks, D. J., Hirsch, S. R., Liddle, P. F., & Grasby, P. M. Turner, M. S., Simons, J. S., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W.
(1997). A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients (2008). Distinct roles for lateral and medial rostral prefrontal
experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain, cortex in source monitoring of perceived and imagined events.
120, 1997–2011. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1442–1453.
Spiers, H. J., & Maguire, E. (2006). Spontaneous mentalizing during an inter- Tyler, T. R. (1991). Psychological models of the justice motive: Antecedents
active real word task: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1674–1682. of distributive and procedural justice. Journal of Personality and
Spunt, R. P., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (in press). Identifying the Social Psychology, 67, 850–863.
what, why, and how of an observed action: An fMRI study of mental- Uddin, L. Q., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Zaidel, E., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). rTMS
izing and mechanizing during action observation. Journal of Cognitive to the right IPL disrupts self-other discrimination. Social Cognitive
Neuroscience. and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 65–71.
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual Urgesi, C., Moro, V., Candid, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Mapping implied
test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and body actions in the human motor system. Journal of Neuroscience, 26,
Social Psychology, 69, 797–811. 7942–7949.
Steele, C., Spencer, S., & Lynch, M. (1993). Self-image resilience and dis-
Urry, H. L., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Kalin, N. H., Thurow, M. E.,
sonance: The role of affirmational resources. Journal of Personality
Schaefer, H. S., et al. (2006). Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal
and Social Psychology, 64, 885–896.
cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A., Keane, J., & Young, A. (2003). predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults.
Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 4415–4425.
amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia, 41, 209–220.
Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P., & Belliveau, J. W.
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contribu- (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in
tions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640–656. humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 98,
Strathearn, L., Li, J., Fonagy, P., & Montague, P. R. (2008). What’s in a smile? 11656–11661.
Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics, 122, 40–51. Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re
Straube, T., Mentzel, H., & Miltner, W. H. R. (2007). Waiting for spi- doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological
ders: Brain activation during anticipatory anxiety in spider phobics. Review, 94, 3–15.
NeuroImage, 37, 1427–1436. Van der Gaag, C., Minderaa, R. B., & Keysers, C. (2007). The BOLD
Stuss, D. T., Floden, D., Alexander, M. P., Levine, B., & Katz, D. (2001). signal in the amygdala does not differentiate between dynamic facial
Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: Dissociation of processes expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 93–103.
and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 39, 771–786. van Duynslaeger, M., van Overwalle, F., & Verstraeten, E. (2007).
Stuss, D. T., Gallup, G. G., Jr., & Alexander, M. P. (2001). The frontal lobes Electrophysiological time course and brain areas of spontane-
are necessary for “theory of mind.” Brain, 124, 279–286. ous and intentional trait inferences. Social Cognitive and Affective
Sugiura, M., Kawashima, R., Nakamura, K., Okada, K., Kato, T., Neuroscience, 2, 174–188.
Nakamura, A., et al. (2000). Passive and active recognition of one’s van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis.
own face. NeuroImage, 11, 36–48. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 829–858.
Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Jeong, H., Horie, K., Sato, S., & Kawashima, R. van’t Wout, M., Kahn, R. S., Sanfey, A. G., & Aleman, A. (2005). Repetitive
(2008). Face-specific and domain-general characteristics of cortical transcranial magnetic stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal
responses during self-recognition. NeuroImage, 42, 414–422. cortex affects strategic decision-making. NeuroReport, 16, 1849–1852.
Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Watanabe, J., Akitsuki, Y., Maeda, Y., Matsue, Y., Vanderwal, T., Hunyadi, E., Grupe, D. W., Connors, C. M., & Schultz, R. T.
et al. (2006). Cortical mechanisms of person perception: Recognition (2008). Self, mother, and abstract other: An fMRI study of reflective
of famous and personally familiar names. NeuroImage, 31, 853–860. social processing. NeuroImage, 41, 1437–1446.

CH05.indd 192 12/22/09 5:34:01 PM


References 193

Vincent, J. L., Patel, G. H., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Baker, J. T., Van Whalen, P. J., Raunch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., &
Essen, D. C., et al. (2007). Intrinsic functional architecture in the Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expres-
anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature, 447, 83–86. sions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. The
Vogt, S., Buccino, G., Wohlschläger, A. M., Canessa, N., Shah, N. J., Zilles, K., Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418.
et al. (2007). Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand Wheatley, T., Milleville, S. C., & Martin, A. (2007). Understanding ani-
actions: Effects of practice and expertise. NeuroImage, 37, 1371–1383. mate agents: Distinct roles for the social network and mirror systems.
Vollm, B. A., Taylor, A. N. W., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., Psychological Science, 18, 469–474.
McKie, S., et al. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and Wheeler, M. E., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Controlling racial prejudice:
empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonver- Social-cognitive goals affect amygdala and stereotype activation.
bal task. NeuroImage, 29, 90–98. Psychological Sciences, 16, 56–63.
Vrticka, P., Andersson, F., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G.
(2008). Individual attachment style modulates human amygdala and (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of
striatum activation during social appraisal. PLoS One, 6, e2868. seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.
Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Driver, J., & Wicker, B., Perrett, D. I., Baron-Cohen, S., & Decety, J. (2003). Being the tar-
Dolan, R. J. (2002). Neural responses to emotional faces with and get of another’s emotion: A PET study. Neuropsychologia, 41, 139–146.
without awareness; Event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual Wicker, B., Ruby, P., Royet, J.-P., & Fonlupt, P. (2003). A relation between
extinction and spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2156–2166. rest and the self in the brain? Brain Research Reviews, 43, 224–230.
Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of Wiethoff, S., Wildgruber, D., Kreifekts, B., Becker, H., Herbert, C., Grodd, W.,
attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event- et al. (2007). Cerebral processing of emotional prosody—influence of
related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829–841. acoustic parameters and arousal. NeuroImage, 39, 885–893.
Vuilleumier, P., Mohr, C., Valenza, N., Wetzel, C., & Landis, T. (2003). Wildgruber, D., Pihan, H., Ackermann, H., Erb, M., & Grodd, W. (2002).
Hyperfamiliarity for unknown faces after left lateral temporo-occipital Dynamic brain activation during processing of emotional intona-
venous infarction: A double dissociation with prosopagnosia. Brain, tion: Influence of acoustic parameters, emotional valence, and sex.
126, 889–907. NeuroImage, 15, 856–869.
Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. Wildgruber, D., Riecker, A., Hertich, I., Erb, M., Grodd, W., Ethofer, T., et
(2004). Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation al. (2005). Identification of emotional intonation evaluated by fMRI.
during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1271–1278. NeuroImage, 24, 1233–1241.
Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., & Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly high Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58,
correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cogni- 425–452.
tion. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 274–290. Williams, K. D., Bernieri, F., Faulkner, S., Grahe, J., & Gada-Jain, N.
Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., & (2000). The Scarlet Letter Study: Five days of social ostracism. Journal
Ochsner, K. N. (2008). Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating suc- of Personal and Interpersonal Loss, 5, 19–63.
cessful emotion regulation. Neuron, 59, 1037–1050. Williams, M. A., Morris, A. P., McGlone, F., Abbott, D. F., & Mattingley, J. B.
Wager, T. D., Rilling, J. K., Smith, E. E., Sokoloik, A., Casey, K. L., (2004). Amygdala responses to fearful and happy facial expres-
Davidson, R. J., et al. (2004). Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the sions under conditions of binocular suppression. The Journal of
anticipation and experience of pain. Science, 303, 1162–1167. Neuroscience, 24, 2898–2904.
Wagner, A. D., Schacter, D. L., Rotte, M., Koutstaal, W., Maril, A., Dale, A. M., Willingham, D. T., & Dunn, E. W. (2003). What neuroimaging and brain
et al. (1998). Building memories: Remembering and forgetting of verbal localization can do, cannot do, and should not do for social psychology.
experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science, 281, 1188–1191. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 662–671.
Wakusawa, K., Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Jeong, H., Horie, K., Sato, S., et al. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and
(2007). Comprehension of implicit meanings in social situations involv- constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understand-
ing irony: A functional MRI study. NeuroImage, 37, 1417–1426. ing of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.
Walter, H., Adenzato, M., Ciaramidaro, A., Enici, I., Pia, L., & Bara, B. G. Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002).
(2004). Understanding intentions in social interactions: The role of the Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trust-
anterior paracingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, worthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 277–283.
1854–1863. Wraga, M., Helt, M., Jacobs, E., & Sullivan, K. (2006). Neural basis of
Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006a). Developmental stereotype-induced shifts in women’s mental rotation performance.
changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Social Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 12–19.
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 107–121. Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural
Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006b). Neural basis basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment.
of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 8235–8240.
and context. Brain, 129, 932–943. Zaki, J., Ochsner, K. N., Hanelin, J., Wager, T. D., & Mackey, S. C. (2007).
Way, B. M., Taylor, S. E., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Variation in the Different circuits for different pain: Patterns of functional connectivity
k-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) is associated with dispositional and reveal distinct networks for processing pain in self and others. Social
neural sensitivity to social rejection. Proceedings of the National Neuroscience, 2, 276–291.
Academy of Science. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812612106. Zhang, L., Tiangang, Z., Zhang, J., Liu, Z., Fan, J., & Ying, Z. (2006). In
Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in social search of the Chinese self: An fMRI study. Science in China, 49, 89–96.
life. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, L., & Han, S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural
social psychology (pp. 446–496). New York: McGraw-Hill. influence on self-representation. NeuroImage, 34, 1310–1316.
Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., & Woldorff, M. G. Zysset, S., Huber, O., Ferstl, E., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The ante-
(2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nature rior frontomedian cortex and evaluative judgment: An fMRI study.
Neuroscience, 9, 971–978. NeuroImage, 15, 983–991.

CH05.indd 193 12/22/09 5:34:02 PM


Chapter 6

Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment


JIM BLASCOVICH AND WENDY BERRY MENDES

In 1969, in the second edition of Handbook of Social of exploring peripheral neurophysiology is to understand
Psychology, David Shapiro and Andrew Crider authored how embodied somatic activity catalyzes social psycho-
a chapter titled “Psychophysiological Approaches to logical processes including, for example, social cognition,
Social Psychology,” describing theory and research relat- memory, emotion, and mimicry (cf. Niedenthal, 2007).
ing biological and social psychological processes; this is The central and peripheral approaches are certainly com-
the first and only such chapter in this series. Reflecting patible, and the goals are not mutually exclusive. Both fit
growing interest and research in neurophysiological within a field that has come to be known as social neuro-
approaches to social psychology, the editors of this fifth science. Indeed, many social psychologists work in both
edition recognized a need for not one but two chap- domains.
ters relating biology and social psychology: one focuses The section topics included in this chapter are deemed
on intracranial processes (see Lieberman, this volume) by us as the most relevant for the intended audience of
embodied via the brain, and one (this chapter) focuses on graduate students, researchers, and readers desiring some
peripheral neurophysiological processes embodied via the familiarity with its entitled topics, though much more
visceral, somatic, and endocrine systems. The function of could have been included about social psychophysiology
the separation of these chapters is primarily organizational and embodiment were it not for page restrictions inherent
as both levels of processing are inexorably intertwined in a project such as the Handbook of Social Psychology.
(cf. Niedenthal, 2007; Niedenthal, Barsalou, Winkielman, Furthermore, this chapter is not intended to be an exhaus-
Krauth-Gruber, & Ric, 2005). tive review of the relevant empirical literature, but rather,
Although the goals for social psychologists exploring given its didactic intent, a selective one.
relevant central and peripheral neurophysiological pro- In the first section, Roots of Social Psychophysiology
cesses are not mutually exclusive, they differ in emphasis. and Embodiment, we provide a historical analysis of
A current important goal for social psychologists explor- how modern-day social psychophysiology and embodi-
ing the brain itself is to understand how online and offline ment came to be, followed by a discussion of the value
neural substrates of conscious and unconscious affective, of these topics for social psychology. The second section,
cognitive, motivational, and motor processes comport Neurophysiological Systems, focuses on the unique value
with social psychological theories, and to use such knowl- of neurophysiological indexes of social psychological con-
edge to bolster, bound, or expand them. An important goal structs for research. The third section, Relative Advantages
of those exploring visceral, somatic, and peripheral endo- of Peripheral and Central Neurophysiological Indexes
crine systems is to understand how such bodily expressions for Social Psychology, describes neurophysiological sys-
relate to centrally controlled affective, cognitive, and moti- tems and functions in broad terms with some discussion
vational processes, and how such peripheral responses can comparing the relative value of various uses of peripheral
be used to index social psychological constructs to mea- and central neurophysiological measures in social psychol-
sure critical independent and dependent variables more ogy. In the fourth section, Peripheral Neurophysiological
precisely than might otherwise be the case. Another goal Indexes, we describe the basis on which validation of

Preparation of this chapter was partially supported by the National Science Foundation (grant 0527377; J.B.) and National Heart, Lung
and Blood Institute (grant RO1 HL079383; W.B.M.). We thank Susan Fiske, Dan Gilbert, Greg Willard, and members of the Health and
Psychophysiology Lab at Harvard University for comments on an early draft of this chapter. We also acknowledge Kristin Concannon for
her editorial assistance in the preparation of this chapter.
194

CH06.indd 194 12/18/09 2:36:56 PM


Roots of Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment 195

peripheral neurophysiological indexes of social psychology century, Theodor Schwann, Matthias Schleiden, and
should rest. This is followed by a fifth section, Evolution of Rudolf Virchow had solidified cell theory. As the living
Social Psychophysiological Theories, which reviews social cell became generally accepted as the common structural
psychophysiological theories, more specifically, reason- and reproductive component of all organic substances,
ably prominent theories incorporating peripheral neuro- including those in both plants and animals, the field of
physiological processes and constructs. In the final section, biology came into its own.
Uses of Peripheral Neurophysiological Indexes In Social Psychology became distinguished from philosophy only
Psychology, we review representative constructs that are in the mid- to late-19th century. During that time, clinicians
relevant to social psychology and have been examined claimed that certain psychic (e.g., hysteria) and some-
using peripheral neurophysiological indexes. times even physical abnormalities (e.g., catatonia) had
no apparent physiological causes, and empiricists applied
observational techniques, including experiments, to their
ROOTS OF SOCIAL PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY study of mind and behavior. Arguably, property dualism,
AND EMBODIMENT as described earlier, provided the metaphysical underpin-
nings of such psychological luminaries as Sigmund Freud,
One can trace historical elements of social psychophysiology Carl Jung, Wilhelm Wundt, William James, and others.
and embodiment nearly three millennia in both Eastern and Wundt, a structuralist, and James, a functionalist, dif-
Western cultures. Indeed, heart rate was reportedly used to fered as the former proceeded introspectively to infer
infer emotion (i.e., “love”) as early as the third century BC common mental structures and processes to explain psy-
(Mesulam & Perry, 1972). Not surprisingly, overt embodi- chology. James looked more outwardly, examining the
ments, especially those involving nonverbal behaviors person’s adaptive relationship with the environment.
(e.g., mutual gaze) were known by ancient peoples to sig- However, their philosophies of mind, like those of many
nify intimacy or the desire for it. other founding fathers of modern psychology, comport
with the metaphysical assumptions of property dualism
more than substance dualism or strict monism; that is, men-
Monism and Dualism
tal activity derives from bodily processes but is not nec-
In many ways, this long history reflects a dialectical struggle essarily reducible to it. The monism–dualism debate was
between proponents of mind-body monism and proponents avoided altogether by Watsonian and Skinnerian behavior-
of mind-body dualism among scholars interested in mind- ism, which held sway in psychology for 50 years or longer.
behavior issues including philosophers, psychologists, Radical behaviorism, as it applied to humans, eschewed
physiologists, and neuroscientists. Generally, the struggle anything to do with the mind and brain, considering it
has been about the corporeal nature of the mind, though metaphorically a “black box,” the operation of which was
even the reality of the physical nature of the body has unimportant to understanding human behavior. Rather,
been questioned in some quarters. Basically, monism is the behaviorists looked to environmental contingencies to
the view that mind and body are not ontologically distinct explain behavior.
entities, whereas dualism is the view that they are. In its By the last half and particularly the last quarter of
strong Cartesian form, or “substance dualism,” mind and the 20th century, a number of factors contributed toward the
brain are composed of different “substances” (e.g., body reemergence, weak at first, but stronger by the dawn of
and soul). In a weaker form, or “property dualism,” the the new millennium, of mind-body interaction in social
mind is thought of as a group of independent properties psychology and related fields. These factors included
that emerge from, but cannot be reduced to, the brain even seminal theoretical and empirical work by social psycholo-
though the mind is not made up of distinct substances (e.g., gists, such as Schachter and Singer ’s (1962) neo-Jamesian
Searle, 1992). model of emotions; by personality psychologists, such as
As might be expected, Cartesian dualism led to a bifurca- Friedman and Rosenman’s (1974) Type A dispositional
tion of philosophy of mind on the one hand and physiology construct; and by health psychologists’ mind-body con-
on the other. Western physiology, freed by Cartesian dual- nectionism, such as Ader and Cohen’s (1975) seminal
ism from concern with the mind, began making rapid psychoneuroimmunology work. It also included work on
advances. For example, William Harvey, the Western anat- metaphysical assumptions underlying social neuroscience.
omist, “first” described the cardiovascular (CV) system Social psychologists John Cacioppo and Louis Tassinary
in the 17th century, though the Arab physician, Ibn al (e.g., 1990) espoused the “identity thesis,” arguing that
Nafis, did so (and also described the pulmonary system) all mental, and hence psychological, states and processes
four centuries earlier (Gregory, 2001). By the mid-19th are embodied corporeally. In 1994, neuroscientist Antonio

CH06.indd Sec1:195 12/18/09 2:36:57 PM


196 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

Damasio published his landmark work, Descartes’ Error: it more simply, the proverbial detached brain would be
Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain, putting another, impoverished in terms of information processing without
perhaps final, nail in the philosophical coffin of mind-body peripheral embodiments.
substance dualism (also see Taylor; Keltner & Lerner; and Modern theoretical embodiment accounts of social
Funder & Fast, this volume). psychological constructs can be traced to Charles Darwin
The rejection of Cartesian mind-body dualism and the (1872), who argued that attitude is a collection of pos-
drift toward monism led Blascovich and Seery (2006) to tural motor behaviors revealing an organism’s emotional
delineate several implications of the view of mind and response toward an object. A decade or so later, William
body stated in Cacioppo and Tassinary’s identity thesis. James and Carl Lange independently arrived at similar but
For one, the identity thesis implies that understanding physiologically expanded notions regarding the nature of
bodily responses can inform our understanding of mental the relationship between body and emotion. James (1884)
states and processes and vice versa. For another, biological and later Lange and James (1922) held that emotion is the
and psychological levels of analysis must be integrated for perception of peripheral physiological conditions (including
social psychologists to understand mind-body relation- those of the musculature viscera) that result from some
ships. Third, the modern monistic view as elucidated in stimulus. The mind’s perception of stronger heartbeat,
the identity thesis argues against reductionism (i.e., that increased or decreased muscle tension, higher adrenaline
a complex system is only the sum of its parts and, there- levels, sweaty palms, and so forth is the emotion according
fore, can be understood and explained by reducing it to to these theorists; that is, physiological reactions form the
accounts of those parts) and determinism (i.e., that every- basis of subsequent emotional reactions. Walter Cannon
thing, including affect, cognition, and behavior, is causally and Philip Bard (Bard, 1929; Cannon, 1929) challenged
determined by an unbroken chain of prior occurrences), as the James-Lange theory. They postulated that perception of
well as pure psychologism (i.e., that mental states play the a stimulus leads to both the emotion and peripheral physi-
pivotal causal role for behavior). Finally, the identity thesis ological activity, and that, indeed, many different emotions
implies that state-of-the-art training in social psychology shared the same peripheral physiological responses.
must include training in neurophysiology without aban- Much later, Stanley Schachter and Jerome Singer
doning social psychology’s substantive purviews. (1962) modified the James-Lange view maintaining that
people perceive peripheral physiological responses and
interpret them emotionally taking account of the context
Body and Mind
within which they find themselves. In their classic experi-
That the body and mind are two sides of the same coin is ment, they injected experimental participants with epi-
arguably the essence of the identity thesis and the current nephrine and found that those who did not expect to be
more monistic, or at least, property dualistic metaphysical affected physiologically by the injection but, in reality,
view of human nature. The common Western view is that were affected nonetheless labeled their emotions positively
the metaphorical coin is the brain or, as we have described or negatively in line with contextual social cues. This was
it, the site of primary control over body and mind. one of the first experiments within the tradition of modern
In his essay “Where am I?” philosopher of mind Daniel embodiment theory. However, several statistical and other
Dennett (1978) grapples with the location of the self. He critiques (e.g., Plutchik & Ax, 1967; Stern, Botto, &
implies that if the brain were somehow remote from the rest Herrick, 1972; Stricker, 1967), as well as replication fail-
of the body, lying in a nutrient bath connected by wires or ures such as those by Maslach (1979) and Marshall and
telemetric microwaves distally to their bodies, most people Zimbardo (1979), eventually dimmed the impact of the
would identify their “self ” as located in brain. Such “com- neo-Jamesian perspective.
mon sense” replies are consistent with the theme, one that Interoceptive (i.e., perception of visceral organ activity)
is science fiction to some and a remote possibility to others accuracy became the target of much research in the 1970s
that human brains sans bodies could simply communicate and 1980s when psychophysiologists such as Jasper Brener
with one another, directly eliminating the need for physical (e.g., Brener & Kluvitse, 1988), Gary Jones (e.g., Brener &
bodies. But would the need for bodies really be eliminated? Jones, 1974), Edward Katkin (e.g., 1985), William
According to embodiment theory, it would not. Whitehead (e.g., Whitehead & Drescher, 1980), and others
The embodiment theory perspective is that a reciprocal investigated the role of people’s perceptual acuity (i.e., intero-
relationship exists between peripheral bodily expressions ceptive accuracy) for detecting peripheral physiological
(e.g., of memories, emotions, and sensorimotor skills) and signals. Stimulated, in part, by Miller’s (1978) notion that
central nervous system (CNS) processing of information accurate visceral perceivers would benefit more from
from those domains (cf. Niedenthal et al., 2005). To put biofeedback therapy (e.g., for relaxation) than less accurate

CH06.indd 196 12/18/09 2:36:58 PM


Roots of Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment 197

perceivers, these and other investigators spent more than Key to emotional efference theory is the likelihood that
a decade perfecting research paradigms for assessing the affectively driven facial muscle contortions differentially
accuracy of individuals’ perceptions of visceral signals, cool venal blood flow that passes near the cavernous sinus
concentrating, for the most part, on heartbeat detection via a heat exchange mechanism with the ambient envi-
(though the idea that accurate visceral perceivers would ronment. In a series of sophisticated experiments using
need external biofeedback seems to be counterintuitive). essentially “one-of-a-kind” noninvasive technology to
In general, most work on interoception (e.g., Katkin, measure forehead temperature at critical locations related
Blascovich, & Goldband, 1981) demonstrated that men, to venal blood flow, Zajonc and colleagues (1989) demon-
and especially women, were extraordinarily poor visceral strated that such differential cooling was correlated with
perceivers—though men benefited somewhat from perfor- the hypothesized differences in affect. Interestingly, these
mance feedback training in discriminating their own bodily experiments were run with the ambient temperature in the
signals (i.e., heartbeats) from simulated ones—calling room in which they were conducted at lower levels (prob-
into question any functional relationship between visceral ably around room temperature) than the range of normal
perceptual ability and emotional expression. Although one human blood temperature (e.g., around 98–99˚ F). If the
or two studies in this research tradition supported the neo- ambient temperature (e.g., around 110˚ F) were greater
Jamesian perspective on emotion, a few others suggested than normal blood temperature, then the predictions based
the opposite. For example, Hantas, Katkin, and Blascovich on the heat exchange principle should be reversed. If local
(1982) found that more accurate heartbeat detectors self- environmental ambient temperature is less than body tem-
reported greater emotional reactions to emotionally evoca- perature, facial expressions (e.g., smiling) associated with
tive photographs than inaccurate ones. Later, consistent cooling venal blood should result in positive affect. In con-
with affect intensity theory (Larsen & Diener, 1987), trast, if local environmental ambient is higher than body
Blascovich et al. (1992) demonstrated that poor heartbeat temperature, those same facial expressions (e.g., smiling)
detectors tended to exaggerate their reported emotions more should be associated with warming venal blood and nega-
than good heartbeat detectors. Still later, Barrett, Quigley, tive affect, thereby providing stronger inferential evidence
Bliss-Moreau, and Aronson (2004) proposed and found for Zajonc and colleagues’ hypothesized mechanism.
evidence that visceral interoceptive ability was positively As far as we know, such an experiment has never been
related to the intensity of emotional experience as delineated conducted.
in Barrett’s core affect model (Barrett & Bliss-Moreau, in Although the blood temperature mechanism that Zajonc
press). In sum, the role of interoception in emotion is and colleagues (1989) specified has received relatively lit-
undoubtedly more complicated than researchers in the past tle attention and remains unverified in strong ways empiri-
have theorized, and more sophisticated theory and research cally, these theorists, themselves, foresaw the importance
are needed to explain the seemingly oppositional effects of of facial expressions as embodiments of emotion regard-
interoceptive ability and emotion reported in the literature. less of their hypothesized blood temperature regulation
Clearly, data from interoception experiments sug- mechanism. Specifically, they stated:
gest, in general, that there is little consistent relationship
between conscious visceral interoception and psychologi- Independent of the validity of the particular physiological
processes that could be involved in producing subjective
cal processes such as emotional ones (cf. Barrett et al.,
effects, the basic principle that facial efferents may have regu-
2004). However, embodiment theorists would argue that
latory functions, and thereby subjective consequences, has
visceral and proprioceptive information (i.e., perception a great deal of plausibility and, if true, profound theoretical
of skeletal muscle activity) need not rise to the level of importance. (p. 397)
consciousness to affect emotion. For example, based on
Waynbaum’s (1907) novel ideas relating cerebral blood Their idea that facial muscular embodiments of emo-
flow and facial expressions, Zajonc, Murphy, and Inglehart tional states, as well as other muscular embodiments,
(1989) proposed the vascular theory of emotional effer- play a causal role in affective and emotional experience
ence. Specifically, they hypothesized that facial muscle has been taken up by many researchers, albeit via hypoth-
movements regulate the temperature of venal blood in the esized mechanisms other than venal blood temperature
cavernous sinus in the brain that, in turn, is causally related regulation.
to emotional experience (with decreases in blood tempera- Niedenthal and colleagues (Niedenthal, 2007; Niedenthal
ture accompanied by more positive affect and increases in et al., 2005) maintain that attitudes, memories, emotion, and
blood temperature accompanied by more negative affect). social perceptions involve perceptual, visceral, and motoric
Accordingly, in the James-Lange tradition, facial somatic “reexperiencing” or “embodiment.” Niedenthal and her
changes lead to emotional experience. colleagues’ literature reviews and explanatory offerings

CH06.indd 197 12/18/09 2:36:58 PM


198 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

provide strong evidence for embodiment processes and psychological states. Nevertheless, the evidence is strong
pose a theoretical explanation. With regard to the evidence that such embodiments play important roles in the devel-
on affect-related somatic embodiments (e.g., facial expres- opment and operation of the processes underlying psycho-
sions and postures), Niedenthal (2007) concludes: (1) logical states.
emotion-specific posturing leads to experience of specific
affect/emotions (e.g., Cacioppo, Priester, & Berntson, 1993;
Values of Psychophysiology and Embodiment in
Wells & Petty, 1980); (2) facilitation of emotion-specific
Social Psychology
postures increases posture-consistent attitudes (e.g., Duclos
et al., 1989); and (3) inhibition of emotion-specific postures The scientific values of examining the interplay among
interferes with experience of posture-specific emotions (e.g., neurophysiological and social processes within social psy-
Strack, Martin, & Stepper, 1988). chology fall into three categories: theoretical, methodolog-
Furthermore, Niedenthal maintains that such embodiments ical, and applied.
are involved in both online and offline processing of psy-
chological states such as attitudes, memories, emotions, Theoretical
and so forth. More specifically, actual or online postural Social psychological theories are strengthened as behav-
movements affect the experience and encoding of such ioral scientists (with social psychologists being no
psychological states. Hence, online embodiments facilitate exception) continue to become more sophisticated about
the initial capture of information relevant to psychological neurophysiological processes. By no account does this
states, whereas offline embodiments are thought to aid mean that social psychologists should become reduc-
recall, reenactment, reexperience, among others, of those tionistic, deterministic thinkers, thereby abandoning the
psychological states. In the words of Niedenthal et al: explanatory value of social psychology as an important
level of analysis. Rather, it means that such knowledge can
In theories of embodied cognition, using knowledge—as in improve the precision of social psychological theories.
recalling memories, drawing inferences, and making plans—is
thus called “embodied” because an admittedly incomplete but Methodological
cognitively useful reexperience is produced in the originally
implicated sensory-motor systems, as if the individual were
In addition to possible theoretical advances stemming
there in the very situation, the very emotional state, or with the from an understanding of mind-body relationships, there
very object of thought. (Niedenthal et al., 2005, p. 18) are substantial methodological, particularly measurement,
advantages as well. Indeed, many social psychologists
For example, in an experiment conducted by Rauscher, have attempted to test hypotheses and bolster theories by
Krauss, and Chen (1996), participants watched a cartoon. including neurophysiological indexes of social psycho-
Afterward, participants who had been prevented from logical constructs. However, the value of such indexes
gesturing compared with those who were unfettered were depends on the quality of the physiologically based
slower to recall cartoon details. Glenberg and Kaschak linkages between measures and constructs as social neu-
(2002) demonstrated that participants more quickly judged roscientists have maintained for more than a quarter of
the sensibility of sentences when motion meaning com- a century (e.g., Blascovich, 2000; Blascovich & Seery,
ported with the arm motion described (e.g., “Close the 2006; Cacioppo, Tassinary, & Berntson, 2000; Cacioppo &
drawer.”). Tassinary, 1990; Obrist, 1976). If a social psychological
Although Zajonc et al. (1989) suggested blood tem- construct is well defined, if a strong theoretical case for its
perature regulation as a possibility for the physiologi- neurophysiological underpinnings is made, and if a neu-
cal mechanism linking facial postures (i.e., expressions), rophysiological index (typically but not necessarily incor-
Niedenthal and colleagues (2005) suggested the opera- porating multiple measures) is independently validated,
tion of motor neurons, specifically mirror neurons, as the then the methodological advantages of neurophysiological
physiological mechanism linking not only facial postures measures in social psychology are substantial. These
but all somatic embodiment postures. Although explicit advantages accrue from the fact that neurophysiological
and detailed discussion and debate regarding the concept, measures can best be measured continuously, covertly, and
operation, and evidence of mirror neurons is more within in an online manner.
the scope of our sister chapter on social neuroscience, it Continuous measurement eliminates the loss of infor-
seems at the time this chapter was written, that mirror neu- mation that typically occurs with point time estimates.
rons represent a plausible mechanism linking peripheral One can examine the topology of construct expression
somatic embodiments to both online and offline central via neurophysiological indexes over time. For example,
processing of underlying social psychologically relevant the four continuous functions revealed in Figure 6.1

CH06.indd 198 12/18/09 2:36:59 PM


Neurophysiological Systems 199

(Friedman & Rosenman, 1974) have been related to health


outcomes. Indeed, voluminous literature links peripheral
neurophysiological measures including neuroendocrine
and immunological ones to a variety of disease states but
Amplitude

is beyond the scope of this chapter (but see Taylor, this


volume).
Future performance can also be predicted via theory-
based neurophysiological indexes. For example, research
has demonstrated that patterns of CV markers related to
motivational states during appropriate assessment tasks
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 can predict future physical and cognitive performance
msex ⫻ 10
(Blascovich, Seery, Mudridge, Norris, & Weisburch, 2004;
Figure 6.1 Four examples of hypothetical activation of a Kassam, Koslov, & Mendes, in press; Schmader, Forbes,
neurophysiological response over time. Zhang, & Mendes, 2009). Discriminatory hostile behav-
ior can be predicted from specific somatic responses per-
mitting body and head movements. For example, McCall,
Blascovich, Young, and Persky (2009) have shown that
depict examples of a hypothetical activation of a single
Anglo-American individuals who maintain more interper-
neurophysiological measure over some period. As can
sonal distance and more gaze aversion between themselves
easily be seen, each function has the same mean and
and African-American opponents display more hostile and
median over the epoch, but the distributions of data they
perceived fatal gunfire toward them than Anglo-Americans
represent differ substantially over time. The difference in
who maintain less distance. Such an effect does not occur
form can not only provide a more fine grained distinction
in Anglo-American to Anglo-American pairings.
between processes underlying the multiple functions, but it
can also better inform theory development.
Covert or implicit measurement, as is well known
NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL SYSTEMS
among social psychologists, avoids the pitfalls of impres-
sion management artifacts that can plague self-report–
Human physiology can be modeled as a cybernetic or con-
based measurement. Nearly all neurophysiological and
tinuous feedback system involving control and operational
embodiment-based measures can be considered in this
systems (see Figure 6.2). The primary control systems are
category.
intracranial and include the CNS—that is, the brain and
Online neurophysiological measurement provides for
spinal cord, and the pituitary gland. The hypothalamus, a
accurate synchronization of neurophysiological indexes
brain structure, links the CNS and the pituitary gland via
with the activities of participants during the measurement
a structure called the “median eminence.” The CNS con-
epoch, typically the critical periods of an experiment.
trols the endocrine system via the hypothalamic-pituitary
Indeed, online measurement also provides the possibility
configuration but with feedback from the peripheral endo-
that certain experimental activities (e.g., presentation of
crine system via the bloodstream (as the arrows illustrate).
stimuli, requirements of action, etc.) can be triggered by
Hence, together, the CNS and cranial endocrine executive,
levels of neurophysiological activity on some variable or
the pituitary, control the lower-level operational systems
variables over time; for example, when facial electromy-
neurally via the peripheral nervous system and peripheral
ography (EMG) reveals that a participant is in a positive or
endocrine glands.
negative affective state. Furthermore, online neurophysi-
Major operational systems include the immune, visceral,
ological measures do not suffer problems associated with
skeletal-motor or somatic, and endocrine systems. Our dis-
prospective and retrospective self-reports.
tinction between control and operational systems does not
preclude control functions at the operational systems level
Applied or even lower within operational components (e.g., heart,
Given social psychology’s value for understanding how thymus). Control and operational functions are even embed-
the exigencies of everyday social life can impact one’s ded in cells, the basic building blocks of all life (e.g., reuptake
health, neurophysiological indexes of constructs such as receptors near neuronal synapses), but the higher-level orga-
stress (Ader & Cohen, 1975; Matthews, 1986; McEwen, nization fits our purposes here. Importantly, our distinction
2002), threat (Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996; Tomaka, between control and operational systems is not meant to
Blascovich, & Kelsey, 1993), and coronary prone behavior imply primacy of intracranial processes as the originating

CH06.indd 199 12/18/09 2:36:59 PM


200 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

Employment of peripheral neurophysiological embodi-


Central ments currently enjoys a few advantages over CNS
Nervous
System embodiments but only insofar as they are used as indexes
of psychological constructs. These include practical and
Cranial
inferential ones. Cost, intrusiveness, and accessibility are
Endocrine some relative practical advantages. For the most part, the
System
monetary costs of recording, scoring, and analyzing periph-
eral measures remain under the costs for recording, scoring,
and analyzing central neurophysiological measures such
Skeletal- as bloodborne signals via positron emission topogra-
Immune Visceral Endocrine
Motor
System System System phy (PET) and blood oxygen level–dependent signals
System
via functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and
are typically somewhat below the costs of high-density
Figure 6.2 Cybernetic feedback system of human physiology.
electroencephalography.
Peripheral measures are typically less intrusive than
central ones. Although neither central nor peripheral mea-
source of all psychological and social-psychological sures need be invasive, both types can be (e.g., intravenous
processes. Although such may or may not be the case during injection of radio-opaque dyes, collection of blood). The
dream and hallucinatory episodes, inputs to the CNS come technological requirements for peripheral measures, even
from sensory intake and feedback from peripheral neuro- for relatively sophisticated technology such as impedance
physiological systems including, importantly, kinesthetic cardiography, does not overly restrict postures and move-
information (cf. Niedenthal et al., 2005). ments, allowing for unconstrained interactions and experi-
Cybernetic cranial-based control of operational systems ences in a more naturalistic settings. Such is not currently
permits continuously operating physiological processes the case with PET- and fMRI-based measures. Generally,
to vary from, and return to, homeostatic levels for func- physiological recording equipment for peripheral measures
tional purposes. The CNS transmits instructions to the is more easily accessible, requires little, if anything, in the
operating systems via efferent neurons and receives feed- way of a specialized recording environment, and has been
back via afferent neurons. Central endocrine control miniaturized, thereby permitting even ambulatory mea-
occurs via pituitary generated gland-specific hormones that surements in the field.
transmit instructions to the endocrine operating system Given the varied types of peripheral physiological pro-
(i.e., glands) and receive feedback via circulating glan- cesses, such as sympathetic and parasympathetic branches
dular hormones, as well as peripheral and central neural of the autonomic nervous system (ANS), muscle activity,
feedback. Endocrine systems communicate with other skin temperature, hormonal and immune changes, to name
operational systems, especially the visceral systems (i.e., a few, measures based on them might have an advantage
pulmonary, CV, gastrointestinal, renal, hepatic, bladder), over central measures in certain contexts because one
often synergistically. can examine convergence and divergence across various
systems simultaneously, whereas the limited mobility
required as part of fMRI and PET procedures, at present,
RELATIVE ADVANTAGES OF PERIPHERAL circumscribes looking at multiple system responses con-
AND CENTRAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL comitantly with the induction of specific mental states.
INDEXES FOR SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Currently, a number of peripheral neurophysiologi-
cal indexes of certain psychological constructs have been
As discussed at the outset, this chapter focuses on the value experimentally validated, but their counterpart upstream
of peripheral visceral, somatic, and endocrine processes CNS indexes have not yet been. For example, CV indexes
as indexes of psychological constructs that can advance of challenge and threat motivational states can be indexed
social psychological theory. Both peripheral and central peripherally via patterns of CV responses (Blascovich &
neurophysiological indexes share certain methodological Tomaka, 1996; Tomaka et al., 1993; see later). However,
measurement advantages over subjective measures by the hypothalamic differentiation putatively underlying these
virtue of their properties (i.e., continuous, online, and peripheral patterns has not yet been and, perhaps, cannot
covert). Peripheral and central measures each have relative yet be distinguished via fMRI- or PET-based brain imag-
advantages over the other, and neither is a measurement ing. For example, functional brain imaging data may be
nor assessment panacea. able to demonstrate activation of the hypothalamus but has

CH06.indd 200 12/18/09 2:37:00 PM


Peripheral Neurophysiological Indexes 201

not yet been shown to distinguish between the oppositional Raichle, a pioneering brain imager and leading figure
motivational states resulting from that activation. Peripheral in human cognitive neuroscience argues, via his “dark
indexes are currently necessary for the latter. On the other matter” metaphor, that the subtractive techniques inherent
hand, there is little agreement regarding valid peripheral to imaging techniques such as fMRI and PET can be mis-
neurophysiological indexes of basic level emotions, point- leading because they highlight certain brain activity while
ing to the possibility that neural activation might be able to seemingly downplaying other brain activity that actually is
identify emotional responses where peripheral physiologi- involved (Raichle, 2006).
cal indexes have failed to do so (see Cacioppo, Berntson, However, if one’s intent is to understand the affective,
Larsen, Poehlmann & Ito, 2000, for meta-analysis). cognitive, somatic, and motoric processes underlying social
However the brain works, we can often discern some- behavior, central neurophysiological measures have the
thing about what it has done via peripheral neurophysi- advantage inferentially. No one doubts that affective, cog-
ological responses. If the brain has “chosen” to increase nitive, somatic, and motivational outputs are processed by
cardiac output, we can measure it via peripheral CV mea- the brain, albeit with inputs from the environment and from
surement (Tomaka et al., 1993). If the brain has caused the peripheral physiological operational systems (i.e., embodi-
endocrine system to increase cortisol level, we can measure ments). Potential direct causal explanation of mind-body
it via its in saliva, urine, or blood (Dickerson & Kemeny, relationships lies much more in the realm of central neuro-
2004). If the brain has caused a change in head orientation, physiological than peripheral neurophysiological processes.
we can measure head movements in a variety of ways (e.g.,
McCall et al., 2009).
PERIPHERAL NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL
In addition, there are several current impediments to
INDEXES
the development of brain-based indexes of psychological
states. The issues of complexity, plasticity, individual dif-
One of the challenges facing researchers interested in
ferences, and “dark matter” must be overcome before we
exploiting the methodological advantages that neurophysi-
can better take advantage of brain-based indexes of social
ological indexes bring to social psychological research is
psychological constructs.
how to select among the seeming plethora of possible ones
Regarding complexity, the structure and functions of the
to use. Just as an investigator would not use just any exist-
human brain represent perhaps the most complex and mas-
ing paper and pencil or behavioral measure to index any
sive living system on earth. Estimates of brain structures
social psychological construct, one should not select just
range as high as 100 billion neurons, each synapsing, on
any physiological measure or measures. Rather, one should
average, to 10,000 other neurons. This presents an impor-
be concerned about the validity of a neurophysiological
tant quantitative challenge to brain science, and presents
measure as an index of a particular construct and, therefore,
difficulties in terms of determining specific purposes and
search for validated neurophysiological indexes of that
functions of all neural connections, subsystems, “mod-
construct (several are discussed later). In the absence in the
ules,” and so forth in the brain. The brain is also highly
literature of such a validated neurophysiological index, one
plastic (i.e., adaptable) in the sense that certain cortical
can take on the task of melding appropriate neurophysi-
areas in the brain devoted to one kind of mental activity
ological theory with psychological processes underlying
can take over mental activities that are normally sup-
the target construct, and developing and validating such an
ported by other areas that are permanently or temporarily
index,1 though that can be a daunting task.
“out of order” (Ramachandran, Rogers-Ramachandran, &
Stewart, 1992). Hence, the psychological processes may
1
appear externally (i.e., peripherally) exactly the same Blascovich and Seery (2006) outlined a methodological approach
across individuals, but the spatial or temporal location, or to establishing and using peripheral neurophysiological indexes
both, of CNS control differs. Apart from insult-caused con- of social psychological constructs including “first principles”
trol dislocations in the brain, there is also a great deal of and “propositions” building on Cacioppo and Tassinary’s (1990)
“strength of inference” classification scheme. In contrast with
between-individual variability in certain measures of CNS
many self-report and even some behavioral measures, most periph-
operations. For example, Miller et al. (2002) have found
eral neurophysiological measures have little, if any, face validity
that brain images aggregated across individuals pointing to
with regard to their applicability to social psychological constructs.
localized brain control of specific cognitive and affec- Consequently, the identification and validation of such indexes
tive processes often mask considerable individual differ- is more complicated than subjective and behavioral ones. As dis-
ences in location. Aggregation techniques can actually cussed later in the chapter, some of this work has already been
point to activation of a brain location across participants accomplished, but not to the degree that a plethora of practical vali-
that no single participant actually exhibits. Finally, Marcus dated peripheral neurophysiological indexes of constructs exists.

CH06.indd 201 12/18/09 2:37:01 PM


202 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

Alternatively, one can manipulate independent variables (see Figure 6.3). The three dimensions are generality
and choose dependent variables for which there are extant (context), specificity, and sensitivity, and combined pro-
validated peripheral neurophysiological indexes. At first duce four categories of psychophysiological relationships:
read, the previous sentence may seem to be suggesting outcomes, concomitants, markers, and invariants that can
changing what one wants to investigate. However, that is vary in sensitivity.
not necessarily the case. Because the inferentially stron- Generality or context refers to the continuum of situa-
gest peripheral neurophysiological indexes that exist are tions, and varies from very limited or “context-dependent”
ones associated with superordinate categories of affect situations to unlimited or “context-independent” situations.
(i.e., positive vs. negative) or motivation (i.e., chal- Context-independent indexes are more desirable than context-
lenge vs. threat), and they can be indexed at the super- dependent ones in terms of the generalizability of results
ordinate level. Given that social psychologists are often based across all situations. However, they are difficult and,
interested in affect and motivational measures, these can in some cases, impossible to achieve especially in terms
be indexed validly neurophysiologically and relatively of neurophysiological indexes of social psychological
easily at the superordinate level. Unless it is important constructs. However, context-dependent indexes can be
for the investigator to distinguish happiness from love or valuable provided that the context represents a reasonable
fear from anxiety, indexing positive and negative affect domain within which the social-psychological processes
neurophysiologically provides important information to represented by the construct operate. Indeed, given Lewin’s
the investigator via the stated advantages of physiological dictum that social psychology represents the study of the
measures. interaction between person and situation, context-specific
indexes, or “markers” in Cacioppo and Tassinary’s taxon-
Propositions for Validating Neurophysiological omy, validated in the specified contexts can be valuable
Indexes of Social Psychological Constructs and, therefore, a reasonable goal.
Specificity refers to the nature of the relationship
Cacioppo and colleagues (Cacioppo & Tassinary, between the index and target construct. Optimal speci-
1990; Cacioppo, Tassinary, & Berntson, 2000, 2007) ficity is a singular or “one-to-one” relationship between
specified three dimensions that yield the taxonomy index and construct. Specifically, if the index is positive,
of psychophysiological relationships presented here the construct is expected to be present; and if the index

High

Context-free
Many-to-one
CONCOMITANT

OUTCOME INVARIANT

y
alit Sp
ner
Sensitivity

e e cif
G i cit
y

MARKER

Context-dependent One-to-one

Low
Figure 6.3 Cacioppo and colleagues’ taxon-
omy of psychophysiological inference.

CH06.indd 202 12/18/09 2:37:01 PM


Evolution of Social Psychophysiological Theories 203

is negative, the construct is expected to be absent. autonomic-based systems, and social-psychological


Furthermore, the relationship is symmetrical so that if the theories. Importantly, at this juncture, additional psycho-
construct is present or absent, the index is correspondingly physiological theories are ripe for increasing our under-
positive or negative; and if the index is positive or nega- standing of social-psychological processes.
tive, the construct is present or absent, correspondingly. Historically, psychophysiological theories have focused
Although this description of a one-to-one relationship on one or both of the two primary biological systems that
between an index and construct is a dichotomous one, one- are active during the operation of affective and cognitive
to-one relationships are certainly possible with continuous processes underlying motivation and performance: the
bipolar indexes as well. sympathetic adrenal medullary (SAM) and hypothalamic-
Sensitivity refers to the likelihood that a physiological pituitary-adrenal cortical (HPA) axes. At the risk of over-
response will covary with a psychological state. At low simplification, one can think of the SAM system as acti-
levels of sensitivity, a physiological response might simply vating during fight-or-flight situations, whereas the HPA
signal the presence or absence of a psychological state. At system is more conservative and activates after longer
higher levels of sensitivity, the amplitude and temporal tra- exposure to stress, such as preparing for comprehensive
jectory are specified to respond to the variable intensity of examinations. When the SAM system is activated, epi-
the psychological event. For example, if skin temperature nephrine is released from the adrenal medulla, which con-
were highly sensitive to the experience of anger, one tributes to several changes in the body, including increasing
would expect that as anger increased there would be a heart rate and blood pressure, dilating pupils, and inhibit-
linear relationship with skin temperature that could index ing the gastrointestinal tract. HPA activation is initiated in
the “amount” of anger experienced. the hypothalamus, which releases corticotropin-releasing
These dimensions are by no means the only dimensions hormone, which triggers the anterior pituitary to release
by which psychophysiological indexes can be established. adrenocorticotropin hormone, which travels to the adrenal
Indeed, Blascovich and Seery (2006) expanded on these cortex, which sits on top of the kidneys, and stimulates an
dimensions within an organizing framework of a multi- area of the adrenal cortex called the “zona fasciculata” to
trait multimethod matrix as a way to explore divergent and release cortisol.
convergent validity of physiological indexes and their rela- These systems can operate relatively independently,
tionship to mental states. though in some contexts are coactivated and can moder-
ate each other. Although the SAM and HPA axes can be
activated concomitantly, their onset and offset times can
Summary
differ. The SAM system can fully respond and peak within
Achieving one-to-one relationships between social- seconds on exposure to a stimulus, thought, or emotion,
psychological constructs and peripheral neurophysiological and its response can be measured in terms of changes
indexes is advanced by targeting less inclusive rather than in the ANS and increasing circulating catecholamines. In
more inclusive contexts in which to use them. Establishing contrast, the HPA system activates more slowly and gen-
valid indexes is optimized by using a divergent valida- erally takes minutes rather than seconds to reach its peak
tional approach, by including a physiologically theoreti- response. Similarly, recovery time (i.e., return to homeo-
cally meaningful pattern of multiple neurophysiological static levels) is longer after activation of the HPA com-
responses, and by assessing such patterns over time. Note pared with the SAM axis.
that a neurophysiological index based on a single physio- Some of the primary questions concerning these systems
logical response can provide basis for inference. However, for psychophysiologists, and especially social psycho-
strong inference (i.e., a marker or invariant) is less likely physiologists, include: What are the different eliciting psy-
though not logically impossible. chological factors associated with activating these systems?
How do emotions and individual differences influence acti-
vation? How can social situations influence responses? and
EVOLUTION OF SOCIAL What can we learn about people’s experiences and behav-
PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL THEORIES ior by examining how these systems activate and recover?
To begin to examine how social psychophysiologists have
As described earlier, the goal of social psychophysiology tried to answer some of these questions linking mind and
is to understand the links between social-psychological body, it is important to look at how modern research in
and neurophysiological theories. Toward this end, social the area of physiological responses to mental states began.
psychophysiological theories have emerged with roots Though one could point to many starting points, an espe-
in psychophysiological, especially neuroendocrine- and cially important figure in the 20th century was Hans

CH06.indd 203 12/18/09 2:37:02 PM


204 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

Selye and what ended up being his clumsy, but fortuitous, distress from the positive or less damaging eustress. This
approach to studying rats. idea, that changes in the body caused by stress could be
beneficial, is a recurring theme in contemporary psycho-
Selye’s General Adaptation Syndrome physiological theory and one we will return to often in this
chapter.
One of the pioneers in stress research, endocrinologist In contemporary psychophysiological research, the
Hans Selye is largely credited with identifying the coordi- word “stress” is rarely used, most likely because of
nated bodily responses that occur with stress (Selye, 1956). the weak inference and fuzzy construct it represents.
As a medical student in 1925 at the University of Prague, Instead, contemporary researchers use “allostasis” as
Selye noted that stress responses could influence the body a general term to represent changes in the body from
in general ways. Although patients differed in primary homeostasis. Allostasis is a combination of the Greek word
diagnoses, they shared common symptoms and identifi- allos or “variable” and statis or “stable,” thus “remaining
able manifestations; for example, they looked and felt ill, stable by being variable.” Allostasis refers to the body’s
their tongues were coated, they experienced loss of appe- response to external demands by activating the fight-or-
tite, and reported pain in their joints. Even though a com- flight response and is viewed as the body’s “swift and effi-
mon disease was not inflicting these patients, they shared cient method of dealing with danger” (McEwen, 2002, p. 5).
similar symptoms suggesting a general bodily response In contrast, the term that is used to highlight the deleteri-
that co-occurred with specific diseases of the body. ous effects on the body is “allostatic load” and is reserved
Selye’s early observations were confirmed a decade for occasions when the allostasis response is not turned off
later when he was attempting to identify a new ovar- and the body begins to break down defenses.
ian hormone. In these experiments, he injected rats with
a variety of hormonal extracts, but apparently being a
Psychophysiological Models of Hypothalamic-
bit short on dexterity, Selye struggled with injecting
Pituitary-Adrenal and Sympathetic Adrenal
the rats and would often end up dropping and chasing the
Medullary Responses
rats around the laboratory. When he autopsied the rats,
regardless of the extract (or placebo) injected, all the rats The importance of James’s, Cannon-Bard’s, and Selye’s
had developed the same triadic syndrome that included theories is unquestionable in terms of generating attention
enlargement of the adrenal cortex, shrinking of lymphatic to the links between mental states and bodily responses,
structures, and bleeding ulcers. Apparently, Selye had in contrast with James, however, the latter two theories
created his own laboratory-based stressor and his rats treated changes in physiological responses as nonspecific.
showed evidence of chronic stress. Selye recalled his Little attention was paid to the different eliciting conditions,
early observations as a medical student and surmised that and thus little or no specificity was identified regarding
the triadic syndrome of bodily response was meaningful, the mental states that preceded the physiological changes.
and that such responses to general or nonspecific demands In subsequent years, several theories attempted to remedy
might explain a general sickness behavior. Thus, the gen- this problem by focusing on relative changes in the two
eral adaptation syndrome (GAS) was born, which Selye primary stress systems described earlier and by defining
argued was a universal response to stress that included which psychological antecedents were associated with
coordinated changes in the body. specific profiles of activation.
The GAS identifies three stages of stress. The first For example, Frankenhäuser (1986) examined changes
stage is alarm, in which the body signals the fight-or- in epinephrine (indexing SAM activation) and cortisol
flight response. Under continued stress, resistance follows, (indexing HPA activation), and focused on individuals’ per-
which results in the body trying to cope or adapt with the ceptions of effort and distress. She theorized that different
new demands. If the stress continues, the bodily demands levels of effort and distress would result in unique physio-
are depleted and the final stage, exhaustion, occurs. Selye logical patterns related to SAM and HPA activation. When
argued that, in the latter stage, the body is most susceptible individuals were distressed but no effort was expended,
to new illnesses or exacerbation of existing diseases. By cortisol production was hypothesized to increase together
adopting the GAS model, many researchers explicitly or with small increases in epinephrine production. When
implicitly argued that stress responses were unidirectional individuals felt distressed and exerted effort, both epineph-
and assumed that all stress responses were the same. rine and cortisol responses increase, though epinephrine
Although Selye may have influenced researchers to predominated. Finally, when individuals expended effort
focus on the damaging effects of stress, he also emphasized but there was not a concomitant experience of distress,
the importance of differentiating harmful and damaging epinephrine production would increase, but cortisol would

CH06.indd 204 12/18/09 2:37:02 PM


Evolution of Social Psychophysiological Theories 205

not. Theoretically, these patterns of responses map onto a associated with adaptive functioning, improved immune
continuum of helplessness (distress without effort) to mas- system, and better performance.
tery (effort without distress), with the question of perceived Incorporating animal and human studies, Dienstbier ’s
control influencing where one fell on the continuum. premise was that small stressors experienced by an
A similar model was proposed by Henry (1980, 1986), animal or human could “toughen” them so that subsequent
who also focused on control as a critical psychological fac- stressors were better coped with physiologically. This
tor that determined patterned physiological responding. perspective was in sharp contrast with stress models that
In this model, defense was linked to SAM activation and assumed that all peripheral increases signified damage and
defeat to HPA. Henry further differentiated defense into impaired cognitive performance. For example, Dienstbier
control and striving. Specifically, control indicated when a reviewed evidence that strongly questioned the commonly
potential threat was effectively managed and under control, held belief that arousal would be related to cognitive or
whereas striving indicated a threat that still needed active behavioral performance in a curvilinear relation (simi-
management. Henry’s model produced three distinct psy- lar to the Yerkes-Dodson principle). Instead, he observed
chological states and multiple outcomes related to behavior, strong linear relations between higher catecholamines and
emotion, neuroendocrine, and autonomic responses. His better cognitive and physical performance. Most typi-
control state was believed to result in aggressive behavior cally, greater catecholamine increases from baseline were
toward the potential threat, elation and loss of anxiety, and associated with better math performance among students
increases in norepinephrine, testosterone, blood pressure, (especially for epinephrine relative to norepinephrine).
and cardiac output. In contrast, the striving defense was Physical performance also yielded a similar finding:
characterized by struggle, tension, fear, and high epineph- Greater increases in catecholamines were associated with
rine levels, moderate increases in cortisol, and moderate better technical competence among military paratroopers
blood pressure and cardiac output increases. Finally, loss in training.
of control or the defeat response was characterized as help- In contrast, strong evidence has been reported that
lessness, anxiety, depression, high cortisol and low testos- end products of HPA activation, specifically cortisol, do
terone, and little or no changes in autonomic reactivity. show an inverted U-relation with performance (Lovallo &
Although the comparison between Frankenhäuser Thomas, 2000). For example, memory is improved when
and Henry highlights the fact that the profiles of physi- there are small increases in cortisol, but impaired at higher
ological responses tend to be similar across theories, what levels of cortisol. Disease models and neural regions associ-
differentiates the theories are the specific psychological ated with memory provide insight into this finding. Patients
antecedents that best represent the eliciting conditions with Cushing disease, characterized by an overactive HPA
associated with the physiological profiles. The profiles response, resulting in chronically high levels of circulating
that Frankenhäuser and Henry outlined can be seen in glucocorticoid responses, tend to have poor memory and
whole or in part in many subsequent theories, all of which smaller hippocampi because of degeneration. The hippo-
have their own explication of the experiential component campus, strongly linked to memory, has high-affinity and
that brings about these profiles. low-affinity receptors for cortisol. At low levels of corti-
sol production, high-affinity receptors are activated, which
Dienstbier ’s Theory of Physiological Toughness improves memory, but at higher levels or chronically acti-
Similar to Frankenhäuser and Henry, Richard Dienstbier vated cortisol, low-affinity receptors are activated, which
focused on the possibility that not all changes in physio- can impair memory (Reul & de Kloet, 1985).
logical responses in a potentially distressful or threatening The physiologically tough pattern extends to better
situation should be viewed negatively and took seriously psychological adjustment as well. Greater increases in cat-
Selye’s notion of eustress. Even with psychoneuroendocri- echolamines from resting levels are associated with ego
nologists attempting to differentiate SAM and HPA acti- strength, stress tolerance, low neuroticism, and daily stress
vation, relatively little attention was paid to beneficial or scores. In contrast, clinically anxious individuals respond
adaptive physiological changes, and the prevailing zeitgeist to laboratory challenges with lower sympathetic nervous
of the 1970s and 1980s was that increases in peripheral system (SNS) increases.
physiological responses were associated with negative psy- What characterizes a physiologically tough (adaptive)
chological states and inherently did damage to biological response from a weak (maladaptive) one? Much of the dif-
systems, as well as impair cognition. Richard Dienstbier ’s ferences lie in the baseline or resting levels, and in the tem-
physiological toughness model (1989) directly ques- poral trajectory once the organism is exposed to a stressor.
tioned these “negative views of arousal,” and he reviewed In Figure 6.4, the hypothesized changes in end products of
the many and varied ways peripheral changes could be SAM and HPA activation (specifically SNS changes and

CH06.indd 205 12/18/09 2:37:03 PM


206 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

Theoretical HPA activation


Theoretical SAM activation

Adaptive Figure 6.4 Hypothesized activation of


sympathetic adrenal medullary (SAM) and
Maladpative
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal cortical (HPA)
responses from Dienstbier’s model of physi-
Rest Stressor Habituation Recovery Rest Stressor Habituation Recovery ological toughness.

cortisol responses, respectively) are depicted to contrast long (i.e., 7 s), heart rate decreased and muscle activity
adaptive profiles from maladaptive responses. (in the neck, arm, and chin) increased, and these responses
For example, Dienstbier (1989) described an adaptive were correlated (i.e., the lower the heart rate the greater the
SAM profile to include low resting baseline responses muscle activity). However, with shorter intertrial intervals
(e.g., low heart rate, low levels of epinephrine), a strong (e.g., 1 s), a correlation also existed between cardiac and
and immediate increase in response to a stressor, followed somatic (body) responses, but in this case, both responses
by quick habituation and recovery. In contrast, an adap- increased: increased heart rate and increased muscle
tive HPA response is characterized by low cortisol, small activity. These data were critical in establishing that bio-
increases after the initiation of a stressor, and quick habitu- logical systems could respond in a coordinated fashion;
ation and recovery. When examining a maladaptive profile, however, the context could change the relationship.
the resting and response reactions are quite different. First, But even the coupled relationship between cardiac
in terms of SAM levels, there are higher resting levels of (heart) and somatic (body) activity at short and long
catecholamines, at the onset of the stressor there is a slug- intervals proved to be dependent on situational factors.
gish increase in activation, an increase in activation over In later research, Obrist (e.g., 1976) established that
the course of the task (rather than habituation), and then a cardiac-somatic coupling occurred in the context of pas-
sluggish recovery once the stressor is complete. The HPA sive stressors—for example, during aversive events that
response that characterizes a malignant profile, accord- were minimally involving or engaging—primarily because
ing to Dienstbier, is increased cortisol level at rest, a large in these situations the heart is controlled by vagal (i.e.,
increase in cortisol level in response to a stressor, together parasympathetic) innervation. Regarding the physiological
with sluggish habituation and recovery. changes in passive coping tasks (such as aversive shock),
Obrist commented that they were “biologically trivial
Obrist’s Somatic Coupling/Uncoupling and . . . tell little about the psychological state of the organ-
Arguably one of the most important psychophysiologists ism other than . . . momentarily suspended somatic activ-
of the 20th century was Paul Obrist. His varied and far- ity” (p. 103). Indeed, he speculated that the cardiac-somatic
reaching contributions to CV psychophysiology included coupling might simply be caused by momentary changes
using pharmacological agents to constrain physiological in posture and body positioning, rather than a function of
responses, exploring psychological precursors of essential a psychological or experiential response. In contrast, Obrist
hypertension, and highlighting the importance of context was quite optimistic about the value of physiological
when examining the link between psychology and physiol- changes brought about during active tasks, or ones that
ogy. Indeed, in his presidential address to the Society for allowed an individual to act or cope. Not only were changes
Psychophysiological Research, Obrist (1976) claimed that, in cardiac responses much larger than those seen in passive
“Cannon was right, he just failed to understand situational tasks, but now cardiac and somatic activity were uncou-
specificity” (p. 103). pled. Changes in body positioning could no longer explain
In his early work, Obrist (1968) published evidence changes in cardiac responses; thus, the psychological state
that cardiac changes were concomitant with somatic- of the individual was the likely candidate for the observed
motor (i.e., bodily movement) changes. Using aversive physiological changes.
conditioning paradigms, Obrist exposed participants to Context distinctions are critical for understanding mind-
paired trials of light (conditioned stimulus [CS]) and shock body interactions for several reasons. For one, the func-
(unconditioned stimulus [UCS]), but also varied the delay tions of the SNS are primarily for fight or flight, but in our
between the CS and UCS. When the intertrial interval was modern age, we often experience SNS changes when no or

CH06.indd 206 12/18/09 2:37:03 PM


Evolution of Social Psychophysiological Theories 207

minimal metabolic demand is required. Obrist’s point was can be compromised (e.g., cognitive load) and situational
that there was little, if any, value in examining peripheral demands will be increased, and all else being equal, a per-
physiology during passive stressors, and that any changes son will experience a threat state.
seen represent somatic or motoric changes that are inde- If the demand construct was all there was to the theory,
pendent of psychological experience. Instead, active tasks, the Challenge and Threat model would be closer to Selye’s
which require an opportunity to cope with environmental GAS model, which highlighted stress factors associated
or internal demands, would result in meaningful periph- with activation and exhaustion of bodily states. However,
eral changes that would correspond to psychological even in the presence of elevated demands (e.g., danger,
states. Obrist’s observation is critically important when we uncertainty, and required effort), increases in resources can
review emotion and motivational research that has occa- offset demands and ultimately produce challenge states.
sionally ignored this distinction often to the detriment of These resource factors include but are not limited to dis-
research progress. positions, knowledge and abilities, and external support.
Dispositions, such as optimism, control, and self-esteem,
Blascovich and Colleagues’ Biopsychosocial Model can be construed as personal resources that can be brought
of Challenge and Threat to bear on a task (see Tomaka & Blascovich, 1994; Seery,
One of the direct descendants of Dienstbier ’s distinction Blascovich, Weisbuch, & Vick, 2004). For example, indi-
of physiological toughness (vs. weakness) and Obrist’s viduals who score high in a Belief in a Just World typically
specification of active coping states is Challenge and experience challenge states when exposed to an ambiguous
Threat Theory (Blascovich & Mendes, 2000; Blascovich & social stressor compared with those low in Belief in a Just
Tomaka, 1996; Tomaka et al., 1993; cf. Wright & Kirby, World, who typically experience more threat. How indi-
2003). This theory states that in situations that are active viduals evaluate their knowledge and abilities can influ-
and goal relevant (based on Obrist’s notion of active tasks), ence resources as well, especially when the knowledge is
a combination of evaluations of situational demands and relevant to the task at hand (Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, &
personal resources to cope produce distinct psychological Salomon, 1999). For example, an advanced degree in
states. At the most general level, when evaluated demands math during the final Jeopardy round would be helpful if
are greater than perceived resources to cope, individuals the category is Euclidian geometry, less so if the category
experience a psychological state of threat. In contrast, is Adriatic port cities. In this case, it is anticipated that
when resources are greater than demands, individuals greater knowledge results in greater evaluated resources.
experience a challenge state (see Lazarus & Folkman, The final identified resource is social support, which can
1991). Of course, many factors influence the evaluated increase feelings of security and hence result in increased
demands and resources in a motivated performance situa- perceived resources.
tion. Factors that have been identified as likely to increase The physiological changes that co-occur with the onset
demand evaluations include danger, uncertainty, novelty, of challenge and threat states have focused primarily on
and required effort, and there are likely additional ones not CV changes. Specifically, CV responses exhibited during
yet specified (Blascovich & Mendes, 2000). challenge states tend to be associated with greater SAM
For example, danger, either psychological or physi- activation, thus increasing ventricular contractility, car-
cal, can be signaled in multiple ways during an active, diac efficiency, and vasodilation in the arterioles, which
goal-relevant task. At a psychological level, danger can provides greater blood flow to the periphery. The CV pat-
be associated with fear of negative evaluation, such as an tern of reactivity associated with challenge is similar (if
unsympathetic or hostile audience during a talk. Physical not identical) to the pattern-1 (activational) response iden-
danger, though rare in active coping tasks used in the labo- tified by medical researchers (see Brownley, Hurwitz, &
ratory (studies using electrical shock are done so in passive Schneiderman, 2000). Similar to challenge states, threat
coping situations with no opportunity for escape), also con- states also are characterized by an increase in sympa-
ceivably would increase demand evaluations. Situational thetic activation, but in contrast with challenge states,
novelty increases demand evaluations, whereas situational threat states consist of less efficient cardiac responses and
familiarity decreases demand evaluations. Finally, effort vasoconstriction. Similar to the pattern-2 (inhibitional)
also has an effect on demand evaluations. In some cases, response, CV reactivity during threat states is believed to
effort can be viewed as strictly metabolic—as physical be associated with avoidant and defeat-related motivation
demands are placed on metabolic output the total demands (Brownley et al., 2000).
are likely to be exceeded. But effort can also relate to Although challenge and threat can be linked to high
strictly cognitive demands. As more information or dis- “arousal” positive and negative emotional states, respectively,
traction in the environment is introduced, internal states more accurately, the states are proposed as motivational

CH06.indd 207 12/18/09 2:37:03 PM


208 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

rather than affective (Blascovich, 2008; Herrald & Tomaka, oblongata and extends to the face, heart, stomach, and
2002; Mendes, Major, McCoy, & Blascovich, 2008), most abdominal organs. One way to examine vagal
and thus are orthogonal to valence. As such, challenge nerve influences on the heart is by examining variability
states index approach motivation and not necessarily of the cardiac cycle. Initially, heart rate variability was
positive emotion. One implication of this is that approach- believed to be a measurement artifact or nuisance, but
negative emotions, such as anger, should be more closely further exploration into spontaneous changes in the tim-
related to challenge than threat. ing of the heart cycle proved to be psychologically and
Table 6.1 presents the psychological states of challenge physiologically meaningful. Though disagreements still
and threat together with the emotional, motivational, and occur on the specifics related to measurement, quantifi-
physiological responses that differentiate these psycho- cation, and psychological meaningfulness of vagal tone
logical states. Notably, the general profile of threat states, and cardiac vagal reactivity (see Porges, 2007), these
in terms of its physiological concomitants, is considerably measures might prove to be especially important for
more pernicious in the amount of damage the physiologi- social and personality psychologists interested in emo-
cal response could enact on the CV system. Indeed, those tion or mental effort.
who experience more threat responses compared with One theory that has received much attention in terms of
challenge responses during stressful laboratory tasks show the psychological inferences one can draw from vagal nerve
accelerated biological aging measured with telomerase, influences is Porges’ polyvagal theory (e.g., Porges, 2007).
an enzyme that protects chromosomal DNA during The polyvagal theory specifies that heart rate variability,
replication, which has been directly linked to disease out- measured as respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), indexes
comes (see Mendes & Epel, 2009). a specific branch of the vagus, unique to primates and not
found in reptiles (but see Grossman & Taylor, 2007), which
Porges’ Polyvagal Theory has evolved as part of the social engagement system. One
Whereas challenge and threat theory focuses exclusively of the postulates of polyvagal theory is that social factors
on differentiating responses associated with SNS activa- (affiliation, social engagement, self-conscious emotions),
tion, there is growing interest in the responses associated personality factors (pessimism, self-esteem, mindfulness),
with parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) changes and clinical factors (depression, autism) can modulate
and specifically the role of the vagus nerve (also known vagal activity.
as “cranial nerve X”), which originates in the medulla Specifically, Porges argues that higher RSA at rest
(higher cardiac vagal tone) can be used as an index of
adaptive emotional regulation and responsiveness to the
social environment. Similarly, cardiac vagal reactivity
Table 6.1 Emotional, Motivational, and Behavioral Responses
Associated with Psychological States of Challenge and Threat (changes in RSA) might also index appropriate social
engagement in that increased vagal reactivity during a
Challenge Threat
task might be associated with calmness, equanimity, and
Evaluations: Evaluations: a lack of distress.
Resources > Demands Resources < Demands Though most work has focused on resting cardiac vagal
Affect: Affect:
tone and its links to dispositions and responses to social
Pride/↑ self-esteem/anger Shame/anxiety/↓ self-esteem
Autonomic nervous system (ANS) ANS reactivity: and emotional situations, there is also a growing literature
reactivity: on cardiac vagal reactivity—focusing on RSA changes—
↑ CO (cardiac output) No change in CO and vagal rebound. Vagal rebound is the extent to which
↑ VC (ventricle contractility) ↑ VC
↓ TPR (total peripheral resistance) ↑ TPR RSA responses return to or even overshoot baseline lev-
Behavior: Behavior: els after suppression of the vagal brake (i.e., decreases in
Open posture, leaning toward Rigid, freeze, withdraw, closed vagal activity).
approach posture, moving away from
stimulus, defeat
Though typically vagal responses have been linked to
Recovery: Recovery: emotional or attentional processes, a recent theory has
Quick sympathetic nervous Slow SNS, PNS, and cortisol linked self-esteem with vagal tone (Martens, Greenberg, &
system (SNS) recovery
Allen, in press). This theory argues that high vagal tone
recovery, vagal rebound
(parasympathetic nervous and high self-esteem buffer individuals from experiences
system [PNS] activity of threat. These researchers then draw specific links to
overshooting baseline levels), suggest that these two outcomes, high vagal tone and high
and quick cortisol recovery
self-esteem, might be linked. For example, they point out
Note: ↑= increases;↓ = decreases. that depression and sadness can co-occur with low levels

CH06.indd 208 12/18/09 2:37:04 PM


Evolution of Social Psychophysiological Theories 209

of vagal tone, and that vagal nerve stimulation, resulting tasks, sympathetic and parasympathetic systems tend to
in an increase in vagal tone, can reduce depressive symp- be co-activated rather than reciprocal. Hence, one cannot
tomatology. These authors also present preliminary evi- simply look at the outcome, heart rate deceleration, and
dence showing that increases in self-esteem can result in conclude that a psychological state was not present. One
increased vagal reactivity. As intriguing as this theory is, it has to consider both the psychophysiological theory and
remains to be seen how strong and specific the relationship the context of the task, both of which can influence SNS
is between self-esteem and vagal tone. and PNS responding.
Adding some complexity to both polyvagal theory and The doctrine of autonomic space provides a critical
the link between self-esteem and vagal tone, however, specification of the relationship between the sympathetic
is the nature of the social context and its influence on vagal and parasympathetic systems. However, the potential
responses. Indeed, in highly stressful situations or tasks exploitation of this model for developing strong inferen-
that require some amount of mental attention or effort, one tial psychophysiological theories has yet to be realized.
expects a withdrawal of the vagal brake (resulting in lower Because of the critical importance of context in the doctrine
RSA) to indicate greater attentional control and effort. of autonomic space, perhaps future social psychophysiolo-
Indeed, cognitive psychophysiologists have used decreases gists will take advantage of this model to develop theories
in RSA as an index of attention or mental effort for several linking psychological states to physiological responses
years (Tattersall & Hockey, 1995). associated with the autonomic space model.

Cacioppo and Colleagues’ Doctrine Dickerson & Kemeny’s Neuroendocrine Model


of Autonomic Space of Social Evaluative Threat
Though most of the psychophysiological theories of Returning to the other stress system, the HPA, a recent the-
the 20th century acknowledged the interplay between ory proposes the importance of examining how different
sympathetic and parasympathetic branches, these theories social factors and eliciting conditions could activate this
treated the relationship between these two branches of the system. To examine the sensitivity of the HPA system to
ANS as lying on a single continuum of reciprocity—as one various social factors, Dickerson and Kemeny (2004) con-
system increased the other decreased. Indeed, the view that ducted a meta-analysis of 208 studies that examined cor-
these systems have a reciprocal relationship is an enduring tisol increases in the context of a laboratory stressor to
myth still promulgated in some physiological textbooks. test their social evaluative threat theory. This theoretical
The psychophysiological team of Gary Berntson, John model specifies that during motivated performance situa-
Cacioppo, and Karen Quigley (1991) argued convincingly tions (i.e., situations likely to engender SAM activation),
against this doctrine, and showed that there were multiple specific contextual and experiential factors will activate
modes of autonomic control and the standard reciproc- HPA responses, specifically perceived uncontrollability of
ity principle had limited utility. Their autonomic space the situation and the extent to which the person’s social
model outlined a two-dimensional space that includes the esteem is threatened. Social evaluative threat hypothesizes
various relationships between sympathetic and parasympa- that situations that could possibly lead to failure or loss of
thetic responses, which can be reciprocal, uncoupled, or social esteem would be more likely to activate the social
co-activated. This distinction is important for reconciling preservation system indicated by activation of the HPA.
uncoupled relationships between the SNS and PNS. For The results of the meta-analysis strongly supported the
example, the orienting response is characterized by cardiac theory and showed that strong situational effects were
deceleration (increased PNS), pupil dilation (decreased related to cortisol increases. When studies examined cor-
PNS), and increases in electrodermal responses (increased tisol changes during passive tasks, even ones with aversive
SNS). If the systems were completely reciprocal, there elements such as shock, noise exposure, or watching a scary
would be great difficulty in explaining reactions to an ori- movie, they did not observe increases in cortisol level;
enting response. indeed, the grand mean across the studies showed a decrease
Understanding the multiple modes of autonomic control in cortisol from baseline to after the task when the task was
sheds light on how situational specificity can differentially passive (likely representing the circadian decreases of cor-
influence ANS responding. For example, the orienting tisol throughout the day). Also, the presence of a motivated
response is typically associated with heart rate decelera- performance situation (i.e., an active task) was not sufficient
tion. If one examines heart rate during a task designed to by itself to increase cortisol responses; again, the grand
be an orienting task and observes no deceleration of heart mean of cortisol changes was less than zero. Instead, when
rate, they might conclude that the orientating response was motivated performance situations included an element of
not present. However, during novel compared with familiar social evaluative threat—potential loss of social esteem—or

CH06.indd 209 12/18/09 2:37:04 PM


210 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

an uncontrollable task, the authors observed significant to fight, flee, or cope in any active way. This state might
increases in cortisol. Uncontrollability and social evalua- be best characterized by defeat or helplessness, which
tive threat also appeared to have independent and additive includes stress signals but without behavioral intentions to
effects on cortisol increases, because when these factors change or modify the situation or the experience.
were examined collectively, the highest cortisol responses
occurred when motivated performance situations included
USES OF PERIPHERAL
both social evaluative threat and uncontrollability.
NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL INDEXES
IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
Summary
As described earlier, psychophysiology can play important
Many of the psychophysiological theories described earlier
theoretical and empirical roles in social psychology. In the
(a nonexhaustive subset of all such theories) involve the
previous section, we described the evolution of social psy-
effects and interplay of two neurophysiological systems. In
chophysiological theories. In this section, we focus more
terms of indexing social psychological constructs, research
on the empirical role of physiological indexes for social
supporting some of these theories measured SAM changes
psychology, ones that provide valid and powerful empiri-
more directly and inferred HPA responses (e.g., challenge
cal physiological indexes of social psychological and
and threat theory; but see Mendes, Ayduk, Akinola, & Epel,
related constructs. This focus is not meant to imply that
2009); other theories measured HPA changes directly and
the theoretical and empirical roles are independent; indeed,
inferred SAM activation (e.g., social evaluative threat).
they are very much related. Nor is our selection of useful
Overall, however, there is much agreement regarding the
peripheral physiological indexes meant to be exhaustive.
physiological and biological outcomes when these systems
For example, we did not include important work that has
are activated, but the eliciting conditions and psychological
relied on physiological measures in the development of the-
states that co-occur or engender these responses are still
ory such as emotion regulation/suppression, effort mobili-
debated. Table 6.2 organizes the various theories by using
zation, decision-making processes, self-enhancement, or
the relative activation of the two neuroendocrine systems
aggression. Many of these topics are covered in other
with the various psychological elicitors.
chapters (e.g., see Bushman, volume 2; Keltner & Lerner,
For example, at high levels of SAM and low levels of
this volume; and Taylor, this volume). In this section, we
HPA activation, the psychological experiences tend to have
begin by reviewing intrapersonal processes, such as affect,
a flavor of eustress and approach-related behavior that
attitudes, emotion, consciousness, and self-control and
might be associated with dominance and control. In con-
self-regulatory processes. We then move to interpersonal
trast, high SAM and high HPA tends to characterize dis-
and intergroup processes such as social facilitation, social
tress and threat, and possibly submissiveness and lack of
interactions, discrimination, intergroup interactions, and
control. In contrast, the final column identifies high HPA
social support processes.
responses with low SAM activation. In laboratory settings
focused on human participants, this might be the most dif-
Affect
ficult combination to study because it requires activation
of the more conservative neurophysiological systems (e.g., Among the superordinate constructs central to social psy-
HPA) without engagement of the SAM activation—in other chology is affect. Social psychologists have been concerned
words, strong alarm signals without the organism trying with positive and negative affect in many contexts including

Table 6.2 Mental States Associated with Activation of the Sympathetic Adrenal Medullary and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal
Cortical Axes

SAM: Medium to large increases SAM: Medium to large increases SAM: No to small increases

HPA: No to small increases HPA: Medium to large increases HPA: Medium to large increases

Effort without distress Effort and distress Helplessness


Challenge Threat Defeat
Defense Self-preservation theory Withdrawal
Anger Striving for control
Being in control Uncertainty
Fear

HPA, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal; SAM, sympathetic adrenal medullary.

CH06.indd 210 12/18/09 2:37:05 PM


Uses of Peripheral Neurophysiological Indexes in Social Psychology 211

attitude, emotion, and well-being. Affect might best be unpleasant pictures. Furthermore, as the normative arousal
measured using facial EMG, which requires placement of rating of the pictures increased, the difference between
sensors over muscle regions to detect electrical activity to the blink magnitudes was exacerbated, with very pleas-
indicate contractions of muscles. With EMG, one of the ant pictures attenuating startle and very unpleasant pic-
more useful measures of affect is startle modulation, which tures potentiating blink responses to auditory startles (see
can provide a powerful and elegant tool to differentiate Bradley, 2000, for a review).
positive and negative affect. Measuring affective states using a startle modulation
Startle modulation theory (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, paradigm requires the use of startle probes; however, there
1990) relies on the observation that most mammals have are circumstances in which researchers want to glean
a strong defensive reflex to a sudden, unexpected sensory affective response without using a probe as an elicitor. In
event. This reflex takes the form of a full-body startle these circumstances, we can still look to the face to pro-
reflex and can include, but is not limited to, a sudden and vide indications of emotional state. One commonly used
strong eye-blink response, retraction of the head, tensing of method to examine more specific emotional responses
muscles, and deceleration of heart rate. The sensory events is measuring changes in specific muscle activation in
that elicit these responses can be audible, visual, or tactile. the face corresponding to facial expressions of emo-
It was first noted by Brown, Kalish, and Farber (1951) that tions. Ekman and Tomkins’s detailed taxonomy of the
animals under threat of attack or pain showed an exagger- facial muscles relating to emotional expressions provides
ated startle reflex. social psychophysiologists a useful map to determine
Capitalizing on this finding and other findings demon- placement of facial EMG sensors to detect small changes
strating potentiated startle responses to negative-affective in muscles related to positive or negative affect, as well
stimuli, as well as attenuated startle responses on exposure as specific emotional expressions such as disgust and fear
to interesting (compared with dull) stimuli, Bradley, Lang, (Fridlund & Cacioppo, 1986). Capitalizing on this tech-
and Cuthbert proposed a model of startle reflex modula- nique, Cacioppo, Petty, Losch, and Kim (1986) used facial
tion (e.g., Bradley, Cuthbert, & Lang, 1990; Bradley & EMG to examine affective responses to valenced stimuli
Lang, 2007; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1997). This model that differed in intensity. Muscle region activity differed by
examines how concomitant emotional experiences can valence and intensity: Brow muscles responded to negative
modulate the startle reflex. Specifically, startle modulation stimuli, whereas cheek muscles showed greater activity to
theory posits that because emotions are organized superor- positive stimuli. Some evidence also existed that intensity
dinately in terms of affective states, and the startle reflex of stimuli was monotonically related to targeted muscle
is a hedonically negative response, when there is a match activity: moderately negative stimuli produced more cor-
between superordinate affective states and the hedonic tone rugator activity than mildly negative stimuli. Importantly,
of the reflex—that is, negative emotions and defensive the muscles activated were so small that when observ-
responses—the startle response will be potentiated. In con- ers examined the videotaped responses of participants,
trast, when there exists a mismatch between hedonic tone they could not detect muscle movement that facial EMG
and reflex, there will be attenuation in the startle response. detected.
Support from this theory comes from a variety of experi-
ments that span clinical science, personality, and social Attitudes
contexts. Methodologically, most research uses the eye-
blink response as its primary startle measure most likely Much of social psychological research has been devoted to
because of the ease in which the eye blink response can be the role of attitudes. Indeed, in the Journal of Personality
measured using EMG (recording electrical impulses gener- and Social Psychology, social psychology’s top-tiered
ated by muscle activity to index the onset, strength, and off- journal, one-third of the journal space is dedicated to atti-
set of that muscle activity). To measure the blink response, tudes and social cognition. Here, we illustrate what psy-
researchers place two sensors on the orbicularis oculi chophysiological and embodiment theory and methods
(i.e., the muscle surrounding the eye), typically below the have brought to bear on our understanding of individuals’
eye, and then present startle probes typically in the form evaluation of the positive or negative qualities associated
of a 95-decibel sound burst. Researchers then measure the with an entity.
magnitude and latency of the eye-blink response. The ini- Assessing attitudes or evaluations associated with
tial studies looking at the eye-blink response as a result of attitudinal objects seems like a relatively easy task to
a startle probe found that the magnitude of muscle activ- accomplish—simply ask people what they like or dis-
ity underlying the startle-blink response was smaller when like. However, much attitude research is replete with
participants were viewing pleasant pictures compared with problems associated with this seemingly straightforward

CH06.indd 211 12/18/09 2:37:05 PM


212 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

technique, some of which we have already described: Scale), activity around the brow muscles increased
demand characteristics, social desirability, need for intro- when exposed to Black targets relative to White targets.
spection, and online continuous recording. Indeed, this last Measuring facial muscles was especially informative in
obstacle, obtaining online changes in attitudes, has been this research because the physiological measures “told” a
addressed using nonphysiological solutions. During elec- different story than the self-reported measures. In all three
tion seasons, a ubiquitous sight on television coverage of studies, participants’ self-reported liking of Black targets
presidential debates is a continuous, online, horizontally was greater than their reported liking of White targets.
scrolling waveform that represents shifts in viewers’ atti- A subsequent examination of affect measured with
tudes during the candidates’ answers and comments. Such facial EMG underscored the predictive validity of these
“audience response systems” allow observers to track how measures (Vanman, Saltz, Nathan, & Warren, 2004). In
viewers are feeling at any given moment toward a candi- this study, participants chose one of three students for a
date’s speech. Though such online devices represent prodi- prestigious teaching award. The students’ pictures (two
gious leaps in advancing election research, they still suffer White and one Black students, same sex as the partici-
from at least two intractable problems. First, respondents pant) were presented together with other favorable infor-
can still mask their “true” attitudes, indeed even deliber- mation in a folder. The students were later recruited for a
ately overcorrecting by indicating that they are more (or second “unrelated” study in which their facial EMG was
less) persuaded than they actually are (Mendes & Koslov, recorded while simply looking at White and Black faces
2009). The second problem is that participants have to and evaluating how friendly they appeared. They found
continuously introspect about how they feel so that they that facial muscle activity predicted the race of the candi-
can indicate their attitudes. It is unclear what the process date chosen. Specifically, when participants showed more
of conscious introspection does, and indeed, the data are cheek activity to White over Black faces, they were
equivocal on this point (e.g., Lieberman et al., 2007). Both more likely to choose a White candidate. When no detect-
of these problems can and have been overcome with the able cheek bias was present (responses to White and Black
use of peripheral psychophysiological methods in attitude faces were not different in terms of the activity at the zygo-
research. maticus major muscle), participants were more likely to
Although early on attitude researchers attempted to use choose the Black candidate.
heart rate and skin conductance changes as indexes of atti- Racial attitudes were also the focus of a study using star-
tudes, these endeavors were inherently flawed and made tle responses on exposure to ingroup (White) or outgroup
little progress (e.g., Rankin & Campbell, 1955). This is not (Black) targets (Amodio, Harmon-Jones, & Devine, 2003).
surprising given that, as described earlier, changes in heart Unlike the previous studies, individual differences in atti-
rate are multiply determined and skin conductance changes tudes were measured with a standard questionnaire, then
can be brought on by multiple psychological states that are each participant’s startle-blink response was measured and
not necessarily valenced and, as such, provided weak infer- used as an indication of their automatic evaluation of the
ences of attitudes. This does not mean that more complex target. This investigation also manipulated the latencies of
ANS measures are inappropriate for studying attitudes (as the startle probe with the justification that at shorter laten-
we will review later). However, the most successful psy- cies (400 ms) between target presentation and startle probe,
chophysiological indexes of attitudes have capitalized on controlled processes could not be used and only automatic
facial EMG and startle modulation, both of which provide attentional responses would be observed. In contrast, at
information along valence and intensity dimensions. longer latencies between target and probe (4000 ms),
controlled processes are theoretically available and, con-
Facial Electromyography sequently, a different pattern of eye-blink responses were
In the late 1990s, Vanman and his colleagues exploited predicted that captured affective responses. In the longer
facial EMG to test subtle affect changes on exposure to latency conditions, participants’ racial bias predicted star-
White and Black target faces with the expressed motive tle responses. Specifically, for those who internalized their
of imagining completing a cooperative task with the tar- motivation not to be prejudiced and were not concerned
get person (Vanman, Paul, Ito, & Miller, 1997). Across with external pressure to appear unprejudiced, there were
three studies, Vanman and colleagues found that White no differences in startle responses to White or Black faces.
participants displayed more negative affect (a compos- However, for those lower in internal motivation not to be
ite of corrugator [brow] and zygomaticus major [cheek] prejudiced or high on both motivations, startle responses
activity) when exposed to Black targets compared with were greater to Black faces than to White faces.
White targets. In addition, they found that at greater levels These examples focus on affective responses that might
of explicit racial bias (measured with the Modern Racism be influenced or shaped by particular attitudes one holds.

CH06.indd 212 12/18/09 2:37:06 PM


Uses of Peripheral Neurophysiological Indexes in Social Psychology 213

What is the effect of processing new information for attitudes not effortlessly and chronically accessible, quick decision
that are not well developed? Bombarded with information making is difficult and potentially “stressful.”
from our social world, we often have to process multiple To explore the idea that attitude accessibility reduces
sources of information. Information that is easy to process distress during decision making, researchers examined CV
is liked more. Consider the two sentences, “Stormy seas responses during quick decision-making trials as a func-
tossed the boat,” and “Stormy seas tossed the lamp.” Not tion of chronically accessible or inaccessible attitudes
surprisingly, the former is pronounced faster and judged (Blascovich et al., 1993). Attitudes were induced experi-
more pleasant than the latter. Similar to the mere exposure mentally by having participants rehearse their attitudes to
effect (exposure to a target can increase liking for it), lik- a series of abstract paintings by stating their degree of lik-
ing of target objects can be increased by familiarity, which ing for each painting aloud. After the rehearsal task, par-
makes targets easier to process. Winkielman and Cacioppo ticipants performed a rapid pairwise preference task during
(2001) tested their hedonic fluency model, which is the which they had to choose which of two paintings they pre-
idea that processing ease is associated with positive affect ferred in a repetitive forced-choice task. The participants
regardless of the valence of the judgment context (i.e., had been randomly assigned to making these rapid pair-
focusing on positive vs. negative evaluations). In their first wise judgments between pairs of the familiar paintings
study, they presented neutral line drawings and had par- for which they had well-rehearsed attitudes or between
ticipants rate whether they liked or did not like the draw- pairs of paintings they had not seen before. Physiological
ing (unipolar ratings of affect) while they measured facial responses showed greater sympathetic activation during
EMG linked to positive and negative affect, specifically decision making when participants did not have well-
zygomaticus major activity (positive affect) and corrugator rehearsed attitudes and little to no increases in sympathetic
supercilii activity (negative affect). They also manipulated activation when participants had well-rehearsed attitudes.
whether a subliminal prime (16 ms) presented just before A physiological interpretation of these findings is that when a
the target object matched the configural display of the tar- well-rehearsed attitude is available to the person, the body
get stimulus. Consistent with their hedonic fluency model, does not have to mobilize responses to cope with the poten-
zygomaticus activity was greater when the prime matched tially stressful rapid pairwise preference task. Instead, the
the target outline and when participants were focused on body can maintain homeostasis during the quick decision-
positive evaluation. Self-reported liking of the targets was making task.
also greater in this condition.
In study 2, instead of subliminally priming matched or Emotion
mismatched figures, they increased presentation time in
200-ms intervals—a duration time imperceptible to partici- Lay theories and observations abound regarding how our
pants—so that target pictures were presented at 300, 500, bodies respond to emotional states: hearts pound with
700, or 900 ms. Also, instead of unipolar ratings of evalu- fear, skin burns with anger, palms sweat from anxiety, and
ation, all participants completed bipolar ratings of liking. cheeks turn red from embarrassment. Indeed, the relation-
Again, target pictures that were easier to process—that is, ship between bodily changes and emotional responses
those that were presented at the longest durations—resulted seems so inextricably linked that William James stated,
in more zygomaticus activity compared with pictures pre- “The only emotions I propose expressly to consider here
sented for briefer duration. Taken together, these studies are those that have a distinct bodily expression” (1884,
support the idea that targets that are easy to process are p. 189). Given what appear to be strong links between
associated with positive affect. emotional states and bodily changes, one would think
that linking discrete emotional states to patterned physi-
Cardiovascular Patterns ological responses would be a relatively easy endeavor.
Covertly measuring attitudes with physiological responses, Unfortunately, emotion-specific ANS profiles have proven
specifically facial muscles and the startle response, has quite elusive.
revealed complex relationships between attitude inten- In more modern times, Levenson, Ekman, and their col-
sity and valence, and how they are related to self-reported leagues (Ekman, Levenson, & Friesen, 1983; Levenson,
attitudes. However, there is also the interesting and Ekman, Heider, & Friesen, 1992) led the way in exploring
long-lived question of the function of attitudes. Fazio the link between specific emotions and distinct changes
(1989) argued that the chronic accessibility of an attitude in the ANS. In an early article (Ekman et al., 1983), they
from memory determines the power and attitude function- argued that specific ANS changes would result from
ality. Therefore, well-established or rehearsed attitudes the experience of discrete emotions. Empirically, these
function to ease decision making, and when attitudes are researchers attempted to manipulate specific emotions

CH06.indd 213 12/18/09 2:37:07 PM


214 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

using two techniques. One, the directed facial action task, specificity—for example, heart rate responses tended to
required participants to isolate and manipulate specific be greater for anger than happiness—commonalities in
facial muscles until the facial expression associated with autonomic responses across specific emotions were also
a discrete emotion was formed. Participants were then observed that were predictable by common emotion valence
instructed to maintain these facial expressions while ANS and underlying motivation.
responses were recorded. Their rationale was based on For example, Cacioppo and colleagues (1997, 2000)
embodiment notions (see earlier), similar to the facial feed- reported stronger ANS changes during negative than posi-
back hypothesis (Strack et al., 1988), which argues that the tive emotions, and differential patterning for approach
manipulation of specific facial muscles linked to happiness compared with withdrawal emotions. The emotion elicita-
(e.g., zygomaticus major muscle region), for example, can tion technique also might have mattered. Consistent with
subsequently engender more happiness. More specifically, Obrist’s (1981) distinction between passive and active cop-
if somatic embodiment informs emotional experience, ing situations, relived emotional experiences, a passive task,
then posed facial expressions linked to emotions should was an especially poor manipulation for revealing ANS
increase the target emotion that, in turn, should elicit spe- patterns of emotion specificity. Furthermore, facial muscle
cific patterns of autonomic responses (see also Adelmann & manipulation might reliably influence cognitive compo-
Zajonc, 1989). The second technique Ekman, Levenson, nents associated with emotion, but might be too weak and
and colleagues used to engender emotion was the relived passive to reliably influence somatovisceral changes.
emotional experience task, in which participants are asked In a landmark study, Stemmler (1989) investigated
to imagine specific emotion-eliciting events while periph- emotion-specific ANS responses using a multitrait multi-
eral changes in ANS are recorded. method approach to examine convergent and discriminant
Based on Ekman’s distinctions of basic emotions, validity. In one of his first studies, female participants
Levenson, Ekman, and colleagues (1992, 1993) focused experienced or imagined two target emotions: fear and
on surprise, disgust, sadness, anger, fear, and happiness, anger. To manipulate the experience of fear, researchers had
and measured a variety of ANS responses. Results showed participants sit in a room in which the lights were dimmed,
some support for ANS differences in discrete emotions, a warning was heard that “something will happen that is
especially from responses during the posed facial expres- out of your control,” and participants then listened to Edgar
sions. Specifically, emotional manipulations of happiness, Allen Poe’s “The Fall of the House of Usher,” which was
disgust, and surprise resulted in lower heart rate; fear and mixed with dramatic music (Prokofiev’s 2nd Symphony).
sadness were associated with higher heart rate and lower The fear induction ended with an unexpected darkness
skin temperature; and anger was associated with higher for 1 minute. To manipulate imagined fear, researchers
heart rate and higher skin temperature. asked participants to first recollect and then speak about
To determine some degree of the universality of dis- a frightening life episode. The critical imagined phase
crete emotions influencing patterned ANS responses, occurred after the speech when participants were instructed
Levenson and colleagues trekked to a remote region of to imagine their speech vividly with their eyes closed. To
West Sumatra to examine whether people in a non-Western manipulate the experience of anger, researchers required
culture, specifically the Minangkabau, exhibited similar participants to solve a series of anagrams during which an
patterns of ANS responses to posed facial expressions as experimenter interrupted them and aggressively stated that
Westerners (Levenson et al., 1992). Indeed, patterns of they were not speaking loud enough. The experimenter
ANS responses after discrete posed expressions were simi- interrupted a second time with a brusque “Louder please!”
lar for the Minangkabau to those observed in the United and then a third interruption “Can’t you speak up?” The
States. These researchers viewed this evidence as support imagined anger phase was similar to the imagined fear
for the idea that ANS responses to specific emotions were in which participants first spoke about an “annoying life
universal. episode”; then for the critical imagined phase, participants
Since these early studies, many other researchers have sat quietly with their eyes closed and imagined their story
taken up the challenge and investigated the possibility vividly.
that discrete emotions might be characterized by specific Stemmler found that self-reports of emotional states
patterning of ANS responses. Cacioppo and colleagues were consistent across contexts (imagined or experienced).
conducted a meta-analysis and performed subsequent However, distinct ANS responses associated with specific
updates on the meta-analysis for editions of Handbook emotions occurred only in the experienced condition. In the
of Emotions (Cacioppo, Bernston, Klein, & Poehlmann, imagined condition, discrete emotions resulted in similar
1997; Cacioppo, Bernston, et al., 2000). Though the physiological changes. One explanation of these findings
meta-analytic evidence showed some patterns of emotion follows from Obrist (1981), who suggested that cardiac and

CH06.indd 214 12/18/09 2:37:07 PM


Uses of Peripheral Neurophysiological Indexes in Social Psychology 215

vascular changes associated with active tasks are likely to person’s awareness of actually running), and metaconscious
result in more ANS specificity, whereas passive tasks are (also known as “higher order thought” processes, such as
unlikely to produce patterned responses. A second possible thinking about thinking or experiencing experience; e.g., a
explanation is that the intensity of the emotional experi- person’s wonderment at their awareness of running) exem-
ence drives the ANS patterning. plifies what is known as “the easy” conscious problem.
Subsequently, Stemmler, Heldman, Pauls, and Scherer Importantly, metaconsciousness, an explicit type of
(2001) again compared experienced versus imagined emo- higher order thought (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006), pro-
tions, and again focused on fear and anger, but this time vides humans with the ability to interact mentally not only
they introduced a “control” condition that consisted of with others but also with themselves. Theorists at least
informing participants of everything that would happen in since James have argued that we have many social selves
the emotion eliciting conditions before induction. In other and these reflect the input of others’ views of us (cf. Cooley,
words, participants experienced the context exactly the 1902). More than seven decades after James, Erving
same as in the experienced condition, but they were fully Goffman expanded James’s notions in his Presentation of
informed of all elements before the emotion-eliciting tasks. Self in Everyday Life (1959).
Based on their model of context-deviation specificity, they During the first three quarters of the 20th century, social-
argued that it is necessary to partition the effects associ- psychological aspects of consciousness were examined
ated with the metabolic demands needed to complete the nearly exclusively by sociologists and sociological social
task from the effects of experiencing the emotions. In this psychologists. However, in the late 1970s and early
study, they examined self-reported emotional responses 1980s, social psychologists took up the challenge.
and 29 physiological responses (all somatovisceral) dur- Work by Nisbet and Wilson (Nisbet & Wilson, 1977;
ing fear and anger inductions that were either experienced Wilson & Nisbet, 1978), Schneider and Shiffrin (1977),
or imagined. Consistent with previous research, imagined and Bargh and colleagues (e.g., Uleman & Bargh, 1989)
emotions tend to produce much less ANS differentiation approached some of these issues directly. The published
than experienced ones. Also, informing participants of the debate (1984) in the American Psychologist between
nature of the upcoming manipulation before it reduced both Richard Lazarus and Robert Zajonc on the primacy of affect
the self-reported experience of the emotion and somatovi- versus cognition highlighted the conscious-unconscious
sceral responses relative to the experimental conditions. distinction in social psychology. During the ensuing quar-
Finally, experienced fear and anger did show some differ- ter century, social cognitive psychologists (e.g., Uleman &
entiated ANS responses, though these differences might Bargh, 1989; Wegner, 2002) verified the important roles
have resulted from underlying motivation that differenti- played by unconscious or automatic processes in social
ates anger from fear. behavior. Such work is discussed more fully in other chap-
Although the search for ANS differentiation of discrete ters of this handbook (Dijksterhuis, chapter 7).
emotions has yet to produce reliable findings, many pro- In terms of social neuroscience, most neurophysiological
ponents of embodied emotion specificity simply claim work attempting to distinguish conscious from uncon-
that the reason we have yet to observe reliable differences scious processes has focused on the brain (cf. Gazzaniga,
of biological responses of emotion specificity is that we Ivry, & Mangun, 2002). Also, there is much in the literature
have not used the right tools. This perspective is based hypothesizing and demonstrating unconscious relation-
on the expectation that advances in neurophysiological ships between psychological factors, including social
measurement techniques will eventually reveal clear and psychological ones, and peripheral physiological states
persistent differences in emotions. Whether this claim is related to health (e.g., Ader & Cohen 1975, Friedman &
eventually supported or rejected remains to be seen (see Rosenman, 1974). However, relatively little attention has
Kemeny, 2003; Panksepp, 2007; cf. Barrett et al., 2007). been paid directly to the relationships among peripheral
physiological activity, social psychological stimuli, and
the simple consciousness problem. However, some efforts
Consciousness
have been worth noting.
The meaning of consciousness has been debated for thousands
Misattribution of Arousal
of years, and little consensus exists. What consciousness is,
or consists of, is sometimes called the “hard consciousness Perhaps the most frequent incursion, albeit most likely
problem” (Chalmers, 2007). Distinguishing among processes unwittingly, within social psychophysiology into the realm
that are unconscious (also known as “subconscious,” “pre- of consciousness research (i.e., on the simple conscious-
conscious,” or “automatic” mental processes), conscious ness problem) fits within the general rubric of “misattribu-
(also known as “first order thought” processes; e.g., a tion of arousal,” a descriptor stemming from Schachter ’s

CH06.indd 215 12/18/09 2:37:08 PM


216 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

(1964) two-factor theory of emotion and empirically on the blind selection of the top card from one of four decks
Schachter and Singer ’s (1962) and others’ experiments (for of facedown cards. Depending on the card turned up on
a review, see Cotton, 1981). Unlike embodiment theory, each trial, participants could either win or lose a lot or
in which proponents do not stake a claim to conscious a little bit of money. The decks differed in their propor-
influence of proprioceptive and interoceptive experiences, tions of gain and loss cards, with two decks providing big
misattribution of arousal theory proponents tends toward gains, but also big losses, and the other two decks provid-
a claim of conscious awareness (cf. Schachter & Singer, ing smaller gains, but also smaller losses. Overall, the two
1979, but see Valins, 1966) of phasic, increased neuro- latter decks provided the optimal choice in terms of money
physiological activity (i.e., nonhomeostatic bodily states), won. Participants’ self-reported “hunches” regarding
with little or no consciousness awareness of its cause. the biased nature of the decks of cards began to develop
According to most versions of relevant misattribution the- by the 50th trial (for non-brain-damaged participants).
ories, residual arousal stemming from some prior event or However, SCRs related to the biased nature of the decks
context has an effect on the current context that may (e.g., typically occurred by the 10th trial, thereby preceding con-
Schachter & Singer, 1962; 1979; Zillman & Bryant, 1975) scious awareness by approximately 40 trials. Specifically,
or may not be conscious (e.g., Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). SCRs suggested an intuition of an impending loss as their
Importantly, with few notable exceptions (e.g., Zillmann & hand reached out to the decks with the higher probability of
Bryant, 1975), few of the studies testing misattribution loss before participants’ conscious awareness or hunches
of arousal theories have actually assessed physiologi- of them.
cal responses. Indeed, as Cotton (1981) has written, few
have even assessed participants’ awareness of increases in Self-Control and Self-Regulatory Processes
physiological activity.
Self-regulation is especially well suited for physiologi-
“Unconscious” Stimuli cal investigation. The capacity for self-regulation appears
In more recent work, social psychophysiologists have to be a limited and consumable resource, such that one
examined the effects of unconscious or subliminal act of self-control reduces one’s subsequent self-control
stimuli on motivational states during performance situ- (Baumeister, 1998). Self-regulation is thus vulnerable to
ations (Dijksterhuis, this volume, chapter 7). In two fatigue, and as demands on the executive self increase,
experiments, Weisbuch-Remington, Mendes, Seery, and one is more likely to experience failures of self-regulation:
Blascovich (2005) subliminally presented (i.e., < 20 ms) inability to remain vigilant about the status of one’s goals
positive and negative Christian religious icons (e.g., and to inhibit one’s prepotent responses. But what are the
angels, devils) outside of participants’ conscious aware- underlying mechanisms of self-regulation and loss of regu-
ness. During a potentially threatening speech task that latory strength, and what can peripheral physiology tell us
immediately followed the stimuli presentations, Christian, about self-control and loss of control?
but not non-Christian, participants exhibited patterns of Goal setting and self-enhancement are often consid-
CV responses consistent with challenge and threat moti- ered in the framework of self-regulatory tasks. Some of
vational states (see earlier), particularly when the speech the questions from this line of research include: Do self-
topic required participants to reflect on their own mor- enhancement strategies promote achievement? Are subtle
tality. Nonreligious icons did not influence Christian exaggerations of our accomplishments deliberate obfusca-
participants, and Christian icons did not influence non- tions to convince others that we are better than we know our-
Christian participants’ CV responses. One can conclude selves to be, or can these exaggerations signify an optimistic
from this study that meaningful unconscious stimuli can projection of future goals? This question has been explored
affect motivational states, and the appraisal process that by examining responses associated with exaggeration, spe-
precedes challenge and threat responses does not require cifically focusing on emotional composure (assessed with
conscious awareness of resources. parasympathetic activity). In previous studies, roughly
Consistent with the idea that many thoughts and emo- 50% of students exaggerated their grade point averages
tions lie beyond individuals’ ability to consciously reflect (GPAs), and these students tended to improve academi-
on them but can still influence behavior is work by cally more than those who did not exaggerate (Willard &
Bechara and colleagues (Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Gramzow, 2009). Was this exaggeration a form of implicit
Damasio, 1997). These investigators measured skin con- goal setting, or was it simply a form of anxious repression?
ductance responses (SCRs; a change in the amount of To examine this, Gramzow, Willard, and Mendes (2008)
sweat in the eccrine glands) while participants engaged in measured RSA reactivity to differentiate anxious orienta-
a decision-making task (“Iowa gambling task”) involving tion from motivated goal setting. In this study, participants

CH06.indd 216 12/18/09 2:37:08 PM


Uses of Peripheral Neurophysiological Indexes in Social Psychology 217

first reported their GPA and course grades in private and of the influence of the physical presence of others on an
then met with an experimenter to review their academic individual’s behavioral performance gave rise to perhaps the
history. During this interview, the participant’s electrocar- most basic of all interpersonal effects, social facilitation and
diogram (ECG: the electrical signal of the heart cycle) and its subsumed counterpart, social inhibition.
respiration were recorded. The investigators found that the Social facilitation and inhibition refer to the enhancement
more participants exaggerated, the greater the increase in and impairment of performance, respectively, brought on by
RSA from baseline to interview. This pattern suggested that the presence of others whether mere observers or co-actors.
participants who exaggerated their GPA were not anxious This seemingly simple consequence of the presence of
about their exaggerated standards and may have a posi- others (i.e., an audience) on actors has been examined and
tive orientation toward the academic domain. Converging reexamined for more than a century, producing nearly 300
evidence from nonverbal behavior coded during the inter- published articles including many more experiments and
view revealed that exaggerators appeared composed rather more than a half-dozen theoretical accounts (see Bond &
than anxious. In addition, those who had greater increases Titus, 1983; Kent, 1994; Sanders, 1984, for reviews).
in RSA when discussing their (exaggerated) GPA tended Triplett (1898) reported a naturalistic observational study
to improve their academic performance in a subsequent and a controlled experiment exploring social facilitation
semester, suggesting emotional composure (and possibly effects. The former described his observation that competi-
RSA itself) as a possible mediator for why implicit goal tive cyclists raced faster against other cyclists than against
setting results in improved performance. the clock. The controlled experiment included children
spinning a fishing wheel-like apparatus either alone or in
co-action with other children. Supporting the social facili-
Interpersonal Processes
tation hypothesis, 50% of the children performed faster
It is fair to say that since the late 1970s, social psycho- in the coaction condition, whereas 25% performed about
logical experiments that involve actual interaction between the same and 25% performed worse in the presence of the
two individuals, let alone among more than two, occur coactors than alone. That is, Triplett found both social facil-
relatively rarely, and many of those that do involve a confed- itation (i.e., performance enhancement) and social inhibi-
erate as one of the interacting parties (Baumeister, Vohs, & tion (i.e., performance impairment) effects. Over the years,
Funder, 2007). Social psychophysiological experimen- research indicated that co-actors were unnecessary for
tation involving actual interaction is rarer still and work social facilitation and inhibition effects to occur, only the
involving CNS measures even more so. There are several presence of others proved necessary (Kent, 1994).
reasons for this paucity including a focus more on social Working independently, Thibaut and Kelly (1959)
cognitive processes; the time and work associated with and Zajonc (1965) introduced the concept of physiologi-
developing actual interaction scenarios and procedures; the cal arousal as a major motivational factor in their theo-
use of proxies for interactants including vignettes, audio retical accounts of social facilitation/inhibition effects.
and video recordings, and confederates; the near abandon- Zajonc, for example, postulated that the presence of oth-
ment by experimental social psychologists of small-group ers increases arousal that, in turn, facilitates dominant
research to scholars in other disciplines; and expense. The responses and inhibits nondominant responses that lead to
latter factor is even greater in studies that involve the rela- improved performance on simple or well-learned tasks and
tively higher expense of taking neurophysiological mea- decreased performance on complex or novel tasks. Based
sures, especially CNS measures. on this notion of arousal (one that is at best metaphori-
Nonetheless, some social psychophysiologists have cal in nature; cf. Carver & Scheier, 1981; Sanders, 1981),
persisted in creating experiments in which actual social many investigators have measured unitary physiological
interaction occurs while assessing peripheral neurophysi- responses (falsely positing or implicitly assuming that
ological indexes. Here, we look at some of the latter with autonomic measures were interchangeable) in social facili-
a focus, first, on basic interpersonal social influence pro- tation studies. However, Bond and Titus’s meta-analysis
cesses including social facilitation, social comparison, and (1983) reported that there were only weak effects on
social support, and second, with a focus on intergroup inter- palmer sweating (e.g., galvanic skin response), leading
actions including stigma, stereotypes, and discrimination. Cacioppo and Petty (1984) to conclude that more sensitive
physiological measures were needed.
Social Facilitation Based on the biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat
Triplett (1898) is usually credited with reporting the first motivation (Blascovich, 2008; Blascovich & Mendes, 2000;
experiment in modern social psychology (cf. Allport, 1954; Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996), Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter,
Gardner, Gabriel, & Diekman, 2000). Triplett’s identification and Salomon (1999) undertook Cacioppo and Petty’s (1986)

CH06.indd 217 12/18/09 2:37:09 PM


218 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

prescription for more valid and sensitive physiological times of actual or potential emotional distress, be it due to
measures to untangle the role of peripheral physiology in loss, infirmity, or performance. Furthermore, it is assumed
social facilitation processes. They reasoned that the pres- that, in general, social support provides health-promoting
ence of others during a person’s performance increases effects, in part, by buffering acute and chronic stress
the self-relevance of task performance and increases task responses (Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996). For
engagement for that performer. If the performer evaluates example, classic studies (e.g., Glaser, Kennedy, Lafuse, &
his or her resources as overcoming the task demands, he or Bonneau, 1990; Kiecolt-Glaser, Speicher, Holliday, & Glaser,
she will be challenged during the task. If the person does 1984) have shown that compared with gregarious ones,
not make such an evaluation, he or she will be threatened. lonely medical students exhibit greater decreases in immune
Hence, a well-learned task should lead to challenge and bet- functionality.
ter performance, and a novel task should lead to threat and Individuals who provide social support to others also
worse performance. benefit from receiving social support themselves. For
The researchers tested these hypotheses in a 2 (pres- example, caregivers of patients with Alzheimer disease
ence of others: audience vs. alone) by 2 (task mastery: well (Kiecolt-Glaser, Dura, Speicher, Trask & Glaser, 1991) or
learned vs. unlearned) completely randomized experimental patients with cancer (Baron, Cutrona, Hicklin, Russell, &
design. In the first phase of the experiment, participants Lubaroff, 1990) who lack social support are more likely to
practiced one of two randomly assigned moderately difficult be immune suppressed than caregivers who receive social
categorization tasks until they reached a proficiency crite- support themselves.
rion of 80% correct on consecutive blocks of task trials. In Positive effects of social support on physiological activ-
the second phase, participants went on to perform either the ity appear to operate largely within the domain of emotional
learned or unlearned task (randomly assigned) in the pres- support. Gardner et al. (2000) suggest two mechanisms by
ence of others (i.e., a male and a female observer) or alone. which the chance to discuss a stressful emotional situation
The physiological results (specifically, increased ven- may help the support target. First, the chance to discuss
tricular contractility and heart rate) indicated that partici- one’s distressful emotional state may cause a more benign
pants who performed either the well-learned or novel task reappraisal of the situation (cf. Tomaka, Blascovich,
with the audience were more task engaged during the Kibler, & Ernst, 1997) and decrease the likelihood of
task than those performing alone. Furthermore, partici- pathophysiological response patterns. Second, the chance
pants performing the well-learned task with an audience to discuss one’s problems provides the substantial benefits
exhibited challenge responses (specifically, increased car- of confiding in others (cf. Pennebaker, 1989), and both
diac output and decreased peripheral vascular resistance), reappraisal and confiding may interact in a synergistically
whereas participants performing the novel task exhibited beneficial way.
threat responses (specifically, decreased cardiac output The plausibility of both of these mechanisms is rein-
and increased peripheral vascular resistance). Behavioral forced in studies in which others were present but with-
results were consistent with social facilitation theory: out opportunity to provide emotional support, and hence,
Participants performing the well-learned task in the pres- the possible benefits of reappraisal or confiding in others,
ence of others performed better when being watched than or both, were absent. For example, Allen, Blascovich,
alone, and those completing the novel task performed Tomaka, and Kelsey (1991) measured autonomic responses
worse when being watched compared with being alone. (i.e., skin conductance, systolic and diastolic blood pres-
This study adds substantially to our knowledge of social sure, and heart rate) in middle-aged women during a stress-
facilitation effects. Theoretically, the authors suggest that ful serial subtraction task in the presence of no supportive
heightened self-relevance and its ensuing task engagement other, their best female friend, or their beloved pet dog (the
brought on by an audience has much to do with an indi- experimenter was present in all three conditions). Friends
vidual’s evaluations of his or her own resources to meet were instructed not to talk or touch the participant, and
the demands of a performance situation. Methodologically, petting the dog was not allowed. In the presence of their
this study illustrates the value of using peripheral best female friend, autonomic reactivity was significantly
neurophysiological markers consisting of patterns of phys- greater on all measures compared with the presence of the
iological responses to test predictions regarding arousal as experimenter and the pet dog. Indeed, the mere presence of
a likely mediator of cognitive-behavioral effects. the dog was associated with significantly lower autonomic
reactivity during the serial subtraction task for partici-
Social Support pants than either of the other two conditions. Later, Allen,
Humans are gregarious, at times seeking out and at other Blascovich, and Mendes (2002) replicated these findings
times providing support to each other, especially during with 240 male/female married couples in the presence of

CH06.indd 218 12/18/09 2:37:09 PM


Uses of Peripheral Neurophysiological Indexes in Social Psychology 219

no one, their spouse, their pet dog or cat (for pet owners), In sum, the effects of social support (or lack of it) on
their best friend (for non-pet owners), their spouse and pet physiological response patterns associated with ill health
(for pet owners), and their spouse and best friend (for non- and motivational threat are somewhat complicated. The
pet owners). Again, they found greater reactivity in the nature of the social support provider, the substance of
presence of the human friend or spouse and lower reactiv- the support, and the context all matter.
ity in the presence of the pet.
The higher reactivity in the friend/spouse conditions Intergroup Interaction
of the Allen et al. (1991) study can be contrasted with a
study reported by Kamarck, Manuck, and Jennings (1990) The use of psychophysiological theory has been especially
in which half the participants were accompanied by a sup- useful for social psychologists interested in studying social
portive other (i.e., a personal friend). Because the support- interactions with stigmatized or racial outgroup members
ive others in that study were instructed to silently cheer the because of increased impression management concerns
participant on, gently touch the participant’s wrist, and wore that can influence self-report and deliberate behavioral
noise-abating headphones, they could not evaluate partici- responses during such interactions. Though there are sev-
pant, which is the likely reason the authors found a reduction eral early examples of attempts to find physiological dif-
in CV reactivity when the participant’s friend was present. ferences on exposure to ingroup versus outgroup members
That the pets had a lowering effect on autonomic reac- (e.g., Rankin & Campbell, 1955), these early investigations
tivity in both of Allen et al.’s studies (1991, 2002) provides were based on putative measures of general “arousal” (e.g.,
additional support for the reappraisal mechanism, albeit heart rate and skin conductance) in fairly unconstrained
perhaps an unconscious one. More specifically, the pres- settings, thus lacking specificity and inference power (see
ence of the dog or cat for pet owners may provide a sense also Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977; Vrana & Rollock, 1998).
of security or positive affect, or both, that minimizes the It should not come as a surprise (given the discussion in
psychological danger of the situation, thereby resulting the early sections of this chapter) that these investigations
in more challenge rather than threatening appraisals (cf. revealed few, if any, differences between individuals inter-
Blascovich & Mendes, 2000). acting with outgroup or ingroup members. It was not until
On the other side of the social support coin is the extent to social psychophysiologists paid attention to the specific-
which people lack social networks or have a sense of being ity and inferential quality of their measures and/or con-
lonely. Cacioppo and his colleagues have studied loneliness strained the context that their research endeavors proved
and its far-reaching and profound effects on physical health, to be fruitful.
primarily by looking at physiological responses that are con- By constraining the emotional context, specifically to
comitant with loneliness as a way to determine how loneli- a fear conditioning paradigm, Banaji and her colleagues
ness gets under the skin to influence health (Cacioppo et al., examined whether individuals might be evolutionarily
2002; Cole, Hawkley, Arevalo, Sung, Rose, & Cacioppo, “prepared” to fear outgroup members (Olsson, Ebert,
2007; Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Bernston, 2003). This group Banaji, & Phelps, 2005). In this study, electric shocks
has shown in various settings that individuals reporting were paired with either ingroup or outgroup male faces.
greater levels of loneliness are more likely to show lower Electrodermal activity was measured to indicate changes in
cardiac output (less efficiency of the heart) and higher vas- sympathetic activation, as evidenced by the amplitude
cular resistance (more constriction of the blood vessels) of the SCRs in response to the pictures. In the learning
than individuals reporting lower levels of loneliness. This phase, the shock (UCS) was repeatedly linked with the
effect has been found in both laboratory-based settings in ingroup or outgroup picture (CS). Later, the shock was
response to social evaluation and field studies using ambu- removed and SCRs were examined on exposure to the CS.
latory impedance and blood-pressure devices. In the field The critical examination was the length of extinction, or
study, because of lack of ability in determining whether how long it took participants to no longer show a SCR to the
individuals were actually in a motivated performance situa- CS once the aversive element was removed. Indeed, when
tion, the authors interpreted these profiles as indicating pas- shocks were paired with outgroup faces compared with
sive versus active coping styles (Sherwood, Dolan, & Light, ingroup faces, SCRs persisted longer and were of greater
1990), with lonely individuals adopting more passive coping magnitude in the extinction phase. In this example, SCRs
styles within the context of their day. Adding causal evidence could be interpreted as fear responses because the context
to the deleterious effects of loneliness, Zadro, Richardson, was constrained to a fear-eliciting (shock) situation.
and Williams (2005) examined social isolation with an ostra- In a subsequent extension of this work, Navarette and
cism manipulation and found that the experience of ostracism colleagues (2009) tested the boundary conditions of this
brought on CV changes consistent with threat states. work within the theoretical context of the subordinate

CH06.indd 219 12/18/09 2:37:10 PM


220 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

male hypothesis (Sidanius & Pratto, 2004), which takes an Not surprisingly, stigma theorists (e.g., Crocker, Major &
evolutionary perspective on intergroup conflict suggesting Steele, 1998) maintain that such interactions cause interac-
that racial tension should be especially pronounced among tants, particularly nonstigmatized interactants, uncertainty,
men and not women. These researchers replicated the discomfort, anxiety, and danger during social encounters.
Olsson study discussed earlier with one additional factor: Only in the new millennium, however, have researchers
the gender of the people depicted in the pictures. The subor- produced physiological evidence of threat in actual social
dinate male hypothesis would predict that only male faces interactions between stigmatized and nonstigmatized
would produce the effect observed in the original study, interactants. Based on the biopsychosocial model of chal-
and that outgroup female faces would not show a sustained lenge and threat, Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, Lickel, and
fear response during the extinction trials. Consistent with Kowai-Bell (2001) hypothesized that nonstigmatized indi-
the theory, results showed that SCRs persisted longer to viduals would exhibit threat as evidenced physiologically
outgroup male faces than any other condition (outgroup even in cooperative performance situations as a result of
female, ingroup male, ingroup female). resource/demand evaluations in which demands for them
Group boundaries and social categories of race, gender, outweighed resources.
class, and so forth are mutable and can be influenced by In their first two studies, these researchers created dyads
presence of other more distinct outgroups or by context. of nonstigmatized and stigmatized individuals as coopera-
One might not think of their gender category while sit- tive partners in performance situations involving speech and
ting in the audience listening to a symphony, but might be word-finding tasks. They manipulated stigma by Kleck
acutely aware of their gender category if they are in the and Strenta’s (1980) technique of using opaque or transparent
numerical minority at a boxing match or a fashion show. makeup to manipulate the appearance of facial birthmarks
In academic environments, subtle and not so subtle cues on confederates. The latter interacted with nonstigmatized
regarding one’s status might influence one’s feelings of others in the cooperative tasks. Their results showed that
acceptance and expected performance. For example, if CV response patterns associated with threat were evidenced
there was only one women’s restroom in an entire building by the nonstigmatized partners during the interactions with
dedicated to math, but six men’s restrooms, a female math partners with birthmarks, and challenge was evidenced dur-
major might be reminded on a daily basis that she was in ing interactions with partners without birthmarks. In a sepa-
the numerical minority. What are the effects for these envi- rate article (Mendes, Blascovich, Lickel, & Hunter, 2002),
ronmental cues that remind some that they are not part of they found similar effects when White individuals interacted
the majority? A study from Stanford University explored with African Americans of the same sex. Interestingly, in
how environments based on the imbalance of males to the stigma studies by Mendes, Blascovich, and colleagues,
females influenced SNS responses (Murphy, Steele, & many possible moderators of physiological threat responses
Gross, 2007). In this research, male and female partici- were attempted. However, the only one to prove reliable
pants viewed one of two videos that presented either a was intergroup contact. In the third study of the Blascovich,
gender-balanced group of students or a gender-unbalanced Mendes, Hunter, Lickel & Kowai-Bell. (2001) article, a pre-
(mostly white males) group of students in the domain of a test measure of intergroup contact with African Americans
math and engineering science camp. Changes in SNS acti- moderated the threat effect such that White participants who
vation (especially skin conductance levels) from a base- had high-quality intergroup contact with African Americans
line period to watching the videos were computed. The (e.g., had dinner at the home of an African American friend)
investigators found that women showed greater increases showed CV responses similar to participants interacting
in SNS activation when watching the gender-unbalanced with a same race partner.
video than when watching the gender-balanced video, and Based on a theoretical argument of the role of
male participants did not differ in their SNS responses uncertainty, Mendes, Blascovich, Hunter, Lickel, and Jost
as function of the gender composition of the video. The (2007) focused on one of several possible mechanisms
authors concluded that the gender imbalance was espe- contributing to threat experienced by members of nonstig-
cially threatening for women. matized groups when interacting with stigmatized groups.
Specifically, they focused on the uncertainty created when
Stigma members of stigmatized groups violate expectancies
People’s interactions with members of stigmatized groups of members of nonstigmatized groups during social inter-
have received a great deal of attention among social psy- actions. In their first two experiments, nonstigmatized
chologists since the 1950s. Goffman (1963) surmised participants interacted with confederates who either vio-
that interactions between stigmatized and nonstigmatized lated or confirmed expectancies based on ethnicity and
individuals are threatening to members of both groups. socioeconomic status (SES). They found that participants

CH06.indd 220 12/18/09 2:37:10 PM


Uses of Peripheral Neurophysiological Indexes in Social Psychology 221

interacting with stereotypical partners (low-SES Latinos hypertensive than European Americans, and disparities
or high-SES Whites) showed the benign challenge pattern, in coronary heart disease are increasing (National Center
whereas participants interacting with counterstereotypical for Health Statistics, 2003; Willems, Saunders, Hunt, &
partners (high-SES Latinos or low-SES Whites) exhibited Schorling, 1997). One possible explanation for racial dis-
CV responses consistent with threat. In the third and final parities in physical health is that perceiving discrimination
experiment, they extended the expectancy violation test engenders physiological responses that are harmful to health
from counterstereotypical partners to atypical partners. (Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999). Among the
Specifically, they paired participants with either a White many complicated facets of this research has been
or Asian confederate who either spoke with an expected or the confounding of the expectation of discrimination
southern U.S. accent. Confirming the uncertainty hypothe- from the experience of discrimination. Several research
sis, when participants were paired with Asian partners and programs have attempted to differentiate these types of
they spoke with southern U.S. accents, they exhibited the discrimination experiences to examine the physiological
greatest levels of CV threat reactions. sequelae to attempt to understand how discrimination may
get under the skin to affect physical health.
Stereotype Threat Evidence is growing that based on past experiences
Steele and Aronson (1995) argue that performing in a with discrimination, some individuals develop a schema
domain (e.g., academia) in which one’s group’s perfor- that the world is a threatening place and become vigilant
mance is negatively stereotyped leads to anxiety, uncer- to signs of prejudice and discrimination in their environ-
tainty, and other reactions. Such stereotype threat is created ment (Allport, 1954; Barrett & Swim, 1998). That is, they
by the possibility that one’s performance will confirm the are likely to expect social rejection in interactions with
negative stereotype by placing increasing task-unrelated the nonstigmatized, and interpret negative or ambiguous
situational demands on the stereotyped individual, impair- social evaluations in terms of discrimination (Frable,
ing performance. In 2001, Blascovich, Spencer, Quinn, and Blackstone, & Scherbaum, 1990; Major, Quinton, &
Steele published an article demonstrating heightened blood McCoy, 2002). Furthermore, these individuals may per-
pressure responses among stereotype threatened African ceive discrimination even in situations where it does not
Americans taking a verbal abilities test. objectively exist.
In a stereotype threat study relying on the interpretation Karen Matthews and her colleagues have examined how
that decreased heart rate variability indexes mental effort, discrimination might influence resting and changes in CV
Croizet et al (2004) examined changes in RSA (a type of responses as a function of perceived discrimination (e.g.,
heart rate variability) during a stereotype threat paradigm. Guyll, Matthews, & Bromberger, 2001). Although no evi-
They found that participants assigned to receive a stereo- dence was found linking baseline blood pressure responses
type threat prime had a greater decrease in RSA and poorer to perceptions of discrimination, they did find that African
performance than those in the control condition, and that American women who reported that past interpersonal
RSA changes mediated the relationship from the condition treatment was due to racial discrimination exhibited sig-
to the performance effects. nificantly larger increases in diastolic blood pressure when
More recently, Vick, Seery, Blascovich, and Weisbuch speaking of a potentially racially charged event (i.e., being
(2008) tested the stereotype threat hypothesis and confirmed accused of shoplifting).
threat response via the challenge/threat CV response pat- But what about situations in which discrimination is
terns. In the Vick et al. experiment, participants took a diffi- not retrospective, but rather online? Do minority and stig-
cult math test, one that they had been led to believe was either matized targets respond with shame, negative affect, and
gender-fair or gender-biased in favor of male individuals. threat when experiencing negative treatment that they attri-
During the test, stereotype-threatened women exhibited bute to discrimination? Although the lay intuition might
a CV pattern associated with threat, whereas nonstereo- be to conclude that racial minorities would be the most
typed threatened women exhibited challenge. Interestingly, distressed in clear discriminatory situations, attributional
the CV patterns were reversed for men. Specifically, men ambiguity theory suggests a different response (Crocker &
exhibited challenge when a male gender bias was implied, Major, 1989). In their original theory, Crocker and Major
but threat when it was not, suggesting that male participants suggested that the ability to attribute negative feedback or
were threatened by the absence of the female stereotype. reactions from others to external reasons rather than inter-
nal causes might be protective for some stigmatized group
Discrimination members. This ability to attribute negative responses to
Health disparities because of racial identity are large, with external rather than internal causes might protect self-
African Americans at least two times more likely to be esteem of stigmatized group members.

CH06.indd 221 12/18/09 2:37:11 PM


222 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

To test these ideas and the possibility that previous work of measures that fit the definition of an invariant. This is
had yielded increases in self-esteem because of defensive not surprising given the requirements for such inferential
responding, a recent study measured CV responses during strength. Indeed, some may argue that the single invari-
a dyadic social interaction between same-race (White- ant, startle modulation, may actually be context bound,
White; Black-Black) or different-race (White-Black) occurring only in passive rather than active performance
partners (Mendes et al., 2008). During the social interac- situations (Schneider, 1997). The fact that a psychophysi-
tion, partners (i.e., confederates) provided explicit written ological index may be context bound or can be linked to
negative or positive social feedback immediately after the many different psychological states does not mean that it is
participant completed a speech regarding “why they made not valuable for the study of social psychological processes.
a good friend.” Social rejection was chosen for manipula- It merely means that there are few, if any, physiological
tion because of its powerful effects on emotions, such as indexes that have symmetrical one-to-one relationships
shame, sadness, and even depression (Ayduk, Mischel, & with specific social psychological constructs. However,
Downey, 2002; Williams, 2001), and its far-reaching neg- investigators utilizing physiological indexes must be aware
ative effects on physical health (Cacioppo et al., 2003; of the strength of the inferences drawn from these indexes
Stroud, Tanofsky-Kraff, Wilfley, & Salovey, 2000) and and be responsible in the interpretation of the meaning of
mental health (Williams, 2001). Following attributional physiological responses within the context of their experi-
ambiguity theory, the prediction was that social rejection mental protocols.
from an outgroup member would be more likely to be
attributed to discrimination than the same feedback from
an ingroup member. Indeed, when a different race partner SUMMARY
gave negative social feedback, participants attributed the
feedback to discrimination. Importantly, in a subsequent Scholars have been interested in the relationship of physi-
cooperative interaction with the partner, participants who ology to topics such as affect and emotion experiences,
received negative social feedback from an outgroup part- motivational states, and cognitive processes for millennia,
ner did not exhibit threat-related CV responses; instead, though with a respite of several hundred recent years in
they exhibited large increases in cardiac responses and Western societies created by adherence to Descartes’ mind-
decreases in vascular resistance—a pattern consistent with body dualism. With the fall of mind-body dualism over the
challenge. When nonverbal behavior manifested during past century, scholars in many of the psychological science
the interaction was coded, it appeared that participants subdisciplines have increasingly integrated biological,
who perceived the rejection as caused by discrimination psychological, and social processes into “biopsychosocial”
were angry not ashamed. Therefore, the challenge-related causal explanations of their phenomena of interest. Social
responses, in this case, were signaling approach-related psychologists are no exception.
motivation, which was consistent with the experience of Although the label social psychophysiology came into
anger. being only in the last 40 years ago or so, the integration of
social psychology and physiology can be traced to semi-
nal integrative notions of William James, Walter Cannon,
Section Summary
Hans Selye, and Stanley Schachter and Jerome Singer.
In this section, we have illustrated a variety of constructs David Shapiro and Andrew Crider ’s (1969) chapter in the
of interest to social psychologists for which physiological second edition of this handbook helped propel a relatively
measures have been utilized as indexes. Most, if not all, of small coterie of “social psychophysiologists,” to articulate
these constructs represent superordinate psychological and illustrate the ways in which neurophysiological mea-
states and processes such as positive and negative affect, sures could be used to advance social psychological theory
approach- and avoidance-type motivation, and functional and research (e.g., Cacioppo & Petty, 1984; Waid, 1984).
and dysfunctional self-regulation. These indexes have John Cacioppo first integrated social psychological and
been derived on the basis of psychophysiological and physiological theory, applying principles of inference to
social psychophysiological theories that span multiple neurophysiological indexes.
biological systems including autonomic, neuroendocrine, Part of the initial and continuing appeal of social neu-
and neural ones. roscience to social psychologists is the proven value of
Notably, these indexes differ in the strength of inference peripheral neurophysiological measures for advancing
they provide to theorists regarding the psychological states social psychological research and theory. Not surprisingly,
that they index vis-à-vis Cacioppo and Tassinary’s taxon- what initially may have seemed to be, or hoped by some to
omy as described earlier. Noticeable is the relative paucity be, a quick and fruitful marriage turned out to be a much

CH06.indd 222 12/18/09 2:37:12 PM


References 223

more complex relationship than expected. But it is a much Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &
G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.,
stronger marriage for that complexity.
pp. 680–740). New York: Oxford University Press.
In this chapter, we have illustrated the evolution Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as the
of impactful social psychophysiological theories by science of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to
describing ones that are biopsychosocial in nature and actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 396–403.
that provide the theoretical bases for developing strong Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Tranel, D. (1997). Deciding advantageously
before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1294.
inferential links between social psychological constructs
Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1991). Autonomic
and peripheral neurophysiological indexes of them, which determinism: The modes of autonomic control, the doctrine of auto-
we have also illustrated. Furthermore, the importance of nomic space, and the laws of autonomic constraint. Psychological
understanding these theories and linkages is highlighted by Review, 98, 459–487.
our brief overview of embodiment theory. The latter argues Blascovich, J. (2000). Using physiological indexes of psychological pro-
cesses in social psychological research. In H. T. Reis & M. Charles
that peripheral neurophysiological processes at least mod-
(Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psy-
erate and may even mediate social psychologically rele- chology (pp. 117–137). New York: Cambridge University Press.
vant affective, cognitive, and motivational processes often, Blascovich, J. (2008). Challenge and threat. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook
but perhaps prematurely assumed to be purely within the of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 431–445). New York:
Psychology Press.
domain of the CNS.
Blascovich, J., Brennan, K., Tomaka, J., Kelsey, R. M., Hughes, P. H., &
Coad, M. L., et al. (1992). Affect intensity, cardiac arousal, and heart-
beat detection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
164–174.
REFERENCES Blascovich, J., Ernst, J. M., Tomaka, J., Kelsey, R. M., Salomon, K. A., &
Fazio, R. H. (1993). Attitude as a moderator of autonomic reactivity.
Adelmann, P. K., & Zajonc, R. B. (1989). Facial efference and the experi- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 165–176.
ence of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 249–280. Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2000). Challenge and threat appraisals:
Ader, R., & Cohen, N. (1975). Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppres- The role of affective cues. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking:
sion. Psychosomatic Medicine, 37, 333–340. The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 59–82). Paris: Cambridge
Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2002). Cardiovascular University Press.
reactivity and the presence of pets, friends, and spouses: The truth Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., Lickel, B., & Kowai-Bell, N.
about cats and dogs. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 727–739. (2001). Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatized others.
Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., Tomaka, J., & Kelsey, R. M. (1991). The Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 253–267.
presence of human friends and pet dogs as moderators of autonomic Blascovich, J., Mendes, W., Hunter, S., & Salomon, K. (1999). Social
responses to stress in women. Journal of Personality and Social “facilitation” as challenge and threat. Journal of Personality and
Psychology, 61, 582–589. Social Psychology, 77, 68–77.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Blascovich, J., & Seery, M. D. (2006). Visceral and somatic indexes of
Addison-Wesley. social psychological constructs. In A. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins
Amodio, D., Harmon-Jones, E., & Devine, P. (2003). Individual differ- (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp.
ences in the activation and control of affective race bias as assessed by 19–38). New York: Guilford Press.
startle eyeblink response and self-report. Journal of Personality and Blascovich, J., Seery, M. D., Mudridge, C. A., Norris, K. R., & Weisburch, M.
Social Psychology, 84, 738–753. (2004). Predicting athletic performance from cardiovascular indexes of
Ayduk, O., Mischel, W., & Downey, G. (2002). Attentional mechanisms challenge and threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40,
linking rejection to hostile reactivity: The role of “hot” versus “cool” 683–688.
focus. Psychological Science, 13, 443–448. Blascovich, J., Spencer, S., Quinn, D., & Steele, C. (2001). African-
Bard, P. (1929). The central representation of the sympathetic system: As Americans and high blood pressure: The role of stereotype threat.
indicated by certain physiological observations. Archives of Neurology Psychological Science, 12, 225–229.
and Psychiatry, 22, 230–246. Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of arousal
Baron, R. S., Cutrona, C. E., Hicklin, D., Russell, D. W., & Lubaroff, D. M. regulation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy-
(1990). Social support and immune function among spouses of cancer chology (pp. 1–51). San Diego: Academic Press.
patients. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 344–352. Bond, C. F., & Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of
Barrett, L. F., & Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primi- 241 studies. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 265–292.
tive. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 41, 167–218. Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo,
Barrett, L., Lindquist, K., Bliss-Moreau, E., Duncan, S., Gendron, M., L. G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology
Mize, J., et al. (2007). Of mice and men: Natural kinds of emotions in (pp. 602–642). New York: Cambridge University Press.
the mammalian brain? A response to Panksepp and Izard. Perspectives Bradley, M., Cuthbert, B., & Lang, P. (1990). Startle reflex modification:
on Psychological Science, 2, 297–311. Emotion or attention? Psychophysiology, 27, 513–522.
Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Aronson, K. R. (2004). Bradley, M., & Lang, P. (2007). Emotion and motivation. Handbook of
Interoceptive sensitivity and self-reports of emotional experience. psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 581–607). New York: Cambridge
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 684–697. University Press.
Barrett, L. F., & Swim, J. (1998). Appraisals of prejudice: A signal detec- Brener, J., & Jones, J. M. (1974). Interoceptive discrimination in intact
tion framework. In J. Swim & C. Stangor (Eds.), Prejudice: The humans: Detection of cardiac activity. Physiology and Behavior, 13,
target’s perspective (pp. 11–36). New York: Academic Press. 763–767.

CH06.indd 223 12/18/09 2:37:12 PM


224 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

Brener, J., & Kluvitse, C. (1988). Heartbeat detection: Judgments of the triggering a disruptive mental load. Personality and Social Psychology
simultaneity of external stimuli and heartbeats. Psychophysiology, 25, Bulletin, 30, 721–731.
554–561. Damasio, A. R. (2006). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human
Brown, J. S., Kalish, H. I., & Farber, I. E. (1951). Conditioned fear as brain. London: Vintage.
revealed by magnitude of startle response to an auditory stimulus. Darwin, C. (1872/1998). The expression of the emotions in man and ani-
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 317–328. mals (1872). New York: Philosophical Library (3rd ed.). New York:
Brownley, K. A., Hurwitz, B. E., & Schneiderman, N. (2000). Cardiovascular Oxford University Press.
psychophysiology. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson Dennett, D. C. (1978). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and
(Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 224–264). New psychology. Montgomery, VT: Bradford Books.
York: Cambridge University Press.
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and corti-
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Klein, D. J., & Poehlmann, K. M. (1997). sol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory
The psychophysiology of emotion across the lifespan. Annual Review research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355–391.
of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 17, 27–74.
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., & Ito, T. for mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84–100.
(2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. Handbook of emotions
(2nd ed., pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford Press. Duclos, S. E., Laird, J. D., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stern, L., & Van
Lighten, O. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions and
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Bernston, G. G. (2003). The anatomy of postures on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social
loneliness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 71–74. Psychology, 57, 100–108.
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, J. M., Ernst, M. H., Burleson, M., Ekman, P., Levenson, R., & Friesen, W. (1983). Autonomic nervous
Kowalewski, R., et al. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mecha- system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221,
nisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 407–417. 1208–1210.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1984). Social psychophysiology: A source- Fazio, R. (1989). On the power and functionality of attitudes: The role
book. New York: Guilford Press. of attitude accessibility. In A.R. Pratkanis, S.J. Breckler, S. J., &
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Losch, M. E., & Kim, H. S. (1986). A.G. Greenwald (Eds.). Attitudes structure and function (pp. 152–178).
Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
the valence and intensity of affective reactions. Journal of Personality Frankenhaeuser, M. (1986). A psychobiological framework for research
and Social Psychology, 50, 260–268. on humans stress and coping. In M. H. Appley & R. Trumbull (Eds.),
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Bernston, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary deter- Dynamics of stress: Physiological, psychological, and social perspec-
minants of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects tives (pp. 101–116). New York: Plenum.
on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17. Frable, D. E., Blackstone, T., Scherbaum, C. (1990). Marginal and mind-
Cacioppo, J. T., & Tassinary, L. G. (1990). Inferring psychological significance ful: Deviants in social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social
from physiological signals. The American Psychologist, 45, 16–28. Psychology, 59, 140–149.
Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (2000). Handbook of psy- Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electro-
chophysiology (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. myographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589.
Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (2007). Handbook of psy- Friedman, M., & Rosenman, R. H. (1974). Type a behavior and your
chophysiology (3rd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. heart. New York: Knopf.
Cannon, W. B. (1929). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear, and rage. Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1977). The subtlety of White racism,
New York: Appleton. arousal, and helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). The self-attention-induced feed- Psychology, 35, 691–707.
back loop and social facilitation. Journal of Experimental Social Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Interpersonal pro-
Psychology, 17, 545–568. cesses. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.),
Chalmers, D. (2007). The hard problem of consciousness. In M. Velmans & Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 643–664). New York:
S. Schneider (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to consciousness Cambridge University Press.
(pp. 225–235). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2002). Cognitive neuro-
Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D. R. (1999). science: The biology of the mind (2nd ed.). New York: W. W. Norton &
Racism as a stressor for African Americans: A biopsychosocial model. Company.
American Psychologist, 54, 805–816. Glaser, R., Kennedy, S., Lafuse, W. P., & Bonneau, R. H. (1990).
Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., Sung, C. Y., Rose, R. M., & Psychological stress-induced modulation of interleukin 2 receptor gene
Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Social regulation of gene expression in human expression and interleukin 2 production in peripheral blood leukocytes.
leukocytes. Genome Biology, 8, R189.1–R189.13. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 707–712.
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action.
Scribner ’s. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 558–565.
Cotton, J. L. (1981). A review of research on Schachter ’s theory of emo- Goffman, E. (1959). The presentations of self in everyday life. New York:
tion and the misattribution of arousal. European Journal of Social Anchor Books.
Psychology, 2, 365–397. Goffman, E. (1963). Embarrassment and social organization. Interaction
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self- ritual: Essays on face-to-face behavior (pp. 97–112). New York:
protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608–630. Anchor Books.
Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C. (1998). Social stigma. In D. T. Gilbert, Gramzow, R. H., Willard, G. B., & Mendes, W. B. (2008). Big tales and
S. T. Fiske, & L. Gardner (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology cool heads: Academic exaggeration is related to cardiac vagal reactivity.
(4th ed., Vols. 1 and 2, pp. 504–553). New York: McGraw-Hill. Emotion, 8, 138–144.
Croizet, J., Després, G., Gauzins, M., Huguet, P., Leyens, J., & Méot, A. Gregory, A. (2001). Harvey’s heart, the discovery of blood circulation.
(2004). Stereotype threat undermines intellectual performance by Cambridge, UK: Icon Books.

CH06.indd 224 12/18/09 2:37:13 PM


References 225

Grossman, P., & Taylor, E. W. (2007). Toward understanding respiratory Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S., Pfeifer, J. H., &
sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobe- Way, B. M. (2007). Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling dis-
havioral functions. Biological Psychology, 74, 263–285. rupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological
Guyll, M., Matthews, K. A., Bromberger, J. T. (2001). Discrimination Science, 18, 421–428.
and unfair treatment: Relationship to cardiovascular reactivity among Lovallo, W. R., & Thomas, T. L. (2000). Stress hormones in psychophysi-
African American and European American women. Health Psychology, ological research: Emotional, behavioral, and cognitive implications.
20, 315–325. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook
Hantas, M., Katkin, E. S., & Blascovich, J. (1982). Relationship between of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 342–367). New York: Cambridge
heart-beat discrimination and subjective experience of affective state. University Press.
Psychophysiology, 19, 563. Major, B., Quinton, W. J., & McCoy, S. K. (2002). Antecedents and con-
Henry, J. P. (1980). Present concept of stress theory. In E. Usdin, sequences of attributions to discrimination: Theoretical and empirical
R. Kvetnansky, & I. J. Kopin (Eds.), Catecholamines and stress: advances. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy-
Recent advances (pp. 557–571). New York: Elsevier/North-Holland. chology (Vol. 34, pp. 251–330). New York: Academic Press.
Henry, J. P. (1986). Neuroendocrine patterns of emotional response. Marshall, G. D., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1979). Affective consequences of
In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and inadequately explained physiological arousal. Journal of Personality
experience: Vol. 3. Biological foundation of emotions (pp. 37–60). and Social Psychology, 37, 970–988.
Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Martens, A., Greenberg, J., & Allen, J. J. B. (2008). Self-esteem and auto-
Herrald, M. M., & Tomaka, J. (2002). Patterns of emotion-specific appraisal, nomic physiology: Parallels between self-esteem and cardiac vagal
coping and cardiovascular reactivity during an ongoing emotional tone as buffers of threat. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
episode. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 434–450. 12, 370–389.
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205. Maslach, C. (1979). Negative emotional biasing of unexplained arousal.
Kamarck, T. W., Manuck, S. B., & Jennings, J. R. (1990). Social support Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 953–969.
reduces cardiovascular reactivity to psychological challenge: A labora- Matthews, K. A. (1986). Handbook of stress, reactivity and cardiovascu-
tory model. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, 42–58. lar disease. New York: Wiley.
Kassam, K., Koslov, K., & Mendes, W. B. (in press). Decisions under dis- McCall, C., Blascovich, J., Young, A., & Persky, S. (2009). Proxemic
tress: Stress profiles influence anchoring and adjustment. Psychological behaviors as predictors of aggression towards black (but not white)
Science. males in an immersive virtual environment. Social Influence, 5,
Katkin, E. S. (1985). Blood, sweat, and tears: Individual differences in 1–17.
autonomic self-perception. Psychophysiology, 22, 125–137. McEwen, B. (2002). The end of stress as we know it. Washington, DC:
Katkin, E. S., Blascovich, J., & Goldband, S. (1981). Empirical assessment Joseph Henry Press.
of visceral self-perception: Individual and sex differences in the acqui- Mendes, W. B., Ayduk, O., Akinola, M., & Epel, E. S. (2009).
sition of heart beat discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Neuroendocrine concomitants of physiological thriving. Manuscript in
Psychology, 40, 1095–1101. preparation.
Kemeny, M. (2003). The psychobiology of stress. Current Directions in Mendes, W. B., Blascovich, J., Hunter, S., Lickel, B., & Jost, J. (2007).
Psychological Science, 12, 124–129. Threatened by the unexpected: Challenge and threat during inter-
Kent, M. V. (1994). The presence of others. In A. P. Hare, H. H. Blumberg, ethnic interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,
M. F. Davies, & M. V. Kent (Eds.), Small group research: A handbook 698–716.
(pp. 81–105). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing. Mendes, W. B., Blascovich, J., Lickel, B., & Hunter, S. (2002).
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Dura, J. R., Speicher, C. E., & Trask, O. J., & Glaser, R. Challenge and threat during interactions with White and Black men.
(1991). Spousal caregivers of dementia victims: Longitudinal changes Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 939–952.
in immunity and health. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 345–362. Mendes, W. B., & Epel, E. S. (2009). Cardiovascular reactivity predicts a
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Speicher, C. E., Holliday, J. E., & Glaser, R. (1984). marker of cellular aging: Evidence for the resilience model of stress
Stress and the transformation of lymphocytes by Epstein-Barr virus. reactivity. Manuscript in preparation.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 7, 1–12. Mendes, W. B., & Koslov, K. (2009). Brittle smiles: Positive biases during
Kleck, R. E., & Strenta, A. (1980). Perceptions of the impact of nega- inter-racial interactions. Manuscript under review.
tively valued physical characteristics on social interaction. Journal of Mendes, W. B., Major, B., McCoy, S., & Blascovich, J. (2008). How attri-
Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 861–873. butional ambiguity shapes physiological and emotional responses to
Lang, P., Bradley, M., & Cuthbert, B. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, social rejection and acceptance. Journal of Personality and Social
activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, P., R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balabang Psychology, 94, 278–291.
(Eds.). Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes Mesulam, M. M., & Perry, J. (1972). The diagnosis of love sick-
(pp. 97–135). New Jersey: Erlbaum. ness: Experimental psychophysiology without the polygraph.
Lange, C. G., & James, W. (1922). The emotions (I. H. Haupt, Trans.). Psychophysiology, 9, 546–551.
Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Miller, M. B., Van Horn, J., Wolford, G. L., Handy, T. C., Valsangkar-
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1987). Affect intensity as an individual difference Smyth, M., Inati, S., et al. (2002). Extensive individual differences
characteristic: A review. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 1–39. in brain activations during episodic retrieval are reliable over time.
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1200–1214.
39, 124–129. Miller, N. E. (1978). Biofeedback and visceral learning. Annual Review of
Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1991). The concept of coping. Stress and Psychology, 29, 373–404.
coping: An anthology (3rd ed., pp. 189–206). New York: Columbia Murphy, M. C., Steele, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat:
University Press. How situational cues affect women in math, science, and engineering
Levenson, R., Ekman, P., Heider, K., & Friesen, W. (1992). Emotion settings. Psychological Science, 18, 879–885.
and autonomic nervous system activity in the Minangkabau of West National Center for Health Statistics. (2003). Health, United States.
Sumatra. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 972–988. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics.

CH06.indd 225 12/18/09 2:37:14 PM


226 Social Psychophysiology and Embodiment

Navarette, C. D., Olsson A., Ho, A., Mendes, W. B., Thomsen, L., & Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. (1977). Controlled and automatic human
Sidanius, J. (2009). The roles of race and gender in the persistence of information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological
learned fear. Psychological Science, 20, 155–198. Review, 84, 1–66.
Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316, 1002–1005. Searle, J. R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Press.
Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Seery, M. D., Blascovich, J., Weisbuch, M., & Vick, S. B. (2004). The
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211. relationship between self-esteem level, self-esteem stability, and car-
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal diovascular reactions to performance feedback. Journal of Personality
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. and Social Psychology, 87, 133–145.
Obrist, P. A. (1968). Heart rate and somatic-motor coupling during clas- Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York: McGraw-Hill.
sical aversive conditioning in humans. Journal of Experimental Shapiro, D., & Crider, A. (1969). Psychophysiological approaches to
Psychology, 77, 180–193. social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The hand-
Obrist, P. A. (1976). Presidential Address, 1975. The cardiovascular- book of social psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 1–49). Reading, MA:
behavioral-interaction as it appears today. Psychophysiology, 13, 95–107. Addison-Wesley.
Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. Sherwood, A., Dolan, C. A., & Light, K. C., (1990). Hemodynamics
New York: Plenum Press. of blood pressure responses during active and passive coping.
Psychophysiology, 27, 656–668.
Olsson, A., Ebert, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). The role
of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science, 309, Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (2004). Social dominance theory: A new synthe-
785–787. sis. In J. T. Sidanius & J. Sidanius (Eds.), Political psychology: Key
readings (pp. 315–332). New York: Psychology Press.
Panksepp, J. (2007, September). Neurologizing the psychology of affects:
How appraisal-based constructivism and basic emotion theory can Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological
coexist. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 281–295. Bulletin, 132, 946–958.
Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Confession, inhibition, and disease. Advances in Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual
Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 211–244. test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.
Plutchik, R., & Ax, A. F. (1967). A critique of determinants of emotional
state by Schachter and Singer (1962). Psychophysiology, 4, 79–82. Stemmler, G. (1989). The autonomic differentiation of emotions revis-
ited: Convergent and discriminant validation. Psychophysiology, 26,
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology,
617–632.
74, 116–143.
Stemmler, G., Heldmann, M., Pauls, C. A., & Scherer, T. (2001).
Raichle, M. E. (2006). The brain’s dark energy. Science, 314, 1249–1250.
Constraints for emotion specificity in fear and anger: The context
Ramachandran, V. S., Rogers-Ramachandran, D., & Stewart, M. (1992). counts. Psychophysiology, 38, 275–291.
Perceptual correlates of massive cortical reorganization. Science, 258,
Stern, R., Botto, R., & Herrick, C. (1972). Behavioral and physiologi-
1159–1160.
cal effects of false heart rate feedback: A replication and extension.
Rankin, R., & Campbell, D. (1955). Galvanic skin response to Negro and Psychophysiology, 9, 21–29.
white experimenters. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating con-
51, 30–33.
ditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback
Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M., & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.
lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production.
Stricker, G. (1967). A pre-experimental inquiry concerning cognitive deter-
Psychological Science, 7, 226–231.
minants of emotional state. Journal of General Psychology, 76, 73–79.
Reul, J., & de Kloet, E. (1985). Two receptor systems for corticosterone in
Stroud, L., Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Wilfley, D., & Salovey, P. (2000). The
rat brain: Microdistribution and differential occupation. Endocrinology,
Yale Interpersonal Stressor (YIPS): Affective, physiological, and
117, 2505–2511.
behavioral responses to a novel interpersonal rejection paradigm.
Sanders, G. S. (1981). Driven by distraction: An integrative review of Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 23, 204–213.
social facilitation theory and research. Journal of Experimental Social Tattersall, A. J., & Hockey, G. R. (1995). Level of operator control and
Psychology, 17, 227–251. changes in heart rate variability during simulated flight maintenance.
Sanders, G. S. (1984). Self-presentation and drive in social facilitation. Human Factors, 37, 682–698.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 312–322. Thibaut, J., & Kelley, H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. Oxford:
Schachter, S. (1964). The interaction of cognitive and physiological deter- Wiley.
minants of emotional state. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi- Tomaka, J., & Blascovich, J. (1994). Effects of justice beliefs on cognitive
mental social psychology (pp. 49–79). New York: Academic Press. appraisal of and subjective physiological, and behavioral responses to
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological potential stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,
determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399. 732–740.
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1979). Comments on the Maslach and Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., & Kelsey, R. M. (1993). Subjective, physiologi-
Marshall-Zimbardo experiments. Journal of Personality and Social cal, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of
Psychology, 37, 989–995. Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 248–260.
Schmader, T., Forbes, C. E., Zhang, S., & Mendes, W. B. (2009). A meta- Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kibler, J., & Ernst, J. M. (1997). Cognitive and
cognitive perspective on the cognitive deficits experienced in intellec- physiological antecedents of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of
tually threatening environments. Personality and Social Psychology Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 63–72.
Bulletin, 35, 584–596. Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competi-
Schneider, T. R. (1997). Psychological and psychophysiological corre- tion. American Journal of Psychology, 9, 507–533.
lates of cognitive appraisals of stress. Unpublished doctoral disserta- Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The rela-
tion, State University of New York at Stony Brook. tionship between social support and physiological processes: A review

CH06.indd 226 12/18/09 2:37:14 PM


References 227

with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Willard, G., & Gramzow, R. H. (2009). Beyond oversights, lies, and pies
Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488–531. in the sky: Exaggeration as goal projection. Personality and Social
Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (1989). Unintended thought. New York: Psychology Bulletin, 35, 477–492.
Guilford Press. Willems, J. P., Saunders, J. T., Hunt, D. E., & Schorling, J. B. (1997).
Valins, S. (1966). Cognitive effects of false heart-rate feedback. Journal of Prevalence of coronary heart disease risk factors among rural
Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 400–408. Blacks: A community based study. Southern Medicine Journal, 90,
814–820.
Vanman, E. J., Paul, B. Y., Ito, T. A., & Miller, N. (1997). The modern face of
prejudice and structural features that moderate the effect of cooperation Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York:
on affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 941–959. Guilford Press.
Vanman, E. J., Saltz, J. L., Nathan, L. R., & Warren, J. A. (2004). Racial Wilson, T. D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1978). The accuracy of verbal reports about
discrimination by low-prejudiced Whites facial movements as implicit the effects of stimuli on evaluations and behavior. Social Psychology,
measures of attitudes related to behavior. Psychological Science, 15, 41, 118–131.
711–714. Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the
Vick, S. B., Seery, M. D., Blascovich, J., & Weisbuch, M. (2008). The effect face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits
of gender stereotype activation on challenge and threat motivational positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
states. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 624–630. 989–1000.
Vrana, S. R., & Rollock, D. (1998). Physiological response to a mini- Wright, R. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2003). Cardiovascular correlates of chal-
mal social encounter: Effects of gender, ethnicity, and social context. lenge and threat appraisals: A critical examination of the biopsy-
Psychophysiology, 35, 462–469. chosocial analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7,
Waid, W. (1984). Sociophysiology. New York: Springer. 216–233.
Waynbaum, I. (1907). La physionomie humaine: Son mécanismé ét son Zadro, L., Williams, K. D, & Richardson, R. (2005). Riding the “O”
rôle social. Paris: Alcan. train: Comparing the effects of ostracism and verbal dispute on tar-
gets and sources. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 8,
Wegner, D. M., (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA:
125–143.
MIT Press.
Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274.
Weisbuch-Remington, M., Mendes, W. B., Seery, M. D., & Blascovich, J.
(2005). The non-conscious influence of religious symbols in motivated Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist,
performance situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 117–123.
31, 1203–1216. Zajonc, R. B., Murphy, S. T., & Inglehart, M. (1989). Feeling and facial
Wells, G., & Petty, R. (1980). The effects of overt head movements on efference: Implications of the vascular theory of emotion. Psychological
persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and Review, 96, 395–416.
Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230. Zillman, D., & Bryant, J. (1975). Viewer ’s moral sanction of retribution
Whitehead, W. E., & Drescher, V. M. (1980). Perception of gastric contrac- in the appreciation of dramatic presentations. Journal of Experimental
tions and self-control of gastric motility. Psychophysiology, 17, 552–558. Social Psychology, 11, 572–582.

CH06.indd 227 12/18/09 2:37:15 PM


Chapter 7

Automaticity and the Unconscious


AP DIJKSTERHUIS

The German philosopher Ernst Platner did not introduce comments. We are confronted with so much information
the term “unconscious” until the 18th century, but the and the capacity of consciousness is so minimal that it fol-
importance of what we now call unconscious processes lows logically that we must have unconscious processes.
was recognized earlier by many influential thinkers such The British philosopher John Norris, a contemporary of
as St. Thomas Aquinas, Kepler, Paracelcus, Shakespeare, Descartes and Locke whom he vehemently criticized, said:
Cervantes, Dante, and da Vinci. It may well be that the “We may have ideas of which we are not conscious. . . .
first written reference to the possibility of unconscious There are infinitely more ideas impressed on our minds
psychological processes was made in the third century than we can possibly attend to” (Koestler, 1964, p. 150).
AD by the Greek philosopher Plotinus, who remarked that So here we are. The foundations of modern social psy-
“the absence of a conscious perception is no proof of the chological thinking about human behavior are humbling
absence of mental activity” (Koestler, 1964, p. 148). as well as salutary: Some behavior is automatic, and con-
Two distinct observations serve as the pillars on which sciousness is a golf ball.
our current knowledge of automaticity and unconscious Although those foundations may be easy to understand,
processes stand. The first is that some of our behavior does the vast structure that has been built on top of them is highly
not feel voluntary. It seems to be caused by the situation complex. Exactly how much of our behavior is automatic?
we are in. It feels automatic. This idea is of central impor- Is automaticity only important for relatively unimportant
tance for social psychology as a science and also touches actions, or does it play a role in our most important endea-
on the big philosophical question of whether people have vours as well? Conversely, what happens when we feel
free will. More than 250 years ago, David Hartley (1749) we control our behavior? How “real” is control? What
observed that “the Motions of the body are of two kinds, do we do with our vast unconscious processing capacity?
automatic and voluntary.” Whereas Hartley did not yet How sophisticated is our unconsciousness? Finally, what is
specify which of the two is the more frequent, many others consciousness good for?
have been much bolder. Some have even suggested that This analysis starts with a description of four tradi-
voluntary behavior is nothing more than an illusion, a pow- tional lines of thought and research that all emphasize
erful trick of consciousness. As the novelist Tom Wolfe the importance of unconscious or automatic processes
said (2004, p. 283), “Let’s say you pick up a rock and you (or both). Subsequently, some key concepts are defined
throw it. And in midflight you give that rock consciousness against the background of current (partly neuroscientific)
and a rational mind. That little rock will think it has free research on attention, consciousness, and the unconscious.
will and will give you a highly rational account of why it In the large middle section, a number of important social
has decided to take the route it is taking.” psychological processes are discussed, and the extent to
The second observation is that the capacity of con- which these processes are automatic is analyzed. The
sciousness is small (Miller, 1956; Norretranders, 1998). subsequent section focuses on control, and finally, the
Filmmaker David Lynch once called it our “golfball-size chapter ends with a brief explanation of the prevailing
consciousness” (Lynch, 2006, p. 28), and many people, espe- view in neuroscience and cognitive science on the function
cially those working in creative areas, have made similar of consciousness.

This chapter was supported by a VICI grant from Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (NWO) (453-05-004).
I thank Henk Aarts, Pamela Smith, and the editors for their helpful comments on an earlier draft.

228
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Four Traditional Lines of Thought 229

FOUR TRADITIONAL LINES OF THOUGHT reads about the problem and sometimes discusses matters
with others. This stage allows the necessary information—
Our current appreciation of the importance of automatic the input for the system—to be obtained, but during this
and unconscious processes is the result of various thought and stage, a truly creative idea is seldom produced. The sec-
research traditions: incubation or unconscious thought, ond stage is the incubation stage. The problem is put aside
lack of introspection, skill acquisition and routine action, for a while, and conscious attention is directed elsewhere.
and preconscious processing. These four traditions are very However, the input is further processed unconsciously,
different. However, they have all emphasized the same sometimes leading to a “Eureka experience” in which
idea: Unconscious processes are of paramount importance the creative product suddenly and unexpectedly enters
for human functioning. consciousness. The third stage is one in which conscious
attention again plays a role. The creative product needs to
be verbalized and communicated. A composition or poem
Incubation or Unconscious Thought
needs to be put on paper, a scientific discovery needs
One might almost believe that half of our thinking takes detailed proof before it can be communicated to others.
place unconsciously . . . . I have familiarized myself with Interestingly, outsiders often see the third stage as the
the factual data of a theoretical and practical problem; actual creative work, whereas the actors themselves see
I do not think about it again, yet often a few days later the the second stage as the most important. For them, the third
answer to the problem will come into my mind entirely stage is merely some after-the-fact business to finalize
from its own accord; the operation which has produced it, matters. Einstein disliked it (“Conventional words or other
however, remains as much a mystery to me as that of an
signs have to be sought for laboriously only in a [later]
adding-machine: what has occurred is, again, unconscious
stage, when the mentioned associative play is sufficiently
rumination.”
—Arthur Schopenhauer (1851, pp. 123–124)
established and can be reproduced at will”; Ghiselin, 1952,
p. 43). After Mozart had sufficiently developed a new
Artists and scientists have long marvelled about the composition, he would write it down quickly and often,
wonders of the unconscious. It often feels as if creativity unbelievable as this may sound, while he was chatting with
solely derives from unconscious processes. Mozart, when his wife or with his friends.
asked to explain his extraordinary creativity, said he just Incubation often feels mysterious, and sometimes peo-
“heard” his compositions: “the whole, though it be long, ple do not even feel responsible for—or proud of—the
stands almost complete and finished in my mind, so that ensuing product. In a recent interview, J. K. Rowling,
I can survey it, like a fine picture or a beautiful statue, at the author of the Harry Potter series, was asked how she
a glance. Nor do I hear in my imagination successively, wanted to be remembered. “As someone who has opti-
but I hear them, as it were, all at once. What a delight mally used her talents,” she answered. When asked about
this is I cannot tell!” (Andreasen, 2005, p. 40). Mozart’s her success and the extent to which she had been lucky,
compositions were the result of elaborate unconscious she exclaimed that “having the idea was luck.” In general
processes, and at some point they just appeared in con- Rowling seemed to be (justifiably) proud of her years of
sciousness. Mozart was also clear about the irrelevance of hard work but not really of the brilliant idea(s) that pre-
conscious will. He did not know when his compositions ceded it. The reason is simple: The idea(s) were not the
would surface, and conscious force did not help at all. result of conscious labor but from the unconscious. They
Anecdotes about the role of the unconscious in creativity felt like they were “given.”
abound. One of the most famous poems ever written, Kubla The process of incubation is at this point only partly
Kahn: Or, a vision in a dream by Samuel Taylor Coleridge, understood. There are two different (but not mutually
was revealed to the author in a dream. Albert Einstein did, exclusive) hypotheses as to why incubation has effects.
often suddenly and unexpectedly, simply “see” solutions The first is that, when trying to solve a problem, a period
to scientific problems. Henri Poincaré’s detailed introspec- of thinking about something else can enable a later return
tive account of his most important mathematical discovery to the problem with a fresh look. The example people often
is justifiably famous. He failed to solve a mathematical use is that of solving a chess problem. You may start to
problem and went to the countryside with the goal to think along the wrong lines (“the first move has to be with
relax and not to think about it for a few days. Completely the knight”) and continued conscious thought is fruitless,
unexpectedly, the solution to the problem came like a flash because, as Schopenhauer once put it, it “robs the mind of
at the exact moment he boarded a train. elasticity.” A period of distraction can lead you to forget the
Creativity is often the result of a three-step process. erroneous lead, and, upon return, you immediately realize
First, a problem is attended to consciously. One thinks and you should start with the queen. Such a process, sometimes
230 Automaticity and the Unconscious

called “set-shifting” has indeed been demonstrated in vari- Lack of Introspection


ous experiments (see e.g., Schooler & Melcher, 1995; S.
Our mind is so fortunately equipped, that it brings us the most
M. Smith, 1995).
important bases for our thoughts without our having the least
However, set-shifting is merely a consequence of the knowledge of this work of elaboration. Only the results of
discontinuation of conscious thought. The unconscious it become conscious. This unconscious mind is for us like an
does not need to do anything. The more interesting second unknown being who creates and produces for us, and finally
possibility is that we think—unconsciously but actively— throws the ripe fruits in our lap.
during incubation (Andreasen, 2005; Dijksterhuis, 2004b; —Wilhelm Wundt (Koestler, 1964, p. 153)
see also Claxton, 1997; Poincaré, 1913). There is indeed
evidence for this. Dijksterhuis and Meurs (2006) asked We sometimes do not know why we do things (although
participants to “list things that one can do with a brick” sometimes we think we know), and we sometimes do
and showed that a period of unconscious thought led people not know how we do things (Wilson, 2002). Our conscious
to come up with more creative activities than a period knowledge about ourselves is limited. Many thoughts,
of conscious thought. Zhong, Dijksterhuis, and Galinsky feelings, and actions come from behind a veil that we can
(2008) presented participants with extremely difficult RAT often barely peer beneath.
triads (groups of three words for which a fourth, associ- More than a century ago, Watt (1905) demonstrated
ated word needs to be found; Mednick, 1962) and showed that we do not have conscious access to how we think. In
that unconscious thought led to greater accessibility of his experiment, participants were presented with nouns
the appropriate answers than conscious thought. In both (e.g., oak) and were asked to come up with an associa-
lines of research, very taxing distractor tasks were used for tion as quickly as they could. Sometimes participants were
unconscious thinkers to make sure participants could not requested to name a superordinate word (oak tree), but on
engage in conscious thought. The conclusion seems to be other occasions, they were asked to come up with a part
that a period of distraction can help creativity and problem (oak acorn) or a subordinate (oak beam). This way, thinking
solving both through set-shifting and through unconscious could be divided into four stages: the instructions (e.g.,
thought. superordinate), the presentation of the noun (e.g., oak), the
Multiple processes are operating during unconscious search for an appropriate association, and the verbalization
thought. During periods of unconscious thought, people of the reply (e.g., tree). Participants were asked to introspect
associate widely and divergently (Bowers, Regehr, on all four stages separately to assess the contribution of
Balthazard, & Parker, 1990; Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, consciousness during each stage. The third stage (search-
2006) and even make new associations (Andreasen, 2005). ing for an association) is the stage during which the actual
During unconscious thought, information becomes orga- thinking takes place, and hence this was considered the
nized in a different, and often better, manner (Dijksterhuis, most interesting stage. However, unlike the other stages,
2004b; Ellenbogen, Hu, Payne, Titone, & Walker, 2007), this stage was introspectively blank: Participants could not
and such processes even continue during sleep. In a report anything. The instruction in combination with the
fascinating recent experiment, Ellenbogen et al. (2007) presentation of the noun automatically started the think-
presented experimental participants with information ing process. The thinking itself was unconscious, at least
about the relations between various stimuli (of the sort of until the answer surfaced. As alluded to earlier, studying
A  B, B  C, C  D, etc.). Immediately after the experi- the unconscious is a humbling experience. We have lim-
ment, participants understood the close relations (e.g., ited introspective access to how we think.
A  B), but only after hours of engaging in other work Our poor introspective abilities have much broader con-
did they understand the moderately remote ones (A  C). sequences in that they often obscure the main causes of our
Finally, participants needed a good night’s sleep to grasp actions, feelings, and choices. As an example of the latter,
the most abstract and remote relations (A  E). people tend to buy clothes at least in part because of their
It is likely that great associative power, divergent position in a display (further to the right is better), but they
thinking, some sort of self-organizing principle, and the are completely unaware of this. In fact, people believe
ability to grasp remote and abstract relations (Andreasen, that their preference for a chosen pantyhose is based on
2005; Claxton, 1997; Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006; something unique, when the chosen pantyhose was in fact
Zhong et al., 2008) are causing people to have creative exactly the same as the alternatives that were not chosen
and new ideas after a period of incubation. In contrast, (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). An even more striking demon-
conscious thought is analytical and focused. It can be used stration of people’s unawareness of the reasons for their
to check the details of creative ideas, but it is not so useful choices comes from recent research by Eastwick and
for generating such new ideas. Finkel (2008). They studied speed-daters and found that
Four Traditional Lines of Thought 231

people really do not know what they value in a partner. number of operations which we can perform without thinking
When participants were asked for their preferences, men about them. Operations of thought are like cavalry charges
indicated that they valued physical attractiveness, whereas in a battle—they are strictly limited in number, they require
women primarily valued the earning prospects of their fresh horses, and must only be made at decisive moments.
—Alfred North Whitehead (1911, p. 61)
future partner. But did these preferences predict their actual
desires, behaviors, and choices? Not at all. William James was not fond of the idea of a nonconscious.
People are sometimes also unaware of the reasons for He called it “a tumbling ground for whimsies” (1890, p.
their actions, even when these actions are quite important. 163). However, James did write extensively about the
A well-known example is the bystander effect: The more importance of habits. He advised young people to develop
people who witness an accident, the less likely it is that useful social and mental habits: “We must make automatic
someone will intervene (i.e., the bystander effect; Latane & and habitual, as early as possible, as many useful actions
Darley, 1970). However, when witnesses are asked why as we can” (1890, p. 122). In his various writings, he
they did not intervene, they tend to come up with other stressed two good reasons for developing good habits. The
reasons (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). first is that strong habits are what they are—strong—and
Another example of poor introspection on the causes because of that, a good habit invariably leads to appropri-
of people’s behavior—in this case, performance on a ate behavior (which is also why he warned against devel-
memory test—comes from recent research by Nordgren, oping bad habits). The second is echoed in this section’s
van der Pligt, and van Harreveld (2006), in their research opening quote. Conscious thought is a scarce resource, so
on the hot–cold empathy gap (Loewenstein, 1996). They the more one can do without it, the better it is. We can
had some participants do a challenging memory test while consciously think about important matters, such as writing,
they were experiencing mild pain (they kept their non- during many routine daily activities, because these routines
dominant arm in a bucket of ice water). Indeed, compared do not need conscious guidance. Writing a chapter such
with control participants, who had kept their hand in room as this would be much more difficult when things such as
temperature water, the participants in pain underperformed driving, taking a shower, or brushing one’s teeth would
on the memory test. Just 10 minutes after the memory test, indeed require conscious guidance (see also Aarts &
some of the participants who had experienced pain before Dijksterhuis, 2000).
were again asked to keep their hand in ice water, whereas Unlike many other concepts, the concept of habit
others were not and placed their hand in room temperature easily survived the start of behaviorism. In fact, behavior-
water. Participants were then asked to explain their earlier ists saw habits, or past behavior in general, as the main
poor performance on the memory test. The participants who determinant of future behavior (Hull, 1943; Watson,
experienced pain while they explained the cause correctly 1914). The current use of the term “habit” is strongly
attributed their performance to the earlier pain. However, rooted in the behaviorist approached to learning theory.
participants who made their attributions pain-free did so to Behaviorists studied the development of habits, albeit in a
a much lesser extent, despite the fact that they were in pain slightly different way than the researchers that investigated
only 10 minutes earlier! skill acquisition and automaticity in the 1970s and 1980s.
Finally, because of poor introspective abilities, people Whereas behaviorists assumed that cognitive processes do
can misjudge the origins of their feelings. Because we mis- not mediate the activation of habitual responses to cues in
attribute arousal, we mistakenly think we are in love when the environment, in later research, the emphasis shifted
we meet an attractive person, when in reality our excite- more and more toward understanding the (cognitive) pro-
ment is caused by crossing a scary suspension bridge cesses underlying habitual behavior. Researchers became
(Dutton & Aron, 1974). In sum, we do not have access to more and more interested in the mechanisms and processes
the processes underlying many behaviors, cognitions, and that enable people to perform complex routine actions with
feelings, and we often do not know why we think, feel, or no or minimal conscious attention.
behave the way we do. Shiffrin and Schneider (1977) did the most widely
known set of experiments. In their experiments, partici-
pants looked at an array of stimuli with the goal to search
Skill Acquisition and Routine Behavior for a particular target stimulus (e.g., the letter G) as quickly
It is a profoundly erroneous truism, repeated by all copy-books as possible. Participants practiced this for a long time,
and by eminent people making speeches, that we should culti- and as one would expect, the more distracter stimuli were
vate the habit of thinking about what we are doing. The precise depicted in an array, the longer it took to find the target
opposite is the case. Civilization advances by extending the stimulus. However, the effect disappeared over time. After
232 Automaticity and the Unconscious

a period of practice, finding the target became an automatic The fourth traditional line of research is the youngest,
process, whereby the number of distracter stimuli (4, 9, or and its origins can only be traced back to the general the-
16) no longer influenced the speed with which the target oretical work (not so much the psychoanalytic work) of
was detected. The key in the Shiffrin and Schneider (1977) Sigmund Freud. The logic behind the notion of preconscious
experiments was attention. At first, participants searched processing is that before something reaches conscious-
for the targets by devoting attention to each stimulus sepa- ness, some kind of constructive perceptual or concep-
rately. After practice, attention became parallel, whereby tual analysis has to take place. We do not “see” a door.
multiple stimuli could be attended to simultaneously. Such We see a red, rectangular shape, a little bigger than a person,
processes can often be witnessed in real life, as when surrounded by something large and white. Only after some
people try to master driving a car. preconscious construction processes are we consciously
In the past 30 years, our knowledge of automaticity in aware of the fact that it is a door.
routine action has increased greatly as a result of a large As Wegner and Bargh (1998) concluded, the “New
number of priming experiments (e.g., Bargh, Chen, & Look” approach in perception (Allport, 1954; Bruner,
Burrows, 1996; Devine, 1989; Higgins, Rholes, & Jones, 1957; Erdelyi, 1974) was the seminal line of experimental
1977; see Dijksterhuis, Chartrand, & Aarts, 2007, for a research on preconscious processing. This approach looked
review). Perhaps the best-known example is an experiment at motivational determinants of conscious perceptual
by Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996, Experiment 2). In thresholds. One important discovery was the phenomenon
this experiment, half the participants were primed with the of perceptual defense—the finding that the threshold for
elderly by doing a language task (they had to make sen- conscious perception is higher for “taboo” words, or more
tences with lists of words) in which words were hidden generally, for emotionally threatening stimuli. This was a
that were related to the elderly (e.g., Old, Bingo, walking breakthrough because the common view at the time was
stick, Florida). Control participants received a language that perception was only affected by properties of the
task in which the critical words were replaced by words stimulus, and that other factors, such as motivational ones,
not related to the elderly. After participants had finished, could only play a role after a stimulus had reached con-
they were told the experiment was over, but it was sur- scious awareness.
reptitiously measured how long it took them to walk to the The literature on selective attention (Broadbent, 1958;
nearest elevator. In line with the stereotype of the elderly Moray, 1959; Treisman, 1960) provided further evidence
as slow, primed participants took significantly longer. for preconscious processing. Various researchers dem-
Whereas in laboratory experiments, activation of onstrated that we perceive much more than what reaches
concepts generally occurs through priming techniques, consciousness, and that information that we do not attend
in the outside world, activation of a concept follows the to is filtered out. For example, Treisman (1960) found that a
perception of relevant stimuli. People repeatedly use story presented to an unattended ear was sometimes attended
the same personality traits and stereotypes when they to when it became relevant for the story presented to the
form an impression of others. In addition, people often attended ear. Another vivid example is the cocktail-party
engage in the same behavior and pursue the same goals effect (Moray, 1959). At a party where people are chatting
in the same context. After a while, such traits, stereotypes, in various small groups, an individual generally only attends
behaviors, and goals become mentally represented as asso- to the conversation in his or her own group. However, when
ciations between features in the (social) environment and someone else in an adjacent group mentions that individu-
the concepts themselves. The environment can automati- al’s first name, he or she hears this immediately.
cally activate the concepts, which then sets the appropriate The recent research on inattentional blindness and
psychological process in motion. change blindness also fits this tradition. In a spectacular
In sum, by repeatedly performing the same action or the demonstration, D. J. Simon and Chabris (1999) showed
same set of actions, behavior can be made automatic. that when people watch a group of people playing basket-
ball with the objective of counting the number of passes,
they can be completely oblivious to a gorilla (well, a person
Preconscious Processing dressed as a gorilla) walking through the group. Perceived
Unconsciousness is a regular and inevitable phase in the stimuli that are normally extremely salient can fail to reach
processes constituting our mental activity; every mental act consciousness when attention is directed elsewhere.
begins as an unconscious one, and it may either remain so or In sum, although controversial at first, the essence of the
go on developing into consciousness. New Look argument is now widely accepted (Wegner &
—Sigmund Freud (1915/1963, p. 53) Bargh, 1998). Perception is not just a function of features
The Unconscious, Goals, and Consciousness: A Taxonomy 233

in the environment but instead is an interaction of internal by many others, with three theoretical analyses shining
states and the environment. In addition, we perceive much through most clearly. The first is the seminal work by
more than what reaches consciousness. Some perceived Bargh (1989, 1994), who made various taxonomies in the
stimuli reach consciousness after some constructive pro- realm of automaticity that proved to be highly useful for
cesses, but the vast majority of percepts never do. analyzing social psychological phenomena. The second is
the recent work by Moors and de Houwer (2006, 2007),
who extensively discussed the relations among the different
THE UNCONSCIOUS, GOALS, AND features of automaticity. The final pillar is the emerging
CONSCIOUSNESS: A TAXONOMY neuroscientific literature on the relative independence of
consciousness and attention (e.g., Dehaene, Changeux,
The various research traditions discussed in the previous Naccache, Sackur, & Sergent, 2006; Koch & Tsuchiya,
section are quite different, and together they show that 2007; Lamme, 2003).
automaticity is a multidimensional concept. The litera- The taxonomy is based on two important distinctions.
ture on unconscious thought demonstrates that important The first is whether a process is unconscious or conscious,
mental activity takes place unconsciously, that is, outside and the second is whether a process is goal-dependent or
of conscious awareness. In addition, effects of incuba- not. One may note that these distinctions are reminiscent
tion do not seem to be under conscious control and feel of the two age-old observations with which this chapter
as if they do not require effort or attention. The work on started. In Table 7.1, the taxonomy is depicted.
people’s lack of introspective abilities again points at the
importance of the unconscious, but also at the fact that Unconscious Versus Conscious
human behavior does not necessarily follow conscious
intention or conscious goals. The experiments on skill The first distinction that can be used to classify social
acquisition are primarily concerned with effort: Practice psychological processes is whether a process is precon-
can lead to automatized behavior that does not require scious, postconscious, or conscious (see also Bargh 1989).
any effort. Finally, the literature on preconscious pro- Preconscious processes are unconscious, and the information
cessing again emphasizes the importance of unconscious the processes are based on was perceived unconsciously.
processes but also of control. What we perceive is not simply While driving, people pass a lot of advertisements—
a matter of conscious choice. for instance, on billboards—without ever being consciously
In the 1970s and 1980s, the prevailing view on the aware of these ads. However, such ads can subtly affect
concepts of automaticity and control was that they cre- people’s attitudes toward advertised brands. Sometimes
ated a dichotomy based on various dimensions (e.g., the results of preconscious processes can become con-
Posner and Snyder, 1975). Automatic behavior was unin- scious later on, but that is not necessary.
tentional, it did not need attention, it occurred outside of A subclass of preconscious processes are sublimi-
conscious awareness, and once started, it was uncontrol- nal processes. One can consciously perceive a billboard
lable. Conversely, controlled behavior was intentional, when one wants to. However, one can also present infor-
effortful, controllable, and dependent on conscious guid- mation subliminally, that is, one can present information
ance. The problem with such a multidimensional dichot- so quickly that it cannot be consciously perceived, even
omy was that it did not capture the full range of human when attention is paid to the information. For example,
behaviors very well. Behavior is rarely controlled on all a word that is presented on a computer screen for only a
dimensions, and although behavior that is fully automatic few milliseconds generally escapes conscious percep-
does exist—reflexes, for instance—these behaviors are not tion, even when a person is looking at the screen at the
the most interesting ones for psychologists. Instead, most right time. The word subliminal comes from the Latin
behavior is automatic on some dimensions and controlled words “sub” (meaning “below”) and “limen” (meaning
on others. Driving a car is largely independent of attention, “threshold”).
and many of the subactions required do not need conscious
guidance. However, it is not unintentional and certainly
not uncontrollable. The contemporary view is that the four Table 7.1 A 2 × 2 Taxonomy to Categorize Social Psychological
dimensions (or “the four horsemen,” see Bargh, 1994) Processes
should be treated as being at least partly independent. Preconscious/postconscious Preconscious/postconscious
To be able to see individual trees in the large forest of goal-independent goal-dependent
automaticity, a taxonomy is now presented that will be used Conscious goal-independent Conscious goal-dependent
throughout this chapter. The taxonomy is based on work Processes that fall in the gray cells are at least to some extent automatic.
234 Automaticity and the Unconscious

Postconscious processes are unconscious processes unpredictable fashion. However, as it currently stands, the
based on information that has been in consciousness previ- crux is not consciousness but attention. Goal pursuit,
ously. Preconscious and postconscious processes will often thought, evaluative conditioning, and implicit learning are
be lumped together because they are functionally equivalent almost certainly not dependent on conscious awareness, but
(Bargh, 1989), and many preconscious processes can also they are goal-directed and they do require attention (Bargh,
take place postconsciously. An example of a postcon- Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Troetschel, 2001; Bos,
scious process is incubation or unconscious thought. Here, Dijksterhuis, & van Baaren, 2008; Custers & Aarts, 2009;
people process information unconsciously, after they have Eitam, Hassin, & Schul, 2008; Field & Moore, 2005).
first encoded this information consciously.
Finally, conscious processes are those that are accom- A 2 × 2 Taxonomy
panied by conscious awareness of aspects of the process.
It important to realize that it is enough to be consciously These two distinctions combined lead to a 2 (preconscious/
aware of some aspects of the process for it to qualify as a postconscious vs. conscious)  2 (goal-independent vs.
conscious process. Speaking is conscious, because we are goal-dependent) table with which social psychological
aware of our speaking while we do it. However, the pro- phenomena can be classified. This taxonomy also takes into
cesses responsible for the production of speech are largely account the role of intention and the role of control, and
preconscious. For instance, we are not consciously aware of the definition of those concepts is based on Moors and de
our search for each and every word. If we had to be aware Houwer (2006, 2007), who classified intentional processes
of all aspects for a process to qualify as conscious, there and controlled processes as special cases of goal-dependent
would in fact be no conscious processes left. Every con- processes. A goal-dependent behavior is a behavior for
scious process is accompanied by unconscious processes. which a goal is needed. This goal may be a proximal goal
to engage in that behavior, in which case the behavior
is intentional, or more remote, in which case it is not.
Goal-Dependency
Furthermore, controlled behavior is behavior in which the
The second distinction is whether a process is goal- goal is to change other behavior—to alter it, to avoid it, to
dependent or goal-independent. A goal represents a desired stop it. This definition of control has consequences for an
state. A discrepancy between a current and a desired state analysis of what we can control and what we cannot con-
is what orients our attention (Moors & de Houwer, 2007). trol, as we shall see later in this chapter.
The goal to drink something will orient a perceiver to One caveat is that some phenomena can fall into
stimuli in the environment that may quench thirst, such as multiple cells of the 2  2 table. For instance, mimicry
a glass, a bottle, or a tap. That is, thirst directs attention to of subtle facial expressions or movements is a basic pro-
stimuli that may help quenching it. It is important to realize cess, in that it can be preconscious and goal-independent
that attention should not be equated with conscious aware- (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). However, the extent to which
ness. Although it is true that a stimulus that is attended to we mimic someone is at least in part driven by goals, such
is more likely to enter consciousness than a stimulus that is as the goal to affiliate (Lakin & Chartrand, 2003), making
not attended to, attention and consciousness are to some it a preconscious goal-dependent process. Finally, people
extent independent (e.g., Lamme, 2003). For instance, we can choose to strategically mimic someone—to establish
know that subliminal priming of stimuli—unconscious by rapport secretly, to learn a skill, or just for fun—in which
definition—only has effects when attention is paid to the case mimicry becomes a conscious goal-dependent act.
stimuli while they are presented (Koch & Tsuchiya, 2006). In what follows, social psychological phenomena
Rather than as a cause for conscious awareness, attention are classified according to this taxonomy. It should be
is better understood as the extent to which the brain pro- noted that the term “automatic” will be used to describe
cesses an incoming stimulus, or perhaps as the effort the psychological processes that diverge in at least one way
brain puts into processing a stimulus. from conscious goal-dependent processes—that is, it is
It is important to realize that there is no one-to-one unconscious, goal-independent, or both.
relation between attention and consciousness. Some
people find it difficult to appreciate that important
psychological processes, such as goal pursuit, thought, UNCONSCIOUS AND AUTOMATIC
evaluative conditioning, and implicit learning, do not PROCESSES IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
require conscious awareness. Perhaps this is caused by
the idea that, because these processes can ensue without One could argue that the role of unconscious pro-
conscious awareness, they are instigated in a random and cesses and the distinction between goal-dependent and
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 235

goal-independent processes is relevant for all areas in preference for, or a positive attitude toward, stimuli they
social psychology. However, for some areas, it is espe- do not remember being exposed to. Presumably, mere
cially important, in that relevant phenomena are impossible exposure effects can be grouped under the most basic of
to understand fully without an analysis of their automatic categories of goal-independent preconscious processes.
components. The areas that are reviewed here in more detail Mere exposure is socially relevant, because its effects
here are as follows: (1) attitudes, (2) person perception, (3) influence social interaction. Bornstein, Leone, and Galley
categorization and stereotyping, (4) imitation and behavior (1987) did an interesting experiment in which partici-
priming, (5) goal pursuit, and (6) judgment and decision pants were subliminally presented with a photograph of
making. one of two confederates. At a later stage, the participant
As Wegner and Bargh (1998, p. 465) noted, such areas met the two confederates. The two confederates disagreed
“share a concern for the genuineness of social behavior” about something rather mundane, and the participant was
(emphasis added). If a person says he is not prejudiced, “forced” to take sides. As the experimenters expected, in
is that really true? If someone explicitly expresses an atti- two-thirds of the cases the participants chose to side with
tude toward something, does that reflect her true opinion? the confederate they had been subliminally exposed to.
We tend to believe that automatic processes are more Recently, it has been shown that mere exposure effects
genuine than controlled processes. In addition, we think generalize from perceived stimuli to stimuli from the same
that tapping the unconscious reflects more “truth” than category (Monahan, Murphy, & Zajonc, 2000), and this has
asking consciousness. The number of social psychological may have consequences for racial prejudice. Zebrowitz,
publications that speaks to this issue has probably doubled White, and Wieke (2008) showed that brief exposure of
in just the past 10 years, which is fortunate for the reader, Black faces to White participants increased these partici-
but a little less so for the person who has to incorporate the pants’ liking for Blacks. P. K. Smith, Dijksterhuis, and
new work. Here we go. Chaiken (2008) found that subliminal presentation of
White faces to White participants decreased their liking
of Blacks.
Attitudes
A second automatic route to attitude formation is
The distinction between unconscious and conscious processes through evaluative conditioning. When an attitude object
is relevant for various areas in attitude research: attitude for- (the conditioned stimulus, or CS) is repeatedly paired with
mation (and change), attitude activation, the relation between a familiar object that already has a positive or negative
attitudes and behavior, and, the new kid on the block of atti- valence (the unconditioned stimulus, or US), this attitude
tude research, the role of implicit versus explicit attitudes. For object takes on the valence of the object it is paired with.
more information on attitudes, see also Banaji and Heiphetz Evaluative conditioning has been applied in many domains.
(this volume) and Albarracín and Vargas (this volume). Most initial demonstrations (e.g., Razran, 1940) were per-
formed in the context of persuasion, whereby a message
Attitude Formation was paired with a positive CS. Recently, evaluative con-
Attitudes can develop in many ways, and many such ways ditioning has been applied to heighten self-esteem (e.g.,
are largely or completely automatic. The most basic process Baccus, Baldwin, & Packer, 2004; Dijksterhuis, 2004a)
of attitude formation is through mere exposure (Zajonc, and to understand clinical phenomena such as spider
1968). Merely perceiving a stimulus repeatedly, such as a phobia (Merkelbach, de Jong, Arntz, & Schouten, 1993).
brand on a billboard one passes every day, makes it more Jones and colleagues (J. T. Jones, Pelham, Carvallo, &
positive. This effect is mediated by perceptual fluency Mirenberg, 2004) reported an engaging demonstration of
(Gordon & Holyoak, 1983). Fluency leads to a positive feel- the effects of evaluative conditioning. People generally
ing of familiarity that is then associated with the stimulus. find the letters in their own name more positive than other
When perceivers evaluate stimuli in an analytic way, such letters. Because of this, people like objects—including
as when they have to extensively justify why they like a other people—more when they are associated with letters
stimulus, the effects of fluency are overridden, and the mere in their own name. For this reason, people are dispropor-
exposure effect disappears (Whittlesea & Price, 2001). tionately likely to marry others whose first or last names
Mere exposure effects do not require a goal to evalu- resemble their own!
ate, nor do they require conscious awareness. In fact, mere Early demonstrations of the effects were open to
exposure effects occur even when novel stimuli are pre- alternative explanations such as demand effects (Razran,
sented subliminally for extremely brief durations (e.g., 1940; Staats & Staats, 1958), because participants were
Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980). Intriguingly, in such sub- often explicitly aware of the possible relation between the
liminal mere exposure experiments, participants indicate a CS and the US, the intentions of the experimenter, or both.
236 Automaticity and the Unconscious

Recent demonstrations have dealt with such problems. way incoming information is evaluated. Although it has not
Various researchers have shown that evaluative condition- been tested specifically, it could well be that these effects
ing occurs when either the CS or the US, or even both, are goal-dependent—for instance, in that they only occur
are presented subliminally (e.g., Dijksterhuis, 2004a; with certain processing goals, such as the goal to evaluate
Krosnick, Betz, Jussim, & Lynn, 1992; Murphy, Zajonc, & incoming stimuli.
Monahan, 1995; Niedenthal, 1990) rendering any alterna- Gilbert and colleagues (Gilbert, 1991; Gilbert,
tive explanation that requires conscious awareness highly Tafarodi, & Malone, 1993) documented what can be seen
unlikely. Krosnick et al. (1992), for instance, presented as an automatic bias in attitude formation. When we
their participants with nine slides of a target person engag- encounter new information (“a spekbokking is an edible
ing in routine daily activities. These slides were preceded fish”), it seems plausible that we would first carefully
by slides of positive or negative events (e.g., a child with deliberate before deciding whether such a statement is true
a Mickey Mouse doll vs. a bloody shark) presented for a or false. However, this is not what happens. On the basis of
mere 13 ms. Later, participants were asked to evaluate the insights that can be traced back to the Dutch philosopher
target person. A target person paired with positive stimuli Baruch Spinoza, Gilbert and colleagues showed that we
was evaluated more positively in general and was rated as automatically believe what we hear, at least momentarily
having a nicer personality compared with a target person (“Indeed, a spekbokking is an edible fish”). If we want to
paired with negative stimuli. determine whether a statement is false, we need to actively
These findings notwithstanding, the extent to which “unbelieve” it, which requires attentional resources and
evaluative conditioning is automatic is still debated. Some most likely also conscious awareness. This is intriguing,
researchers have suggested that conditioning effects only because it shows that agreeing with something is an auto-
take place when perceivers are aware of the contingency matic process (that is, it can take place preconsciously),
between a CS and a US (Pleyers, Corneille, Luminet, & whereas disagreeing is a controlled process. Gilbert et al.
Yzerbyt, 2007). However, given the results on conditioning (1993) also showed this asymmetry can have deleterious
with subliminal stimuli, it is more likely that contingency real-life consequences. In a jury trial simulation, false
awareness is merely the consequence of some other factor information about a defendant was believed by partici-
that is necessary for conditioning. That is, it is likely that pants and used to establish a sentence if their attention was
conditioning is goal-dependent and that a certain degree of occupied by a second task.
attention—driven by a goal—is necessary during encod-
Attitude Activation
ing for conditioning to be able to occur (see also Field &
Moore, 2005). For an attitude to guide behavior, it has to be activated
In addition, there are various other demonstrations of in the first place. For a long time, the dominant position
preconscious (or postconscious) routes to attitude forma- was that attitudes were activated by conscious retrieval
tion. Wells and Petty (1980) showed that bodily feedback and, hence, that attitudes would not affect behavior under
affects attitude formation. Participants in their experiment circumstances in which conscious retrieval was impeded.
were led to believe they were testing the sound quality of Simply stated, a positive attitude toward the Democratic
headphones for people who wanted to wear these head- presidential candidate would only lead to a vote for the
phones during biking or jogging. To simulate biking, some Democrats when that attitude was consciously retrieved
participants were asked to move their heads sideways immediately before voting. If for whatever reason the atti-
(thereby making a head movement implying “no”), and to tude is not consciously retrieved at the right time, it will not
simulate jogging, some others were asked to move their guide behavior. Although behavior can indeed be evoked
heads up and down (making a head movement implying in such a way, it is likely that such consciously activated
“yes”). While they did this, participants listened to a per- attitudes merely reflect a minute portion of all attitudes
suasive message advocating changes in college tuition. that guide behaviour.
As predicted, participants who nodded while listening Fazio and colleagues (Fazio, Chen, McDonel, & Sherman,
later agreed more with the message than participants who 1982; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986)
shook their heads. Later, comparable findings have been brought the idea of automaticity into the realm of attitude
obtained with other bodily feedback such as arm flexion research. They defined an attitude as the evaluation that
and extension (Cacioppo, Priester, & Berntson, 1993). becomes active upon—or the evaluative tag associated
Bodily feedback can trigger a preconscious approach reac- with—an attitude object. Again, the crucial factor in deter-
tion (heads up and down, arm flexed with hand toward mining whether an attitude affects behavior is that the
shoulder) or avoidance reaction (heads sideways, arm attitude is active, but whereas it was widely assumed this
extended as if pushing something away) that affects the was the consequence of an intentional, conscious process,
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 237

Fazio argued that the mere presence of an attitude object no. In the most telling set of studies, Bargh et al. (1996)
was enough to activate the attitude. made a crucial change to the dependent variable compared
In a first set of studies, Fazio and colleagues (1982) with the original experiments by Fazio et al. (1996). Rather
showed that attitudes can guide behavior postcon- than having participants evaluate adjectives, they had to
sciously, by investigating the role of attitude accessibility. merely verbalize them. With this procedure, where the
Participants in their experiments were asked to repeat- goal to evaluate was absent, automatic attitude activation
edly express their attitudes, and this in itself resulted in was again found, or so it seemed at first. As it turned out,
behavior that was more consistent with the attitude. the new procedure did produce the effects, but now they
In further studies, Fazio and Williams (1986) measured occurred for all attitude objects. That is, attitude strength
rather than manipulated attitude accessibility, and they failed to moderate, unlike in Fazio et al. (1986) experi-
showed that the accessibility of an attitude greatly influences ments. Bargh et al. (1996) named the effect the automatic
behavior. They asked their participants for their opinions evaluation effect, and proposed that all incoming stimuli
of the 1984 presidential candidates Ronald Reagan and are automatically—preconsciously and independent of
Walter Mondale, and the speed with which participants goals—activated.
replied (i.e., the accessibility of their attitudes) predicted To restate the findings and to reconcile the differences
their evaluations of the candidates in their first debate and between the data obtained by Bargh and colleagues and
even their actual voting behavior 6 months later. Fazio and colleagues, it seems reasonable to make the
Fazio et al. (1986) took the next important step and following assumption: Automatic evaluation is a goal-
greatly influenced attitude research in the years to come by independent, preconscious effect that pertains to all incom-
demonstrating that attitudes can be automatically activated ing information (based on Bargh et al., 1996). However,
upon the perception of the attitude object. They used a pro- when perceivers do have a goal to evaluate, automatic
cedure that has been used widely ever since (see also the attitude activation (or the affective priming effect) ensues,
next subsection, “Which Attitude?”). In their experiments, whereby the strength of the attitude activation is dependent on
participants were presented with prime/target pairs. The attitude strength. This makes the latter a goal-dependent
primes were words referring to attitude objects in which preconscious effect (based on Fazio et al., 1986).
the authors were interested, and the targets were positive However, to make matters more complex, there is
or negative adjectives. Participants were asked to indicate, recent evidence demonstrating that although the process of
as quickly as possible, whether each adjective was positive automatic evaluation is goal-independent, the evaluation
or negative by pressing buttons labelled “good” or “bad.” of specific stimuli is at least partly dependent on goals and
The researchers made sure the stimulus onset asynchrony varies between different contexts. For example, smokers
(SOA, the time between onset of prime and target) was automatically evaluate a cigarette as positive when they
very short (250 ms) to ensure participants could not engage truly crave one, but they may evaluate a cigarette as nega-
in conscious processing. tive after they have just had one (Sherman, Rose, Koch,
If it is true that attitudes are activated automatically, Presson, & Chassin, 2003). Ferguson and Bargh (2004)
participants should have been faster to respond to negative demonstrated that objects that are normally evaluated as
adjectives after negative primes (because negativity is neutral can be automatically evaluated as positive when
activated) and faster to respond to positive adjectives after detecting that the neutral words serve a current goal. Hence,
positive primes. This is indeed what the authors found the process of automatic evaluation itself is an example of
in their series of experiments. However, the effect was goal-independent preconscious automaticity, whereas the
moderated by attitude strength. Fazio and colleagues found outcome of the process is goal-dependent.
their effects (what is now generally called the affective Relatively few effects that are interesting for social
priming effect by most people) only for attitudes that were psychologists fall in the category of goal-independent
pretested as strong, but not for those that were pretested preconscious automaticity, but automatic evaluation does.
as weak. It shows that this faculty is very basic and of fundamen-
Bargh, Chaiken, and colleagues (Bargh, Chaiken, tal importance for human evolution. The functionality of
Govender, & Pratto, 1992; Bargh, Chaiken, Raymond, & automatic evaluation is further emphasized by the fact that
Hymes, 1996) tested whether attitude activation was goal- negative or threatening information attracts more attention
dependent or intentional. In the experiments done by than positive information (the automatic vigilance effect;
Fazio et al. (1986), participants had the goal of evaluating Pratto & John, 1991) and that negative stimuli need less
the target adjectives. Is this goal necessary for automatic stimulus input to be detected than positive stimuli. Hansen
attitude activation to occur? The answer based on the and Hansen (1988) showed that we quickly see an angry
experiments by Bargh and others was intriguing: yes, and face among happy faces and that this is not moderated by
238 Automaticity and the Unconscious

the number of happy faces surrounding the angry face (at propose that implicit and explicit attitudes are essentially
least not within the confines of the paradigm they used). measuring the same underlying construct but in very dif-
Others have shown that we detect the valence of a stimulus ferent ways, it is more appropriate to talk about explic-
before we are conscious of its meaning (Bargh, Litt, itly measured attitudes and implicitly measured attitudes
Pratto, & Spielman, 1989) and that this is especially true (Fazio & Olson, 2003).
for negative stimuli (Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2003). Finally, The distinction between explicit and implicit attitudes
Fiske (1980) demonstrated that negative information often bears on the question touched on earlier of what people’s
receives more weight in impression formation than positive true attitudes are. Is the implicit or the explicit attitude
information. more representative of what people really feel? Almost 200
years before people started to measure implicit attitudes,
Which Attitude? the philosopher Schopenhauer already had an opinion
The literature on automatic attitude activation and auto- about it: “If you want to know how you really feel about
matic evaluation led to a highly interesting new research someone take note of the impression an unexpected letter
tradition that has grown so quickly, it completely bulldozed from him makes on you when you first see it on the door-
some other areas in the realm of attitudes: the relation mat” (1851/ 1970, p. 171). In other words, it is not what
between implicit and explicit attitudes (see also Banaji & one explicitly says about someone that matters. Instead,
Heiphetz, this volume). the evaluation that is automatically activated upon perceiv-
The procedure used to measure automatic attitude ing the object is what counts.
activation discussed earlier speaks not only to the process Of course, a discussion about which attitude is more
of attitude activation but also to the content. Extremely genuine is only relevant if there is a serious discrepancy
brief reaction times on positive adjectives presented after an between the two. If explicit and implicit attitudes correlate
attitude object, together with a long reaction times on negative highly, the debate is futile. However, it quickly became
adjectives presented after that same object, are indicative of apparent that correlations between explicit and implicit
a very positive attitude toward that object. Indeed, the attitudes were often very low or even completely absent.
affective priming task is now widely used an attitude mea- Moreover, this seemed especially true for important
sure, and the later developed Implicit Association Test (the attitudes, such as racial attitudes (Fazio et al., 1995)
IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwarz, 1998) is so influen- or attitudes toward the self (i.e., self-esteem; Bosson,
tial that it can justifiably be seen as the driver of the bull- Swann, & Pennebaker, 2000). Nosek (2005) related atti-
dozer mentioned previously. tudes measured with an IAT measured explicitly and found
Researchers soon discovered that attitudes measured in that the correlations varied enormously. Some were high
such a way—that is, implicitly—often differed from atti- (Pro-choice/Pro-life .70, Bush/Gore .66), some were mod-
tudes measured the traditional, explicit way (e.g., Fazio, erate (United States/Japan .36, Microsoft/Apple .33), and
Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995), and a new field was for other objects they were low or absent (Rich/Poor .16,
born: the study of the difference and relation between Females/Males .05).
implicit and explicit attitudes. The terminology may need These wildly varying correlations led to two views
more explanation. Explicit attitudes are defined as atti- about the relation between implicit and explicit attitudes.
tudes that can be verbalized and generally measured with One possibility is that they are independent in that they
rating scales, whereas implicit attitudes are attitudes that do not share an underlying representation or construct,
are based on automatically activated evaluations. People implying that one is just as genuine as the other. Another
tend to have poor access to implicit attitudes (although possibility is that they do in part rely on the same underly-
there are exceptions; see Nosek, 2005). Intriguing as this ing construct and that implicitly measured attitudes are
area may be, it is also relatively new, so many fundamental more closely linked to this underlying construct than
issues are not yet resolved. One is that we do not yet know explicitly measured attitudes, because the latter is affected
what the most influential of implicit measures, the IAT, more by biases that take place during conscious thought
really measures (Blanton & Jaccard, 2006). Most would (e.g., demand effects, social desirability concerns). In
agree that the findings are at least partly based on the other words, explicit attitudes are based on implicit atti-
strength of the evaluation that is automatically activated tudes and some other influencing factors (see e.g., Fazio &
upon perceiving an object, but there is no more consensus Olson, 2003). The latter view implies that the strength of
than that. Another issue has to do with terminology. If one the link between implicit and explicit attitudes should vary
were to maintain that explicit and implicit attitudes are two as a function of moderators that “enable” implicit attitudes
independent things, there is nothing wrong with using the to better influence explicit attitudes. Such moderators
terms “explicit” and “implicit” attitudes. However, if you have indeed been found. The link between implicit and
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 239

explicit attitudes is stronger when (a) demand effects and knowledge we acquire, the more we realize that key social
social desirability concerns are either attenuated or con- psychological processes are largely automatic.
trolled for, (b) when explicit attitudes have to be expressed
quickly and/or under load, and (c) when implicit attitudes Person Perception
are strong rather than weak (see, e.g., Fazio et al., 1995;
see Dijksterhuis, Albers, & Bongers, 2008; Hofmann, For this chapter, the term “person perception” is used to
Gschwender, Nosek, & Schmitt, 2005; Nosek, 2005, for encompass all processes that are related to forming an
reviews). impression of, evaluating, or judging another person (see also
Why is the distinction between implicit and explicit atti- Macrae & Quadflieg, this volume). The range of relevant
tudes relevant for the domain of automaticity? Implicit processes are divided into three subareas: automatic evalua-
attitudes can be seen as unconscious attitudes, whereas tion, inference and attribution, and effects of accessible infor-
explicit attitudes can be seen as conscious attitudes. mation on judgment. As we shall see, person perception is
Incidentally, this does not mean that implicit attitudes can- dominated by goal-dependent preconscious processes.
not become conscious. People doing an IAT often “feel”
Automatic Evaluation
what the result will be and can be said to have some con-
scious access to their implicit attitude. The consequence is Automatic evaluation was discussed extensively in the
that implicit attitudes will mostly influence preconscious previous section. However, because it is (justifiably)
behavior, whereas explicit attitudes affect behavior mostly categorized by most people as an attitudinal process, the
conscious behavior. For example, implicit attitudes toward important role it plays in person perception is often forgotten.
Blacks predict nonverbal behavior during an interaction After all, automatic evaluation is a goal-independent pre-
with a Black person, whereas explicit attitudes predict conscious process that may well precede other processes
consciously made judgments—for instance, as to who was during person perception. That is, if we meet someone, we
guilty in causing the riots in Los Angeles in 1992 (Fazio immediately and automatically evaluate him or her, and
et al., 1995). the outcome of this process may well guide (or bias) subse-
The findings of the experiments by Fazio and colleagues quent processes, such as the inferences we make.
(1995) are telling in this regard. They measured implicit
Inference and Attribution
attitudes with the affective priming task and explicit atti-
tudes with the Modern Racism Scale. They then used One could propose that humans are such skilled person
both measures to predict various behaviors. Indeed, the perceivers that they need little information to draw infer-
explicit measure was a better predictor of more con- ences about people’s personality traits. What do we base
scious behaviors (such as ratings of the attractiveness of such inferences on? The answer is on almost anything that
Blacks versus Whites or an evaluation of the Rodney King is potentially useful.
verdict), whereas the implicit measure predicted behavior First of all, research has shown that facial features
that is largely guided preconsciously, such as participants’ evoke trait inferences. From a baby face—a round face
friendliness toward a Black experimenter. Such findings are with big eyes, small chin, and a small nose—we infer that
common now (see, e.g., Dovidio, Kawakami & Gaertner, a person is weaker, more naive, sweeter, more honest, and
2002). Dotsch and Wigboldus (2008) recently published less competent (e.g., Zebrowitz-McArthur & Montepare,
an intriguing extension with the help of virtual reality tech- 1992). Men with small eyes, thin lips, and a broad jaw
nology. They showed that Dutch undergraduates showed are seen as mature and dominant (Cunningham, Barbee,
more skin conductance and kept more interpersonal dis- & Pike, 1990). Todorov, Mandisodza, Goren, and Hall
tance from a Moroccan man as a function of their implicit, (2005) recently reported a spectacular demonstration of
but not their explicit, attitudes toward Moroccans. just how important such facially based inferences can be.
To summarize, automatic processes play a key role in These researchers showed participants pairs of political
attitudinal processes. Attitude formation can ensue precon- candidates running against each other in previous elections
sciously (and postconsciously) in various ways, whereby for the Senate and House. Participants only saw faces of
some processes are goal-independent and others goal- candidates that they did not recognize, and their simple
dependent. Automatic evaluation is a goal-independent task was to judge which person in of each pair was the
preconscious process, whereas attitude activation is a goal- most competent. Intriguingly, with this simple judgment
dependent preconscious process. A significant part of our made by a group of naive participants, the researchers
(interpersonal) behavior is accounted for by our implicit could “predict” the results of approximately 68% of the
(or unconscious) attitudes. Finally, attitude research is in elections. We may not want to judge a book by its cover,
keeping with a general trend in social psychology: The more but we do. The candidate with the most competent face
240 Automaticity and the Unconscious

won in two-thirds of the cases. In subsequent experiments, effect was caused by participants spontaneously inferring
competence judgments made after seeing the faces for a these traits during encoding. Later research with different
single second still predicted election results. This strongly paradigms showed that such spontaneous trait inferences
suggests we are dealing with a form of goal-dependent were indeed automatic or, more precisely, a form of goal-
preconscious automaticity. dependent preconscious automaticity (Todorov & Uleman,
Research on so-called thin slices of behavior also dem- 2003; Uleman, Hon, Roman, & Moskowitz, 1996; Wigboldus,
onstrate the remarkable capacity of people to quickly—and Dijksterhuis, & van Knippenberg, 2003).
often accurately—judge others on important dimensions Gilbert and colleagues (e.g., Gilbert & Malone, 1995;
(e.g., Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992). On the basis of short Gilbert et al., 1988) applied this knowledge of spontaneous
and silent video clips, perceivers can often accurately trait inferences to explain the correspondence bias. If we
deduce such wide-ranging characteristics as how well a try to find the cause of someone’s behavior, we should,
teacher teaches, how extraverted someone is, and whether ideally at least, look at both the person and the situation.
someone is heterosexual or homosexual (e.g., Ambady, Could it be that there is some sort of natural asymmetry
Hallahan, & Corner, 1999). In a fascinating line of that explains why the person generally receives more
work, Gosling and colleagues (Gosling, Ko, Mannarelli, & weight? Indeed, it was found that whereas personality traits
Morris, 2002) even demonstrated that people can form are inferred automatically, as discussed earlier, situational
remarkably accurate impression of someone they have inferences are usually not. In fact, attribution often follows
never met by merely looking into that person’s bedroom. a particular temporal order. People first automatically infer
Such results show that when people have the goal to form traits, and only then they may engage in “situational cor-
an impression, they quickly (and presumably precon- rection.” That is, you first automatically infer that Ap is
sciously) apply an impressive body of largely implicit an unfriendly and rude person, and only subsequently you
knowledge of the relation between someone’s living con- may correct for the fact that his national team has just lost
ditions and personality traits. the quarter finals of the Euro Cup.
Most of the research on inferences has been done to In one of their experiments, Gilbert and colleagues
better understand the correspondence bias or fundamental (Gilbert, Pelham & Krull, 1988) had participants watch
attribution error (Gilbert & Malone, 1995; Jones & Davis, a silent video fragment showing a woman being inter-
1965; E. E. Jones & Harris, 1967; Ross, 1977). The corre- viewed. The woman was evidently restless and nervous.
spondence bias refers to the tendency of person perceivers Some participants were led to believe that the woman
to attribute the behavior of others to their personalities was interviewed about something that would make most
rather than to situational forces, even when this is clearly of us nervous (i.e., sexual fantasies), whereas other were
unwarranted. People who read essays that are either favor- told that the woman talked about something mundane that
able or unfavorable toward Fidel Castro indeed believe the should not give rise to nervousness (i.e., plans for her next
authors to be pro- or anti-Castro, even if they are told holiday). Moreover, some participants watched the video
the writers had been forced to take this particular position under conditions of cognitive load, whereas others did
(E. E. Jones & Harris, 1967). When two people are ran- not. When asked to judge how dispositionally nervous the
domly assigned the role of quizmaster or contestant, with woman was, participants who watched without load indeed
the quizmaster asking the contestant questions, perceivers thought the woman was more dispositionally nervous
see the inability of the contestant to answer all questions when she talked about holiday plans rather than about sex.
correctly as a sign that he or she is less intelligent than the However, this difference disappeared under conditions of
quizmaster (Ross, 1977). cognitive load. Apparently, participants under load made
Research on automaticity in the 1980s has revealed the the automatic inference (“she’s very nervous”) but then
major cause of the correspondence bias (Gilbert, Pelham, & failed to make the situational correction (“yes, but that is
Krull, 1988; Winter & Uleman, 1984). First, Winter and because she has to talk about sex”).
Uleman discovered that perceivers infer traits from other Gilbert and colleagues published a large number of
people’s behavior. In their experiments, they presented par- experiments in which they showed that many processes
ticipants with sentences that strongly implied personality prevent people from making situational corrections. For
traits (“Dan helped the old man across the busy street” or example, if behavior is ambiguous or obscure, characteriz-
“Susan won the science quiz”). Participants read many such ing the behavior is more difficult than usual and indeed not
sentences and were later asked to recall as many of them as fully automatic. In such a case, inferring what someone is
possible. When they were first presented with the implied doing takes up cognitive resources that cannot be used later
traits (“helpful,” “intelligent”), recall improved. The traits in the process—for instance, for a situational correction.
functioned as retrieval cues, and the authors reasoned the Ironically, when an inference is not made automatically
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 241

because behavior is ambiguous, making this inference kind of inference (trait, goal, situation, etc.) perceivers
takes up resources that make a later situational correction make.
even more unlikely, resulting in a strong correspondence
Priming and Accessibility Effects on Judgment
bias (e.g., Gilbert, McNulty, Guiliano, & Benson, 1992).
Likewise, people who try to regulate their behavior during Person perceivers not only make automatic inferences
a social interaction (for instance, because they have to about others; they also make explicit judgments. When
maintain that their interaction partner is friendly when he we are asked, “Do you think Pam is smart?” or “Do you
isn’t) use up resources that lead to an enhanced correspon- think Henk is a warm person?” we engage in conscious
dence bias (Gilbert, Krull, & Pelham, 1988). judgment. Nevertheless, even such conscious judgments are
Although the asymmetry between the way we infer traits influenced—infused, if you wish—by preconscious pro-
and the way we take into account the situation can explain cesses. The way we interpret information or judge a person
the ubiquity of the correspondence bias, recent research has is highly dependent on the information that is accessible at
shown that person perceivers are more flexible than we had that time.
first thought (Krull, 1993). People have implicit theories Higgins et al. (1977) reported what is arguably the
about situational causes of behavior (Gawronski, 2004), first real priming study in social psychology. In a first
and they can make spontaneous situational inferences phase, participants were asked to study a list of words.
(Krull & Dill, 1996; see also Ham & Vonk, 2003) if they For some participants, the positive trait “adventurous”
have the goal to do so. In that sense, both trait inferences as was primed, for others the negative trait “reckless” was
well as situational inferences are forms of goal-dependent primed. Subsequently, participants were led to believe
preconscious (or postconscious) automaticity. Hence, the the experiment was over, and they moved on to another
fact that people often have the goal to form an impression experiment, in which they had to form an impression of a
of another person (thereby leading to automatic trait infer- person named Donald on the basis of a brief description.
ences rather than situational inferences) is presumably at Donald’s behavior was ambiguous in that it could be con-
least partly responsible for the frequency with which people strued as both adventurous and reckless (e.g., he wanted to
fall prey to the correspondence bias. cross the Atlantic in a small boat). The researchers found
In the past decade, the research on trait inferences has that the prime had a profound impact on the way Donald
expanded in interesting ways (see Uleman, Saribay, & was judged. Participants primed with adventurous had a
Gonzalez, 2008, for a recent review). Hassin and col- more positive impression of Donald than participants
leagues showed that people automatically infer goals from primed with reckless. These findings were soon replicated
the behavior of others (Hassin, Aarts, & Ferguson, 2005). and extended (e.g., Srull & Wyer, 1979). Although the pro-
When we read, “While passing the pet shop, the girl tells cedure used by Higgins and colleagues only allowed the
her father that everyone in her class has a dog,” we auto- conclusion that the effects of priming were postconscious,
matically infer that she wants her father to acquire a dog. a few years later Bargh and Pietromonaco (1982) showed
In other research, it was demonstrated that perceivers make comparable effects with subliminal primes.
other causal inferences automatically (Hassin, Bargh, & The finding that we apply information that is highly
Uleman, 2002). If we hear that Jane has lost her wallet after accessible has an interesting implication. Not only are
spending a day exploring the sights in New York City, we we all susceptible to the effects of priming, we also differ
automatically infer pickpocketing. Dunning and Sherman in what information is chronically accessible. Higgins,
(1997) found that people make so-called tacit inferences on King, and Mavin (1982) found that when people formed an
the basis of stereotypes. If you hear that a nun is unhappy impression of another person, they later remembered some
with the amount of alcohol that is consumed at a party, kinds of information better than others. The specific infor-
you make a different inference than when you hear that mation people remembered depended on the traits that were
a rock star is unhappy with the amount of alcohol that is most accessible. Indeed, they correlated with the traits that
consumed at a party. Finally, Ham and van den Bos (2008) people listed most often when they had to describe friends
demonstrated that people automatically infer values such and close others. Presumably, when we describe people,
as whether certain procedures (such as grading or hiring) some traits come to mind first. These traits are chroni-
were fair or unfair, or just or unjust. cally accessible and guide our impressions of novel people
In sum, people can make many inferences on the basis as well.
of all sorts of information, and as it currently stands, In a later experiment, Bargh and Pratto (1986) demon-
these inferences can be made preconsciously, but they strated that traits that are (chronically) accessible affect the
are goal-dependent. The goal that is active in a certain impression we form of a person at a very early stage. It
situation may be the most important determinant of the is not just the case that accessible information affects the
242 Automaticity and the Unconscious

way we interpret ambiguous information (as the Higgins, domain in social psychology that did benefit the most from
Rholes, and Jones study had shown), but it also determines our increased knowledge of automaticity and control, it is
what information we attend to in the first place. They used research on categorization and stereotyping—although the
the Stroop paradigm to measure attentional interference. reverse is obviously true as well.
Participants had to name the color in which trait words
Categorization
were printed, and they took longer to name the color of
chronically accessible traits than other traits. This find- Research on categorization can be traced back to Allport
ing is also interesting because participants merely had the (1954) and even to Lippman (1922), who argued that cate-
goal to name the word, not to judge or form an impres- gorization and stereotyping are spontaneous processes that
sion, demonstrating that effects of accessible constructs naturally accompany social perception. Taylor and col-
on attention are not just preconscious but can also be leagues (Taylor et al., 1978) showed this indeed to be the
goal-independent. case. When perceivers watch a group discussion involving
Whereas priming generally leads to assimilation, members of two categories (e.g., three women and three
whereby a judgment converges toward the primed con- men) and are later asked to recall who said what during
struct, it can also result in contrast. Herr (1986) found that the discussion, they more often confuse people from the
when people were primed with traits (“hostile”) or mod- same category than people from opposing categories.
erately extreme exemplars (“Joe Frazier”), a target was Hence, perceivers use categories spontaneously to orga-
perceived as more hostile, but when a very extreme exem- nize information in memory. In later years, it became clear
plar (“Hitler”) was primed, a target was perceived as less that categories are functional in the sense that their use
hostile. The explanation is that when people are primed during social perception is efficient. When we apply social
with an extreme exemplar, they may use this as a standard categories, the process of impression formation requires
of comparison against which a target is judged (“Well, fewer resources than when we do not apply them (e.g.,
George is not quite as hostile as Hitler”). For some time, Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). Moreover, when resources are
it was believed by at least some researchers that contrast scarce (such as when we are under load or time pressure),
effects resulted from conscious correction processes, and we engage in more categorization (Bodenhausen, 1988;
indeed, they sometimes do (e.g., Martin, Seta, & Crelia, Macrae, Stangor, & Milne, 1994). Finally, people who use
1990; Moskowitz & Roman, 1992; Strack, Schwarz, Bless, categorization during impression formation perform better
Kubler, & Wanke, 1993). However, it is now clear that at a secondary task than people who do not, demonstrat-
there are also unconscious routes to contrast, as various ing that categorization indeed literally frees up resources
researchers have found contrast effects with paradigms (Macrae, Milne, & Bodenhausen, 1994).
that rule out conscious mediation (e.g., Dijksterhuis et al., Although it was initially thought that category acti-
1998; Stapel & Suls, 2004). vation was fully automatic—preconscious and goal-
To recapitulate, effects of accessible constructs— independent—this is not the case. Gilbert and Hixon (1991)
whether assimilative or contrastive—can be preconscious. demonstrated that category activation does require some
Moreover, effects of accessible constructs on attention resources. Ironically, once a category is activated, it is
seem to be goal-independent. used more when resources are scarce, but scarce resources
can prevent category activation in the first place. Macrae
and colleagues (Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, Thorn, &
Categorization and Stereotyping
Castelli, 1997) demonstrated that participants do not nec-
Social psychologists have been struggling to understand essarily activate a category upon the perception of a face.
categorization and stereotyping for a long time. Whether They will do so under most circumstances, but participants
we perceive people as individuals or merely as members who process faces as mere objects and not people (i.e., they
of a group or category (Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, only have to report whether a white dot is present on pho-
1990; Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff, & Ruderman, 1978) deter- tographs) show no signs of category activation. Thus, cat-
mines to a significant extent how we behave toward them. egorization is a preconscious but goal-dependent process:
And there is a paradox we grapple with: The vast majority Perceivers need the goal to process a face as a social stimu-
of us do not want to stereotype or discriminate, but there is lus in order to categorize.
no denying that we often do. Whereas research in the 1990s treated categorization as
Is stereotyping inevitable? The answer seems to be an all-or-none process, recent research has demonstrated
something like “Sort of, but perhaps not completely.” The that the process is actually more complex and, to some
current state of affairs is heavily influenced by our knowl- extent, gradual. This newer research, in which the effects
edge of automatic and controlled processes. If there is one of distinct facial features were investigated, also brought
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 243

us a step closer to the source of categorization. Livingston of features is more difficult to control than prejudice as a
and Brewer (2002) showed that negative affective reactions result of abstract categorization (Blair et al., 2004).
(i.e., prejudice) toward African Americans are automati-
The Relation Between Categorization and
cally activated in response to various phenotypic features
Stereotype Activation
rather than to the abstract racial category. In fact, African
Americans who had fewer prototypical features but who The problem with categorization is not the activation of
were still unambiguously categorized as African American, categories per se. After all, categorization in and of itself
elicited less prejudice than African Americans with more is a functional process (see, e.g., Park & Judd, 2005).
prototypical features. Blair, Judd, and Fallman (2004) Instead, the problem is that category activation can lead
compared stereotyping on the basis of facial features with to the activation of stereotypes, and these stereotypes are
stereotyping on the basis of abstract categories and found often erroneous and negative. Devine (1989) demonstrated
that both were highly efficient processes but that the former that categorization indeed seems to lead automatically to
is harder to control. Participants who were asked to sup- stereotype activation. White Americans’ cultural stereo-
press their stereotypes while judging faces could do so for types of African Americans became active when stimuli
the stereotypes associated with the abstract racial category, related to the stereotype were presented subliminally
but not for stereotypes associated with individual facial (see also Dovidio, Evans, & Tyler, 1986; Pratto & Bargh,
features. Dotsch and colleagues (Dotsch, Wigboldus, 1991). Importantly, Devine also showed that stereotype
Langner, & van Knippenberg, 2008) recently showed activation was not moderated by people’s level of explicit
that just as automatic stereotype activation correlates prejudice (i.e., their scores on the Modern Racism Scale).
with measures of prejudice, our representations of proto- The conclusion of this work was bleak: We preconsciously
typical features do as well. With an ingenious procedure, activate cultural stereotypes, and this is true for all of us,
they obtained from White Dutch participants their unique even for people who are not explicitly prejudiced.
individual representation of the prototypical face of a One initial reaction was to investigate the possibility of
Moroccan male (the least liked group of immigrants in the suppressing stereotypes. If it is true that the activation
Netherlands). Participants high in implicit prejudice had a of stereotypes is inevitable, the way to prevent stereo-
more negative representation of a typical Moroccan face. types from affecting impressions and social behavior is
The relation between these facial features and an abstract to actively inhibit or suppress them once they have been
racial category is not yet fully understood. Whereas some activated (Devine, Monteith, Zuwerink, & Elliot, 1991).
(such as Blair et al., 2004) seem to conceive of them as two Various researchers have investigated this possibility,
independent factors that can lead to prejudice, others seem with mixed success. It is certainly true that stereotypes
to suggest that categorization is a downstream effect of can sometimes be suppressed (Monteith, 1993; Monteith,
the detection of certain facial features (e.g., Ito & Urland, Sherman, & Devine, 1998), but as with the suppression of
2003; Macrae, Quinn, Mason, & Quadflieg, 2005). Yet other constructs (Wegner, 1994), stereotype suppression
other theoretical perspectives assume that categorization is is prone to pitfalls: It may backfire once active suppres-
based on features and that an abstract, amodal representa- sion ceases, leading the stereotypes to be more active than
tion of a category does not exist (or is at least not necessary before (Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne & Jetten, 1994).
to explain empirical findings: Niedenthal, Barsalou, Later research showed that the automatic, one-to-one
Winkielman, Kraut-Gruber, & Ric, 2005). Cloutier, Macrae, relation between category activation and the activation of
and colleagues (Cloutier & Macrae, 2007; Cloutier, Mason, & cultural stereotypes, as proposed by Devine (1989), was
Macrae, 2005) showed that sometimes one feature can more complex than initially assumed. Devine presented par-
dominate social perception and automatically lead to cat- ticipants not just with words related to categories but also
egorization. In their experiments, participants saw faces with some of the associated stereotypes themselves. Hence,
with one important cue (e.g., long vs. short hair in a sex these studies did not really assess whether category activa-
categorization task) that were rotated to various degrees, up tion inevitably leads to stereotype activation. Lepore and
to the point that some faces were presented upside down. Brown (1997) presented participants with words related only
The results showed that the speed of categorization did not to the social categories themselves, not the associated
suffer from rotation as long as the dominant feature (length stereotypes, and showed that different people did activate
of hair) was clearly visible. different associations. What they activated was dependent on
As for the automaticity of categorization on the basis their level of explicit prejudice. Prejudiced people activated
of facial features, it is preconscious (e.g., Ito & Urland, negative stereotypes and nonprejudiced did not. Hence, it
2003) and goal-dependent (Macrae et al., 2005). As noted, is true that category activation automatically leads to the
it is interesting and disconcerting that prejudice as a result activation of associated traits, but these are not necessarily
244 Automaticity and the Unconscious

(negative) cultural stereotypes (see also Dijksterhuis, Aarts, Sadler, & Keesee, 2007; Eberhardt, Atiba Goff, Purdie, &
Bargh, & van Knippenberg, 2000). In addition, stereotype Davies, 2004; Payne, 2001). Even merely showing a Black
activation is at least somewhat malleable. Kawakami and face makes people more likely to misidentify a tool as a
colleagues (Kawakami, Dovidio, Moll, Hermsen, & Russin, gun (Eberhardt et al., 2004; Payne, 2001). These effects
2000) showed that participants can be trained to activate are also affected by priming. People who have just read
positive associations rather than negative cultural stereo- stories about Black criminals show more bias (Correll
types when they encounter a category member. et al., Park, Judd & Wittenbrink, 2007). Finally, activating
the abstract concept of “crime” induces attentional bias to
Downstream Consequences of Categorization and black males (Eberhardt et al., 2004).
Stereotyping In addition to effects on overt behavior, categoriza-
There are many immediate consequences of categorization tion strongly affects other processes in person perception.
and stereotyping, and this is not the place to review them all Some of the most influential models in social psychol-
(see Dovidio & Gaertner, volume 2). However, to appreci- ogy are (in part) built on this idea (Brewer, 1988; Fiske &
ate the importance of categorization and stereotyping for Neuberg, 1990). Stereotypes unobtrusively guide our impres-
social interaction, it is necessary to briefly emphasize some sions of others in a stereotype-confirming way. Subliminally
of the ways in which stereotyping affects social interac- priming the category of Blacks leads people to judge an (unre-
tion, especially because most of these downstream effects lated) target as more aggressive (Devine, 1989). However,
of stereotyping are themselves automatic. there are many processes that can mediate the relation
First of all, stereotypes can have a direct effect on overt between category activation and the resulting stereotypical
behavior (see also the next section on imitation and behav- impression. One example is that category activation influ-
ioral priming). Bargh et al. (1996) had participants do a ences spontaneous trait inferences. Stereotype-congruent
laborious task. During this task, some participants had been and neutral inferences are made automatically, but once a
subliminally primed with African American faces, whereas stereotype is activated, stereotype-incongruent inferences
others had not. At some point, the computer program appar- are inhibited, thereby reducing the chance a stereotype is
ently crashed, and participants had to start all over again. In actually disconfirmed (Wigboldus et al., 2003).
the interaction that followed the request to start over again, To summarize, categorization is a preconscious goal-
participants primed with African Americans were more hos- dependent process, and the activation of a category leads
tile than participants who were not primed. Chen and Bargh to the activation of associated characteristics, although not
(1997) demonstrated that such an effect can lead to detri- necessarily cultural stereotypes. Many downstream effects
mental self-fulfilling prophecies (for a classic demonstration, of category activation are also automatic. Finally, catego-
see Word, Zanna, & Cooper, 1974). Because the mere pres- rization may be more than an all-or-nothing phenomenon,
ence of an African American activates the hostile stereotype, and it may be driven more by the individual perception of
people display hostility when interacting with an African different features rather than by the activation of holistic,
American, and African Americans are hostile in return, thus abstract constructs.
creating the impression that, indeed, African Americans are
hostile. Needless to say, such a complex social process can Imitation and Behavior Priming
ensue without any conscious awareness of any stereotype.
After Amidou Diallo, an unarmed Black immigrant Not only are our judgments and impressions largely the
from Africa, was killed by White New York City police consequence of automatic processes, much of our overt
officers who thought he was armed, new research was con- behavior is, too. The vast majority of the empirical dem-
ducted to investigate the automatic effects of categoriza- onstrations of automaticity in overt behavior have been
tion. Various researchers have used a version of what is published in the past 10 to 12 years, so this topic features
now called the “shooter paradigm.” Participants see photos prominently in the Handbook of Social Psychology for
of Whites and Blacks holding either a weapon or some- the first time. Here, a distinction is made between imita-
thing harmless (e.g., a cell phone) and are asked to decide tion and behavior priming. Imitation involves perceivers
as quickly as possible to “shoot” (when the target holds a mimicking the observable behavior of others. Behavioral
weapon) or not (when the target does not hold a weapon), priming is when priming a social category or a personality
generally by pressing one of two keys on a computer key- trait changes a person’s overt behavior.
board. Unfortunately, participants in such experiments are
Imitation
generally affected by race, such that there is a bias to shoot
more often when the target is Black (Correll, Park, Judd, & Imitation is ubiquitous, among human beings as well as
Wittenbrink, 2002, 2007; Correll, Park, Judd, Wittenbrink, many other species. The evidence for imitation among
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 245

people is abundant and covers various concrete behavioral Strack, 2000; Strack, Martin, & Stepper, 1988). As a result,
domains. First, there is support for imitation of various imitation of facial expressions leads to shared emotions.
speech-related variables. One phenomenon is that of syn- For example, seeing a happy face can lead to a happy
tactic persistence, or the tendency to use a certain syntax expression on the perceiver, leading the perceiver in turn
when that syntax is made cognitively available. In an to begin to feel happy. Zajonc et al. (1987) found a strong
experiment by Levelt and Kelter (1982), an experimenter relationship between shared facial expressions and shared
called various shops and asked either, “What time does emotions. They found that partners who had grown to look
your shop close?” or “At what time does your shop like each other more were happier together than those who
close?” If the former was asked, the shopkeepers most had not, because their resemblance was due to a greater
often answered, “Five o’clock.” However, if the latter history of shared emotions, and shared emotions lead to a
was asked, the shopkeepers most often answered, “At stronger bond between partners.
five o’clock.” Syntactic structures appear to carry over Unconscious mimicry is not limited to speech and
from one sentence to another (see also Bock, 1986, 1989). facial expressions. We also mimic others’ postures, ges-
Importantly, these effects have also been found for single tures, and mannerisms. Bernieri (1988; see also Bernieri,
words, clauses, and for the structural format of entire sen- Reznick, & Rosenthal, 1988) provided solid evidence for
tences (Levelt & Kelter, 1982). Additionally, Levelt and posture imitation. First, two participants (A and B) were
Kelter found that cognitive load did not increase speech asked to interact. Later, the two participants A and B
imitation effects, which shows that such imitative effects were asked to switch partners with another dyad, C and D,
are highly efficient. Finally, Neumann and Strack (2000) such that A interacted with C and B interacted with D. All
found that interaction partners tend to mimic the tone of interactions were videotaped. Subsequently, two video-
each other ’s voice. tapes were constructed, both appearing to show partici-
There is also abundant evidence that people imitate pants A and B interacting with each other. One videotape
the facial expressions of others (e.g., Dimberg, 1982; showed the actual interaction between A and B. The other
Vaughan & Lanzetta, 1980, Zajonc, Pietromonaco, & tape, using a splicing technique, appeared to show A and B
Bargh, 1982). Yawning is an example of a familiar, together, but actually showed A when A was interacting
highly contagious facial “expression.” If someone around with C, and B when B was interacting with D. Observers—
you starts to yawn, it is common to feel the need to yawn who were unaware that one interaction was actually a
as well. Another example of imitation of facial expres- “fake” interaction—estimated the degree of posture simi-
sions comes from research conducted by Bavelas and col- larity. The degree of similarity was deemed greater on the
leagues (Bavelas, Black, Lemery, & Mullett, 1986, 1987). first tape (the actual interaction) than on the second, pro-
During their research studies, participants witnessed the viding evidence for posture matching.
confederate have a painful accident that caused the con- At first, the general belief was that although imitation
federate to wince. The more visible the wince was to the was unconscious, it was also likely to be goal-directed.
participant, the more the participant imitated the expres- For instance, Bavelas and colleagues (Bavelas et al., 1987;
sion. In other words, the easier it was for the participants see also Scheflen, 1964) argued that imitation serves a
to perceive the expression, the greater the effect on their communicative function, whereby the imitator conveys
own behavior. empathy (“I know how you feel”). Such a process is
Research on couples has revealed similar results. Zajonc indeed likely to be goal-dependent. However, a study by
and colleagues (Zajonc, Adelmann, Murphy, & Niedenthal, Chartrand and Bargh (1999) demonstrated that imitation
1987) reasoned that couples who have lived together for a can ensue independent of goals. Their study focused on
long period of time have experienced many similar emo- specific mannerisms rather than on basic posture mirror-
tions at the same time. Because frequent facial expressions ing. Specifically, participants interacted with two different
eventually lead to changes in facial lines, they predicted confederates during two consecutive dyadic sessions. Each
that partners should start to look more like each other the session involved the participant and the confederate taking
longer they are together. Indeed, participants judged cou- turns describing some photographs. One confederate
ples who had been married for 25 years to resemble each touched her face, while the other shook her foot through-
other more than random pairs of people of the same ages or out their respective interactions with the participant.
newlywed couples. Interactions were videotaped and coded for amount of face
Imitation of facial expressions is related to the study of touching and foot shaking displayed by the participant.
emotional contagion (e.g., Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, Chartrand and Bargh found that participants were more
1994). Facial expressions affect emotions through a likely to touch their own faces when they interacted with
feedback process elicited by facial muscles (Neumann & the touching confederate than when they interacted with the
246 Automaticity and the Unconscious

shaking confederate, but were more likely to shake their A few other moderators have been identified. First,
own feet when they interacted with the foot-shaking con- one’s style of information processing affects mimicry. If one
federate than when they interacted with the face-touching is in a context-dependent mind-set, more imitation occurs
confederate. Importantly, the mannerisms the participants than when one is in a context-independent mind-set (Van
imitated were mundane, making it difficult to argue that Baaren, Horgan, Chartrand, & Dijkmans, 2004). Likewise,
they served a communicative function. This makes it likely self-construal has an effect on mimicry. Van Baaren,
that imitation is (or at least can be) a goal-independent Maddux, Chartrand, de Bouter, and van Knippenberg (2003)
process. primed participants with independence, interdependence,
New evidence that for the idea of goal independence or neither through a scrambled sentence task. Participants
comes from research showing that imitation during social then interacted with a confederate who was playing
interaction is “default” behavior and that not engaging in with a pen. A hidden videocamera recorded the extent
imitation requires an additional process. This had been to which each participant mimicked the confederate.
hypothesized in the past (e.g., Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001) Interdependence-primed participants mimicked more than
and was recently confirmed empirically. For example, independent-primed participants. Chartrand and Bargh
Finkel and colleagues (2006) showed that someone who (1999) hypothesized that individual differences in empathy
is “antimimicked” during a social interaction shows signs might influence the likelihood of mimicking the behavior
of ego-depletion relative to someone who is mimicked. In of an interaction partner. Perspective taking, or the ability
addition, Lee, Dolan, and Critchley (2008) demonstrated to adopt and understand the perspective of others, is one
that when viewing an emotional expression, express- component of empathy (Davis, 1983). Indeed, Chartrand
ing a different emotion leads to behavioral conflict and and Bargh (1999) found that the higher people scored on
interference. the perspective-taking subscale of Davis’s (1983) empathy
That being said, the amount of imitation that occurs questionnaire, the more likely they were to imitate the
during social interaction can increase or decrease as a behavior of others.
function of goals. Both people who are given a conscious To recapitulate, although imitation does serve a clear
affiliation goal (i.e., they were told “you will be interacting social function, it does not seem to be goal-dependent. In
with this person as part of a cooperative task in which it fact, it seems to be default social behavior, and antimim-
is important to get along and work well together”) or an icry may be the exception rather than the rule. Goals are
unconscious affiliation goal (i.e., they were subliminally important, however, in that they affect the extent to which
primed with words related to affiliation—affiliate, friend, people mimic.
together) are more likely to mimic the behaviors of an
Behavioral Priming
interaction partner than people who do not have a goal to
affiliate (Lakin & Chartrand, 2003). That is, situations in The evidence for behavioral priming has been reviewed
which there is a conscious or unconscious desire to affiliate previously (Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001; Dijksterhuis et al.,
activate the tendency to mimic the behaviors of interaction 2007; Wheeler & Petty, 2001). A few examples to illustrate
partners. the wide variety of behavioral dependent variables that
In addition, recent research shows that imitation increases have been used in this area of research follow.
after being socially excluded. In one of the studies reported The previously discussed experiment by Bargh, Chen,
by Lakin, Chartrand, and Arkin (2006), participants were and Burrows (1996, Experiment 2), in which priming the
told that they would complete two unrelated experiments. category of elderly led to changes in walking speed, is an
The first involved playing an online ball-tossing game example of the effects of priming on motor behavior. The
(Cyberball; Williams, Cheung, & Choi, 2000) with three effect has been replicated by Hull, Slone, Meteyer, and
other participants. With the exception of the three other Matthews (2001) with the same social category and the
players’ initials, participants had no information about their same dependent variable. Spears, Gordijn, Dijksterhuis, and
fellow players and did not expect to ever meet or interact Stapel (2004) primed the category of businesspeople among
with them again. In reality, the other players were com- their participants and showed that it made people walk
puter-controlled and were programmed either to exclude (and generally behave) more quickly. Macrae et al. (1998)
or to include participants while playing Cyberball. After primed people with Formula 1 World Champion Michael
the Cyberball game was completed, participants com- Schumacher and showed that people started to read faster.
pleted a photo description task with a new confederate. As Finally, Aarts and Dijksterhuis (2002) made participants
expected, participants who were excluded mimicked the walk faster or slower by activating the representation of
behaviors of the confederate more than participants who animals associated with speed (cheetah, gazelle) or slow-
were included. ness (turtle, snail).
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 247

Apart from walking speed, activated categories have corresponding behavioral effects. Others have behavioral
also been shown to affect reaction times in a lexical priming effects on people’s willingness to help others
decision task. Dijksterhuis, Spears, and Lepinasse (2001) (Macrae and Johnson, 1998; Nelson & Norton, 2005;
had some participants form an impression of a group Walther, Muller, & Schott, 2001).
of elderly people, whereas other participants formed Finally, priming affects mental performance. Dijksterhuis
an impression of young people. In a later lexical deci- and van Knippenberg (1998) improved people’s intellec-
sion task, participants who had thought about the elderly tual performance by activation of both social categories as
reacted more slowly compared with participants who well as traits. In their first experiment, some of their partic-
had thought about young people. Kawakami, Young, and ipants wrote down everything that came to mind regarding
Dovidio (2002) and Schubert and Häfner (2003) also dem- the typical attributes of college professors, whereas others
onstrated effects of the activation of the elderly categories were not given this task. In an ostensibly unrelated second
on reaction times. phase, participants were asked to answer 42 general knowl-
Carver and colleagues (Carver, Ganellen, Froming, & edge questions that were taken from the game “Trivial
Chambers, 1983) were the first to show effects of trait Pursuit” (such as “What is the capital of Bangladesh?” a.
priming on overt interpersonal behavior. They asked Dhaka, b. Bangkok, c. Hanoi, d. Delhi). In line with the
participants to complete a scrambled sentence task (see, prevailing stereotype of professors as being intelligent,
e.g., Srull & Wyer, 1979) in which words or phrases primed participants answered more questions correctly
related to hostility were hidden (e.g., “hits he her them”). than did no-prime control participants. In other experi-
In the experimental condition, 80% of the content of the ments, participants performed worse on a general knowl-
scrambled sentence task pertained to hostility, whereas in edge task after having thought about soccer-hooligans, a
the neutral priming condition, only 20% did. In a second, social group that is associated with stupidity. Finally, it was
unrelated part of the experiment, participants had to fulfill shown that performance on the general knowledge task
the role of a teacher in a learning experiment based on could be changed by priming traits (intelligent or dumb)
Milgram’s (1963) work. Participants asked the “learner” rather than social categories.
(a confederate separated from the participant by a wooden The effects obtained by Dijksterhuis and van Knippenberg
partition) 34 questions, of which the confederate answered (1998) have been replicated and extended a number of
20 incorrectly. Participants were asked to administer times (Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg, 2000; Dijksterhuis,
an electric shock to the learner upon every incorrect Spears et al., 1998; Haddock, Macrae, & Fleck, 2002; Hull
answer. Importantly, however, the participants could et al., 2001; LeBoeuf & Estes, 2004; Schubert & Häfner,
determine the level of each shock. Participants primed 2003). In addition to soccer-hooligans, the categories of
with hostility gave shocks of higher intensity than control supermodels (Dijksterhuis, Spears et al., 1998; Haddock
participants. et al., 2002) and “tramps” (Schubert & Häfner, 2003) have
Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996, Experiment 1) also been shown to temporarily reduce intellectual perfor-
presented their participants with a scrambled sentence mance. Hull and colleagues (2001) obtained their effects
task. In one condition, the scrambled sentences con- on intellectual performance after priming participants with
tained words related to rudeness (e.g., aggressively, bold, “success.” Changes in intellectual performance have not
rude), whereas in a second condition, the scrambled sen- only been shown with general knowledge questionnaires
tences contained words pertaining to politeness (e.g., but also on tasks assessing the proficiency with which
respect, patiently, polite). In a third (control) condition, participants acquired and understood new knowledge
the scrambled sentence task did not contain words related (Haddock et al., 2002). Finally, various researchers have
to either rudeness or politeness. Participants were told to demonstrated that social category priming can affect peo-
meet the experimenter in a different office after finishing ple’s ability to do arithmetic. For instance, Wheeler, Jarvis,
the task. When participants approached the experimenter, the and Petty (2001) primed their participants with the stereo-
experimenter was talking to a confederate. Of interest type of African Americans, after which these participants
was the proportion of participants who interrupted the performed worse on a math test compared with control
conversation within 10 minutes. As expected, participants participants (see also Shih, Ambady, Richeson, Fujita, &
who were primed with rudeness were more likely to inter- Gray, 2002).
rupt (63%) than were control participants (38%), whereas It has also been shown that activation of the stereotype
participants primed with politeness were least likely to of the elderly affects memory performance among under-
interrupt (17%). These findings were recently replicated graduate participants (Dijksterhuis, Aarts, et al., 2000;
and extended by Krolak-Schwerdt (2003), who primed par- Dijksterhuis, Bargh & Miedema, 2000; but see Levy,
ticipants with the traits extraverted or introverted and found 1996). In an experiment conducted by Dijksterhuis, Bargh
248 Automaticity and the Unconscious

and Miedema (2000) participants were seated behind a first used by Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996). Whereas
desk on which 15 objects were placed (a book, a pencil, Bargh and colleagues had shown that priming the elderly
a bag, etc.). Some participants were asked to answer ques- stereotype led participants to walk slower, Dijksterhuis,
tions about elderly people (“How often do you meet elderly Spears, and colleagues (1998) showed that priming an
people?” Do you think elderly people are conservative?”), elderly exemplar (the 88-year-old Dutch Queen Mother)
whereas others were asked to answer questions about col- prompted participants to walk faster.
lege students. After answering questions for 3 minutes, To recap, behavioral priming has been demonstrated for
participants were placed in a different room and asked to various behaviors, and it can be elicited by using catego-
recall as many of the objects present in the previous room ries as well as traits as primes. The effects are preconscious
as possible. Participants primed with the elderly stereotype (they can even be brought about subliminally) or postcon-
recalled fewer objects than other participants (see also scious, and they are likely to be goal-independent. The
Dijksterhuis, Aarts, et al., 2000). underlying process is more complex than initially thought
Finally, recent research shows that stereotype activation (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996; Dijksterhuis & Bargh,
can lead to converging attitudes (Kawakami, Dovidio, & 2001). The effects can be explained by the ideomotor
Dijksterhuis, 2003). In the experiments by Kawakami mechanism, that is, the notion that perception and thought
and colleagues, half of the participants were primed with have a direct link with overt behavior (e.g., James, 1890).
the stereotype of the elderly. In different experiments, This may make behavioral priming a “default” effect,
different priming methods were used, ranging from con- where additional processes, such as goals, are needed
scious manipulations to subliminal manipulations. In a for the effects not to occur. However, there is evidence
second, ostensibly unrelated task, participants were asked that at least in some cases the process is more complex
to what extent they agreed with attitude statements such in that people’s self-concept is involved in guiding overt
as, “There is too much sex and nudity on TV these days” and behavior after priming in a specific direction (i.e., contrast
“More people should go to church these days.” Following or assimilation; Wheeler, DeMarree, & Petty, 2007).
the prevailing stereotype of elderly expressed more con- In addition, the fact that these effects can be seen as
servative attitudes relative to control participants who a form of goal-independent, default social behavior (just
were not primed. In a conceptual replication, Kawakami like imitation) does not mean that goals do not play a role.
and colleagues (2003) also showed that activation of the Research has shown that goals can moderate the effects of
social category “skinhead” led to more racist attitudes. behavioral priming. Cesario, Higgins, and Plaks (2006)
Dijksterhuis, Spears, and colleagues (1998) demon- proposed that behavioral priming effects are caused by
strated that priming does not always result in behavioral perceivers preparing to interact with the primed group
assimilation but that it can also result in behavioral con- members. For instance, in one of their studies, participants
trast. Based on earlier work showing that category or trait primed with a disliked outgroup (gay men) showed evidence
activation usually leads to assimilation in judgments, for interaction preparation (i.e., increased aggression)
whereas activation of exemplars usually leads to contrast rather than assimilation behavior (i.e., passivity). Jonas and
(Stapel, Koomen, & van der Pligt, 1996; 1997; see also Sassenberg (2006) also demonstrated that primed partici-
Wyer & Srull, 1989), Dijksterhuis, Spears, and colleagues pants do not always show assimilative behavior. Instead,
assessed behavioral effects of exemplar activation versus they sometimes show appropriate response behavior. For
category activation. In their first experiment, they example, if one is primed with a nurse, one can show
primed participants with either categories or exemplars. assimilative behavior such as helpfulness but also response
Concretely, participants were primed with either the cat- behavior that is most appropriate in the situation, such as
egory “college professors” or the category “supermodels,” cooperativeness.
or with the exemplar “Albert Einstein” or the exemplar
“Claudia Schiffer.” After the priming procedure, partici- Goal Pursuit
pants answered a number of trivia questions. In line with
earlier work (Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg, 1998), To analyze the automaticity of goal pursuit, it is helpful to
participants primed with professors outperformed those divide the process into various stages. The first stage is the
primed with supermodels. More important, however, prim- adoption stage. The second stage is the activation stage.
ing exemplars lead to behavioral contrast. Participants After a goal has been formed, it can be activated time and
primed with Albert Einstein performed worse on the again, but this step is necessary before a goal can affect
general knowledge questions than participants primed behavior. The third stage is the planning stage, in which
with Claudia Schiffer. In further studies, these behavioral people develop strategies to attain the goal. The final stage is
contrast effects were also demonstrated in the paradigm the goal-striving and monitoring stage. People engage in
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 249

goal-directed behavior, and progress toward a reference rather than laughs. This can be perceived as a threat to the
point needs to be monitored. If there are discrepancies higher-level goal to affiliate or maintain self-esteem, and
between the current state of affairs and the reference hence, the goal representation of affiliation or to height-
point, the individual has to persist (or to decide whether ened self-esteem is activated.
to quit, revise, or disengage; Carver & Scheier, 1998). If But can goals be adopted without conscious awareness?
there are no discrepancies between the behavior and the There is considerable evidence—for instance, from the
reference point, the goal has been attained. Traditionally, literature on evaluative conditioning—that the valence of
people have assumed that conscious awareness of the goal stimuli can be changed without awareness (see earlier this
is necessary to successfully complete all stages. Recently, chapter). If stimuli are paired with positive stimuli, these
however, it has been proposed that goals can be activated stimuli become more positive, whereas if stimuli are paired
outside consciousness and, in fact, that the entire process with negative stimuli, these stimuli become more negative.
of goal pursuit may ensue without conscious awareness of This means that behavior can be made more desirable.
the goal (e.g., Bargh et al., 2001). However, this does not yet show that people will commit to
The proposal that goals can be activated automatically goals that were created this way. That is, although people
is based on two ideas. First, goals are mentally represented may evaluate a behavior more positively, it does not auto-
as desired states in a hierarchically ordered knowledge matically mean that they want to engage in it. Concretely,
structure (Kruglanski et al., 2002). Such hierarchical I am positive about running a marathon, but I do not see
knowledge structures include desired states, actions, and myself ever running one.
means to reach the desired states. For this reason, goals However, recent research disentangled these “liking”
can be activated automatically, just like other mental effects from “wanting” effects (Custers & Aarts, 2005).
representations (Bargh et al., 2001; Chartrand & Bargh, In some clever experiments, neutral behavioral states (i.e.,
1996; Fishbach, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2003). Second, activities such as doing puzzles or going for a walk) were
consciously chosen goals can develop the capacity to paired unconsciously with positive, neutral, or negative
become unconsciously activated. When goals are activated words, and subsequently participants were asked to evalu-
consistently and repeatedly in a specific situation, these ate these activities and to indicate whether they wanted to
goals will be activated automatically whenever the person engage in these activities. Consistent with the evaluative
encounters that same situation. The question addressed conditioning literature, activities paired with positive words
here is whether conscious awareness is necessary in any were evaluated more positively, and activities paired with
of the above stages of goal pursuit (see also Bongers & negative words were evaluated more negatively, compared
Dijksterhuis, 2009; Moskowitz, Li, & Kirk, 2004). with activities paired with neutral words. More important,
wanting was higher for activities that were paired with
Goal Adoption positive words, compared with activities that were paired
Goals can be formed internally as well as externally. Goals with neutral and negative words, but there was no differ-
that originate externally are often assigned goals, such ence in wanting between activities that were paired with
as when a mother asks her young child to finish the food neutral words and activities that were paired with negative
on his or her plate. Such goals have to be endorsed by words. These data demonstrate that goals can be formed
the individual for them to be pursued. Whether a goal and adopted unconsciously.
is endorsed or not depends on an evaluation process, in As mentioned earlier, goals that originate internally are
which constraints (will my mother be angry when I do not often invoked when other goals are not reached. Detecting
finish my plate?) as well as the attractiveness of the goal a discrepancy between a current state and a chronic goal
(how awful is the food?) will be weighted. If people evalu- state will lead to the activation of middle-level goals and
ate a goal positively, they may endorse it; if not, they may a selection of lower-level goals. Recent research has dem-
not accept the goal and will not commit to it. onstrated that this can occur unconsciously as well. In an
Goals that originate internally can be elicited when experiment by Custers and Aarts (2005), participants were
other goals are not met. This can occur as a top-down presented with 25 sentences, each immediately followed
process as well as a bottom-up process. An example of a probe word. In one condition, five of these sentences
a top-down process is that when the higher-level goal to described actual states or situations that were discrepant
achieve academically is not reached. One fails an exam, with the goal to look well groomed (i.e., “The shoes you
the goal to pass the retest will be activated, and hence, the put on look dirty.”), and in the other condition these five
lower-level goal to study hard will be adopted. An example sentences were nondiscrepant with that goal (i.e., “The
of a bottom-up process is a joke that falls flat: You attract shoes you put on have laces.”). After presenting each sen-
attention, deliver the punchline, and you find blanks stares tence, participants were asked to indicate whether the probe
250 Automaticity and the Unconscious

word appeared in that sentence. The probe word referred to Gollwitzer, & Hassin, 2004). They reasoned that goals
an applicable instrumental action (i.e., to polish) that never inferred from another person’s actions may be readily
appeared in the sentence. If people automatically activate adopted when these goals represent a positive, desired
goal-directed actions upon reading goal-discrepant sen- state for the perceiver (cf. Heider, 1958), thus leading to
tences, the reaction time to indicate that the probe word did what they termed goal contagion. To investigate this, in
not appear in the sentence would be longer after discrepant one of their studies, they briefly exposed participants to
than nondiscrepant sentences. The results indeed showed a short script implying either the goal of earning money
this reaction time difference, but only for participants or not. After reading the goal-implying scenario, partici-
who frequently pursued the goal to be well groomed. In pants were told that they could participate in a lottery in
sum, detecting a discrepancy between a current state and a which they could win money, but only if there was enough
chronic goal state automatically leads to the activation of time left. They were then given a computer task, and the
middle-level goals. question was whether participants would speed up their
performance as a means to ensure that they could participate
Goal Activation in the goal-relevant task. Results showed that participants
Well-established goals can be unconsciously triggered who were exposed to the behavior implying the goal of
by the environment. For example, Bargh and colleagues earning money were indeed faster than those in the con-
(2001) unobtrusively exposed participants to words such trol condition. These behavioral changes occurred without
as “strive” and “succeed” to prime the achievement goal conscious intent and were more pronounced when their
(a goal held by most students) and then gave them the desire to earn money was relatively strong. These findings
opportunity to perform well by giving them an anagram were replicated in another experiment with heterosexual
task. Participants primed with the achievement goal out- male students and the goal of seeking casual sex.
performed those who were not primed with the goal. Bargh Goals may also be automatically activated by being
et al. (2001) also demonstrated that such goal priming leads primed with significant others. Research has shown that
to qualities associated with motivational states or “goal- goals and resultant actions are activated when people are
directedness,” such as persistence and increased effort in exposed to the names of friends, parents, and spouses
working toward the goal. These and other recent experi- (Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2003; Shah, 2003). For exam-
mental demonstrations (e.g., Aarts, Custers, & Marien, ple, Fitzsimons and Bargh (2003) hypothesized that
2008; Lakin & Chartrand, 2003; Oikawa, 2004; Shah, people readily take on the goals they share with others
Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2002; Sheeran, Aarts, et al., on the basis of the interpersonal interactions they have
2005) indicate that the mere activation of a goal represen- with them. Hence, priming the names of these others leads
tation suffices to motivate people to work on the primed to the automatic adoption of the goals associated with
goal without any conscious thought. them. Accordingly, capitalizing on the notion that the goal
In another recent study, Holland, Hendriks, and Aarts of helping is part of, and strongly associated with, inter-
(2005) examined whether the mere perception of odor is personal interactions with good friends, Fitzsimons and
capable of directly activating goals. They exposed some Bargh (2003) found that participants who were merely
participants to the scent of all-purpose cleaner with- instructed to think of a good friend were more often
out participants’ conscious awareness of the presence of helpful to the experimenter by agreeing to participate.
the scent. Because the scent of all-purpose cleaner was These automatic goal-adoption effects were replicated for
assumed to enhance the accessibility of the concept of different relationships and different interpersonal goals,
cleaning, Holland et al. (2005) hypothesized that partici- such as child–mother relations and the goal of achieve-
pants exposed to the scent would spontaneously start to be ment and were moderated by the degree to which partici-
cleaner. Participants were requested to eat a very crumbly pants associated the goal with the relationship.
cookie in the lab, and indeed, participants exposed to the
Choosing Means
scent put in more effort to keep their direct environment
clean and crumb-free. The planning stage refers to the development of strategies
An interesting implication of the automatic goal to attain a goal. After a goal has been adopted and acti-
inference effect described earlier (Hassin et al., 2005, see vated, alternative behavioral paths by which a goal can
the “Person Perception” section) is that people may also be attained must be evaluated and selected. The goal to be
be able to automatically activate goals that they infer healthy, for instance, can be achieved by quitting smok-
from others’ goal-directed actions. Evidence that per- ing, by exercising more often, by eating more healthy food
ceivers indeed start to strive for such goals comes from and less unhealthy food, by reducing alcohol intake, or
a recent study conducted by Aarts and colleagues (Aarts, by any combination of these and many other behaviors.
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 251

For successful goal pursuit, people need to select means or capture your attention, even when you did not hear a
lower-level goals. word the same person said beforehand. As soon as some-
Although the selection of means is sometimes a thing relevant or important is said (i.e., goal-related or
conscious process, such as when we engage in conscious self-relevant information), this unconscious monitoring
planning (this is discussed in the section “Control”), choos- process brings that information into your attention and into
ing means can also occur unconsciously. If people consis- your consciousness.
tently and repeatedly choose the same lower-order goal to Other examples of unconscious monitoring processes
attain a higher-order goal (such as always taking the stairs can be found in the literature on thought suppression
to be healthy), activation of this lower-order goal can become (Wegner, 1994; Wegner & Erber, 1992). When people are
automatic. This happens, for instance, in habitual behavior. not allowed to think about something or do not want to
Aarts and Dijksterhuis (2000) directly tested the idea that think about something, the “forbidden” thought is, ironi-
habits are represented as goal-mean links in the realm of cally, more likely to pop into consciousness. For instance,
travel behavior. In one set of studies, they employed a when participants are asked not to think about white bears
response latency paradigm to demonstrate that for habitual (i.e., they are asked to suppress these thoughts) for a
cyclists (an easy group to find in the Netherlands), cycling period of time, after that period, they report more thoughts
is automatically activated by the goal to travel to the uni- about white bears than participants who were not asked
versity. In their experiments, habitual and nonhabitual to suppress the thoughts in the first place. The reasoning
bicycle users were unobtrusively primed with the goal to behind these findings is that for participants who were not
travel to the university or not. In a subsequent reaction time allowed to think about white bears, a monitoring process
task, accessibility of the concept of bicycle was measured. searched for failures to suppress these thoughts. In other
Results showed that the travel goal facilitated access to the words, a monitoring process keeps track of whether some-
concept of bicycle but only for those people who regularly one is still engaging in goal pursuit, and threats to this
use the bicycle for this trip. It appears that conscious atten- pursuit are signaled to the person (this research is discussed
tion is often not necessary in the planning stage. more elaborately in the “Control” section).
The two examples described here show that the monitor-
Goal Striving and Monitoring ing process indeed signals to the person that opportunities
During the goal-striving stage, an individual engages or threats have been detected in the environment, with-
in goal-directed behavior, and progress toward goal out the person being aware of this monitoring process.
attainment is monitored. This is a crucial stage, and an Moreover, there is evidence that threats to current goal
active monitoring process is also what distinguishes goal- pursuit are inhibited automatically. In recent research (Shah
directed behavior from other behavior. Although perhaps et al., 2002), participants were asked to provide three cur-
counterintuitive, converging evidence has shown that goals rent goals. Then, in a modified lexical decision, participants
can be monitored and run to completion without the person were presented with various stimuli, including their three
ever becoming consciously aware of the goal at any point. current goals, and had to indicate whether the stimulus was
The monitoring process serves to increase the likelihood a personal goal or not. Each stimulus was preceded by an
of goal attainment by scanning the environment for threats unconscious prime: one of the three current goals or a con-
and opportunities. If an opportunity for goal pursuit is trol stimulus. Responses to current goals were slower when
detected, the monitoring process increases the salience of the preceding prime was one of the other two current goals
this opportunity by heightening its accessibility or by signal- compared with when the preceding prime was a control
ing to the person that an opportunity has been detected. If stimulus. There was no effect of prime on response time
a threat (i.e., alternative goals or temptations) is detected, when the target word was not a goal. These data indicate
the monitoring process inhibits this threat by decreasing its that the activation of a current goal results in the inhibition
accessibility. If inhibition fails—for instance, if the threat of alternative “threatening” goals. It has also been demon-
is too strong or cannot be avoided—the monitoring process strated that the activation of a goal can lead to inhibition of
signals to the person that goal pursuit might be under threat. temptations (Fishbach, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2003).
One example of unconscious monitoring is the cocktail- In sum, monitoring can occur outside of awareness. If
party effect (Moray, 1959). This effect is based on the opportunities for successful goal pursuit are detected in the
idea that people have a chronic goal to process self- environment, these opportunities then increase in acces-
relevant information. Imagine you are at a big party. You sibility, whereas if threats to successful goal pursuit are
consciously focus on a conversation with a friend, filtering detected, these threats then decrease in accessibility. Taken
out the noise of others. However, if someone other than together, there is ample evidence that conscious attention
your conversation partner mentions your name, it will is not necessary in the goal-striving and monitoring stage.
252 Automaticity and the Unconscious

More broadly, the recent literature on goal pursuit shows often leads to the reply—accompanied by a wagging
that all stages of goal pursuit can proceed preconsciously. finger—that this is “not very rational.” But is it truly irra-
tional? Answering this question is important, because it
can in part explain why decision-making research was
Judgment and Decision Making
hardly influenced by automaticity research.
Perhaps because the research area of judgment and decision So how do our choices hold up against the standard of
making is strongly influenced by economics’ emphasis on rationality? This depends on the definition of rationality.
rationality, the influence of researchers interested in auto- There are two distinct ways to define it: rationality from
maticity have been modest. A fine-grained analysis of which a normative viewpoint and rationality from a subjective
aspects of decision making are automatic, and to what extent viewpoint. Evans and Over (1996, 1997) defined the nor-
they are automatic, is impossible at this time. However, mative version as follows: “Thinking, speaking, reasoning,
there is a clearly visible trend toward a more prominent role making a decision, or acting when one has a reason for
for intuitive and automatic processes in understanding deci- what one does sanctioned by a normative theory” (Evans &
sion making. For a full discussion on judgment and decision Over, 1997, p. 2, emphasis added). The second defini-
making, see Gilovich and Griffin (this volume). tion they offered is more subjective: “Thinking, speaking,
Leading theories in the realm of decision making have reasoning, making a decision, or acting in a way that is
long been based on the idea of subjective expected utility generally reliable and efficient for achieving one’s goals”
(Edwards, 1961): Decisions should be aimed at finding the (Evans & Over, 1997, p. 2, emphasis added).
alternative with the best possible outcome(s). More spe- For a long time, the first, normative definition was favored
cifically, a choice between alternatives should be based by researchers. A problem with the normative definition,
on weighted adding: For each alternative, the valence of however, is that it irrevocably leads to the somewhat sober-
the various attributes is determined, and later these attri- ing conclusion that human decision makers are indeed often
butes are given a weight corresponding to their relative highly irrational. The number of well-documented phenom-
importance (see, e.g., Janis & Mann, 1977). In the end, the ena in human decision making that show violations of a nor-
weighted sum should determine the choice. mative definition of rationality is so enormous (Kahneman &
At first this approach was treated as both normatively Tversky, 2000; Shafir & LeBoeuf, 2002) that the conclusion
desirable and descriptively accurate. That is, people should that we are almost all poor decision makers, continually find-
do it this way, and, by and large, they seem to do it this way. ing ourselves in trouble, would be almost inescapable. Yet
However, a rational choice made in such a way requires a life experience shows that many people are doing quite well,
lot of deliberation or conscious thought, and people rarely thank you, suggesting that it may be illuminating to pay more
engage in the amount of deliberation necessary for accu- attention to people’s own goals and standards (Gigerenzer &
rate weighted adding. Moreover, to engage in accurate Todd, 1999). It makes more sense from both a psychological
weighted adding, people need to determine how important and an evolutionary perspective to use a subjective definition
different attributes are. We now know that this requires a (Cosmides & Tooby, 1996). Evolution cares about whether
degree of introspection that people simply cannot achieve people achieve their goals (especially the most basic ones,
(see, e.g., Gilbert, 2006). That is, rationality is bounded by such as finding food, avoiding danger, and procreating), and
human and environmental limitations (H. A. Simon, 1955), psychology should reflect these priorities. Stated bluntly,
and people use heuristics—or mental shortcuts—to arrive evolution does not care about logic.
at decisions (Kahneman, 2003; Tversky & Kahneman, If we are willing to accept that rationality should be
1973, 1974). These heuristics, in turn, can lead to various defined in terms of goal achievement (and not in terms
biases that can violate statistical or logical principles. of normative theories), it becomes clear that the relation
between rationality and conscious thought or deliberation
Are We Rational? is completely different than once assumed. After all, in the
One effect of the classic approach to studying decision previous section, evidence was discussed for the notion
making is that rationality became more or less equated that goal pursuit can ensue without conscious intervention.
with deliberation or with conscious thought. A decision The bottom line is that the relation between the rationality
that has been thought about carefully is rational, whereas of a decision and the “consciousness” of the preceding
a quick decision is usually irrational. This interchangeable thought process is likely to be orthogonal.
use of the terms “rational” and “conscious” can also be wit-
Heuristics and Biases
nessed outside academia. A remark that an important deci-
sion, such as buying a new car, has been made on the basis Two insights were important for the development of a
of intuition rather than on a period of conscious thought more realistic and less normative look at decision making.
Unconscious and Automatic Processes in Social Psychology 253

The first was the realization that people often use heuristics it should be noted that the researchers who first described
to make judgments and are subject to systematic biases these heuristics (e.g., Tversky & Kahneman, 1973; see also
(e.g., Kahneman & Tversky, 2000; Tversky & Kahneman, Nisbett & Ross, 1989) portrayed them as highly adaptive,
1973, 1974). In an extremely impressive line of research, a viewpoint that has also been advocated by Gigerenzer
Amos Tversky and Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman more recently (e.g., Gigerenzer, 2008; Gigerenzer & Todd,
investigated many such heuristics and biases. Their 1999). A good example of a highly adaptive heuristic is
research was guided by “the idea that intuitive judgments the recognition heuristic (Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002).
occupy a position—perhaps corresponding to evolutionary In an experiment, participants in both the United States
history—between the automatic operations of perception and Germany were asked which of two U.S. cities, Detroit
and the deliberate operations of reasoning” (Kahneman, or Milwaukee, was bigger. Intuitively, one would predict
2003, p. 697). In a drastic departure from the past, Tversky U.S. participants would do better, because they should
and Kahneman opened the door to intuitive, or even auto- have more information about their own cities than German
matic, processes. It is impossible to review their entire participants. However, as it turned out, the German par-
body of work, so only one example of the use of a heuris- ticipants did better. Approximately 90% of the German
tic, as well as of the effect of a bias, is discussed here. participants gave the correct answer (Detroit) as opposed to
People use the availability heuristic to assess frequency 60% of the U.S. participants. The U.S. participants indeed
and probability. For example, Kahneman and Tversky knew more, and used their information—including nondi-
(1973) asked participants whether there are more words agnostic information—to make their guesses. The German
beginning with the letter “r” or with “r” as the third letter. participants, devoid of much information, simply chose the
Most participants thought there were more words begin- one they recognized best, and indeed arrived at the right
ning with an “r,” whereas there are in fact more words with answer. The recognition heuristic is highly useful under
an “r” as the third letter. The reason participants made this many circumstances. Gigerenzer and colleagues even
error is that words with an “r” in first position are easier to showed that simply following the recognition heuristic can
bring to mind—they are more available—than words with lead to investment decisions that are much better than those
an “r” in third position. Slovic, Fischoff, and Lichtenstein made by experts (Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002).
(1982) showed effects of the availability heuristic on
The Fallibility of Conscious Thought
people’s assessments of perceived frequency of causes of
death. Causes that come to mind easily because of media The second important insight was that conscious thought
attention are overestimated (accidents, tornadoes, homi- is not always beneficial for decision making. Wilson and
cide), whereas causes that are difficult to bring to mind are colleagues (e.g., Wilson & Schooler, 1991; Wilson et al.,
underestimated (vaccinations, diabetes, asthma). 1993; see also Levine, Halbertstadt, & Goldstone, 1996)
An example of a bias that people can fall prey to is the have demonstrated that conscious contemplation disturbs
framing effect. Tversky and Kahneman (1981; see also what they called “natural weighting schemes.” In one well-
Kahneman & Tversky, 1979) discovered that whether known experiment, Wilson et al. (1993) looked at the post-
choice outcomes are framed in terms of gains or losses has choice satisfaction of people who had to choose from five
a profound effect on the choices. The well-known “Asian art posters. Some participants had been merely asked to
disease” problem provides a good example. Assume 600 choose, whereas others had been asked to deliberate. More
people are infected with a disease. You have to choose specifically, they had been asked to carefully scrutinize the
between two intervention programs, both framed in terms reasons for their preference. As it turned out, people who
of gains: With program A, you save 200 people. With pro- had engaged in thorough conscious thought were less happy
gram B, you have a one third chance that you will save with their choice. Wilson and colleagues (1993, p. 332)
all 600 people, and a two thirds chance that no one will attributed this to suboptimal weighing: “Introspection . . .
be saved. Faced with this dilemma, most people avoid risk can change an optimal weighing scheme into a suboptimal
and choose program A. However, one can also frame the one. When people analyze reasons, they might focus on
choice in terms of losses: With program A, 400 people will those attributes of the attitude object that seem like plausi-
die. With program B you have a one third chance that no ble causes of the evaluations but were not weighted heavily
one will die and a two thirds chance that all will die. In this before.” Conscious thought leads people to put dispropor-
case, most people choose to be risky and select program B. tionate weight on attributes that are accessible, plausible,
Heuristics have been seen by many people as unavoid- and easy to verbalize (see also Schooler, Ohlsson, & Brooks,
able human shortcomings. We should not really use 1993) and therefore too little weight on other attributes.
heuristics, but alas, given cognitive and environmental There is also research on how consistently people weight
constraints, we sometimes have to rely on them. However, attributes. In experiments by Levine and colleagues (1996),
254 Automaticity and the Unconscious

participants had to evaluate a large number of faces that a model for decision making based on what Stanovich and
varied along six dimensions (such as the shape of the nose). West (2000) had called System 1 and System 2. System 1
Participants either merely evaluated these faces or had to is the intuitive system, and its features map onto the fea-
think about the reasons for their evaluations before doing tures we associate with automatic processes: fast, parallel,
so. Of interest to the experimenters was the way people effortless, associative, and so on. System 2 is the reason-
used and weighted the six dimensions to evaluate the faces. ing system, and its features are the ones we associate with
The data clearly demonstrated that conscious thought made conscious, controlled processes: slow, serial, effortful,
weighting more varied and inconsistent. Recently, these rule-governed, and so on. As of yet, there is no clear “road-
findings were replicated and extended by Nordgren and map” to show when automatic versus controlled processes
Dijksterhuis (2009), who showed that when people judge dominate the decision process. However, there is no more
the exact same object under the exact same circumstances escaping the conclusion that an important portion of judg-
twice (with 45 minutes in between), they become more ment and decision making is done by intuitive and auto-
inconsistent the longer they think about their judgments. matic processes.
Finally, there is also considerable research showing that
Unconscious Thought
conscious deliberation is not a successful antidote against
certain biases or against the use of inappropriate heuristics. In my own laboratory, my colleagues and I have sought to
Igou and Bless (2008) demonstrated that the more moti- find evidence for the relative automaticity of one stage of
vated people are to deliberate about a choice, the bigger the decision process. When people have processed informa-
framing effects become. Pelham and Neter (1995) demon- tion about various choice alternatives, how do they form a
strated that motivating people to think harder may increase, preference for one of the alternatives? Traditionally, people
rather than decrease, the effects of the representativeness have thought that this is when conscious deliberation (per-
heuristic. More thought may sometimes simply lead to haps even with a balance sheet listing plusses and minuses)
more use of whatever is available, such as a heuristic. As comes in. However, my colleagues and I (Dijksterhuis,
Pelham and Neter put it (1995, p. 583), “If the only tool at Bos, Nordgren, & van Baaren, 2006) reasoned that, because
a person’s disposal is a hammer, convincing the person to consciousness has such a low processing capacity and
work harder can only lead to more vigorous hammering.” because consciousness is often poor at weighting the
In general, there is strong evidence demonstrating that relative importance of attributes (Wilson et al., 1993), so-
more conscious thought does not simply lead to better called unconscious thought may be preferable for arriving
choices. Camerer and Hogarth (1999; see also Shafir & at a preference in a complex decision situation.
LeBoeuf, 2002) reviewed 74 studies on the relation between In one of our experiments, participants read information
motivation (manipulated with incentives) and proneness pertaining to a choice problem. More specifically, they
to heuristics and biases. Although they found that moti- would be presented with information about four apart-
vation can help, it can just as well deteriorate choosing. ments, in which each apartment was described using 12
The problem is that whatever the decision problem is, aspects. We rigged the information so that one apartment
people always have to apply the right strategy to solve it. had many more positive attributes (and therefore fewer
More motivation may occasionally help, but often it just negative attributes) than the others. After participants
leads to more enthusiastic application of a wrong strategy, had read all the information, some were asked to choose
thereby leading to inferior choices (e.g., Arkes, Dawes, & between the apartments immediately. Others were given
Christensen, 1986). some time to consciously think before they chose, whereas
a third group was distracted for a while and then asked to
The Current State of Affairs: Two Systems choose. Participants in this latter group were performing
The literature on heuristics shows that people often do not a very taxing task, preventing conscious thought. Instead,
engage in thorough conscious thought when making deci- they were hypothesized to engage in unconscious thought.
sions, whereas the literature on the fallibility of conscious What we have generally found with this paradigm is that
thought shows that this is not necessarily a bad thing. One unconscious thinkers make better decisions than either con-
may then assume that decision makers sometimes engage scious thinkers or immediate choosers, in that they choose
in thorough conscious thought and on other occasions rely alternatives with more positive and fewer negative char-
on more intuitive, automatic processes. Indeed, Kahneman acteristics (for more details, see Dijksterhuis, 2004b;
(2003) recently proposed such a dual-process (or dual- Dijksterhuis et al., 2006; Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006;
systems) model. Ham, van den Bos, & van Doorn, 2009). Moreover, we
On the basis of a long tradition of dual-process models have also obtained similar effects with participants who
(e.g., Chaiken & Trope, 1999), Kahneman (2003) proposed chose an actual object (such as a poster) rather than a
Control 255

hypothetical one, with quality of choice operationalized Preconscious Goal-Independent Processes


as postchoice satisfaction (Dijksterhuis et al., 2006;
Strictly speaking, control is impossible for these processes.
Dijksterhuis & van Olden, 2006).
Control is a goal, and these processes are independent
In a recent set of experiments, we showed that uncon-
of active goals, including control goals. The only form of
scious thought is a goal-dependent process (Bos et al., 2008).
control that applies here is correction of the outcome of the
In our experiments, we again gave participants information
process. You cannot control closing your eye upon the per-
about a decision problem. All participants were distracted
ception of an approaching snowball, but you can “control”
before they made a decision. However, one group was told
opening your eye later on. Hence, the form of control that
that we would later ask them some questions about the deci-
can be used for preconscious goal-independent processes
sion problem before they were distracted, because we have
is after-the-fact correction. Importantly, to do this one
always instructed unconscious-thought participants in pre-
has to become consciously aware of the consequence of
vious experiments. The other group was instead told that
the preconscious goal-independent process (Wilson &
they were done with the decision problem and would not
Brekke, 1994), and this often does not happen. A social-
be asked anything about it later. In other words, one group
psychological example especially familiar to people who
had the goal to further process the information, whereas the
speak multiple languages is the following: You are engag-
other group had no such goal. Results showed that the former
ing in a conversation and at some point you notice that
group made better decisions than the latter. This means that
you are mimicking the accent of your conversation partner.
unconscious thought is a postconscious goal-dependent pro-
After you are consciously aware—and perhaps a little
cess. Merely distracting people does not help.
embarrassed—of this spontaneous mimicry, you can
As noted earlier, it would be premature to provide a
engage in an attempt to control.
detailed treatment of the relative automaticity of various
stages in the decision-making process. However, intuitive
and automatic processes do claim a more prominent role, and Preconscious Goal-Dependent Processes
there is increasing recognition that this is not by definition
a bad thing (Brainerd & Reyna, 1990; Dijksterhuis & Many of the processes social psychologists are interested
Nordgren, 2006; Gigerenzer & Todd, 1999; Kahneman, in (attitudinal processes, processes involved in stereotyping
2003; Wilson & Schooler, 1991). and prejudice, attribution, social judgment, etc.) are pre-
conscious (or postconscious) goal-dependent processes,
and control is highly relevant here. Just as preconscious
CONTROL goal-independent processes can be controlled by after-the-
fact correction, so, too, can preconscious goal-dependent
One may draw the conclusion that because social behavior processes. However, there is a much more interesting way
is largely an automatic affair, there is little explanatory in which preconscious goal-dependent processes can be
space left for the flipside of the coin: control. Although it controlled. It follows from the definition of control as a
is indeed true that social behavior is largely automatized, goal, that, in principle at least, all these processes can be
it does not necessarily follow that people do not (attempt controlled. After all, if they are guided by a goal, they can
to) control their actions. On the contrary, they do so very also be guided by a control goal. Given that goals can exert
often. The questions addressed here are when and how? their effects preconsciously, control goals should be able
Moors and de Houwer (2006, 2007) defined control as a to do so as well. This opens the possibility of preconscious
special kind of goal. Control is the goal to change, alter, or control.
stop an ongoing process. To answer the questions of what Evidence for preconscious control has been gath-
we can control and how we can do this, it is useful to refer ered in various domains, arguably the most interesting
to the earlier taxonomy of social psychological processes in in the domain of stereotyping and prejudice. Moskowitz,
terms of a 2 (preconscious/postconscious vs. conscious) × Gollwitzer, Wasel, and Schaal (1999) recruited male par-
2 (goal-dependent vs. goal-independent) table. The pos- ticipants with chronic egalitarian goals toward women
sibility of controlling a psychological process—and the as well as participants without such goals. In the experi-
strategies that can be deployed for such control—depend ment, participants had to pronounce trait words as quickly
on the cell of the 2 × 2 table the process falls into. Control as possible. Some of these traits were stereotypical for
is discussed for the cells individually, distinguishing women, and some were stereotypical for men. These trait
between four kinds of control: preconscious control, cor- words were preceded by a photograph of either a man
rection, suppression, and planning. A separate subsection of woman. Under normal circumstances, participants should
is devoted to the role of consciousness in control. show evidence of stereotype activation: Female traits
256 Automaticity and the Unconscious

should pronounced more quickly when they follow a The reason lies in the nature of the suppression process.
photograph of a woman than a photograph of a man. This When we try to suppress a thought—for instance, about
was indeed found for individuals who had no goal to be smoking a cigarette—two processes start simultaneously:
egalitarian. However, for individuals who had the chronic an operating process and a monitoring process. The operat-
goal to be egalitarian, no stereotype activation was appar- ing process is effortful and is responsible for finding dis-
ent. In fact, they seemed to inhibit stereotype-activation, tractors: If I think about tonight’s soccer game between
thereby successfully engaging in preconscious control. In Holland and Italy, it is easier not to think about a ciga-
other studies (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000), it was also rette. The monitoring process is automatic and searches the
found that preconscious control of stereotype activation mind for the unwanted thought itself. However, this pro-
was possible with temporary goals (such as perspective cess can have ironic consequences because it can actually
taking) rather than chronic goals. increase the activation of the unwanted thought. As long
Again, although the evidence is lacking for most phe- as the operating process functions properly, no problems
nomena, it is likely that preconscious control is possible arise, but if the effortful operating process breaks down, a
for all preconscious goal-dependent phenomena one wants rebound effect occurs.
to control. Stereotyping and prejudice are two phenomena Wegner and colleagues have documented numerous
that people obviously may want to control, but one could such rebound effects, especially when people are under
also think about persuasion or unwarranted negative attri- load—that is, when the automatic monitoring process is
butions. Krull and Ericksson (1995) showed that people effective but the operating process is not. People who try
make situational (rather than dispositional) attributions to be happy under load become sad, whereas the reverse
when given the goal to do so, and there is no reason to is true for people who try to be sad (Wegner, Erber, &
assume that this cannot happen preconsciously. Zanakos, 1993). People who try to sleep stay awake and
those who want to stay awake have an easier time fall-
Conscious Goal-Independent Processes ing asleep (Ansfield, Wegner, & Bowser, 1996). People
This is a somewhat odd class of processes, but one can who are afraid to fail to control sexist thoughts are espe-
think about fleeting daydreams, other random thoughts, cially likely to express them (Wegner, Erber, Bowman, &
and unwanted conscious thoughts. It is not perfectly clear Shelton, 1996). Finally, others have found rebound
whether such thoughts are functional in some way (although effects among people who wanted to suppress stereotypes
daydreams probably are; Antrobus & Singer, 1964), but it is (Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, & Jetten, 1994).
clear that consciousness is sometimes occupied with thoughts
Conscious Goal-Dependent Processes
that are extremely difficult to link to ongoing goals. In fact,
empirical studies from the 1960s on daydreaming (for a In a way, one could argue that these processes represent
review, see Singer, 1975) showed that experimental manipu- (conscious) control. They do not need to be controlled
lation to decrease participants’ “task-irrelevant thoughts” because they are control. Suppression falls into this cat-
such as daydreams were often surprisingly unsuccessful, egory, and so does correction. Another example is plan-
suggesting that we may daydream more than we realize. ning (e.g., Gollwitzer, 1993). People try to affect their own
In addition to thoughts that seem not to serve a goal, peo- future behavior by planning their actions. As mentioned
ple also have thoughts that go against current goals (Wegner, before during the discussion of goal pursuit, when behavior
1994). In order to control the contents of consciousness, is habitual, a goal automatically activates the appropriate
which usually means to ban thoughts that we do not want, we means to achieve the goal. When this is not the case,
can engage in suppression (e.g., Wegner, 1994; Wegner & however, people may benefit from planning. One way of
Erber, 1992). Wegner and colleagues (Wegner, Schneider, selecting means is by forming implementation intentions
Carter, & White, 1987) started to investigate thought sup- (Gollwitzer, 1993). With implementation intentions, antici-
pression in the 1980s, with a paradigm that became known pated situations are associated with goal-directed behav-
as the “white bear paradigm.” Participants in their first stud- iors. Implementation intentions specify the when, where,
ies were simply asked to think about whatever they wanted, and how of goal-directed behavior (i.e., “If my colleague
except for a white bear. Should they think about a white offers me a cigarette after lunch, I will reject that offer”).
bear, they were to press a button so that the frequency of Research has shown that actively forming implementation
thoughts of a white bear could be assessed. This was done intentions can greatly help successful goal pursuit (for a
under varying circumstances, and Wegner and colleagues recent overview, see Gollwitzer, Parks-Stamm, Jaudas, &
found that suppression can have ironic effects: The to-be- Sheeran, 2008). In a telling experiment, for example,
suppressed thought can rebound and become more acces- student participants were asked to write a report during
sible than ever before. Christmas break. Half of the participants were asked to form
Control 257

implementation intentions by specifying the when and (association between Black and gun), as well as the con-
where of their goal-directed behavior, whereas the other half trolled process (identification of the gun) should lead to
was not asked to form implementation intentions. Two-thirds identification of the gun. Hence, the likelihood of saying
of the participants who formed implementation intentions there was a gun on a congruent trial is a function of the
pursued their goal successfully, compared with one-third of probability of control (C), plus the probability of an auto-
the participants who did not form these intentions. Although matic association between Black and gun, in case control
implementation intentions lead to automatic goal-directed fails, (A (1  C)). Hence, congruent  C  A(1  C).
behavior, the forming of implementation intentions them- When a Black person holds something harmless, the
selves is generally a conscious and effortful process. trial is inconsistent. A controlled response should lead to
Concrete implementation intentions aside, the workings correct identification of the tool, whereas the automatic
of planning are fairly poorly understood. Wegner and Bargh process (association between Black and gun) still steers
(1998, p. 455) talked about the “wobbliness in the causal the participant toward perceiving a gun. The probability
linkage from planning to acting” and used the example by that a person indeed identifies a gun on an incongruent trial
William James (1890, Vol. 2, p. 524), who planned to leave is as follows: incongruent  A(1  C).
his warm bed on a cold morning and failed to do so until With these two equations, automatic and controlled
a lapse of consciousness made him suddenly get up. The components can be estimated separately. The estimate of
relation between a conscious thought about an action and control is the difference between a “gun” response on con-
the action itself is discussed more generally later. gruent versus incongruent trials, after which the automatic
estimate can be solved.
Control Versus Automaticity in a Single Paradigm
Control (C)  congruent  incongruent
Various researchers have tried to pit automatic and controlled
processes against each other directly with (variations of) Automatic (A)  Incongruent/(1  C)
the process dissociation procedure (PDP; Jacoby, 1991).
Their approach is different from most others in that the per- In Payne’s work (e.g., Payne, 2001, 2005), the relative
spective is reversed. Rather than starting with explanatory contributions of automatic and controlled components in
concepts such as “automatic” or “controlled” that are then the shooter paradigm were estimated under different cir-
mapped onto (classes of) psychological phenomena, in this cumstances and correlated with various other measures.
approach the phenomenon is the starting point, followed As one may have expected, time pressure reduces the con-
by an attempt to estimate the relative contribution of automatic trolled component but not the automatic one. The auto-
and controlled processes (Conrey, Sherman, Gawronski, matic component was related to explicit racial attitudes.
Hugenberg, & Groom, 2005; Payne, 2001, 2005). The more negative the attitude, the greater the automatic
The PDP has been developed by Jacoby (1991) and was bias. However, this pattern only emerged for people with-
initially used to investigate memory. The logic behind it is out a motivation to control prejudice. People with such a
compelling: To pit automatic and controlled components of motivation showed the reverse pattern. Payne (2005) also
a process against each other, an experiment must contain related his findings to people’s general ability to engage
conditions where they are congruent (i.e., they work in the in executive functioning. People with poor versus good
same direction), as well as conditions where they are incon- executive functioning showed the same level of automatic
gruent (i.e., they produce opposite effects). With these two stereotype activation but strongly differed in the extent
conditions and a few algebraic equations, the contribution that they could control expressing racial bias. This effect
of automatic and controlled components can be determined. was found in the shooter paradigm, as well as in a task in
The previously mentioned “shooter paradigm” provides a which participants had to form an impression of a Black
nice example. In the shooter paradigm, a participant sees person on the basis of a biographical sketch.
pictures of Black and White men, holding either a gun or Recently, others have proposed a more complex pro-
something harmless. The task is generally to determine as cess. In their quad-model, Conrey and colleagues (2005)
quickly as possible whether the person is holding a gun. proposed four, rather than two, distinct components of per-
How does one determine the automatic and controlled formance in the shooter paradigm. A decision may be influ-
components of behavior in this paradigm? What now fol- enced by the association between Black men and guns (AC,
lows is almost literally taken from Payne (2001, p. 183), an automatic process), by the discriminability of the object
who applied PDP to the shooter paradigm: (D, a controlled process), the capacity to overcome bias and
When a Black person holds a gun, the participant is to respond appropriately in case the man does not hold a
faced with a congruent trial. Both the automatic process gun (OB, a controlled process), and finally, when the earlier
258 Automaticity and the Unconscious

three processes fail to drive behavior, by a general response it would allow us to conclude that consciousness is indeed
bias that can be influenced by many factors, such as whether the starting point for the action. However, if conscious
the person feels threatened (G, an automatic process). It is awareness of the decision to act precedes the action by
not necessary to explain the algebra of the quad-model, but much less than 800 milliseconds, we have a problem.
the studies by Conrey et al. (2005) do show some merits We would have to conclude that the action does not start
of their more fine-grained analysis with multiple controlled in consciousness. Indeed, Libet and colleagues (Libet,
and automatic processes. For instance, in one experiment Gleason, Wright, & Pearl, 1983) showed exactly this. In
the two controlled processes (D and OB) responded differently their famous experiments, they replicated the Kornhuber
to the same contextual variable: Performing the shooter and Deecke (1965) experiments while adding another mea-
task publicly negatively influenced discriminability (D) but sure: conscious awareness of the decision to act. It became
not the capacity to overcome bias (OB). clear that conscious decisions follow unconscious prepara-
tion and only precede the actual execution of the action
by about 200 milliseconds. In other words, the unconscious
The Illusion of Conscious Control
makes the decision to act.
Some readers may have raised eyebrows earlier because of In many cases, the unconscious communicates its deci-
the notion of preconscious control. With control defined as sion to consciousness, making it feel as if consciousness
a goal and goal pursuit often taking place unconsciously, has decided (see, e.g., Norretranders, 1998; Wegner, 2002).
preconscious control is a logical consequence. However, However, it did not. Such “feelings of will” are illusory
one strong connotation of control is that consciousness is and, as work by Wegner and his colleagues has shown,
involved. Isn’t the kernel of control the idea that I con- often result when conscious thought precedes correspond-
sciously intend—or want—an action before it takes place? ing behavior by a natural time interval. For instance,
The question can be phrased in another way. Where does if I flex my finger about a second after I thought about
action start? Does this happen consciously, that is, with a doing so, it may feel like a consciously willed behavior.
conscious decision? If so, we can indeed engage in con- However, if I flex my finger 30 seconds after I thought
scious control. Or is it the case that the initiation of an about doing so, it does not feel like consciously willed, as
action is always unconscious? If this is true, conscious we do not tend to see a causal relation between thought
control is, strictly speaking, nonexistent. and action that are separated by so much time (see, e.g.,
While walking to the kitchen after the sudden real- Wegner & Wheatley, 1999). Importantly, such effects do
ization one is thirsty, one can think about what to drink not speak to the actual cause of behavior, because they are,
(“let’s have some lapsang souchong tea”). Such conscious as we argue, unconscious. They do show, however, when
thoughts often seem predictive of actual behavior. We are and why people so firmly believe in a decisive or causal
often conscious of what we are about to do, and it feels role of consciousness.
like conscious thought, or conscious decisions, shape One could argue that this reasoning, as well as the find-
our actual behavior (see Wegner, 2002). This, however, ings of Libet and colleagues, may not generalize across more
does not mean the preparation of behavior starts in con- complex or more important behavior. However, there is no
sciousness. If you end up having lapsang souchong, is the a priori reason why it should not. Libet’s findings empha-
moment you consciously decide to have lapsang souchong size the idea that conscious thought is always the result of
the actual starting point? Was that conscious thought the unconscious processes, irrespective of how important the
first step toward having lapsang souchong, rather than, say, thoughts are. If you set yourself the goal to become dean
a glass of juice or a beer? of Harvard at some point in the future, you will often be
The answer is no. The unconscious preparation of even the consciously aware of this goal. However, the first moment
simplest action starts before we are conscious of the you were aware of this goal was not the starting point.
action we are about to perform. Quite some time ago, Apart from scientific evidence, there is also an appealing
Kornhuber and Deecke (1965) did experiments in which logical argument. These days, most of us assume that con-
they asked their participants to perform a simple action, sciousness resides in the brain. This necessarily means that
such as flexing a finger. At the same time, they conducted conscious awareness is a function of other (unconscious)
electroencephalograms to assess when the brain started to processes in the brain, except if one maintains the belief of
prepare for the action. The outcome was that the first sign a true “ghost in the machine.” It would be truly mystifying
of unconscious preparation preceded an action by about if behavior would start consciously. Norretranders (1998,
800 milliseconds. This really is quite a lot of time, and it led p. 221) put it as such: “unless consciousness just hovers
Libet to wonder whether conscious awareness of the deci- freely in the air, it must be linked to processes in the brain,
sion to act appears as long in advance as well. If it does, and they must necessarily start up before consciousness
What is Consciousness Good for? 259

appears. It is not our consciousness that initiates, for only exactly. These days, many neuroscientists and cognitive
the conscious is conscious.” psychologists seem to gravitate toward embracing global
The fact that behavior starts unconsciously is important workspace theory (Baars, 2002; Deheane & Naccache,
for our understanding of automaticity. It follows that we 2001). Global workspace theory maintains that the goal of
should treat the fact that behavior is unconscious as the sending information to consciousness is to create global
default. This in turn means that there really are two kinds access. Different circuits in our brain are responsible for
of behaviors, as described in the quote by Freud earlier in different tasks, and these tasks can be perceptual, cogni-
this chapter. The essence is this: First, there are behaviors tive, affective, motivational, motoric, and so on. These
of which we are never consciously aware in that the entire circuits generally do their job unconsciously, but at times
process from initiation to completion is unconscious. the different circuits need to communicate with each
Second, there are behaviors of which we do become aware other.
(once or more often), somewhere between initiation and A good example is verbal communication. Verbal com-
final completion. In other words, there is true unconscious munication seems to be almost impossible without con-
behavior, and there is unconscious behavior that is charac- scious awareness, and brief conscious access to verbal
terized by instances of conscious awareness. information is indeed a way for other modules to “observe”
what is going on and to respond appropriately (Levelt, per-
sonal communication, August 6, 2008). There are many
WHAT IS CONSCIOUSNESS GOOD FOR? other examples. Perception of an approaching car may
make immediate motoric adjustments necessary. A basic
At the time Wegner and Bargh wrote their chapter for the need such as thirst may elicit motivational and behavioral
previous Handbook of Social Psychology, many research- processes. In at least some such cases (but definitely not
ers were interested in the extent to which certain phe- all), it seems we become consciously aware of the output
nomena were unconscious, or at least the extent to which of the first circuit (e.g., the perception of the car, the sen-
they could be unconscious. More than 10 years later, we sation of thirst). In such cases, a specific unconscious cir-
know that many social-psychological processes can take cuit can tell other circuits to jump into action by putting
place unconsciously. Stated differently, we now know that information into consciousness. That being said, global
consciousness does not seem to be necessary to guide or workspace theory is plausible, but the evidence for it is
steer a number of processes that are of interest to social still far from rock solid. In fact, the direct evidence for the
psychologists. Ironically, even control, traditionally seen theory is largely confined to experiments on basic visual
as the opposite of automaticity, can be automatic. perception.
For quite some time, it was believed that consciousness, Social psychologists have mostly avoided studying the
or conscious control, was necessary for most, if not all, function of consciousness in a direct way, but they have
relatively “smart,” flexible, and complex processes. This been highly productive in an indirect way. Social psychol-
turns out not be true, but the fact that these smart processes ogy has done more than any other discipline to investigate
can ensue unconsciously does not mean they are uncon- the processes we do not need consciousness for. The list
trollable. After all, most of these processes are guided by of such processes is impressive, and this sometimes forces
goals. Indeed, the focus of social-psychological research us to draw humbling conclusions. However, the fact that
seems to shift more and more from investigating the we cannot yet pin down the function of conscious expe-
conscious–unconscious distinction toward emphasizing rience does not mean we could just as well do without
the relevance of goals. Consciousness is not crucial, but it. Some time ago, I visited a conference on unconscious
goals are, because they guide attention. In other words, processes. In many presentations, the relative importance
phenomena for which we long assumed conscious guid- of unconscious processes, as well as the relative unimpor-
ance was necessary turn out to be preconscious, but they tance of conscious processes, were emphasized. At some
are goal-dependent. point, after I had yet again advocated the crucial role of
Understanding the origin and function of conscious the unconscious, a colleague asked me a question (see
experience is seen by some as the last great frontier also Dijksterhuis, 2007). For me, it was a highly insightful
of science. Is there room left for an explanatory role of question.
consciousness? It has been popular among philosophers to Imagine the following. You can choose to have a per-
argue that there is no explanatory role and that conscious fect life. You and your loved ones are, and always will be,
experience is merely an epiphenomenon. Most scientists healthy. You are as wealthy as you want to be. You have a
believe consciousness does affect human behavior in some great love life and wonderful friends. You can really have
way, but they do not yet agree on what consciousness does everything you desire. This can all be yours, but there
260 Automaticity and the Unconscious

is one catch. To get it all, you have to hand in conscious Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A. Y., Barndollar, K., &
Troetschel, R. (2001). Bypassing the will: Automatic and controlled
experience. Would you do that?
self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
I pondered on this for no more than a second, but before 1014–1027.
I could say “no,” he already said, “me neither.” Bargh, J. A., Litt, J., Pratto, F., & Spielman, L. A. (1989). On the precon-
scious evaluation of social stimuli. In A. F. Bennett & K. M. McConkey
(Eds.), Cognition in individual and social contexts: Proceedings of
the XXV International Congress of Psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
REFERENCES Bargh, J. A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information process-
ing and social perception: The influence of trait information presented
outside of conscious awareness on impression formation. Journal of
Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Marien, H. (2008). Preparing and motivating Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 437–449.
behavior outside of awareness. Science, 319, 1639.
Bargh, J. A., & Pratto, F. (1986). Individual construct accessibility and
Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: perceptual selection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22,
Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of Personality and 293–311.
Social Psychology, 78, 53–63.
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). “I show
Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2002). Comparability is in the eye of the how you feel”: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of
beholder: Contrast and assimilation effects of primed animal exem- Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322–329.
plars on person judgments. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41,
123–138. Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1987). Motor mim-
icry as primitive empathy. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy
Aarts, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Hassin, R. R. (2004). Goal contagion:
and its development (pp. 317–338). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Perceiving is for pursuing. Journal of Personality Social Psychology,
University Press.
87, 23–37.
Bernieri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA:
student interactions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12(2), 120–138.
Addison-Wesley.
Bernieri, F., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudo
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Conner, B. (1999). Accuracy of judgments of
synchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process
sexual orientation from thin slices of behavior. Journal of Personality
in mother-infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social
and Social Psychology, 77, 538–547.
Psychology, 54, 243–253.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behav-
Blair, I. V., Judd, C. M., & Fallman, J. L. (2004). The automaticity of
ior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis.
race and afrocentric facial features in social judgments. Journal
Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 763–778.
Andreasen, N. C. (2005). The creative brain. New York: Plume.
Blanton, H., & Jaccard, J. (2006). Arbitrary metrics in psychology.
Ansfield, M. E., Wegner, D. M., & Bowser, R. (1996). Ironic effects of American Psychologist, 61, 62–71.
sleep urgency. Behavior Research and Therapy, 34, 523–531.
Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in sentence production. Cognitive
Antrobus, J. S., & Singer, J. L. (1964). Visual signal detection as a func- Psychology, 18, 355–387.
tion of sequential variability of simultaneous speech. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 68, 603–610. Bock, J. K. (1989). Closed-class immanence in sentence production.
Cognition, 31, 163–186.
Arkes, H. R., Dawes, R. M., & Christensen, C. (1986). Factors influenc-
ing the use of a decision rule in a probabilistic task. Organizational Bodenhausen, G. V. (1988). Stereotypic biases in social decision making
Behavior and Human Decision Processing, 37, 93–110. and memory: Testing process models of stereotype use. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 726–737.
Baars, B. (2002). The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent
evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 47–52. Bongers, K. C. A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). Goals and conscious-
ness: Consciousness as a trouble-shooting device? In E. Morsella,
Baccus, J. R., Baldwin, M. W., & Packer, D. J. (2004). Increasing implicit
J. A. Bargh, & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human
self-esteem through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 15,
action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
498–502.
Bornstein, R. F., Leone, D. R., & Galley, D. J. (1987). The generalizability
Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic
of subliminal mere exposure effects: Influence of stimuli perceived
influence in social perception and social cognition. In J. S. Uleman &
without awareness on social behavior. Journal of Personality and
J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford
Social Psychology, 53, 1070–1079.
Press.
Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Baaren, R. B. (2008). On the goal-
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, inten-
dependency of unconscious thought. Journal of Experimental
tion, efficiency and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer Jr. and
Psychology, 44, 1114–1120.
T. K. Srull (Eds.), The handbook of social cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 1–40).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., Jr., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the
of the automatic evaluation effect. Journal of Personality and Social elephant revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
Psychology, 62, 893–912. 631–643.

Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The auto- Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C., & Parker, K. (1990). Intuition in
matic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 72–110.
with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1990). Gist is the grist: Fuzzy-trace theory
32, 104–128. and the new intuitionism. Developmental Review, 10, 3–47.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on R. S, Wyer, Jr., and T. K. Srull (Eds.), Advances in social cognition
action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244. (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
References 261

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit motivator: On
Pergamon Press. the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality
Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, and Social Psychology, 89, 129–142.
123–152. Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2009). Learning of predictive relations does not
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary depend on awareness, but on goals. Manuscript under review.
determinants of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differen- Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy:
tial effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and
65, 5–17. Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.
Camemer, C. F., & Hogarth, R. M. (1999). The effects of financial incen- Dehaene, S., Changeux, J., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006).
tives in experiments: A review and capital-labor-production frame- Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable
work. Journal of Risk Uncertainty, 19, 407–441. taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 204–211.
Carver, C. S., Ganellen, R. J., Froming, W. J., & Chambers, W. (1983). Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of
Modeling: An analysis in terms of category accessibility. Journal of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition,
Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 403–421. 79, 1–37.
Carver, S. C., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and con-
New York: Cambridge University Press. trolled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Cesario, J., Higgins, E. T., & Plaks, J. E. (2006). Automatic social behav- 56, 5–18.
ior as motivation to interact. Journal of Personality and Social Devine, P. G., Monteith, M. J., Zuwerink, J. R., & Elliot, A. J. (1991).
Psychology, 90, 893–910. Prejudice with and without compunction. Journal of Personality and
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. Social Psychology, 60, 817–830.
New York: Guilford Press. Dijksterhuis, A. (2004a). I like myself but I don’t know why: Enhancing
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impression implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. Journal of
formation and memorization goals: Nonconscious goal priming repro- Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 345–355.
duces effects of explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality and Dijksterhuis, A. (2004b). Think different: The merits of unconscious
Social Psychology, 71, 464–478. thought in preference development and decision making. Journal of
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception- Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 586–598.
behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Dijksterhuis, A. (2007). Het slimme onbewuste [The smart unconscious].
Psychology, 76, 893–910. Amsterdam: Bert Bakker.
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confir- Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2003). On wildebeests and humans: The
mation processes: The self-fulfilling consequences of automatic preferential detection of negative stimuli. Psychological Science, 14,
stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14–18.
33, 541–560. Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., Bargh, J. A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2000).
Claxton, G. (1997). Hare brain, tortoise mind: How intelligence increases On the relation between associative strength and automatic behavior.
when you think less. New York: HarperCollins. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36(5), 531–544.
Cloutier, J., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Who or what are you? Facial orienta- Dijksterhuis, A., Albers, L. W., & Bongers, K. C. A. (2008). Digging for
tion and person construal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, the real attitude: Lessons from research on implicit and explicit self-
1298–1309. esteem. In R. Petty, R. Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights
Cloutier, J., Mason, M. F., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). The perceptual deter- from the wave of implicit attitude research (pp. 229–249). New York:
minants of person construal: Reopening the social-cognitive toolbox. Taylor and Francis.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 885–894. Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception–behavior express-
Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & Groom, C. J. way: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In
(2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33,
quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and pp. 1–40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Social Psychology, 89, 469–487. Dijksterhuis, A., Bargh, J. A., & Miedema, J. (2000). Of men and mack-
Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police erels: Attention, subjective experience, and automatic social behavior.
officer ’s dilemma: Using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threat- In H. Bless & J. P. Forgas (Eds.), The message within: The role of
ening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, subjective experience in social cognition and behavior (pp. 37–51).
1314–1329. Philadelphia: Psychological Press.

Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2007). The influ- Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006).
ence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot. European Journal of Social On making the right choice: The deliberation-without-attention effect.
Psychology, 37, 1102–1117. Science, 311, 1005–1007.

Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., Wittenbrink, B, Sadler, M. S., & Keesee, T. Dijksterhuis, A., Chartrand, T. L., & Aarts, H. (2007). Effects of priming
(2002). Across the thin blue line: Police officers and racial bias in the and perception on social behavior and goal pursuit. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.),
decision to shoot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher
1006–1023. mental processes (pp. 51–132). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians Dijksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: The genera-
after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judg- tive power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 15,
ments under uncertainty. Cognition, 58, 1–73. 135–146.
Cunningham, M. R., Barbee, A. P., & Pike, C. L. (1990). What do women Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious
want? Facialmetric assessment of multiple motives in the perception of thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 95–109.
male facial physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., & Lepinasse, V. (2001). Reflecting and deflect-
Psychology, 59, 61–72. ing stereotypes: Assimilation and contrast in impression formation and
262 Automaticity and the Unconscious

automatic behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition
286–299. research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54,
Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., Postmes, T., Stapel, D. A., Koomen, W., van 297–327.
Knippenberg, A., & Scheepers, D. (1998). Seeing one thing and doing Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986).
another: Contrast effects in automatic behavior. Journal of Personality On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and
and Social Psychology, 75, 862–871. Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.
Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). The relation between per- Fazio, R. H., & Williams, C. J. (1986). Attitude accessibility as a moderator
ception and behavior or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Journal of the attitude-perception and attitude-behavior relations—An investi-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 865–877. gation of the 1984 presidential elections. Journal of Personality and
Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2000). Behavioral indecision: Social Psychology, 51, 505–514.
Effects of self-focus on automatic behavior. Social Cognition, 18, Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects
55–74. of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and
Dijksterhuis, A., & van Olden, Z. (2006). On the benefits of thinking Social Psychology, 87, 557–572.
unconsciously: Unconscious thought increases post-choice satisfac- Field, A. P., & Moore, A. C. (2005). Dissociating the effects of attention
tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 627–631. and contingency awareness on evaluative conditioning effect in the
Dimberg, U. (1982). Facial reactions to facial expressions. visual paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 217–243.
Psychophysiology, 19, 643–647. Finkel, E. J., Campbell, W. K., Brunell, A. B., Dalton, A., Scarbeck, S. J., &
Dotsch, R., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2008). Virtual prejudice. Journal of Chartrand, T. L. (2006). High-maintenance interaction: Inefficient
Experimental Psychology, 44, 1194–1198. social coordination impairs self-regulation. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 91, 456–475.
Dotsch, R., Wigboldus, D. H. J., Langner, O., & van Knippenberg, A.
(2008). Ethnic out-group faces are biased in the prejudiced mind. Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not
Psychological Science, 19, 978–980. unto temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activa-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296–309.
Dovidio, J. F., Evans, N., & Tyler, R. B. (1986). Racial stereotypes: The
contents of their cognitive representations. Journal of Experimental Fiske, S. T. (1980). Attention and weight in person perception: The impact
Social Psychology, 22, 22–37. of negative and extreme behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 38, 889–906.
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K. L., Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and
explicit prejudice and racial interaction. Journal of Personality Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. E. (1990). A continuum of impression formation
and Social Psychology, 82, 62–68. from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of informa-
tion and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. Zanna (Ed.),
Dunning, D., & Sherman, D. A. (1997). Stereotypes and tacit inference.
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). San
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 459–471.
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dutton, D. G., & Aron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of you: Nonconscious
attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality and
pursuit of interpersonal goals associated with relationship partners.
Social Psychology, 30, 510–517.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 148–163.
Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008). Sex differences in mate preferences
Freud, S. (1915/1963). General psychological theory. New York: Simon &
revisited: Do people know what they initially desire in a romantic
Schuster.
partner? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 254–264.
Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Perspective taking: Decreasing
Eberhardt, J. L., Atiba Goff, P., Purdie, V. J., & Davies, P. G. (2004). Seeing
stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility and in-group favourit-
black: Race, crime, and visual processing. Journal of Personality and
ism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 708–724.
Social Psychology, 871, 876–893.
Gawronski, B. (2004). Theory-based bias correction in dispositional infer-
Edwards, W. (1961). Behavioral decision theory. Annual Review of
ence: The fundamental attribution error is dead, long live the corre-
Psychology, 12, 473–498.
spondence bias. European Review of Social Psychology, 15, 183–217.
Eitam, B., Hassin, R. R., & Schul, Y. (2008). Nonconscious goal pursuit
Ghiselin, B. (1952). The creative process. New York: New American
in novel environments—The case of implicit learning. Psychological
Library.
Science, 19, 261–267.
Gigerenzer, G. (2008). Why heuristics work. Perspectives on Psychological
Ellenbogen, J. M., Hu, P. T., Payne, J. D., Titone, D., & Walker, M. P.
Science, 3, 20–29.
(2007). Human relational memory requires time and sleep. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 7723–7728. Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P., & ABC Group (1999). Simple heuristics that
make us smart. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Erdelyi, M. H. (1974). A new look at the New Look: Perceptual defense
and vigilance. Psychological Review, 81, 1–25. Gilbert, D. (2006). Stumbling on happiness. New York: Knopf.
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove, Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist,
UK: Psychology Press. 46, 107–119.
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1997). Rationality in reasoning: The Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation
problem of deductive competence. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and
16, 1–35. Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.
Fazio, R. H., Chen, J., McDonel, E. C., & Sherman, S. J. (1982). Attitude Gilbert, D. T., Krull, D. S., & Pelham, B. W. (1988). On thoughts unspo-
accessibility, attitude-behavior consistency, and the strength of ken: Social inference and the self-regulation of behavior. Journal of
the object-evaluation association. Journal of Experimental Social Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 685–694.
Psychology, 18, 339–357. Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias.
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., & Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38.
Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial Gilbert, D. T., McNulty, S. E., Giuliano, T., & Benson, J. E. (1992). Blurry
attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social words and fuzzy deeds: The attribution of obscure behavior. Journal of
Psychology, 69, 1013–1027. Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 18–25.
References 263

Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior
When person perceiver meets person perceived. Journal of Personality theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
and Social Psychology, 54, 733–740. Hull, J. G., Slone, L. B., Meteyer, K. B., & Matthews, A. R. (2002). The
Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can’t not believe nonconsciousness of self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and
everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Social Psychology, 83, 406–424.
65, 221–233. Igou, E. R., & Bless, H. (2008). On undesirable consequences of think-
Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: ing: Framing effects as a function of substantive processing. Journal of
The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review, 109, 75–90. Behavioral Decision Making, 20, 125–142.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. In Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain:
W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychol- Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of mul-
ogy (Vol. 4, pp. 141–185). London: Wiley. tiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social
Gollwitzer, P. M., Parks-Stamm, E. J., Jaudas, A., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Flexible Psychology, 85, 616–626.
tenacity in goal pursuit. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating
motivation science (pp. 325–341). New York: Guilford Press. automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and
Gordon, P. C., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Implicit learning and generaliza- Language, 30, 513–541.
tion of the “mere exposure” effect. Journal of Personality and Social James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt
Psychology, 45, 492–500. and Company.
Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A room Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making: A psychological analy-
with a cue: Personality judgments based on offices and bedrooms. sis of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York: Free Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379–398. Jonas, K., & Sassenberg, K. (2006). Knowing what to do: Automatic
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring response activation from social categories. Journal of Personality and
individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Social Psychology, 90, 709–721.
Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attri-
Haddock, G., Macrae, C. N., & Fleck, S. (2002). Syrian science and smart bution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
supermodels: On the when and how of perception-behavior effects. in experimental social psychology (pp. 219–266). San Diego, CA:
Social Cognition, 20, 461–481. Academic Press.
Ham, J., & van den Bos, K. (2008). Automatically inferring the justness Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of
of events: Evidence from three implicit measurement paradigms. Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24.
Manuscript under review. Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Carvallo, M., & Mirenberg, M. C. (2004). How
Ham, J., van den Bos, K., & van Doorn, E. A. (2009). Lady justice thinks do I love thee? Let me count the Js: Implicit egotism and interpersonal
unconsciously: Unconscious thought can lead to more accurate justice attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 665–683.
judgments. Social Cognition, 27, 509–521. Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice. Mapping
Ham, J., & Vonk, R. (2003). Smart and easy: Co-occuring activation of bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720.
spontaneous trait inferences and spontaneous situational inferences. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of deci-
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 434–447. sions under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1988). Finding the face in the crowd: An Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (2000). Choices, values, and frames. New
anger superiority effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, York: Cambridge University Press.
54, 917–924.
Kawakami, K. L., Dovidio, J. F., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). Effects of social cat-
Hartley, D. (1749). Observations on man, his frame, his duty, and his egory priming on personal attitudes. Psychological Science, 14, 315–319.
expectations. London: Richardson.
Kawakami, K. L., Dovidio J., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000).
Hassin, R. R., Aarts, H., & Ferguson, M. J. (2005). Automatic goal infer- Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of ste-
ences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 129–140. reotype associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality
Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., & Uleman, J. S. (2002). Spontaneous causal and Social Psychology, 78, 871–888.
inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 515–522. Kawakami, K., Young, H., & Dovidio, J. F. (2002). Automatic stereotyping:
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Category, trait, and behavioral activations. Personality and Social
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Psychology Bulletin, 28, 3–15.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2006). Attention and consciousness: Two
Wiley. distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 16–22.
Herr, P. M. (1986). Consequences of priming: Judgment and behavior. Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. London: Penguin.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1106–1115. Kornhuber, H. H., & Deecke, L. (1965). Hirnpotentialanderungen
Higgins, E. T., King, G. A., & Mavin, G. H. (1982). Individual con- bei Wilkurbewegungen und passiv Bewegungen des Menschen:
struct accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. Journal of Berietschaftpotential und reafferente Potentiale. Pflugers Archiv fur
Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 35–47. Gesamte Psychologie, 284, 1–17.
Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility Krolak-Schwerdt, S. (2003). The cognition–behavior link: Effects of acti-
and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, vated trait categories on social behavior. Zeitschrift fur sozialpsycholo-
13, 141–154. gie, 34, 79–90.
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Nosek, B. A., & Schmitt, M. (2005). Krosnick, J. A., Betz, A. L., Jussim, L. J., & Lynn, A. R. (1992). Subliminal
What moderates the implicit-explicit consistency? European Review of conditioning of attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Social Psychology, 16, 335–390. 18, 152–162.
Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H. (2005). Smells like clean spirit: Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W. Y., & Sleeth-
Nonconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior. Psychological Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal-systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances
Science, 16, 689–693. in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34). New York: Academic Press.
264 Automaticity and the Unconscious

Krull, D. S. (1993). Does the grist change the mill? The effect of the Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as
perceiver ’s inferential goal on the process of social inference. energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 340–348. Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 37–47.
Krull, D. S., & Dill, J. C. (1996). On thinking first and responding fast: Macrae, C. N., Quinn, K. A., Mason, M. F., & Quadflieg, S. (2005).
Flexibility in social inference processes. Personality and Social Understanding others: The face and person construal. Journal of
Psychology Bulletin, 22, 949–959. Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 686–695.
Krull, D. S., & Ericksson, D. J. (1995). Judging situations: On the effort- Macrae, C. N., Stangor, C., & Milne, A. B. (1994). Activating social
ful process of taking dispositional information into account. Social stereotypes: A functional analysis. Journal of Experimental Social
Cognition, 4, 417–438. Psychology, 30, 370–389
Kunst-Wilson, W., & Zajonc, R. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli Martin, L. L., Seta, J. J., & Crelia, R. A. (1990). Assimilation and contrast
that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557–558. as a function of people’s willingness and ability to expend effort in
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral forming an impression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 59, 27–37.
14(4), 334–339. Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process.
Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. (2006). I am too just like you: Psychological Review, 69, 220–232.
The effects of ostracism on nonconscious mimicry. Unpublished manu- Merkelbach, H., de Jong, P. J., Arntz, A., & Schouten, E. (1993). The role
script, Drew University. of evaluative learning and disgust sensitivity in the etiology and treat-
Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. ment of spider phobia. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 12–18. 15, 243–255.
Latane, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal
doesn’t he help? New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378.
LeBoeuf, R. A., & Estes, Z. (2004). “Fortunately, I’m no Einstein”: Miller, G. A. (1956). Information theory. Scientific American, 195, 42–46.
Comparison relevance as a determinant of behavioral assimilation and Monahan, J. L., Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Subliminal mere
contrast. Social Cognition, 22, 607–636. exposure: Specific, general, and diffuse effects. Psychological Science,
Lee, T., Dolan, R. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2008). Controlling emotional 11, 462–466.
expression: Behavioral and neural correlates of nonimitative emotional Monteith, M. J. (1993). Self-regulation of prejudiced responses:
responses. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 104–113. Implications for progress in prejudice reduction efforts. Journal of
Lepore, L., & Brown, R. (1997). Category and stereotype activation: Is Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 469–485.
prejudice inevitable? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Monteith, M. J., Sherman, J. W., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Suppression
72, 275–287. as a stereotype control strategy. Personality and Social Psychology
Levelt, W. J. M., & Kelter, S. (1982). Surface form and memory in question Review, 2, 63–82.
answering. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 78–106. Moors, A., & de Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and con-
Levine, G. M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Goldstone, R. L. (1996). Reasoning and ceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297–326.
the weighing of attributes in attitude judgments. Journal of Personality Moors, A., & de Houwer, J. (2007). What is automaticity? An analysis of
and Social Psychology, 70, 230–240. its component features and their interrelations. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.),
Levy, B. (1996). Improving memory in old age through implicit self- Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher
stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, mental processes (pp. 11–50). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
1092–1107. Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and
Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of con- the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
scious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness- Psychology, 11, 56–60.
potential): The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, Moskowitz, G. B., Gollwitzer, P. M., Wasel, W., & Schaal, B. (1999).
106, 623–642. Preconscious control of stereotype activation through chronic egali-
Lippman, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Harcourt Brace. tarian goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
Livingston, R. W., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). What are we really priming? 167–184.
Cue-based versus category-based processing of facial stimuli. Journal Moskowitz, G. B., Li, P., & Kirk, E. R. (2004). The implicit volition
of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 5–18. model: On the preconscious regulation of temporarily adopted goals. In
Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 36, pp. 317–413). San Diego, CA: Elsevier/Academic Press.
272–292. Moskowitz, G. B., & Roman, R. J. (1992). Spontaneous trait inferences
Lynch, D. (2006). Catching the big fish. New York: Tarcher/Penguin. as self generated primes: Implications for conscious social judgment.
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., Castelli, L., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 728–738.
Schloerscheidt, A. M., & Greco, S. (1998). On activating exemplars. Murphy, S. T., Zajonc, R. B., & Monahan, J. L. (1995). Additivity of non-
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 330–354. conscious affect: Combined effects of priming and exposure. Journal
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 589–602.
of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Nelson, L. D., & Norton, M. I. (2005). From student to superhero:
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 808–817. Situational primes shape future helping. Journal of Experimental
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., Thorn, T., & Castelli, L. Social Psychology, 41, 423–430.
(1997). On the activation of social stereotypes: The moderating role Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). “Mood contagion”: The automatic
of processing objectives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, transfer of mood between persons. Journal of Personality and Social
33, 471–489. Psychology, 79, 211–223.
Macrae, C. N., & Johnston, L. (1998). Help, I need somebody: Automatic Niedenthal, P. M. (1990). Implicit perception of affective information.
action and inaction. Social Cognition, 16, 400–417. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 505–527.
References 265

Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P. Krauth-Gruber, S., & Schooler, J. W., Ohlsson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond
Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emo- words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental
tion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211. Psychology: General, 122, 166–183.
Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and short- Schopenhauer, A. (1851/1970). Essays and aphorisms. London: Penguin.
comings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Schubert, T. W., & Häfner, M. (2003). Contrast from social stereotypes in
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: automatic behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39,
Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 577–584.
231–253. Shafir, E., & LeBoeuf, R. (2002). Rationality. Annual Review of
Nordgren, L. F., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). The devil is in the delibera- Psychology, 53, 491–517.
tion: Thinking too much reduces judgmental consistency. Journal of Shah, J. (2003). The motivational looking glass: How significant oth-
Consumer Research, 36, 39–46. ers implicitly affect goal appraisals. Journal of Personality & Social
Nordgren, L. F., van der Pligt, J., & van Harreveld, F. (2006). Visceral Psychology, 85(3), 424–439.
drives in retrospect: Explanations about the inaccessible past. Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else:
Psychological Science, 17, 635–640.
On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of
Norretranders, T. (1998). The user illusion. New York: Viking. Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1261–1280.
Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relation between implicit and Sheeran, P., Aarts, H., Custers, R., Webb, T., L, Cooke, R., & Rivis, A.
explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, (2005). The goal-dependent automaticity of drinking habits. British
134, 565–584. Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 1–18.
Oikawa, M. (2004). Moderation of automatic achievement goals by con- Sherman, S. J., Rose, J. S., Koch, K., Presson, C. C., & Chassin, L. (2003).
scious monitoring. Psychological Reports, 95, 975–980. Implicit and explicit attitudes toward cigarette smoking: The effects of
Park, B., & Judd, C. M. (2005). Rethinking the link between categorization context and motivation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
and prejudice within the social cognition perspective. Personality and 22, 13–39.
Social Psychology Review, 9, 108–130. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and information processing II: Perceptual learning, automatic attending,
controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.
and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192. Shih, M., Ambady, N., Richeson, J. A., Fujita, K., & Gray, H. (2002).
Payne, B. K. (2005). Conceptualizing control in social cognition: How Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of self-relevance and the
executive control modulates the expression of automatic stereotyping. manner of stereotype-activation. Journal of Personality and Social
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 488–503. Psychology, 83, 638–647.
Pelham, B. W., & Neter, E. (1995). The effect of motivation on judgment Simon, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inat-
depends on the difficulty of the judgment. Journal of Personality and tentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.
Social Psychology, 68, 581–594. Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioural model of rational choice. Quarterly
Pleyers, G., Corneille, O., Luminet, O., & Yzerbyt, V. (2007). Aware Journal of Economics, 69, 99–118.
and (dis)liking: Item-based analyses reveal that valence acquisition Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the stream of consciousness. American
via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency Psychologist, 30, 727–738
awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory
Slovic, P., Fischoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Facts versus fears:
and Cognition, 33, 130–144.
Understanding perceives risks. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, &
Poincaré, H. (1913). The foundations of science. Lancaster, PA: Science A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgments under uncertainty: Heuristics and
Press. biases (pp. 463–489). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive con- Smith, P. K., Dijksterhuis, A., & Chaiken, S. (2008). Subliminal expo-
trol. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The sure to faces and racial attitudes: Exposure to Whites makes Whites
Loyola symposium (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. like Blacks less. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
Pratto, F., & Bargh. J. A. (1991). Stereotyping based on apparently indi- 50–64.
viduating information: Trait and global components of sex stereotypes Smith, S. M. (1995). Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory and cre-
under attention overload. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, ative thinking. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The
27, 26–47. creative cognition approach (pp. 135–156). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing Press.
power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Spears, R., Gordijn, E., Dijksterhuis, A., & Stapel, D. A. (2004). Reaction
Social Psychology, 61, 380–391. in action: Intergroup contrast in automatic behavior. Personality and
Razran, G. H. S. (1940). Conditioned response changes in rating and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 605–616.
appraising socio-political slogans. Psychological Bulletin, 37, 481. Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1979). The role of category accessibility
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants
Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). San Diego, 1660–1672.
CA: Academic Press. Staats, A. W., & Staats, C. K. (1958). Attitudes established by classi-
Scheflen, A. E. (1964). The significance of posture in communication cal conditioning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 57,
systems. Psychiatry, 27, 316–331. 37–40.
Schooler, J. W. & Melcher, J. (1995). The ineffability of insight. In Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning.
S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
approach (pp. 97–134). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 23, 645–665.
266 Automaticity and the Unconscious

Stapel, D. A., Koomen, W., & van der Pligt, J. (1996). The referents of Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA:
traits inferences: The impact of trait concepts versus actor-trait links on MIT Press.
subsequent judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in social
70, 437–450. life. In S. T. Fiske & D. T. Gilbert (Eds.), The handbook of social
Stapel, D. A., Koomen, W., & van der Pligt, J. (1997). Categories of psychology (pp. 446–496). New York: McGraw-Hill.
category accessibility: The impact of trait versus exemplar priming on Wegner, D. M., & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed
person judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 903–912.
44–76.
Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., Bowman, R., & Shelton, J. N. (1996). On trying
Stapel, D. A., & Suls, J. (2004). Method matters: Effects of explicit not to be sexist. Unpublished manuscript.
versus implicit social comparisons on activation, behavior, and self-
views. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 860–875. Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in
the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1093–1104.
conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feed-
back hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S., & White, T. (1987). Paradoxical
768–777. effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 53, 5–13.
Strack, F., Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Kubler, A., & Wanke, M. (1993).
Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation and con- Wegner, D. M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Why it feels as if we’re doing things:
trast. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 53–62. Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480–492.
Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N., & Ruderman, A. (1978). The categorical Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overt head-movements
and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Journal
Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 778–793. of Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230.
Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences Wheeler, S. C., DeMarree, K. G., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Understanding the
of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308, role of the self in prime-to-behavior effects: The active-self account.
1623–1626. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 234–261.
Todorov, A., & Uleman, J. S. (2003). The efficiency of binding spontaneous Wheeler, S. C., Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E. (2001). Think unto others:
trait inferences to actors’ faces. Journal of Experimental Social The self-destructive impact of negative racial stereotypes. Journal of
Psychology, 39, 549–562. Experimental Social Psychology, 37(2), 173–180.
Treisman, A. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Wheeler, S. C., & Petty, R. E. (2001). The effects of stereotype-
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 347–353. activation on behavior: A review of possible mechanisms. Psychological
Bulletin, 127, 797–826.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging
frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232. Whitehead, A. N. (1911). An introduction to mathematics. New York:
Holt.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty:
Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131. Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Price, J. R. (2001). Implicit/explicit memory
versus analytic/nonanalytic processing: Rethinking the mere exposure
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the
effect. Memory & Cognition, 29, 234–246.
psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458.
Wigboldus, D. H. J., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). When
Uleman, J. S., Adil Saribay, S., & Gonzalez, C. M. (2008). Spontaneous
stereotypes get in the way: Stereotypes obstruct stereotype-inconsistent
inferences, implicit impressions, and implicit theories. Annual Review
trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
of Psychology, 59, 329–360.
470–484.
Uleman,, J. S., Hon, A., Roman, R. J., & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). On-line
Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism:
evidence for spontaneous trait inferences at encoding. Personality and
Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 377–394.
Social Psychology, 79, 748–762.
Van Baaren, R. B., Horgan, T. G., Chartrand, T. L., & Dijkmans, M. (2004).
Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.
The forest, the trees, and the chameleon: Context dependence and mim-
icry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 453–459. Wilson, T. D., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental
Van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., de Bouter, C., & correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations.
van Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral con- Psychological Bulletin, 116, 117–142.
sequences of self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D., Schooler, J. W., Hodges, S. D., Klaaren, K. J., &
Psychology, 84, 1093–1102. LaFleur, S. J. (1993). Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice
Vaughan, K. B., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1980). Vicarious instigation and satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 331–339.
conditioning of facial expressive and autonomic responses to a Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection
model’s expressive display of pain. Journal of Personality and Social can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of
Psychology, 38, 909–923. Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181–192.
Walther, E., Muller, D., & Schott, O. (2001). Automatic social behavior: Winter, L., & Uleman, J. (1984). When are social judgments made?
How does priming of egoism and altruism influence helping behavior? Evidence for the spontaneousness of trait inferences. Journal of
Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 248–257. Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 237–252.
Watson, J. B. (1914). Behavior: An introduction to comparative behavior. Wolfe, T. (2004). I am Charlotte Simmons. London: Jonathan Cape.
New York: Holt. Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal media-
Watt, H. J. (1905). Experimentelle Beitrage zur einer Theorie des Denkens. tion of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of
Archiv für die Geschichte der Psychologie, 4, 289–436. Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 109–120.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Wyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social
Review, 101, 34–52. context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
References 267

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Zebrowitz, L. A., White, B., & Wieneke, K. (2008). Mere exposure and
Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1–27. racial prejudice: Exposure to other-race faces increases liking for
Zajonc, R. B., Adelmann, K. A., Murphy, S. T., & Niedenthal, P. M. (1987). strangers of that race. Social Cognition, 26, 259–275.
Convergence in the physical appearance of spouses. Motivation and Zebrowitz-McArthur, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (1992). Impressions of
Emotion, 11, 335–346. babyfaced individuals across the life span. Developmental Psychology,
Zajonc, R. B., Pietromonaco, P., & Bargh, J. A. (1982). Independence and 28, 1143–1152.
interaction of affect and cognition. In M. S. Clark & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Zhong, C. B., Dijksterhuis, A., & Galinsky, A. (2008). The merits
Affect and cognition: The seventeenth annual Carnegie symposium of unconscious thought in creativity. Psychological Science, 19,
on cognition (pp. 211–227). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 912–918.
Chapter 8

Motivation
JOHN A. BARGH, PETER M. GOLLWITZER, AND GABRIELE OETTINGEN

It is Friday afternoon. On Monday, you have an important cognitively (e.g., searching), affectively (e.g., excitement),
examination. Even though you have been aware of this and behaviorally (e.g., running). To the question of what
deadline during the week, you did not find the time to drives motivation: The history of the psychology of moti-
study for the test. So you set yourself the goal to study vation is revealing ever more sophisticated answers. This
over the weekend. How do you arrive at a strong commit- chapter discusses the field of motivation from its origins in
ment to attain this goal? You may even plan when to start animal studies to its investigations into the human mind,
and how you will deal with potential disruptions. How do focusing on how humans in social contexts pursue both
you make effective plans for initiating goal striving and for conscious and unconscious goals.
shielding it from getting derailed? Based on learning theory advanced by early animal psy-
In the first part of this chapter, we address how people chologists (Hull, 1943, 1952; Spence, 1956), the strength
may best use conscious thought to make goal pursuit effec- of the tendency to respond was at first considered to be
tive. In the second part of the chapter, we discuss an alter- a function of an organism’s skills (or habit strength),
native, unconscious route to effective goal pursuit. Think its needs, and the incentive value of the outcome. For
now of another student who faces the same examination example, how fast an animal runs toward a box contain-
on Monday. Arriving at home from school on Friday after- ing food depends on its habit strength, its hunger, and the
noon, she walks by a picture of her father who had been quality and quantity of food. However, with the advance of
her model to perform well in school. Without conscious the cognitive revolution in psychology, these determinants
thought, she pulls out her notebook, places it on her desk, of motivation, as well as the concept of motivation itself,
and gets started. In the second part of the chapter, we became ever more elaborated.
discuss how this unconscious activation of goals can influ- The neobehaviorist and cognitive learning theorist
ence people’s thoughts, feelings, and actions. Tolman (1932, 1952) influenced the field through his
observations of mus norvegicus albinus, postulating
that various mental processes are “intermediate in the
THE GOAL CONCEPT IN THE PSYCHOLOGY causal equation between environmental stimuli and . . .
OF MOTIVATION overt behavior” (Tolman, 1932, p. 2). These intermediate
processes entailed concepts of purpose (ends and means)
Social psychologists use the term “motivation” to describe and expectations (e.g., mean expectations, end expecta-
why a person in a given situation selects one response tions, and mean–end expectations). The social psycholo-
over another or makes a given response with great ener- gists Festinger (1942) and Atkinson (1957) drew on that
gization or frequency. Imagine a person searching for work in their research on what motivates humans to select
someone else in a crowd. She gets excited when she finds and perform tasks of varying difficulty. They suggested
that person, and then she runs toward him. Each of those that people weight the incentive value of the desired out-
responses involves motivation, which can manifest itself come with the expectancy that it would actually occur.

Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by Grant R01-MH60767 from the U.S. National Institutes of Health to JAB, by a
German Research Foundation Grant GO 387/14-2 to PMG, and by German Research Foundation Grants OE 237/10-1 and 237/12-1
to GO.

268

CH08.indd 268 12/18/09 2:39:58 PM


The Goal Concept in the Psychology of Motivation 269

Social cognitive learning theorists (e.g., Bandura, 1982) self-evaluations (e.g., “I did really well!”), positive
went a step further, factoring in whether one could suc- evaluations by others (e.g., praise by the teacher), higher-
cessfully perform the necessary behavior required to arrive order positive consequences (e.g., successful professional
at a desired outcome (so-called efficacy or control beliefs). career), and consequences that go beyond achievement
These theorists also alluded to further relevant expectan- (e.g., having a good time with co-workers) can all motivate
cies, such as whether the situation by itself would pro- people to do well on given tasks.
duce the desired outcome (Heckhausen, 1977), whether But the psychology of motivation has not only inves-
performing a given behavior would lead to the desired tigated a person’s readiness to make a certain response: It
outcome (Bandura, 1977, 1982), whether achieving the has also explicated this readiness itself to predict whether
desired outcome would be instrumental to accruing further one response is chosen over another or is engaged in with
positive consequences (Vroom, 1964), whether the desired a high intensity or frequency. Most importantly in this
outcome could be brought about somehow by the person’s regard, Ajzen and Fishbein (1969) suggested that this read-
actions (Oettingen, 1996), and whether the future in general iness should be assessed in terms of a person’s intention
would be bright (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; to make the response. Mischel (1973) went a step further
Scheier & Carver, 1987). and argued that such intentions can be conceived as self-
Adding these expectancy-related variables helped to imposed goals that imply standards that the person intends
explicate in more detail what Hull and Spence tried to cap- to meet (with respect to quality and quantity criteria).
ture with the concept of habit strength, that is, the “can” It is important to note here that this conceptualization of
aspect (or feasibility aspect) of the motivation to make a the term “goal” is quite different from how the same term
certain response: “Can the desired outcome be brought was used by the behaviorists. According to the behavior-
about?” But the cognitive revolution also helped to explain ists (e.g., Bindra, 1959; Skinner, 1953), a goal is nothing
the “want” aspect (or desirability aspect) of the motivation but a powerful incentive, defined as objects and events that
to make a certain response: “Do I really want the desired affect an organism’s behavior radically and reliably (e.g.,
outcome?” This desirability issue was captured by Hull food, sexual stimulation, or a sudden loud noise). Whether
and Spence in two components, as the concept of need and an object or event is treated as a goal, however, depends
the concept of incentive. solely on the investigator ’s perspective on the organism’s
With respect to need, the cognitively inspired psychol- behavior. If the investigator selects a certain incentive as a
ogy of motivation ventured into the concept of motives reference point for the description of a respective behavior,
(for a summary, see McClelland, 1985b), defined as the this incentive becomes a goal. For example, Skinner (1953)
class of incentives that a person finds attractive (e.g., refers to the food that is provided as incentive to explore
achievement, power, affiliation, or intimacy). More impor- a maze as the animal’s goal (or reason) to run through the
tantly, McClelland (1985a) discovered that depending on maze. However, a behaviorist researcher ’s statement that
whether this preference for certain classes of incentives food is a goal to the hungry organism means nothing more
was measured implicitly (as assessed by the Thematic than (1) it is known that food is an incentive to the organism,
Apperception Test) or explicitly (as assessed by attitude and (2) the researcher has chosen to describe the behavior
questionnaires), it predicts the execution of different types of the organism in relation to food rather than in relation
of motive-related responses: actions people spontaneously to any other object or event. In the behaviorist tradition,
engage in versus actions people decide to engage in after the reference point for goal-directed behavior is thus
thoughtful deliberation. not the goal set by the organisms themselves. Behaviorists
It was also found that whether an incentive is hoped do not analyze internal goals or the processes leading to
for or feared matters greatly. For instance, a person with goal setting. Skinner (1953) phrased this most cogently
a strong achievement motive, longing for the pride associ- when he referred to “goal directedness” as an effective and
ated with success, will choose a task of medium difficulty easy-to-use term for the description of persistent, appropri-
to pursue; this level of difficulty provides the most infor- ate, and searching behavior toward an incentive that results
mation about achievement level. However, a person who from some kind of learning.
abhors the shame associated with failure (Atkinson, 1958, To the contrary, cognitive social learning theorists (e.g.,
1964) will choose either an easy or a difficult task, which is Bandura, 1989; Mischel, 1973) considered goals as internal
an effective strategy to avoid shame (easy tasks are likely and subjective processes and states, and this had tremendous,
to be solved, and failure on too-difficult tasks can easily be transformational consequences for the study of motivation:
explained). Finally, researchers have differentiated among
types of incentives (Heckhausen, 1977). For instance, 1. If one conceptualizes goal-directed responses in rela-
in the realm of achievement, anticipation of positive tion to subjective goals held by the individual (e.g., to

CH08.indd 269 12/18/09 2:39:59 PM


270 Motivation

get to know an attractive person), then one begins to The first part of the chapter reviews and discusses
ask new questions, such as how people set themselves research in the classic tradition of motivation psychology.
such goals and how they strive to achieve them. Here, the individual or self is considered an active, con-
Noticing the unique nature of both of these problems, scious agent, involved in setting goals and striving to
Lewin (Lewin, Dembo, Festinger, & Sears, 1944) attain them. The second part turns to the research litera-
suggested adopting a distinct theoretical perspec- ture on automatic or nonconscious goal pursuit, which has
tive for goal setting versus goal striving. Present-day analyzed whether goals can also operate independently
researchers rediscovered Lewin’s approach (e.g., see the of conscious selection and guidance. That review shows
action-phase model; Gollwitzer, 1990; Heckhausen & that consciously set and unconsciously activated goals
Gollwitzer, 1987). Today, research on motivation alike lead to goal striving with features similar to those
explicitly targets either the determinants and processes shown in the conscious goal pursuit literature. It also pro-
of goal setting or the determinants and processes of vides a unifying framework for conscious and unconscious
goal striving and successful goal attainment. goal pursuit by pointing out that unconscious motivational
Conceptualizing goals as internal and subjective mechanisms have the place of primacy in the evolution of
has the additional advantage of differentiating the con- human motivation.
tent of the goal from the responses performed in its
service. This distinction has led to research on how goals
are framed and how their framing facilitates different CONSCIOUS GOAL PURSUIT
responses. For example, it matters whether an achieve-
ment goal is framed as a learning goal or a performance This section on conscious goal pursuit is subdivided into
goal (e.g., “I want to learn from performing the task” goal setting versus goal striving (goal implementation).
versus “I want to show my abilities”; Dweck, 1996). The discussion of goal setting first targets various deter-
2. Conceptualizing goals as subjective internal states also minants of goal content and goal structure. For instance,
raises the question of whether the mental representa- what determines whether people set themselves goals
tion of a goal can be activated outside of a person’s in the achievement or the affiliation domain and whether
awareness (as suggested in the auto-motive model; these goals are framed as learning versus performance
Bargh, 1990). Given that goals are internal mental goals? The goal-setting discussion then moves to the ques-
representations, how do such mental representations tion of how people can strategically regulate the process of
relate to the representations of other goals, subgoals, goal setting so that they arrive at particularly strong goal
and means of attainment (Kruglanski, 1996)? Such commitments.
considerations have been the focus of the psychology Then the section on conscious goal pursuit switches to
of motivation over the past 10 years. New questions a discussion of the issue of goal implementation. How do
then emerged (e.g., How does goal striving that is content (e.g., cooperative vs. competitive) and structural
nonconsciously activated compare with goal striving features (e.g., specific vs. vague standards) of goals affect
that is consciously activated? Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee- people’s success in striving for their goals, and what con-
Chai, Barndollar, & Trötschel, 2001; Oettingen, Grant, textual variables (e.g., affective states, competing action
Smith, Skinner, & Gollwitzer, 2006), and new answers tendencies, power position) matter? The discussion ends
to old questions were found (e.g., How does striving with an analysis of the self-regulation strategies people can
for a goal linked to several means differ from striving for use to promote goal attainment (i.e., planning and persis-
a goal where the same means can serve a host of goals? tent acting toward the goal).
Kruglanski & Kopetz, 2008).
Research on goals is now flourishing. This upsurge Goal Setting
of the goal concept becomes evident when perusing
recent relevant edited volumes (e.g., Heckhausen & What makes people set themselves goals of a certain con-
Heckhausen, 2008; Kruglanski & Higgins, 2007; tent and structure, and how can they self-regulate their
Morsella, Bargh, & Gollwitzer, 2009; Moskowitz & goal setting? When trying to answer this question, it is
Grant, 2009; Shah & Gardner, 2008) or the contents of important to recognize that many goals are assigned by
major conference programs, in which the goal concept others (e.g., employers, teachers, or parents). In this case,
is now one of the most frequently appearing theoreti- it matters who assigns what goal to whom and how the
cal constructs. Accordingly, this chapter discusses the perceived message is delivered. Locke and Latham (1990,
recent advances in studying motivation in social psy- 2006) suggest that source variables, such as legitimacy and
chology by focusing on goals. trustworthiness, play important roles in the transformation

CH08.indd 270 12/18/09 2:39:59 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 271

of an assigned goal into a personal goal. For recipients of what they wish to become in the future (i.e., the possible
such assigned goals, perceiving the goal as desirable and self ). These possible selves should give people ideas on
feasible, redefining it as a personal goal, and integrat- what kinds of personal goals they might want to strive for.
ing it with other existing goals are vital. Finally, relevant Once people have set themselves higher-order goals, such
message variables include the discrepancy between the as becoming a physician, a good parent, or a moral person
assigned goal and the recipient’s respective current goal (Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998), these higher-order goals
(e.g., when a low-calorie diet is suggested to a person with may determine the contents of lower-order goals as well.
a moderate dieting goal) and whether fear appeals are used The contents of such “be” goals determine the contents of
(e.g., information on the dramatic medical consequences respective “do” goals (Carver & Scheier, 1998). “Be” goals
of health-damaging behavior). Anyone promoting a par- have been described by using terms such as “current con-
ticular goal in others should also consider the processing cerns” (Klinger, 1977), “self-defining goals” (Wicklund &
skills and motivation of the recipient as a moderator of Gollwitzer, 1982), “identity goals” (Gollwitzer & Kirchhof,
the effects of source, recipient, and message variables on 1998), “personal projects” (Little, 1983), “personal striv-
accepting assigned goals as personal goals. ings” (Emmons, 1996), and “life tasks” (Cantor & Fleeson,
But people arrive at personal goals in ways other than 1994). The degree to which higher-order “be” goals deter-
by accepting the goals assigned by others: People also mine the choosing of lower-order “do” goals depends on
set goals for themselves. Such goal setting may still be the degree of commitment to the respective higher-order
influenced by others—for example, when goals are con- “be” goals (Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996).
jointly set (e.g., in participative decision making and But the perceived desirability of a potential behavioral
employee involvement) or when goals are adopted from goal also depends on the person’s attitude toward that goal.
highly respected models (e.g., adopting standards for As Ajzen and Fishbein (1980; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975)
self-rewards). Social context cues, such as normative have pointed out, attitude in theory is the sum total of the
expectations of the social community, may also influ- possible positive and negative consequences associated
ence a person’s goal selection (Ajzen, 1985). So what are with the attainment of the behavioral goal, with each of
the internal (personal) determinants of the content and these consequences weighted by its perceived likelihood
structure of the goals people set for themselves? of occurrence. In practice, people may not go through
such a comprehensive reasoning process to develop each
Goal Content
attitude. Furthermore, these consequences do not have to
Assuming that people set themselves desirable goals (i.e., be experienced; false suggestions that they might have
incentive valence of goal attainment is high), goal content occurred (e.g., the false suggestion of a negative child-
should be influenced by people’s needs, wishes, and higher- hood event) suffice to affect a person’s behavioral goals
order goals. Ryan, Sheldon, Kasser, and Deci (1996), for (Geraerts et al., 2008). Finally, as suggested by Bandura
instance, report that the content of people’s goals reflects (1977, 1997), feasibility concerns play an important role
their needs for autonomy, competence, and social integra- in goal setting as well. It matters whether people feel that
tion (see also Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Harris, 2006). they can make the responses that produce the desired goal.
For example, people may set themselves the goal to per- Self-efficacy beliefs (or “control beliefs” as referred to
form a given task independently, to acquire the necessary by Ajzen, 1985, 1991, in his theory of planned behavior)
skills to perform the task, or to become integrated into an need to be high for strong intentions (goal commitments)
admired team. Interestingly, goal choice in line with these to emerge. Again, people do not need to have made such
needs is more pronounced when people are confronted responses successfully in the past for high self-efficacy to
with reminders of mortality (Lykins, Segerstrom, Averill, emerge; rather, the belief in their own skills suffices (e.g.,
Evans, & Kemeny, 2007). With respect to the translation beliefs based on observing similar others making similar
of the power motive or need (i.e., influencing others is responses; Bandura, 1977).
highly attractive, as assessed implicitly by the Thematic Recent research has focused on the question of what
Apperception Test; Winter, 1991) into respective goals, motivates people to reflect on the desirability or feasibility
Schultheiss and Brunstein (1999) observed that an exercise (or both) of a given goal choice. For instance, Cioffi and
of imagining others perform well on a power-related task Garner (1996) found that requiring active choices of a goal
produces more a motive-congruent setting of the goal to (i.e., answering affirmative items to do volunteer work) led
succeed on this task. to thinking of more reasons such a decision could be attrac-
Markus and Nurius (1986; Oyserman, Bybee, & Terry, tive than did requiring only a passive choice to be made
2006) suggested that people conceive of themselves not (i.e., skipping items that affirmed the opposite choice).
only in terms of what they are (i.e., the self concept) but also Liberman and Trope (1998) reported that reflecting on a

CH08.indd 271 12/18/09 2:40:01 PM


272 Motivation

potential goal that is psychologically distant (e.g., acting the potential of emotions to elicit behavior directly: for
on the goal is required in the distant future rather than example, fear produces fight or flight or disgust leads to
near future) makes people focus on the goal’s desirability, rejection. Baumeister, Vohs, DeWall, and Zhang (2007)
whereas a goal that is psychologically near (e.g., acting on have argued that behaviors can produce emotional out-
the goal is required soon) triggers feasibility concerns. But comes. For example, stealing may lead to feelings of guilt.
considering a potential goal that is psychologically distant By cognitively anticipating such outcome emotions, people
not only may turn the person’s mind to the desirability of can learn about the desirability of performing the respec-
this goal but actually may increase its perceived desirabil- tive behavior (e.g., pride may signal high desirability) and
ity. Specifically, when Vasquez and Bühler (2007) varied about its feasibility (e.g., surprise may signal that the out-
near versus far psychological distance by having people come is more easily attained than originally expected). As a
imagine the performance of a future task from a first- or consequence, when people deliberate whether to perform
third-person perspective, they observed that the impor- a certain behavior, they may anticipate relevant outcome
tance of doing well (i.e., high desirability of goal attain- emotions. These anticipated emotions in turn may provide
ment) increased by taking a third-person perspective. The valuable feedback on whether to set the goal to perform
latter perspective produced higher desirability that in turn the behavior or not.
led to a stronger goal to do well on the task at hand (for
Goal Structure
similar findings using a different psychological distance
manipulation, see Fujita, Trope, Liberman, & Levin-Sagi, The previous paragraph on goal setting described how
2006). So it appears that psychologically close versus people arrive at goals of different content (e.g., to help a
distant construals of a goal do more than affect to what colleague or to compete) and concluded that people set
extent desirability or feasibility is considered; psychologi- goals that they perceive as desirable and feasible. Goals
cally distant construals can also make a goal seem more with the same content may, however, have different struc-
desirable. ture. The next part of the chapter addresses relevant types
Another mode of thought that affects a person’s readi- of goal structure and their determinants.
ness to reflect on the desirability or feasibility of a goal is People prefer to interpret the behavior of others as
counterfactual thinking (Epstude & Roese, 2008; McCrea, approach motivated, even when they recognize that their own
2008). Failing to reach a set goal (e.g., not doing well identical behaviors are motivated by avoidance (Miller &
in a midterm examination after setting out to receive an Nelson, 2002). This interpretation bias implies that people
A) often triggers thoughts such as “If only I had studied are capable of framing the same goal in terms of either
harder, I would have done better on the midterm exam!” approach or avoidance (Elliot, 2008). For instance, a per-
Such counterfactual thought in turn triggers thoughts on son who wants to be a good student may frame the goal
the desirability and feasibility of studying harder for the of doing well in class as either approaching good grades
class, potentially leading to the goal to study harder for (earning As and Bs) or avoiding bad grades (no Cs and Ds).
this class in the future. But when it comes to adjusting Whereas both of these goals would be expected to energize
goals to internal and external demands, counterfactual behavior, the direction pointed to in the first case is toward
thinking is not the only form of thinking attentively, repeti- positive stimuli, whereas in the second case it is away from
tively, or frequently about one’s self and one’s world. negative stimuli.
A summary article by Watkins (2008) points out many others, People with the trait disposition of extraversion prefer
for instance, rumination (or brooding on negative stimuli), to set themselves approach goals, whereas people with the
worry, mind wandering, chronic self-consciousness, and trait disposition of neuroticism prefer to set themselves
mental simulation. Each of these forms of thinking may avoidance goals (Larsen & Augustine, 2008). These differ-
produce a differential readiness to consider desirability ential preferences for approach versus avoidance framing
and feasibility of potential goals. Researchers have also are also supported by the evidence for state extraver-
investigated whether a future outcome being perceived sion and state neuroticism (Heller, Komar, & Lee, 2007).
as highly desirable leads to being overly optimistic about Gray’s (1990, 1994) Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory
its feasibility. The evidence is mixed, and the mechanisms points to a further individual difference variable, that is,
that lead to such overly optimistic expectations still need reward sensitivity versus punishment sensitivity (see also
further research (Krizan & Windschitl, 2007). the behavioral approach system vs. behavioral inhibition
It is not only mode of thought that influences desir- system scales; Carver & White, 1994). Moreover, whether
ability and feasibility considerations and assessments; people set themselves approach versus avoidance goals in
emotional states also play a role. Traditional analyses the achievement and affiliation domains depends on their
of emotion (e.g., Frijda, 1986; Russell, 2003) emphasize motive dispositions of hope for success versus fear of

CH08.indd 272 12/18/09 2:40:01 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 273

failure (Elliot, 1997) and hope for affiliation versus fear of prefer either low or high levels of difficulty. Assuming that
rejection, respectively (Gable, 2006). low-difficulty tasks are associated with a high likelihood
Higgins (1997; Scholer & Higgins, 2008) has suggested of success, and assuming that failing at difficult tasks can
that people may not only frame outcome goals in terms be attributed to the task rather than to one’s lack of ability,
of approach and avoidance but also may frame strategy choosing low- or high-difficulty levels is a self-protective
goals, such as how they want to strive for a given out- mechanism (Weiner, 1992). Second, Hollenbeck, Williams,
come goal in terms of approach versus avoidance. For and Klein (1989) observed that commitment to difficult
instance, one may want to approach a desired end-state goals was higher when goals were made public rather than
either by promotion strategies (i.e., with eagerness) or stayed private and when locus of control was perceived
prevention strategies (i.e., with vigilance). Equally, when as internal as compared with external. And third, Bandura
one moves away from an undesired end-state, one can also (1997) reports that having successfully achieved an earlier
use either promotion strategies (eagerness) or prevention goal stimulates the setting of ever more challenging goals;
strategies (vigilance). The framing of strategy goals in this is assumed to be caused by a person’s heightened
terms of promotion versus prevention has been found to be sense of efficacy originating from just having successfully
a consequence of whether people construe their self either attained the prior goal.
as an ideal self that they desire to be or as an ought self that
Self-Regulation of Goal Setting
they feel compelled to be: ideal-self individuals prefer a
promotion framing, whereas ought-self individuals favor Knowing the determinants of the content and the structure
a prevention framing. of the goals people set for themselves still does not answer
Dweck (1996) has suggested a framing distinction the question of how people arrive at strong goal commit-
between performance goals and learning goals. Goals in ments in the first place. Perceiving a goal as desirable and
the achievement domain, for example, may either focus feasible does not guarantee that one actually commits to
on finding out how capable one is (performance goals) and strives for this goal (i.e., becomes committed to its
or on learning from the task (learning goals). Molden and realization). For instance, someone may wish to learn to
Dweck (2006) argue that implicit theories on the nature of play the violin because that person loves to make music
ability determine the preference for performance versus and feels capable of doing so, yet committing oneself to
learning goals. If people believe that ability is fixed and realize this wish takes a further step. It is this further step
cannot be easily changed (i.e., hold an entity theory of that is addressed next.
ability) they prefer to set performance goals. However, if Various mental strategies advance the transition from
people believe that ability can be improved by learning wishes and fantasies to goal commitments. The theory
(i.e., hold an incremental theory of ability), they prefer to of fantasy realization specifies three respective self-
set learning goals. regulation strategies (Oettingen, 2000): mental contrasting,
Another structural feature of goals is their level indulging, and dwelling. In mental contrasting, people first
of abstractness. People generally prefer to set themselves imagine the fulfillment of a wish or fantasy (e.g., giving
abstract goals. They adopt concrete goals predominantly a good presentation at a conference) and then reflect on
when they run into problems attaining an abstract goal (see the present reality that stands in the way of attaining the
action identification theory; Vallacher & Wegner, 1987). desired future (e.g., evaluation anxiety). Mental contrast-
However, people also vary to the extent to which they ing is a problem-solving strategy that makes people rec-
typically think of their actions in low-level terms or pre- ognize that they have not fulfilled their wish yet and that
fer high-level identifications (Vallacher & Wegner, 1989). they need to take action to achieve the desired future. As
Importantly, this general preference for either an abstract a consequence, expectations of attaining the desired future
or a concrete level of identifying actions may also be become activated and determine a person’s goal commit-
reflected in the choice of abstract versus concrete goals. ment and subsequent striving to attain the desired future.
Finally, goals of any content (e.g., solving a math prob- When perceived expectations of success are high, people
lem, writing a book, getting to know a stranger) can be actively commit to realizing the desired future; when
specified at different levels of difficulty. Three lines of expectations of success are low, people refrain from doing
research identified determinants of difficulty preference. so and thus venture on to alternative wishes and desired
First, Atkinson (1957) pointed out early on that whether futures. In this way, mental contrasting helps people dis-
a person’s achievement motive is dominated by hope for criminate between feasible and unfeasible goals.
success or fear of failure is crucial for the level of difficulty The theory of fantasy realization specifies two further
preferred. Individuals with hope for success prefer medium routes to goal setting. People may engage either in indulg-
levels of difficulty, whereas individuals with fear of failure ing (envisioning only the attainment of the wished-for

CH08.indd 273 12/18/09 2:40:02 PM


274 Motivation

future) or in dwelling (reflecting only on the present nega- on studying abroad, acquiring a second language, getting
tive reality). Neither of these mental strategies produces to know an attractive stranger, finding a balance between
any experienced discrepancy between future and real- work and family life, self-improvement, and idiosyncratic
ity; thus, the individual fails to recognize that actions interpersonal wishes of great importance. Furthermore,
(making responses) are necessary to achieve the desired strength of goal commitment was assessed by cognitive
future. Therefore, expectations of success do not become (e.g., making plans), affective (e.g., feelings of frustration),
activated, and goal setting does not reflect the perceived motivational (e.g., feelings of energization), and behavioral
likelihood of reaching the desired future. Individuals who (e.g., amount of invested effort) indicators. These indica-
indulge and dwell show a medium level of goal commit- tors were measured via self-report or observations, either
ment, even though the resource-efficient strategy to follow directly after the experiment or weeks later. All of these
would be for no engagement in the case of low expecta- studies evidenced the same patterns of results: given high
tions of success and full engagement in the case of high expectations of success, participants in the mental contrast-
expectations of success. For example, when it comes to the ing group showed the strongest goal commitment; given
goal of giving a good presentation at a conference, both an low expectations, mental contrasting participants showed
indulging and a dwelling person will show moderate prep- least goal commitment. Participants who indulged in posi-
aration, regardless of whether a successful performance is tive images about the future or dwelled on negative images
perceived as within reach or as hardly possible. of reality showed medium commitment no matter whether
Various experiments support these claims (e.g., expectations of success were high or low. It is important to
Oettingen, 2000; Oettingen, Hönig, & Gollwitzer, 2000). In note that the outcomes of mental contrasting do not occur
one study (Oettingen, Pak, & Schnetter, 2001, study 4), first- as a result of changes in the level of expectations (feasi-
year students enrolled in a vocational school for computer bility) or incentive valence (desirability) but rather as a
programming indicated their expectations of excelling in result of the mode of self-regulatory thought (i.e., mental
mathematics. Next, they named positive aspects that they contrasting, indulging, or dwelling), with mental contrast-
associated with excelling in mathematics (e.g., feelings of ing aligning strength of goal commitment to expectations.
pride or increasing job prospects) and negative aspects Furthermore, the effects of mental contrasting depend on
of reality, that is, potential obstacles (e.g., being distracted the person perceiving the present reality as an obstacle,
by peers or feeling lazy). In the mental contrasting con- that is, as standing in the way of realizing the desired future
dition, participants had to elaborate in writing two posi- (Oettingen et al., 2001, study 3).
tive aspects of the future and two aspects of reality, in Recent research shows that mental contrasting does
alternating order beginning with the positive aspect of the not have to pertain to the attainment of a positive future;
future. Participants in the indulging condition were asked people can also fantasize about a negative future and con-
to elaborate four positive aspects of the future only; in the trast these negative fantasies with reflection on positive
dwelling condition, they instead elaborated four negative reality. Oettingen, Mayer, Thorpe, Janetzke, and Lorenz
aspects of reality only. As a dependant variable, participants (2005) observed in a group of xenophobic high school stu-
indicated how energized they felt with respect to excelling dents that when negative fantasies (i.e., fears that social
in math (e.g., how active, eventful, or energetic). conflicts would arise from foreign youth moving into their
Two weeks after the experiment, the participants’ neighborhood) are contrasted with reflections on a posi-
teachers reported how much effort each student had invested tive reality standing in the way of the feared future (i.e.,
over the interim and provided each student with a grade for youth having wonderful and exciting soccer matches with
that period. As predicted, only in the mental contrasting foreigners), this produces expectancy-dependent goal
condition did the students feel energized, exert effort, and commitments as well (i.e., the goal of approaching the for-
earn grades based on their expectations of success. Those eigners by investing time and effort in welcoming foreign-
with high expectations of success felt the most energized, ers in the neighborhood). Moreover, Oettingen, Mayer,
invested the most effort, and received the highest course and Thorpe (in press) found that setting oneself the goal
grades; those with low expectations of success felt the to stop smoking can be facilitated by mentally contrasting
least energized, invested the least effort, and received the feared future of negative health consequences with the
the lowest course grades. To the contrary, participants in current positive reality of still having a healthy body.
both the indulging and the dwelling conditions felt moder- The mediating processes of mental contrasting pertain
ately energized, exerted medium effort, and received medium to energization (Oettingen, Mayer, Sevincer, et al., 2009).
grades independent of their expectations of success. Specifically, mentally contrasting a desired future with
Various studies pertaining to different life domains rep- obstacles of present reality leads to energization, which
licated this pattern of results, for example, experiments in turn creates goal commitments strong enough to lead

CH08.indd 274 12/18/09 2:40:03 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 275

to effective goal striving and successful goal attainment. Kazén & Kuhl, 2005). However, these facilitating effects
Mediating effects of energization on goal commitment are evince for individuals who perform these tasks while
shown on physiological indicators of energization (e.g., being in a positive affective state. Note that in the studies
systolic blood pressure) as well as on experiential indica- reported earlier, performance was assessed long after the
tors (e.g., self-report of feeling energized). Mental con- hype produced by the positive affective state had van-
trasting also spurs planning, a known cognitive mediator ished. Therefore, performance was a function of goal com-
between expectations of success and goal commitment mitment, and a binding goal commitment cannot emerge
(Oettingen & Stephens, 2009). when people indulge in their wishes and fantasies about
Mental contrasting, because it is a problem-solving strat- the future.
egy, necessitates heightened cognitive activity. A recent A further strategy of goal setting is suggested by
experiment attesting to this idea used continuous magne- the mindset theory of action phases (Gollwitzer, 1990;
toencephalography, a brain imaging technique measuring Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987). This theory maintains
magnetic fields produced by electrical activity in the brain that setting goals means selecting one of many wishes and
(Achtziger, Fehr, Oettingen, Gollwitzer, & Rockstroh, deciding to realize it. The theory posits that goal pursuit
2009). Mental contrasting, as compared with indulging has multiple stages, called action phases, that people need
or simply resting, produced heightened brain activity in to successfully navigate to attain a goal: the predecision,
areas associated with working memory, episodic memory, the preaction, the action, and the postaction phases. Each
intention maintenance, action preparation, and vivid visu- phase is characterized by a distinct task that must be accom-
alization. That is, mental contrasting implies vividly imag- plished, and engaging in each of these tasks produces a
ining a desired future, anticipating hindrances to realizing typical mindset that facilitates task completion. Setting
this future, and making plans on how to overcome these a goal is the result of navigating the predecisional phase.
barriers. The brain activity associated with indulging, on Assuming that people generally entertain more wishes
the other hand, did not differ from resting. than they have time or means to realize, they face the task
Given this latter finding, one might think that indulging of having to decide among the wishes to accomplish at least
in the future could also potentially lead to strong goal some of them. The deliberation of desirability and feasibil-
commitments—if individuals were to engage only in ity guides this decision. Accordingly, whenever people start
highly positive fantasies about the future. But research to deliberate their wishes, cognitive procedures become
on engaging in positive versus negative fantasies about activated that allow for open-minded processing of avail-
the future speaks against this argument. Oettingen and able information (Fujita, Gollwitzer, & Oettingen, 2007),
Wadden (1991) observed that obese women who would tune people toward processing information related to the
spontaneously indulge in positive fantasies about their desirability and feasibility of their wishes (Gollwitzer,
weight loss were less successful in achieving a lower body Heckhausen, & Steller, 1990), and allow even-handed
mass (after 4 months and 2 years) than were obese women (impartial) and objective (realistic) analysis of this infor-
whose spontaneously produced fantasies were more nega- mation (Armor & Taylor, 2003; Bayer & Gollwitzer, 2005;
tive. Moreover, Oettingen and Mayer (2002) observed that Gagné & Lydon, 2001a, 2001b; Gollwitzer & Kinney,
people who indulge in positive fantasies (valence and fre- 1989; Taylor & Gollwitzer, 1995).
quency) show comparatively weaker goal commitments (as Mindset theory assumes that the transition from the pre-
assessed by their efforts to strive for the goal) in the areas decisional phase to the preaction phase takes the form of
of academic achievement (i.e., achieving a good grade in a resolution that leads to a determination to act. Through
a psychology class), professional achievement (i.e., find- this resolution, the desired end-state specified by the
ing a job after graduation), interpersonal relations (i.e., wish becomes a goal that the individual feels committed
finding a romantic partner), and health (i.e., recovering to achieve. According to mindset theory, the desirability
from hip surgery). Importantly, it did not matter whether and feasibility of a wish need to be fully and completely
the spontaneously produced positive fantasies pertained deliberated before people can move from indecisiveness
to the desired outcome or to the ways of getting there. Also, to decisiveness. Accordingly, when people feel that they have
goal commitment in these studies was assessed 2 weeks or deliberated enough, they should be able to justify to them-
even 2 years after the assessment of the spontaneously pro- selves that they can now make such a move (i.e., “cross the
duced positive future fantasies. Rubicon”). Indeed, Gollwitzer, Heckhausen, and Ratajczak
At first, the reported findings seem to be in contrast (1990) observed that as-yet-undecided people were more
to research observing the facilitating effects of posi- likely to make a decision after they had been asked to
tive affect on performance in executive-functions tasks list likely positive and negative, short-term and long-
(Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004; Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2008; term consequences of goal attainment and to judge the

CH08.indd 275 12/18/09 2:40:03 PM


276 Motivation

likelihood of goal attainment. Importantly, another strategy effects of intrinsic goal striving extend beyond the indi-
helped people decide among wishes: When undecided vidual. Gore and Cross (2006) observed that goals based on
people were lured into planning the implementation of the intrinsic reasons implicating others (e.g., the people involved
wish on the pretense that a resolution to act on the wish make it fun or it is important to the people close to me) also
had already occurred, they became more likely to make facilitate goal attainment.
and commit themselves to a decision. Goal striving based on autonomy, competence, and
social integration needs has also been analyzed with respect
to subjective well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Such goals
Goal Striving
are observed to be positively associated with high well-
Once people have set themselves goals, it cannot be being and life satisfaction, whereas goals of making money,
assumed that attaining the goal is inevitable; rather, only becoming famous, and acquiring high status are negatively
the first step has been taken. People need then to move related. The latter is particularly true for individuals who
on and to engage in goal striving. Whether a desired goal feel highly efficacious, implying that people who suc-
is attained depends on how well this goal striving is exe- cessfully attain materialistic goals are particularly at risk
cuted. Successful goal striving depends first on what kind for low well-being (Kasser & Ryan, 1993). More recent
of goals people have set for themselves; again, the relevant research on the link between goal striving and well-being
variables are goal content and how this content is struc- has found that when it comes to striving for goals that sat-
tured or framed. However, successful goal striving also isfy autonomy, competence, and social integration needs,
depends on coping effectively with a few typical problems: the balance of meeting these goals also matters (Sheldon &
initiating goal-directed actions, persisting in the face of dif- Niemiec, 2006). People who experience balanced need sat-
ficulties, shielding the goal from distractions, disengaging isfaction report higher well-being than those with the same
from ineffective means, and not overextending oneself. sum score but high variability in need satisfaction.
This self-regulatory issue of what people can do to make Recent research also suggests that the two intrinsic
their goal striving more effective in the face of these prob- reasons for striving for academic success—having joy
lems (i.e., the effective self-regulation of goal striving) is and fun during striving versus feeling it to be important
discussed in the section following those on goal content to strive for academic success—have differential effects
and goal framing. on well-being (Burton, Lydon, D’Alessandro, & Koestner,
2006). Whereas striving for fun leads to high well-being
Goal Content independent of the performance level achieved, striving
Goal content strongly affects the chances of implementing because it is personally important (also referred to as iden-
a goal successfully. Ryan et al. (1996) have argued that tified striving) leads to well-being that is contingent on the
goals of autonomy, competence, and social integration achieved performance level. Imagine a scientist who writes
favor creativity, cognitive flexibility, deep processing a manuscript because it is fun versus a scientist who
of information, and effective coping with failure. These writes because writing is felt as important, and assume that
effects are assumed to be mediated by an intrinsic self- both scientists submit their manuscript for publication. The
regulation (see the self-concordance model by Sheldon & Burton et al. (2006) research suggests that the latter should
Elliot, 1999), because the needs of autonomy, compe- be in a worse position to cope with a rejection letter than
tence, and social integration are associated with intrinsic the former.
goal striving in line with a person’s interests or core values Brunstein, Schultheiss, and Grässmann (1998) have
rather than with extrinsic goal striving in line with envi- shown that the relation between a person’s progress toward
ronmental pressures or internal sanctions. Intrinsic goal a personal goal and well-being is moderated by the fit
striving is preferred by individuals with positive self- between the content of the goal and the person’s motive
regard (Judge, Bono, Erez, & Locke, 2005), and it can disposition. For instance, people with strong achieve-
be facilitated from outside by teachers who provide ment and power needs (measured as implicit motives by
autonomy support (e.g., when law school faculty provide au- the Thematic Apperception Test; McClelland, 1985b)
tonomy support, grade point average improves; Sheldon & who have goals with the same theme report higher emo-
Krieger, 2007). Moreover, intrinsic goal striving fosters not tional well-being when progressing toward their goals
only the attainment of behavioral goals but also the attainment than those whose needs and goals do not match. The same
of goals that pertain to cognitive and affective responses (e.g., is true of people with strong affiliation and intimacy needs
the goal to evaluate others in a fair, nonprejudicial manner who have goals with the same theme. Differential con-
as assessed by implicit and explicit measures of prejudice; sequences of successful goal striving on well-being also
Legault, Green-Demers, Grant, & Chung, 2007). The positive relate to friendship goals based either on compassion or on

CH08.indd 276 12/18/09 2:40:04 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 277

promoting a positive self-image. Whereas the former type the earlier discussion of goal setting pertained to what
of goal striving produces feelings of closeness and con- determines that a person sets goals with various structural
nectedness, the latter type leads to loneliness and feelings features. What follows is a discussion of the kinds of con-
of anxiety (Crocker & Canevello, 2008). Finally, a recent sequences choosing one or the other structural framing has
meta-analysis on the relative consequences of striving for successful goal attainment.
for cooperative, competitive, and individualistic goals for Bandura and Schunk (1981) argue that the time frame
achievement in adolescents suggests that higher achieve- of a set goal can be broken into many small units or one
ment and more positive peer relationships are associated large unit. Children who were uninterested and performed
with cooperative rather than competitive or individualistic poorly in mathematics pursued a program of self-directed
goal striving (Roseth, Johnson, & Johnson, 2008). learning (a total of 42 pages of instructions) under con-
ditions involving either a distal goal only (42 pages in
Goal Structure seven sessions) or the distal goal plus proximal subgoals
In addition to goal content, structural features of set goals (6 pages per session for seven sessions). Additional proxi-
affect whether goal striving is successful or not. For exam- mal goals improved the children’s arithmetic scores by
ple, goal striving is said to depend on the strength of goals providing more performance feedback, thus making it
(intentions; Ajzen, 1985, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). easier to monitor progress in goal pursuit. However, this
But most tests of this goal–behavior relationship involve feedback advantage may turn into a disadvantage when the
only correlational studies that preclude causal inferences. goals require inhibition (e.g., goals to refrain from chat-
A recent meta-analysis by Webb and Sheeran (2006) took a ting in class), as people more readily discover failures that
closer look at this assumption by selecting studies where the may cause them to give up prematurely. Indeed, Cochran
strength of the goal was manipulated relative to a control and Tesser (1996) observed that the goal proximity effect
group, and differences in subsequent goal-directed behavior reverses for goals framed in terms of preventing failures.
were observed. They found 47 experimental tests of the Framing of orientation of social goals in terms of
intention (goal)–behavior relationship that actually used approach and avoidance clearly affects their attainment. For
an experimental manipulation of the strength of the goal instance, striving for the goal of making new friends versus
(intention). The meta-analysis showed that the medium- striving for the goal of not being lonely produces quite differ-
to-large change in strength of intention (d ⫽ .66) led to a ent outcomes. With respect to the outcome variable of satis-
small-to-medium change in respective behavior (d ⫽ .36). faction with social bonds versus loneliness, the latter leads
But success in goal striving not only depends on the to less favorable results than the former (Elliot, Gable, &
strength of the goal; it also depends on what kind of aspi- Mapes, 2006; Strachman & Gable, 2006). Recent research
ration or standard is specified in the goal (i.e., whether suggests that these differences are mediated by differential
the person wants to achieve a lot or only a little). Locke attention and memory processes, differential interpretation
and Latham (2002, 2006) report that participants are more and weighting of available information, and differential
likely to attain challenging goals spelled out in specific evaluation of the progress made toward goal attainment.
terms than to attain moderately specific goals or chal- Higgins (2000; Förster, Higgins, & Idson, 1998;
lenging but vague (i.e., “do your best”) goals. This effect Shah, Higgins, & Friedman, 1998) reports that approach
has several prerequisites: frequent performance feedback, goals benefit more from goal striving that uses eagerness-
strong goal commitment, low goal complexity, and avail- related approach strategies (such as pulling things toward
ability to the individual of necessary skills and means. oneself) than from vigilance-related avoidance strategies
What does not seem to matter is whether goal setting is (such as pushing things away from oneself), whereas the
determined from outside (assigned goals), freely chosen reverse is true for avoidance goals. The assumed reason
by the individual (self-set goals), or chosen in interaction for this is value from fit. Higgins (2000, 2006) argues that
with others (participative goals). As potential mediators people engage more in goal striving when the strategies
of the goal-specificity effect, Locke and Latham point to used match the goal orientation (i.e., eagerness strategies to
heightened persistence, attention to the execution of goal- positive outcome focus and vigilance strategies to negative
directed behaviors, greater readiness to plan the goal pur- outcome focus) than when there is a mismatch (i.e., vigi-
suit, and feedback and self-monitoring advantages. lance strategies to positive outcome focus and eagerness
Goal implementation is also affected by the structural strategies to negative outcome focus). This heightened
features of time frame and goal orientation (i.e., approach engagement in turn leads to higher perceived value and
vs. avoidance orientation, promotion vs. prevention, learn- strength of attraction to this outcome.
ing vs. performance orientation, low vs. high psychological Framing goals in terms of learning versus performance
distance, and low vs. high identity relation). Note that has been found to have different effects on achievement

CH08.indd 277 12/18/09 2:40:04 PM


278 Motivation

(Dweck, 1996). Learning goals lead to better achievement a person’s own behavioral goals. Again, “why” construals
than performance goals because the former allow for more led to more delayed enactment estimates than “how”
effective coping with negative feedback than the latter. For construals. This effect was replicated even when induced
people with performance goals, negative feedback signals by other means than “why” versus “how” construal (e.g., by
failure and lack of ability and thus causes them to give up having participants think about implicated traits vs. means
prematurely. People with learning goals, on the other hand, or describing the activity in concrete, low-level terms, such
view negative feedback as a setback and as a valuable cue as “checking a paper for spelling and typing errors,” vs.
on how to focus on new strategies, thus furthering goal abstract, high-level terms, such as “proofread a paper”).
attainment. Most importantly, McCrea, Liberman, Trope, and Sherman
Elliot and Church (1997) observed that performance (2008) assessed actual enactment times of intended activ-
goals are less detrimental when they are framed as ity (i.e., returning a filled-out questionnaire to the experi-
approach goals (e.g., “I want to get good grades”) rather menter on time) that was framed in terms of high versus
than avoidance goals (e.g., “I do not want to get bad low psychological distance. Even though psychologi-
grades”). Recent studies by Darnon, Harackiewicz, Butera, cal distance was again manipulated by various methods,
Mugny, and Quiamzade (2007), however, show that this low psychological distance led to earlier enactment of
is only true when the achievement context does not allow the intended activity than high psychological distance. It
for the emergence of fear of failure (i.e., the task is easy or appears, then, that framing a goal in terms of high versus
the feedback on achievement is unambiguously positive). low psychological distance engenders the risk of procrasti-
Also, the effort that people put into performing a task (El- nating about the goal pursuit.
Alayli, 2006) depends on whether the framing of the task Finally, it matters whether a person frames a given
goal is in line with their naïve theories of whether the impli- task goal in terms of its identity-relatedness. For instance,
cated personal attribute is malleable (incremental theory) the task of solving a certain arithmetic problem can be
or fixed (entity theory). This finding is in line with obser- approached with the goal of solving it effectively or the goal
vations by Plaks and Stecher (2007), indicating that viola- of identifying oneself as a mathematician. The latter
tions of both implicit theories lead to comparatively more goal has been referred to as a self-defining goal or iden-
anxiety and impaired subsequent task performance (i.e., tity goal, as it specifies an identity as a desired end-state.
when entity theorists learn that performance has declined Self-completion theory (Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982) pro-
and when incremental theorists learn that performance poses that people who are committed to identity goals can
has not improved despite having had access to a learning undertake various activities to claim identity-goal attain-
opportunity). ment, because many behaviors indicate the possession
Recent research on the framing of achievement goals in of such identities. For a scientist, for example, such self-
terms of learning versus performance has also investigated symbolizing activities might include engaging in professional
its influence on interactions in social achievement situa- duties (e.g., giving lectures), making positive self-descriptions
tions (Poortvliet, Janssen, Van Yperen, & Van de Vliert, (e.g., “I discovered a new principle!”), exerting identity-
2007). People with learning goals are oriented reciprocally. relevant social influence (e.g., advising students), or acquiring
They give information openly, and they process received respective skills, tools, and material symbols (e.g., program-
information with a focus on those pieces of information ming skills, fast computers, or a large office).
that fit well and add value to their chosen task strategy. On Failing to perform an identity-relevant activity or lack-
the other hand, people with performance goals are oriented ing an identity symbol produces a state of incompleteness;
exploitatively. They provide information to others reluc- to restore completeness, people engage in self-symbolizing
tantly, and they process received information with a sus- efforts (summary by Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, 1998). People
picious attitude that leads them to focus on detecting and then emphasize the possession of alternative symbols or
disregarding low-quality information that might hurt their set out to acquire new identity symbols (e.g., engaging
task performance. in identity-relevant activities, Brunstein & Gollwitzer,
Psychological distance is another important structural 1996; describing oneself as having the required personal-
feature. Liberman, Trope, McCrea, and Sherman (2007) ity attributes, Gollwitzer & Wicklund, 1985; or showing
had research participants indicate either why or how off relevant status symbols, Harmon-Jones, Schmeichel, &
another person would perform an activity (e.g., open a Harmon-Jones, 2009). Importantly, affirming one’s gen-
bank account) and then asked them to guess when this per- eral self-integrity or bolstering one’s self-esteem are not
son would enact the activity. As it turned out, “why” con- sufficient to offset incompleteness regarding an identity
struals of the activity revealed longer time estimates than goal; rather, one must acquire specific identity symbols
“how” construals. Interestingly, this effect also applied to (Ledgerwood, Liviaton, & Carnevale, 2007).

CH08.indd 278 12/18/09 2:40:05 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 279

Research on self-completion theory has discovered that people did not act on their goals was simple: They felt that
a higher level of completeness is reached when a social inaction would alleviate their negative emotional states.
audience notices the individual’s self-symbolizing activi- Positive affect, on the other hand, has been observed
ties (Gollwitzer, 1986). In addition, incomplete individuals to facilitate goal striving. In early childhood (2 to 4 years
are more concerned with finding an audience for their iden- of age), positive emotionality in children’s interactions
tity strivings than are completed individuals (Brunstein & with their mothers facilitates the difficult self-regulation
Gollwitzer, 1996). This self-symbolizing, however, has required for tasks such as slowing down, lowering one’s
its costs. Self-symbolizing individuals see others only in voice, or delaying the unwrapping of a received gift.
terms of the potential to notice their compensatory efforts; Research with adults has focused on how positive affect
thus, they lack social sensitivity (Gollwitzer & Wicklund, achieves this positive effect. Kazén and Kuhl (2005;
1985). Most interestingly, when people make public their Kuhl & Kazén, 1999) argue that even though decreases
intention to acquire a certain self-definitional indicator in positive affect make it easier to maintain an intention in
(e.g., when a person who wants to become a great stu- working memory, it takes an increase in positive affect to
dent publicly utters the behavioral intention to enroll in facilitate the successful behavioral implementation of dif-
an inspiring course), it turns out that actual efforts toward ficult intentions (e.g., to do well on the Stroop task). Tamir
completion are reduced (Gollwitzer, Sheeran, Michalski, & and Robinson (2007) report data suggesting that positive
Seifert, 2009). Apparently, when others take notice of a moods (measured or induced) are associated with selec-
stated identity-relevant behavioral intention, the superordi- tive attention to reward stimuli. Gable and Harmon-Jones
nate goal of claiming the identity is already reached; thus, (2008) observed that positive affect induced by imagin-
performing the intended behavior becomes less necessary. ing rewards (such as tasty desserts) produces the reduced
This finding is in line with results of earlier self-completion breadth of attentional focus, which facilitates focusing on
studies; public, positive self-descriptions claiming the specific action tendencies and thus tenacious goal striving.
possession of an identity symbol produced the same sense Apparently, positive affect makes people focus on reward-
of self-definitional completeness as actual identity-relevant ing stimuli that in turn produce a narrowing of attentional
achievements (Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996; Gollwitzer, focus that makes it easy to strive for the goal at hand.
Wicklund, & Hilton, 1982). It is also consistent with the Given that positive affect seems to foster goal striving
general goal turnoff effect phenomenon discussed later in on well-structured tasks (e.g., Stroop and task-switching
this chapter. paradigms), this does not imply that positive affect is
beneficial to striving for all kinds of tasks. Complex and
Context Variables ill-defined tasks require that people anticipate potential
obstacles and hindrances. This is easier when people
Striving for a given goal depends on more than the con-
experience negative affect. Not surprisingly, then, posi-
tent of the goal and its structural features. It also depends
tive affect was found to be a hindrance for complex and
on the context in which the person is situated. Although
ill-defined tasks rather than a facilitator of goal attainment
context variables have not yet been analyzed with respect
(Markman, Lindberg, Kray, & Galinsky, 2007; Oettingen &
to goal setting, research on context variables in goal striv-
Mayer, 2002; Taylor, Pham, Rivkin, & Armor, 1998). And
ing differentiates various internal and external context
people are found to prefer to be in negative emotional
variables. Internal context variables relate to the person’s
states if those states better facilitate goal striving: Soldiers
affective state and the array of competing action tenden-
entering battle or football players during a game prefer an
cies, whereas external variables are focused on the power
angry, aggressive (negative) mood rather than a relaxed,
position of the goal striver.
positive mood (Tamir, 2009).

Affective State In a series of studies, Tice, Bratslavsky, Competing Action Tendencies The success of goal striv-
and Baumeister (2001) focused on negative affect and ing depends on whether it stays undisturbed by competing
observed that feeling emotionally distraught (i.e., having action tendencies. Kuhl’s action control theory (Kuhl &
been asked to imagine that one has caused a traffic acci- Beckmann, 1994) postulates that for an ordered action
dent that killed a child) makes it difficult to follow through sequence to occur the striving for a current guiding goal
with goals of not eating unhealthy food or delaying grati- must stay shielded from other competing responses (e.g.,
fication to attain better long-term rewards. Moreover, this the goal of making a phone call must not be derailed by the
emotionally negative state intensifies procrastination: for competing response to tidy one’s desk). In such shielding,
example, people did not use the time provided to study for Kuhl’s action control theory differentiates several control
an upcoming test. In each study, it appeared that the reason mechanisms, such as attention control, emotion control,

CH08.indd 279 12/18/09 2:40:06 PM


280 Motivation

and environment control. High environment control, for promote goal shielding whereas negative emotions hinder
example, is achieved when the situation in which the focal it; in this case, positive emotions apparently indicate a
goal is to be enacted is freed from distractions. Whether and feeling of high goal commitment. When closer to the goal,
how effectively these control mechanisms operate depends positive emotions decrease shielding of the focal goal
on the current control mode of the individual: action ori- whereas negative emotions prompt increased goal shield-
entation is seen as beneficial, whereas state orientation is ing; here, positive emotions apparently indicate a feeling
seen as a handicap. The former is characterized by a con- of high goal attainment.
centration on planning and initiating goal-directed actions, Finally, from a cognitive, executive-functions perspec-
as well as responding flexibly to situational demands; the tive, goal shielding in the service of a focal goal must strike
latter is associated with failing to disengage from unattain- a balance between two antagonistic challenges: on the one
able goals, dwelling on setbacks, or thinking unrealistically hand, the focal goal must be shielded from interferences,
about future successes. An individual’s orientation may be and thus distracting information should be inhibited; on
influenced by situational variables (e.g., surprising events the other hand, the environment must be monitored for
or persistent failures), but it is grounded in one’s personal potentially significant information that may necessitate
disposition. a goal switch (background monitoring). The benefits of
Interestingly, state-oriented individuals readily misper- goal shielding (i.e., preventing interferences) incur costs
ceive assigned goals as self-generated (Kazén, Baumann, & in terms of overlooking significant information occurring in
Kuhl, 2003); this finding has given rise to a new theoretical the background; the benefits of background monitoring
perspective called Personality Systems Interaction theory (i.e., noticing task-irrelevant but potentially significant
(Kuhl, 2000). This theory sees the control of goal-directed stimuli) incur costs in terms of increased susceptibility
action as a result of the interaction of various mental to interference. How does the human brain handle this
subsystems (i.e., intention memory, extension memory, dilemma? Using a prospective memory task paradigm,
intuitive behavior control, and object recognition) under Goschke and Dreisbach (2008) observed that goal shield-
conditions of change from low to high positive affect ver- ing is intensified at the cost of background monitoring
sus change from low to high negative affect. From this per- when the former is most needed (i.e., when goal striving
spective, action versus state orientation is understood as a is at risk of being derailed) and background monitoring is
parameter that modulates the cooperation between these reinstated when goal striving runs smoothly (i.e., when
systems, thus leading to different kinds of action control goal shielding is no longer needed).
with different outcomes. Goal systems theory provides a further cognitive per-
The analysis of the question of what makes for effective spective on goal shielding (Kruglanski et al., 2002). The
goal shielding has received much research attention. For theory has three major tenets. First, any given goal can
instance, Shah, Friedman, and Kruglanski (2002) observed be connected to any sensible number of means. If a goal
that high commitment to the focal goal facilitates goal is connected to six means, for instance, then the goal can
shielding (measured in terms of reduced accessibility of be attained through any of these means or any combina-
a competing goal), whereas feeling anxious and sad hin- tion of them. Said differently, there may be many means
ders it. Moreover, when people consider the progress they to a given goal (equifinality configuration), and there may
have made toward the goal, there is less goal shielding, as be many goals linked to a single means (multifinality
people open up to competing goals; this effect occurs even configuration). Second, it is always the active (focal) goal
when people are told merely to intend to make progress in that captures the means for its accomplishment. Given that
the future (Fishbach & Dhar, 2005). However, this nega- cognitive resources are limited, investing attention and
tive effect of goal progress on goal shielding should only effort into a focal goal implies removal of resources from
be expected if the goal-directed actions taken (or intended) a competing goal (allocational property). Attainment of
are interpreted by the individual as completing the goal; the focal goal becomes more likely, and attainment of the
if the action is instead interpreted as indicating a strong competing goal becomes less likely. Third, not all means
commitment to the focal goal, then improved goal shield- of a given goal are equally substitutable (contextual depen-
ing would be expected (Fishbach, Dhar, & Zhang, 2006; dence property). Only contextually available means can
Koo & Fishbach, 2008). In line with this reasoning, Louro, be considered for selection, and among these available
Pieters, and Zeelenberg (2007) report on the basis of diary means the most salient, vivid, and accessible will win out.
and experimental studies that the effects of perceived prox- Moreover, context matters, as some contexts render certain
imity to the goal are moderated by the experience of posi- means more desirable than others. Based on these consid-
tive or negative goal-related emotions. That is, when the erations, recent research shows that when a given means
attainment of the focal goal is remote, positive emotions for goal striving is perceived as serving multiple goals, it is

CH08.indd 280 12/18/09 2:40:06 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 281

less likely to be selected or valued as highly instrumental power), as determinants of successful goal striving and
than when it is perceived as serving only one goal (Zhang, goal attainment. The self-regulation approach to goal striv-
Fishbach, & Kruglanski, 2007). Accordingly, goal striv- ing, on the other hand, focuses on how the individual can
ing should be less vulnerable to disruptions (i.e., should be master the problems inherent in goal striving. To attain the
associated with enhanced goal shielding) when it is based set goal, the individual needs to seize opportunities to act,
on using means unique to that goal. ward off distractions, flexibly step up efforts in the face of
difficulties, bypass barriers, and compensate for failures and
Power Position Researchers have focused on the contex- shortcomings. Various theories address how the individual
tual variable of being in a position of power versus being can effectively solve these problems of goal striving.
powerless. Power has been manipulated experimentally in
several ways: participants remember an incident in which Planning Goal Implementation As noted earlier, the
they had power over someone or someone had power over mindset model of action phases (Gollwitzer, 1990;
them, they imagine or act in a powerful role (manager or Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987) conceives of goal pur-
evaluator) or a powerless role (subordinate or worker), or suit as solving a series of consecutive tasks. The task
power is primed outside of awareness, for example, by hav- that follows goal setting is that of getting started with
ing participants perform a scrambled sentences task using goal-directed responses and then bringing goal striving
words related to having power (“authority” and “dominate”) to a successful end. The model assumes that becoming
or to lacking power (“subordinate” and “obey”). These stud- intensively involved with this task activates a functional
ies have shown that not only do powerful (as compared with mindset, namely, the implemental mindset. This mindset
powerless) participants relate differently to people by treat- is characterized by several functional cognitive features
ing them as a means to the attainment of their goals (i.e., (see summary by Gollwitzer & Bayer, 1999). Participants
objectivation; Gruenfeld, Inesi, Magee, & Galinsky, 2008) become closed-minded (no longer distracted by irrel-
but they also differ in the ways in which they strive for per- evant information) and process information related to
sonal goals. goal implementation effectively (e.g., information on the
Guinote (2007) observed that people in power procras- sequencing of actions; Gollwitzer et al., 1990). Moreover,
tinate less in pursuing their goals, they persist longer in the desirability of the set goal becomes enhanced through the
the face of difficulties, they show more willingness to try favoring of pros over cons, and the feasibility of the set
different strategies to attain the goal, and they more readily goal is enhanced by an illusory optimism. This optimism
seize good opportunities to make goal-directed responses. extends to an illusion of control over uncontrolled out-
In addition, they more readily recognize whether a given comes (Gollwitzer & Kinney, 1989) and occurs even in
situation can be used to serve their goals and then allow individuals who are depressed. Self-perception of impor-
suitable situations to guide their behavior (Guinote, 2008). tant personal attributes (e.g., cheerfulness, smartness, and
All of this appears to be facilitated by a change in execu- social sensitivity) is strengthened, while perceived vulner-
tive functioning. Smith, Jostmann, Galinsky, and van Dijk ability to both controllable and uncontrollable risks (e.g.,
(2008) report that powerful individuals are better than developing an addiction to prescription drugs and losing a
powerless ones at updating goal-relevant information (i.e., partner to an early death, respectively) is lowered (Taylor &
new information is monitored for goal relevance, and rel- Gollwitzer, 1995).
evant information replaces old, irrelevant information These cognitive features of the implemental mindset
in working memory). They are also better at inhibiting mediate the positive effects that planning how to imple-
responses that may interfere with the present goal and at ment the goal has on the rate of goal attainment (Armor &
planning by continuously switching between the main goal Taylor, 2003). Implemental mindsets also attenuate the
and the respective subgoals. Finally, these powerful partic- classic problems associated with goal striving, such as
ipants show less goal neglect (i.e., forgetting to strive for doubting the attractiveness of the pursued goal (Gagné &
the goal; Kane & Engle, 2003) by actively maintaining the Lydon, 2001a, 2001b) and showing too much or too little
goal in working memory. Future research could profitably persistence (Brandstätter & Frank, 2002). Both the posi-
explore the extent to which these effects are mediated by tive effects and the problems occur because people in
heightened efficacy beliefs or to which control beliefs are an implemental mindset feel less ambivalent about the
likely to be stimulated by the power manipulations. attitudes they hold toward common issues (objects,
persons, or ideas) of daily life (Henderson, de Liver, &
Self-Regulation of Goal Striving Gollwitzer, 2008); instead, they show heightened attitude
The preceding discussion considered goal content and strength that more effectively translates their attitudes
structure, as well as contextual variables (e.g., relative into behavior.

CH08.indd 281 12/18/09 2:40:07 PM


282 Motivation

Planning can also be used to add implementation (e.g., corridor or red door) compared with participants
intentions to set goals (or goal intentions). Set goals only who only formed the goal intention to collect the cou-
commit an individual to attaining the specified, desired pon; and implementation-intention participants also were
outcome; they do not commit the individual to when, more likely to collect the coupon subsequently (Aarts,
where, and how to act toward the goal. Such additional Dijksterhuis, & Midden, 1999).
commitments can be facilitated through the formation of Implementation intentions also forge a strong associa-
implementation intentions, having the basic format of “If tion between the specified opportunity and the specified
I encounter situation x, then I will perform the goal- response (Webb & Sheeran, 2007, 2008). These strong
directed behavior y.” Thus, an implementation intention links then automate the initiation of the goal-directed
that serves the goal intention to “get an A in Introductory response specified in the if–then plan; that is, action initia-
Psychology” would follow the form “if situation Y arises tion exhibits features of automaticity, including immediacy,
(e.g., my roommates ask me to go out tonight), then I will efficiency, and redundancy of conscious intent. Thus, peo-
perform behavior Z (e.g., I will say that I will join them ple no longer have to deliberate about when and how they
next week when my exam is over).” Note that this type should act if they have already formed an implementation
of if–then planning commits the individual to acting in a intention—unlike people who have formed mere goal inten-
specific, goal-directed way (the “then” component of the tions. Indeed, if–then planners act quickly (Gollwitzer &
plan) whenever the critical cue selected in the “if” compo- Brandstätter, 1997, study 3), deal efficiently with cogni-
nent is encountered. In contrast, the more reflective way tive load (Brandstätter, Lengfelder, & Gollwitzer, 2001),
of planning referred to as process simulation by Taylor et and even respond to the critical situational cue when it is
al. (1998) only explores possible ways to achieve a goal; presented subliminally (Bayer, Achtziger, Gollwitzer, &
it does not yet commit the person to one preferred way of Moskowitz, 2009).
implementing a goal, as is the case with implementation These component processes of implementation intentions
intentions (Faude-Koivisto, Würz, & Gollwitzer, 2008). (enhanced cue accessibility and automation of respond-
Implementation intentions have been shown to provide ing) are the underlying reason if–then planning enables
benefits beyond those of goal intentions: A meta-analysis people to effectively seize good opportunities to move
by Gollwitzer and Sheeran (2006) involving more than toward their goals. Forming if–then plans thus strategi-
8,000 participants in 94 independent studies reported an cally automates goal striving (Gollwitzer & Schaal, 1998)
effect size of d ⫽ .65. This medium-to-large effect size because people delegate control of goal-directed behav-
(Cohen, 1992) represents the additional facilitation of goal iors to preselected situational cues with the explicit pur-
achievement by implementation intentions compared with pose of reaching their goals (i.e., a conscious act of will
goal intentions alone. As goal intentions by themselves produces subsequent automatic action initiation). Given
already have a facilitating effect on behavior enactment these special features of implementation intentions,
(Webb & Sheeran, 2006), the size of this effect is remark- researchers have explored whether people benefit from
able. But how do implementation-intention effects on goal forming implementation intentions when they are con-
attainment come about? Gollwitzer (1993, 1999) suggests fronted with the most challenging problems of goal imple-
that the mental links created by implementation intentions mentation: getting started, staying on track, calling a halt,
facilitate goal attainment on the basis of psychological and not overextending oneself.
processes that relate to both the anticipated situation (the Numerous studies suggest that the problems of get-
“if ” part of the plan) and the intended behavior (the “then” ting started on goals can be solved effectively by forming
part of the plan). Because forming an implementation implementation intentions. For instance, Gollwitzer and
intention implies the selection of a critical future situation, Brandstätter (1997, study 2) analyzed a goal intention (i.e.,
the mental representation of this situation becomes highly writing a report about how the participants spent Christmas
activated and hence more accessible (Gollwitzer, 1999). Eve) that had to be performed at a time period when peo-
This heightened accessibility of the “if” part of the plan ple are commonly busy with other things (i.e., during the
has been observed in several studies (e.g., Parks-Stamm, subsequent Christmas holiday). Still, research participants
Gollwitzer, & Oettingen, 2007; Webb & Sheeran, 2007, who had furnished their goal intention with an implemen-
2008; Wieber & Sassenberg, 2006). It puts people in a tation intention that specified when, where, and how they
good position to identify and take notice of the critical sit- wanted to start this project were about three times as likely
uation when they subsequently encounter it (e.g., Webb & to actually write the report as mere goal intention partici-
Sheeran, 2004). For instance, participants who formed pants. Similarly, Oettingen et al. (2000, study 3) observed
implementation intentions to collect a coupon were faster that implementation intentions helped people act on their
to recognize words related to the location of the coupon task goal of performing weekly math homework over

CH08.indd 282 12/18/09 2:40:07 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 283

1 month on time (e.g., at 10 AM every Wednesday over the Bayer, & McCulloch, 2005). Using again the example of a
next 4 weeks). person who is approached by her friend with an outrageous
Other studies have examined the ability of implemen- request, assume that this person is also tired or irritated and
tation intentions to initiate goal striving where there is an thus particularly likely to respond in an unfriendly manner.
initial reluctance because the activities involved are some- If, in advance, this person has stipulated in an implementa-
what unpleasant. For instance, implementation intentions tion intention what topic she will converse about with her
improved the success rates for the goals of performing friend, the critical interaction may simply run as planned
regular breast examinations (Orbell, Hodgkins, & Sheeran, and being tired or irritated will fail to hurt the interaction
1997) or cervical cancer screenings (Sheeran & Orbell, with her friend.
2000), resuming functional activity after joint replacement Implementation intentions not only help people move
surgery (Orbell & Sheeran, 2000), eating a low-fat diet smoothly and effectively toward a goal but also can be used
(Armitage, 2004), recycling (Holland, Aarts, & Langendam, to redirect misplaced goal striving. To justify themselves
2006), and engaging in physical exercise (Milne, Orbell, & and their choices, people often fail to readily relinquish
Sheeran, 2002). Moreover, implementation intentions chosen means and goals that turn out to be faulty (Brockner,
were found to help attainment of goal intentions where it 1992). Such escalation phenomena (also referred to as
is easy to forget to act (e.g., regular intake of vitamin pills, “throwing good money after bad”) can be controlled by
Sheeran & Orbell, 1999, or the signing of work sheets with the use of implementation intentions that specify when and
the elderly; Chasteen, Park, & Schwarz, 2001). how to consider a switch to a different means or a different
Many goals cannot be accomplished by simple, discrete, goal (Henderson, Gollwitzer, & Oettingen, 2007).
one-shot actions but require that people maintain their Finally, when task performance is regulated by imple-
efforts over an extended period. Staying on track may mentation intentions, it becomes easier not to overextend
get difficult when certain internal stimuli (e.g., being anx- oneself. Student participants who used implementation
ious, tired, and overburdened) or external stimuli (e.g., intentions to perform a first difficult task did not show
temptations and distractions) are not conducive to goal real- reduced performance on a subsequent difficult task.
ization but instead generate interference that could poten- Whether the initial task was controlling emotions while
tially derail the ongoing goal. Implementation intentions watching a humorous movie (Gollwitzer et al., 2005) or
facilitate the shielding of such goal striving from outside performing a Stroop task (Webb & Sheeran, 2003, study 1),
interference by suppressing it. For instance, imagine some- implementation intentions successfully preserved self-
one has the goal of being supportive to friends. When regulatory resources as demonstrated by greater persis-
the friend surprises the person with an outrageous request, tence on subsequent difficult tasks (i.e., solving difficult
she could prevent the unwanted unfriendly response by anagrams).
forming suppression-oriented implementation intentions. Implementation intentions have been shown to help
Such suppression-oriented implementation intentions people solve the major problems of goal striving (i.e., get-
may take various forms: “And if my friend approaches ting started, staying on track, calling a halt, and not over-
me with an outrageous request, then I will not respond in extending oneself). But do these benefits persist when
an unfriendly manner!” or “. . . , then I will respond in a goal striving is limited by conditions that seem quite resis-
friendly manner!” or “. . . , then I’ll ignore it!” Experimental tant to change by self-regulatory strategies? Bayer and
research on this issue by Gollwitzer and Schaal (1998) and Gollwitzer (2007) found that implementation intentions
Mischel and Patterson (1978) has indicated that in general geared toward high self-efficacy facilitated higher scores
the “ignore” plans seem the most effective of the three pos- on math and intelligence tests, even though such perfor-
sible suppression plans. mances are known to be limited by the individual’s capa-
Suppression-oriented implementation intentions have bilities in these domains. Implementation intentions also
also been found to effectively shield ongoing goal striv- helped people succeed in sports competitions (i.e., if–then
ing from disruptive inner states. Achtziger, Gollwitzer, and plans pertaining to effective coping with critical internal
Sheeran (2008) report a field experiment concerned with and external situations; Achtziger et al., 2008, study 2) and
dieting (study 1) in which goal shielding was supported by negotiate with others the distribution of limited resources
suppression implementation intentions geared at control- (i.e., if–then plans to make fair and cooperative counterof-
ling potentially interfering inner states (i.e., cravings for fers; Trötschel & Gollwitzer, 2007), even though in such
junk food). In addition, forming implementation intentions competitive situations a person’s goal striving is signifi-
geared at stabilizing the ongoing goal striving can protect cantly constrained by the opponents’ behavior.
it from getting derailed by adverse inner states (e.g., inap- Moreover, implementation intentions were found to
propriate moods, ego depletion, or irritation; Gollwitzer, help goal striving even in cases where effective goal

CH08.indd 283 12/18/09 2:40:08 PM


284 Motivation

striving is threatened by competing habitual responses; Another characteristic of the goal that moderates the
this seems to be true no matter whether these automatic effectiveness of implementation intentions is its difficulty.
competing responses are behavioral (e.g., habitual classifi- Implementation intentions typically benefit difficult rather
cation responses in a Simon task, Cohen, Bayer, Jaudas, & than easy goals (Gollwitzer & Brandstätter, 1997), because
Gollwitzer, 2008; habitual littering, Holland et al., 2006; or easy goals do not need the assistance of implementation
habitual coping with relationship threats, Lydon, Menzies- intentions to be successfully completed. Moreover, because
Toman, Burton, & Bell, 2008), cognitive (e.g., habitual implementation intentions are subordinate to goal inten-
stereotypical and prejudicial responses; Gollwitzer & tions, the strength of implementation-intention effects is
Schaal, 1998; Stewart & Payne, 2008), or affective (e.g., positively related to the strength of the goal’s commitment
habitual disgust and fear responses; Schweiger Gallo, Keil, and activation state (Sheeran, Webb, & Gollwitzer, 2005).
McCulloch, Rockstroh, & Gollwitzer, 2009). The latter Implementation intentions respect the status of the super-
findings suggest that forming implementation intentions ordinate goal (strength and activation), guaranteeing that
turns top-down control by a person’s goals into bottom- the goal striving produced by implementation intentions
up control by the situational cues specified in the “if ” is both tenacious and flexible. Recent research shows that
component. forming implementation intentions to use a certain oppor-
This strategic switch from top-down control of one’s tunity for goal-directed action does not imply that other
actions by set goals to bottom-up control through speci- suitable opportunities to move toward the goal are wasted;
fied situational cues is typical of habitual or automatic the effective seizing of the specified opportunity does not
behavior. In a recent functional magnetic resonance come at a cost to seizing alternative good opportunities to
imaging study conducted by Gilbert, Gollwitzer, Cohen, act toward the goal (Gollwitzer, Parks-Stamm, Jaudas, &
Oettingen, and Burgess (2009), brain activity in lateral area Sheeran, 2008).
10 was observed to move toward medial area 10 when par- Researchers have raised the question of how to teach
ticipants switched from performing an executive-functions people the goal-striving strategy of forming implementa-
task by the guidance of a goal intention to performing the tion intentions. To answer this question, it is important to
same type of task by the guidance of an implementation recognize that forming implementation intentions is a self-
intention; on the basis of an extensive meta-analysis on regulation strategy for goal striving only and that effective
various executive-functions tasks, it is known that lat- goal striving necessitates prior effective goal setting. An
eral and medial area 10 are implicated in top-down and effective self-regulation strategy that helps people meet
bottom-up action control, respectively (Burgess, Simons, their goals would thus have to address goal setting and
Dumontheil, & Gilbert, 2005). This switch in action con- goal striving in concert.
trol from top-down to bottom-up also explains why special One such self-regulation strategy, called mental con-
populations that are known to suffer from ineffective trasting with implementation intentions (MCII), has been
conscious control of their thoughts, feelings, and actions developed and tested. To unfold their beneficial effects,
(e.g., heroin addicts during withdrawal and schizophrenic implementation intentions require strong goal commitments
patients, Brandstätter et al., 2001, studies 1 and 2; frontal to be in place (Sheeran et al., 2005, study 1), and mental
lobe patients, Lengfelder & Gollwitzer, 2001; and children contrasting creates such strong commitments (Oettingen
with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, Gawrilow & et al., 2001). In addition, mental contrasting guarantees the
Gollwitzer, 2008; Paul et al., 2007) benefit greatly from identification of obstacles that hinder goal striving. These
forming implementation intentions when initiating goal- same obstacles can then be addressed with if–then plans by
directed actions or inhibiting unwanted habitual actions. specifying them as critical situations in the “if” component
Various moderators of the effects of implementation that are linked to instrumental goal-directed responses in
intentions on goal attainment pertain to characteristics of the “then” component. As mental contrasting increases a
the superordinate goal and the individual. One goal char- person’s readiness to make if–then plans (Oettingen et al.,
acteristic that moderates the success of implementation 2001), people should be likely to follow through with the
intentions is the extent to which it reflects the individu- formation of such plans. Accordingly, the mental con-
al’s actual interests and values. Koestner, Lekes, Powers, trasting part of the MCII self-regulation strategy prepares
and Chicoine (2002) have shown that the positive effects people motivationally and cognitively to form implemen-
of implementation intentions on goal attainment partially tation intentions so that they should find it easier to per-
depend on whether they are formed in the service of intrin- form the implementation-intention part.
sic (high autonomy) versus extrinsic (low autonomy) goals. Indeed, in a recent intervention study with middle-aged
Implementation intentions that advance intrinsic goals are women (Stadler, Oettingen, & Gollwitzer, 2009), all par-
more effective than those that advance extrinsic goals. ticipants first were informed about the types of exercise

CH08.indd 284 12/18/09 2:40:08 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 285

appropriate for their gender and age group and the multiple effective strategies delay gratification, such as reducing
health benefits that accrue from regular exercise. In the attention to the treats (e.g., hiding them, playing with dis-
MCII group (as compared with the information-only tracting toys, and engaging in fun thoughts) and mentally
control group), participants also learned about the mental reconstructing the treats as objects instead of consumables
contrasting technique with respect to the goal of exercising (e.g., pretending that the rewards are just pictures by put-
regularly (e.g., going for a run three times per week); the ting a frame around them). Mischel and Ayduk (2004)
interventions then taught them to form three implementa- argue that people have a better chance of delaying gratifi-
tion intentions regarding the central personal obstacle dis- cation for the ultimate goal (i.e., getting the double treat) by
covered during mental contrasting (e.g., feeling too tired dealing with the treats through the cool “know” system of
in the evening to go for a run) in the form of if–then state- information processing rather than the hot “go” system.
ments: one to overcome the obstacle generated by mental Trope and Fishbach (2000) also addressed the short-term
contrasting (e.g., “If I feel exhausted when I get home from costs of striving for a rewarding but long-term goal. They
work tonight, then I will put on my running shoes and go observed that simply anticipating such costs (e.g., the pain
for a jog in the neighborhood”), one to prevent this obstacle associated with the goal to undergo a certain preventive
(e.g., “If I hear the clock chime five o’clock, then I will medical procedure) makes people more persistent in their
pack my things and leave the office to go for a run”), and goal striving by instigating self-control strategies such as
one to identify a good opportunity to act (e.g., “If the sun is applying self-imposed penalties for failure to move for-
shining, then I will go for a 30-minute jog in the park”). ward on the goal or bolstering the value of persisting with
Next, participants were told to apply this MCII proce- the goal, which in turn facilitates goal attainment (Trope &
dure to the wish of exercising more by themselves when- Fishbach, 2000). A follow-up series of studies showed
ever possible; they were free to choose whatever form that such self-control strategies compensate for a lack of
of exercising they wished, and they were encouraged external control over the goal activity (Fishbach & Trope,
to anticipate those obstacles that were personally most 2004); in other words, externally imposed control and self-
relevant. As dependent measures, participants maintained control are interchangeable when it comes to striving for
daily behavioral diaries to keep track of the amount of time goals that have long-term benefits but short-term costs.
they exercised every day. Overall, the MCII technique Researchers have developed new insights into how
enhanced exercise more than the information-only group; people deal effectively with alluring temptations that
this effect showed up immediately after the intervention, threaten their goal pursuits. Certainly, people can always
and it stayed stable throughout the entire period of the make plans to suppress these alluring temptations
study (16 weeks after the intervention). More specifically, (Gollwitzer & Schaal, 1998; Mischel & Patterson, 1978).
participants in the MCII group exercised nearly twice as But this strategy implies that people can and are willing to
much: an average of 1 hour more per week more than par- anticipate what kind of temptation might disrupt their goal
ticipants in the information-only control group. striving so that they can target their if–then plans toward
them (e.g., by using mental contrasting or process simula-
Persistent Striving for the Goal Persistence is another tions; Oettingen et al., 2001; Taylor et al., 1998). Are there
powerful way to assure goal attainment. Various issues any more general temptation-suppressing strategies than
make persisting toward a goal problematic, however. if–then plans or implementation intentions?
Often, people have to accept short-term costs, they have Recent research hints at such strategies. For instance,
to escape alluring temptations that disrupt continued goal Fishbach, Friedman, and Kruglanski (2003) report that
striving, they have to cope with negative feedback, they exposure to goal-related stimuli reduces the cognitive
need to interpret where they stand on the way to the goal, accessibility of temptation-related stimuli and exposure to
and they cannot exhaust their self-regulation resources. In temptation-related stimuli activates their goals. The more
addition, some chronic beliefs or self-evaluations might people are committed to their goals, the more pronounced
make persistent striving for the goal more difficult. these deactivation and activation effects; in addition, the
With respect to the issue of accepting short-term costs, strength of these activation and deactivation effects medi-
Mischel’s (1974) research on delay of gratification is prom- ates people’s intensity of striving for the goal. Fishbach and
inent. This research uses a standardized task paradigm. Shah (2006) observed that whenever the conflict between
A child is presented with a desired treat (e.g., pretzel sticks persisting on a goal and giving in to temptation is high (when
or little marshmallows). Then a dilemma is posed: If the the attraction to temptations is high and people are strongly
child waits until the experimenter returns, she gets two of committed to the goal, e.g., partying and studying for
the desired treats; if the child rings a bell, the experimenter college students), individuals start to offset the influence of
returns immediately but she gets only one treat. Various temptations by automatically avoiding temptation-relevant

CH08.indd 285 12/18/09 2:40:09 PM


286 Motivation

stimuli and by approaching stimuli related to the focal goal motivation (e.g., through self-rewards); in turn, the achieved
(assessed in terms of faster pushing and pulling responses energization helps to prevent failures from occurring.
to such stimuli, respectively). Research by Fishbach and Other theories are concerned with persistence in the
Zhang (2008) suggests another successful strategy in deal- face of negative feedback. According to Bandura (1997;
ing with temptations: keeping goal objects and objects Bandura & Locke, 2003), goals have no motivational conse-
related to temptations spatially apart from each other (e.g., quences per se. They only specify the conditions (standards)
for a person who has the goal of studying hard this may be that allow a positive or a negative self-evaluation. Meeting
textbooks vs. entertaining CDs). Fishbach and Zhang (2008) the standard leads to positive self-evaluation, whereas not
observed that the combined presence of goal and tempta- meeting the standard leads to negative self-evaluation. The
tion objects leads to a positive evaluation and preference individual is pushed by the negative self-evaluation asso-
for temptation objects, whereas separating them facilitates ciated with the discrepancy and pulled by the anticipated
positive evaluation and preference for goal objects. positive self-evaluation linked to closing the gap between
One of the most intractable problems in attaining goals the status quo and the standard. Accordingly, goals stimu-
is how to persist in the face of negative feedback. What are late effortful action only when people recognize that there
the strategies people can use to achieve such persistence? is still a discrepancy between the status quo and the stan-
First, people can step up efforts whenever difficulties are dard (e.g., when people encounter difficulties that need to
encountered, thus warding off failures. The energization be overcome). Bandura thus proposes obtaining frequent
theory of motivation (Brehm & Self, 1989; Wright, 1996) feedback as a powerful measure to stimulate goal pursuit.
proposes that the readiness to exert additional effort is However, this works only when people feel self-efficacious
directly determined by the perceived difficulty of a task; with respect to goal-directed actions; thus, they need to
as perceived difficulty increases, so does effort expendi- improve relevant skills and competences as well.
ture unless a task is perceived as unsolvable. Most impor- Carver and Scheier (1998) propose a different model for
tantly, there is a second limit to the increase of effort in reducing discrepancies in goal striving. Based on cyber-
response to heightened test difficulty: potential motiva- netic control theory, the central component of the analysis
tion. Potential motivation is fed by need-related variables is the negative feedback loop. Carver and Scheier (1998)
(i.e., strength of the related need or higher-order goal, the highlight the hierarchical structure of goal striving and
incentive value of the task, and the instrumentality of task assume cascading loops. Goal-directed behavior is regu-
completion for need satisfaction or goal attainment). If lated at the middle level (“do” goals), with actions at higher
potential motivation is low, people do not find it worth- levels (“be” goals) suspended until the individual becomes
while to expend more effort when an easy task becomes self-aware. Discovery of discrepancies on the “be” level or
more difficult. The upper limit of effort expenditure is low “do” level triggers lower-level goals or behaviors aimed
and quickly reached. If potential motivation is high, how- at reducing discrepancies in those levels, respectively. An
ever, an increase in difficulty is matched by investment of individual tries to close discrepancies only when outcome
effort up to high levels of difficulty. expectations are high.
Empirical tests of the theory have varied potential moti- Attaining a goal does not necessarily lead to a posi-
vation by offering high or low rewards for task completion, tive affective response, nor does detecting a discrepancy
making high rewards more or less likely, or describing the necessarily produce negative affect. Rather, the source of
task as diagnostic of an important scholastic skill or not positive or negative feelings in goal pursuit is the speed
(Gendolla & Richter, 2006). Effort mobilization is usually of progress. The intensity of these feelings is regulated
assessed by cardiovascular responses (i.e., heart rate and again in a negative feedback loop. If the speed meets a
systolic blood pressure). In general, low potential motiva- set criterion, positive feelings result; if it does not, then
tion curbs the linear relationship between task difficulty negative feelings are the outcome. Recent extensions of
and effort (i.e., participants with low motivation give up this control theory perspective on goal striving consider a
striving when task difficulty moves from medium to high). potential moderator variable: the effect of individual dif-
Energization theory has been used to understand the dif- ferences in behavioral approach and withdrawal systems.
ferences between men and women in effort on sex-typed One person may be sensitive for positive stimuli and not
tasks and to explore the effects of private versus pub- sensitive for negative stimuli (Carver, 2004); for another,
lic performance conditions on effort (Wright, Murray, the reverse may be true.
Storey, & Williams, 1997; Wright, Tunstall, Williams, It is important to note that both of these models for reduc-
Goodwin, & Harmon-Jones, 1995). The important mes- ing discrepancies down to standards (i.e., Bandura’s model
sage of these findings is that people can facilitate ener- and Carver and Scheier ’s model) construe goals as “cold”
gization in the face of difficulties by increasing potential mental representations of performance standards with no

CH08.indd 286 12/18/09 2:40:10 PM


Conscious Goal Pursuit 287

links to needs or incentives. This conceptualization of goals the interpersonal domain were most successful in solving a
makes it difficult to explain why motivation (see Brehm & subsequent analytical reasoning task.
Wright’s notion of potential motivation) moderates the rela- After negative feedback, the next potential threat
tion between task difficulty and effort. Moreover, according to effective goal striving is exhausting self-regulatory
to discrepancy theory, an increase in task difficulty should resources. Baumeister, Muraven, and Tice (2000) have
reduce efforts at task completion, because an increase in argued that the effortful and deliberate self-regulation of
task difficulty should lead to reduced self-efficacy and less thoughts, feelings, and actions uses up self-regulation
positive outcome expectations. As Brehm and Wright have resources. The waxing and waning of these resources is
repeatedly demonstrated, however, high potential motiva- understood by applying a muscle metaphor. Accordingly, it
tion makes it worthwhile for people to mobilize additional is predicted and found that people’s self-regulation perfor-
effort whenever heightened task difficulty threatens task mance is relatively weak when self-regulation has recently
completion. Finally, Carver and Scheier ’s theory assumes been exerted, even in a wholly different context (e.g.,
that positive discrepancies (i.e., moving toward a goal too Fischer, Greitemeyer, & Frey, 2008; Inzlicht & Gutsell,
fast) are reduced as readily as negative discrepancies (i.e., 2007; Schmeichel, 2007). Like athletes conserving their
moving toward a goal too slowly). However, from the per- strength after exercise, people who have just exerted self-
spective that goals represent a desired outcome, a person regulation hold back on exerting self-regulation when they
should be less motivated to reduce positive discrepancies anticipate further demands (DeWall, Baumeister, & Vohs,
than negative discrepancies (Gollwitzer & Rohloff, 1999). 2008).
Zhang, Fishbach, and Dhar (2008; see also Koo & However, just as regular exercise gradually improves
Fishbach, 2008) discovered that looking back on one’s past physical stamina, self-regulation exercises can gradually
striving for the goal can affect persistence in goal striv- make people chronically more able to sustain self-regulation
ing quite differently, depending on how past goal-directed exertions (e.g., Gailliot, Plant, Butz, & Baumeister, 2007).
activities are interpreted. When people are highly commit- This research offers specific guidance to people who want
ted to their goals, interpreting their past activities in terms to bolster persistence. First, they should try to conserve
of lack of progress leads to superior goal striving com- self-regulation resources when difficulties in persistent
pared with when they interpret their past activities in terms striving for a given goal are anticipated. This is true for
of having made good progress. However, when people are any prior goal striving, as well as for striving for the goal
only weakly committed to their goals, interpreting their at hand (Webb & Sheeran, 2003). Second, people should
past activities in terms of lack of progress discourages goal practice persistent striving wherever possible to improve
striving and interpretations in terms of having made good their stamina for it.
progress encourages it. Apparently, for noncommitted peo- Finally, several individual difference variables have
ple, the latter interpretation leads to stronger goal commit- been shown to affect persistent goal striving. Research on
ments, which in turn intensifies persistence. self-handicapping has found repeatedly that men create
Mental contrasting (i.e., contrasting fantasies about a more handicaps (e.g., reduction of effort) for themselves
desired future with reflections on negative reality) also than do women when it comes to striving for goals such as
facilitates effective coping with negative feedback. In making good grades in an examination. The most impor-
a series of studies, Oettingen and Kappes (2009) asked tant mediator of this gender effect turns out to be the rela-
participants to use mental contrasting regarding a feasi- tive value placed on effort: Women evaluate the reduction
ble and desirable future before working on tasks testing of effort more negatively than do men (McCrea, Hirt, &
interpersonal or academic competencies. The researchers Milner, 2008). Another individual difference relevant to
observed that goal-relevant negative feedback was more the exertion of effort is action orientation (a tendency
thoroughly encoded in the mental contrasting condition. toward decisiveness and initiative) versus state orientation
Moreover, despite strong and normative negative feed- (a tendency toward indecisiveness and hesitation; Kuhl &
back, participants kept a positive self-view regarding rel- Beckmann, 1994). Jostmann and Koole (2007) observed
evant competencies and skills and found optimistic causal no difference in performance between action- and state-
attributions for their setbacks. Findings by Houser-Marko oriented individuals on easy executive-functions tasks;
and Sheldon (2008) suggest that this pattern of dealing however, the action-oriented individuals outperformed
with negative feedback (open-mindedness on the low level state-oriented individuals when task demands increased,
but self-protectiveness on the high level) should facilitate because high task demands lead to more reduced cogni-
persistent goal striving. Indeed, Oettingen and Kappes tive functioning in state-oriented individuals. Finally,
(2009) observed in a final study that participants who were Park, Crocker, and Kiefer (2007) report that persistent
induced to mentally contrast a feasible desired future in striving for academic task goals depends on the level of

CH08.indd 287 12/18/09 2:40:10 PM


288 Motivation

academically contingent self-esteem. Low levels of 1963; Mischel, 1973; Ross & Nisbett, 1991). This research
contingent self-esteem lead to giving up on these goals, had historical continuity (see especially Bem, 1967) with
whereas high levels of contingent self-esteem lead to fight- the radical neobehaviorist tradition in which situational
ing back; the latter is accompanied by an increase in state variables were considered the exclusive causes of behavior
self-esteem and a strengthened desire to be perceived as (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000). The second important wellspring
academically competent. was the introduction of “dual process” models (e.g., Posner &
Snyder, 1975; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977), in which con-
scious mental processes were contrasted with “automatic,”
UNCONSCIOUS GOAL PURSUIT implicit, or unconscious ones. Research within this dual-
process framework amassed evidence of a second route
As outlined in the first section of this chapter, traditional to the selection and guidance of higher mental processes
models of human motivation have assumed an agentic, con- such as are involved in social judgment and behavior, a
scious self at the controls, making decisions about courses route that is triggered directly by relevant environmental
of action to take and then guiding behavior along those stimuli and that bypasses the consciousness bottleneck
lines (e.g., Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Bandura, 1977, 1986; (see reviews in Bargh, 2007; Chaiken & Trope, 1999).
Locke & Latham, 1990, 2002; Mischel, 1973; Mischel, Much of this evidence has come from studies using
Cantor, & Feldman, 1996). In line with the action-phase priming techniques. “Priming” refers to the passive, subtle,
model of goal pursuit (Gollwitzer, 1990; Heckhausen & and unobtrusive activation of relevant mental representa-
Gollwitzer, 1987), Bandura (2006) distinguishes four tions by external, environmental stimuli such that people
aspects of conscious human agency: People form inten- are not and usually do not become aware of the influence
tions that include action plans and strategies for realizing exerted by those stimuli (Bargh & Chartrand, 2000; Higgins,
them (intentionality), people anticipate likely outcomes 1996). This research has shown that one important reason
of prospective actions to guide and motivate their actions for the observed power of the situation in determining
(forethought), people are not only intenders and forethink- behavior is that the mere, passive perception of environ-
ers but also self-regulators in the sense of controlling the mental events directly triggers higher mental processes
effective execution of the courses of action taken (self- in the absence of any involvement by conscious, inten-
reactiveness), and finally, people are self-examiners of their tional processes (see reviews in Bargh & Ferguson, 2000;
own functioning in the sense of evaluating their pursuits Dijksterhuis, Chartrand, & Aarts, 2007; Higgins, 1996). In
(self-reflectiveness). Self theorists also have emphasized other words, much of the power of situational and contextual
the agentic (ego, volition) functions of the (conscious) self: stimuli comes from the direct, automatic, and unconscious
making choices and decisions, initiating and inhibiting effect they have over social behavior, an effect relatively
behavior, forming plans of action and carrying them out. independent from that of their dual-process partner,
Through carrying out these functions, theorists hold that conscious processes.
“the self exerts control over itself and over the external This emerging evidence that conscious, agentic self-
world” (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998, control is not necessary for the production and guidance of
p. 1252). purposive human behavior (and other higher mental pro-
According to the traditional model, then, human goal cesses) raises an important issue for motivation research:
pursuits are internally generated and put into motion by a What then is in charge when the conscious self is not? To
central executive or “self,” are guided to completion by address this question, an important new line of research
processes accessible to conscious awareness, and are fol- has focused on the mechanics of motivation in an effort
lowed by a conscious self-evaluation stage (see especially to specify the underlying cognitive, affective, and actional
Bandura, 1986). In the second section of this chapter, we systems that together generate and guide complex social
focus on an alternative route to human goal pursuit, one behavior (Aarts, Custers, & Marien, 2008; Bargh, 1990;
that does not require instigation and guidance by an agentic Higgins, 1997, 2000; Kruglanski et al., 2002). Much prog-
self and that can operate outside of conscious intention ress has been made on this front through reconceptualiz-
and awareness. This alternative path, which emphasizes ing motivations and goals in terms of cognitive structures
an unconscious mode of goal pursuit, has two important and then applying what is already known about how such
historical wellsprings within social psychology. First was structures become active—that is, from the perception
research demonstrating the power of external situational of external stimuli relevant to the goal (Bargh, 1990) or
variables in determining behavior, often shown to be more from internal activation spreading along associative net-
powerful than internal causes such as attitudes, personality, works (Kruglanski, 1996). Accordingly, this research has
or values (e.g., Bem, 1972; Darley & Latane, 1968; Milgram, been able to take advantage of concurrent advances in

CH08.indd 288 12/18/09 2:40:11 PM


Unconscious Goal Pursuit 289

knowledge regarding conceptual priming (Bargh, 2006; control? Based on evolutionary considerations, as well as
Higgins, 1996) and accessibility or likelihood of concept rising from the recent cognitive-motivation research evi-
activation (Bruner, 1957; Higgins, 1996), applying that set dence alluded to earlier, it seems that the level of proximal
of logic and principles to the case of goal representations. control over behavior and higher mental processes may be
This approach has been successful in illuminating (1) not the self but, rather, the currently active goal.
how motivational structures can operate without need This new approach can also help account for emerg-
of explicit activation and guidance by an agentic, con- ing evidence of the remarkable transformational power
scious self (see Bargh, 2005); (2) the internal operating of currently active goals over the rest of the cognitive and
dynamics of active goal structures (i.e., means–ends affective machinery of the mind. That is, the findings sug-
relations, Fishbach et al., 2006; McCulloch, Ferguson, gest the dominance of motivational processes—the active
Kawada, & Bargh, 2008; Shah et al., 2002); and (3) the goal—over even presumably hardwired, chronic, “auto-
dynamic relations between competing and complementary matic” processes. The active goal is not just in charge; it is
goal structures (Fishbach et al., 2003; McCulloch, Aarts, so much in charge that it is capable of temporarily “rewir-
Fujita, & Bargh, 2008). Perhaps as importantly, the new ing” the cognitive apparatus to better achieve its ends.
cognitive approach was built on the theoretical legacy of Chronic, automatic processes are found to be inhibited and
Kurt Lewin (e.g., 1926, 1935), affording a continuity with shut down if they conflict with the active goal—and are
the rich empirical base of classic motivational theory. found to be created where they did not previously exist, if
For example, the dynamics of goal pursuit first identified this automaticity helps to attain the goal (e.g., by increas-
by Lewin and his students Zeigarnik and Ovsiankina such ing the efficiency, speed, or reliability of the process). Such
as perseverance toward the goal, resumption of incomplete findings cause us to rethink the assumed “wired-in” nature
goals, and the “turnoff” effect of completed goals have all of automatic processes, long assumed in classic models of
been replicated by the modern research, and these dynamic skill acquisition to develop only gradually over time
effects have been recouched in knowledge-accessibility through frequency and consistency of use (see Bargh &
terms (see Bargh et al., 2001; Förster, Liberman, & Higgins, Chartrand, 1999; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977); they point
2005; Liberman, Foerster, & Higgins, 2007). In addition, instead to a highly plastic cognitive architecture that can
the general approach to the underlying cognitive, affec- flexibly adapt to best serve the attainment of the active
tive, and neural mechanics of motivation is much in the goal (see especially Hassin, 2005). By revealing such
Lewinian spirit, as Lewin himself (1935, 1951) strove to heretofore unknown features of the human motivational
describe motivational dynamics in terms of the physical system, the new social cognitive approach to motivation is
science of his day (e.g., “fields of forces,” vectors, and shown to be more than merely a redescription of past moti-
graph theory)—it is just that today the underlying science vational theory and variables in the language of cognitive
to which these motivational concepts are pinned is not psychology—it is causing us to rethink the fundamental
19th-century physics but 21st-century cognitive science nature of cognitive and affective processes.
and neuroscience.
The remainder of this chapter comes to grips with the The Primacy of the Unconscious
emerging fact that the conscious, agentic self plays less of
a role in the production of human behavior and the guid- The primacy of conscious thought in the ways people
ance of the other higher mental processes than traditional historically have thought about the mind is illustrated
models have assumed. It accounts for and explains recent today in the words we use to describe other kinds of
findings showing that the same outcomes are attained processes—all modifications or qualifications of the starting
when goals operate entirely unconsciously as when they point, “conscious” (i.e., “unconscious,” “preconscious,”
are pursued consciously and that they have the same phe- and “subconscious”). When considering the causal forces
nomenal qualities during operation as originally noted by underlying one’s mental life and overt behavior, one’s
Lewin—findings blatantly inconsistent with models that introspections naturally focus exclusively on those influ-
place the agentic, conscious self exclusively at the helm. ences of which one is consciously aware, and these serve
Several other lines of new findings appear to run coun- as a first-pass or starting point for theoretical assumptions
ter to the notion of an agentic self in control, and these about the phenomenon in question (see Bargh, 1997). This
may be better accounted for by the present approach. Often natural tendency characterized the assumptions of many of
the effect of an active goal on behavior and judgment is the most influential philosophers of the past (most notably
shown to produce effects that are unwanted or undesirable Descartes and John Locke, but with important exceptions
at the level of the individual person. How can this be if such as Aristotle, Spinoza, and Schopenhauer; see Arendt,
the agentic self, with its associated values and beliefs, is in 1978; Gottlieb, 2000) just as it operates in us today.

CH08.indd 289 12/18/09 2:40:11 PM


290 Motivation

However, scientific psychology learned a century ago to more” subliminal presentation conditions, even though both
be skeptical of the relatively superficial answers provided were “subliminal.”
by introspection (e.g., Watson, 1912; also Nisbett & Wilson, The study by Ruys and Stapel (2008) provides a dra-
1977). Therefore, it does not appear to be solely a fond- matic empirical confirmation that the reason the uncon-
ness for tradition or historical inertia that accounts for the scious appears “dumb” (when defined in terms of responses
continued assumption of largely conscious control over to subliminal stimuli) is that subliminal stimuli are weak
the higher mental processes. Accordingly, there seem to by definition and thus have only weak effects—increasing
be additional reasons for the continuing presumption of their intensity while still keeping them subliminal increases
conscious primacy, and one of the more important of these the strength and sophistication of their effects. The reason
is the rather narrow and restricted view of unconscious for the apparent dumbness of the unconscious revealed by
processes held by mainstream contemporary cognitive subliminal stimulation studies is therefore not the actual
science. information processing powers of the unconscious itself but
In cognitive psychology, unconscious information the weakness of the stimuli used to discover those powers.
processing has been equated with subliminal informa- Second, dividing stimuli into dichotomous classes of sub-
tion processing—to wit, how good is the mind at extract- liminal versus supraliminal is too simplistic an approach
ing meaning from stimuli the presence of which one is not even to how subliminal stimuli are processed, because dif-
consciously aware (e.g., Greenwald, Klinger, & Schuh, ferent outcomes are produced for more versus less intense
1995)? A special issue of the American Psychologist stimulus presentations within the set of subliminal stimuli.
(Loftus & Klinger, 1992) once posed the question, “Is the Therefore, it is impossible to draw unambiguous conclu-
unconscious smart or dumb?” Because subliminal-strength sions regarding the capabilities of unconscious information
stimuli are by definition relatively weak and of low inten- processing from studies that merely compare “subliminal”
sity, the mental processes they drive are necessarily mini- stimuli with those of which the participant is consciously
mal and unsophisticated—associationist only with minimal aware.
flexibility. No surprise then that the consensus reached by Importantly as well, the definition of the unconscious
the contributors and issue editors was that the unconscious in terms of processing subliminal-strength stimuli was
was rather dumb, capable only of highly routinized activi- not the original, historic one. In On the Origin of Species,
ties and of doing little without the aid of consciousness Darwin (1859) used the term to refer to “unconscious
(Loftus & Klinger, 1992). selection” processes in nature, contrasting them with the
From the cognitive psychology perspective that uncon- intentional and deliberate selection long engaged in by
scious equals responses to subliminal stimuli (at least farmers and animal breeders to develop better strains of
operationally), it is understandable that one might find it corn, fatter cows, and woollier sheep. Freud as well (see
difficult to accept that such a dumb system could be respon- Brill, 1938; Goldsmith, 1934) used the term to refer to
sible for the production of so much in the way of complex, behavior and ideation that were not consciously intended
sophisticated judgmental and behavioral phenomena. But or caused—for example, “Freudian slips” and nearly
this “subliminal” definition is historically inaccurate at best all the examples Freud gives in The Psychopathology of
and potentially misleading at worst. The pioneering work Everyday Life (1901/1914) involve unintended behavior,
of Werner (1956) on perceptual microgenesis revealed that the source or cause of which was unknown to the individ-
within the set of subliminally presented stimuli there could ual performing the actions (often Freud himself). For both
be variations in the extent and sophistication of the mental Darwin and Freud, then, the term “unconscious” referred
responses obtained, as a function of the duration or inten- to the unintentional nature of the behavior or process, and the
sity of the stimulus presentation. More recently, Ruys and concomitant lack of awareness was not of the stimuli that
Stapel (2008) reintroduced this concept of microgenesis as provoked the behavior but of the influence or consequences
an approach to the issue of “unconscious emotion.” They of those stimuli (see also Bargh, 1992).
showed that “superquick” subliminal presentations of This expanded and enhanced view of the unconscious
emotional stimuli produced only global mood and valence is also more compatible with theory and evidence in the
effects on responses, but that “quick” (still subliminal but field of evolutionary biology than is the subliminal-only
longer in duration than “superquick”) presentations pro- view of cognitive psychology. As did Darwin and Freud,
duced emotion-specific responses (e.g., specific anger or evolutionary biologists also think of the unconscious more
disgust reactions, instead of merely a global negative mood). in terms of its unintentional rather than its unaware qual-
In other words, dramatically different conclusions about ity. In his seminal book The Selfish Gene, Dawkins (1976)
the sophistication of unconscious emotion processes would noted the countless awe-inspiring and intelligent designs in
have been drawn from their subliminal versus their “even nature that arose merely through “blind” natural selection

CH08.indd 290 12/18/09 2:40:12 PM


Unconscious Goal Pursuit 291

processes. He called nature the “blind watchmaker, the Jost et al. (2008) also note how operation of this
unconscious watchmaker” because no conscious, inten- unconscious system justification motive may often pro-
tional guiding hand produced these intelligent designs (see duce effects that violate social norms, such as stereotypical
also Dennett, 1991, 1995). beliefs and discrimination against minority and low-status
It is this original, “unintentional” definition that has groups, “and thus may interfere with social desirability
guided contemporary social-psychological research on concerns at a conscious level” (p. 596). In other words, the
unconscious phenomena over the past 30 years, with its unconscious system justification motive is posited to pro-
focus on the effects of mental processes of which the indi- duce outcomes that are negative and unwanted at the level
vidual is unaware, not on the effects of stimuli of which of the individual person—an analysis entirely consistent
one is unaware. The seminal article of Nisbett and Wilson with the position advanced here, that the locus of control
(1977) first posed the question “To what extent are people over behavior and judgment is not the conscious self as
aware of and able to report on the true causes of their much as it is the currently active goal.
behavior?” Since then, there has been much research on Indeed, among the original clues to the existence of
the extent to which people are unaware of the important unconsciously motivated social behavior were highly pub-
influences on their judgments and decisions and of actual licized cases of political corruption (examples in Bargh &
causal reasons underlying their behavior. This research Raymond, 1995) and U.S. Supreme Court cases of sexual
effort has produced widespread agreement that higher harassment (discussed in Fitzgerald, 1993), in which the
mental processes such as are involved in social judgment, perception of potential conflict of interest or of misuse
social behavior, and goal pursuit are driven by uncon- of power was strong in the media and public but not to
scious, as well as conscious, processes (see Dijksterhuis, the perpetrators themselves, who seemed genuinely unable
this volume). For example, the 1999 collection of dual pro- to appreciate what all the fuss was about. The behavior
cess models in social psychology by Chaiken and Trope seemed perplexing to many because often its detection
includes 28 different models, applying the dual-process could easily have been avoided, yet the offending behavior
notion to a range of empirical phenomena. was conducted in full public view.
What a difference this change in operational definition Indeed, the blatant nature of many of these cases com-
makes! If we shift the operational definition of the uncon- bined with the strong protestations of innocence—or, more
scious from the processing of stimuli of which one is not to the present point, of a seeming lack of awareness of hav-
aware to the influences or effects of stimulus processing of ing done anything wrong—led to the prediction that at least
which one is not aware, suddenly the power and scope some of the harassment or corrupt behavior was driven by
of the unconscious in daily life become apparent. Defining unconscious motives (for sex, wealth, etc.), activated
the unconscious in terms of the first leads directly to the by the situation of having personal power, and then oper-
conclusion that it is dumb as dirt (Loftus & Klinger, 1992); ating without the person’s conscious awareness. Bargh,
defining it in terms of the second instead reveals it to be Raymond, Pryor, and Strack (1995) then showed empiri-
highly intelligent and adaptive. cally, through priming techniques, that power did auto-
Moreover, just as had Freud in Psychopathology (1914), matically trigger the sex motive in those participants most
contemporary social-psychological theorists are finding likely to possess sexual harassment tendencies. Attraction
the concept of unconscious motivation useful in explain- toward a female confederate was significantly higher for
ing why people behave in ways that seem to run against these participants when power had been primed, com-
their self-interest and self-values. Jost, Pietrzak, Liviaton, pared with when it had not been, and they were unaware of
Mandisodza, and Napier (2008) did so in the case of sys- the influence of power over their attraction. Here, as in the
tem justification effects, in which the current status quo case of system justification effects, the unconscious opera-
regarding political power and division of resources is tion of motives (triggered by the power-related features of
perceived as fair and legitimate, even among those who the situation) produced behavioral outcomes that were pre-
are low status and for whom the system operates against sumably unwanted and undesired at the level of the indi-
their self-interests (see also Frank, 2004). Jost et al. (2008) vidual self-concept.
appeal to the operation of an unconscious system justifica- If a goal is capable of operating independently of any
tion motive to help account for these “relatively puzzling conscious intention or awareness of its operation, then
cases of conservatism, right-wing allegiance, and outgroup active goals and conscious intentions cannot be the same
favoritism among members of low-status groups,” which things (Bargh, 1990). This is most obvious in the case of
become more understandable if people are “not even aware addictions, in which there is a strong goal (need state)
of the extent to which they are privileging the status quo to smoke the cigarette, down the drink, or take the pill,
and resisting change” (p. 596). even when the addict knows doing so is against his or her

CH08.indd 291 12/18/09 2:40:12 PM


292 Motivation

long-term interests and no longer wants (at the level of assumption of conscious primacy is not nearly as prevalent
the self) to engage in this behavior (e.g., Baker, Piper, as in psychology. Complex and intelligent design in living
McCarthy, Majeskie, & Fiore, 2004; Loewenstein, 1996). things is assumed to be driven not by conscious processes
Other examples include encapsulated bodily systems such on the part of the plant or animal but instead by “blindly”
as breathing that insist on operation even when doing so, as adaptive processes that accrued through natural selection
when underwater, is fatal to the individual (see Morsella, (Dennett, 1995).
2005, for this and further examples). This is not to say that human consciousness plays no
While they are dramatic and extreme, these cases do important causal or moderating role over the operation of
illustrate the general principle that the individual person or the higher mental processes. It is rather that this conscious
self comprises many different, often conflicting, motives capacity is not necessary to achieve the sophisticated,
and goals—for example, self-interest versus empathic adaptive, and intelligent behavioral guidance demonstrated
concern for others, short-term pleasure versus long-term in the emerging literature on unconscious processes. As
health and happiness, and competition versus coopera- Dawkins (1976) contended, unconscious processes are
tion with others (Elster,1990; Miller, 1999; Sen, 1978; see smart and adaptive throughout the living world, and since
Mansbridge, 1990). As a consequence, it is often the case his writing social cognition research has shown that this
that the operation of one goal in pursuit of its own agenda principle extends to humans, producing a stream of find-
produces outcomes unwanted by other aspects of the self ings regarding complex human judgmental, motiva-
system. The important point here is the apparent absence of tional, and behavioral phenomena that operate outside of
any coherent “self” guiding operations—the coherence awareness.
and control are one step lower in the hierarchy, at the level Because these findings did not make sense given the
of the currently active goal. dumb unconscious perspective of the cognitive science
That the active goal is in charge (of selective attention, mainstream (i.e., how could a processing system so
evaluation, and behavior) is most obvious in the case of dumb accomplish so much in the way of adaptive self-
unconscious goal pursuit, in which goals are shown to regulation?), one had to look outside of psychology to
operate independently of conscious awareness. Cognitive understand them and their implications for the human
neuroscience research has supported the structural disso- mind. As it turns out, when placed in the broader context
ciation that is strongly implied by the unconscious goal of the natural sciences, especially evolutionary biology,
pursuit evidence, finding that the operation of a goal pro- the widespread discoveries of sophisticated unconscious
gram and one’s awareness of its operation are located in higher mental processes not only make more sense but turn
distinct, separate anatomical structures within the frontal out to have been predicted on a priori grounds (Dawkins,
cortex. Conscious intentions appear to be represented in 1976; Dennett, 1991, 1995).
the prefrontal and premotor cortex, yet it is the parietal First, consciousness, or the conscious mode of thought,
cortex that houses the representation used to guide action was a late development in hominid evolution (Corballis,
(Frith, Blakemore, & Wolpert, 2000)—making it struc- 2007; Deacon, 1997; Dennett, 1991; Donald, 1991). Among
turally possible to have one (e.g., goal operation) in the the evidence supporting this conclusion is anthropological
absence of the other (e.g., awareness of pursuing that data on skull (brain) sizes and evidence of tool use. Brain
goal). Behaviorally, the phenomenon of “environmental size remained constant after the hominid divergence from
dependency syndrome” (Lhermitte, 1983, 1986) is addi- the chimpanzee genetic line but suddenly began to expand;
tional evidence of the same dissociation. In this disorder, at the same time, evidence for tool use and primitive
the behavior of patients with lesions in the same region forms of speech and communication appeared for the first
of the frontal lobes is found to be almost entirely at the time. Because millions of years of hominid development
mercy of situational cues—gardening in a public park (for preceded the advent of consciousness, some other (i.e.,
hours) after seeing a rake, giving medical examinations unconscious) system must have guided hominid behav-
to others in a doctor ’s office—with the patients oblivious to ioral responses over those eons of time.
the unusual nature of their behavior. According to this logic, one should be able to find
evidence in humans of these original unconscious mech-
The Natural Unconscious of Evolutionary Biology anisms underlying, and guiding, even the higher men-
The preceding analysis suggests that it may be beneficial to tal processes of judgment, social behavior, and goal
depart from the traditional model of motivational science, pursuit. And evidence of these unconscious mechanisms
with its agentic, autonomous, and conscious self argued to has indeed accrued over the past quarter century: auto-
be in control of executive processes and goal pursuits. In matic evaluations and preferences, automatic influences
the rest of the natural sciences, especially neurobiology, the of the perceived environment on behavioral responses, and

CH08.indd 292 12/18/09 2:40:13 PM


Unconscious Goal Pursuit 293

an automatic mode of goal pursuit (see reviews in Bargh, what is important to do or attend to (Damasio, 1996;
2007; Bargh & Ferguson, 2000). This evidence supports Schwarz & Clore, 1996). These “guides” do not arise out
the present evolutionary approach to motivation, in which of thin air, however, as our modern preferences are derived
unconscious processes are considered primary (see also from those that served adaptive ends in the past. For
Dijksterhuis, this volume; Neuberg et al., volume 2). example, our strong human preference for sweet and fatty
foods evolved because of their high energy value during
Unconscious Behavioral Guidance Systems those eons of time in which they were in relatively short
A second prediction from the preceding evolutionary supply, yet we find it hard to resist them today even though
considerations is that each of the varieties of automatic they are available now in relatively ample quantities and we
or unconscious processes discovered by recent research are now aware of the long-term costs associated with eating
should be found to be directly connected to overt actional too much of them (e.g., Brownell & Horgen, 2004; Rozin &
response tendencies; that is, to produce behavioral output Geier, 2007).
entirely through unconscious means. This prediction fol- In general, default preferences as to what is good and
lows from the principle that the forces of natural selection what is bad in our environment represent hard-earned
can only operate on overt behavior, not internal states such knowledge gained during our long-term evolutionary
as thoughts or feelings (Mayr, 1976). If it turned out that past. These evolved preferences are fed upward as a start-
these automatic processes were found not to be directly ing point, appearing as a priori knowledge, the source of
connected to behavioral responses, this would falsify the which we are unaware (Dennett, 1995). Donald Campbell
argument that the varieties of automatic processes discov- (1974), a champion of the evolutionary approach to epis-
ered by social cognition research are manifestations of an temology, called these “shortcut processes” because they
unconscious behavioral guidance system that existed before spare us from having to figure out, each of us individually
the advent of consciousness. Again, however, the evidence from scratch, which are the good and helpful things and
(reviewed later) has come in on the side of unconscious which are the dangerous.
primacy. Each of the main forms of automatic processing Recent evidence has supported the assertion that con-
relevant to social psychology—on evaluation, perception scious preferences are based on prior unconscious prefer-
and categorization, social behavior, and goal pursuit—have ences. In a study of voters whose explicit, self-reported
been shown to directly (i.e., without need of conscious preference in the 2008 U.S. presidential election was
involvement) produce behavioral response tendencies “undecided,” Galdi, Arcuri, and Gawronski (2008) showed
(Bargh, 1997; Bargh & Morsella, 2009). that the eventual conscious preference of these undecided
voters could be predicted from measures of their automatic
Preferences or unconscious attitudes toward the candidates. That is,
First, evolutionary forces have shaped our tendencies for undecided voters, the favorability of automatic asso-
to approach or avoid certain aspects of our environment. ciations to the candidate at time 1 predicted subsequent
Such approach and withdrawal responses are found through- conscious attitudes at time 2. For voters who had already
out the animal kingdom; even single-celled paramecia have made up their minds, only consciously expressed attitudes
them (Schneirla, 1959). Cacioppo, Gardner, and Berntson at time 1 predicted conscious attitudes at time 2 (nearly
(1997) concluded that humans have a general default perfectly); automatic associations to the candidate at
tendency to approach rather than withdraw from stimuli, time 1 were unrelated to consciously expressed attitudes
which encourages exploratory behavior and thus gain of at time 2.
useful information about the environment. However, this In another demonstration of this phenomenon,
has the downside of putting the individual in the vicinity Duckworth, Bargh, Garcia, and Chaiken (2002) found that
of negative stimuli and thus at risk. The adaptive solution the automatic preferences toward novel attitude objects
achieved by our species is that negative stimuli when pres- measured in one group predicted the explicit self-reported
ent elicit a stronger, withdrawal response, dominating and preferences toward those same stimuli in another group,
inhibiting approach tendencies. Thus, “with both a positiv- even though in the unconscious condition the objects were
ity offset and a negativity bias [humans] enjoy the benefits presented for only 250 milliseconds, whereas participants
of exploratory behavior as well as the self-preservative in the conscious condition took on average about 8 full
benefits of a predisposition to avoid or withdraw from seconds to give their preference.
threatening events” (Cacioppo et al., 1997, p. 13). Consistent with the hypothesis that these preferences
Evolutionary forces have helped to shape a person’s spe- are part of an unconscious behavioral guidance system,
cific preferences as well. People are often guided by “feel- attitudes toward a wide variety of objects and events
ings,” “intuitions,” and “gut reactions,” which prioritize were found to become automatically activated by the

CH08.indd 293 12/18/09 2:40:13 PM


294 Motivation

mere presence of the attitude object in the environment (see uncertainty exists as to the situational norms and appropriate
reviews in Fazio, 2001; Ferguson, 2007). These automatic responses (e.g., Asch, 1961; Meltzoff, 2002; Tomasello,
preferences are activated immediately, do not require the Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll, 2005). Asch (1961), for
conscious intention to evaluate stimuli (Bargh, Chaiken, instance, argued that much conformity behavior was driven
Govender, & Pratto, 1992), and occur even for novel by the cuing or informational value of the perceived behav-
stimuli such as nonrepresentational (i.e., abstract) art and ior of others. As a default option or starting point for your
spoken nonsense words (Duckworth et al., 2002). From own behavior, “blindly” or unconsciously adopting what
the accumulating evidence then, it appears that all incom- others around you are doing makes good adaptive sense,
ing stimuli elicit an initial positive or negative evaluation, especially in new situations and with strangers.
even those stimuli with which one has no prior conscious In many species, the perception of the behavior of
experience. conspecifics (fellow members of the same species) auto-
Under the present argument that the unconscious matically causes the animal to behave in the same way,
evolved as a behavioral guidance system, a source of adap- producing precise coordination of movement within
tive and appropriate actional impulses, these unconsciously the group, as in schools of fish or herds of antelope (see
activated preferences should be found to be directly con- Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001). Humans possess this same
nected to behavioral mechanisms. Several studies have tendency, with the behavior of others automatically creat-
now established this connection: Immediate and unin- ing behavioral impulses or tendencies to act in the same
tended evaluation processes are directly linked to approach way. Developmental researchers have highlighted this
and avoidance behavioral predispositions. Chen and Bargh perception–behavior link as an important, innate basis
(1999; see also Kawakami, Dovidio, Moll, Hermsen, & of imitation and mimicry and thus of vicarious learning of
Russin, 2000; Neumann, Förster, & Strack, 2003) showed appropriate behavioral responses to events by merely wit-
that participants are faster to make approach movements of nessing the behavior of one’s adult caretakers and older
the arm (pulling a lever toward oneself) when responding to children (Meltzoff, 2002; Tomasello et al., 2005). And
positive attitude objects and faster to make avoidance cognitive neuroscience research has confirmed the exis-
movements (pushing the lever away) when responding to tence of a direct anatomical connection between perceptual
negative attitude objects. This was true even though their and behavioral representations of the same actions with
conscious task in the experiment was not to evaluate the the discovery of “mirror neurons” in the premotor cortex,
objects but merely to “knock off the screen” the names of which become active both when one perceives a given type
these objects as soon as they appeared. of action by another person and when one engages in that
Thus, humans automatically evaluate environmental action (see Frith & Wolpert, 2003).
stimuli as either good or bad and further possess corre- Thus, what other people are doing in the current situ-
sponding muscular, behavioral tendencies to approach or ation and environment is another important input to the
to withdraw from those stimuli, without the involvement unconscious behavior guidance system, with the perceived
of conscious awareness or intent. This is compelling evi- behavior of others directly and unconsciously creating ten-
dence in support of the hypothesis that automatic forms of dencies to act in that same way. We naturally take on the
social cognition arose through natural selection pro- physical postures and gestures of others without realizing
cesses to afford humans adaptive, unconscious guidance it (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999), but the perception–behavior
of behavioral responses to the environment (Bargh & link extends to include the content of more complex,
Morsella, 2009). Again, the discovery of automatic atti- abstract representations—such as trait concepts and social
tude activation and its automatic connection to behavioral stereotypes—that are automatically activated during percep-
tendencies—both of which were unheard of 30 years ago— tual activity (see reviews in Chartrand, Maddux, & Lakin,
was surprising at the time from the perspective that actions 2005; Dijksterhuis et al., 2007; Dijksterhuis & Bargh,
and behavior are always a function of conscious intent and 2001). For example, priming the concept of “elderly”
guidance (e.g., Bandura, 1986; Locke & Latham, 2002) but causes college students both to move more slowly
is unsurprising from the unconscious-first perspective of while leaving the experimental session (Bargh, Chen, &
evolutionary biology (Bargh & Morsella, 2008; Dawkins, Burrows, 1996) and to have poorer memory for the fea-
1976). tures of a room they just left (Dijksterhuis, Aarts, Bargh, &
van Knippenberg, 2000)—both effects predicted from the
Social Perception content of the elderly stereotype activated through percep-
Theorists have long noted that what other people are doing tual activity (i.e., priming).
within a situation is important information as to what we The evolved, innate basis of these ubiquitous perceptual
ourselves should be doing, especially when ambiguity or priming effects on behavior is revealed by their presence

CH08.indd 294 12/18/09 2:40:14 PM


Unconscious Goal Pursuit 295

soon after birth, underpinning the infant’s imitative the innate, evolved nature of contextual priming influences
abilities. In a review of 25 years of infant imitation on behavior (as well as the rapidity and thoroughness with
research, Meltzoff (2002) concluded that young children which young children absorb the attitudes and beliefs of
learn much about how to behave by mere passive imita- their culture).
tion of fellow children and their adult caretakers: infants
Goals and Motivations
in particular are open to such imitative tendencies, having
not yet developed cognitive control structures to suppress The third component of the unconscious behavior guidance
or inhibit them. Further evidence of the adaptive nature of system is purposive and motivational. Purposive behavior
these tendencies comes from the work on automatic mim- and goal pursuits are widespread in the world of living
icry tendencies in humans by Chartrand and colleagues things (e.g., Mayr, 1976); thus, in evolutionary biology,
(e.g., Chartrand et al., 2005); those studies have found not goal pursuit is not considered to require human conscious-
only that people do tend to adopt the physical behavior ness or its equivalent; for most organisms, goal-directed
(posture, facial gestures, and arm and hand movements) behavior is achieved entirely through unconscious means
of strangers with whom they interact, without intending (Dawkins, 1976). Theory and research in the field of evo-
to or being aware they are doing so, but also that this lutionary psychology has held that evolutionarily relevant
unconscious imitation tends to increase liking and bonding goals are of unconscious origin, because reproduction and
among individuals—serving as a kind of natural “social survival were paramount concerns to hominids for eons of
glue” for new acquaintances and group members. time before the development of consciousness (Barrett &
Baillargeon (2008) has argued that priming is itself Kurzban, 2006; Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Donald, 2001;
an adaptation, as it helps to cue the very young child to Neuberg et al., volume 2).
selectively attend to the currently important features and While theorists differ somewhat as to the set of evolved
dimensions of the world (see Higgins & Bargh, 1987, for goals they posit, general consensus exists that self-protection
a similar functional argument regarding priming in the (including avoidance of disease and contamination) and
case of adults). Baillargeon’s research has shown that even mating are fundamental human motives, along with a need
8-month-old infants show priming effects. According to to understand or comprehend environmental events and to
Piaget (1955), this is much too young to possess the con- belong to social groups and have supportive social relation-
cept of object permanence. Indeed, in a control condition, ships (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Fiske, 2004; Haidt,
8-month-olds do not act surprised when a doll is placed in 2001; Neuberg, Kenrick, Maner, & Schaller, 2004).
a box and a differently colored version of the same doll Moreover, an emerging domain of research has shown that
then emerges from that box. However, if one first shows these evolved goals can become active (i.e., primed) and
these infants a collection of different-colored dolls, thus then operate entirely unconsciously, outside of conscious
cuing (priming) “color” as a relevant doll dimension for awareness or intention, to influence selective attention,
the moment, the infants suddenly show evidence of object judgment, and social behavior in present-day contexts
permanence, as now they are surprised when a doll of a (e.g., Haidt, 2001; Huang & Bargh, 2008; Neuberg et al.,
different color emerges from the same box. 2004; Schaller, Park, & Faulkner, 2003).
Ambady, Shih, Kim, and Pittinsky (2001) have shown The goal-priming literature (for review see Dijksterhuis
stereotype-priming effects on the behavior of children as et al., 2007) has shown that goals can be activated with-
young as 5 years. The cultural stereotype of Asian Americans out the individual knowing about or intending it—either
is that they are superior to other social groups in mathe- through subliminal presentation of goal-relevant stimuli
matical ability, but the cultural stereotype of girls and or through subtle and unobtrusive supraliminal presenta-
women is that they are inferior to men in this regard. tion. Various environmental triggers have been demon-
Ambady et al. (2001) first primed Asian American pre- strated: not only verbal stimuli semantically related to the
school children with drawings emphasizing either their goal (as in many studies) but also material objects such as
Asian American identity or their gender and then gave backpacks and briefcases (Kay, Wheeler, Bargh, & Ross,
them an age-appropriate math test to work on. When their 2004), scents such as cleaning fluids (Holland, Hendriks, &
Asian American identity had been primed, Asian American Aarts, 2005), power-related features of a situation such as
girls outperformed the other children on the test, but when a professor ’s desk chair (Chen, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 2001),
their female identity had been primed, they underper- and the names of significant others (Fitzsimons & Bargh,
formed relative to the other children (thus showing evi- 2003; Shah, 2003).
dence of “stereotype threat” on their math performance). Moreover, a range of goals have been studied and shown
That such cultural-stereotype priming effects are obtained capable of unconscious operation: information processing
in children as young as 5 years of age is further evidence of goals such as impression formation (Chartrand & Bargh,

CH08.indd 295 12/18/09 2:40:14 PM


296 Motivation

1996; McCulloch, Ferguson, et al., 2008), achievement and Goals for types of interpersonal behavior, as well as
task performance goals (Bargh et al., 2001; Hassin, 2005), information processing goals, are capable of unconscious
and interpersonal goals such as helping and cooperation operation. In one study, unobtrusive priming of the goal of
(Bargh et al., 2001; Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2003). These cooperation caused participants playing the role of a fish-
studies have shown further that once activated outside the ing company to put more fish back into a lake to replenish
person’s knowledge, these goals operate autonomously, the fish population (compared with a nonprimed control
without any conscious guidance, to direct cognition and condition), the same effect as when, in the same study,
behavior toward the desired end-state (see reviews in Bargh, another group of participants were explicitly instructed to
2005; Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Chartrand & Bargh, 2002; cooperate (Bargh et al., 2001). Although the goal-priming
Dijksterhuis et al., 2007; Ferguson, Hassin, & Bargh, 2008; manipulation produced the same effect on behavior as did
Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2004). conscious (explicitly instructed) pursuit of the same goal,
only in the latter case were participants aware of having the
goal to cooperate. Postexperimental reports of how coop-
Similarity of Conscious and Unconscious
erative they had just been on the task were significantly
Goal Pursuits
correlated with actual degree of cooperation in the con-
From the assumption of the historical primacy of uncon- scious goal pursuit condition but were uncorrelated with
scious motivational structures, a further prediction can actual behavior in the unconscious goal pursuit condition.
be made. Evolution is an incremental process; new pro- The goal-primed participants thus gave every appearance
cesses and abilities are not created de novo but are cob- of pursuing the cooperation goal without knowing they
bled onto and use existing processes to the extent possible were doing so.
(see Allman, 2000; Bargh & Morsella, 2008). Thus, it is Second, regarding the prediction of similar underlying
likely that when conscious processing capabilities evolved processes, not only do unconsciously operating goals
they used relevant existing unconscious mental structures; produce the same outcome as when consciously pursued
in the case of goal pursuit, the assumption would be that but they do so following the same processing stages.
conscious forms of goal pursuit use preexisting uncon- McCulloch, Ferguson, et al. (2008) have shown this in the
scious motivational processes and brain structures. This case of the impression formation goal. Compared with a
leads to the prediction not only that one should observe nonprimed control group, priming the impression forma-
a high degree of similarity in the outcomes of conscious tion goal caused participants (1) to be faster to encode
and unconscious pursuit of the same goal but also that the behaviors in trait-categorical terms, (2) to be more likely to
two modes should share phenomenal qualities of the goal form associations between behaviors, and (3) to notice and
pursuit process—such qualities as persistence in the face remember impression-inconsistent behaviors, all known
of obstacles, resumption and completion of interrupted subprocesses of conscious impression formation (e.g.,
goal pursuits, self-evaluation following the goal pursuit Hamilton, Katz, & Leirer, 1980; Srull & Wyer, 1989).
attempt, and temporary inhibition of the goal following Priming a goal, therefore, puts the means to attain the goal
the attempt (see Atkinson & Birch, 1970; Heckhausen, (i.e., component subgoals) into active operation as well
1991; Lewin, 1935). Moreover, at the level of neurophysi- (see Shah & Kruglanski, 2003, for the reverse means-to-
ology, the unconscious-primacy hypothesis would predict goal priming effect).
that the observed similarities should extend to the use of Once active, the goal directs one’s attention toward
the same underlying brain regions. some (i.e., goal-relevant) stimuli and away from others;
Regarding the prediction of similar outcomes of the world is filtered through the goal’s “eyes.” The active
goal pursuit, recent studies have shown that unconscious goal goal’s effect on selective attention has long been known
pursuit produces the same outcomes as when that goal is in the case of consciously pursued goals (Anderson &
pursued consciously (reviews in Dijksterhuis et al., 2007; Pichert, 1978; Bruner, 1957; Hastie & Park, 1986), but
Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2004). The goal concept, once acti- recent research shows this effect occurs in unconscious
vated without the participant’s awareness, operates over goal pursuit as well (Chartrand & Bargh, 1996, study 2;
extended periods (also without the person’s conscious Maner et al., 2005; McCulloch, Ferguson, et al., 2008;
intent or monitoring) to guide thought or behavior toward Neuberg et al., 2004). For example, an unconscious impres-
the goal. For instance, primed, unconscious goals to form sion formation goal causes greater selective attention to
an impression of a target person operate without the par- behavioral information inconsistent with the target’s gen-
ticipant’s knowledge and compute the evaluation as if the eral pattern of behavior (Chartrand & Bargh, 1996, study
person consciously and intentionally had the goal to form 2; McCulloch, Ferguson, et al., 2008), and an uncon-
an impression (Chartrand & Bargh, 1996). scious mating goal drives greater selective attention to the

CH08.indd 296 12/18/09 2:40:15 PM


Unconscious Goal Pursuit 297

potential romantic partners shown on a videotape (Maner More direct evidence of unconscious changes in goal
et al., 2005; Neuberg et al., 2004). strength has emerged from cognitive neuroscience studies
Third, regarding the prediction that the same phenom- of the brain regions involved in motivated behavior. This
enal qualities should occur during goal pursuit, whether it research also bears on the earlier prediction that conscious
be unconscious or conscious, research has supported this and unconscious goal pursuits use the same underlying
as well. Unconscious goal pursuit is shown to possess the brain regions and structures. In a functional magnetic reso-
same phenomenal qualities previously demonstrated and nance imaging study of motivation-relevant brain regions,
ascribed to conscious, deliberate goal pursuit (Bandura, Pessiglione et al. (2007) showed that subliminal reward
1977, 1986; Gollwitzer & Moskowitz, 1996; Heckhausen, cues moderated the amount of effort participants gave on
1991; Lewin, 1926). These include persistence in the face a handgrip squeezing task; the higher the value of a coin
of obstacles, resumption of interrupted goal pursuits in the flashed subliminally at the beginning of the trial, the more
face of intrinsically more attractive activities, and evalu- effort participants expended on that trial. The researchers
ative and motivational consequences of the goal pursuit also included conditions in which the incentive was con-
attempt (see reviews in Bargh & Huang, 2009; Chartrand & sciously perceived by participants, and their imaging
Bargh, 2002; Ferguson, Hassin, & Bargh, 2008). Kawada, data revealed that the same region of the basal forebrain
Oettingen, Gollwitzer, and Bargh (2004) have demon- moderated task effort level in both the conscious and the
strated another similarity: Both conscious and unconscious unconscious incentive conditions. Pessiglione et al. (2007)
goals, when active, are “projected onto” (i.e., attributed to) concluded from their findings that “the motivational pro-
other people in the course of impression formation. cesses involved in boosting behavior are qualitatively simi-
The affective (mood) and motivational consequences lar, whether subjects are conscious or not of the reward at
of conscious and unconscious goal pursuit are also the stake” (p. 906).
same. The consequences of conscious goal attempts for affec- Similarly, recent experimental work by Custers and
tive experience (mood) and the future strength of that goal Aarts (2005, 2007) has shown that conditioning a positive
have long been established (e.g., Bandura, 1977; Carver & affective response to the name of a particular goal increases
Scheier, 1981; Heckhausen, 1991). Success at the attempt the chances the individual will pursue that goal over other
produces positive mood and increased tendencies to pursue possible alternatives, again without the participant being
that goal in the future; failure produces the opposite conse- aware of this influence on their choice of goals. Aarts et al.
quences. Research on unconscious goal pursuit has shown (2008) showed that both the goal of high performance
that the same consequences accrue for goal attempts the indi- (exertion) and the incentive attached to that goal could
vidual is not even aware of making (Bongers, Dijksterhuis, & be manipulated unconsciously; participants subliminally
Spears, 2009; Chartrand & Bargh, 2002). In the Chartrand primed with the goal of exertion outperformed a control
study, participants were given an anagram task that was easy group in a handgrip squeezing task, but those primed
or impossible to solve; the importance of this task was down- simultaneously with both the exertion goal and the positive
played by the experimenter as a “filler task” within the larger stimuli performed the best. Moreover, the complementary
study. However, for participants previously primed with the effect is found to occur when negative stimuli are paired
achievement (high performance) goal, but not for control with a goal; this increases the likelihood of disengagement
group participants, working on the easy anagram task (suc- from the goal (Aarts, Custers, & Holland, 2007). Thus,
cess condition) resulted in improved mood and increased both the goal itself and the incentives associated with the
motivation to work on a subsequent verbal task, and work- goal can be triggered through unconscious means and will
ing on the difficult anagram task produced depressed mood then influence task performance just as if they had been
and lower effort on the subsequent task. consciously perceived and chosen. These studies provide
Thus, successful goals become stronger (more likely to be further support for the hypothesis that the same underlying
pursued again by the individual) and unsuccessful goals mechanisms and processes are involved in conscious and
become weaker, all without the individual’s knowledge or unconscious goal pursuit.
consent. Presumably, this change in future goal strength
Autonomy of Active Goals
is driven by the positive versus negative affect associated
with the goal; that is, its “incentive value.” This research Unconscious goal pursuit therefore produces the same
by Bongers et al. (2009) and Chartrand and Bargh (2002) outcomes and with the same processing stages and phe-
suggests that the incentive value can be changed automati- nomenal, subjective qualities of goal pursuit as established
cally, without conscious involvement or awareness, as the previously for conscious goal pursuit. This high degree of
result of success versus failure at goals individuals do not similarity is consistent with the proposition that conscious
even know they are pursuing. goal pursuit uses preexisting, unconscious motivational

CH08.indd 297 12/18/09 2:40:15 PM


298 Motivation

structures. However, the research on unconscious motivation presented, participants were given a surprise impression
indicates that there should be an additional important qual- task in which they were asked not about the job candidate
ity shared by conscious and unconscious goal pursuit— (on which they had been consciously focused) but simply
autonomy of goal operation, once the goal becomes how much they liked Mike.
activated. Under the hypothesis that the active conscious goal
If conscious and unconscious goal pursuits use the same of evaluating a specific type of job candidate would also
underlying motivational system, then the autonomy of oper- be applied to other people encountered at the same time,
ation clearly demonstrated in the case of unconscious goal Bargh and colleagues (2008) expected that participants in
pursuit (in which the goal guides cognition and behavior the control and waiter-goal conditions would like “polite
toward the desired end-state without deliberate conscious Mike” more than “rude Mike” but that those in the reporter-
knowledge or guidance) should hold in the case of con- goal condition would like rude Mike better. Results con-
scious goal pursuit. In other words, even those goals one firmed this prediction. Because Mike’s behavior matched
intends to pursue, and of which one is aware, should “self- the qualities that the active goal was looking for, and that
ishly” pursue their agenda once activated, even if doing so would be evaluated positively by the active goal (see
runs against the self-interests of the individual. Ferguson, 2008; Ferguson & Bargh, 2004), participants in
In a test of this prediction, Bargh, Green, and Fitzsimons the reporter-goal condition showed a significant reversal
(2008) hypothesized that conscious and unconscious goal of preferences compared with the other two conditions:
pursuit would share another important feature, that of they liked rude Mike more than polite Mike.
autonomous operation once active. Note that up to now, In a third study, some participants were instructed to
potential similarities between the two modes of goal pur- help another participant (actually a confederate) with
suit had been assessed by testing whether unconscious goal an experimental task, while others were not assigned
pursuit possesses qualities that had been already estab- this “helper” role. Consistent with the autonomous-goal
lished for the case of conscious goal pursuit (Chartrand & hypothesis, participants who were concurrently helping
Bargh, 2002; Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2004). Bargh et al. someone (compared with those who were not) showed a
(2008) made the reverse assertion: that conscious goal greater willingness to donate money to a charity and to
pursuit should be found to share a quality previously dem- commit their time to helping a stranger who stopped by the
onstrated only for unconscious goal pursuit—namely, that experimental room to ask if the participant would fill out
of operating on any relevant, applicable information in the a lengthy questionnaire. Note that these are costs that one
environment regardless of whether the individual intends would not choose to incur were it not for this unintended
or is aware of this operation. In the case of conscious influence of the active goal (as shown by the control and
goals, the person is aware of pursuing them with regard deactivated-goal conditions of that study), just as one
to a specific target or set of targets; however, it should be would normally prefer a polite to a rude person.
kept in mind that just as unconscious goals operate on all These findings support the hypothesis that conscious
perceived information to which they are applicable, con- and unconscious goals, once activated, operate autono-
scious goals will, too—even information that was not the mously in an open-ended fashion on all relevant infor-
originally intended focus of the goal. mation in the environment, even information that was
Bargh et al. (2008) tested this hypothesis in two experi- not the original intended focus of the goal pursuit (in the
ments by having participants watch a videotape of an case of conscious goals). Intended goal pursuits thus can
ostensible job interview (in the control condition, partici- have unintended consequences. As shown by the results of
pants were told it was of two people getting acquainted). the control condition, people do not normally find rude,
They were told that the job in question was either a crime aggressive people likable, and it is doubtful they would
reporter for the New York Daily News or a restaurant waiter like “rude Mike” under normal circumstances—but one
position. The two jobs were pretested so that the desired would tend to like him if one was concurrently evaluating
personality characteristics were opposite of each other: the others for some purpose in which rudeness and aggressive-
ideal crime reporter is tough and aggressive, while the ideal ness happen to be useful traits.
waiter is deferential and polite. During the taped interview, Thus, the similarity between conscious and uncon-
the two participants were interrupted several times by sec- scious forms of goal pursuit extends to autonomy of
retaries and co-workers, as in an actual busy office situ- operation once activated. Even goals that are consciously
ation. The behavior of one of these interrupters (“Mike”) chosen (or explicitly given to us by another person, such as
varied across the two experimental conditions. In one tape, through experimental instructions) and of which the indi-
Mike was polite and deferential after interrupting; in the vidual is aware of pursuing nonetheless operate to attain
other, he was rude and aggressive. After the tape had been their desired end-states whether or not these outcomes are

CH08.indd 298 12/18/09 2:40:16 PM


Unconscious Goal Pursuit 299

intended or desirable at the level of the individual (self). operating autonomously within their individual hosts, is
This is evidence that the locus of proximal control over strikingly similar to Dawkins’s (1976) famous demonstra-
thought, judgment, and behavior may not reside in a con- tion in The Selfish Gene that across the domain of living
scious, agentic self (e.g., Baumeister et al., 1998) so much organisms the locus of control over behavior is at the level
as in the operation of the currently active goal. of the gene, not the individual organism. If even con-
sciously pursued, as well as unconsciously pursued, goals
An Important Difference: Awareness operate autonomously and independently once activated,
of the Goal Pursuit then one can speak of the “selfish goal” pursuing its own
There is one critical difference between conscious and agenda just as the “selfish gene” is ultimately concerned
unconscious goal pursuit: unlike unconscious goal strivers, with its own propagation. Dawkins (1976) described how
conscious goal strivers know why they do what they do. our genes have designed us (through the blind process of
In study 2 of Bargh et al. (2001) the goal of cooperation natural selection) to be their “survival machines” on which
was primed (unconsciously activated) in one condition and they depend for their propagation into future generations—
given explicitly (consciously pursued) in another. Following thus making genes, not individual organisms, the basic unit
a “resource commons” game in which their degree of coop- of natural selection. Analogously, active goals are argued
eration was measured, all participants were asked to rate here to be the unit of control over higher mental processes,
how committed they had been to the goal of cooperation not the self or individual person, and active goals single-
during the task. For participants with the conscious goal mindedly pursue their agenda independent of whether
of cooperation, these ratings correlated significantly with doing so is in the overall good of the individual person.
their actual degree of cooperative behavior—but for those That goals are to the self as genes are to their host organ-
pursuing the goal unconsciously (i.e., in the goal-priming ism is more than a metaphor. Evolutionary biologists and
condition), these ratings were unrelated to the amount of psychologists, as well as philosophers of science, consider
cooperation they had just exhibited on the task. Even though motivations to be the crucial link between genetic influ-
the cooperation prime produced significantly more coop- ences and adaptive behavior (Campbell, 1974; Mayr, 1976;
eration than found in the control group, those participants Neuberg et al., 2004; Pinker & Bloom, 1990, p. 468; Popper,
pursuing the unconscious cooperation goal were unable 1972; Symons, 1992, p. 138; Tetlock, 2002; Tomasello
to report with any accuracy on how cooperative they had et al., 2005; Tooby & Cosmides, 1992, p. 99). The close cor-
been on the task, even though they were asked immediately respondence between genes and goals is attested to by the
after completing the task. Accurate introspective access to prevalence of goal-directed behavior in the organic world,
the meaning and purpose of behavioral responses thus may which of course largely lacks the strategic, conscious infor-
require conscious awareness of the goal being pursued. mation processing capabilities of humans. As evolutionary
Awareness of goal pursuits may also be important for theorist Ernst Mayr (1976, p. 389) stressed, “the occurrence
accurate attribution of the emotional states that result from of goal-directed processes is perhaps the most characteristic
the goal pursuit. In studies of unconscious achievement feature of the world of living organisms (p. 389).” For
motivation, it has been shown that success versus failure example, a predator stalking its prey or the prey fleeing
at the task produces a positive versus a negative mood, from the pursuing predator, a bird starting on its migration,
with the person unaware of the actual source of the mood an insect selecting its host plant, a male displaying to a
and thus liable to misattribute it to some plausible cause of female—all are acting purposefully yet unconsciously.
which they are aware (Bongers et al., 2009; Chartrand & Why do genes require the proxy of goals and motives?
Bargh, 2002). Similarly, Oettingen and colleagues (2006) have The latter are necessary stand-ins because the rate of
shown that when goals are not consciously adopted (i.e., are genetic change is slow, too slow for direct genetic controls
unconsciously activated) and not explained by the situational over behavior to adapt quickly enough to constantly chang-
context (i.e., are norm violating), people find themselves in ing and shifting environmental conditions. Life offers too
an “explanatory vacuum” when attempting to interpret their many possible eventualities for all of them to be antici-
behavior, which in turn leads to the experience of negative pated by specific genetic instructions; thus, genes have to
affect (see McGraw, 1987), the actual source of which they “instruct” their host organisms not in specifics but in the
are unaware (and are thus again likely to misattribute). generally useful strategies and tactics of life. Therefore,
evolution has shaped us to be open-ended systems (Mayr,
1976), with goal programs serving as the “local agents” in
The Selfish Goal
the present to carry out genetic instructions from the dis-
As Bargh and Huang (2009) noted, the observed relation tant past but to do so adaptively, in the context of current
between goals and the individuals holding them, with goals environmental conditions and contingencies.

CH08.indd 299 12/18/09 2:40:16 PM


300 Motivation

Mayr (1976, p. 23) identified two types of inherited Attention


behavior programs in the organic world: open and closed. Bruner (1957) was facing a similar situation 50 years ago
Closed programs are those containing a nearly complete set when seeking to explain how the “New Look” findings of
of ready-made responses to particular stimuli in the environ- motivational and value-driven biases in perception could
ment; these characterize organisms with short life spans or occur in terms of basic underlying psychological processes.
highly stable and unchanging environments who have little Bruner ’s efforts gave us the extremely useful notions of
time or need to benefit from experience or adapt to local goal-directed (selective) attention, in which the active goal
variations. Because humans, on the other hand, enjoy longer drives what one attends to in the busy world, and of cat-
life spans and a long childhood under the supervision and egory accessibility, the theoretical mechanism by which
protection of caregivers, most genetic behavior programs goals could exert this influence on attention. An active goal
(goals) in humans are open. This is a great advantage to suc- (e.g., to find something to eat) caused the mental repre-
cessful adaptation, because it allows the general tendencies sentations relevant to attaining the goal (e.g., restaurants
furnished genetically to be fine-tuned to the specific local and bodegas) to become somewhat more active than usual and
conditions into which the infant happens to be born. A well- thus more ready to become activated by corresponding
known example of an open-ended program in humans is the stimuli in the environment.
young child’s ability to quickly learn the local language and This selective attention process can be quite striking in
absorb the local culture; infants can be taken to any location its ability to filter incoming stimuli so that we notice what
on earth and over time learn that language and that culture is relevant to the goal and do not notice what might distract
as well as if they had been born there (Pinker, 1994). us from the goal pursuit. The power of the effect of active
The open-ended nature of human goal pursuit is further goals on attention is such that salient, unusual events can
illustrated by the readiness and ability of unconsciously be missed entirely, as in “attentional blindness” research
activated goals to operate on whatever goal-relevant input (Mack, 2003; Most, Scholl, Clifford, & Simons, 2005;
occurs in the environment. The unconsciously active goal Simons & Chabris, 1999; see also related earlier work by
operates on whatever goal-relevant information happens Neisser, 1979; Neisser & Becklen, 1975). In one such study,
to occur next in the experimental situation, which could not participants given the explicit, conscious task of counting
be known to the participant beforehand—just as genetic the number of ball tosses between characters on a computer
influences from the distant past programmed us through display failed to notice a gorilla walking right through the
open-ended motivations to be capable of adapting to local ball-tossing game while they were busy counting tosses.
conditions far into a future that could not have been antici- Need states, such as hunger and thirst, drive selective
pated in any detail (Dawkins, 1976). Moreover, based on attention to stimuli that will satisfy the need; the stronger
the striking similarity of outcomes and process between the need, the stronger the attentional bias (e.g., Aarts,
conscious and unconscious goal pursuit, and evidence Dijksterhuis, & de Vries, 2001; Lavy & van den Hout, 1993).
that consciously pursued goals are also characterized by For example, participants who had fasted for 24 hours were
autonomy of operation once active (Bargh et al., 2008), the faster at detecting food-related words in a display than those
principle of autonomous operation appears to holds for all who had fasted for 10 hours, who were faster than nonfast-
goal pursuits, conscious and unconscious alike. ing participants; moreover, having a snack right before the
experiment eliminated the effect (Lavy & van den Hout,
The Selfish Goal, in Control 1993). In summarizing this research, Strack and Deutsch
(2004) concluded that need states become strongly linked
If the currently active goal is the proxy of genetic influences in memory with the behaviors and situations in which
and the most proximal locus of control over behavioral the need was satisfied so that subsequent experiences of
responses, then it should be capable of recruiting and direct- that need activate an attentional bias, or perceptual readi-
ing other psychological systems and processes in the service ness (Bruner, 1957), for those same situational features.
of that goal. Accordingly, one should find that the active goal
drives selective attention to the goal-relevant aspects of the
Evaluation
environment, evaluation of environmental stimuli (objects,
people, and events) in terms of whether they help or hin- Goal and need states not only drive selective attention to
der the goal pursuit, and perceptual interpretation of stimuli relevant environmental stimuli but also cause those stimuli
in manners best suited to attainment of the goal. In other to be automatically evaluated as positive or negative. Lewin
words, the active goal tends to take the helm of one’s mental (1935, p. 78) held that the evaluation or valence of an envi-
machinery and “adjust its settings” in a single-minded way ronmental object or event was a function of whether it
to maximize the chances of goal attainment. helps or hinders attainment of current goals or satisfaction

CH08.indd 300 12/18/09 2:40:17 PM


Unconscious Goal Pursuit 301

of current needs. For example, a state of food deprivation The warm–cold dimension of interpersonal behavior has
(hunger) causes both greater selective attention to food long been known to be critical in impression formation; the
cues and more positive automatic evaluations of them presence of “warm” versus “cold” in an otherwise identical
(Drobes et al., 2001). Because automatic positive evalu- description of a target individual dramatically alters the
ations are directly linked to approach motivations toward perceived likability of the individual (Asch, 1946; Kelley,
the stimulus and negative automatic evaluations to avoid- 1951). Moreover, Fiske and colleagues have shown that
ance motivations toward the stimulus (e.g., Chen & Bargh, the warmth dimension and competence are the two basic
1999; Kawakami et al., 2000; Lewin, 1935), changes in dimensions of all outgroup stereotypes around the world,
evaluation caused by the active goal or need state would with warmth being the primary of the two (e.g., Fiske,
direct the individual toward goal-helpful stimuli and away Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002). Warmth judgments turn out
from goal-hindering stimuli. to be a function of whether the individual or group is seen
This implies that changes in goal or need state should as not being an obstacle or threat to obtaining one’s own
produce corresponding changes in automatic evaluation goals: “Outgroups are seen as relatively warm and nice
of goal-relevant stimuli. Supporting this prediction is evi- to the extent that they do not compete with others” (Fiske
dence reviewed by Fazio and Olson (2003) that the results et al., 2002, p. 881), and “a primary source of negative affect
of the well-known Implicit Association Test (Greenwald, toward out-groups results from perceived incompatibility of
McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) measure of an individual’s their goals with in-group goals” (Fiske & Ruscher, 1993).
automatic attitudes can be moderated by current goal or The active goal’s effect on evaluations is so power-
need states. For example, Sherman, Rose, Koch, Presson, ful that it can modify our feelings toward the significant
and Chassin (2003) had long-term cigarette smokers (many others in our lives. Given the power of the active goal to
of whom wanted to quit) abstain from cigarettes for several transform the valence of external stimuli to suit the needs
hours before the experimental session. One group was of the goal pursuit, it may be the case that at least part of
allowed to smoke just before the experiment (thus satisfy- our positive feelings toward our friends is that they gener-
ing the need), while another group was not (the need was ally facilitate our important goal pursuits. Such an effect
still strongly present). Automatic attitudes toward smok- has been demonstrated by Fitzsimons and Shah (2008).
ing and smoking-related stimuli (cigarettes, ashtray, and They asked participants to identify a set of friends who
matches) were found to be negative in participants without helped them achieve academically and a set who did not.
the strong need to smoke but positive for those participants These participants were then nonconsciously primed with
in whom the need was present. the achievement goal (or not, in the control condition); the
Similar results have been obtained with a different achievement-primed participants consequently evaluated
measure of automatic attitudes, the sequential priming their academic-helper friends more positively than their non-
task. Ferguson and Bargh (2004) showed that active goal academic-helper friends. Control participants who did not
pursuits temporarily cause stimuli (e.g., the letter C) to be have the achievement goal currently active did not exhibit
automatically evaluated as positive if they help to attain the the same momentary favoritism for goal-instrumental
currently active goal (e.g., find as many letter Cs as pos- friends.
sible in a page of text). Importantly, when the goal task has These findings are reminiscent of the successful inter-
been completed (i.e., the goal is now inactive), the valence vention by Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, and Sherif (1961)
of that attitude object reverted to its default state. Ferguson in the classic Robbers’ Cave study. Boys attending a
(2008) has coined the term evaluative readiness to refer summer camp were arbitrarily divided into two groups,
to the tendency of active motivational states to assign the Rattlers and the Eagles, each with their own cabin and
valences to environmental objects and events on the basis T-shirts. Soon the two groups were warring, raiding the
of whether they help or hinder the active goal pursuit. other group’s cabin and behaving in a generally antago-
Brendl, Markman, and Messner (2003) have demon- nistic manner toward the other group. Sherif et al. (1961)
strated a complementary effect: In their studies, activat- restored harmony and friendship at the camp by giving
ing a need caused objects unrelated to satisfaction of the both groups a common goal for which everyone’s help and
need to be less positively evaluated than otherwise. Thus, cooperation was needed. Sharing this goal dramatically
the autonomous active goal keeps the individual on track changed the feelings of the Rattlers and Eagles toward one
toward attainment of the goal by reducing positivity and, another, making friends out of summerlong enemies.
in turn, strength of approach motivation toward stimuli that
Perception
might distract or divert the individual away from the goal.
Goal-driven evaluations are also powerful determi- A long-standing research tradition in social psychology
nants of our impressions of other people and social groups. has established the phenomenon of motivated perceptual

CH08.indd 301 12/18/09 2:40:17 PM


302 Motivation

interpretation of events (Bruner, 1957; Hastorf & Cantril, and Snyder (1975) and tested in a series of experiments by
1954; Kunda, 1990). Motivated biases in perception are so Neely (1976, 1977). These studies showed that category
commonplace in daily life as to be unremarkable—in sport- labels such as “body” and “furniture” automatically acti-
ing events, fans of both teams involved are convinced that vated their associated categories and thus primed the cat-
the referees are against their team (Hastorf & Cantril, 1954); egory members (i.e., names of parts of the body or kinds of
similarly, the fervently expressed belief of political party furniture); in a sequential priming task (prime-target pairs
members on both sides of the aisle is that the media coverage on each trial), participants were faster to respond (make
of an election is biased against their candidate. Self-protective lexical decisions) to body-part names (e.g., heart and leg)
biases operate to transform ego-threatening outcomes (e.g., when “body” was the prime compared with “furniture”
failure on a test) into more palatable versions that exonerate and with types of furniture (e.g., chair and table) when
oneself and put the blame instead on forces outside personal “furniture” was the prime compared with “body.” Thus,
control or responsibility (e.g., Bradley, 1978; Crocker & the chronic automatic effect of presenting a category label
Park, 2004; Sinclair & Kunda, 2000). is to activate members of that category.
Motivational states are found to bias perception of However, what if the current state of the world is such
ambiguous events across a range of domains, always in that the usual rules do not apply? This situation was cap-
the furtherance of goal pursuit. In the classic study by tured in the Neely (1977) paradigm by a condition in which
McClelland and Atkinson (1948), for example, participants on 80% of the trials “body” was followed by names of
deprived of food (compared with nonhungry participants) types of furniture (and only 20% by body parts), and “fur-
were more likely to report that briefly presented slides con- niture” was followed by names of parts of the body (and
tained objects associated with eating (e.g., plates), when only 20% of trials by types of furniture). In this condition,
in actuality all participants were exposed to blank slides. the long-term automatic association between category
Similarly, participants in an ostensible “taste-testing” label and category members would lead the participant
study by Balcetis and Dunning (2006) were told that the astray by activating the wrong set of targets and thereby
computer running the study would determine whether they slow responses. This is indeed what occurred in the condi-
would drink fresh orange juice or an unappealing veggie tion in which the time gap between prime and target (i.e.,
smoothie and would signal this outcome by flashing a letter stimulus onset asynchrony) was too short (250 ms) for a
for one drink or a number for the other drink. All partici- strategic, task goal–driven process to guide responses on
pants were then exposed to an ambiguous cue for 400 ms, that trial. But in a condition where the stimulus onset asyn-
one that could be interpreted either as the letter B or chrony was long enough to permit a strategic, goal-driven
the number 13. Participants for whom a letter signaled the response (750 ms), it was found to override the “hard-
desirable drink were more likely to report seeing wired” automatic generation of same-category responses.
the letter B, whereas those for whom a number signaled the That is, participants in this condition actually responded
desirable drink were more likely to perceive the number 13. faster to the prime-target mismatch trials (e.g., “body” to
Research on evolved goals has shown that an active mat- “table”) than the match trials (e.g., “body” to “heart”). The
ing goal increases the perceived attractiveness of potential task goal to respond as quickly as possible to the target
partners (Maner et al., 2003; Maner, Gailliot, & DeWall, words adapted quickly to the reversed contingencies of
2007; Neuberg et al., 2004) and even increases the degree of the situation and caused the normally automatic encoding
arousal perceived in the faces of attractive women (Maner effect to be inhibited, and the novel contingency to become
et al., 2005). Commitment to a close relationship (the per- (temporarily) automatic.
son is motivated to maintain the relationship) produces an Logan and Zbrodoff (1979) demonstrated the same
increase in positive illusions concerning the relationship effect in a spatial paradigm in which participants responded
(Lemay & Clark, 2008; Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 1996). as quickly as they could on each trial as to whether a pre-
In each of these cases, the goal’s effect on perception sented target word appeared above versus below a fixa-
increases the probability of goal attainment, for example tion point on the screen. On some trials the actual words
by keeping the individual in a relationship despite evidence “above” or “below” were presented, and not surprisingly,
that might lead to a breakup if viewed more objectively. participants were faster than usual to make the above or
the below response when the word “above” appeared
Overriding Chronic, Automatic Processes above the fixation point or the word “below” appeared below
The executive power of the active goal is indicated further the fixation point. (The design of this experiment is concep-
by its ability to override chronic, automatic encoding ten- tually identical to the classic Stroop (1935) color-naming
dencies. The principle that the current goal can dominate paradigm, in which naming a presented word’s color
otherwise automatic processes was first proposed by Posner is faster if the presented word is the color name itself,

CH08.indd 302 12/18/09 2:40:18 PM


Unconscious Goal Pursuit 303

e.g., the word “red” presented in red.) But what if on temporarily creating “habit”-like behavioral responses that
the majority (80%) of trials the reverse contingency is true: the aid in goal pursuit instead of threaten it.
word “above” appears below the fixation point and the word Moskowitz, Gollwitzer, Wasel, and Schaal (1999) pro-
“below” appears above it? Here again, participants are vided a particularly compelling demonstration of the active
faster to respond “above” when the word “below” appears goal dominating antagonistic automatic processes. In their
above the fixation point than when the word “above” studies, all participants showed evidence of automatic
appears there, and vice versa. stereotype activation on the mere perception of minority
In another demonstration of an active (unconscious) goal group features. However, those participants who possessed
overriding automatic, habitual responding, Sassenberg and a chronic motivation to treat others in an egalitarian fash-
Moskowitz (2005) primed a “think different” goal of gen- ion were found to inhibit or shut down the automatically
erating creative solutions to a problem. Participants in the activated stereotype almost immediately after it became
think-different condition, compared with a control group, active, such that the stereotype did not influence their per-
indeed generated more unusual uses for a given object and ceptions of the minority group members. The active egali-
more uncommon answers in a free-association task instead tarian goal overrode the otherwise automatic tendency to
of the habitual or automatically generated ones. In each of categorize people in terms of the group stereotype, because
these studies, the currently active goal (i.e., the explicit task such stereotypic treatment would run counter to the active
goal given to participants by the experimental instructions) goal’s aim of thinking about people the same regardless of
dominates otherwise automatic, chronic responses if the their race, gender, or ethnicity.
latter interfere with the attainment of the goal. (For more
Temporary Creation of Automatic Processes
on the neural mechanics of how a current task goal over-
rides automatically suggested alternative responses, see Treating other people fairly is a positive social goal, but
Cohen, Dunbar, & McClelland, 1990.) in line with the notion that the self comprises many, often
The principle that the active goal can temporarily “recon- conflicting goals, people also have strong goals to protect
figure” the automatic (immediate, efficient) responses their self-esteem. If this self-protective goal is active, for
made to environmental stimuli, if this helps to attain the example, following a threat to self-esteem (e.g., failure at a
goal, extends to complex social information process- task), it can instead cause the stereotyping of minority group
ing situations. For example, much evidence points to the members, even under conditions known to normally
automatic manner in which other people are encoded or prevent such stereotyping. In a series of experiments by
categorized in terms of their race, age, and gender (e.g., Spencer, Fein, Wolfe, Fong, and Dunn (1998), automatic
Bargh, 1999; Brewer, 1988). However, if doing so hinders stereotyping effects were shown to be blocked by atten-
the successful completion of the active goal, this does not tional load (secondary task) manipulation, replicating
happen. Kurzban, Tooby, and Cosmides (2001, study 2) earlier work by Gilbert and Hixon (1991). The secondary
gave participants the explicit goal of coalition detection task thus created conditions under which it was normally
and subsequently presented them with a situation in which difficult for automatic stereotyping of minority group
allied targets were visibly linked by shirt color. The experi- members to occur.
menters found that participants encoded targets using the However, Spencer et al. (1998) then threatened the self-
most useful information current to that context, which in esteem of some participants through bogus task-failure
this case was target shirt color, not their race (race did not feedback to trigger a goal of restoring positive self-regard.
designate targets’ group affiliations). The goal of coalition As one common method of restoring self-esteem is to deni-
detection overrode the usual automatic encoding of people grate others, especially minority group members, the active
in terms of racial categories, because race was not diagnostic self-protective goal was expected, and found, to cause
of ingroup membership in this context. automatic stereotyping effects to reemerge. Participants
As described earlier, implementation intentions are who had been told they had failed on the task thus showed
also examples of the power of the active goal to override automatic stereotyping effects under conditions known to
habitual, automatic responses. Much research has shown prevent such stereotyping under normal conditions. The
implementation intentions to facilitate goal striving even active goal of restoring positive self-esteem thus created
when it is threatened by strongly competing habitual automatic effects where none existed without the goal.
responses (e.g., Cohen et al., 2008; Gollwitzer & Schaal, Lakin, Chartrand, and Arkin (2008) reported a similar
1998; Holland et al., 2006; Lydon et al., 2008). Through the effect, in which socially excluded individuals subsequently
formation of strategic implementation intentions, one can become more likely to mimic and imitate interaction partners.
strategically switch from top-down control of one’s actions The exclusion presumably triggered the need-to-belong goal
by active goals to bottom-up control by situational cues, (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Fiske, 2004) and the mimicry

CH08.indd 303 12/18/09 2:40:18 PM


304 Motivation

behavior as a means to that end (mimicry has been shown to The “Macbeth effect” (Zhong & Liljenquist, 2006)
increase bonding and liking among individuals; Chartrand & provides another illustration of the goal-completion
Bargh, 1999), without the participant’s awareness of either effect running against the individual’s presumed values
the mimicry behavior or the reason for it. and behavioral intentions. In this study, participants were
The original demonstration of such “strategic auto- induced to consider performing some unethical behaviors
maticity” was the research on implementation intentions and were then given a choice among several small gifts for
(Gollwitzer & Brandstätter, 1997), in which an active goal taking part in the study. Compared with a control condition,
creates temporarily automatic processes in service of the these participants were more likely to choose a package of
goal pursuit. Forming implementation intentions turns antiseptic tissue wipes than other items. Most importantly,
top-down conscious control into bottom-up automatic or those who were given an opportunity to wash their hands
unconscious control by the situational cues specified in the after contemplating the unethical behavior subsequently
“if” component of an if–then implementation intention. were less likely to help a stranger. Considering an unethical
act thus triggered the participants’ goal to cleanse them-
Completed Goals Turn Off Regardless selves in any way possible (i.e., morally or physically),
of the Individual’s Best Interests and satisfying that goal by washing of the hands (physical
It has been argued here that the operation of the active goal cleansing) turned off the goal and made it less likely they
is autonomous from the interests and values of the indi- would engage in ethical behavior (moral cleansing).
vidual person or agentic self. Another class of motivational When morally threatened participants washed their
phenomena consistent with this hypothesis is the goal hands, the selfish goal was fulfilled—and therefore no
turnoff effect: Once a goal pursuit attempt is completed, longer on the lookout for opportunities to restore its host’s
the goal deactivates (e.g., Atkinson & Birch, 1970; Lewin, moral self. Consequently, morally restored participants
1926) and then inhibits the mental representations used to were more likely to decline helping a person in need. Once
attain the goal (Förster et al., 2005; Liberman, Foerster, again, as with the moral-credentialing effect, the effect of
et al., 2007). In the case of unconscious goal pursuit, it is completion of the cleansing goal runs counter to the pre-
clear that the deactivation of the goal must occur indepen- sumed conscious intentions of the individual. As one of
dently of conscious intention and awareness (because the the Macbeth-effect study’s authors asked rhetorically in an
individual was not even aware the goal was active). Thus, online interview, “Do you really want your past sins to be
several studies have found that once an unconscious goal easily washed away, which discourages you from engaging
is satisfied, its influence on cognition and behavior disap- in ethical behaviors to help others?” (Hirshon, 2006, p. 1).
pears (e.g., Kawada et al., 2004). In Bargh et al. (2008), study 3, in which the conscious
The same goal turnoff effect occurs for conscious goal to help one’s experimental-task partner carried over
goals—even for positively valued, prosocial goals such as to helping strangers who interrupted the experiment, a
helping another person (Bargh et al., 2008, study 3). The condition was included in which the conscious helping
goal turnoff effect is part of the autonomous operation of goal was completed before any further requests for help.
the active goal and is not under the individual’s (or self ’s) Because it had been completed, the helping goal “turned
awareness and control; this can be seen most clearly in off,” so in this condition the participant no longer tended
studies where an unequivocally positive goal deactivates to help those who asked for it. Completing the pursuit of
after fulfillment, actually inhibiting the individual from the helping goal caused the individual to subsequently
continuing to behave in this positive fashion. A dramatic become less likely to help others in need—less prosocial
example of this phenomenon is found in recent research on and altruistic—a tendency running against the likely self-
“moral credentials.” concepts of the participants that they are helpful to those
Monin and Miller (2001) found that participants who in need.
were given the opportunity to disagree with blatantly sexist In summary, traditional models of motivation in which
comments (thus fulfilling their goal to be egalitarian and a conscious, agentic self is posited to be the ultimate con-
nonsexist) were later more willing (compared with a control troller of individual human behavior need to take into
condition) to recommend a man over a woman for a stereo- account the accumulating evidence to the contrary. For one
typically male job. According to the authors, after partici- thing, conscious intentions and awareness are not neces-
pants had been allowed to establish their moral credentials sary to put motives and goals into operation or to guide
in the first part of the experiment, they stopped pursuing this them to completion. For another, once activated, these
goal in a subsequent part. Thus, after the egalitarian goal was goals operate to produce the same outcomes and with
fulfilled, it shut off, leaving “host” individuals vulnerable to the same signature set of phenomenal qualities as when
behaving in a manner contrary to their egalitarian values. consciously pursued. Third, all goals, whether consciously

CH08.indd 304 12/18/09 2:40:19 PM


Summary 305

or unconsciously pursued, operate autonomously from cooperation, helping, and putting the welfare of others over
control by the individual person or self—they operate on one’s own have been shown to operate automatically and
all relevant information in the environment, even on target unconsciously, testifying to their innate or well-practiced
people or events for which their operation is unintended by nature (see Bargh et al., 2001, study 2; Chen et al., 2001;
the individual and might produce unwanted consequences. Tomasello et al., 2005). Because selfishness means put-
As part of their operation, they turn off after completion ting one’s own welfare and needs above those of other
even if this causes the individual to be less likely during people (Elster, 1990; Jencks, 1990), unconsciously operat-
that time to act in line with important (e.g., prosocial and ing goals for cooperation and for ensuring the welfare of
egalitarian) self-values. another person (see Clark & Mills, 1993) show that “self-
The evidence thus points to the existence of uncon- ish” goals (in terms of their single-minded pursuit of their
scious motivational structures that guided human behav- own agendas) do not necessarily produce “selfish” indi-
ior before the development of conscious psychological viduals (in terms of pursuing outcomes that benefit others
processes (Bargh & Huang, 2009; Bargh & Morsella, more than oneself).
2009). The striking similarities observed in the outcomes That goals can be selfish without making their host indi-
and qualities of unconscious versus conscious goal pursuit viduals behave selfishly is a nice thought on which to end
suggest that the later-developing conscious route to goal this section, because it shows yet again that the active goal is
pursuit used preexisting unconscious motivational pro- the unit of autonomous behavior control, not the individual
cesses and structures. Moreover, this approach to human human (or self)—just as Dawkins (1976) and others had
motivation in which unconscious, not conscious, processes shown earlier that the gene is the unit of natural selec-
are given primacy of place fits well with the long-term tion, not the individual organism. The active goal pursues
perspective of evolutionary biology, in which unconscious its agenda regardless of whether this fits the agenda of its
processes capably and adaptively drive the behavior of individual host, just as selfish genes pursue their own
all living organisms (e.g., Mayr, 1976). It is a strength propagation whether or not this is good for their host
of the unconscious-primacy viewpoint that it is in harmony organisms.
with research and evidence in the other natural sciences, as
opposed to existing in anomalous detachment.
The other important themes emerging from the research SUMMARY
on unconscious motivation are the autonomy of active
goal operation and the power of the active goal to trans- These are halcyon days for the scientific study of human
form cognitive and affective processes—even habitual and motivation. Research under the traditional model, in which
automatic ones—in the furtherance of the goal pursuit. the individual consciously sets goals to attain, commits
Autonomy of operation appears to be yet another feature of to, and then strives for them, is flourishing. Important
goal pursuit that characterizes both conscious and uncon- advances have been made in our knowledge of effective
scious goal pursuit: active goals operate independently to ways to set goals, such as mental contrasting, in which the
pursue their own agendas and thus can produce outcomes individual’s present circumstances (e.g., obesity) are con-
that are unintended and even unwanted at the level of the trasted with a future state (e.g., a healthy, trim physique)
individual person (hence the notion of the “selfish goal”; that the person desires to attain. Goal-striving research has
Bargh & Huang, 2009). Moreover, while in operation the focused on effective ways to overcome problems such as
goal has great power over other psychological systems getting started; implementation intentions in which
such as evaluation and perceptual interpretation, driv- the individual forms a concrete if–then plan to carry out the
ing the outcomes of these processes toward those helpful desired action have been shown to be quite effective in
to the goal pursuit; this power appears to be so strong that this regard. Other threats to successful goal striving have
otherwise automatic (immediate, efficient) effects (e.g., received research attention as well, such as knowing when
stereotype activation) can be prevented from occurring to disengage from a fruitless goal pursuit, ego depletion
and normally nonautomatic effects can be made to operate effects (self-control as a limited resource), and overcoming
automatically. short-term temptations for the greater long-term good.
It must be stressed that the notion of the “selfish goal” This ongoing research on conscious goal pursuit has
in no way implies “selfishness” at the level of the indi- been complemented by an increasing focus on the uncon-
vidual person, mainly because the individual or self is scious mode of goal pursuit, in which goal representations
composed of many goals—self-interested ones to be become activated automatically by relevant environmental
sure, but also prosocial and morally principled ones (e.g., stimuli and then operate, without conscious guidance or
Mansbridge, 1990; Miller, 1999). Prosocial goals such as awareness, over extended periods to guide cognition, affect,

CH08.indd 305 12/18/09 2:40:19 PM


306 Motivation

and behavior toward the desired end-state. This research Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & de Vries, P. (2001). On the psychology of
drinking: Being thirsty and perceptually ready. British Journal
has led to the conclusion, in line with dominant models
of Psychology, 92, 631–642.
in evolutionary biology, that unconscious motivational
Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan?
structures existed before the emergence, over evolution- Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behav-
ary periods, of conscious forms of thought and information iors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 971–980.
processing and that the conscious mode of goal pursuit Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned
likely uses those preexisting processes and structures. This helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.
is shown by the high degree of similarity recent research
Achtziger, A., Fehr, T., Oettingen, G., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Rockstroh, B.
has found between conscious and unconscious modes of (2009). Strategies of intention formation are reflected in continuous
goal pursuit in the outcomes they produce, the phenomenal MEG activity. Social Neuroscience, 4, 11–27.
or experiential qualities associated with the goal pursuit, Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Implementation
and the brain regions supporting them. intentions and shielding goal striving from unwanted thoughts and feel-
ings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 381–393.
Taken together, the two lines of research have advanced
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior.
our knowledge of human motivation not only by revealing In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.), Action-control: From cognition to
a unifying single (or at least highly similar) mechanism behavior (pp. 11–39). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.
underlying both conscious and unconscious goal pursuit but Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior
also by pointing to what is perhaps the key functional dif- and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
ference between conscious and unconscious thought. It is Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1969). The prediction of behavioral intentions
in a choice situation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 5,
clear from the different focuses and agendas of the research 400–416.
on conscious versus unconscious motivation that the former Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting
has been concerned mainly with planning—the creation, social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
establishment, and maintenance of new set goals, with top- Allman, J. M. (2000). Evolving brains. New York: Scientific American
ics such as how new goals are chosen, how one becomes Library.
committed to them, and how one overcomes obstacles and Ambady, N., Shih, M., Kim, A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2001). Stereotype sus-
ceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quantitative per-
difficulties in their way. The research on unconscious goal
formance. Psychological Science, 12, 385–390.
pursuit, on the other hand, has focused on existing goal rep-
Anderson, R. C., & Pichert, J. W. (1978). Recall of previously unrecall-
resentations and how they are capable of operating outside able information following a shift in perspective. Journal of Verbal
of conscious knowledge and guidance. Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 1–12.
Planning, after all, still seems to be a uniquely human Arendt, H. (1978). The life of the mind. New York: Harcourt.
activity—some have argued that it is the ability that distin- Armitage, C. J. (2004). Evidence that implementation intentions reduce
guishes us from all other species, including our closest pri- dietary fat intake: A randomized trial. Health Psychology, 23,
319–323.
mate neighbors (e.g., Byrne & Bates, 2007; among other
Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2003). The effects of mindset on behavior:
candidates being actively debated are theory of mind capa- Self-regulation in deliberative and implemental frames of mind.
bilities and an innate cooperation motive; see Tomasello Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 86–95.
et al., 2005). Such mental time travel appears to be the spe- Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of
cial domain of conscious thought, giving us the ability to Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 258–290.
escape or transcend our immediate present circumstances Asch, S. E. (1961). Issues in the study of social influences on judgment.
In I. A. Berg & B. M. Bass (Eds.), Conformity and deviation (pp. 143–158).
by planning for the future or remembering the past (Bargh, New York: Harper.
1997). Unconscious processes make this time travel pos- Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior.
sible by keeping the individual adaptively in touch with Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.
the present, “minding the store” while consciousness is Atkinson, J. W. (1958). Towards experimental analysis of human motiva-
away, thanks to the various unconscious behavioral guid- tion in terms of motives, expectancies and incentives. In J. W. Atkinson
(Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 288–305). Princeton,
ance systems as discovered by social cognition research
NJ: Van Nostrand.
over the past quarter century.
Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ:
Van Nostrand.
Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action. New York:
REFERENCES Wiley.
Baillargeon, R. (2008). Innate ideas revisited: For a principle of persistence
Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Holland, R. W. (2007). The unconscious cessa- in infants’ physical reasoning. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
tion of goal pursuit: When goals and negative affect are coactivated. 3, 2–13.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 165–178. Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, M. R., & Fiore, M. C.
Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Marien, H. (2008). Preparing and motivating (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing
behavior outside of awareness. Science, 319, 1639. model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111, 33–51.

CH08.indd 306 12/18/09 2:40:20 PM


References 307

Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see: Motivational Bargh, J. A., Green, M. L., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2008). The selfish goal:
influences on visual perception. Journal of Personality and Social Unintended consequences of intended goal pursuit. Social Cognition,
Psychology, 91, 612–625. 26, 534–554.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral Bargh, J. A., & Huang, J. Y. (2009). The selfish goal. In G. Moskowitz &
change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. H. Grant (Eds.), The psychology of goals (pp. 127–150). New York:
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human agency. American Guilford Press.
Psychologist, 37, 122–147. Bargh, J. A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social on Psychological Science, 3, 73–79.
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bargh, J. A., & Morsella, E. (2009). Unconscious behavioral guidance sys-
Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American tems. In C. R. Agnew, D. E. Cavlston, W. G. Graziano, & J. R. Kelly
Psychologist, 44, 1175–1184. (Eds.), Then a miracle occurs: Focusing on behavior in social psycho-
logical theory and research. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York:
Freeman. Bargh, J. A., & Raymond, P. (1995). The naïve misuse of power:
Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspectives Unconscious sources of sexual harassment. Journal of Social Issues,
26, 168–185.
on Psychological Science, 1, 164–180.
Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects Bargh, J. A., Raymond, P., Pryor, J., & Strack, F. (1995). Attractiveness
revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 87–99. of the underling: An automatic power–sex association and its conse-
quences for sexual harassment and aggression. Journal of Personality
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-efficacy and Social Psychology, 68, 768–781.
and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 586–598. Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: Framing the
debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628–647.
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social inter-
action. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motiva- Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego
tion and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 93–130). New York: Guilford Press. depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.
Bargh, J. A. (1992). Why subliminality does not matter to social psychology:
Awareness of the stimulus versus awareness of its effects. In R. Bornstein & Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire
T. Pittman (Eds.), Perception without awareness: Cognitive, clinical, for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.
and social perspectives (pp. 236–255). New York: Guilford Press. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. Baumeister, R. F., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2000). Ego depletion:
(Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition A resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled processing.
(Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Social Cognition, 18, 130–150.
Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster: The case against controllabil- Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, C. N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion
ity of automatic stereotype effects. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct
Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 361–382). New York: causation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 167–203.
Guilford Press. Bayer, U. C., Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. (2009).
Bargh, J. A. (2005). Bypassing the will: Towards demystifying behavioral Responding to subliminal cues: Do if–then plans facilitate action prep-
priming effects. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. Bargh (Eds.), The new aration and initiation without conscious intent? Social Cognition, 27,
unconscious (pp. 37–58). Oxford: Oxford University Press. 183–201.
Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years? On the Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). Mindset effects on information
development, mechanisms, and ecology of unconscious social behavior. search in self-evaluation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35,
European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 147–168. 313–327.
Bargh, J. A. (Ed.). (2007). Social psychology and the unconscious: The Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2007). Boosting scholastic test scores
automaticity of higher mental processes. Philadelphia: Psychology by willpower: The role of implementation intentions. Self and Identity,
Press. 6, 1–19.
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cogni-
of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and tive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183–200.
Social Psychology, 62, 893–912. Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York:
American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. Academic Press.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). A practical guide to priming and Bindra, D. (1959). Motivation: A systematic reinterpretation. New York:
automaticity research. In H. Reis & C. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of Ronald.
research methods in social psychology (pp. 253–285). New York: Bongers, K. C. A., Dijksterhuis, A., & Spears, R. (2009). Self-esteem regu-
Cambridge University Press. lation after success and failure to attain unconscious goals. Journal of
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 468–477.
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype priming on Bradley, G. W. (1978). Self-serving biases in the attribution process:
action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244. A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. Journal of Personality
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: The auto- and Social Psychology, 36, 56–71.
maticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, Brandstätter, V., & Frank, E. (2002). Effects of deliberative and implemen-
925–945. tal mindsets on persistence in goal-directed behavior. Personality and
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trötschel, R. Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1366–1378.
(2001). The automated will: Unconscious activation and pursuit of Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation
behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and
1004–1027. Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.

CH08.indd 307 12/18/09 2:40:20 PM


308 Motivation

Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The
Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131. perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality
Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2003). The devaluation and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.
effect: Activating a need devaluates unrelated objects. Journal of Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (2002). Unconscious motivations: Their
Consumer Research, 29, 463–473. activation, operation, and consequences. In A. Tesser, D. Stapel, &
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. J. Wood (Eds.), Self and motivation: Emerging psychological per-
In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Eds.), Advances in social cognition spectives (pp. 13–41). Washington, DC: American Psychological
(Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Association Press.
Brill, A. A. (1938). Introduction. In A. A. Brill (Ed. & Trans.), The basic Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W., & Lakin, J. (2005). Beyond the perception–
writings of Sigmund Freud (pp. 1–32). New York: Modern Library. behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of
unconscious mimicry. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.),
Brockner, J. (1992). The escalation of commitment to a failing course of
The new unconscious (pp. 334–361). New York: Oxford University
action: Toward theoretical progress. Academy of Management Review,
Press.
17, 39–61.
Chasteen, A. L., Park, D. C., & Schwarz, N. (2001). Implementation inten-
Brownell, K. D., & Horgen, K. B. (2004). Food fight. New York:
tions and facilitation of prospective memory. Psychological Science,
McGraw-Hill.
12, 457–461.
Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64,
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation:
123–152.
Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimu-
Brunstein, J. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). Effects of failure on subse- lus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224.
quent performance: The importance of self-defining goals. Journal of
Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). Relationship orientation
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 395–407.
as a moderator of the effects of social power. Journal of Personality
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal and Social Psychology, 80, 173–187.
goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispo-
Cioffi, D., & Garner, R. (1996). On doing the decision: Effects of active
sitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508.
versus passive choice on commitment and self-perception. Personality
Burgess, P. W., Simons, J. S., Dumontheil, I., & Gilbert, S. J. (2005). The and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 133–147.
gateway hypothesis of rostral PFC function. In J. Duncan, L. Phillips, &
Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1993). The difference between communal and
P. McLeod (Eds.), Measuring the mind: Speed control and age
exchange relationships: What it is and is not. Personality and Social
(pp. 215–246). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Psychology Bulletin, 19, 684–691.
Burton, K. D., Lydon, J. E., D’Alessandro, D. U., & Koestner, R. (2006).
Cochran, W., & Tesser, A. (1996). The “what-the-hell” effect: Some effects
The differential effects of intrinsic and identified motivation on
of goal proximity and goal framing on performance. In L. L. Martin &
well-being and performance: Prospective, experimental, and implicit
A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals,
approaches to self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and
affect, and self-regulation (pp. 99–120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Social Psychology, 91, 750–762.
Erlbaum.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolution-
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
ary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.
Cohen, A.-L., Bayer, U. C., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Self-
Byrne, R. W., & Bates, L. A. (2007). Sociality, evolution, and cognition.
regulatory strategy and executive control: Implementation intentions
Current Biology, 17, R714–R723.
modulate task switching and Simon task performance. Psychological
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar Research, 72, 12–26.
conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of
space. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3–25. automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the
Campbell, D. T. (1974). Evolutionary epistemology. In P. A. Schilpp (Ed.), Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361.
The philosophy of Karl Popper (pp. 413–463). La Salle, IL: Open Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo,
Court Publishing. M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of
Cantor, N., & Fleeson, W. (1994). Social intelligence and intelligent consciousness (pp. 571–595). New York: Cambridge University Press.
goal pursuit: A cognitive slice of motivation. In W. Spaulding (Ed.), Crocker, J., & Canevello, A. (2008). Creating and undermining social support
Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 41, pp. 125–180). Lincoln: in communal relationships: The role of compassionate and self-image
University of Nebraska Press. goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 555–575.
Carver, C. S. (2004). Negative affects deriving from the behavioral Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem.
approach system. Emotion, 4, 3–22. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 392–414.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit motivator:
A control-theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer. On the unconscious operation of behavioral goals. Journal of
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 129–142.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2007). In search of the unconscious sources of
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral acti- goal pursuit: Accessibility and positive affective valence of the goal
vation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: state. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 43, 312–318.
The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible
67, 319–333. functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual process theories in social psychology. Royal Society of London B, 351, 1413–1420.
New York: Guilford Press. Darley, J., & Latane, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergen-
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of social cies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social
information processing goals: Unconscious priming reproduces effects Psychology, 8, 377–383.
of explicit conscious instructions. Journal of Personality and Social Darnon, C., Harackiewicz, J. M., Butera, F., Mugny, G., & Quiamzade,
Psychology, 71, 464–478. A. (2007). Performance-approach and performance-avoidance

CH08.indd 308 12/18/09 2:40:21 PM


References 309

goals: When uncertainty makes a difference. Personality and Social Elster, J. (1990). Selfishness and altruism. In J. J. Mansbridge (Ed.), Beyond
Psychology Bulletin, 33, 813–827. self-interest (pp. 44–52). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species. London: John Murray. Emmons, R. A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Personal goals and subjective
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University well-being. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of
Press. action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 313–337).
New York: Guilford Press.
Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language
and the brain. New York: Norton. Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual
thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 168–192.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits:
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Faude-Koivisto, T., Würz, D., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Implementation inten-
Inquiry, 11, 227–268. tions: The mental representations and cognitive procedures of IF–THEN
planning. In W. Klein & K. Markman (Eds.), The handbook of imagination
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little, Brown.
and mental simulation (pp. 69–86). New York: Guilford Press.
Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea: Evolution and the mean-
Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations:
ings of life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 1–27.
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2008). Satiated with
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition
belongingness? Effects of acceptance, rejection, and task framing
research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54,
on self-regulatory performance. Journal of Personality and Social
297–327.
Psychology, 95, 1367–1382.
Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., Bargh, J. A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2000). Ferguson, M. J. (2007). The automaticity of evaluation. In J. A. Bargh
On the relation between associative strength and automatic behavior. (Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of
the higher mental processes (pp. 219–264). Philadelphia: Psychology
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 531–544.
Press.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception–behavior express-
way. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology Ferguson, M. J. (2008). On becoming ready to pursue a goal you don’t
(Vol. 33, pp. 1–40). San Diego: Academic Press. know you have: Effects of nonconscious goals on evaluative readiness.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1268–1294.
Dijksterhuis, A., Chartrand, T. L., & Aarts, H. (2007). Effects of priming
and perception on social behavior and goal pursuit. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects
Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and
mental processes (pp. 51–132). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Social Psychology, 87, 557–572.
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Ferguson, M. J., Hassin, R., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Implicit motivation:
University Press. Past, present, and future. In J. Shah & W. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of
motivational science (pp. 150–166). New York: Guilford Press.
Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human conscious-
ness. New York: Norton. Festinger, L. (1942). A theoretical interpretation of shifts in level of aspira-
tion. Psychological Review, 49, 235–250.
Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cog-
nitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distract- Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., & Frey, D. (2008). Self-regulation and selec-
ibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and tive exposure: The impact of depleted self-regulation resources on
Cognition, 30, 343–353. confirmatory information processes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 94, 382–395.
Drobes, D. J., Miller, E. J., Hillman, C. H., Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N., &
Lang, P. J. (2001). Food deprivation and emotional reactions to food Fishbach, A., & Dhar, R. (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The liberat-
cues: Implications for eating disorders. Biological Psychology, 57, ing effect of perceived goal progress on choice. Journal of Consumer
153–177. Research, 32, 370–377.
Duckworth, K. L., Bargh, J. A., Garcia, M., & Chaiken, S. (2002). The Fishbach, A., Dhar, R., & Zhang, Y. (2006). Subgoals as substitutes or
automatic evaluation of novel stimuli. Psychological Science, 13, complements: The role of goal accessibility. Journal of Personality
513–519. and Social Psychology, 91, 232–242.
Dweck, C. S. (1996). Implicit theories as organizers of goals and behavior. Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not
In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: unto temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal tempta-
Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 69–90). New York: tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296–309.
Guilford Press. Fishbach, A., & Shah, J. Y. (2006). Self-control in action: Implicit disposi-
El-Alayli, A. (2006). Matching achievement contexts with implicit theories tions towards goals and away from temptations. Journal of Personality
to maximize motivation after failure: A congruence model. Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 820–832.
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1690–1702. Fishbach, A., & Trope, Y. (2004). The substitutability of external control and
Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating the “classic” and “contemporary” approaches self-control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 256–270.
to achievement motivation: A hierarchical model of approach and Fishbach, A., & Zhang, Y. (2008). Together or apart: When goals and temp-
avoidance achievement motivation. In M. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), tation complement versus compete. Journal of Personality and Social
Advances in motivation and achievement (pp. 243–279). Greenwich, Psychology, 94, 547–559.
CT: JAI Press. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An
Elliot, A. J. (2008). Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation. introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social beings: A core motives approach to social psy-
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and chology. New York: Wiley.
avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often
Psychology, 72, 218–232. mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively fol-
Elliot, A. J., Gable, S. L., & Mapes, R. R. (2006). Approach and avoidance low from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and
motivation in the social domain. Personality and Social Psychology Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Bulletin, 32, 378–391.

CH08.indd 309 12/18/09 2:40:21 PM


310 Motivation

Fiske, S. T., & Ruscher, J. B. (1993). Negative interdependence and Geraerts, E., Bernstein, D. M., Merckelbach, H., Linders, C., Raymaeckers,
prejudice: Whence the affect? In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton L., & Loftus, E. F. (2008). Lasting false beliefs and their behavioral
(Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in consequences. Psychological Science, 19, 749–753.
group perception (pp. 239–268). San Diego: Academic Press. Gilbert, S. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., Cohen, A.-L., Oettingen, G., & Burgess,
Fitzgerald, L. F. (1993). Sexual harassment: Violence against women in the P. W. (2009). Separable brain systems supporting cued versus self-
workplace. American Psychologist, 48, 1070–1076. initiated realization of delayed intentions. Journal of Experimental
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of you: Unconscious Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 905–915.
pursuit of interpersonal goals associated with relationship partners. Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 148–164. and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Automatic self-regulation. Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.
In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Goldsmith, M. (1934). A history of mesmerism. Garden City, NY:
Research, theory and applications (pp. 151–170). New York: Guilford Doubleday, Doran.
Press. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1986). Striving for specific identities: The social reality
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Shah, J. Y. (2008). How goal instrumentality of self-symbolizing. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private
shapes relationship evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social self (pp. 143–159). New York: Springer.
Psychology, 95, 319–337. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins &
Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Idson, L. C. (1998). Approach and avoid- R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition:
ance strength during goal attainment: Regulatory focus and the “goal Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 53–92). New York:
looms larger” effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Guilford Press.
75, 1115–1131. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions.
Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility from European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.
active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of sim-
41, 220–239. ple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.
Frank, T. (2004). What’s wrong with Kansas? New York: Metropolitan Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bayer, U. C. (1999). Deliberative versus implemental mind-
Books. sets in the control of action. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process
Freud, S. (1914). The psychopathology of everyday life (A. Brill, Trans.). theories in social psychology (pp. 403–422). New York: Guilford Press.
New York: Macmillan. (Original work published 1901) Gollwitzer, P. M., Bayer, U. C., & McCulloch, K. C. (2005). The control of
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge the unwanted. In R. Hassin. J. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new
University Press. unconscious (pp. 485–515). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S.-J., & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and
the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Royal Society of London, 355, 1771–1788. 73, 186–199.
Frith, C., & Wolpert, D. (Ed.). (2003). The neuroscience of social interac- Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Ratajczak, H. (1990). From weigh-
tion. New York: Oxford University Press. ing to willing: Approaching a change decision through pre- or post-
Fujita, K., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Mindsets and pre- decisional mentation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
conscious open-mindedness to incidental information. Journal of Processes, 45, 41–65.
Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 48–61. Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Steller, B. (1990). Deliberative and
Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal implemental mind-sets: Cognitive tuning toward congruous thoughts
levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59,
17, 278–282. 1119–1127.
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and
Gable, P. A. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and goals.
implemental mind-sets on illusion of control. Journal of Personality
Journal of Personality, 74, 175–222.
and Social Psychology, 56, 531–542.
Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit of identity. In
reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19, 476–482.
J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation
Gagné, F. M., & Lydon, J. E. (2001a). Mind-sets and close relationships: across the life span (pp. 389–423). Cambridge, England: Cambridge
When bias leads to (in)accurate predictions. Journal of Personality and University Press.
Social Psychology, 81, 85–96.
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, E. M. (1996). Goal effects on action
Gagné, F. M., & Lydon, J. E. (2001b). Mindset and relationship illusions: and cognition. In E. T. Higgins & A.W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psy-
The moderating effects of domain specificity and relationship commit- chology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 361–399). New York:
ment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1144–1155. Guilford Press.
Gailliot, M. T., Plant, E. A., Butz, D. A., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Increasing Gollwitzer, P. M., Parks-Stamm, E. J., Jaudas, A., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Flexible
self-regulatory strength can reduce the depleting effect of suppressing ste- tenacity in goal pursuit. In J. Shah & W. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of
reotypes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 281–294. motivation science (pp. 325–341). New York: Guilford Press.
Galdi, S., Arcuri, L., & Gawronski, B. (2008, 22 August). Automatic men- Gollwitzer, P. M., & Rohloff, U. B. (1999). The speed of goal pursuit. In
tal associations predict future choices of undecided decision-makers. R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 12, pp. 147–159).
Science, 321, 1100–1102. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gawrilow, C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Implementation intentions facil- Gollwitzer, P. M., & Schaal, B. (1998). Metacognition in action: The impor-
itate response inhibition in children with ADHD. Cognitive Therapy tance of implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology
and Research, 32, 261–280. Review, 2, 124–136.
Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2006). Cardiovascular reactivity during Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and
performance under social observation: The moderating role of task dif- goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances
ficulty. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 62, 185–192. in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119.

CH08.indd 310 12/18/09 2:40:22 PM


References 311

Gollwitzer, P. M., Sheeran, P., Michalski, V., & Seifert, A. E. (2009). When Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and
intentions go public: Does social reality widen the intention–behavior cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind.
gap? Psychological Science, 20, 612–618. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120.
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Wicklund, R. A. (1985). Self-symbolizing and the Heckhausen, J., & Heckhausen, H. (2008). Motivation and action.
neglect of others’ perspectives. Journal of Personality and Social New York: Cambridge University Press.
Psychology, 48, 702–715. Heller, D., Komar, J., & Lee, W. B. (2007). The dynamics of personal-
Gollwitzer, P. M., Wicklund, R. A., & Hilton, J. L. (1982). Admission of ity states, goals, and well being. Personality and Social Psychology
failure and symbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory. Bulletin, 33, 898–910.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 358–371. Henderson, M., de Liver, Y., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). The effects of
Gore, J. S., & Cross, S. E. (2006). Pursuing goals for us: Relationally implemental mindset on attitude strength. Journal of Personality and
autonomous reasons in long-term goal pursuit. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 94, 96–411.
and Social Psychology, 90, 848–861. Henderson, M., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Implementation
Goschke, T., & Dreisbach, G. (2008). Conflict-triggered goal shielding: intentions and disengagement from a failing course of action. Journal
Response conflicts attenuate background monitoring for prospective of Behavioral Decision Making, 20, 81–102.
memory cues. Psychological Science, 19, 25–32. Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and
Gottlieb, A. (2000). The dream of reason: A history of philosophy from the salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology:
Greeks to the Renaissance. New York: W. W. Norton. Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York: Guilford Press.
Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist,
Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269–288. 52, 1280–1300.
Gray, J. A. (1994). Three fundamental emotion systems. In P. Ekman & Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American
R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 243–247). New York: Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230.
Oxford University Press. Higgins, E. T. (2006). Value from hedonic experience and engagement.
Greenwald, A. G., Klinger, M. R., & Schuh, E. S. (1995). Activation by Psychological Review, 113, 439–460.
marginally perceptible (“subliminal”) stimuli: Dissociation of uncon- Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social perception and social cogni-
scious from conscious cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: tion. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 369–425.
General, 124, 22–42.
Hirshon, B. (2006, October 5). Washing away your sins. Science Update.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). Measuring Retrieved August 12, 2009, from http://www. scienceupdate. com/
individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association show. php?date⫽20061005.
Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.
Holland, R. W., Aarts, H., & Langendam, D. (2006). Breaking and creating
Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). habits on the working floor: A field-experiment on the power of imple-
Power and the objectification of social targets. Journal of Personality mentation intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42,
and Social Psychology, 95, 111–127. 776–783.
Guinote, A. (2007). Power and goal pursuit. Personality and Social Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H. (2005). Smells like clean spirit:
Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1076–1087. Unconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior. Psychological
Guinote, A. (2008). Power and affordances: When the situation has Science, 16, 689–693.
more power over powerful than powerless individuals. Journal of Hollenbeck, J. R., Williams, C. R., & Klein, H. J. (1989). An empiri-
Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 237–252. cal examination of the antecedents of commitment to difficult goals.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Harris, J. (2006). From psy- Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 18–23.
chological need satisfaction to intentional behavior: Testing a moti- Houser-Marko, L., & Sheldon, K. M. (2008). Eyes on the prize or nose
vational sequence in two behavioral contexts. Personality and Social to the grindstone? The effects of level of goal evaluation on mood
Psychology Bulletin, 32, 131–148. and motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34,
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuition- 1556–1569.
ist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834. Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Peak of desire: Activating the mating
Hamilton, D. L., Katz, L. B., & Leirer, V. O. (1980). Cognitive represen- goal changes life stage preferences across living kinds. Psychological
tation of personality impressions: Organizational processes in first Science, 19, 573–578.
impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-
39, 1050–1063. Century-Crofts.
Harmon-Jones, C., Schmeichel, B. J., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Hull, C. L. (1952). A behavioral system: An introduction to behavior
Symbolic self-completion in academia: Evidence from department web theory concerning the individual organism. New Haven, CT: Yale
pages and email signatures files. European Journal of Experimental University Press.
Social Psychology, 39, 311–316.
Inzlicht, M., & Gutsell, J. N. (2007). Running on empty: Neural signals for
Hassin, R. R. (2005). Unconscious control and implicit working memory. self-control failure. Psychological Science, 18, 933–937.
In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious. New
Jencks, C. (1990). Varieties of altruism. In J. J. Mansbridge (Ed.), Beyond
York: Oxford University Press.
self-interest (pp. 53–67). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hastie, R., & Park, B. (1986). The relationship between memory and
Jost, J. T., Pietrzak, J., Liviaton, I., Mandisodza, A. N., & Napier, J. L.
judgment depends on whether the judgment task is memory-based or
(2008). System justification as conscious and unconscious goal pursuit.
online. Psychological Review, 93, 258–268.
In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science
Hastorf, A. H., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case study. (pp. 591–605). New York: Guilford Press.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.
Jostmann, N. B., & Koole, S. L. (2007). On the regulation of cognitive
Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cog- control: Action orientation moderates the impact of high demands
nitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 283–329. in Stroop interference tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. New York: Springer. General, 136, 593–609.

CH08.indd 311 12/18/09 2:40:22 PM


312 Motivation

Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Erez, A., & Locke, E. A. (2005). Core self- Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological
evaluations and job and life satisfaction: The role of self-concordance Bulletin, 108, 480–498.
and goal attainment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 257–268. Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased?
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the
control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response com- National Academy of Sciences, 98, 15387–15392.
petition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. (2008). I am too just like you:
Psychology: General, 132, 47–70. Unconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: exclusion. Psychological Science, 19, 816–822.
Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Larsen, R. J., & Augustine, A. A. (2008). Basic personality dispositions
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410–422. related to approach and avoidance: Extraversion/neuroticism, BAS/
Kawada, C. L. K., Oettingen, G., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). BIS, and positive/negative affectivity. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook
The projection of implicit and explicit goals. Journal of Personality of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 151–164). Philadelphia:
and Social Psychology, 86, 545–559. Psychology Press.
Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Lavy, E. H., & van den Hout, M. A. (1993). Attentional bias for appetitive
Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of ste- cues: Effects of fasting in normal subjects. Behavioral and Cognitive
reotypic associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality Psychotherapy, 21, 297–310.
and Social Psychology, 78, 871–888.
Ledgerwood, A., Liviaton, I., & Carnevale, P. J. (2007). Group identity
Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004). Material prim- completion and the symbolic value of property. Psychological Science,
ing: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal 18, 873–878.
and competitive behavioral choice. Organizational Behavior and
Legault, L., Green-Demers, I., Grant, P., & Chung, J. (2007). On the self-
Human Decision Processes, 95, 83–96.
regulation of implicit and explicit prejudice: A self-determination theory
Kazén, M., Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Self-infiltration and self- perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 732–749.
compatibility checking in dealing with unattractive tasks and unpleas-
Lemay, E. P., Jr., & Clark, M. S. (2008). How the head liberates the heart:
ant items: The moderating influence of state vs. action-orientation.
Projection of communal responsiveness guides relationship promotion.
Motivation and Emotion, 27, 157–197.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 647–671.
Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Intention memory and achievement moti-
Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action
vation: Volitional facilitation and inhibition as a function of affective
contents of need related stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social control in frontal lobe patients. Neuropsychology, 15, 80–100.
Psychology, 89, 426–448. Lewin, K. (1926). Vorsatz, Wille, and Bedürfnis [Intention, will, and need].
Kelley, H. H. (1951). The warm–cold variable in first impressions of per- Psychologische Forschung, 7, 330–385.
sons. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 457–469. Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void. Minneapolis: University of Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Harper.
Minnesota Press. Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of aspira-
Koestner, R., Lekes, N., Powers, T. A., & Chicoine, E. (2002). Attaining tion. In McHunt (Ed.), Personality and the behaviour disorders (Vol. 1,
personal goals: Self-concordance plus implementation intention equals pp. 333–378). New York: Ronald.
success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 231–244. Lhermitte, F. (1983). Utilization behavior and its relation to lesions of the
Koo, M., & Fishbach, A. (2008). Dynamics of self-regulation: How frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.
(un)accomplished goal actions affect motivation. Journal of Personality Lhermitte, F. (1986). Human anatomy and the frontal lobes: Part II. Patient
and Social Psychology, 94, 183–195. behavior in complex and social situations: The “environmental depen-
Krizan, Z., & Windschitl, P. D. (2007). The influence of outcome desirabil- dency syndrome.” Annals of Neurology, 19, 335–343.
ity on optimism. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 95–121. Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability
Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Goals as knowledge structures. In P. M. Gollwitzer & considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal
J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action (pp. 599–618). New York: construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5–18.
Guilford Press.
Liberman, N., Foerster, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2007). Completed vs. inter-
Kruglanski, A. W., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.). (2007). Social psychology: rupted priming: Reduced accessibility from post-fulfillment inhibition.
Handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford Press. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 258–264.
Kruglanski, A. W., & Kopetz, C. (2008). The role of goal-systems in self- Liberman, N., Trope, Y., McCrea, M. S., & Sherman, S. (2007). The effect
regulation. In E. Morsella, J. A. Bargh, & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), The of level of construal on the temporal distance of activity enactment.
Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 350–367). New York: Oxford Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 143–149.
University Press.
Little, B. R. (1983). Personal projects: A rationale and methods for investi-
Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W. Y., & gation. Environment and Behavior, 15, 273–309.
Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. In M. P. Zanna
(Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 331–378). Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task
San Diego: Academic Press. performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Kuhl, J. (2000). A functional-design approach to motivation and self- Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory
regulation: The dynamics of personality systems interactions. of goal setting and task performance: A 35-year odyssey. American
In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self- Psychologist, 57, 705–717.
regulation (pp. 111–169). San Diego: Academic Press. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2006). New directions in goal-setting the-
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994). Volition and personality. Göttingen, ory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 265–268.
Germany: Hoegrefe. Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behav-
Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1999). Volitional facilitation of difficult inten- ior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65,
tions: Joint activation of intention memory and positive affect removes 272–292.
Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Loftus, E. F., & Klinger, M. R. (1992). Is the unconscious smart or dumb?
128, 382–399. American Psychologist, 47, 761–765.

CH08.indd 312 12/18/09 2:40:22 PM


References 313

Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled: Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). Elements of a developmental theory of imitation.
Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in In A. N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development,
a Stroop-like task. Memory & Cognition, 7, 166–174. evolution, and brain bases (pp. 19–41). New York: Cambridge
Louro, M. J., Pieters, R., & Zeelenberg, M. (2007). Dynamics of multiple- University Press.
goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal
174–193. and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378.
Lydon, J. E., Menzies-Toman, D., Burton, K., & Bell, C. (2008). If–then Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist,
contingencies and the differential effects of the availability of an attrac- 54, 1–8.
tive alternative on relationship maintenance for men and women. Miller, D. T., & Nelson, L. D. (2002). Seeking approach motivation in the
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 50–65. avoidance behavior of others: Implications for understanding of plu-
Lykins, E. L. B., Segerstrom, S. C., Averill, A. J., Evans, D. R., & Kemeny, ralistic ignorance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
M. E. (2007). Goal shift following reminders of morality: Reconciling 1066–1075.
posttraumatic growth and terror management theory. Personality and Milne, S. E., Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2002). Combining motivational and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1088–1099. volitional interventions to promote exercise participation: Protection
Mack, A. (2003). Inattentional blindness: Looking without seeing. Current motivation theory and implementation intentions. British Journal of
Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 180–184. Health Psychology, 7, 163–184.
Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., & DeWall, C. N. (2007). Adaptive attentional Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization
attunement: Evidence for mating-related perceptual bias. Evolution of personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252–283.
and Human Behavior, 28, 28–36. Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratification. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Delton, A. W., Hofer, B., Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 249–292).
Wilbur, C., et al. (2003). Sexually selective cognition: Beauty cap- New York: Academic Press.
tures the mind of the beholder. Journal of Personality and Social Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective
Psychology, 85, 1107–1120. processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification.
Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Robertson, T. E., Hofer, B., In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
Neuberg, S. L., Delton, A. W., & Butner, J. (2005). Functional pro- (pp. 99–129). New York: Guilford Press.
jection: How fundamental social motives can bias interpersonal Mischel, W., Cantor, N., & Feldman, S. (1996). Principles of self-
perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 63–78. regulation: The nature of willpower and self-control. In E. T. Higgins &
Mansbridge, J. J. (1990). The rise and fall of self-interest in the explana- A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic prin-
tion of political life. In J. J. Mansbridge (Ed.), Beyond self-interest (pp. ciples (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford Press.
3–22). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Mischel, W., & Patterson, C. J. (1978). Effective plans for self-control
Markman, K. D., Lindberg, M. J., Kray, L. J., & Galinsky, A. D. (2007). in children. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposium on child
Implications of counterfactual structure for creativity and analytical prob- psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 199–230). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
lem solving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 312–324. Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology:
Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social
41, 954–969. development. American Psychologist, 61, 192–203.
Mayr, E. (1976). Evolution and the diversity of life. Cambridge, MA: Monin, B., & Miller, D. T. (2001). Moral credentials and the expression of
Harvard University Press. prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 33–43.
McClelland, D. C. (1985a). How motives, skills, and values determine Morsella, E. (2005). The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular
what people do. American Psychologist, 41, 812–825. interaction theory. Psychological Review, 112, 1000–1021.
McClelland, D. C. (1985b). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Morsella, E., Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Oxford handbook of
Foresman. human action. New York: Oxford University Press.
McClelland, D. C., & Atkinson, J. W. (1948). The projective expression Moskowitz, G. B., Gollwitzer, P. M., Wasel, W., & Schaal, B. (1999).
of needs: Part I. The effect of different intensities of the hunger drive Preconscious control of stereotype activation through chronic egali-
on perception. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, tarian goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
25, 205–222. 167–184.
McCrea, S. M. (2008). Self-handicapping, excuse-making, and counter- Moskowitz, G. B., & Grant, H. (2009). The psychology of goals. New York:
factual thinking: Consequences for self-esteem and future motivation. Guilford Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 274–292. Most, S. B., Scholl, B. J., Clifford, E., & Simons, D. J. (2005). What you
McCrea, S. M., Hirt, E. R., & Milner, B. J. (2008). She works hard for see is what you set: Sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of
the money: Valuing effort underlies gender difference in behavioral awareness. Psychological Review, 112, 217–242.
self-handicapping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The benefits of posi-
292–311. tive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in close
McCrea, S. M., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Sherman, S. J. (2008). Construal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
level and procrastination. Psychological Science, 19, 1308–1314. 70, 79–98.
McCulloch, K. C., Aarts, H., Fujita, K., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Inhibition Neely, J. H. (1976). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical
in goal systems: A retrieval-induced forgetting account. Journal of memory: Evidence for facilitatory and inhibitory processes. Memory &
Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 857–865. Cognition, 4, 648–654.
McCulloch, K. D., Ferguson, M. J., Kawada, C., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory:
Taking a closer look: On the operation of unconscious impression for- Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity atten-
mation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 614–623. tion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.
McGraw, K. M. (1987). Guilt following transgression: An attribution of Neisser, U. (1979). The control of information pickup in selective look-
responsibility approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ing. In A. Pick (Ed.), Perception and its development (pp. 201–219).
53, 247–256. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

CH08.indd 313 12/18/09 2:40:23 PM


314 Motivation

Neisser, U., & Becklen, R. (1975). Selective looking: Attending to visually Paul, I., Gawrilow, C., Zech, F., Gollwitzer, P. M., Rockstroh, B.,
specified events. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 480–494. Odenthal, G., Kratzer, W., & Wienbruch, C. (2007). If–then planning
Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Maner, J. K., & Schaller, M. (2004). From modulates the P300 in children with attention deficit hyperactivity dis-
evolved motives to everyday mentation: Evolution, goals, and cogni- order. NeuroReport, 18, 653–657.
tion. In J. Forgas & K. Williams (Eds.), Social motivation: Conscious Pessiglione, M., Schmidt, L., Draganski, B., Kalisch, R., Lau, H.,
and unconscious processes (pp. 133–152). New York: Cambridge Dolan, R., & Frith, C. (2007, 12 April). How the brain translates money
University Press. into force: A neuroimaging study of subliminal motivation. Science,
Neumann, R., Förster, J., & Strack, F. (2003). Motor compatibility: The 316, 904–906.
bidirectional link between behavior and evaluation. In J. Musch & Piaget, J. (1955). The child’s construction of reality. London: Routledge
K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes and Kegan Paul.
in cognition and emotion (pp. 371–391). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–784.
Oettingen, G. (1996). Positive fantasy and motivation. In P. M. Gollwitzer &
J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and Plaks, J. E., & Stecher, K. (2007). Unexpected improvement, decline, and
motivation to behavior (pp. 236–259). New York: Guilford Press. stasis: A prediction confidence perspective on achievement success
Oettingen, G. (2000). Expectancy effects on behavior depend on self- and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 667–684.
regulatory thought. Social Cognition, 18, 101–129. Poortvliet, P. M., Janssen, O., Van Yperen, N. W., & Van de Vliert, E.
Oettingen, G., Grant, H., Smith, P. K., Skinner, M., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2007). Achievement goals and interpersonal behavior: How mastery
(2006). Unconscious goal pursuit: Acting in an explanatory vacuum. and performance goals shape information exchange. Personality and
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 668–675. Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1435–1447.
Oettingen, G., Hönig, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2000). Effective self- Popper, K. R. (1972). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach.
regulation of goal attainment. International Journal of Educational Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Research, 33, 705–732. Posner, M. J., & Snyder, C. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In
Oettingen, G., & Kappes, A. (2009). Mental contrasting of the future and R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing in cognition: The Loyola
reality to master negative feedback. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & symposium (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation
Roseth, C. J., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2008). Promoting early
(pp. 395–412). New York: Psychology Press.
adolescents’ achievement and peer relationships: The effects of coop-
Oettingen, G., & Mayer, D. (2002). The motivating function of thinking erative, competitive, and individualistic goal structures. Psychological
about the future: Expectations versus fantasies. Journal of Personality Bulletin, 134, 223–246.
and Social Psychology, 83, 1198–1212.
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation. New York:
Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., Sevincer, A. T., Stephens, E. J., Pak, H., & McGraw-Hill.
Hagenah, M. (2009). Mental contrasting and goal commitment: The
mediating role of energization. Personality and Social Psychology Rozin, P., & Geier, A. B. (2007, April 6). Want fewer fries with that? The
Bulletin, 35, 608–622. Chronicle of Higher Education, B16.
Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., & Thorpe, J. S. (in press). Self-regulation of goal Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of
commitment to reduce cigarette consumption: Mental contrasting of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172.
future and reality. Psychology and Health. Ruys, K. I., & Stapel, D. A. (2008). The secret life of emotions.
Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., Thorpe, J. S., Janetzke, H., & Lorenz, S. Psychological Science, 19, 385–391.
(2005). Turning fantasies about positive and negative futures into self- Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. (1996). All
improvement goals. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 237–267. goals are not created equal: An organismic perspective on the nature
Oettingen, G., Pak, H., & Schnetter, K. (2001). Self-regulation of goal- of goals and their regulation. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.),
setting: Turning free fantasies about the future into binding goals. The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 736–753. behavior (pp. 7–26). New York: Guilford Press.
Oettingen, G., & Stephens, E. J. (2009). Fantasies and motivationally Sassenberg, K., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2005). Do not stereotype, think dif-
intelligent goal setting. In G. B. Moskowitz & H. Grant (Eds.), The ferent! Overcoming automatic stereotype activation by mindset prim-
Psychology of goals (pp. 153–178). New York: Guilford Press. ing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 317–413.
Oettingen, G., & Wadden, T. A. (1991). Expectation, fantasy, and weight Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Faulkner, J. (2003). Prehistoric dangers and
loss: Is the impact of positive thinking always positive? Cognitive contemporary prejudices. European Review of Social Psychology, 14,
Therapy and Research, 15, 167–175.
105–137.
Orbell, S, Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997). Implementation intentions
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1987). Dispositional optimism and physi-
and the theory of planned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 23, 953–962. cal well-being: The influence of generalized outcome expectancies.
Journal of Personality, 55, 169–210.
Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2000). Motivational and volitional processes in
action initiation: A field study of implementation intentions. Journal of Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emo-
Applied Social Psychology, 30, 780–797. tion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 241–255.
Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K. (2006). Possible selves and aca-
demic outcomes: How and when possible selves impel action. Journal Schneirla, T. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory
of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 188–204. of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal.
Park, L. E., Crocker, J., & Kiefer, A. K. (2007). Contingencies of self- In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 7,
worth, academic failure, and goal pursuit. Personality and Social pp. 27–58). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1503–1517. Scholer, A. A., & Higgins, E. T. (2008). Distinguishing levels of approach
Parks-Stamm, E. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Action con- and avoidance: An analysis using regulatory focus theory. In A. J. Elliot
trol by implementation intentions: Effective cue detection and efficient (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 489– 503).
response initiation. Social Cognition, 25, 248–266. New York: Psychology Press.

CH08.indd 314 12/18/09 2:40:24 PM


References 315

Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: Bridging Smith, P. K., Jostmann, N. B., Galinsky, A. D., & van Dijk, W. W. (2008).
the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. Journal of Lacking power impairs executive functions. Psychological Science,
Personality, 67, 1–38. 19, 441–447.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, CT:
E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook Yale University Press.
of basic principles (pp. 433–465). New York: Guilford Press. Spencer, S. J., Fein, S., Wolfe, C., Fong, C., & Dunn, M. (1998). Stereotype
Schweiger Gallo, I., Keil, A., McCulloch, K. C., Rockstroh, B., & activation under cognitive load: The moderating role of self-image
Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Strategic automation of emotion control. threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1139–1152.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 11–31. Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1989). Person memory and judgment.
Sen, A. K. (1978). Rational fools: A critique of the behavioral foundations Psychological Review, 96, 58–83.
of economic theory. In H. Harris (Ed.), Scientific models and men Stadler, G., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Physical activity in
(pp. 317–344). New York: Oxford University Press. women: Effects of a self-regulation intervention. American Journal of
Shah, J. Y. (2003). Automatic for the people: How representations of sig- Preventive Medicine, 36, 29–34.
nificant others implicitly affects goal pursuit. Journal of Personality Stewart, B. D., & Payne, B. K. (2008). Bringing automatic stereotyping
and Social Psychology, 84, 661–681. under control: Implementation intentions as efficient means of thought
Shah, J. Y., & Gardner, W. L. (2008). Handbook of motivation science. control. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1332–1345.
New York: Guilford Press. Strachman, A., & Gable, S. L. (2006). What you want (and do not want)
Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else: affects what you see (and do not see): Avoidance social goals and social
On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1446–1458.
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1261–1280. Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants
Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). Performance incentives of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8,
and means: How regulatory focus influences goal attainment. Journal 220–247.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 285–293. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.
Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). When opportunity knocks: Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 643–662.
Bottom-up priming of goals by means and its effects on self-regulation. Symons, D. (1992). On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1109–1122. human behavior. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Implementation intentions and adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture
repeated behavior: Augmenting the predictive validity of the theory of (pp. 137–159). New York: Oxford University Press.
planned behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 349–369. Tamir, M. (2009). What do people want to feel and why? Pleasure and
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000). Using implementation intentions to utility in emotion regulation. Current Directions in Psychological
increase attendance for cervical cancer screening. Health Psychology, Science, 18, 101-105.
19, 283–289. Tamir, M., & Robinson, M. D. (2007). The happy spotlight: Positive mood
Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The interplay between and selective attention to rewarding information. Personality and
goal intentions and implementation intentions. Personality and Social Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1124–1136.
Psychology Bulletin, 31, 87–98. Taylor, S. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1995). The effects of mindsets on
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, positive illusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of 69, 213–226.
Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497. Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998). Harnessing
Sheldon, K. M., & Krieger, L. S. (2007). Understanding the negative the imagination: Mental simulation, self-regulation, and coping.
effects of legal education on law students: A longitudinal test of self- American Psychologist, 53, 429–439.
determination theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, Tetlock, P. E. (2002). Social functionalist frameworks for judgments
883–897. and choice: Intuitive politicians, theologians, and prosecutors.
Sheldon, K. M., & Niemiec, C. P. (2006). It’s not just the amount that Psychological Review, 109, 451–471.
counts: Balanced need satisfaction also affects well-being. Journal of Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional dis-
Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 331–341. tress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad,
Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C. (1961). do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 53–67.
Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The Robbers Cave experiment. Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York:
Norman: University of Oklahoma. Appleton-Century.
Sherman, S. J., Rose, J. S., Koch, K., Presson, C. C., & Chassin, L. (2003). Tolman, E. C. (1952). A cognition motivation model. Psychological
Implicit and explicit attitudes toward cigarette smoking: The effects of Review, 59, 389–400.
context and motivation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005).
22, 13–39. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cogni-
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human tion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–691.
information processing: Part II. Perceptual learning, automatic attend- Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of cul-
ing, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190. ture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted
Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inat- mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture
tentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074. (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.
Sinclair, L., & Kunda, Z. (2000). Motivated stereotyping of women: She’s Trötschel, R., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2007). Implementation intentions
fine if she praised me but incompetent if she criticized me. Personality and the willful pursuit of prosocial goals in negotiations. Journal of
and Social Psychological Bulletin, 26, 1329–1342. Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 579–589.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Trope, Y., & Fishbach, A. (2000). Counteractive self-control in overcoming
Macmillan. temptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 493–506.

CH08.indd 315 12/18/09 2:40:25 PM


316 Motivation

Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re Werner, H. (1956). Microgenesis and aphasia. Journal of Abnormal Social
doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Psychology, 52, 347–353.
Review, 94, 215–228. Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality Wieber, F., & Sassenberg, K. (2006). I can’t take my eyes off of it:
and Social Psychology, 57, 660–671.
Attention attraction of implementation intentions. Social Cognition,
Vasquez, N. A., & Bühler, R. (2007). Seeing future success: Does imagery 6, 723–752.
perspective influence achievement motivation? Personality and Social
Winter, D. G. (1991). A motivational model of leadership: Predicting long-
Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1392–1405.
term management success from TAT measures of power motivation
Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley. and responsibility. Leadership Quarterly, 2, 67–80.
Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm’s theory of motivation as a model of effort and
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206. cardiovascular response. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The
Watson, J. B. (1912). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior
Review, 20, 158–177. (pp. 424–453). New York: Guilford Press.
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation intentions help to Wright, R. A., Murray, J. B., Storey, P. L., & Williams, B. J. (1997). Ability
overcome ego-depletion? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, analysis of gender relevance and sex differences in cardiovascular
39, 279–286. response to behavioral challenge. Journal of Personality and Social
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Identifying good opportunities to act: Psychology, 73, 405–417.
Implementation intentions and cue discrimination. European Journal Wright, R. A., Tunstall, A. M., Williams, B. J., Goodwin, J. S., & Harmon-
of Social Psychology, 34, 407–419. Jones, E. (1995). Social evaluation and cardiovascular response.
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 530–543.
engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evi- Zhang, Y, Fishbach, A., & Dhar, R. (2008). When thinking beats doing:
dence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249–268. The role of optimistic expectations in goal-based choice. Journal of
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2007). How do implementation intentions Consumer Research, 34, 567–578.
promote goal attainment? A test of component processes. Journal of Zhang, Y., Fishbach, A., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2007). The dilution model:
Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 295–302. How additional goals undermine the perceived instrumentality of
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Mechanisms of implementation intention a shared path. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,
effects: The role of goal intentions, self-efficacy, and accessibility of 389–401.
plan components. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 373–395. Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006, September 8). Washing away your sins:
Weiner, B. (1992). Human motivation. London: Sage. Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 1451–1452.

CH08.indd 316 12/18/09 2:40:25 PM


Chapter 9

Emotion
DACHER KELTNER AND JENNIFER S. LERNER

Conceptions of human nature derive from beliefs about fictional causes to which we ascribe behavior” and “use-
human emotion. Are humans competitive and aggressive less and bad for our peace of mind and our blood pressure”
by nature or cooperative and kind? Do people seek to max- (p. 92). The cognitive revolution that followed behaviorism
imize personal desire or to enhance the welfare of others? made significant progress in showing how cognitive pro-
What is the nature of human rationality? What is the path cesses could fully account for seemingly “hot” emotional
to the good life? Answers to these age-old questions hinge processes such as prejudice, attraction, and group hatred.
on an understanding of the emotions. In the last 30 years, however, a robust science of emotion
Western constructions of emotions have been guided by has emerged, one that appears to represent a paradigm shift
the Romanticism thesis, which dates back to Plato and found in thinking about human nature (Damasio, 1994; Davidson,
its clearest expression in the writings of Rousseau (Oatley, Scherer, & Goldsmith, 2003; Frank, 1988; Keltner, 2009;
Keltner, & Jenkins, 2006; Solomon, 1976). The romanticism LeDoux, 1996). The sources of this new science of emotion
thesis holds that emotions are powerful, involuntary forces are numerous. Developmental psychologists began to docu-
and that the experience of emotion guides patterns of rea- ment emotions as central to parent–child attachment and to
soning, self-expression, and social behavior that are vital children’s navigation of their environment before language
to healthy social communities. For many theorists, such as acquisition (Barrett & Campos, 1987; Campos, Campos, &
Kant, the power of emotions, in particular to shift reason- Barrett, 1989; Cohn & Tronick, 1983). Neurological stud-
ing in context-specific fashion, necessitated that emotions ies of split-brain patients discovered that one hemisphere of
play minor roles in moral judgment, ethical conduct, and the brain (the right) preferentially responds to the emotional
social organization. For others, such as Rousseau, Hume, content of stimuli (Gazzaniga, 1985). Ethological studies
and Darwin, these same properties qualified emotions as of different hunter-gatherer cultures revealed striking uni-
a source of moral intuition and ethical behavior and thus versalities in the expression of emotion and the prominent
deserving of a privileged place in social life. place of emotions in social interaction (Eibl-Eibesfeldt,
Founding figures in psychology—Darwin and James— 1989; Konner, 2003). And in the early 1980s, cognitive
focused their theoretical energies on the nature of emotion. psychologists began to document how emotions shape
The psychological science that would follow, however, would memory (Bower, 1981), as well as judgment and decision
have little systematic to say about emotion (this is only the sec- making (Isen, 1987; Schwarz & Clore, 1983).
ond Handbook of Social Psychology chapter devoted to the The study of emotion now claims a central position in
topic). During the heyday of behaviorism and learning social psychology (e.g., Tiedens & Leach, 2004; Zajonc,
theory, emotions resided inside the proverbial “black box 1998). As social-psychological studies have advanced an
of the mind,” outside the purview of observable measure- understanding of the emotions, they have yielded answers
ment and undeserving of scientific inquiry. Reacting against to some of the age-old questions about human nature.
drive-based, psychodynamic approaches to the human This review focuses on the classic and contemporary ques-
mind, B. F. Skinner (1948) inveighed that emotions “are the tions that frame recent findings in this new science of

This chapter was prepared with support from the National Science Foundation (PECASE SES0239637), the National Institute of Mental
Health, the Russell Sage Foundation, the Metanexus Institute, and the Fetzer Institute.

317
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
318 Emotion

emotion. The first section addresses definitions of emotion Emotions, which represent a third level of analysis, are
from taxonomic, cross-cultural, and functional perspectives. assumed to be briefer, more context specific, and more
The second section considers the extent to which emotions focused on a particular cause or intentional object than on
are universal and culturally specific. The next two sec- positive moods and traits (Ekman, 1992; Schwarz, 1990).
tions ask how emotions are shaped by cognitive and social How specific positive emotions, such as gratitude, differ
processes and, in turn, shape cognitive and social processes. from related mood-like experiences, such as feeling appre-
In the concluding section, the contributions emotions make ciative, and coalesce into trait-like tendencies, such as a
to happiness are considered. grateful disposition, has emerged as an essential question
for the field (e.g., McCullough et al., 2002).
Sensory experiences of pleasure and pain represent the
WHAT IS AN EMOTION? final, most specific level of analysis (Kahneman, 1999).
Sensory experiences, such as an itch or the taste of a Belgian
In 1884, James famously titled an essay in the journal Mind, beer, are likely to have unique temporal dynamics (e.g.,
“What Is an Emotion?” A consensual answer to this ques- Fredrickson & Kahneman, 1993) that, when filtered through
tion has been hard to reach. Debates have flared regarding an individual’s social goals and aspirations, can evolve into
which emotions are “basic,” that is, elemental, evolved, emotional experiences. For example, Rozin (1996) has
and genetically encoded in the nervous systems, and which offered an evolutionary account of how “distaste” for nox-
emotions are secondary, or constructed from other emotions ious smells and tastes (a sensory experience) evolves into
(Ekman, 1992; Ortony & Turner, 1990). More recently, “disgust,” an emotion felt toward contaminating actions and
controversies have arisen regarding the boundaries between traits.
emotions and whether or not (or to what extent) emo- Building on this basic conceptualization of emotion,
tions are discrete entities or “natural kinds” (Barrett, 2006; Table 9.1 presents definitions of emotion from leading fig-
Ekman & Davidson, 1994; Keltner, Ekman, Gonzaga, & ures in the field, including those who have studied emo-
Beer, 2003; Panksepp, 1998). tions through the lens of cultural constructivist assumptions
One answer to James’s question is to differentiate emo- (e.g., Lutz & White, 1986; Frijda & Mesquita, 1994) and
tions from other kinds of affective experience. The consen- evolutionary theory (Ekman, 1992; Tooby & Cosmides,
sus now is that affective experiences, which involve feelings 1990). Across these definitions, differences emerge: Many
of good or bad (Russell, 2003), can be studied at four lev- prioritize the physiological basis of emotion, for example,
els of analysis (Kahneman, 1999; Rosenberg, 1998). At the but some do not. Areas of convergence also appear; most
broadest level, emotional traits refer to general styles of theorists assume that emotions orient people to respond to
emotional responses that persist across context and time ongoing events in their environment and that in this sense,
(Larsen & Ketelaar, 1989; Shiota, Keltner, & John, 2006). emotions are “relational.” In addition, these theorists tend to
For example, people who experience trait-like gratitude assume that emotions involve multiple responses. Thus, any
tend to feel grateful at different points and in different situ- attempt to answer James’s question, “What is an emotion?”
ations throughout their lives (McCullough, Emmons, & must consider the multiple components of emotion.
Tsang, 2002). People who report such trait-like tenden-
cies for particular emotions show a subjective, expressive,
Components of Emotion
and physiological profile that closely resembles that of the
momentary emotion or emotional state (Gross, Sutton, & In The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals,
Ketelaar, 1998; Lazarus, 1991; Malatesta, 1990). Darwin (1872/1998) presented his principle of serviceable
At a second level of analysis, moods are assumed to be habits, which holds that the expressions of emotion that peo-
longer lasting, less focused on a particular cause, and less ple observe today derive from habitual patterns of behavior
context bound than specific emotions (Watson & Tellegen, that proved useful in the evolution of humans’ mammalian
1985). For example, anger, an emotion, has a specific predecessors. For example, sneering is a behavioral vestige of
source (a slight at work or a humiliating comment from snarling and preparing to bite, and affectionate embrace and
a spouse), whereas irritability, a mood, is less defined by a soothing touch evolved from patterns of protective physical
clear cause or intentional object. Numerous studies have contact.
characterized the temporal stability of positive and nega- Darwin’s rich descriptions of more than 30 emotions
tive moods and their covariation with personality traits, presage the notion that emotions involve multiple bodily
such as extraversion and agreeableness, and subjective systems (Gross, 1998; Levenson, 1999). Darwin himself
well-being (Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005; Watson & focused on facial muscle action, vocalization patterns, ges-
Clark, 1992). ture, postural shifts, and gaze activity, as well as autonomic
What is an Emotion? 319

Table 9.1 Definitions of Emotion responses such as facial coloring and fainting (see Table 9.2
Source Definition for his analyses of four emotions). Contemporary analyses
of emotion presuppose that emotions involve not only dis-
James, 1884 “My thesis. . . is that the bodily
changes follow directly the play behavior and physiological response but also specific
perception of the exciting fact, appraisals and action tendencies, conceptual knowledge,
and that our feeling of the same language, and experience.
changes as they occur is the
emotion.”
In general, empirical studies of the components of emo-
tion have engaged in the surprisingly complex task of
Arnold & Gasson, 1954 “An emotion or an affect can be
considered as the felt tendency characterizing the response profiles of different emotions
towards an object judged (which in Darwin’s eyes would reveal their evolution-
suitable, or away from an ary origins). This descriptive task translates to empirical
object judged unsuitable,
reinforced by specific bodily attempts to answer two questions. First, within a response
changes.” system, say facial muscle action, how do the emotions dif-
Lutz & White, 1986 “Emotions are a primary idiom fer? Ultimately, this kind of work speaks to the question of
for defining and negotiating how many emotions are signaled, for example, in the face
social relations of the self in a
and voice, or registered in specific patterns of physiologi-
moral order.”
cal response. Second, how do the components of emotion
Barrett & Campos, 1987 “We conceive of emotions
as bidirectional processes of
relate to subjective experience? Data relevant to this ques-
establishing, maintaining, tion inform age-old questions about the subjective qual-
and/or disrupting significant ity of affective experience, which is considered in a later
relationships between
an organism and the (external
or internal) environment.”
Table 9.2 Darwin’s Descriptions of Four Emotions
Tooby & Cosmides, 1990 “An emotion corresponds
to a distinctive system of Emotion Expressive Behaviors
coordination among the
mechanisms that regulate each Devotion (reverence) Face upward, eyelids upturned,
controllable biological process. fainting, pupils upward and
That is, each emotional state inward, humbling kneeling
manifests design features posture, hands upturned
‘designed’ to solve particular Laughter Tears, deep inspiration,
families of adaptive problems, contraction of chest, shaking
whereby psychological of body, head nodding to and
mechanisms assume unique fro, lower jaw quivering up
configuration.” and down, lip corners drawn
Lazarus, 1991 “Emotions are organized backward, head thrown
psychophysiological reactions backward, shaking, head and
to news about ongoing face red, muscle around eyes
relationships with the contracted, lip pressing and biting
environment.” Rage Uncovered teeth, hair bristled,
Ekman, 1992 “Emotions are viewed as face reddened, chest heaving,
having evolved through their nostrils dilated, quivering,
adaptive value in dealing trembling, teeth clenched,
with fundamental life-tasks. respiration labored, gestures
Each emotion has unique frantic, veins on forehead and
features: signal, physiology, neck distended, body erect, bent
and antecedent events. Each forward, rolling on ground and
emotion also has characteristics kicking, screaming (children),
in common with other emotions: brow furrowed, glaring,
rapid onset, short duration, protruding lips, retracted lips,
unbidden occurrence, automatic tossing arms about, shaking fist,
appraisal, and coherence among hissing
responses.” Terror (intense fear) Pallor, nostrils flared, gasping,
Frijda & Mesquita, 1994 “Emotions . . . are, first and gulping, protruding eyeballs,
foremost, modes of relating to pupils dilated, hands clenched
the environment: states or opened, arms protruded,
of readiness for engaging, sweat, prostration, body relaxed,
or not engaging, in eyebrow corners tightened and
interaction with that raised, upper eyelids raised, lip
environment.” corners pulled sideways
320 Emotion

section. Within the literatures on the different components By contrast, dimensional approaches to appraisal pre-
of emotion, theoretical issues, tensions, and advances have suppose that core dimensions of appraisal, when combined,
arisen that frame the ensuing reviews. give rise to specific emotions (e.g., Ellsworth & Smith,
1988; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). In their review of numer-
Emotion-Eliciting Appraisals: Discrete Versus ous studies of the semantic content of emotions, Smith and
Dimensional Approaches and the Automaticity Ellsworth (1985) derived eight dimensions that capture the
of Emotion appraisal processes that lead to various emotions (see also
Scherer, 1997). These appraisal dimensions can be thought
Emotions are rooted in appraisals. At the most general of as the basic units of meaning that people ascribe to events
level, emotion appraisals involve evaluative judgments of (see Table 9.3).
whether an event is good or bad and whether people’s cur- Guided by dimensional approaches, studies of emotion-
rent actions and environment correspond to their personal related recall (Ellsworth & Smith, 1988; Smith & Ellsworth,
goals and expectations (Carver & White, 1994; Davidson, 1985) have documented that each emotion is defined by a
2004; Higgins, 1997; Russell, 2003). The study of emotion- fairly distinct pattern of appraisal (for critiques of this meth-
eliciting appraisals, or the “meaning making” processes odology, see Parkinson & Manstead, 1992). For example,
that give rise to different emotions (Clore & Ortony, 2008; interest is associated with appraisals of increased pleasant-
Roseman, 1991, 1984; Roseman, Spindel, & Jose, 1990; ness, the desire to attend, the sense that situational factors
Roseman, Wiest, & Swartz, 1994; Scherer, 1997; Scherer & are producing events, a perceived need to expend effort,
Wallbott, 1994; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985), was the intellec- moderate certainty about future outcomes, and little sense
tual offspring of two literatures: (1) research on stress and of obstacles or the illegitimacy of events.
health, particularly Lazarus’s (1991) reframing of specific Moreover, certain appraisal dimensions are central to the
stresses as emotion appraisals, and (2) the study of attribu- differentiation of clusters of emotions (Smith & Ellsworth,
tion, achievement motivation, and emotion (Weiner, 1985) 1985). For example, agency, a combination of control and
and its documentation that successes and failures could responsibility, differentiates anger, sadness, and guilt. In the
lead to different emotions depending on how outcomes are face of a negative event, blaming others produces anger,
interpreted. believing that the situation is responsible produces sadness,
Discrete approaches to emotion appraisals focus on the and self-blame produces guilt (see also Weiner, 1985).
coherent themes, or core-relational themes, in Lazarus’s Dimensional accounts of emotion appraisal have gen-
words (1991), that give rise to the experience of emo- erated several lines of inquiry. They identify mechanisms
tions and that differentiate emotions from one another. For
example, people feel anger when they appraise an unjusti-
fied offense against themselves or someone close; they feel
compassion when they feel moved to help someone who is Table 9.3 Dimensions of Emotion-Related Appraisal
suffering. These core-relational themes are thought to result Dimension Definition
from two stages of appraisal: in the first, people appraise
Attention Degree to which you focus on
whether an event is congruent or incongruent with their and think about the stimulus
goals; in the second, people consider a causal attribution Certainty Degree to which you are certain
for the event, potential responses, and future consequences about what is going to happen
of different courses of action (e.g., Folkman & Lazarus, Control coping Extent to which you have control
1989). over outcomes in the environment
Discrete approaches to appraisal help to illuminate Pleasantness Degree that the event is positive
sources of individual variation in emotion—for example, or negative
why an angry person appraises ongoing events in ways that Perceived obstacle Extent to which the pursuit of
lead to a life rife with frustration and hostility (Rosenberg, your goals is blocked

1998). Discrete emotion-eliciting appraisals can be cap- Responsibility Extent to which other people,
you, and situational factors are
tured in spontaneous discourse and relate to emotion- responsible for events
specific experiences and facial expressions (Bonanno &
Legitimacy Extent to which the event is
Keltner, 2004). Yet discrete approaches to appraisal fail to fair and deserved or unfair and
yield simple explanations of the similarities among emo- undeserved
tions (e.g., between anger and fear) and do not readily explain Anticipated effort Extent to which you must expend
rapid transitions between emotional states (Ellsworth, energy to respond to the event
1991). Adapted from Smith & Ellsworth, 1985.
What is an Emotion? 321

by which emotions influence different cognitive processes negatively valenced stimuli may be of greater magnitude
(see the section on emotion and reason) and pinpoint likely than those of positive stimuli.
emotional processes associated with different central ner- Inspired in part by this literature, LeDoux (1996) argued
vous system regions (see Davidson, Pizzagalli, Nitschke, & that the amygdala, an almond-shaped region in the mid-
Kalin, 2003; Ochsner, 2008). For example, the experience brain, is centrally involved in generating primary apprais-
of anger involving high levels of agency has been associ- als that give rise to emotion before conscious recognition
ated with activation in the left-frontal regions of the cortex, and categorization of a stimulus (for a recent review, see
an area of the brain thought to facilitate approach-related Phelps, 2006). He grounds this claim in neuroanatomy: the
behavior (Harmon-Jones, Sigelman, Bohlig, & Harmon- amygdala receives inputs from sensory processing systems
Jones, 2003). Dimensional accounts also illuminate likely via the thalamus that are faster than the inputs that arrive
areas of cultural variation in emotion-related appraisals. For from the hippocampus and cortex, where the semantic
example, based on how cultures vary in their conceptions classification of the stimulus may originate. Once acti-
of human agency (Morris & Peng, 1994), similar events are vated, the amygdala, via projections to the hypothalamus,
likely to trigger different emotions in members of different regulates emotion-related behavior through activation of
cultures, probably because of differences in appraisal. the autonomic nervous system, a branch of the peripheral
Discrete and dimensional approaches both assume that nervous system considered in detail later (Adolphs, Tranel,
emotion-eliciting appraisals begin with simple appraisals Damasio, & Damasio, 1994; Aggleton, 2000).
and proceed to complex meaning-making attributions. Moreover, recent human imaging studies indicate that
Along these lines, a critical question that has emerged con- the amygdala is involved in primary evaluative appraisals
cerns automaticity: which emotion-eliciting appraisals are (Baxter & Murray, 2002). The amygdala (along with other
automatic—that is, fast, beyond deliberative control, and brain regions) becomes more active in response to sad film
preconscious—and which are more deliberative, controlled clips (Levesque et al., 2003), erotic film clips (Beauregard,
processes. Inspired by Zajonc’s theorizing (1980), research- Levesque, & Bourgouin, 2001), disturbing slides (Phan,
ers now widely assume that an automatic, preconscious Wager, Taylor, & Liberzon, 2004), and unpleasant tastes
appraisal produces an evaluation of whether a stimulus and odors (Zald, 2003). People show increased amygdala
is good or bad (LeDoux, 1996; Mischel & Shoda, 1995; activation to faces of individuals from ethnic groups other
Russell, 2003; Winkielman, Zajonc, & Schwarz, 1997). This than their own (Hart et al., 2000), and amygdala activation
system gives rise to automatic affective reactions that moti- predicts whether people will recall emotionally evocative
vate rapid approach or avoidance responses and core feelings stimuli (Canli, Zhao, Desmond, & Gabrieli, 1999).
of positivity or negativity (Barrett, 2006; Russell, 2003). The literature on emotion appraisals is rich in theo-
Indeed, rapid exposure below conscious awareness to retical development, but several areas of inquiry await
certain classes of stimuli triggers primary appraisals and empirical attention. Given critiques of self-report mea-
core positive or negative feelings. Zajonc found that expo- sures as assessments of online appraisals (Parkinson &
sure to positive or negative stimuli at presentation rates that Manstead, 1992), methods are needed to study the con-
prevented conscious recognition of the stimulus colored tents of appraisal processes as they occur. In addition, new
participants’ evaluations of subsequently presented stimuli questions have arisen concerning the semantic content of
(Murphy & Zajonc, 1993). Subliminally presented photos of primary appraisals: Are primary appraisals attuned to the
smiles or anger displays trigger emotion-related facial expres- valence of a stimulus, its novelty, its salience, or its inten-
sions, subjective experience, and physiological response in sity? Are other dimensions, such as agency, involved in
the perceiver (Dimberg & Öhman, 1996; Öhman & Dimberg, automatic, primary appraisals? Can discrete emotions be
1978). In one study, snake phobics, but not control partici- generated through automatic appraisals? To what extent
pants, showed a galvanic skin response when presented with do primary appraisals give rise to conscious experiences
photos of snakes so quickly that the images could not be con- (Clore & Ortony, 2008; Winkielman, Knutson, Paulus, &
sciously recognized (Öhman & Soares, 1994). Trujillo, 2007)? Answers to these questions will shed light
Subsequent work has begun to characterize the stimulus on how emotions arise.
features that generate positive or negative primary apprais-
als. Positive as opposed to negative appraisals are generated Signaling Behavior: New Emotions, New Signaling
by smooth rather than sharp-edged objects, light rather than Systems, and the Grammar of Social Interaction
dark objects, high rather than low pitch (Huron, 2006), and
high versus low physical location (Clore & Ortony, 2008). In The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals,
Ito, Larsen, Smith, and Cacioppo (1998) have documented Charles Darwin limned the expressive signatures of doz-
that brain activity associated with primary appraisals of ens of emotions (Darwin, 1872/1998; Matsumoto, Keltner,
322 Emotion

Shiota, O’Sullivan, & Frank, 2008). He drew comparisons negative emotions do not tend to possess these temporal
between human and nonhuman emotional display to chal- and morphological properties and are less reliable indica-
lenge creationists’ claims that God graced humans with tors of positive states.
special facial muscles that allow us to express uniquely Initial encoding studies documented distinct facial
human emotions. expressions of anger, disgust, fear, sadness, surprise, and
The study of signaling behavior has enabled the devel- happiness (Ekman, Friesen, & Ancoli, 1980; Hess, Banse, &
oping science of emotion (Ambady, this volume, Ekman, Kappas, 1995; Matsumoto, 1987). More recently, encoding
1993). Comparisons of human and nonhuman emotional studies have measured gaze, head, and postural activity to
display reveal the evolutionary origins of specific emo- chart the displays of other emotions. Embarrassment is sig-
tions, for example, that embarrassment evolved out of naled by gaze aversion, a controlled smile, head turns (typi-
appeasement processes in nonhuman primates (Beer, cally down and to the left), and face touches (Harris, 2001;
Heerey, Keltner, Knight, & Scabini, 2003; Keltner & Keltner, 1995; Miller & Leary, 1992). A brief pattern of
Buswell, 1997; Miller & Leary, 1992), that laughter and smiling, mutual gaze, affiliative hand gestures, open posture,
smiling evolved out of distinct affiliative displays in other and forward leans predicts increased self and partner reports
primates (Preuschoft, 1992; van Hooff, 1972), and that of romantic love, but not desire, as well as peripheral oxy-
human emotion vocalization resembles other mammals’ tocin release (Gonzaga, Keltner, Londahl, & Smith, 2001;
vocalizations related to food, sex, affiliation, caretaking, Gonzaga, Turner, Keltner, Campos, & Altemus, 2006). In
and play (Snowdon, 2003). Studies of the perception and contrast, this same research finds that sexual desire is sig-
the production of emotional display have been central naled in puckers, lip licks and wipes, and tongue protru-
to affective neuroscience (Keltner, Ekman, et al., 2003). sions. Pride is reliably signaled with expansive posture, head
Emotional displays serve as building blocks for social movements up and back, and upward arm thrusts (Tracy &
relationships—a theme of the section on the social con- Matsumoto, 2008; Tracy & Robins, 2004). Sympathy corre-
struction of emotion. lates with oblique eyebrows, forward leans, and concerned
The study of emotional display is a descriptive enterprise: gaze (Eisenberg et al., 1989), although this pattern of behav-
encoding studies ascertain which signaling behaviors covary ior is not reliably decoded as sympathy (Haidt & Keltner,
with which distinct experiences, and decoding studies deter- 1999).
mine whether observers can make accurate inferences about Dozens of decoding studies have likewise documented
emotion from nonverbal displays (Ekman, 1993; Ekman, that several facial expressions of emotion—the six origi-
Friesen, & Ellsworth, 1982; Keltner, 1995; Matsumoto et al., nally investigated by Ekman and colleagues (1982), as well
2008). Initially, and controversially, studies of emotional as contempt, embarrassment, laughter, pride, shame, love,
display largely focused on displays of five negative emo- desire, and awe—are reliably judged by observers, at least
tions in the face and on one display of positive emotion, the in prototypical forms (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Haidt &
smile. In the last 15 years, previously unstudied emotions Keltner, 1999; Keltner, 1995; Tracy & Robins, 2004).
(e.g., love, pride, gratitude, and embarrassment) have been
Vocal Displays of Emotion
shown to have distinct displays, and signaling systems
such as touch have been investigated, revealing emotional Few species communicate with as much precision and mean-
displays to be central to the formation and maintenance of ing as humans do with the voice: people tease, laugh, exhort,
relationships. flirt, criticize, soothe, and engage the attention of infants
with subtle and brief variations in vocal tone (Bachorowski,
Facial, Postural, and Gestural Displays of Emotion 1999; Bachorowski & Owren, 2001; Scherer, 1986).
The 30 to 40 facial muscles beneath the skin’s surface are Researchers can capture more than 20 acoustic properties
involved in numerous actions: breathing, eating, swallow- of speech, including speech rate and fluency, number of syl-
ing, speaking, prosodic vocalization, and expressing emo- lables per second, syllable duration, number and duration
tion. Of the tens of thousands of possible configurations of of pauses, pitch and pitch variability and range, perceived
the facial muscles, a limited set express emotion (Ekman, tempo, loudness, and perceived rhythm (Bachorowski,
1993). Emotion-specific facial muscle movements tend to 1999; Scherer, Johnstone, & Klasmeyer, 2003).
be brief (lasting between 1 and 5 seconds), symmetrical, Most evidence regarding the vocal display of emotion
and hard to produce voluntarily (Dimberg, Thunberg, & derives from decoding studies (Banse & Scherer, 1996;
Grunedal, 2002; Ekman, 1993; Frank, Ekman, & Friesen, Scherer et al., 2003). In one line of research, people, often
1993). A genuine smile of pleasure, for example, is marked trained actors, express different emotions in the voice
by these properties and serves as a reliable indicator of while reading nonsense syllables or relatively neutral text.
positive emotion; by contrast, polite smiles that mask Other studies have had participants communicate emotions
What is an Emotion? 323

through vocal bursts, which are brief, nonword utterances facial expressions, vocalizations, posture, gesture, gaze, and
that arise between speech incidents, such as shrieks, groans, touch. In this research, various emotions, including grati-
or sighs. tude, pride, shame, embarrassment, love, desire, sympathy,
This research has revealed the human voice to be rich and awe, have emerged as objects of empirical inquiry.
with information about emotion. In recent reviews of more The characteristics of displays of such emotions suggest
than 60 studies of this kind, Juslin and Laukka (2003) con- their evolutionary origins—for example, the likelihood that
cluded that hearers can judge five emotions in the voice— shame and pride originated in nonhuman displays of domi-
anger, fear, happiness, sadness, and tenderness—with nance and submission (Tracy & Matsumoto, 2008).
accuracy rates that approach 70% (see also Scherer et al.,
2003). More recent studies have documented similarly high Autonomic Physiology:
rates of identification of emotion vocal bursts conveying The Controversial Search for Emotion Specificity
admiration, achievement, amusement, boredom, contempt,
contentment, elation, pleasure, and relief (Sauter & Scott, James’s own answer to his question “What is an emotion?”
2007). has proven to be one of the most controversial (Cacioppo,
Berntson, Larsen, Poehlmann, & Ito, 2000; Cacioppo, Klein,
Tactile Displays of Emotion Berntson, & Hatfield, 1993; Larsen, Berntson, Poehlmann,
Touch is central to species-characteristic patterns of sooth- Ito, & Cacioppo, 2008; Levenson, 1992, 2003). James argued
ing, flirtation, greeting, play, and proximity maintenance that emotions are defined by distinct “reverberations in the
(Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989; Hertenstein, 2002). With the evolu- viscera,” or patterns of activation in the autonomic nervous
tion of the skin and the increasing dexterity of the hand in system. This claim translates to two hypotheses: first, that
humans, touch took on several functions related to emotion. each emotion is associated with a distinct activation pattern
First, touch soothes. Married women anticipating an electric in peripheral physiology, and second, that the experience of
shock showed decreased threat-related activity in the brain emotion is based on the interoception of peripheral physi-
when holding the hand of a spouse but not that of a stranger ological response.
(Coan, Schaefer, & Davidson, 2006). Rat pups that are han- James’s thesis is anatomically plausible (see Table 9.4).
dled extensively by their mothers (rat dams) show reduced The autonomic nervous system involves approximately 20
levels of corticosterone, a stress-related hormone, in the bundles of neurons originating in the spinal cord that receive
bloodstream, later in development (Francis & Meaney, signals from regions of the cortex, the amygdala, and the
1999). Second, touch rewards and punishes: the experi- hypothalamus and that activate different target organs,
ence of gentle touch triggers activation in the orbitofrontal glands, muscles, and blood vessels distributed throughout
cortex, a brain region involved in the representation of sec- the body. Notably, the autonomic nervous system controls
ondary rewards (Rolls, 2000). Touch also signals safety several responses that people routinely report during emo-
and danger to developing infants (Hertenstein, Verkamp, tional experiences: tears, dry mouth, goose bumps, blushing,
Kerestes, & Holmes, 2006). Finally, touch enables recipro- fainting, increased blood pressure, sexual arousal, changes
cal altruism (De Waal, 1996); in humans, friendly patterns in breathing, and cooling or heating up of the skin, to name
of touch increase compliance to requests (Willis & Hamm, just a few.
1980) and cooperation toward strangers in economic games Are autonomic responses emotion specific? Hypotheses
(Kurzban, 2001). regarding this question can be arrayed on a continuum
Touch conveys a great deal of information about emo- (Larsen et al., 2008; Levenson, 1992), with James’s thesis of
tions. In one study, an encoder (or toucher) and decoder autonomic specificity on one end and, on the other, the claim
(or touchee) sat at a table separated by a black curtain, which that there is no emotion specificity (e.g., the early claims of
prevented all communication other than touch (Hertenstein, Cannon, 1927, and Schachter and Singer, 1962). This latter
Keltner, App, Bulleit, & Jaskolka, 2006). With brief touches view might hold, for example, that all negative emotions
to the forearm, participants in the United States and Spain involve elevated activation in the sympathetic branch of
could reliably communicate anger, disgust, fear, love, sym- the autonomic nervous system, which involves responses
pathy, and gratitude (but not pride, embarrassment, or sad- such as increased heart rate and patterns of vasoconstric-
ness) at levels of accuracy comparable to those observed in tion that enable fight-or-flight behavior (see Table 9.4
studies of the face and voice. for other responses). Resolving this question would require
In summary, assuming that specific signal behavior is the study of many indexes of autonomic activation and
a defining characteristic of emotion (Ekman, 1992; Izard, many emotions. Although empirical data lag in this area,
1971), the recent science of emotional display reveals a recent studies suggest that different regions of the auto-
large array of states that can be readily signaled in fleeting nomic nervous system covary with different emotions.
324 Emotion

Table 9.4 Effects of the Activation of the Parasympathetic and Moving Facial Muscles Generates Autonomic
Sympathetic Branches of the Autonomic Nervous System
Responses That Differentiate Negative Emotions
Activation of
In one set of studies, Levenson, Ekman, and Friesen (1990)
Parasympathetic compared patterns of autonomic activity (largely sympathetic)
Activation of Organ Nerves Sympathetic Nerves
associated with the facial muscle configurations associated
Heart muscle Decrease of heart Increase of heart rate with anger, disgust, fear, and sadness (Ekman, Levenson, &
rate Friesen, 1983). Moving muscles into these configurations
Decrease of Increase of (known as the directed facial action task) often triggered the
contractility contractility
experience of the target emotion and some degree of auto-
Blood vessels: nomic specificity. Large increases of heart rate occurred for
arteries
fear, anger, and sadness but not for disgust. Finger tempera-
Trunk, limbs 0 Vasoconstriction
ture was greater for anger than for fear, suggesting that anger
Skin of face Vasodilation Vasoconstriction
is associated with increased blood flow to the hands (perhaps
Visceral domain 0 Vasoconstriction to aid in combat), whereas blood remained near the chest dur-
Skeletal muscle 0 Vasoconstriction ing experiences of fear, presumably to support flight-related
Erectile tissue Vasodilation Vasoconstriction locomotion (a finding not consistently replicated across stud-
Cranium 0 Vasoconstriction ies; see Cacioppo et al., 2000).
Blood vessels: 0 Vasoconstriction
veins
Blush
Gastrointestinal tract
The blush involves the spontaneous reddening of the face,
Circular muscle Increased motility Decreased
motility ears, neck, and upper chest produced by increased blood
Sphincters Relaxation Contraction
volume in the subcutaneous capillaries in those regions
(Leary, Britt, Cutlip, & Templeton, 1992). By contrast,
Urinary bladder Contraction Relaxation
a flush is a nonsocial response associated with physical
Reproductive
organs
exertion, temperature changes, or alcohol consumption
(Leary et al., 1992). Mark Twain’s famous observation—
Seminal vesicles 0 Contraction
“Man is the only animal that blushes. Or needs to.”—is
Vas deferens 0 Contraction
not quite apt: Some nonhuman primates show reddening
Uterus 0 Contraction
in the face, perhaps as an appeasement gesture (Hauser,
Pupil Constriction Dilation 1996). Twain was prescient, however, in highlighting the
Tracheobronchial Contraction Relaxation centrality of the blush to human social life (Leary et al.,
Muscles 1992; Miller, 1996, 2004).
Piloerector muscles 0 Contraction The situations that produce the blush, which range from
Salivary glands Strong secretion Weak secretion the proverbial faux pas to sudden exposure of the body,
Lachrymal glands Secretion 0 involve negative, self-focused attention (Leary et al., 1992).
(tears) People report that they are more likely to blush when embar-
Sweat glands 0 Secretion rassed than when feeling shame or guilt (Miller & Tangney,
Digestive Secretion Decreased 1994). Shearn, Bergman, Hill, Abel, and Hinds (1990) have
glands secretion documented that the blush associated with embarrassment is
Metabolism distinct from the autonomic profile of fear. In this research,
Liver 0 Glycogenolysis participants’ cheek blood flow and cheek skin temperature
Fat cells 0 Free fatty acids in increased more when, in the presence of four confederates,
blood they were embarrassed by a videotape of themselves sing-
Pancreas Secretion of insulin Decreased secretion ing “The Star-Spangled Banner” than when they watched
of insulin the frightening shower scene from the film Psycho (see also
Adrenal medulla 0 Secretion of Shearn, Bergman, Hill, Abel, & Hinds, 1992).
adrenaline,
noradrenaline
Taken together, the findings from the directed facial action
Lymphoid tissue 0 Depression of
task and studies of the blush rebut a one-arousal-fits-all
activity (e.g., of model of autonomic activity and negative emotions (see
natural killer cells) also Stemmler, 1989). All negative emotions do not involve
Adapted from Janig, 2003. a similar pattern of elevated sympathetic nervous system
What is an Emotion? 325

arousal; meaningful differences in autonomic response are negative films clips and accompanying high levels of vagal
observed among fear, anger, disgust, and embarrassment. response (Gruber, Johnson, Oveis, & Keltner, 2008).

Parasympathetic Response and Positive Emotion Emotion and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal


Axis Activation, Oxytocin, and Immune System
Early studies of emotion and autonomic nervous system
Response
activity could be charged with a fight-or-flight bias—a focus
on anger, fear, and sympathetic activation. How are posi- Specific emotions also appear to map onto specific neuroen-
tive emotions embodied in autonomic response? One possi- docrine and immune system responses (Kemeny & Shestyuk,
bility is that positive emotions covary with the cessation of 2008). One active area of inquiry concerns the hypotha-
elevated sympathetic autonomic response (Tomkins, 1984), lamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and the stress-related
a claim that has some empirical support (Fredrickson & hormone cortisol. The HPA axis is regulated by neurons in
Levenson, 1998). New studies suggest that activation of the the hypothalamus and the amygdala. Through the release of
vagus nerve—a branch of the parasympathetic autonomic hormones, these regions of the brain stimulate the adrenal
nervous system—may be involved in positive emotion glands, which release cortisol into the bloodstream, to act
(Porges, 1998). on organs to facilitate fight-or-flight responses to stress. A
Based on comparisons of the autonomic nervous systems recent meta-analysis of 208 studies found that the stress-
of different species, from fish species to humans, Porges ful events that most robustly trigger the release of cortisol
(1998) has made a case for three stages in the evolution involve negative self-evaluations (Dickerson & Kemeny,
of the autonomic nervous system, which evolved differ- 2004). Lerner, Dahl, Hariri, and Taylor (2007) demonstrated
ent bundles of neurons to enable the increasingly complex that fearful facial expressions, but not angry and disgusted
social behavior of different species. A first stage produced expressions, were associated with increased cortisol in
the dorsal vagal complex, located in the brainstem, which is response to a stressful task.
present in all reptiles and mammals. It regulates basic pro- What cortisol is to fear-related stress, oxytocin is to
cesses common to reptiles and mammals, including diges- attachment-related affect (Taylor, 2002; Taylor, Klein,
tion and immobilization responses when attacked. Next Lewis, Gruenewald, Gurung, & Updegraff, 2000). Oxytocin
to emerge in phylogenetic evolution was the sympathetic is a peptide of nine amino acids that is produced in the hy-
nervous system, which emerges in several neural bundles pothalamus and released into both the brain and the blood-
in the middle of the spinal cord and controls fight-or-flight stream. Receptors for this peptide are found in the olfactory
behavior. The last portion of the autonomic nervous system system, limbic–hypothalamic system, brainstem, and regions
to evolve, and only in mammals, is the ventral vagal com- of the spinal cord that regulate the autonomic nervous
plex. It is controlled by the 10th cranial nerve, known as system, especially the parasympathetic branch (Morberg,
the vagus nerve. As Porges points out, the vagus nerve also 2003). Oxytocin is involved in uterine contractions, lactation,
controls several behaviors critical to social interaction and maternal bonding, and sexual interaction (Carter, 1998).
attachment, such as facial muscle actions, head movements, In nonhuman species, oxytocin regulates pair-bonding
and vocalizations. The ventral vagal complex also influ- and caregiving behavior. Comparisons between prairie voles,
ences cardiac output in ways that allow people to rapidly which display pair-bonding, and closely related montane
adapt to changing social circumstances and, in particular, voles, which do not, have revealed differences in the location
in ways that allow people to be calm and in close proximity of oxytocin receptors in the brains of each species (Carter,
with others. 1998; Insel, 1993; Insel, Young, & Zuoxin, 1997). Injections
Researchers measure activation in the vagus nerve by of oxytocin or oxytocin agonists into these two species
first assessing heart rate and then filtering out respiratory promote preferences for single partners or nonmonoga-
and sympathetic influences to yield an index of parasym- mous behavior, respectively (Williams, Insel, Harbaugh, &
pathetic influence on heart rate (Berntson, Cacioppo, & Carter, 1994). Injections of oxytocin increase attachment-
Quigley, 1993). Select empirical studies suggest that acti- related behaviors in primates (Holman & Goy, 1995),
vation in the vagus nerve may be associated with posi- voles (Witt, Carter, & Walton, 1990), and rats (Nelson &
tive emotion. Resting cardiac vagal tone was associated Panksepp, 1996).
with increases in spontaneous positive emotion during the Oxytocin influences the attachment-related emotions,
Rorschach test (Kettunen, Ravaja, Naatanen, & Keltikangas- such as love or compassion, by reducing anxiety (Carter &
Jarvinen, 2000) and increased reports of positive emotion Altemus, 1997; Taylor et al., 2000) and making social
in response to emotionally evocative film clips (Oveis et al., contact pleasant (Insel et al., 1997; Panksepp, 1998). In
2009). College students prone to mania reported extremely studies of lactating women, for example, oxytocin reduced
high levels of positive emotion in response to positive and the activity of the HPA axis (Carter & Altemus, 1997).
326 Emotion

Soothing touch and sexual behavior lead to the release of judgments of ensuing stimuli, and identification of emotion-
oxytocin (Murphy, Seckl, Burton, Checkley, & Lightman, related concepts (Niedenthal, Barsalou, Winkielman,
1987). Participants playing a trust game who received Krauth-Gruber, & Ric, 2005). In other words, activation
oxytocin nasally were more than twice as likely as com- of one component of an emotion (specific facial muscles)
parison participants to trust strangers (Kosfeld, Heinrichs, triggers activation of emotion concepts. Drawing on this
Zak, Fishbacker, & Fehr, 2005). Gonzaga and colleagues framework, the study of emotion knowledge offers a win-
(2006) have documented that nonverbal displays of love, dow onto the study of how emotions are constructed.
but not sexual desire, covary with oxytocin release. One layer of emotion knowledge is a culture’s emo-
tion lexicon. Words used to describe emotions clarify
Proinflammatory Cytokines the intentional object—or perceived cause—of an experi-
Metaphors often describe emotions as kinds of disease— ence (Schwarz, 1990). Many words and concepts used
“I’m sick with love,” for example, or “I’m dying of envy.” to describe emotions have a rich metaphorical content.
New research on the immune system suggests a biological Emotions have been represented as natural forces (“being
basis to these metaphors. Proinflammatory cytokines (PICs) swept away”), as opponents (“we wrestle with anger”), as
are released in immunological cells to enable immune diseases (“sick with grief ”), as fluids (“bubbling over with
response and to activate “sickness behaviors”—increased joy”), and as animals or living organisms (“my love will
sleep and inhibited social, exploratory, and aggressive wither and die”; Kövesces, 2003; Lakoff & Johnson, 1980).
behaviors (Kemeny & Shestyuk, 2008). These behaviors The emotion lexicon can be organized into concepts
resemble the submissive behaviors seen in species other and categories (Romney, Moore, & Rusch, 1997; Shaver,
than humans, suggesting that PICs might be systematic- Schwartz, Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987). At the superordinate
ally involved in the submissive emotions, such as shame. level, emotion knowledge distinguishes between positive
Consistent with this thesis, Dickerson, Gruenewald, and and negative, or good and bad. At the next, basic level of
Kemeny (2004) found that induced shame was associated knowledge, emotion concepts, such as love, joy, surprise,
with increases in PICs whereas guilt and other negative anger, sadness, and fear, are formed. In light of the literature
emotions were not. on prototypes and language use (Rosch, 1973), one might
The debate over emotion specificity in peripheral physi- expect such words to be most readily used in the descrip-
ology has evolved in its methods and answers (Levenson, tion of emotional experience. At the subordinate level of
2003). The empirical data suggest that general arousal mod- analysis, more specific states exist; for example, the basic
els of emotion and autonomic and neuroendocrine response emotion concept “love” embodies love, compassion, lust,
are inadequate (Kemeny & Sheystuk, 2008; Levenson, and longing.
2003), as the autonomic responses for high-arousal nega- People also represent emotional experiences in narra-
tive emotions (fear and anger) differ in discernible ways. tives or scripts (e.g., Johnson-Laird & Oatley, 1989; Russell,
The blush covaries with self-conscious emotion but not fear. 1991; Shaver et al., 1987). Emotion narratives tend to take
Positive emotions may preferentially activate the vagus the form of prototypes, with lists of more central and more
nerve. Prosocial emotions may map onto oxytocin release peripheral features of the narrative: characteristic causes,
and submissive emotions onto immune-related responses. thoughts, feelings, actions, and expressive signs and con-
Several other branches of the autonomic nervous system— sequences (e.g., see Table 9.5, which portrays a prototype
goose bumps and activation in the digestive and sexual for sadness). Narrative data are often a first step in differ-
organs, for example—await empirical attention. These entiating emotions; for example, they have clarified distinc-
studies, and the research they anticipated, provide prelimi- tions among embarrassment, shame, and guilt (Keltner &
nary support for James’s counterintuitive speculations. Buswell, 1996; Miller, 1992; Miller & Tangney, 1994;
Parrott & Smith, 1991; Tangney, Miller, Flicker, & Barlow,
1996). Humans construct emotion narratives in other modal-
Language, Concepts, and Discourse:
ities, including through visual art (Oatley et al., 2006), music
The Construction of Emotion
(Juslin & Laukka, 2003), dance (Hejmadi, Davidson, &
Emotions are not only experiences embodied in the periph- Rozin, 2000), and fiction and poetry (Oatley, 2003).
eral branches of the nervous system but also experiences that Finally, people actively represent emotions in emotion
people represent with language, concepts, and discourse— discourses, or acts of communication that take the form of
or what is known as emotion knowledge (Niedenthal, 2008). gossip, teasing, jokes, satire and irony, songs, and poetry
As evidence of this emotion knowledge, simply moving (Abu-Lughod, 1986; Griffin, 1994; Heath, Bell, & Sternberg,
emotion-related facial muscles influences categorization 2001; Lutz, 1990). Researchers have made headway in illumi-
of other facial expressions, recall of emotional memories, nating how emotion is represented in discourse in children’s
What is an Emotion? 327

Table 9.5 A Prototype of Sadness (Roberts & Pennebaker, 1995) and greater attunement
Features of Emotion Specific Elements of Sadness to the emotions of others (Hall, Carter, & Horgan, 2000).
Discourses about emotion, this first literature suggests,
Causes Death, loss, not getting what one
wants channel women and men into different emotional styles that
Feelings Helpless, tired, run down, slow
place them into different roles within the social moral order
(Citrin et al., 2004; Fischer, 2000; Tiedens, Ellsworth, &
Expression Drooping posture, saying sad
things, crying, tears Mesquita, 2000).
Thoughts Blaming, focusing on and A second literature centers on the question of how emo-
criticizing self, irritable tion representations shape the impact of powerful emotional
Actions Negative talk to others, taking events on social adjustment. Dozens of studies have docu-
action, suppressing negative mented that expressing deeply emotional, often traumatic
feelings, disposing of present
experiences in emotion-centered writing yields health
possessions and acquiring new
ones benefits (Pennebaker, 1989, 1997; Pennebaker & Seagal,
Adapted from Cryder, Lerner, Gross, & Dahl, 2008; Lerner, Gonzalez, Small, & 1999; Pennebaker, Mehl, & Niederhoffer, 2003). People
Fischoff, 2003; Shaver et al., 1987. who write about the most difficult emotions associated with
bereavement, divorce, the experience of earthquakes, and
the attacks of September 11, 2001, compared with people
books (Tsai, 2007; Tsai, Louie, Chen, & Uchida, 2007), music who write in more factual fashion about the same trauma,
(Juslin & Laukka, 2003; Snibbe & Markus, 2005), advertise- benefit in myriad ways. They are less likely to visit the
ments (Tsai, 2007), parenting manuals (Shields, 1991), and doctor, they experience more life satisfaction, they show
teasing (Keltner, Young, Oemig, Heerey, & Monarch, 1998). enhanced immune function, they report fewer absentee
In the most general sense, expressing emotions through days at work or school, and perform better in school if they
language brings many benefits, such as relationship build- are college students (Pennebaker, 1997).
ing (Clark & Finkel, 2004); indeed, people seem almost Expressing emotion in written form enables people
reflexively inclined to share their emotions with others to reflect on their emotions, to look at them from an out-
(Rimé, Finkenauer, Luminet, Zech, & Philippot, 1998; side perspective, and to gain insight into the causes and
Rimé, Mesquita, Philippot, & Boca, 1991). How do con- implications of emotional experience (Pennebaker, 1997).
cepts, words, narratives, and discourses shape emotional Similarly, putting feelings into words reduces the anxiety
response? Three recent, distinct literatures seek differ- and uncertainty associated with an emotion (Wilson &
ent answers to this age-old question, which dates back to Gilbert, 2008) by specifying its causes and relevance to the
Aristotle’s ideas about how the dramatic expression of emo- self (Keltner, Locke, & Audrain, 1993; Wilson & Brekke,
tion leads to catharsis, or insight into the nature of one’s 1994; Wilson, Centerbar, & Brekke, 2002). Representing
emotions (Oatley et al., 2006). emotional experiences in words (e.g., through reappraisal
The first literature, which originates in claims about the instructions) reduces the sympathetic autonomic arousal
social construction of emotion, holds that representations of associated with emotional suppression (Gross, 1998) and
emotion channel individuals into identity-based profiles activates frontal lobe regions of the brain (e.g., the ven-
of emotional response (e.g., Abu-Lughod, 1986; Briggs, tromedial prefrontal cortex), which down-regulate limbic-
1970). As an example, consider how emotion is gendered in based emotional responses (Ochsner, 2008). Representing
emotional discourse (Citrin, Roberts, & Fredrickson, 2004; negative emotions from an abstract perspective (focusing
Shields, 1991). Mothers talk about emotions, with the excep- on why an event occurred rather than how) reduces stress-
tion of anger, more with daughters than with sons (Fivush, related cardiovascular response (Ayduk & Kross, 2008).
1991). These different emotion discourses socialize girls and Rumination, by contrast, offers no perspective, no distance,
boys into different patterns of emotional response. Females no third-person perspective on emotional events, and it tends
report higher levels of other-oriented positive emotions to prolong experiences of emotion, both negative and posi-
(e.g., love) than males, who in turn report higher levels of tive (Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995; Morrow &
achievement-oriented emotions than women, such as pride, Nolen-Hoeksema, 1990).
that separate self from other (Shiota et al., 2006). Females A third area of inquiry asks whether people’s represen-
are assumed to express more submissive emotions, such as tations of emotional events capture experiences in the past
embarrassment, while males are assumed to express more or future. Emotion representations removed temporally
dominant emotions, such as anger (Plant, Hyde, Keltner, & from actual experiences often fail to capture the content or
Devine, 2000). Women systematically show greater sensi- duration of that experience. For example, people anticipate
tivity to social contextual cues when interpreting emotion experiencing more intense emotions associated with a future
328 Emotion

event than they recall experiencing them in retrospect (Van Levenson, McCarter, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2005). Cross-cultural
Boven & Ashworth, 2007). studies of emotion experience are hindered by the difficulty
The literature on affective forecasting reveals that peo- of finding equivalent terms to capture the feeling of interest
ple mispredict the impact of emotional events on their well- across cultures.
being (Gilbert, Lieberman, Morewedge, & Wilson, 2004; Several theories have attempted to explain subjective
Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998). emotional experience (Barrett et al., 2007; Lambie & Marcel,
People routinely under- and overestimate the influences of 2002; Reisenzein, 1983). One school of thought follows
emotional events—such as a breakup with a romantic part- bottom-up assumptions: The experience of emotion closely
ner or the failure of an academician to get tenure—on their tracks somatovisceral changes in the musculature of the
well-being. Lay theories about the impact of emotional body or in different peripheral physiological systems (e.g.,
events overlook the human capacity to respond with resil- Damasio, 1994; Matsumoto, 1987). According to this view,
ience (Gilbert et al., 1998) as well as the effects of other emotional experience guides social action, with somatovi-
events on personal satisfaction (Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, sceral changes serving as input into the online assessment
Gilbert, & Axson, 2000). of the individual’s adaptation to the environment (for analo-
Studies of the recollection of emotion reveal similar dis- gous argument about self-representation, see Sedikides &
junctions between emotion representation and actual expe- Skowronski, 1997).
rience. People under- or overreport past emotions in ways This approach presupposes fairly sensitive interorecep-
that fit their current circumstances (Levine & Pizarro, 2004; tive processes, an assumption that has been challenged
Levine & Safer, 2002). Bereaved individuals’ reports of past since Cannon (1927) first critiqued James’s account of auto-
grief, for example, were more highly correlated with their nomic specificity. It also assumes that emotional experience
current grief than with actual levels of the past grief being closely tracks activation in bodily movement, facial muscle
reported on (Safer, Bonanno, & Field, 2001). Romantic contraction, or changes in peripheral physiology. More than
partners who had become more attached to their partner 20 studies do indicate that experiences of specific emotions
over time recalled having more positive initial evaluations (e.g., anger, disgust, embarrassment, love, and desire) covary
of their partner than was actually the case, while those who with emotion-specific facial muscle movements (Gonzaga
became less attracted to their partner over time recalled et al., 2001; Hess et al., 1995; Keltner & Bonanno, 1997;
initial feelings that were more negative than they actually Matsumoto, 1987; Ruch, 1995; for review, see Matsumoto
experienced (McFarland & Ross, 1987). et al., 2008). However, studies of the covariation between
peripheral physiology and emotional experience yield less
coherent results: some find associations between auto-
Experience of Emotion: Bottom-Up and
nomic response and emotional experience (e.g., Dickerson
Top-Down Processes
et al., 2004; Eisenberg et al., 1989; Shearn et al., 1990), but
Ironically, the component that may well define emotion— many others do not (Cacioppo et al., 2000). Given more
subjective experience—is in serious need of rigorous data refined measures of peripheral physiology and a focus on
and theoretical development (Barrett, Mesquita, Ochsner, & more specific emotions, one might expect more robust and
Gross, 2007). On the one hand, numerous self-report mea- precise associations to arise between emotional experience
sures of emotional experience have been validated, including and behavioral and physiological responses.
measures of global positive and negative moods (Watson, A second, more top-down approach to emotional expe-
Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), anger (Spielberger, 1996), shame rience holds that the experience of emotion is more of
and guilt (Tangney, 1990), embarrassment (Miller, 1995), a conceptual act, constructed in top-down, knowledge-
fear (Spielberger, 1983), gratitude (McCullough et al., 2002), based processes grounded in language and representation
and various positive emotions (Shiota et al., 2006). In addi- (e.g., Barrett, 2006; Russell, 2003). Rapid, primary appraisals
tion, methods have been developed to capture the online, of a stimulus’ goodness or badness, or harm or benefit, trigger
in-the-moment experience of emotion—for example, with a diffuse “core affect” that causes an individual to experience a
experience sampling techniques (Bolger, Davis, & Refaeli, broad, valenced emotional state. In this view, more specific
2003). Yet the empirical study of emotional experience faces emotions (such as sadness, guilt, compassion, or love) arise
enormous challenges. As considered earlier, self-reports of from situation-specific interpretations and categorizations.
emotion are prone to powerful memory biases (Levine &
Pizarro, 2004; Safer et al., 2001), and several measures of Summary of the Components of Emotion
emotion (e.g., autonomic physiology, facial expression,
and self-report) inconsistently correlate with one another The scientific answer to James’s question “What is an
(e.g., Lang, Greenwald, Bradley, & Hamm, 1993; Mauss, emotion?” is that emotions are complex, multidimensional
What is an Emotion? 329

phenomena. Emotions involve appraisals, some automatic, Higgins, 1997) and the tendency to avoid or approach (e.g.,
which give rise to distinct experiences. More than 15 emo- Davidson, 2000, 2004). This theoretical synthesis necessar-
tions are signaled in different facial, postural, vocal, and ily simplifies, and the complexities that are ignored present
tactile behavior, which in their forms and variations provide interesting avenues of empirical inquiry.1 The taxonomies
clues to the evolution of emotion and the influence of culture neglect the many nuances of states within a particular emotion
on emotional response. Clusters of emotion involve specific category; for instance, empirical and theoretical treatments
autonomic, neuroendocrine, and immune system responses. have identified many forms of disgust (Rozin, 1996),
Emotions are represented in a rich language of words, meta- embarrassment (Tangney, 1992), and awe (Keltner & Haidt,
phors, discourses, and theories that reveals how cultures 2003). Relations among the subtypes of an emotion repre-
construct emotions and how expression shapes emotional sent an important area for future theoretical development
response. Although the experience of emotion can be readily (see Rozin, 1996). The taxonomies heuristically posit single
measured, it remains mysterious, as contrasting theoretical appraisal and action tendencies for each emotion, when in
perspectives can seem plausible. actuality, emotional experience is certain to involve com-
plex combinations of appraisal and action tendencies.
Taxonomy of Emotions and Emotion Functions Importantly, much of emotional experience, perhaps even
most, involves experiences of mixtures of emotions (Larsen,
The study of emotion has historically emphasized taxono- McGraw, Mellers, & Cacioppo, 2004; Schimmack, Oishi, &
mies, with a particular focus on examining which affective Diener, 2002). A better understanding of these mixed emo-
states should be considered emotions. In an early wave tions requires precise measures of distinct emotions.
of emotion research and theory (e.g., Ortony, Clore, & As evident in Tables 9.6 and 9.7, the field of emotion
Collins, 1988; Ekman & Davidson, 1994), scholars devel- has evolved dramatically in the last 20 years. With these
oped criteria to answer this question, for example, that an developments, a consensus has emerged that emotions serve
emotional state be brief and involuntary and that it should important functions; most typically, emotions enable indi-
have a distinct eliciting appraisal, signal, and physiological viduals to meet particular threats, challenges, and oppor-
profile (Ekman, 1992). The field focused on six or seven tunities within their social environment (Consedine, 2008;
negative emotional states and a general state of happiness. Keltner & Gross, 1999; Mesquita, 2003; Oatley & Jenkins,
As the field of emotion has evolved, researchers have turned 1992). Within an evolutionary framework, it is assumed
their attention to new states, such as the self-conscious emo- that emotions enable individuals to meet specific problems
tions (e.g., Kemeny & Sheystuk, 2008; Tangney & Fischer, and opportunities that increase their chances of physical
1995) and the positive emotions (Bartlett & DeSteno, 2006; survival, reproduction, and gene replication (Keltner &
Fredrickson, 1998, 2001; Shiota et al., 2006). Haidt, 2001; Nesse, 1990; Simpson & Kenrick, 1998;
Tables 9.6 and 9.7 synthesize theoretical claims about
negative emotions and positive emotions, respectively. The
definitions focus on two components of each emotion. 1 In one example of a possible oversimplification, decades of
The first is an appraisal tendency for the emotion, or the research and theorizing have classified anger as a negative emo-
core subjective meaning of each emotion, which should tion (Lazarus, 1991). Yet Lerner and Tiedens (2006) have argued
relate systematically to the content of emotional experience that anger does not follow many typical patterns associated with
and the influence of emotions on cognitive processes such as negative emotions. For example, rather than triggering pes-
causal attribution, memory, risk assessment, and expectation. simism, it triggers optimism about one’s outcomes (Lerner &
Because emotions dispose individuals toward goal-based Keltner, 2000, 2001; Lerner et al., 2003), and rather than trig-
actions (e.g., Fridlund, 1992; Frijda, Kuipers, & ter Schure, gering careful thought, it triggers careless thought (Bodenhausen,
1989), the second component is an action tendency for each Kramer, et al., 1994; Lerner et al., 1998; Tiedens & Linton,
emotion (Frijda, 1986; Rodriquez Mosquera, Fischer, & 2001). Anger even resembles happiness in terms of hemispheric
laterality; both state and trait anger are associated with relatively
Manstead, 2004) or the organizing principle that motivates
greater left frontal cortical activity than right frontal activity (for
specific signaling behaviors, as well as supportive physiolog-
a review, see Harmon-Jones et al., 2003). To reconcile these find-
ical response (Levenson, 2003).
ings, Lerner and Tiedens (2006) proposed assessing the positivity
The emotions in Tables 9.6 and 9.7 are classified as of anger across a temporal dimension. Specifically, they pro-
negative and positive, respectively, based on the scientific posed that anger would be experienced as relatively unpleasant
consensus that has arisen concerning the valence of their and unrewarding when reflecting back on the source of one’s
experience (Russell, 2003). The negative or positive valence anger but may be experienced as relatively pleasant and reward-
of the emotion most typically reflects relative progress (or ing when looking forward due to the belief that one can change
lack thereof) in meeting goals (Carver & White, 1994; the situation for the better.
330 Emotion

Table 9.6 A Synthesis of Theoretical Accounts of Negative Emotion Table 9.7 A Synthesis of Theoretical Accounts of Positive Emotion

Emotion Appraisal Tendency Action Tendency Emotion Appraisal Tendency Action Tendency

Anger Offense against self 1 Restore justice, Contentment Pleasing stimulus1 Savoring 2,3
hold individuals Enthusiasm Reward likely1 Goal approach 4
responsible 2–4
Love Perceived Affection6
Contempt Other violates role, Lower the reputation commitment 5
duty, obligation5 of perpetrator
Sexual desire Sexual cue or Sexual release
Disgust Contact with impure Push away 6–8 opportunity 7,8
object or action6
Compassion Undeserved Prosocial approach11
Embarrassment Self has transgressed Apologize10 suffering9,10
a social convention9
Gratitude Unexpected gift Promote
Envy Other is superior to Reduce status of reciprocity 12,13
self11 other
Pride Self-relevant Status display
Fear Imminent threat to Flee, reduce achievement 14
self12 uncertainty13,14
Awe Self is small vis-à- Devotion, reverence16
Guilt Self has violated Remedy harm vis something
moral standard vast, beyond current
regarding harm15 understanding15
Jealousy Other threatens Protect source of Interest Novel opportunity 17 Exploration18
source of affection16 affection from others
Amusement Recognize Play 20
Sadness Irrevocable loss17 Acquire new incongruity 19
goods7,13,18
Relief Cause of distress Signal safety
Shame Self has transgressed Hide, avoid scrutiny ends21
aspiration or ideal19
Source: 1. Berridge, 2003; 2. Fredrickson, 1998; 3. Wood, Heimpel, & Michaela,
Source: 1. Lazarus, 1991; 2. Lerner et al., 1998; 3. Small & Lerner, 2008; 4. Small,
2003; 4. DePue & Collins, 1999; 5. Gonzaga et al., 2001; 6. Hazan & Shaver, 1987;
Lerner, & Fischoff, 2006; 5. Rozin, Lowery, Imada, & Haidt, 1999; 6. Rozin &
7. Diamond, 2003; 8. Buss, 1992; 9. Davidson & Harrington, 2001; 10. Nussbaum,
Fallon, 1987; 7. Lerner, Small, & Loewenstein, 2004; 8. Han, Lerner, & Zeckhauser,
1996; 11. Taylor, 2002; 12. McCullough, Kilpatrick, Emmons, & Larson, 2001; 13.
2008; 9. Miller & Tangney, 1992; 10. Keltner & Buswell, 1997; 11. Salovey, 1991;
Trivers, 1971; 14. Tracy & Robins, 2007; 15. Keltner & Haidt, 2003; 16. Woodruff,
12. Öhman, 1986; 13. Raghunathan & Pham, 1999; 14. Tiedens & Linton, 2001;
2002; 17. Izard, 1977; Reeve, 1989; Silvia, 2005; 18. Panksepp, 1998; 19. Ruch, 1993;
15. Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1994; 16. DeSteno & Salovey, 1996; 17.
20. Pelligrini, 1992; 21. Tomkins, 1984.
Lazarus, 1991; 18. Cryder et al., 2008; 19. Tangney, 1991.

Tooby & Cosmides, 1990). Within a cultural constructiv- individual (e.g., Clore, 1994; Johnson-Laird & Oatley, 1989;
ist framework, researchers assume that emotions help to Schwarz & Clore, 1983). For example, they signal whether
reify and embody important facets of cultures, such as a situation is benign or dangerous, albeit sometimes incor-
roles, ideologies, and values (Abu-Lughod, 1986; Citrin rectly. These informative properties of the subjective quality
et al., 2004). Finally, functional arguments add an impor- of emotion are thought to be guides for specific courses of
tant interpretive context for understanding the origins, pur- action (Damasio, 1994; Frijda, 1988).
pose, and design of different components of emotion, such At the level of the dyad, emotions provide rapid and reli-
as signal behavior, autonomic response, or the subjective able information to others and thus coordinate brief social
feeling of emotion (e.g., Levenson, 1999). interactions (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989; Keltner & Kring, 1998;
Functionalist analyses open many areas of inquiry. Why Kring, 2008; Öhman, 1986). Emotional displays provide
do humans have emotions? What are the deepest origins of information about stimuli in the environment and about
emotion in biological and cultural evolution? What are the others’ states and dispositions; they also serve as incentives
systematic dysfunctions associated with excesses or defi- and elicitors of action.
cits in emotion (e.g., Keltner & Kring, 1998; Rottenberg & Third, at the level of the group, emotional experiences
Johnson, 2007)? Table 9.8 synthesizes hypotheses concern- and displays help to define group roles, boundaries, and
ing the functions of emotions at four levels of analysis (see identities within and across groups (Citrin et al., 2004;
Keltner & Haidt, 2001; Fischer & Manstead, 2008). First, at Mackie, Devos, & Smith, 2000; Mackie, Silver, & Smith,
the individual level of analysis, the subjective experience of 2004; Rodriguez Mosquera et al., 2004). In one recent line of
an emotion and its accompanying memories and cognitive empirical inquiry, Cortes, Demoulin, Rodriguez, Rodriguez,
tendencies signal particular conditions in the world to the and Leyens (2005) found that group members define their
Universals and Cultural Variations in Emotion 331

group in contradistinction to others by attributing more psychological science, from conceptions of psychological
complex or “secondary” emotions (e.g., shame and com- disorder to judgment and decision making.
passion) to the ingroup than to the outgroup. The emotional
display and experience of status-relevant emotions, such as
embarrassment and contempt, shape how individuals nego- UNIVERSALS AND CULTURAL
tiate rank within social hierarchies (e.g., Hall, Coats, & VARIATIONS IN EMOTION
LeBeau, 2005; Tiedens et al., 2000).
Finally, at the cultural level of analysis, emotions embody To ascertain whether members of different cultures express
cultural values, concerns, and ideologies (e.g., Rodriquez emotion in universal fashion, Darwin sent queries to 36
Mosquera et al., 2004). The experience and expression of missionaries stationed in different corners of the British
sympathy, for example, are imbued with commitments to Empire, asking whether they had seen emotional expres-
culturally proscribed values regarding caretaking and gen- sions unknown to Victorian England. They had not.
der identity (Lutz, 1990). The likelihood that an individual When anthropologist Lutz (1988) did her ethnographic
smiles with affection is shaped by gender identity and the research with the Ifaluk, a people on a Micronesian island,
commitment to being oriented toward others (LaFrance & she documented radically different meanings of emotion:
Banaji, 1992). The experience, enactment, and expression a child’s enthusiasm was not encouraged, as in the West,
of specific emotions reflect an engagement with a set of but frowned on for its impertinence and immodesty. These
cultural values and commitments. Deviations from cultur- contrasting observations highlight a central tension in the
ally valued emotions, by implication, are likely to lead to study of emotion: How are emotions universal, and how do
feelings of cultural disengagement, anomie, and depres- they vary across cultures (for reviews, see Mesquita, 2001,
sion (Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, 2006). 2003; Mesquita & Frijda, 1992)? This area of research
If we were to review the empirical literature on emotion has engaged founding figures in the field, from Darwin to
only 25 years ago, we would have found numerous stud- Asch (see Keltner, Ekman, et al., 2003, for history), and
ies of facial expressions that concentrated on a limited set been the source of impassioned debate (e.g., Ekman, 1994;
of emotions: anger, disgust, fear, sadness, surprise, and Russell, 1994).
happiness. Since then, the field of emotion has expanded The study of cultural variations and universality in
dramatically, incorporating dozens of states, measures of emotion brings into focus contrasting predictions of evo-
multiple systems, and theories of many specific emotions lutionist and constructivist approaches to emotion, sum-
(see Fredrickson, 1998; Keltner & Haidt, 2003; Rozin & marized in Table 9.9 (see Abu-Lughod, 1986; Hochschild,
Fallon, 1987; Tracy, Robins, & Tangney, 2007). Studies of 1983, 1990; Keltner & Haidt, 1999; Mesquita, 2003;
emotion have made inroads in every conceivable area in Oatley, 1993; Oatley & Jenkins, 1992). Evolutionists and
constructivists alike start from the assumption that emo-
tions are solutions to basic problems of social living. From
there, the approaches diverge in essential ways. Within an
Table 9.8 Functions of Emotion at Four Levels of Analysis evolutionary framework, emotions are genetically encoded
Level of Analysis Functions biological processes that emerged in hominid evolution
Individual Inform individual of problems or
as adaptations to problems or opportunities specific to
opportunities the environment of evolutionary adaptedness. Emotions
Prepare individual for action
Dyadic Signal mental states
Table 9.9 A Comparison of Evolutionary and Cultural Approaches
Reward or punish prior action
Evoke complementary or Evolutionary
reciprocal behavior Question of Interest Approach Cultural Approach

Group Define group boundaries and What is an emotion? Genetically encoded Language, discourse,
members beliefs, roles
Define group roles and identities What are the origins of Environment Practices,
emotions? of evolutionary institutions, values
Motivate collective action
adaptedness
Culture Define cultural identity
Function Individual: Action Reify roles, values
Identify norms and values readiness
Reify cultural ideologies and Dyadic: Social Reify identities,
power structures coordination ideologies
332 Emotion

are species-characteristic patterns of action and therefore This distinction between potential and practice illumi-
universal. nates how emotion-eliciting appraisals are both univer-
For constructivists, emotions are words, concepts, rep- sal and culturally variable. At the most abstract level of
resentations, and metaphors. Emotions are forms of dis- analysis, the appraised antecedents of emotion are similar
course that emerge within culturally specific institutions, across cultures (Mauro, Sato, & Tucker, 1992; Mesquita &
values, technologies, narratives, and social practices. What Ellsworth, 2001; Mesquita & Frijda, 1992; Scherer, 1997).
is most striking are pronounced cultural differences in emo- For example, in an early study, young Americans and
tion that reflect culturally specific concerns about identity, Malaysians described events that made them feel emotions
morality, and social structure (Averill, 1980; Mesquita, such as fear, disgust, and joy (Boucher & Brandt, 1981).
2003; Shweder & Haidt, 2000). New participants from both cultures showed high levels of
As empirical data have been gathered, the field has agreement in predicting which emotions would be produced
moved from either–or assertions about universality and cul- by these events, even for events generated by individuals
tural variation (e.g., Haidt & Keltner, 1999; Mesquita, 2003; from a different culture. There appears to be a high degree
Rodriguez Mosquera et al., 2004; Russell, 1991, 1994). New of universality in the core appraisals that, in the abstract,
evidence suggests that some emotions, such as shame or give rise to emotions such as anger, embarrassment, or grat-
sympathy, may prove to be more variable across cultures than itude in different cultures.
other emotions, such as anger and disgust (Haidt & Keltner, In practice, cultural variations in the actual events that
1999; Russell, 1991). Work by Tsai and colleagues reveals elicit specific emotions are readily documented and often
that some components of emotion (e.g., autonomic response) profound. For example, in Hindu India, people are angered
may vary less across cultures than do self-reports of the by several events that would not elicit much anger in
experience of emotion (for relevant studies, Tsai, Chentsova- Western European cultures (Shweder, Much, Mahapatra, &
Dutton, Friere-Bebeau, & Przymus, 2002; Tsai & Levenson, Park, 1997). These include when a child cuts his hair after
1997; Tsai, Levenson, & Carstensen, 2000; Tsai, Levenson, & the death of his father, when a woman eats with her hus-
McCoy, 2006). With these considerations as a backdrop, the band’s elder brother, when a husband cooks for his wife
literature on the universality and variability of emotion is or massages her legs, and when upper-caste individuals
framed by four generative ideas. come into physical contact with lower-caste individuals. In
the West, jealousy tends to be felt when the sexual atten-
tion of a primary partner turns toward someone else (Buss,
Potential Versus Practice
1994; DeSteno & Salovey, 1996; Harris, 2003; Harris &
Empirical attempts seeking to document universality or cul- Christenfeld, 1996). Ethnographic work with the Toda of
tural variability in emotion are guided by different assump- India, in contrast, found that jealousy is not associated with
tions (Mesquita, 2001). Those interested in universality a sexual partner having intercourse with an ingroup mem-
(evolutionists) focus on the “potential” for emotion; that is, ber, but only with an outgroup, non-Toda male (Hupka,
given a highly controlled stimulus, do members of different 1991).
cultures show similar experiences, expressions, and physi- In the abstract, making progress toward personal goals
ological response? Those interested in cultural variation has the potential to elicit positive emotions across cultures;
tend to focus on the actual “practice” of emotion; that is, in practice, those goals and conceptions of progress vary
how do emotions arise, and how are they experienced and dramatically. For example, members of interdependent
expressed in daily living? cultures, such as the Japanese, Surinamese, and Turkish,
As one example, anger in East Asian cultures is thought tend to experience positive emotions in socially engag-
to be highly muted, given its likely disruption of social har- ing situations, such as in informal exchanges with friends
mony (Markus & Kitayama, 1991, 1994). In contrast, in the (Kitayama, Karasawa, & Mesquita, 2003; Kitayama,
Ifaluk, anger, or song, is a highly public, dramatized display Markus, & Kurokawa, 2000). By contrast, Americans
of expressing grievances and remedies through apology and and Dutch people are more likely to experience positive
reconciliation. Given these observations, constructivists emotions in relatively disengaged situations, for example,
would highlight the profound differences between the cul- in activities oriented toward personal accomplishments
tures in the actual expression of anger; evolutionists would (see also Frijda & Mesquita, 1994). Evolutionists are right
likely see similarities in the potential of emotion—that in arguing that in the abstract across cultures emotions
anger across the two cultures is organized around similar arise in response to similar events and serve similar func-
appraisal themes and expressive behaviors (but intensified tions; constructivists are right in concluding that in practice
in the Ifaluk) and functions to restore just relations when the specific events that trigger emotion often vary dramati-
they have gone awry. cally in different cultures.
Universals and Cultural Variations in Emotion 333

Prototypes and Variations evidence of universality in the recognition of prototypical


facial displays (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Matsumoto
Analyses of emotion-related appraisal, facial expression, et al., 2008) and emotion-specific vocalizations (Juslin &
and knowledge have found it useful to view emotions from a Laukka, 2003; Sauter & Scott, 2007). Even chimpanzees
prototype perspective, as introduced earlier (e.g., Ekman, reliably differentiate among five human facial expressions
1992; Fehr & Russell, 1984; Shaver et al., 1987). This argu- (Parr, 2003). These findings follow from evolutionary
ment holds that each emotion is defined by a constellation of accounts of emotional display: Humans across radically
central features, which reliably occur with an experience different cultures share the same facial musculature, vocal
of a specific emotion, as well as peripheral features that less apparatus, and sensory receptors in the skin and signal emo-
systematically occur with the emotion (and are less power- tion in prototypical displays in similar fashion.
ful in discriminating one emotion from close relatives). The At the same time, cultures vary in ritualized displays
appraisal processes that give rise to emotions involve more or emotion accents, which involve peripheral features of
central and more peripheral features (for such an analysis emotional displays that acquire culture-specific meaning
of awe, see Keltner & Haidt, 2003). Emotion-related dis- (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). For example, throughout
plays involve more central and more peripheral actions: much of Southeast Asia, the tongue bite and shoulder shrug
For example, Ekman has detailed prototypical displays of are ritualized displays of embarrassment. The tongue bite and
anger—the furrowed brow, glare, and tightened and pressed shoulder shrug are peripheral components of the embarrass-
lips—as well as variations that involve more peripheral ment display: exaggerated versions of the inhibitory muscle
facial muscle movements, such as the tightened lower eyelid actions around the mouth (the tongue bite) and constricted,
(Ekman, 2004). Representations of specific emotions have size-reducing posture (the shoulder shrug). In an emotion
more central and peripheral features (Shaver et al., 1987). recognition study, Indian participants readily perceived the
One intriguing possibility is that emotions show greater expressions that included the tongue bite as embarrassment,
universality in their central features and greater cultural whereas U.S. participants saw no reliable emotion in the
variability in their peripheral features. This claim helps display (Haidt & Keltner, 1999). These findings dovetail
to synthesize the evidence for universality and cultural with constructivists’ claims that, as with the phonemes of
variation in emotional display. The communication of each language, cultures select and arrange the elements of emo-
emotion involves facial muscle actions, bodily movements, tional expression in culturally specific ways.
acoustic markers, gestures, and tactile behaviors. Some dis- Empirical studies likewise find that certain appraisals are
play behaviors occur more reliably with an emotion; others central to each emotion and others are more peripheral (e.g.,
less reliably so. For example, more central actions of the Smith & Ellsworth, 1985), as well as that certain themes are
prototypical embarrassment display are gaze down, head central to the knowledge about an emotion and other themes
turns and movements down, and a controlled smile; more are more peripheral (Shaver et al., 1987). The same may be
peripheral elements of the display are face touches, head true for other components of emotion (e.g., peripheral phys-
shakes, and shoulder shrugs (Keltner, 1995). iological response), and the foregoing analysis suggests that
Fairly strong evidence exists for the universality of there is likely to be greater universality to the central fea-
prototypical emotional displays. Japanese and American tures of an emotion and greater variability to the peripheral
students’ facial muscle movements in response to evoca- features.
tive film clips demonstrated correlations that ranged from
0.86 to 0.96 (Ekman, 1972). A recent review of 25 stud-
Focal Emotions Within Cultures
ies involving participants from more than 35 cultures found
that people in different cultures show similar prototypi- Early in the anthropological study of emotion, scholars
cal facial displays of anger, contempt, disgust, fear, pride, claimed that cultures vary in how prominent, or hypercog-
sadness, surprise, and happiness in comparable situations nized, different emotions are in the language and discourse
(see Matsumoto et al., 2008). Unsighted athletes from dif- of the particular culture (Lutz & White, 1986). Distinctions
ferent cultures show remarkably similar facial expressions were drawn between “shame” and “guilt” cultures (Benedict,
of emotions such as anger, sadness, shame, and pride fol- 1946). Romantic love seems to be a highly salient emotion
lowing victory and loss (Matsumoto & Willingham, 2006; in the West. Tahiti has no word for guilt and, perhaps, no
Tracy & Matsumoto, 2008). Ethological studies of sev- occurrence of it.
eral preindustrial cultures uncovered similar prototypical Cultural psychologists have drawn on these observa-
displays of anger, embarrassment, fear, sadness, surprise, tions to propose that cultures vary in which emotions are
and several varieties of smiles and laughs (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, focal (Mesquita, 2003; Rodriguez Mosquera et al., 2004).
1989). Meta-analyses of more than 200 data sets find strong Members of a particular culture, the implication is, may be
334 Emotion

more or less prone to regularly feel and express emotions enthusiasm are more highly valued than emotions such
such as anger, compassion, gratitude, or awe. The proximal as contentedness and modesty. Early work by Matsumoto
sources of variations in focal emotions are cultural differ- (1989, 1990) found that Americans rated negative emotions
ences in self-construals, values or concerns, or epistemolo- as more appropriate than the Japanese did when expressed
gies (Markus & Kitayama, 1994). One would expect focal toward ingroup members, consistent with individualistic val-
emotions to be more readily elicited, experienced more ues of the expression of the true self around intimates. The
intensely, represented in a richer lexicon, and signaled in Japanese, in contrast, rated the expression of negative emo-
more intense display behavior. Preliminary findings lend tion as more appropriate than Americans did when directed
credence to these assertions. toward outgroup members, consistent with the interdepen-
For example, Rodriguez Mosquera and colleagues (2000) dent, collectivist emphasis on ingroup harmony.
have documented that in cultures that prioritize concerns over Cultural differences in ideal emotions are likely to
honor (e.g., respect and face), honor-protecting emotions explain cultural variation in emotion regulation. Members
such as shame or anger are more focal. In relevant empiri- of interdependent cultures are more likely to regulate many
cal studies, individuals from high honor cultures (Spaniards) emotions, in particular negative emotions, which impose
responded with greater shame and anger when insulted than on others, thus disrupting social harmony (Markus &
did individuals from other cultures, because these emotions Kitayama, 1991). Early work found that the Japanese
protect honor and “face.” regulate with polite smiles the display of negative emo-
According to self-construal theories, emotions that fold tion in the presence of an authority figure more so than
people into harmonious, cooperative relations should be do Americans (Friesen, 1972). Conceptually similar work
more focal in more interdependent cultures (see Markus & by Tsai, Levenson, and colleagues (2006) has found that
Kitayama, 1991). For example, self-conscious emotions Asian Americans are more likely to regulate their emo-
such as shame and embarrassment express modesty and a tional expression than Western European or American
sense of place within a social collective and can be thought students. And in a recent study of 19 cultures, Matsumoto
of as highly interdependent emotions (Keltner & Buswell, and colleagues (2008) documented that individuals from
1997). In keeping with the analysis here, self-conscious interdependent cultures report higher levels of emotion
emotions are indeed more focal in interdependent cul- regulation than members of independent cultures. These
tures (Goetz & Keltner, 2008). For example, in China at differences in emotion regulation flow from cultural ideals
least 113 words are related to shame and embarrassment about social harmony versus self-expression.
(Li, Wang, & Fischer, 2004). Olympic athletes from inter- The debate over the universality and the cultural vari-
dependent cultures showed stronger shame displays in ability of emotion has long been an intellectual battle-
response to losing than did individuals from independent ground for evolutionists and constructivists. The many
cultures (Tracy & Matsumoto, 2008). new studies of emotion and culture are framed nicely by
Cultures also vary according to which components of provocative ideas and findings that should buoy the spirits
emotion are focal. Compared with Western European par- of evolutionists and constructivists alike. In the abstract,
ticipants, East Asian participants were found to be more certain responses of emotion are universal, whereas in
sensitive to emotion-related nonverbal vocalizations (Ishii, practice, cultures construct quite different emotions. The
Reyes, & Kitayama, 2003). Consistent with claims about more prototypical features of an emotional response are
cultural variation in dialectical thought, East Asians are likely to be universal, whereas the more peripheral fea-
more likely to report the simultaneous experience of contra- tures are more likely to vary. Some emotions appear to be
dictory emotions (Kitayama et al., 2000; Schimmack et al., more focal in specific cultures, and cultures vary in which
2002). emotions, and which styles of emotional expression, are
valued. Emotions are universal and, at the same time, cul-
turally variable.
Idealization

A final area of inquiry is guided by the assertion that mem-


bers of different cultures value specific emotions differ- EMOTION AND REASON
ently according to how those emotions enable individuals
to more readily enact culturally valued concerns related to In Western thought, emotions have widely been viewed
social organization (Tsai, 2007). Cultures idealize different as lower, less sophisticated ways of perceiving the world
emotions. when juxtaposed with loftier, principled forms of reason
This thesis helps to synthesize several areas of research. (Calhoun & Solomon, 1984; Nussbaum, 2001). Emotions,
In the United States, emotions such as excitement and this view continues, subvert rational judgments and decisions
Emotion and Reason 335

about matters of justice, causality, right and wrong, and directly into cognitive processes (Forgas, 1995; Lerner &
the good life, and they should be extirpated from the mind Keltner, 2001; Schwarz, 1990). Emotions are fast, embodied
and social exchange (Nussbaum, 1996; Oatley, 2004). For gut feelings that feed into important judgments. Framed by
example, Kant (1960), in writing about judgments related these concerns, the study of emotion and reason reveals that
to justice, suggested that emotions such as “sympathy” be almost every cognitive process—attention, evaluative judg-
considered unreliable because of their subjective nature, ments, probability estimates, perceptions of risk, outgroup
claiming such emotions reduce humans to “tender-hearted biases, and moral judgment—is shaped by momentary emo-
idlers.” The rare exception was 18th-century moral philoso- tions in systematic and profound ways (Clore & Gasper,
pher Hume, who contended that emotions should guide rea- 2000; DeSteno, Petty, Rucker, Wegener, & Braverman,
soning (Hume, 1739/1978). 2004; Forgas, 1995, 2000).
This dualistic perspective on emotion and reason has
been countervailed by 25 years of research on the inter- Emotion and Selective Attention
play between emotion and cognitive processes (Clore,
1994; Clore & Gasper, 2000; Clore, Gasper, & Garvin, Jean Paul Sartre (1957) wrote of the “magical transforma-
2001; Clore & Parrott, 1991; Forgas, 1995, 1998, 2000, 2003; tion” that emotions bring about in the perceptual world:
Fredrickson, 2001; Isen, 1987). This literature can be traced that they direct attention to select classes of stimuli in the
back to an influential article by Simon (1967), who argued environment. The most fully researched effects of emo-
that emotions solve a general problem faced by intelligent tions on attention concern fear. Fear and anxiety narrow
agents: Emotions set priorities among the many goals and attention, leading to the selective perception of threats and
stimuli that impinge on individuals at any moment (see also dangers (Mathews & MacLeod, 1994; Mineka, Rafaeli, &
Oatley & Johnson-Laird, 1987, 1996; Winkielman et al., Yovel, 2003). For example, in the dot probe paradigm,
1997). Research by Bower (1981) on mood and memory participants are presented with two words, one threatening
and Isen (1987) on positive affect and judgment were early (e.g., “disease”) and the other neutral (e.g., “table”), on a
empirical inspirations to the studies that would follow. screen, which then are replaced by a dot (Mathews, 1993;
As the literature has developed, several conceptual dis- Mathews & Klug, 1993). Participants press a button when
tinctions have become clear. A first is between the inciden- the dot appears. Highly anxious individuals demonstrate
tal and the integral influences of emotion on reasoning. shorter reaction times to the appearance of the dot above
Incidental effects occur when an emotion triggered by one threatening words when compared with nonanxious indi-
event influences judgments in an unrelated domain. In the viduals and when compared with neutral words. In studies
studies that follow, people prove to quite consistently fail using the dichotic listening paradigm, highly anxious indi-
to understand that incidental emotions are not relevant to a viduals more readily have their attention drawn away from
judgment at hand. As a result, emotions can influence unre- the message they are asked to track in one ear when threat-
lated judgments in profound ways. Integral effects refer to ening words are presented to the other ear (Mathews &
the influences of emotion on judgments of the object that MacLeod, 1994). In studies using the Stroop paradigm, the
elicited the emotion (Forgas, 1995). Trait- and state-based slowing of color naming is greatest with words that cor-
approaches can be taken to characterize the incidental respond to the individual’s greatest anxiety: people with
and integral influences of emotion on cognitive processes social phobias are slowed by words about confidence;
(Lerner & Keltner, 2001). people with eating disorders are slowed by words for food
A second, concerns the nature of the influence on judg- (Mathews & Klug, 1993).
ment. Processing style accounts posit that emotions engage In a similar vein, work by Niedenthal and col-
qualitatively different kinds of processing, which account leagues (Niedenthal, 2008; Niedenthal & Halberstadt,
for influences of emotions on cognition. Anger triggers more 2000; Niedenthal & Setterlund, 1994) has found that cur-
automatic forms of reasoning and sadness more controlled rent emotions lead individuals to more quickly categorize
forms, accounting for why these two emotions lead to dif- other stimuli that are congruent with the current emotional
ferent likelihoods of relying on stereotypes (Bodenhausen, state. In lexical decision studies, people in happy moods
Kramer, & Süsser, 1994). Positive emotions trigger more were found to be faster at identifying happy words than sad
associative, creative, or broadening patterns of thought words, whereas sad individuals were happier at identify-
(Fredrickson, 1998; Isen, 1987). Fear triggers a narrowing ing sad words than happy words (Niedenthal & Setterlund,
of attention or vigilance to threat (Mathews & MacLeod, 1994). People feeling anger identified anger faces more
1994; Mineka & Sutton, 1992). quickly compared with appropriate controls (Niedenthal
Informational accounts, by contrast, presuppose that & Halberstadt, 2000). Emotions bias selective attention,
emotions involve specific kinds of information that feed perception, and categorization in an emotion-congruent
336 Emotion

fashion, which raises interesting questions about the duration tendency, but not judgments of blame or fairness, which
of emotions and moods (which should be extended by these are more closely related to anger.
effects of affect on selective attention). This is a likely place Several studies guided by this perspective have revealed
where regulation strategies should alter the course of an the extensive influences of specific emotions on judgments
emotion episode. and decisions (Lowenstein & Lerner, 2003). People feel-
ing sad were more likely to attribute ambiguous events
to situational causes and to judge future events produced
Emotions and Evaluative Judgments
by situational factors (e.g., lightning sets your house on
Momentary emotions wield powerful influences on evalua- fire) as more likely than people feeling anger, who attrib-
tive judgments, a robust empirical generalization accounted uted the same events to the actions of others and judged
for by a feelings-as-information perspective (Clore, 1992; future events produced by others’ actions to be more likely
Clore & Gasper, 2000; Clore & Parrott, 1991; Schwarz, (Keltner, Ellsworth, & Edwards, 1993). Fear amplifies the
1990; Schwarz & Clore, 1983). This perspective assumes expectation of pessimistic life outcomes and risk com-
that emotions provide rapid signals about objects in the pared with anger (Lerner, Gonzalez, Small, & Fischoff,
environment and that these momentary feelings feed into 2003; Lerner & Keltner, 2001). Anxious decision-makers
ongoing judgments about issues that are too complex to preferred uncertainty-reducing options, whereas sad
review and synthesize all relevant evidence. decision-makers preferred the reward-seeking option
In a seminal study, Schwarz and Clore (1983) asked (Raghunathan & Pham, 1999). Momentary anger increases
people in Illinois either on a cloudy day or on a sunny day the reliance on heuristic cues because of the underlying
“All things considered, how satisfied or dissatisfied are you appraisal of certainty (Tiedens & Linton, 2001; see also
with your life as a whole these days?” Participants either Bodenhausen, Sheppard, & Kramer, 1994). When angry,
rated their life satisfaction or did so after first responding to individuals judge unfair actions to be more likely in their
the question “How’s the weather down there?” Participants future, whereas when sad, individuals judge losses to be
reported greater life satisfaction on a sunny day than on more likely (DeSteno, Petty, Wegener, & Rucker, 2000).
a gloomy day, consistent with a robust literature showing
that current feelings determine levels of subjective well- Positive Emotions Broaden and Build
being (Lucas & Diener, 2008). The joys of the sunny day
only influenced evaluations of life satisfaction, however, Early in the study of emotion and judgment, Isen (1987)
when participants did not attribute their current feelings to argued that happiness prompts people to think in more flexible
the weather (e.g., Martin, 2000). Subsequent studies have and creative ways. People induced to feel happiness through
revealed that current moods and emotions exert power- trivial events, for example, in receiving candy, watching a
ful influences on evaluative judgments of life satisfaction pleasurable film clip, or finding a dime in a public telephone,
(Lucas & Diener, 2008), political leaders (Forgas & Moylan, were more likely to find creative solutions to novel problems,
1987), and consumer choices (Han, Lerner, & Keltner, to produce unusual associations to words, and to categorize
2007). objects in inclusive or novel ways (Isen, 1987).
In an important extension of this work, Fredrickson
(1998, 2001) has argued that the overarching function of pos-
Emotions, Judgment, and Decision Making
itive emotions is to broaden and build thought repertoires.
An appraisal tendency framework has been offered to These basic broadening effects of positive emotion enable
account for the effects of discrete emotions on judgment and more creative and flexible thought, which help the individual
decision making (Han et al., 2007; Lerner & Keltner, 2000, in forming important bonds and exploring the environment.
2001; Lerner & Tiedens, 2006; Tiedens & Linton, 2001). Relevant research has documented how positive emotions
An appraisal tendency framework assumes that each such as joy, amusement, contentment, and relief facilitate
emotion is defined by a core appraisal: Compassion, for global visual processing relative to local processing, counter
example, involves appraisals of the undeserved suffering; the outgroup homogeneity effect, and prompt self-expansion
pride involves appraisals of strength of the self vis-à-vis in interpersonal relationships (Fredrickson, 2001; Johnson &
others (see Tables 9.6 and 9.7). Specific emotions influ- Fredrickson, 2005; Waugh & Fredrickson, 2006).
ence judgments, it is posited, in a manner consistent with
the emotion’s underlying appraisal tendency, but only in Emotions as Moral Intuitions
domains related to the appraisal. For example, fear should
influence judgments of certainty and risk, the judgment Moral judgments of an action as right or wrong, a person of
domains most closely related to its underlying appraisal good character or not, or a punishment as just or not have
Emotion and Reason 337

long been assumed to be founded on higher-order cognitive Table 9.10 Emotions and Their Associated Moral Concerns
processes (Haidt, 2001). The individual in the act of mak- Emotion Moral Concern
ing a moral judgment is assumed to be guided by a priori,
Anger Rights, freedoms, retributive
abstract principles (e.g., conceptions of rights or equality) justice
that apply to all contexts and individuals. Moral judgments Compassion Harm, need
hinge on the development of basic cognitive processes, Contempt Community role, position within
such as the capacity to take another ’s perspective. hierarchy
A different view has emerged, one that prioritizes emo- Disgust Purity, both sexual and spiritual
tions as important intuitions, or fast, automatic judgments Gratitude Reciprocity, equality
of right and wrong (Damasio, 1994; Greene & Haidt,
Guilt Duty, obligation
2002; Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom, Darley, & Cohen,
Shame Own character flaws
2001; Haidt, 2007). This view flows readily from appraisal
Awe, elevation Other ’s virtue
accounts of emotion, which identify morally significant
themes (e.g., harm and fairness) involved in specific emo-
tions, as well as evolutionary proposals that emotions
speak to the teacher in the same way that student talks to
orient cognitive processes to solving problems of social
friends”), and disgust faces to label purity violations (e.g.,
organization. The claim that emotions act as moral intu-
“A person is eating a piece of rotten meat”).
itions has found expression in the somatic marker hypoth-
Weiner and colleagues have documented how anger and
esis (Damasio, 1994) and Haidt’s (2001, 2003, 2007)
sympathy lead to different punitive judgments of moral
two-system view of moral judgment. Table 9.10 summa-
transgressions (Rudolph, Roesch, Greitemeyer, & Weiner,
rizes claims about relations between specific emotions and
2004; Weiner, Graham, & Reyna, 1997). Individuals
moral concerns. “Moral concern” refers to the principles or
angered by moral transgressions prefer the most venge-
rules that govern judgment and action with respect to mat-
ful form of punishment—retributive punishment (see also
ters of the distribution of resources, punishment, and judg-
Carlsmith, Darley, & Robinson, 2002; Harmon-Jones et al.,
ments of character and virtue (Haidt, 2001, 2007; Rozin,
2003; Lerner, Goldberg, & Tetlock, 1998). When angry,
Lowery, Imada, & Haidt, 1999; Shweder et al., 1997;
people blame others, attributing violations to stable, con-
Vasquez, Keltner, Ebenbach, & Banaszynski, 2001).
trollable, and internal causes (Quigley & Tedeschi, 1996).
One of the most widely investigated emotion–morality
In contrast, participants who feel sympathy in response to
associations is that between disgust and purity. Feeling dis-
the same crime prefer less severe forms of punishment,
gusted by apparent purity violations correlates with greater
ones that protect the criminal and society, namely, utilitar-
moral condemnation of those violations (Haidt & Hersh,
ian punishment (Weiner et al., 1997).
2001; Haidt, Koller, & Dias, 1993). Participants induced
through posthypnotic suggestion to experience pangs of Emotional Components of Prejudice
disgust in response to an innocuous target word (“take” or
“often”) reported greater feelings of disgust when the word Long ago, Gordon Allport (1954) argued that emotion orga-
was embedded in descriptions of moral violations and greater nizes the content of different forms of prejudice. Empirical
moral condemnation of those violations (Wheatley & Haidt, research has begun to provide data fitting with this asser-
2005). Opposition to two purity-relevant behaviors, meat tion: that biases toward outgroups are colored by different
consumption and cigarette smoking, coincided with greater emotions (Alexander, Brewer, & Herrmann, 1999; Cottrell &
disgust toward those behaviors and was better predicted by Neuberg, 2005; DeSteno, Dasgupta, Bartlett, & Cajdric,
felt disgust than by perceived health risks (Rozin & Singh, 2004; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002; Mackie et al.,
1999). 2000; Tapias, Glaser, Vasquez, Keltner, & Wickens, 2007).
In one of the most systematic studies of emotion and Stereotypes of competence and warmth trigger feelings of
moral judgment, Rozin and colleagues (1999) documented pity, contempt, and envy in systematic ways (Fiske et al.,
fairly clear associations between anger, contempt, and dis- 2002). Perceptions of relative outgroup strength generate
gust and three moral domains: autonomy (rights, justice, different negative emotions, such as anger, disgust, or fear
and freedom), community (duties and obligations), and (Mackie et al., 2000).
purity, respectively. In their research, participants consis- Outgroups trigger different threat appraisals, which
tently selected anger faces to label violations of autonomy account for the emotional component of distinct preju-
(e.g., “A person is seeing someone steal a purse from a dices (Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005). Prejudice toward African
blind person”), contempt faces to label violations of com- Americans has been associated with anger, in terms of
munity (e.g., “A person is hearing an 8-year-old student self-reported emotion (Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005; Tapias
338 Emotion

et al., 2007), and measured changes in facial musculature, where of social behavior in preindustrialized cultures revealed
Whites who viewed African American faces showed changes that brief emotional displays are a grammar of social inter-
in the corrugator supercilii muscle associated with anger actions (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989). Brief displays of coyness
(Vanman, Paul, Ito, & Miller, 1997; Vanman, Saltz, Nathan, & and desire, for example, are the basic elements of flirtatious
Warren, 2004). Prejudice against gays has been found to be interactions; flashes of anger, contempt, and embarrass-
systematically associated with increased disgust, which may ment constitute negotiations of rank. Constructivists have
be accounted for by the centrality of impurity to both gay long contended that emotions embody culturally specified
stereotype (e.g., “diseased,” “abnormal,” and “inappropriate roles and social identities (Averill, 1980; Hochschild, 1983,
sexuality”) and disgust (Rozin et al., 1999; Vasquez et al., 1990; Lutz & White, 1986; Markus & Kitayama, 1994).
2001). People asked to describe their spontaneous reactions In the expression of sympathy for a vulnerable child, for
toward homosexuality indicate feeling “disgust” (Haidt & example, a woman assumes culturally based gender identi-
Hersh, 2001). Individuals predisposed toward experienc- ties and roles (Citrin et al., 2004; Clark, 1990).
ing disgust tend to report prejudice toward gays (Haidt, Two empirical traditions have emerged as responses to
McCauley, & Rozin, 1994; Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, arguments that emotions are socially constructed. A first
2000; Van de Ven, Bornholt, & Bailey, 1996). pertains to how social contextual factors—status, familiar-
ity, intimacy, power, social class—shape emotional response.
Moderators of the Influences on Emotion Emotions vary dramatically in interactions among friends
and Cognition versus among those of strangers, among bosses versus
among subordinates, and in informal versus in formal set-
The empirical literature suggests that emotions influence
tings. New studies, as the next section shows, are revealing
numerous cognitive processes—selective attention, evalu-
how this is so.
ative judgments, perceptions of risk and estimates of value,
A second emergent interest reverses the causal direction
causality, moral judgments of right and wrong, and biases
and asks how emotions give rise to specific social relation-
toward different outgroups. Emotions might be thought of
ships. Here, the concern is in documenting how specific
metaphorically as social sensory systems, guiding cogni-
emotions or emotional processes create specific patterns
tive processes to significant classes of stimuli in the social
of relationships. Expressions of gratitude, for example,
environment, presumably laying the groundwork for par-
have been theorized to give rise to cooperative relations
ticular courses of action (e.g., anger attunes the individual
among nonkin (Nesse, 1990; Trivers, 1971). As codes of
to matters of injustice and courses of action that potentially
etiquette spread through 17th- and 18th-century Europe,
remedy injustice).
embarrassment at others’ lack of manners created social
These different literatures raise an essential question:
boundaries between those in the court and those outside
What processes moderate the influences of emotion on cog-
(Elias, 1939/1978). Certain emotions, for example, expres-
nition? The most systematic answer to this question has been
sions of contempt, are especially powerful in predicting the
provided by Forgas (1995) in the affect infusion model. This
demise of marriages (Gottman, 1993). With advances in
model posits that emotions infuse into a cognitive process to
the study of interdependent data of participants, such as cou-
the extent that the task is complex, involves constructive pro-
ples or friends in dyadic interactions (Gonzalez & Griffin,
cessing, and is not based on preexisting knowledge structures
1997), new studies are revealing that fleeting expres-
(e.g., prototypes). When judgments are less complex and pre-
sions of emotion do indeed shape the course of different
existing schema or prototypes are salient, emotions influence
relationships.
cognition to a reduced extent. Other processes that mitigate
the influences of emotion on cognition include accountability
(Lerner et al., 1998) and the degree to which the individual has Social Contextual Shaping of Emotion
labeled the current state in words and narrative (Clore et al.,
Experience sampling and diary studies reveal that people’s
2001; Keltner, Locke, & Audrain, 1993).
emotional profiles shift dramatically according to whether
they are with friends or acquaintances, family or work col-
SOCIAL CONSTRUCTION OF EMOTION leagues, higher-status individuals or subordinates, or are
in formal or less formal contexts (e.g., Bolger et al., 2003;
Emotions arise in social contexts and shape and are shaped Moskowitz, 1994). Early experimental studies converge on
by social dimensions of the situation—hierarchical con- a similar theme. With the increasing sociality of the con-
cerns, interdependence, familiarity, intimacy (Clark & text, certain kinds of emotional behaviors—smiling, for
Finkel, 2004; Fischer & Manstead, 2008; Keltner & Haidt, example—are amplified (Fridlund, 1992; Kraut & Johnson,
1999; Tiedens & Leach, 2004). Early ethological analyses 1979). More recent empirical studies have documented how
Social Construction of Emotion 339

two pervasive dimensions to the social context—power and did not adjust their demands to their opponent’s emotion
affiliation—shape momentary emotion. (Van Kleef, De Dreu, Pietroni, & Manstead, 2006). High-
power individuals have been shown to be less emotionally
Status, Power, and Emotion reactive to the suffering of another: In a study of conversa-
Social status and power refer to different facets of the indi- tions between two strangers, high-power individuals’ expe-
vidual’s relative rank, or position, vis-à-vis others, and rel- riences of compassion decreased as their partner disclosed
ative capacity to alter the states of other individuals (Fiske, more distressing experiences (Van Kleef et al., 2008).
1993; Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003). Almost all The tendency for high-power individuals to respond less
relationships are imbued with power concerns, from inter- empathically to the emotions of others is certain to give rise
actions among school children on the playground to work to problems in relationships—an area ripe with interesting
colleagues jockeying in meetings. Power dynamics influ- possibilities. For example, leaders who avoid empathy fail-
ence emotions in several important ways. ures are more likely to maintain the respect and status of
Power influences the overall valence of emotional expe- their group members (Coté & Miners, 2006; Keltner, Van
rience. High-power individuals are prone to experience Kleef, Chen, & Kraus, 2008). Empathy failures may cost
more positive emotions than are low-power people (Collins, high-power individuals in more intimate relationships as
1990; Langner & Keltner, 2008). In contrast, low-power well.
individuals tend to experience increased negative emotion. Affiliation, Warmth, and Emotion
Children of low sociometric status report higher levels of
negative moods, guilt, and depression (Hecht, Inderbitzen, & Affiliation or warmth is a basic dimension of social rela-
Bukowski, 1998). Lower socioeconomic status also relates tionships, and it shapes emotional response in profound
to increased negative mood in adults (e.g., Link, Lennon, & ways (Clark & Finkel, 2004). Affiliation or warmth leads
Dohrenwend, 1993). Select studies yield relations between to the convergence, or mimicry, of emotional responses
status and more specific negative emotions: In a study that (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994). In remarkable work
manipulated status, low-status individuals reported more on the acoustics of laughter, for example, Bachorowski and
guilt and sadness in response to negative events, whereas colleagues documented that within milliseconds the laughs
high-status individuals reported more anger (Tiedens et al., of friends as opposed to those of strangers begin to mimic
2000). one another (e.g., Smoski & Bachorowski, 2003).
Power also influences the expression of emotion. High- The degree of affiliation increases the likelihood of
power people express their positive emotions more read- convergent autonomic physiology. For example, Shearn
ily in facial display (Hecht & LaFrance, 1998) and have and colleagues (1992) brought two friends or two strang-
been found to express more dominant emotions, such as ers to the laboratory and induced embarrassment in one of
anger and contempt (Keltner et al., 1998). High-power the participants. Friends showed a greater empathic blush
individuals show greater coherence between expression at their friends’ mortification than did strangers (see also
and experience of emotion (Hecht & LaFrance, 1998). The Miller, 1987; Provine, 1992).
coherence between experience and expression contributes These studies suggest that as individuals form more inter-
to social adjustment and physical health (Gross, 1998; dependent relationships their emotions converge. Increased
Gross & John, 2003), suggesting that power-related influ- affiliation is certain to influence other facets of emotional
ences on emotional coherence may contribute to the poor response, including which emotions are experienced, the
health outcomes of low-power individuals (see Adler et al., intensity of emotional response (Fridlund, 1992), the accu-
1994). racy with which individuals identify emotions in others,
Perhaps more provocatively, high-power individuals are and the degree to which individuals regulate their emotions
less sensitive to the emotions of others. High-power indi- (Butler, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2006).
viduals are less accurate in judging posed displays of emo-
tions (Galinsky, Magee, Inesi, & Gruenfeld, 2006), as well Emotions Create Patterns of Social Relationships
as the spontaneous displays of emotion of an interaction
partner (Gonzaga, Keltner, & Ward, 2008). High-power Anthropologists have offered rich characterizations of how
individuals also react less to others’ emotions. Anderson, emotions establish relationships, helping individuals “nego-
Keltner, and John (2003) found that low-power friends tiate the social and moral order” (Abu-Lughod & Lutz,
assimilated more to their higher-power friends in their 1990). For example, in Abu-Lughod’s provocative analy-
emotional responses than vice versa. In negotiation studies, sis of emotion, poetry, and ritual in a Bedouin community
lower-power negotiators conceded more to angry oppo- in Egypt, the ritualized expression of hasham—a form
nents than to happy ones, whereas high-power negotiators of embarrassment and modesty—in submissive gesture,
340 Emotion

facial and postural display, and patterns of dress—enables Mills, 1979). Specific emotions produce particular relation-
hierarchical relations among group members (1986). The ship orientations—motivations to be monogamous, to trust,
experience and expression of emotion helps individuals to reciprocate, and so on—that are vital to the maintenance
act out certain roles and identities and signal or mark the of the relationship.
parameters of particular social relationships. Early empirical studies, for example, documented that
the degree of emotional mimicry predicted later reports of
Emotion and the Establishment of Status Relations closeness in various relationships (Hatfield et al., 1994).
In nonhuman species, ritualized displays of dominance More recent empirical studies have found that emotional
(deep vocalizations and postural expansion) and submis- convergence over time predicts increased friendship. In
siveness (head bobbing and cowering) are less costly than one illustrative study, friends came to the laboratory at
direct aggressive encounters and have evolved as a means two different times during the year and reported their
by which individuals negotiate rank (Krebs & Davis, emotional reactions to different evocative stimuli, such
1993). Emotions contribute to the formation of status rela- as humorous or disturbing film clips (Anderson et al.,
tions in humans in similar fashion. 2003). The emotions of friends converged over the
Emotions evoke social inferences that clarify status course of the year: they became more similar in valence
relations among individuals. Emotional displays convey and intensity.
information about the sender ’s relative status and power Early empirical studies of emotion largely focused on
vis-à-vis the receiver: displays of anger, for example, sig- individuals extracted from the social context. As a result,
nal elevated status (Knutson, 1996), whereas displays of claims about the social construction of emotion, about
embarrassment mark relative submissiveness (Keltner, how emotions vary across relationships, and about how
1995). People assume that high-power people respond to emotions give rise to different relationships extended well
difficulties with anger (Tiedens et al., 2000). Even more beyond what was empirically known. With the rise in inter-
on point, another study found that participants attributed est in the study of relationships, and advances in statisti-
more elevated status to an individual who displays anger cal approaches to interdependent data of people in dyadic
compared with other emotions (Tiedens et al., 2000). interactions, studies of emotions in social relationships are
Quite remarkably, the same individual displaying anger in remedying this state of affairs. Dimensions of the social
the face was assumed to be larger physically than when context—status and affiliation—shape emotions in impor-
displaying a submissive emotion such as embarrassment tant ways. Patterns of emotional exchange give rise to spe-
(Ketelaar, 2004). Other things being held constant, indi- cific enduring relationships.
viduals who express more powerful emotions (e.g., anger This literature on the social construction of emotion is
and pride) are afforded more status within hierarchies; in its infancy, and its progress will depend on new kinds of
individuals who express more submissive emotions (e.g., data—dyadic data, longitudinal studies, and careful stud-
embarrassment) are afforded less status. ies of people in particular relationships. The promise of this
Specific interactions—humans’ status contests—are line of inquiry is great and has profound implications for
likely to ritualistically evoke patterns of emotions that emotion theory. Models of emotion appraisal will need to
establish rank relationships. Teasing is one such interaction incorporate social contextual factors as basic elements of
(Keltner & Haidt, 2001). In one study, high- and low-power emotion-related appraisal. Emotion-related physiological
fraternity members took turns in a round-robin design teas- responses, for example, oxytocin release or vagus nerve
ing one another by making up nicknames and embarrassing activation, which are both associated with more prosocial
stories about one another (Keltner & Kring, 1998). High- emotions, are certain to prove highly sensitive to features of
power members tended to display anger and contempt, the social context. Emotions will prove to be integral to the
emotions associated with high power. In contrast, the low- social order, rather than disruptive of it.
power members were more likely to show submissive emo-
tions such as embarrassment.
EMOTION AND HAPPINESS
Emotion and the Establishment of Intimate Bonds
Particular emotional exchanges help to establish inti- Cultural theories of happiness involve different claims
mate bonds (e.g., Clark & Mills, 1979). Sexual desire and about the place of emotion in social life (McMahin, 2006;
romantic love are the sine qua non of short- and long-term Oatley, 2004). In many traditions, happiness is believed to
reproductive relations (Buss, 1992; Ellis, 1992; Ellis & be orthogonal to emotional experience: In classical Greek
Malamuth, 2000; Gonzaga et al., 2001). Brief displays of thought, happiness derives from virtuous action; for medi-
sympathy are a foundation of communal relations (Clark & eval Christian scholars, happiness was to be found in the
Emotion and Happiness 341

afterlife, in communion with God when the soul is liberated These kinds of results beg for data that explain how pat-
from the earthly passions of living. More skeptical stances terns of emotion contribute to happiness and health. One
in other ways of knowing presuppose that the emotions are central hypothesis is that positive emotions buffer against the
impediments to happiness, a position found in the writings toxic effects of chronic anxiety and stress (e.g., Fredrickson &
of the stoics, the Puritans, and some strains of Buddhism. Levenson, 1998; Taylor et al., 2000). The effects of chronic
A third view, perhaps more in keeping with the social- stress are well known and include damaging different organs
psychological study of emotion, is that individual and col- and branches of the nervous system, as well as undermining
lective happiness require the experience and expression personal well-being. The buffering hypothesis suggests that
of emotions, from sympathy to love to anger. This view positive emotions enable people to respond with resilience to
has its advocates in writers such as Hume, Rousseau, and the stresses and trauma that can lead to disease and despair.
Darwin. For example, in longitudinal research, individuals who
What is robustly clear in the empirical literature is that reported higher levels of positive emotion responded with
the balance of negative to positive emotions is a power- reduced traumatic symptoms, anxiety, and health problems
ful determinant of happiness, or subjective well-being to the September 11 terrorist attacks (Fredrickson, Tugade,
(Bentham, 1996; Lucas & Diener, 2008; Lyubomirsky et al., Waugh, & Larkin, 2003). In research on bereavement, mea-
2005). This is not a surprising relationship given the degree sures of laughter and smiling gathered in a semistructured
of semantic overlap between measures of well-being interview about the deceased spouse 6 months after loss
(“I feel satisfied with my life”) and emotion (“I feel happy, predicted reduced grief as assessed in independent inter-
content, proud,” etc). Still, the relationship is robust: Self- views conducted at 6, 14, and 25 months after loss, whereas
reports of increased positive emotion and reduced negative increased expressions of anger, disgust, and fear in the face
emotion strongly predict overall well-being, in particular predicted increased grief at these assessments (Bonanno &
for Western Europeans (Lucas & Diener, 2008). Studies Keltner, 1997).
of really happy people find, on average, that they experi- Positive emotions are vital to adjustment to trauma
ence about three positive emotions for every negative one for numerous reasons. Positive emotions build strong
(Fredrickson, 2001). Studies of really happy marriages relationships, so essential to adaptive responses to stress
find that five positive emotions transpire for every nega- (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Fredrickson, 1998, 2001).
tive one (Gottman, 1993). Positive emotions enable more creative, resilient, insight-
A synthesis of 250 studies of state and trait positive ful patterns of thought (Fredrickson, 2001). Positive emo-
affect found that increased positive emotion promotes tions enhance immune function (Kemeny & Shestyuk,
greater marital satisfaction, better outcomes at work, and 2008) and reduce stress-related cardiovascular arousal
improved physical health (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005). For (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998).
example, the degree of positive emotion expressed in the A second line of inquiry has begun to explore how the
face as captured in a college yearbook photo (measured in cultivation of more prosocial emotions gives rise to boosts
the activity of the orbicularis oculi and zygomatic major in well-being. These sorts of interventions are all the more
muscles) predicted less daily anxiety and distress, greater relevant given claims that upward of 40% of individual
warmth felt toward others, increased sense of accomplish- variation in happiness is due to freely chosen practices,
ing goals, warmer responses evoked in strangers 20 years actions, and thought patterns (Lyubomirsky, 2007). The rel-
later, and increased marital satisfaction and overall well- evant evidence fits with age-old wisdom: The cultivation
being 30 years later (Harker & Keltner, 2001). of different positive emotions is a pathway to happiness.
Positive emotion makes for greater success at work. For Reflecting on reasons for being grateful leads to increased
example, in one study, highly cheerful undergraduates com- happiness and fewer problematic health symptoms mea-
pared with less cheerful peers made on average $25,000 sured several weeks later (Emmons, McCullough, & Tsang,
more per year on entering into the workforce (Diener, 2003). Forgiving someone increases well-being and pro-
Nickerson, Lucas, & Sandvik, 2002). A preponderance of motes reduced stress-related physiology (Lawler et al.,
positive emotion promotes more robust physical health and 2003). Practicing mindfulness meditation, with a focus on
longevity. One well-known study found that nuns who in being mindful of breathing and extending loving kindness
personal narratives at age 20 reported greater happiness to others, boosts happiness several weeks later, as well as
were 2.5 times less likely to die between the ages of 80 and the relative left hemispheric lateralization in the brain, a
90 than were nuns who reported in their narratives being pattern of activation associated with increased well-being
less happy (Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001). Being (Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008; Davidson,
happy at age 70 was found to add 20 months, on average, to Kabat-Zinn, et al., 2003). The positive emotions can be cul-
an individual’s life expectancy (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005). tivated and are a pathway to more general well-being.
342 Emotion

SUMMARY Barrett, K. C., & Campos, J. J. (1987). Perspectives on emotional develop-


ment II: A functionalist approach to emotions. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.),
Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 558–578). New York:
Answers to age-old questions about human nature involve Wiley.
assumptions about the emotions. As the empirical science of Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on
emotion has matured, long-standing notions that emotions Psychological Science, 1, 28–58.
are disruptive, irrational forces that undermine the social Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The
order have given way to a much different view. Emotions experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373–403.

involve highly sophisticated systems—display, physiology, Bartlett, M. Y., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behavior:
Helping when it costs you. Psychological Science, 17, 319–325.
language, representation, and experience—that enable peo-
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire
ple to adapt to changing social circumstances and fold into for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.
different relationships. More specific empirical literatures Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
reveal emotions to be both universal (as evolutionists hold) Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An
and culturally variable (as constructivists contend) and to interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267.
be central elements of humans’ most important judgments Baxter, M. G., & Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 563–574.
and decisions. In addition, emotions are shaped by, and
Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates
shape, social interaction, and the balance of emotions gives of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience,
rise to the sense of the life well lived. 21, 1–6.
Beer, J., Heerey, E. A., Keltner, D., Knight, R., & Scabini, D. (2003).
The regulatory function of self-conscious emotion: Insights from
REFERENCES patients with orbitofrontal damage. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85, 594–604.
Benedict, R. (1946). The chrysanthemum and the sword: Patterns of
Abu-Lughod, L. (1986). Veiled sentiments. Berkeley: University of
Japanese culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
California Press.
Bentham, J. (1996). An introduction to the principles of morals and legis-
Abu-Lughod, L., & Lutz, C. A. (1990). Introduction to language and the
lation: An introduction. J. H. Burns & H. L. A. Hart (Eds.). New York:
politics of emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Oxford University Press.
Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., & Kahn, R. L.
Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1993). Respiratory sinus
(1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradi-
arrhythmia: Autonomic origins, physiological mechanisms, and psy-
ent. American Psychologist, 49, 15–24.
chophysiological implications. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 296–322.
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1994). Impaired rec-
Berridge, K. C. (2003). Comparing the emotional brains of humans and
ognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to
other animals. In R. J. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith
the human amygdala. Nature, 22, 669–672.
(Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 25–51). New York: Oxford
Aggleton, J. P. (Ed.). (2000). The amygdala (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
University Press.
Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P., & Süsser, K. (1994). Happiness and ste-
Alexander, M. G., Brewer, M. B., & Herrmann, R. K. (1999). Images reotypic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social
and affect: A functional analysis of out-group stereotypes. Journal of Psychology, 66(4), 621–632.
Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 78–93.
Bodenhausen, G., Sheppard, L., & Kramer, G. (1994). Negative affect and
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: social judgment: The different impact of anger and sadness. European
Addison-Wesley. Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 45–62.
Anderson, C., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2003). Emotional conver- Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life
gence between people over time. Journal of Personality and Social as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579–616.
Psychology, 84, 1054–1068. Bonanno, G. A., & Keltner, D. (1997). Facial expressions of emotion and
Arnold, M. B., & Gasson, J. A. (1954). Feelings and emotions as dynamic the course of conjugal bereavement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
factors in personality integration. In M. B. Arnold & S. J. Gasson 106, 126–137.
(Eds.), The human person (pp. 294–313). New York: Ronald. Bonanno, G. A., & Keltner, D. (2004). The coherence of emotion systems:
Averill, J. R. (1980). A constructionist view of emotion. In R. Plutchik & Comparing “on-line” measures of appraisal and facial expressions, and
H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience self-report. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 431–444.
(pp. 305–339). New York: Academic Press. Boucher, J. D., & Brandt, M. E. (1981). Judgment of emotion: American
Ayduk, O., & Kross, E. (2008). Enhancing the pace of recovery: Self- and Malay antecedents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 3,
distanced analysis of negative experiences reduces blood pressure 309–314.
reactivity. Psychological Science, 19, 229–231. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36,
Bachorowski, J. A. (1999). Vocal expression and perception of emotion. 129–148.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 53–57. Briggs, J. L. (1970). Never in anger. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Bachorowski, J. A., & Owren, M. J. (2001). Not all laughs are alike: Voiced Press.
by not voiced laughter readily elicits positive affect. Psychological Buss, D. M. (1992). Mate preference mechanisms: Consequences for part-
Science, 12, 252–257. ner choice and intrasexual competition. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, &
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expres- J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 267–288). New York: Oxford
sion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636. University Press.
References 343

Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. Clore, G. L., Gasper, K., & Garvin, E. (2001). Affect as information. In J. P.
New York: Basic Books. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 121–144).
Butler, E. A., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2006). Respiratory sinus Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
arrhythmia, emotion, and emotion regulation during social interaction. Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2008). Appraisal theories: How cognition
Psychophysiology, 43, 612–622. shapes affect into emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, &
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., & Ito, T. A. L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 628–644). New York:
(2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & Guilford Press.
J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. Clore, G. L., & Parrott, W. G. (1991). Moods and their vicissitudes:
173–191). New York: Guilford Press. Thoughts and feelings as information. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and
Cacioppo, J. T., Klein, D. J., Berntson, G. G., & Hatfield, E. (1993). The social judgment (pp. 107–123). Oxford: Pergamon.
psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Lending a hand:
Handbook of emotions (pp. 119–142). New York: Guilford Press. Social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychological
Calhoun, C., & Solomon, R. (1984). What is an emotion? In C. Calhoun & Science, 17, 1032–1039.
R. Solomon (Eds.), Readings in philosophical psychology. New York: Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three month old infants’ reactions to
Oxford University Press. simulated maternal depression. Child Development, 54, 185–193.
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emergent themes Collins, R. C. (1990). Stratification, emotional energy, and the transient
in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. emotions. In T. D. Kemper (Ed.), Research agendas in the sociol-
Developmental Psychology, 25, 394–402. ogy of emotions (pp. 27–57). Albany: State University of New York
Press.
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., & Gabrieli, J. (1999). fMRI identifies a
network of structures correlated with retention of positive and negative Consedine, N. S. (2008). Health-promoting and health-damaging effects
emotional memory. Psychobiology, 27, 441–452. of emotion: The view from developmental functionalism. In M. Lewis,
J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions
Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James–Lange theory of emotion: A criti-
(pp. 676–690). New York: Guilford Press.
cal examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of
Psychology, 39, 106–124. Cortes, B. P., Demoulin, S., Rodriguez, R. T., Rodriguez, A. P., & Leyens, J. P.
(2005). Infrahumanization or familiarity? Attribution of uniquely
Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we pun-
human emotions to the self, the ingroup, and the outgroup. Personality
ish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 243–253.
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 284–299.
Coté, S., & Miners, C. T. H. (2006). Emotional intelligence, cognitive intelli-
Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment
gence, and job performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 51, 1–28.
and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23(8), 779–818.
Cottrell, C. A., & Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Different emotional reactions to
Carter, C. S., & Altemus, M. (1997). Integrative functions of lactational
different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to “preju-
hormones in social behavior and stress management. In C. S. Carter &
dice.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 770–789.
I. I. Lederhendler (Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
(Vol. 807, pp. 164–174). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Cryder, C. E., Lerner, J. S., Gross, J. J., & Dhal, R. E. (2008). Misery is not
miserly. Psychological Science, 19, 525–530.
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral acti-
vation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human
The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, brain. New York: Free Press.
67, 319–333. Danner, D. D., Snowdon, E., & Friesen, W. V. (2001). Positive emotions
Citrin, L. B., Roberts, T. A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Objectification early in life and longevity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of
theory and emotions: A feminist psychological perspective on gendered Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 804−813.
affect. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach (Eds.), The social life of emo- Darwin, C. (1872/1998). The expression of the emotions in man and ani-
tions (pp. 203–226). New York: Cambridge University Press. mals (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Clark, C. (1990). Emotions and the micropolitics in everyday life: Some Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology, and resil-
patterns and paradoxes of “place.” In T. D. Kemper (Ed.), Research ience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American Psychologist, 55,
agendas in the sociology of emotions (pp. 305–334). Albany: State 1196–1214.
University of New York Press. Davidson, R. J. (2004). What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect:
Clark, M. S., & Finkel, E. J. (2004). Does expressing emotion promote Perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry research. Biological
well-being? It depends on relationship context. In L. Z. Tiedens & Psychology, 67(1–2), 219–233.
C. W. Leach (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 105–126). New Davidson, R. J., & Harrington, A. (2001). Visions of compassion: Western
York: Cambridge University Press. scientists and Tibetan Buddhists examine human nature. New York:
Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange Oxford University Press.
and communal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D.,
Psychology, 37, 12–24. Santorelli, S. F., Urbanowsky, F., Harrington, A., Bonus, K., &
Clore, G. L. (1992). Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construc- Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function pro-
tion of judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction duced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65,
of social judgments (pp. 133–163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 564–570.
Clore, G. L. (1994). Why emotions are felt. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., & Kalin, N. H. (2003).
(Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 103–111). New York: Cambridge Parsing the subcomponents of emotion and disorders: Perspectives from
University Press. affective neuroscience. In R. J. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith
Clore, G. L., & Gasper, K. (2000). Feeling is believing: Some affective (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 8–24). New York: Oxford
influences on belief. In N. H. Frijda, A. S. R. Manstead, & S. Bem University Press.
(Eds.), Emotions and beliefs: How feelings influence thoughts Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2003). Handbook of
(pp. 10–44). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. affective science. New York: Oxford University Press.
344 Emotion

DePue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of Elias, N. (1939/1978). The history of manners: The civilization process
personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extra- (Vol. 1, E. Jephcott, Trans.). New York: Pantheon.
version. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569. Ellis, B. (1992). The evolution of sexual attraction: Evaluative mecha-
DeSteno, D., Dasgupta, N., Bartlett, M. Y., & Cajdric, A. (2004). Prejudice nisms in women. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The
from thin air: The effect of emotion on automatic intergroup attitudes. adapted mind (pp. 267–288). New York: Oxford University Press.
Psychological Science, 15, 319–324. Ellis, B. J., & Malamuth, N. M. (2000). Love and anger in romantic
DeSteno, D., Petty, R., Wegener, D., & Rucker, D. (2000). Beyond valence relationships: A discrete systems model. Journal of Personality, 68,
in the perception of likelihood: The role of emotion specificity. Journal 525–556.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 397–416. Ellsworth, P. (1991). Some implications of cognitive appraisal theories
DeSteno, D., Petty, R. E., Rucker, D. D., Wegener, D. T., & Braverman, J. of emotion. In K. T. Strongman (Ed.), International review of studies
(2004). Discrete emotions and persuasion: The role of emotion- on emotion (pp. 143–161). Chichester, England: Wiley.
induced expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). From appraisal to emotion:
86, 43–56. Differences among unpleasant feelings. Motivation and Emotion, 12,
DeSteno, D. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Jealousy and the characteristics of 271–302.
one’s rival: A self-evaluation maintenance perspective. Personality Emmons, R. A., McCullough, M. E., & Tsang, J. (2003). Counting bless-
and Social Psychology, 22, 920–932. ings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and
De Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social
humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Psychology, 84, 377–389.
Diamond, L. M. (2003). What does sexual orientation orient? A biobe- Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a proto-
havioral model distinguishing romantic love and sexual desire. type perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113,
Psychological Review, 110, 173–192. 464–486.
Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Fischer, A.H. (2000). Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspec-
Immunological effects of induced shame and guilt. Psychosomatic tives. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Medicine, 66, 124–131. Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2008). Social functions of emotion.
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and corti- In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook
sol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory of emotions (pp. 456–470). New York: Guilford Press.
research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355–391. Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on ste-
Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional reotyping. American Psychologist, 48(6), 621–628.
affect and job outcomes. Social Indicators Research, 59, 229–259. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often
Dimberg, U., & Öhman, A. (1996). Behold the wrath: Psychophysiological mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively fol-
responses to facial stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 149–182. low from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Grunedal, S. (2002). Facial reactions to
emotional stimuli: Automatically controlled emotional responses. Fivush, R. (1991). Gender and emotion in mother–child conversations
Cognition and Emotion, 16, 449–471. about the past. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 1, 325–341.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989). Human ethology. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. J. (1989). Coping and emotion. In N. L. Stein,
B. Leventhal, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Psychological and biological
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R. M.,
approaches to emotion (pp. 313–332). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
et al. (1989). Relation of sympathy and distress to prosocial behavior:
A multi-method study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM).
57, 55–66. Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 39–66.

Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions Forgas, J. P. (1998). On being happy and mistaken: Mood effects on the
of emotion. In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1971 fundamental attribution error. Journal of Personality and Social
(pp. 207–283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Psychology, 75(2), 318–331.
Forgas, J. (Ed.). (2000). Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social
Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99,
cognition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
550–553.
Forgas, J. P. (2003). Affective influences on attitudes and judgments. In
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist,
R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of
48, 384–392.
affective sciences (pp. 596–218). New York: Oxford University Press.
Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply
Forgas, J. P., & Moylan, S. (1987). After the movies: The effect of mood
to Russell’s mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 268–287.
on social judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13,
Ekman, P. (2004). Emotions revealed. New York: Owl Books. 465–477.
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of emotion. New York: Francis, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Maternal care and the development of
Oxford University Press. stress responses, Development, 9, 128–34.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W., & Ancoli, S. (1980). Facial signs of emotional expe- Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the
rience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1125–1134. emotions. New York: W. W. Norton.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1982). Emotion in the human Frank, M., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1993). Behavioral markers and rec-
face. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. ognizability of the smile of enjoyment. Journal of Personality and
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic ner- Social Psychology, 64, 83–93.
vous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of
1208–1210. General Psychology, 2, 300–319.
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psy-
specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological chology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American
Bulletin, 128, 203–235. Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
References 345

Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent-and-response-focused emotion regulation:
(2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology.
loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1045–1062. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion
Fredrickson, B. L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retro- regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
spective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.
Social Psychology, 65, 45–55. Gross, J. J., Sutton, S. K., & Ketelaar, T. (1998). Relations between affect
Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed and personality: Support for the affect-level and affective-reactivity
recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. views. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(3), 279.
Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191–220. Gruber, J., Johnson, S. L., Oveis, C., & Keltner, D. (2008). Risk for
Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. (2003). mania and positive emotional responding: Too much of a good thing?
What good are positive emotions in crises? A prospective study of Emotion, 8, 23–33.
resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuition-
States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social ist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834.
Psychology, 84, 365–376.
Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, &
Fridlund, A. J. (1992). The behavioral ecology and sociality of human H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852–870).
faces. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 90–121. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Friesen, W. V. (1972). Cultural differences in facial expressions in a Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316,
social situation: An experimental test of the concept of display rules. 998–1002.
Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of California, San Francisco.
Haidt, J., & Hersh, M. (2001). Sexual morality: The cultures and emotions
Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge of conservatives and liberals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
University Press. 31, 191–221.
Frijda, N. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 349–358. Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (1999). Culture and facial expression: Open-ended
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emo- methods find more faces and a gradient of recognition. Cognition and
tion, appraisal, and action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Emotion, 13, 225–266.
Psychology, 57(2), 212–228. Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and moral-
Frijda, N. H., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions of ity, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social
emotions. In S. Kitayama & H. Marcus (Ed.), Emotion and culture: Psychology, 65, 613–628.
Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 51–87). Washington, DC: Haidt, J., McCauley, C. R., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in
American Psychological Association. sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elici-
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power tors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701–713.
and perspectives not taken. Psychological Science, 17, 1068–1074. Hall, J. A., Carter, J. D., & Horgan, T. G. (2000). Gender differences in
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). The social brain. New York: Basic Books. nonverbal communication of emotion. In A. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and
Gilbert, D. T., Lieberman, M. D., Morewedge, C. K., & Wilson, T. D. emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 97–117). New York:
(2004). The peculiar longevity of things not so bad. Psychological Cambridge University Press.
Science, 15, 14–19. Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological
(1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective fore- Bulletin, 131(6), 898–924.
casting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617–638. Han, S., Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2007). Feelings and consumer deci-
Goetz, J., & Keltner, D. (2008). Cultural variation in self-conscious emo- sion making: The appraisal-tendency framework. Journal of Consumer
tion. In J. Tangney, R. Robins, & J. Tracy (Eds.), Handbook of self- Psychology, 17, 158–168.
conscious emotions. New York: Guilford Press. Han, S., Lerner, J. S., & Zeckhauser, R. (2008). Disgust promotes disposal:
Gonzaga, G. C., Keltner, D., Londahl, E. A., & Smith, M. D. (2001). Love Souring the status quo. Manuscript under review.
and the commitment problem in romantic relationships and friendship. Harker, L. A., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of positive emotion in
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 247–262. women’s college yearbook pictures and their relationship to personality
Gonzaga, G. C., Keltner, D., & Ward, D. (2008). Power in mixed-sex inter- and life outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social
actions. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1555–1568. Psychology, 80, 112–124.

Gonzaga, G. C., Turner, R. A., Keltner, D., Campos, B., & Altemus, M. (2006). Harmon-Jones, E., Sigelman, J. D., Bohlig, A., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2003).
Romantic love and sexual desire in close bonds. Emotion, 6, 163–179. Anger, coping, and frontal cortical activity: The effect of coping potential
on anger-induced left frontal activity. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 1–24.
Gonzalez, R., & Griffin, D. (1997). On the statistics of interdependence:
Harris, C. R. (2001). Cardiovascular responses of embarrassment and
Treating dyadic data with respect. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of per-
effects of emotional suppression in a social setting. Journal of
sonal relationships: Theory, research, and interventions (2nd ed.,
Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 886–897.
pp. 271–302). Chichester, England: Wiley.
Harris, C. R. (2003). A review of sex differences in sexual jealousy, includ-
Gottman, J. (1993). Why marriages succeed or fail. New York: Simon and
ing self-report data, psychophysiological responses, interpersonal
Schuster.
violence, and morbid jealousy. Personality and Social Psychology
Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment Review, 7, 102–128.
work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 517–523.
Harris, C. R., & Christenfeld, N. (1996). Gender, jealousy, and reason.
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, Psychological Science, 7, 364–366.
J. D. (2001). An fMRI study of emotional engagement in moral judg-
Hart, A. J. Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., &
ment. Science, 293, 2105–2108.
Rausch, S. L. (2000). Differential response of the human amyg-
Griffin, D. (1994). Satire: A critical reintroduction. Lexington: University dala to racial outgroup vs. ingroup face stimuli. NeuroReport, 11,
of Kentucky Press. 2351–2354.
346 Emotion

Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias
Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social
MIT Press. Psychology, 75, 887–900.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-
attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, Crofts.
511–524. Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.
Heath, C., Bell, C., & Sternberg, E. (2001). Emotional selection in James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205.
memes: The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Janig, W. (2003). The autonomic nervous system and its coordination by the
Psychology, 81, 1028–1041. brain. In R. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook
Hecht, D. B., Inderbitzen, H. M., & Bukowski, A. L. (1998). The relation- of Affective Science (pp. 135–186). London: Oxford University Press.
ship between peer status and depressive symptoms in children and ado- Johnson, K. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2005). “We all look the same to me”:
lescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 153–160. Positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias in face recognition.
Hecht, M., & LaFrance, M. (1998). License or obligation to smile: The Psychological Science, 16, 875–881.
effect of power and sex on amount and type of smiling. Personality Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Oatley, K. (1989). The language of emotions: An
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1332–1342. analysis of a semantic field. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 81–123.
Hejmadi, A., Davidson, R. J., & Rozin, P. (2000). Exploring Hindu Indian Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal
emotion expressions: Evidence for accurate recognition by Americans expression and music performance: Different channels, same code?
and Indians. Psychological Science, 11, 183–187. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 770–814.
Hertenstein, M. J. (2002). Touch: Its communicative functions in infancy. Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener,
Human Development, 45, 70–94. & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Hedonic psychology. New York: Cambridge
Hertenstein, M. J., Keltner, D., App, B., Bulleit, B. A., & Jaskolka, A. R. University Press.
(2006). Touch communicates distinct emotions. Emotion, 6, 528–533. Kant. I. (1960). Observations on the feeling of the beautiful and the sub-
Hertenstein, M. J., Verkamp, J. M., Kerestes, A. M., & Holmes, R. M. lime. J. T. Goldthwait (Trans.). Berkeley: University of California
(2006). The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman Press.
primates, and rats: A review and synthesis of the empirical research. Keltner, D. (1995). The signs of appeasement: Evidence for the dis-
Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132, 5–94. tinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame. Journal of
Hess, U., Banse, R., & Kappas, A. (1995). The intensity of facial expres- Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 441–454.
sion is determined by underlying affective states and social situations. Keltner, D. (2009). Born to be good: The science of a meaningful life.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 280–288. New York: W. W. Norton.
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter and dissociation:
52, 1280–1300. The distinct correlates of laughter and smiling during bereavement.
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 687–702.
human feeling. Berkeley: University of California Press. Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1996). Evidence for the distinctness of embar-
Hochschild, A. R. (1990). Ideology and emotion management. In rassment, shame, and guilt: A study of recalled antecedents and facial
T. D. Kemper (Ed.), Research agendas in the sociology of emotions expressions of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 10(2), 155–171.
(pp. 117–142). Albany: State University of New York Press. Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrassment: Its distinct form and
Holman, S. D., & Goy, R. D. (1995). Experiential and hormonal correlates appeasement functions. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 250–270.
of care-giving in Rhesus macaques. In C. R. Pryce & R. D. Martin Keltner, D., Ekman, P., Gonzaga, G. C., & Beer, J. (2003). Facial expres-
(Eds.), Motherhood in human and nonhuman primates: Biosocial sion of emotion. In R. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. Goldsmith (Eds.),
determinants (pp. 87–93). Basel: Karger. Handbook of affective science (pp. 415–432). London: Oxford
Hupka, R. B. (1991). The motive for the arousal of romantic jealousy: Its University Press.
cultural origin. In P. Salovey (Ed.), The psychology of jealousy and Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P. C., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pes-
envy (pp. 252–270). New York: Guilford Press. simism: Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of
Hume, D. (1739/1978). A Treatise of Human Nature. Oxford: Oxford Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 740–752.
University Press. Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions.
Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation and the psychology of expectation. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 467–480.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach,
Insel, T. R. (1993). Oxytocin and the neuroendocrine basis of affiliation. and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110(2), 265–284.
In J. Schulkin (Ed.), Hormonally induced changes in mind and brain Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels
(pp. 225–251). San Diego: Academic Press. of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 505–521.
Insel, T. R., Young, L., & Zuoxin, W. (1997). Molecular aspects of Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2001). Social functions of emotions. In T. Mayne &
monogamy. In C. S. Carter & I. I. Lederhendler (Eds.), Annals of the G. Bonanno (Eds.), Emotions: Current issues and future directions
New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 807, pp. 302–316). New York: (pp. 192–213). New York: Guilford Press.
New York Academy of Sciences. Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and
Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes and social behavior. aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 17(2), 297–314.
In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (1998). Emotion, social function, and psycho-
(Vol. 20, pp. 203–253). New York: Academic Press. pathology. Review of General Psychology, 2, 320–342.
Ishii, K., Reyes, J. A., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Spontaneous attention to Keltner, D., Locke, K. D., & Audrain, P. C. (1993). The influence of attri-
word content versus emotional tone: Differences among three cultures. butions on the relevance of negative emotions to personal satisfaction.
Psychological Science, 14, 39–46. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 21–29.
References 347

Keltner, D., Van Kleef, G. A., Chen, S., & Kraus, M. W. (2008). A recipro- Larsen, J. T., McGraw, A. P., Mellers, B. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). The
cal influence model of social power: Emerging principles and lines of agony of victory and the thrill of defeat: Mixed emotional reactions
inquiry. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 151–192. to disappointing wins and relieving losses. Psychological Science, 15,
Keltner, D., Young, R. C., Oemig, C., Heerey, E., & Monarch, N. D. (1998). 325–330.
Teasing in hierarchical and intimate relations. Journal of Personality Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1989). Extraversion, neuroticism, and suscep-
and Social Psychology, 75, 1231–1247. tibility to positive and negative mood induction procedures. Personality
Kemeny, M. E., & Shestyuk, A. (2008). Emotions, the neuroendocrine and and Individual Differences, 10(12), 1221–1228.
immune systems, and health. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & Lawler, K. A., Younger, J. W., Piferi, R. L., Billington, E., Jobe, R.,
L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp 661–675). New York: Edmondson, K., & Jones, W. H. (2003). A change of heart:
Guilford Press. Cardiovascular correlates of forgiveness in response to interpersonal
Ketelaar, T. (2004). Ancestral emotions, current decisions: Using evolu- conflict. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 26, 373–393.
tionary game theory to explore the role of emotions in decision-making. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford
In Crawford, C. & Salmon, C. (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology, public University Press.
policy and personal decisions (pp. 145–168). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Leary, M. R., Britt, T. W., Cutlip, W. D., II, & Templeton, J. L. (1992).
Kettunen, J., Ravaja, N., Naatanen, P., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. Social blushing. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 446–460.
(2000). The relationship of respiratory sinus arrhythmia to the co- LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.
activation of autonomic and facial responses during the Rorschach test.
Lerner, J. S., Dahl, R. E., Hariri, A. R., & Taylor, S. E. (2007). Facial
Psychophysiology, 37, 242–250.
expressions of emotion reveal neuroendocrine and cardiovascular
Kitayama, S., Karasawa, M., & Mesquita, B. (2003). Collective and per- stress responses. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 253–260.
sonal processes in regulating emotions: Emotion and self in Japan and
Lerner, J. S., Goldberg, J. H., & Tetlock, P. E. (1998). Sober second
the United States. In P. Philipott & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), The regula-
thoughts: The effects of accountability, anger, and authoritarianism
tion of emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
on attributions of responsibility. Personality and Social Psychology
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emo- Bulletin, 24, 563–574.
tions, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States.
Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischoff, B. (2003). Effects
Cognition and Emotion, 14, 93–124.
of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field
Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal experiment. Psychological Science, 14, 144–150.
trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 165–182.
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of
Konner, M. (2003). The tangled wing: Biological constraints on the emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition and
human spirit. New York: Holt. Emotion, 14, 473–493.
Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fishbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of
Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435, 673–76. Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159.
Kövesces, Z. (2002). Metaphor. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and
Kraut, R. E., & Johnson, R. E. (1979). Social and emotional messages of purse strings: Carry-over effects of emotions on economic transactions.
smiling: An ethological approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychological Science, 15, 337–341.
Psychology, 37, 1539–1553. Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision
Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (1993). An introduction to behavioural ecology. maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger ’s influence on cognition.
Oxford: Blackwell. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 115–137.
Kring, A. M. (2008). Emotion disturbances as transdiagnostic processes in Levenson, R. W. (1992). Autonomic nervous system differences among
psychopathology. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett emotions. Psychological Science, 3, 23–27.
(Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 691–705). New York: Guilford Levenson, R. W. (1999). The intrapersonal functions of emotion. Cognition
Press. and Emotion, 13(5), 481–504.
Kurzban, R. (2001). The social psychophysics of cooperation: Nonverbal Levenson, R. W. (2003). Autonomic specificity and emotion. In
communication in a public goods game. Journal of Nonverbal R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook
Behavior, 25, 241–259. of affective sciences (pp. 212–224). New York: Oxford University
LaFrance, M., & Banaji, M. (1992). Towards a reconsideration of the Press.
gender–emotion relationship. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial
behavior: Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 14). activity generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–384.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: Chicago Levesque, J., Eugene, F., Joanette, U., Paquette, V., Mensour, B., Beaudoin,
University Press. G., Leroux, J. M., Bourgouin, P., & Beauregard, M. (2003). Neural
Lambie, J., & Marcel, A. J. (2002). Consciousness and emotion experi- circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness. Biological
ence: A theoretical framework. Psychological Review, 109, 219–259. Psychiatry, 53, 502–510.
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Levine, L., & Pizarro, D. (2004). Emotion and memory research: A grumpy
Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reac- overview. Social Cognition, 22, 530–554.
tions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273. Levine, L. J., & Safer, M. A. (2002). Sources of bias in memory for emo-
Langner, C. A., & Keltner, D. (2008). Social power and emotional experi- tions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 169–173.
ence: Actor and partner effects within dyadic interactions. Journal of Li, J., Wang, L., & Fischer, K. W. (2004). The organization of Chinese
Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 848–856. shame concepts. Cognition and Emotion, 18(6), 767–797.
Larsen, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Poehlmann, K. M., Ito, T. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. Link, B. G., Lennon, M. C., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (1993). Sociometric
(2008). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland- status and depression: The role of occupations involving direction,
Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 180–195). control, and planning. American Journal of Sociology, 98,
New York: Guilford Press. 1351–1387.
348 Emotion

Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2006). The thrill of victory and the
making. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), agony of defeat: Spontaneous expressions of medal winners of
Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 619–642). New York: Oxford the 2004 Athens Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social
University Press. Psychology, 91 (3), 568–581.
Lucas, R. E., & Diener, E. (2008). Subjective well-being. In. M. Lewis, Matsumoto, D., Hee Yoo, S., Nakagawa, S., & 27 members of the
J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions multinational study of cultural display rules (2008). Culture, emotion
(pp. 471–484). New York: Guilford Press. regulation, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social
Lutz, C. A. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Psycholog, 94, 925–927.
Micronesian atoll and their challenge to Western theory. Chicago: Mauro, R., Sato, K., & Tucker, J. (1992). The role of appraisal in human
University of Chicago Press. emotions: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Personality and Social
Lutz, C. (1990). Engendered emotion: Gender, power, and the rhetoric Psychology, 62(2), 301–317.
of emotional control in American discourse. In C. A. Lutz & L. Abu- Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J.
Lughod (Eds.), Language and the politics of emotion (pp. 69–91). (2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotional experience,
New York: Cambridge University Press. behavior, and autonomic physiology. Emotion, 5, 175–190.
Lutz, C., & White, G. M. (1986). The anthropology of emotions. Annual McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J.-A. (2002). The grate-
Review of Anthropology, 15, 405–436. ful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of
Lyubomirsky, S. (2007). The how of happiness. New York: Penguin Press. Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 112–127.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of fre- McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S. D., Emmons, R. A., & Larson, D. B.
quent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological (2001). Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychological Bulletin, 127,
Bulletin, 131, 803–855. 249–266.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused McFarland, C., & Ross, M. (1987). The relation between current impres-
rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. sions and memories of self and dating partners. Personality and Social
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190. Psychology Bulletin, 13, 228–238.
Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: McMahin, D. M. (2006). Happiness: A history. New York: Grove Press,
Explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. 2006.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602–616. Mesquita, B. (2001). Culture and emotion: Different approaches to the
Mackie, D. M., Silver, L. A., & Smith, E. R. (2004). Intergroup emo- question. In T. J. Mayne & G. A. Bonanno (Eds.), Emotions: Current
tions: Emotion as an intergroup phenomenon. In L. Z. Tiedens & issues and future directions. Emotions and social behavior. (pp.
C. W. Leach (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 227–246). 214– 250). New York: Guilford Press.
New York: Cambridge University Press. Mesquita, B. (2003). Emotions as dynamic cultural phenomena. In R. J.
Malatesta, C. Z. (1990). The role of emotions in the development and Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of
organization of personality. In R. A. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska sym- affective sciences (pp. 871–890). New York: Oxford University Press.
posium on motivation: Vol. 36. Socioemotional development (pp. Mesquita, B., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2001). The role of culture in appraisal.
1–56). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. In K. R. Scherer & A. Schorr (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion:
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications Theory, methods, research. New York: Oxford University Press.
for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. (1992). Cultural variations in emotions:
224–253. A review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 179–204.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). The cultural construction of self Miller, R. S. (1987). Empathic embarrassment: Situational and personal
and emotion: Implications for social behavior. In S. Kitayama & determinants of reactions to the embarrassment of another. Journal of
H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1061–1069.
influence (pp. 89–130). Washington, DC: American Psychological Miller, R. S. (1992). The nature and severity of self-reported embarrass-
Association. ing circumstances. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18,
Martin, L. L. (2000). Moods do not convey information: Moods in context 190–198.
do. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social Miller, R. S. (1995). On the nature of embarrassability: Shyness, social-
cognition (pp. 153–177). New York: Cambridge University Press. evaluation, and social skill. Journal of Personality, 63, 315–339.
Mathews, A. (1993). Biases in emotional processing. The Psychologist: Miller, R. S. (1996). Embarrassment: Poise and peril in everyday life.
Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 6, 493–499. New York: The Guilford Press.
Mathews, A., & Klug, F. (1993). Emotionality and interference with color- Miller, R. S. (2004). Emotion as adaptive interpersonal communication:
naming in anxiety. Behavior Research and Therapy, 29, 147–160. The case of embarrassment. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach (Eds.), The
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion social life of emotions (pp. 87–104). New York: Cambridge University
and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50. Press.
Matsumoto, D. (1987). The role of facial response in the experience of Miller, R. S., & Leary, M. R. (1992). Social sources and interactive func-
emotion: More methodological problems and a meta-analysis. Journal tions of embarrassment. In M. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior
of Personality & Social Psychology, 52(4), 769–774. (pp. 322–339). New York: Sage.
Matsumoto, D. (1989). Cultural influences on the perception of emotion. Miller, R. S., & Tangney, J. P. (1994). Differentiating embarrassment from
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20(1), 92–105. shame. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 273–287.
Matsumoto, D. (1990). Cultural similarities and differences in display Mineka, S., Rafaeli, E., & Yovel, I. (2003). Cognitive biases in emotional
rules. Motivation & Emotion, 14(3), 195–214. disorders: Information processing and social-cognitive perspectives.
Matsumoto, D., Keltner, D., Shiota, M. N., O’Sullivan, & Frank, M. (2008). In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of
Facial expressions of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & affective sciences (pp. 976–1009). New York: Oxford University Press.
L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 211–234). New York: Mineka, S., & Sutton, S. K. (1992). Cognitive biases and the emotional
Guilford Press. disorders. Psychological Science, 3, 65–69.
References 349

Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of Ochsner, K. N. (2008). The social–emotional processing stream: Five
personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, core constructs and their translational potential for schizophrenia and
and invariance in personality structures. Psychological Review, 102, beyond. Biological Psychiatry, 64(1), 48–61.
246–268. Öhman, A. (1986). Face the beast and fear the face: Animal and social fears
Morberg, K. U. (2003). The oxytocin factor. Cambridge, MA: De Capo, as prototypes for evolutionary analysis of emotions. Psychophysiology,
2003. 23, 123–145.
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese Öhman, A., & Dimberg, U. (1978). Facial expressions as conditioned stim-
attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and uli for electrodermal responses: A case of “preparedness?” Journal of
Social Psychology, 67, 949–971. Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1251–1258.
Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depres- Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). “Unconscious anxiety”: Phobic
sion on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,
and Social Psychology, 58, 519–527. 231–240.
Moskowitz, D. S. (1994). Cross-situational generality and the interper- Ortony, A., Clore, G., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of
sonal circumplex. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.
921–933. Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions?
Murphy, M. R., Seckl, J. R., Burton, S., Checkley, S. A., & Lightman, S. L. Psychological Review, 74, 431–461.
(1987). Changes in oxytocin and vasopressin secretion during sexual Oveis, C., Cohen, A. B., Gruber, J., Shiota, M. N., Haidt, J., & Keltner, D.
activity in men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, (2009). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with tonic but not
65(4), 738–742. phasic positive emotionality. Emotion, 9, 265–270.
Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human
Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739. Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1992). Appraisal as a cause of emo-
Nelson, E. E., & Panksepp, J. (1996). Oxytocin mediates acquisition of tion. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
maternally associated odor preferences in pre-weaning rat pups. Parr, L. (2003). The discrimination of faces and their emotional con-
Behavioral Neuroscience, 110(3), 583–592. tent by chimpanzees (pan troglodytes). In P. Ekman, J. J. Campos,
Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanation of emotions. Human R. J. Davidson, & F. B. M. De Waal (Eds.), Emotions inside out: 130
Nature, 1(3), 261–289. years after Darwin’s “The expression of the emotions in man and ani-
Niedenthal, P. M. (2008). Emotion concepts. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland- mals” (pp. 56–78). New York: New York Academy of Science.
Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 587–600). Parrott, W. G., & Smith, S. F. (1991). Embarrassment: Actual vs. typical
New York: Guilford Press. cases and classical vs. prototypical representations. Cognition and
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Emotion, 5, 467–488.
Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emo- Pellegrini, A. D. (1992). Rough-and-tumble play and social problem solv-
tion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211. ing flexibility. Creativity Research Journal, 5(1), 13–26.
Niedenthal, P. M., & Halberstadt, J. H. (2000). Grounding categories in Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Confession, inhibition, and disease. In
emotional response. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
of affect in social cognition (pp. 357–386). New York: Cambridge (Vol. 22, pp. 211–244). San Diego: Academic Press.
University Press. Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a thera-
Niedenthal, P. M., & Setterlund, M. B. (1994). Emotion congruence in per- peutic process. Psychological Science, 8, 162–166.
ception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 401–411. Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological
Nussbaum, M. (1996). Compassion: The basic social emotion. Social aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review
Philosophy and Policy, 13, 27–58. of Psychology, 54, 547–577.
Nussbaum, M. (2001). Upheavals of thought: The intelligence of emo- Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. D. (1999). Forming a story: The health ben-
tions. New York: Cambridge University Press. efits of narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1243–1254.
Oatley, K. (1993). Social construction in emotion. In M. Lewis & Phan, K. L., Wager, T. D., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2004). Functional
J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 342–352). New York: neuroimaging studies of human emotions. CNS Spectrum, 9(4),
Guilford Press. 258–266.
Oatley, K. (2003). Creative expression and communication of emotions Phelps, E. (2006). Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of the
in the visual and narrative arts. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 27–53.
H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 481–502). Plant, A., Hyde, J. S., Keltner, D., & Devine, P. D. (2000). The gender ste-
New York: Oxford University Press. reotyping of emotions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 80–91.
Oatley, K. (2004). Emotions: A brief history. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Porges, S. (1998). Love: An emergent property of the mammalian auto-
Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1992). Human emotions: Function and nomic nervous system. Psychoendocrinology, 23, 837–861.
dysfunction. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 55–85. Preuschoft, S. (1992). “Laughter” and “smile” in Barbary macaques
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of (Macaca sylvanus). Ethology, 91, 220–236.
emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29–50. Provine, R. R. (1992). Contagious laughter: Laughter is a sufficient stimulus
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1996). The communicative theory of for laughs and smiles. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 1–4.
emotions: Empirical tests, mental models, and implications for social Quigley, B., & Tedeschi, J. (1996). Mediating effects of blame attributions
interaction. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: on feelings of anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22,
Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 363–393). 1280–1288.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal:
Oatley, K., Keltner, D., & Jenkins, J. M. (2006). Understanding emotions Motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making.
(2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 79, 56–77.
350 Emotion

Reeve, J. (1989). The interest–enjoyment distinction in intrinsic motiva- Ruch, W. (1995). Will the real relationship between facial expression and
tion. Motivation and Emotion, 13, 83–103. affective experience stand up: The case of exhilaration. Cognition and
Reisenzein, R. (1983). The Schachter theory of emotion: Two decades Emotion, 9, 33–58.
later. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 239–264. Rudolph, U., Roesch, S. C., Greitemeyer, T., & Weiner, B. (2004). A meta-
Rimé, B., Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Zech, E., & Philippot, P. (1998). analytic review of help giving and aggression from an attributional per-
Social sharing of emotion: New evidence and new questions. European spective: Contributions to a general theory of motivation. Cognition &
Review of Social Psychology, 9, 145–189. Emotion, 18, 815–848.
Rimé, B., Mesquita, B., Philippot, P., & Boca, S. (1991). Beyond the emo- Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions.
tional event: Six studies on the social sharing of emotions. Cognition Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426–450.
and Emotion, 5, 435–465. Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial
Roberts, T. A., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1995). Gender differences in per- expression? A review of methods and studies. Psychological Bulletin,
ceiving internal state: Toward a his-and-hers model of perceptual cue 115, 102–141.
use. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of
(Vol. 27, pp. 143–176). New York: Academic Press. emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172.
Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2000). Safer, M. A., Bonanno, G. A., & Field, N. P. (2001). It was never that bad:
The role of honor-related values in the elicitation, experience, and Biased recall of grief and long-term adjustment to the death of a spouse.
communication of pride, shame, and anger: Spain and the Netherlands Memory, 9, 195–204.
compared. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 833–844.
Salovey, P. (1991). The psychology of jealousy and envy. New York:
Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004). Inside Guilford Press.
the heart of emotion: On culture and relational concerns. In L. Z. Tiedens &
Sartre, J. P. (1957). Existentialism and human emotions. New York:
C. W. Leach, The social life of emotions (pp. 187– 202). New York:
Kensington.
Cambridge University Press.
Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, Sauter, D. A., & Scott, S. K. (2007). More than one kind of happiness: Can
10, 284–294. we recognize vocal expressions of different positive states? Motivation
and Emotion, 31, 192–199.
Romney, A. K., Moore, C. C., & Rusch, C. D. (1997). Cultural univer-
sals: Measuring the semantic structure of emotion terms in English Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social and physiological
and Japanese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.
5489–5494. Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: A review and a model for
Rosch, E. H. (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328–350. future research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 143–165.
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion: A structural the- Scherer, K. R. (1997). The role of culture in emotion antecedent appraisal.
ory. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 902–922.
Vol. 5. Emotions, relationships and health (Vol. 5, pp. 11–36). Beverly Scherer, K. R., Johnstone, T., & Klasmeyer, G. (2003). Vocal expression of
Hills, CA: Sage. emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.),
Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Handbook of affective science (pp. 433–456). New York: Oxford
Cognition and Emotion, 5, 161–200. University Press.
Roseman, I. J., Spindel, M. S., & Jose, P. E. (1990). Appraisals of emotion- Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. B. (1994). Evidence for universality and
eliciting events: Testing a theory of discrete emotions. Journal of cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal
Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 899–915. of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–328.
Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2002). Cultural influences on the
and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and relation between pleasant emotions and unpleasant emotions: Asian
Social Psychology, 67, 206–221. dialectic philosophies or individualism–collectivism? Cognition and
Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Emotion, 16, 705–719.
Review of General Psychology, 2, 247–270. Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motiva-
Rottenberg, J., & Johnson, S. L. (Eds.). (2007). Emotion and psychopa- tional functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino
thology: Bridging affective and clinical science. Washington, DC: (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561).
American Psychological Association. New York: Guilford Press.
Rozin, P. (1996). Towards a psychology of food and eating: From motiva- Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments
tion to module to model to marker, morality, meaning, and metaphor. of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 18–24. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (1997). The symbolic self in evolution-
Review, 94, 23–41. ary context. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 80–102.
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2000). Disgust. In M. Lewis & Shaver, P., Schwartz, J. C., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion
J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 637–653). knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of
New York: Guilford Press. Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypoth- Shearn, D., Bergman, E., Hill, K., Abel, A., & Hinds, L. (1990). Facial
esis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, dis- coloration and temperature responses in blushing. Psychophysiology,
gust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal 27, 687–693.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 574–586. Shearn, D., Bergman, E., Hill, K., Abel, A., & Hinds, L. (1992). Blushing
Rozin, P., & Singh, L. (1999). The moralization of cigarette smoking in as a function of audience size. Psychophysiology, 29, 431–436.
America. Journal of Consumer Behavior, 8, 321–337. Shields, S. A. (1991). Gender in the psychology of emotion: A selec-
Ruch, W. (1993). Exhilaration and humor. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland tive research review. In K. T. Strongman (Ed.), International review
(Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 605–616). New York: Guilford of studies in emotion (Vol. 1, pp. 227–245). Chichester, England:
Press Publications. Wiley.
References 351

Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion disposi- Tiedens, L. Z., Ellsworth, P. C., & Mesquita, B. (2000). Stereotypes about
tions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment sentiments and status: Emotional expectations for high- and low-status
style. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 61–71. group members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(5),
Shweder, R. A., & Haidt, J. (2000). The cultural psychology of the emotions: 560–574.
Ancient and new. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of Tiedens, L. Z., & Leach, C. W. (2004). The social life of emotions.
emotions (2nd ed., pp. 397–414). New York: Guilford Press. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The “big Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty
three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity), and the “big three” and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information pro-
explanations of suffering. In A. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.), Morality and cessing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 973–988.
health (pp. 119–169). New York: Routledge. Tomkins, S. S. (1984). Affect theory. In K. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.),
Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of Approaches to emotion (pp. 163–195). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
interest. Emotion, 5, 89–102. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional
Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and
Psychological Review, 74, 29–39. Sociobiology, 11, 375–424.
Simpson, J. A., & Kenrick, D. T. (1998). Evolutionary social psychology. Tracy, J. L., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous display of
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal dis-
Skinner, B. F. (1948). Walden two. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. plays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105,
Small, D. A., & Lerner, J. S. (2008). Emotional policy: Personal sadness 11655–11660.
and anger shape judgments about a welfare case. Political Psychology, Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Show your pride: Evidence for a dis-
29, 149–168. crete emotion expression. Psychological Science, 15, 94–97.
Small, D. A., Lerner, J. S., & Fischoff, B. (2006). Emotion priming and Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). Emerging insights into the nature
spontaneous attributions for terrorism: Americans’ reactions in a and function of pride. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
national field experiment. Political Psychology, 27, 289–298. 16, 147–150.
Smith, C., & Ellsworth, P. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emo- Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P. (2007). The self-conscious
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838. emotions: Theory and research. New York: Guilford Press.
Smoski, M. J., & Bachorowski, J.-A. (2003). Antiphonal laughter between Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly
friends and strangers. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 327–340. Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.
Snibbe, A. C., & Markus, H. R. (2005). You can’t always get what you want: Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral conse-
Educational attainment, agency, and choice. Journal of Personality quences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 242–259.
and Social Psychology, 88, 703–720. Tsai, J. L., Chentsova-Dutton, Y., Friere-Bebeau, L. H., & Przymus, D.
Snowdon, C. T. (2003). Expression of emotion in nonhuman animals. (2002). Emotional expression and physiology in European Americans
In R. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affec- and Hmong Americans. Emotion, 2, 380–397.
tive science (pp. 457–480). London England, Oxford University Press. Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in
Solomon, R. C. (1976). The passions. New York: Anchor Press/Doubleday. affect valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90,
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). State–trait anxiety inventory for adults. Palo 288–307.
Alto, CA: Mind Garden. Tsai, J. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Cultural influences on emotional
Spielberger, C. D. (1996). Manual for the state–trait anger expression responding: Chinese American and European American dating couples
inventory (STAXI). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. during interpersonal conflict. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
Stemmler, D. G. (1989). The autonomic differentiation of emotions revis- 28, 600–625.
ited: Convergent and discriminant validation. Psychophysiology, 26, Tsai, J. L., Levenson, R. W., & Carstensen, L. L. (2000). Autonomic,
617–632. expressive, and subjective responses to emotional films in older and
Tangney, J. P. (1990). Assessing individual differences in proneness to shame younger Chinese American and European American adults. Psychology
and guilt: Development of the self-conscious affect and attribution inven- and Aging, 15, 684–693.
tory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(1), 102–111. Tsai, J. L., Levenson, R. W., & McCoy, K. (2006). Cultural and tempera-
Tangney, J. P. (1991). Moral affect: The good, the bad, and the ugly. mental variation in emotional response. Emotion, 6, 484–497.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 598–607. Tsai, J. L., Louie, J., Chen, E. E., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Learning what feel-
Tangney, J. P. (1992). Situational determinants of shame and guilt in young ings to desire: Socialization of ideal affect through children’s story-
adulthood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 199–206. books. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 17–30.
Tangney, J. P., & Fischer, K. W. (1995). Self-conscious emotions: The Van Boven, L., & Ashworth, L (2007). Looking forward, looking back-
psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride. New York: ward: Anticipation is more evocative than retrospection. Journal of
Guilford Press. Experimental Psychology, General, 136, 289–300.
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, Van de Ven, P., Bornholt, L., & Bailey, M. (1996). Measuring cogni-
guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Personality tive, affective, and behavioral components of homophobic reaction.
and Social Psychology, 70, 1256–1264. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 155–179.
Tapias, M., Glaser, J., Vasquez, K. V., Keltner, D., & Wickens, T. (2007). van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1972). A comparative approach to the phylogeny
Emotion and prejudice: Specific emotions toward outgroups. Group of laughter and smiling. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Nonverbal communica-
Processes and InterGroup Relations, 10, 27–41. tion. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Taylor, S. E. (2002). The tending instinct. New York: Henry Holt. Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Pietroni, D., & Manstead, A. S. R.
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, (2006). Power and emotion in negotiation: Power moderates the
R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress interpersonal effects of anger and happiness on concession making.
in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological European Journal of Social Psychology: Special Issue on Social
Review, 107, 411–429. Power, 36, 557–581.
352 Emotion

Van Kleef, G. A., Oveis, C., van der Löwe, I., LuoKogan, A., Goetz, J., & Willis, F. N., & Hamm, H. K. (1980). The use of interpersonal touch in
Keltner, D. (2008). Power, distress, and compassion: Turning a blind securing compliance. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 5(1), 49–55.
eye to the suffering of others. Psychological Science, 19, 1315–1322. Wilson, T., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental
Vanman, E. J., Paul, B. Y., Ito, T. A., & Miller, N. (1997). The modern face of correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations.
prejudice and structural features that moderate the effect of cooperation Psychological Bulletin, 116, 117–142.
on affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 941–959. Wilson, T., Centerbar, D., & Brekke, N. (2002). Mental contamination and
Vanman, E. J., Saltz, J. L., Nathan, L. R., & Warren, J. A. (2004). Racial dis- the debiasing problem. In T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, & D. Kahneman
crimination by low-prejudiced Whites: Facial movements as implicit mea- (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment
sures of attitudes related to behavior. Psychological Science, 15, 711–714. (pp. 185–200). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Vasquez, K., Keltner, D., Ebenbach, D. H., & Banaszynski, T. L. (2001). Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining why: A model of
Cultural variation and similarity in moral rhetorics: Voices from the affective adaptation. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 3,
Philippines and United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 372–388.
32, 93–120. Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axson, D.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: General and (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting.
specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821–836.
factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441–476. Winkielman, P., Knutson, B., Paulus, M. P., & Trujillo, J. T. (2007).
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali- Affective influence on decisions: Moving towards the core mecha-
dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS nisms. Review of General Psychology, 11, 179–192.
scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R., & Schwartz, N. (1997). Subliminal affective
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Towards a consensual structure of priming resists attributional intervention. Cognition and Emotion, 11,
mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235. 433–465.
Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Nice to know you: Positive Witt, D. M., Carter, C., & Walton, C. (1990). Central and peripheral effects
emotions, self–other overlap, and complex understanding in the forma- of oxytocin administration. Physiology and Behavior, 37, 63–69.
tion of a new relationship. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 93–106. Wood, J. V., Heimpel, S. A., & Michaela, J. L. (2003). Savoring versus
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and dampening: Self-esteem differences in regulating positive affect.
emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 566–580.
Weiner, B., Graham, S., & Reyna, C. (1997). An attributional examina- Woodruff, P. (2002). Reverence: Renewing a forgotten virtue. New York:
tion of retributive versus utilitarian philosophies of punishment. Social Oxford University Press.
Justice Research, 10, 431–452. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences.
Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.
more severe. Psychological Science, 16, 780–785. Zajonc, R. B. (1998). Emotions. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey
Williams, J. R., Insel, T. R., Harbaugh, C. R., & Carter, C. S. (1994). (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 591–634).
Oxytocin administered centrally facilitates formation of a partner New York: McGraw Hill.
preference in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Journal of Zald, D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of
Neuroendocrinology, 6, 247–250. sensory stimuli. Brain Research Reviews, 41, 88–123.
Chapter 10

Attitudes
MAHZARIN R. BANAJI AND LARISA HEIPHETZ

From the simplest and most ordinary acts to the highly be the central concept? To these pioneers, understanding
complex and rare ones our species can perform—eating a the predisposition to treat entities with favor or disfavor
fruit or spitting out a chili pepper, gazing intently into an seemed even more basic to understanding social relations
infant’s face or scrupulously avoiding a neighbor, saving than the faculties of thought and knowledge. Writing in
a life or taking one at a moment’s notice—we are crea- the first handbook, Gordon Allport (1935) provided some
tures of preferences. Bundles of preferences characterize insight when he says that the popularity of the attitude
every living organism; without them, plants would not turn concept “is not difficult to explain. It has come into favor,
toward the Sun and cockroaches would not run away from first of all, because it is not the property of any one
it. In us, preferences exist not only in these built-in forms psychological school of thought . . . furthermore, it is a
shared with other living beings but in distinctly human concept which escapes the ancient controversy concerning
ways, such as the consciously molded attitudes we convey the relative influence of heredity and environment . . . The
through artistic expression, the moral codes by which we term is likewise elastic enough to apply either to the dispo-
judge our worth and our failings, or the words we craft to sitions of single individuals or to broad patterns of culture”
describe imagined utopias. (p. 798). Indeed, as Ross, Lepper, and Ward (this volume)
The group that gave this concept scientific birth in the point out, the study of attitudes has continued to be a cor-
early 20th century chose attitude as the name to refer to nerstone of social psychology.
such preferences. So intently did the pioneers focus on a A hundred years after the study of attitudes came to be a
study of attitudes that the field of social psychology came legitimate science, Mitchell (2009) has offered an intrigu-
to be synonymous with the study of this single concept ing pair of observations: First, a small set of concepts
(Bogardus, 1931; Thomas & Znaniecki, 1918). It is, in involving preferences and attitudes, as well as an under-
this sense, the oldest of the children displayed in the family standing of the minds of others and ourselves (see Epley &
photographs that constitute these handbooks, the oldest Waytz, this volume), have been the core concerns of social
child that is assured front and center seat in every previ- psychology. Second, although this cluster of self, mind
ous photograph taken (see Gilbert, Fiske, & Lindzey, perception, and attitude has seemed to be a somewhat arbi-
1998; Lindzey, 1954; Lindzey & Aronson, 1969, 1985; trary grab bag, it may not be so at least in one sense; it
Murchison, 1935). now appears that these processes share a common neural
Why does the study of attitudes have this status? What substrate in the medial prefrontal cortex.
made the first social psychologists decide, even as they So vast is the topic of attitudes—covering as it does all
were still marking the boundaries of their new country, forms of preferences and evaluations, measured in a diver-
that evaluations along the good-bad continuum should sity of ways, toward all manner of things, events, ideas and

Our thanks to Claire McGuire, who shared with us an unabridged, unpublished version of the 1985 handbook chapter on attitudes by
William McGuire; it inspired us even as we became aware of our own more modest treatment. We are especially grateful to experts
and friends for pointing out relevant works and/or providing comments and corrections with lightning speed: Sanden Averett, Wil
Cunningham, Susan Fiske, Daniel Gilbert, Richard Hackman, Andrea Heberlein, John Jost, Jason McCoy, Brian Nosek, Bill Swann, and
Talee Ziv.
We were supported by the National Science Foundation (grant #BCS-0642448) and a National Science Graduate Fellowship while we
worked on this chapter.

353

CH10.indd 348 10/5/09 7:19:21 PM


Landmarks 354

people—that even before the research had acquired today’s no other contributions, modern researchers should still
sprawl, some questioned the value of a term that seemed be grateful. These measurement men achieved the previ-
to be so undiscriminating in scope (McDougall, 1933). ously unthinkable; for the first time ever, they took the
It is hardly surprising, then, that different periods of the ephemeral mental quality of favoring and disfavoring and
last century have been partial to particular aspects of rendered it the subject of scientific study. Writing under
the concept of attitude, attending more to the questions the title “Attitudes Can Be Measured,” Thurstone (1928a)
of structure, function, or mechanisms of change. Likewise, intended to erase all doubt about whether feelings toward
different questions have varied in dominance, depending things could be measured by comparing them with physi-
on the availability of new technologies and innovations cal objects such as tables: “We say without hesitation that
such as census tracking, surveys, telephones, the computer, we measure a man when we take some anthropometric
and the Internet, not to mention the ability to measure elec- measures of him . . . his height or weight or what not. Just
trical activity and blood oxygenation levels. in the same sense we shall say here that we are measuring
Moreover, at different periods in American history, ideol- attitudes” (p. 530).
ogy and propaganda, consumer behavior, and intergroup rela- Eighty years later, psychologists must show whether
tions have differentially grabbed interest and dictated where they have delivered on this remarkable first attempt to mea-
the most precious attitudes may be excavated. (For previous sure the immeasurable—people’s likes and dislikes. This
chapters on the topic of attitudes in earlier handbooks in this chapter engages the question of measurement more so than
series, see Allport, 1935; Eagly & Chaiken, 1998; Green, some others because the decades since the 1980s have wit-
1954; McGuire, 1968, 1985; Petty & Wegener, 1998; for nessed a surge of measures that involve response latencies
major volumes devoted to the concept of attitudes published and other ways of inferring preferences (Bargh, Chaiken,
since the late 1980s, see also Albarracin, Johnson, & Zanna, Govender, & Pratto, 1992; Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, &
2005; Eagly & Chaiken, 1993; Gawronski, 2007; Maio & Kardes, 1986; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). In
Olson, 2000; Petty, Fazio, & Brinol, 2008; Petty & Krosnick, addition, researchers have developed and enhanced mea-
1995; Pratkanis, Breckler, & Greenwald, 1989). Because the sures of physiology (Blascovich & Mendes, this volume)
research covered here must necessarily be limited to works and brain activity (Lieberman, this volume). Since the pub-
appearing since the publication of the last handbook in 1998, lication of the last handbook, the very first studies of atti-
this chapter mentions some of the foundational research tudes using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
topics that must be largely set aside. There is no danger in have appeared (Cunningham, Johnson, Gatenby, Gore, &
doing so, however, given the remarkable treatment these sub- Banaji, 2003; Hart et al., 2000; Phelps et al., 2000). In the
jects have received in previous handbooks and other current time since these first tentative fMRI experiments, there is
volumes. already a trajectory pointing out the subcortical and corti-
The experimental analysis of attitudes, as well as its cal systems that support attitude formation, which may be
classical and modern history, could not have had a more involved in the subjective experience of preference, and
painstaking reviewer than William J. McGuire, who the control exerted by conscious processes to modulate
wrote both the 1969 and 1985 chapters on the subject and less conscious ones.
even attempted to predict what the two decades beyond After the 1930s, measurement ceased to be as intense a
would hold. The 1998 handbook was the first to include focus, although there were consistent contributions moti-
two chapters on attitudes: one focusing on the basics of vated by various factors, including concerns about threats
structure and function (Eagly & Chaiken, 1998) and the to validity, the specificity of measures (Ajzen & Fishbein,
other focusing on persuasion and attitude change (Petty & 1980), recognition of the interdependence of theory and
Wegener, 1998). They likewise provide excellent closure method (Ostrom, 1989), and statistical advances (Bentler,
on the first century’s contributions. 1980). This new century returns to the fore a set of epis-
temological questions concerning how scientists may
know the nature of preferences. Such a focus is possible
LANDMARKS because the field as a whole has turned toward the study
of the cognitive unconscious (see Gilbert, 1991; Hassin,
The earliest decades of attitude research, the 1920s and Uleman, & Bargh, 2005; Uleman & Bargh, 1989). This
1930s, were marked by a strong commitment to mea- chapter describes dozens of experiments concerning
surement (Bogardus, 1925; Guttman, 1941; Likert, 1932; unconscious attitudes as a substantial contribution of the
Thurstone, 1928a; see Himmelfarb, 1993). From these attitude literature.
early contributors psychologists have the staples of formal The 1950s and 1960s witnessed the grand era of atti-
scaling techniques. If this era of attitude research made tude research with one stream of work beginning at Yale

CH10.indd 349 10/5/09 7:19:22 PM


355 Attitudes

University and expanding to Ohio State University. Early Johnson, and Zanna (specifically see chapters by Brinol &
research using behaviorist learning theory as a model for Petty, 2005; Johnson, Maio, & Smith-McLallen, 2005;
understanding attitude change emerged out of the World Prislin & Wood, 2005; Wegener & Carlston, 2005; Wyer
War II program of Hovland, Lumsdaine, and Sheffield and Albarracin, 2005).
(1949), and prospered in the work of Hovland’s student If attitudes are predispositions to act favorably or unfa-
William McGuire (1960, 1961). In the decades that fol- vorably, then the attitudes that one has should predict one’s
lowed, these ideas gave birth to the notion of cognitive behaviors. From the 1930s on, however, studies showed the
responses as determinants of attitude change, and a series weak prediction of behavior from attitude (e.g., LaPiere,
of edited volumes on attitudinal foundations, structure, 1934). This conception escalated in the writings of Wicker
function, strength, and implicit measures have appeared, (1969), who provided an analysis of 42 studies that pro-
continuing to this day (Greenwald, Brock, & Ostrom, 1968; duced a low overall attitude-behavior correlation. Over the
Petty & Krosnick, 1995; Petty et al., 2008; Petty, Ostrom, & past several decades, scholars have devoted a great deal
Brock, 1981; Pratkanis et al., 1989). Two significant dual- of attention to figuring out when attitudes and behaviors
process models of attitude change were offered by Petty are related and when they are not. Research shows that
and Cacioppo (1984) and Chaiken and Eagly (1983; see the attitude-behavior relationship depends on the person, the
also Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). Their theories may be situation, the attitude (e.g., how much knowledge people
viewed as precursors to the spurt of later research on dual- have about the attitude object), and the measurement
process accounts of attitudes, that is, processes that divulge match between attitudes and behavior (e.g., are researchers
their conscious and unconscious components (Albarracin & measuring specific attitudes and specific behaviors or spe-
Vargas, this volume; Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Devine, cific attitudes but general behaviors; see Ajzen & Fishbein,
1989; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2007; Lieberman, 1975, 1977; Fabrigar, Petty, Smith, & Crites, 2006; Fazio &
Gaunt, Gilbert, & Trope, 2002; Payne, Burkley, & Stokes, Zanna, 1981; Kelman, 1974; Lord, Lepper, & Mackie,
2008; Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000). 2008; Smith, Terry, & Hogg, 2006).
A second stream of attitude research that developed in The engagement with questions of persuasion and atti-
the 1950s and 1960s came from the ideas of Fritz Heider, tude change has slowed down relative to the intensity of
Charles Osgood, and Leon Festinger, each of whom was such work in the decades of the 1960s to 1990s, whereas
uniquely inspired by the idea of the consistency between other areas of interest have grown. Fortunately, a pair of
mental components and the complex processes that are set chapters on the topic of attitudes is offered in this hand-
in motion to achieve mental alignment (see Abelson et al., book, and other experts fully cover persuasion and attitude
1968). Even today, the sophistication of the theorizing and change (see Albarracin & Vargas, this volume).
the quintessential style of research on cognitive dissonance When considering the structure of attitudes, a domi-
capture the imagination of students, so counterintuitive the nant way of thinking of evaluations as consisting of three
ideas and so extravagant the experiments. These scholars components—affect, cognition, and behavior—has been a
emphasized attitude change and the bidirectional relationship guiding force for much of the century; only in recent years
between attitudes and behavior. Perhaps it was the public’s has this perspective faded as the preferred way of thinking.
engagement in social change during the 1960s that kept this Because of the influence of this tradition, studying what
interest in questions of attitude change alive, and psycholo- Eagly and Chaiken (1993) refer to as intra-attitudinal
gists studied transformations of all kinds of preferences— structure has involved a close look at the interrelation-
those toward widgets, robots, and Fidel Castro being not the ships among these three variables, with a focus on beliefs
strangest of them all. The idea of cognitive-affective consis- as the building blocks of attitudes. A second way to study
tency and its consequences displayed in some of the most attitude structure has involved looking at interattitudinal
prominent psychological research of that time continue to structure—that is, the relationship between and among
have direct bearing on modern work on cognitive dissonance multiple attitudes—and here focus has been directed to the
(see Cooper, 2007; Harmon-Jones, 1999). consistency among various evaluations. Among the impor-
The study of attitude change and persuasion is not lim- tant analyses of intra-attitude structure, a primary one has
ited to the most well-known laboratories. This topic has been on the question of how attitudes are represented, and
dominated research since the mid-20th century, and this specifically their possibly bipolar structure (Judd & Kulik,
is visible in the much larger proportion of pages devoted to 1980; Sherif, Sherif, & Nebergall, 1965; for an argument
this subject in most previous reviews of the concept in this against the view of attitudes as bipolar, see Kerlinger,
handbook’s predecessors. For readers who are interested in 1984). The remainder of the research on intra-attitudinal
the question of persuasion, several noteworthy treatments are structure has been consumed with the all-important question
available in the 2005 Handbook of Attitudes by Albarracin, of the role of beliefs, conceived of as associations between

CH10.indd 350 10/5/09 7:19:22 PM


Definitions of Attitude 356

an attitude and its various attributes, and their ability the self, better known in the field as self-esteem, is another
to predict attitudes. A prominent contribution here is example of an ego-enhancing preference that keeps the
Fishbein’s expected-value model (1963), which conceptu- self “lifted” and able to function in a world that constantly
alizes attitudes as a function of beliefs, particularly their demands comparisons with others.
subjective probabilities. In this model, attitudes consist Finally, attitudes that help individuals express their core
of evaluations of particular beliefs. The idea that attitudes values or foundational aspects of themselves are assumed to
are a function ofthe totality of the evaluative beliefs about serve the fourth, or value-expressive, function. These atti-
the attitude object may seem so obvious as to be banal, tudes may be inherently rewarding insofar as expressing
but this assertion allowed empirical prediction of attitudes one’s core values is gratifying. Just such a process may be at
from knowledge of beliefs. A theory of mathematical preci- work in the process of self-affirmation, which among other
sion, one might even say an aesthetic theory, is Anderson’s consequences demonstrates the power of value-expression
account of information integration (1971, 1981); this model to diminish feelings of self-threat (e.g., eliminates dissonance
of some generality was also applied to the analysis of atti- effects; Steele & Liu, 1983). Value-expressive attitudes are
tudes. Multiplying expectancies with values was deemed privileged in other ways as well; research has shown that
insufficient to explain the relationship between beliefs and such attitudes are particularly resistant to change (Maio &
attitudes, and the theory focused instead on the value of the Olson, 1995) and promote commitment to relevant behav-
incoming information and its integration into the current iors (e.g., volunteerism; Lydon & Zanna, 1990; Murray,
attitude. Haddock, & Zanna, 1996). The final section of this chapter
A final landmark from the first century concerns the takes on the question of ideology; underlying that discus-
basic question of pragmatics: Why do people have atti- sion is the assumption that “thick” attitudes such as ideol-
tudes? As the first to explicitly theorize about attitude func- ogy, composed as they are of several strands of individual
tions, M. B. Smith (1947) laid out several objectives that attitudes, especially may serve a value-expressive function.
evaluations can accomplish (see Kruglanski & Stroebe,
2005). For instance, attitudes can serve the function of
appraisal, informing individuals as to whether approach or DEFINITIONS OF ATTITUDE
avoid is the correct response. However, different attitudes
may serve this function to different degrees; this role is ful- Definitions are hair-raising, in the sense of creating excite-
filled more by readily accessible attitudes, and this concept ment and terror. Allport (1935) offered 16 definitions of
of attitude accessibility has dominated current thinking attitude that others before him had generated, only to slip
(Fazio, 2000). in a 17th of his own that has been so well-cited that any stu-
Katz (1960) proposed four functions of attitudes that dent of attitudes is able to recite it even when half asleep:
have intuitive appeal even today. They include a utilitarian “A mental and neural state of readiness, organized through
function, a knowledge function, an ego-defensive function, experience, exerting a directive or dynamic influence upon
and a value-expressive function. Utilitarian attitudes are the individual’s response to all objects and situations with
those that help individuals obtain rewards and avoid pun- which it is related” (p. 810).
ishments. For example, holding a positive attitude toward McGuire (1968) regarded definitions to be “stifling and
capital punishment might create a feeling of belonging yet, since we have to use words when we talk, it probably
within a certain ideological group while avoiding poten- helps to sketch out occasionally what we mean by our terms”
tial rejection from these socially significant others. Indeed, (p. 142). Instead of providing a new one, he brilliantly side-
Kelman (1958) pointed out that attitudes can foster identi- stepped the issue by using Allport’s definition and dissect-
fication with social groups, and Shavitt and Nelson (1999; ing each of its terms and phrases not only to focus on the
see Shavitt, 1989) added that attitudes toward specific various meanings of “mental and neural” and “organized”
issues assist in conveying critical information about our- but also to riff on the philosophical orientations, from posi-
selves to others. Attitudes that meet the second, or knowl- tivist to interactionist, that a phrase such as “readiness to
edge, function allow an understanding of the situations in respond” evokes. In the 1985 handbook chapter, McGuire
which one finds oneself. Those that meet the third, ego- is no more forthcoming regarding a definition, offering only
defensive, function (or externalization; Smith, Bruner, & that empirical investigations have used as a working defini-
White, 1956), protect the individual from psychic threats. tion of attitude “responses that locate ‘objects of thought’
Katz (1960) associated this purpose primarily with preju- on ‘dimensions of judgment’” (p. 239).
dice, arguing from psychodynamic principles that indi- Eagly and Chaiken (1993, 1998) provided a simple and
viduals may project feelings of inferiority onto stigmatized intuitive definition that wins on ease and broad endorse-
outgroups (see Fein & Spencer, 1997). An attitude toward ment. According to them, an attitude is “a psychological

CH10.indd 351 10/5/09 7:19:24 PM


357 Attitudes

tendency that is expressed by evaluating a particular entity created by other factors inherent in the viewer ’s perceptual
with some degree of favor or disfavor” (1993, p. 1). Others and reasoning systems. Given the data using implicit mea-
have agreed that the concept of evaluation is central to the sures of attitudes that have accumulated since the 1980s,
definition of attitude, noting that “[a]ttitudes have been this is an attractive way of thinking about preferences.
defined in a variety of ways, but at the core is the notion Attitudes are extraordinarily malleable: Implicit pref-
of evaluation” (Petty, Wegener, & Fabrigar, 1997, p. 611). erence for Whites decreases in the presence of a Black
Likewise, Crano and Prislin (2006) brought together diverse experimenter (Lowery, Hardin, & Sinclair, 2001) and a
characterizations of attitudes through their definition: likable experimenter who is perceived as holding egalitar-
“Attitudes are the evaluative judgments that integrate and ian views (Sinclair, Lowery, Hardin, & Colangelo, 2005).
summarize . . . cognitive/affective reactions” (p. 347). Implicit racial preferences also change depending on the
Among the changes that the concept of attitude has under- stimulus materials: Participants who viewed a photograph
gone over the century, none is as clear as the stripping away of African Americans taken in front of a church showed
of all ancillary items to leave the core idea of “favor and less bias than those who saw a photograph taken on an
disfavor” intact. urban street corner (Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 2001), and
The major objection to such a definition comes from participants who saw pictures of popular Blacks before
those who endorse the position that attitudes have been taking an Implicit Association Test (IAT) showed less
mischaracterized as entities of some permanence. The racial bias than those who did not view the positive pic-
boldest criticism of the standard view of attitudes is tures (Dasgupta & Greenwald, 2001). Emphasizing sex
found in the position of Schwarz and Bohner (2001), who rather than race can also decrease bias (Mitchell, Nosek, &
take their lead from theories of situated cognition (e.g., Banaji, 2003). Malleability is not limited to racial attitudes;
Barsalou, 2005; Smith & Semin, 2004). Their view can be context also influences evaluations of everything from ciga-
summarized as follows: Attitude theorists have tradition- rettes to Bill Clinton, Mike Tyson, chocolate, and the beach
ally defined their construct as if it represents fixed “things” (see Ferguson & Bargh, 2004; Ferguson, Bargh, & Nayak,
that sit in memory waiting to be pulled out, used, and put 2005; Pratto & Shih, 2000; Seibt, Hafner, & Deutsch, 2007;
back in place. Instead, Schwarz and Bohner claim that atti- Sherman, Rose, Koch, Presson, & Chassin, 2003). These
tudes are more parsimoniously conceived of as evaluations data support a constructionist-contextualist view that is
that are “formed when needed, rather than enduring personal counterintuitive; it seems easier to imagine preferences as
dispositions” (Schwarz, 2007, p. 639; see also Wilson & enduring because that is the intuitive, self-reflective sense
Hodges, 1992). The intuitive appeal of the standard defini- most people have of their own preferences.
tion that attitudes represent “a tendency,” that is, a thing of In response to such evidence regarding the malleability
stability, he argues, comes from the appeal of all disposi- of attitudes, Fazio (2007) has offered a persuasive coun-
tional accounts such as the fundamental attribution error terpoint to the radical notion of attitudes-as-constructions.
(Ross, 1977). Instead, in line with other commentaries Although fully open to the idea of the flexibility of atti-
that are consistent in their rendition of the attitude concept tudes, he points out that an extreme “attitudes as on-the-
(e.g., Lord & Lepper, 1999; Smith & DeCoster, 2000; spot constructions” view must deal with the opposing
Zaller & Feldman, 1992), Schwarz favors the idea of atti- evidence that some attitudes seem to consistently pop out
tude construal—the view that attitudes do not necessarily in the same form across time and situations. Given that
exist in some preformed state but can be built, created, human beings do learn, and learning means remembering
generated, on the spot, in fine-tuned response to contextual in some form, why should attitudes not reveal some stabil-
demands. Attitudes, conceived of in this way, are viewed as ity? They are, after all, products of repeated experiences
potentially adaptive reactions to environmental demands. that reinforce particular object-attribute pairings and pro-
Such a view is in synch with mounting empirical evidence vide similar responses to the same stimulus over repeti-
pointing toward attitude variability across time and situa- tions. Thus, a strong constructionist view must answer the
tions, often producing large differences with minimal vari- question of why savings in memory would explain other
ation in the instantiation of the attitude object. aspects of mental function (e.g., semantic knowledge) but
An argument of this nature is not entirely new to psy- not involve simple learning and retrieval of preferences.
chology. A similar challenge was put forth in the influen- Greenwald and Banaji (1995) attempted to reflect the
tial view Mischel (1968) offered concerning the concept of field’s engagement with relatively less conscious and less
personality. Mischel argued that, to a large extent, the idea controlled forms of attitudes by offering a definition of
of personality, the very essence of a person, the dimensions implicit attitudes as “introspectively unidentified (or inac-
that are assumed to lie in the disposition of the person curately identified) traces of past experience that mediate
and that seemingly provide stability, may be a mirage attributions of qualities to members of a social category”

CH10.indd 352 10/5/09 7:19:27 PM


Attitudes Are Basic 358

(p. 15). Unlike the Allport definition, no student, however potential (LPP) of the event-related brain potentials (ERP)
awake, has been known to recite this definition, but with showing that evaluative responses (e.g., answering the ques-
it, the assumption inherent in the way attitudes were tradi- tion, Is this good or bad?) are qualitatively distinguishable
tionally measured—that they must be consciously accessed from nonevaluative responses (e.g., answering the ques-
contents of the mind—was explicitly set aside. tion, Is this a vegetable or not?). The former showed sig-
nificantly larger spread over the right scalp region, whereas
the latter showed equal spread over right and left scalp
ATTITUDES ARE BASIC regions.
In line with classic Eastern and Western notions of the
The introductory material that sets the background to this three spheres of thought, feeling, and action, the earli-
chapter closes with a selection of empirical discoveries, est framework for grappling with the concept of attitude
each of which points to the basic nature of attitudes. involved a similar tripartite configuration. Attitudes were
Together, the nuggets that follow show that the evaluative assumed to consist of affective, cognitive (belief), and
dimension of information has robust orienting power, and conative (behavioral) components, a view that “came early
that the human predisposition to evaluate is fundamental and stayed late” (McGuire, 1968; cf. Brown, 1965; Krech &
to all aspects of social behavior. Readers may use this as a Crutchfield, 1948; Sherif & Cantril, 1945). In recent years,
starter list and add their own favorite demonstrations that this conceptualization has been given up in favor of one
reveal the fundamental nature of attitudes and why they that privileges the feeling component, and such a view
remain social psychology’s “most central and indispens- has been helped by discoveries that demonstrate the pri-
able construct” (Allport, 1935). macy of this factor. For example, the affective component
In the mid-twentieth century, Osgood, Suci, and of attitudes is more readily accessible than other compo-
Tannenbaum (1957) analyzed the structure of ordinary nents (Verplanken, Hofstee, & Janssen, 1998), and when
words’ meanings along three dimensions: evaluation (good- beliefs and feelings toward presidential candidates are in
bad), potency (strong-weak), and activity (active-passive). conflict, feelings are stronger predictors of voting (Lavine,
They found that the first dimension, captured through Thomsen, Zanna, & Borgida, 1998).
poles as warm-cold, good-bad, and favorable-unfavorable, To investigate the automatic nature of preferences,
accounted for most of the variance in meaning more than Glaser (1999) and Gregg (2000) attempted to derive a
twice that of the other two dimensions. series of nonsense syllables (e.g., RAS, FEQ, DAT) that
Zajonc’s (1980) influential view that “preferences need should carry no particular evaluation and serve as the per-
no inferences” alerted scientists to the possibility that even fect material that could, via association, be imbued with
when an evaluative response is not requested, the good-bad positive and negative meaning. To their surprise, they
dimension of information “pops out” and cannot be set aside; discovered that it was nearly impossible to find nonsense
it is automatically detected, engages, and registers in some words that did not evoke some relative positive or negative
way. Bargh, Chaiken, Raymond, and Hymes (1996) showed evaluation.
that such is the case by asking for mere pronunciation of Increasingly, the idea that social judgments across a
words and demonstrating an evaluative priming effect even variety of domains can be meaningfully separated into
so (see Giner-Sorolla, Garcia, & Bargh, 1999, for a simi- goodness/warmth and agency/competence dimensions has
lar effect with pictures). In the original article (Fazio et al., proved to be useful (see Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002).
1986), evidence for evaluative priming led to a reconcep- Its appeal draws from supporting empirical evidence and
tualization of attitudes as simple associations between an the intuition that even in the late Pleistocene, social inter-
object and its evaluation. This was based on their previous action must surely have required two basic assessments:
research (Fazio, Chen, McDonel & Sherman, 1982; Fazio, how good is the other (i.e., is help or harm likely?) and how
Powell, & Herr, 1983). competent is the other (i.e., how effective will action based
Because a “need to evaluate” is regarded to be universal, on intention be?). Items that belong to the warmth category
Jarvis and Petty (1996) developed a measure of it, showing (sincere, generous) are more readily accessible, spontane-
that the tendency to evaluate is a highly consistent and reli- ously generated, and regarded as more important in assess-
able single factor. Individual differences in the need to eval- ing others than items that belong to the competence category
uate (measured by face valid items such as “It bothers me to (intelligence, foresight; Wojciszke, Bazinska, & Jaworski,
remain neutral”) are meaningful predictors of behavior. 1998). This is different from the assumption made by some
In early research on brain activity, Cacioppo, Crites, intergroup theorists, who argue that competence, but not
Berntson, and Coles (1993; Cacioppo, Crites, & Gardner, warmth, is associated with high status (e.g., Fiske et al.;
1996) provided the first evidence using the late positive Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004).

CH10.indd 353 10/5/09 7:19:27 PM


359 Attitudes

OVERVIEW OF REMAINING SECTIONS Rather, these accounts provide excellent explanations for
miniature pieces on the attitude landscape. The contribu-
The remainder of the chapter presents an overview of the tions of laborers in attitude villages are primarily in the
attitude literature, focusing on the contributions that were form of new discoveries. To a large extent, their tractabil-
offered between 1995 and 2010. These analyses have been ity and quality depend on the methods, increasingly the
conducted almost entirely with human subjects, using lan- technologies, scientists are able to invent or adopt to study
guage and pictures as the primary vehicle to probe and attitudes. Some have argued that the lack of a primary
elicit evaluations, and staying close to questions of mea- focus on theory building is hardly an embarrassment. Quite
surement, structure, and change. Many of the theoretical the contrary, in the other natural and life sciences, theo-
questions originate in the need to make sense of the over- retical contributions are rarely significant enough or fre-
lapping and divergent patterns of data across implicit and quent enough to deserve the highest recognitions. Instead,
explicit measures of attitude, and this chapter describes advances that are regarded as the most important typically
both types of measures in depth. involve the development of a method or technology and
The next section on measurement carries substan- the new discoveries that these inventions permit.
tial detail because a cluster of new conceptual questions Greenwald (2004) offered such a view, noting the addi-
about the nature of attitudes has emerged through atten- tional difficulty that theoretical differences are rarely suc-
tion to methods. To signal its importance, a new entry in cessfully resolved, often resembling the argument about
this chapter concerns the origins of attitudes, highlighting whether the parrot is alive or dead in Monty Python’s
some of the research on the development of preferences sidesplitting skit. Suggesting that perhaps Lewin’s famous
in infants and young children. The remaining pages ana- dictum may be usefully reversed to read that there is “noth-
lyze self-attitudes (e.g., self-esteem), attitude dissociations ing so theoretical as a good method,” Greenwald’s asser-
and malleability, and ideological systems. Clear overlap tions are not meant to claim that theory is unimportant;
exists between some of these topics and other chapters in rather, the point is to convey that method development and
this handbook, and readers are referred to the appropri- empirical discoveries more often lead to, rather than fol-
ate experts in those areas. Between 1995 and 2010 alone, low, theory development. If this is the case, the importance
more than 13,000 articles on the topic of attitudes have of each individual contribution need not be judged solely
appeared. The remainder of the chapter presents a small on the grounds of advance in theory.
sample of them to demonstrate the progress that has been The following section is divided into four parts. First,
made on this fundamental orientation to evaluate. traditional methods that have relied on posing questions
that require introspectively rich answers using verbal self-
report are examined. Here, the chapter focuses on improve-
MEASURING ATTITUDES: CONCEPTUAL ments that have been made to methods that have existed for
ISSUES AND SPECIFIC TECHNIQUES several decades. Next, in a departure from previous hand-
book chapters, this one focuses on measures of implicit
Everybody loves a good theory. Theories represent the core attitude. For those whose interests have included questions
of creating new knowledge because theories provide expla- of method and technique, the last decade of the 20th cen-
nations. They help people understand why they might have tury has been rich in discovery and invention. Thus, the
attitudes at all, why preferences express themselves as they section on implicit attitude measures is longer than most
do, what evaluations are related to, and how attitudes dictate and is divided into three parts: measures based on response
actions. But theories that make people rub their bellies with latency, autonomic responses, and neural activity, including
satisfaction are not easy to come by, and in the last 50 years, earlier work using ERPs and more recent work focusing
only a handful of attitude theories or models have guided on fMRI as indicators of attitudes.
substantial programs of research and survived experimen-
tal wear and tear: Festinger ’s (1957) theory of cognitive Advances in Survey-type Measures of Attitudes
dissonance, Ajzen and Fishbein’s (1980) theory of rea-
soned action, and Fazio’s (1990) MODE model to explain Most commonly, psychologists have measured behavior
attitude-behavior consistency; Petty and Cacioppo’s (1981; via verbal self-reports of mental states, the exception being
1986) Elaboration Likelihood Model; and Chaiken’s (1980; those who could not, such as those working with babies
Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989) Heuristic-Systematic and nonhuman animals. The study of attitudes has been no
Model of attitude change. exception; verbal self-reports are of great interest because
Even within this top-notch collection, nobody will claim they provide an assessment of the knower ’s subjective
to have grand (or even grandish) unifying theories of attitudes. assessment of preferences, opinions, beliefs, or values. For

CH10.indd 354 10/5/09 7:19:28 PM


Measuring Attitudes: Conceptual Issues and Specific Techniques 360

much of what the psychologist seeks to understand, there is Bradburn, & Schwarz, 1996). The question of data analysis
little of greater value than what is elicited through simple is paramount in understanding the data obtained by all
questions such as: What do you like? How much do you measures, and detailed coverage of this topic is provided
like it? Do you like this better than that? by Judd and Kenny (this volume).
Early measures, including Thurstone’s (1928b) Equal- In a comprehensive chapter on attitude measures,
Appearing Intervals Method, Likert’s (1932) Method of Krosnick, Judd, and Wittenbrink (2005) detail the consider-
Summated Ratings, and Osgood et al.’s (1957) Semantic ations that must go into creating self-report scales (see also
Differential, all relied on participants circling, marking, Krosnick & Fabrigar, in press), with issues of reliability
checking off, and otherwise offering some behavioral indi- and validity of paramount importance. There are, indeed,
cator of their accessible mental states. These three mea- ways to improve the reliability and validity of self-report
sures provide the foundation for most self-report measures scales. Open-ended questions, though requiring a great deal
that are used even today. Their advantage is a relatively of care at the coding stage, are typically more reliable and
low rate of random error variance because of the large valid than close-ended questions. Researchers who desire
number of items that usually compose each scale or test. close-ended questions should choose a proper number of
However, because building a true Thurstone or Likert scale points. Scales with too few (e.g., 2–3) or too many (e.g.,
from scratch can be difficult and expensive, psychologists more than 11) points from which to choose have less reli-
have rendered some scales simpler and easier to use. ability than scales with approximately 4 to 10 points, and
Those who attempt to construct scales that measure traits, validity, which increases quickly with each additional point
such as “the need for evaluation” scale (Jarvis & Petty, in short measures, begins to flatten for long scales. A mod-
1996), need to follow the standard components of scale erate number of points make it easier to label each one, a
construction, paying particular attention to reliability and practice that improves interpretability, and scales of moder-
validity. However, experimental psychologists who study ate length also decrease error variance because of question
attitudes tend to be more interested in constructing inde- order.
pendent variables and presenting particular stimuli. Posing Likewise, creators of a self-report scale must pay
questions that measure attitudes has been largely a matter attention to the absolute versus relative manner in which
of keeping them simple and clear. If one is interested in the attitude questions are posed. Here one is reminded of
effects of misattributing one’s affective state to life overall William McGuire, who when asked, “How was your trip
rather than the beautiful or dreary weather on a particular to London?” would reply, “Compared to what?” Attitude
day, the question is posed as directly as possible: “How questions are often posed without sufficient comparison
happy do you feel about your life as a whole?” (Schwarz & context, and although both types are appropriate, relative
Clore, 1983, p. 519). It is remarkable how much valuable questions can reduce error variance by giving all respon-
information can be learned by posing a single, simple ques- dents a fixed comparison. In some cases, relative scales
tion if the idea underlying the questioning is important predict reported behavior, domain knowledge, and peer
and clever. reports of attitude and behavior better than absolute scales
The bulk of the attention to the question of how best (Olson, Goffin, & Haynes, 2007).
to measure deliberately reportable attitudes has come from Rather than providing participants with a numeric scale,
social scientists, including sociologists, political scien- some measures may be more suited to including a set of
tists, and psychologists, all of whom are interested in the response options. For example, rather than asking partici-
designs of surveys. Agreement exists that a paradigm shift pants to rate how likely they are to vote for each candidate
has occurred in survey measurement, with the emphasis in an election, researchers may provide the names of all
having shifted from the statistical models of sampling errors the candidates and ask participants to select the one for
(with a focus on the effects of survey errors on estimates) to whom they will most likely cast their ballot. When many
a psychological concern with the interpretation of ques- responses are present, however, participants may be more
tions, the reasons for nonresponses, and the effects of con- likely to select from the first few options presented to
text on responses (with a focus on the causes of errors on them, either because they are uninterested in the question
surveys; see Tourangeau, 2003). The influence of models of or because the various options become difficult to remem-
cognition, including social cognition, is evident in the ques- ber. Though this difficulty can be attenuated through
tions that have been posed about the self-report data. These the use of counterbalancing, doing so introduces error
models shifted the focus to understanding the mind of the variance that may not be completely random. It is helpful
respondent and the natural correlates of self-report data, to take steps to prevent order effects from turning into a
including age, culture, and context effects of every form problem in the first place, such as shortening the length
(see Schwarz, 1996; Schwarz & Sudman, 1996; Sudman, of the questionnaire to increase participant motivation and

CH10.indd 355 10/5/09 7:19:30 PM


361 Attitudes

providing response options that are easy to remember understanding of the nature of attitudes and suggested
and understand (Krosnick et al., 2005). improvements in survey measures of attitudes.
Although most social psychologists rarely conduct
research using random probability samples (because their Measures of Automatic Attitudes
interest is in a psychological process rather than where
the country stands on a particular issue), obvious con- Psychologists have sought and used indirect attitude mea-
nections exist between those who study the processes of sures for decades. Unobtrusive measures have always
attitude formation and change, and those whose concern been of interest in social psychology because of engage-
is measuring respondents’ positions on the big issues of ment with topics that naturally breed a concern to appear
the day. Studying the content of a particular attitude and its socially desirable (Webb, Campbell, Schwartz, & Sechrest,
changing nature, such as why changes in death penalty atti- 1966). What is distinct about the modern era of interest in
tudes are rapid at particular moments in history, often pro- unobtrusive measures is a simpler worry about the lack of
vides useful information not only about such attitudes per access to the contents of the mind. This concern is based
se but about the process of attitude change itself. However, on discoveries about the mind’s two modes of operation:
concerns involving the administration and interpretation the conscious, deliberate, explicit, aware, intended, con-
of data from large samples selected to randomly reflect trolled aspects, as well as the less conscious, mindless,
the population (e.g., of all Americans) are also a part of the implicit, unaware, unintended, and automatic aspects. This
study of attitudes. chapter tends to use the terms “deliberate” or “automatic”
Interviews conducted in person, over the phone, or online to refer to attitudes appearing to be under the respondent’s
are a part of the data-gathering machine, and researchers control to a greater or lesser extent. On the other hand, terms
have given the limits and flexibilities of these a great deal such as “conscious” or “nonconscious” and “explicit” or
of thought. Recent attempts have focused almost exclu- “implicit” refer to attitudes that are more or less within the
sively on the manner in which the respondent understands respondent’s perimeter of conscious awareness.
the question—its syntax, semantics, and pragmatics— Speaking of the mind’s two modes of operation pro-
as well as cognitive biases in encoding and memory (see vides a way of speaking about data that points to disso-
Tourangeau, Rips, & Rasinski, 2000). For example, a ciations in the attitudes that emerge based on variations in
particular difficulty with phone surveys involves their methods used to measure attitudes; it is not meant to imply,
auditory nature and the inability of participants to reread except where directly stated, that the underlying represen-
options. Because of other concerns, such as the lack of tations are independent. Questions of whether the implicit
response in random-digit-dialing sampling, researchers and explicit versions of attitude toward an attitude object
often deem in-person administration to be superior when are two representations or a single one are difficult to test
resources permit. To obtain large sample sizes, researchers (see Conrey, Sherman, Gawronski, Hugenberg, & Groom,
may also conduct studies via the Internet, where it is possi- 2005; Greenwald & Nosek, 2008), although in some cases, as
ble to obtain responses from several thousand participants described later, neural data may give some indication.
simultaneously. Internet sampling provides the added ben- In many circumstances, the most appropriate measure
efit of demographic diversity in all regards except socio- of attitude or preference is obtained by asking participants
economic; participants from any state and almost any for a verbal self-report. Preferences do not have a right or
country may complete an online measure (Gosling, Vazire, wrong answer in many situations—one can express a pref-
Srivastava, & John, 2004; Kraut et al., 2004; Reis & erence pro or con for anchovies or liver, for bell-bottoms
Gosling, this volume). Until Internet fatigue in responding or dreadlocks, for socialism or libertarianism. In each such
to surveys sets in, as it did with the telephone, this technol- case, because the person’s expressed preference and some-
ogy is a useful way to generate both random and nonrandom times the reasons for that preference are of interest, a per-
samples. fectly good avenue is to ask the traditional direct questions
None of the issues raised here is rocket science; yet (e.g., Why do you like liver?).
without attention to these empirically derived solutions The difficulty with relying solely on self-report mea-
and improvements to survey measurement, the ability to sures stems from two sources. First and most obviously,
obtain an accurate read of preferences can be poor. Among not all attitudes are equally socially desirable. One would not
the most interesting research on attitudes, with vast impli- be caught dead confessing a love of liver in a junior high
cations for survey research, is work on the cognitive cafeteria, much less speaking one’s mind about condom
processes that inform the posing of questions, the effects use. In ways that psychologists do not understand fully,
of context, and the limits of respondents (see Sirken et al., the attitudes people express are ones they are often “try-
1999). Such work has simultaneously provided a deeper ing out” or representing in ever-so-slightly different ways

CH10.indd 356 10/5/09 7:19:31 PM


Measuring Attitudes: Conceptual Issues and Specific Techniques 362

because of the situations in which they find themselves. ways in which the mind works. Verbal self-report measures
Such attitudes may or may not predict later expressions or are a fine way to tap the conscious aspects of the mind’s
behaviors. functioning, but if, indeed, a large amount of cognitive
Because social psychologists have always gravitated and affective processing happens without reliance on
toward studying the contents of consciousness that mat- some facet of consciousness (as Bargh & Chartrand, 1999
ter to people, this question of reaching the core of what a have indicated), how should such content be accessed and
person “really thinks” has been a perennial worry. In the understood? These questions have been asked about all
course of ordinary experimentation, researchers use sev- aspects of social cognition, and this chapter pays close
eral procedures to reduce the effects of social desirability: attention to the discussion as it has affected the measure-
They create environments in which all attitudes are equally ment of attitudes.
easily expressed, provide assurances and evidence that Researchers have often examined hidden attitudes
the reports are anonymous, measure the attitude unobtru- while studying prejudice and intergroup relations. Social
sively, and mask the true purpose of the questioning (see scientists discovered dramatic transformations of atti-
Campbell, Kruskal, & Wallace, 1966; Crosby, Bromley, & tudes toward members of social groups, especially those
Saxe, 1980; Devine, 1989; Dovidio & Fazio, 1992; Fazio, that are stigmatized, over time. For example, the attitudes
Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995; Gaertner & Bickman, and stereotypes expressed about Black Americans, Asian
1971; Jacoby, Toth, Lindsay, & Debner, 1992; Milgram, Americans, and Jews are vastly different today than they
Mann, & Harter, 1965; Nuttin, 1985; Schwarz & Clore, were even a few decades ago (Katz & Braly, 1933; Devine &
1983; Webb et al., 1966; Word, Zanna, & Cooper, 1974). Elliot, 1995; Madon et al., 2001). Behavioral shifts reflect
Even more interesting is the second threat to the assump- these attitude changes, but not to nearly the same extent as
tion of introspective ability. The issue was brought home one might predict.
most strikingly by Nisbett and Wilson (1977), who showed Group differences in access to housing, education, health
that human beings may not know and, therefore, may be care, jobs, and political power have raised questions about
unable to report the reasons for their behaviors. Through why such discrimination continues when, in fact, attitudes
argument and evidence, Nisbett and Wilson offered a major show much weaker evidence of animus. Might measures of
blow to standard assumptions about introspective access. attitude be limited to particular aspects of evaluation that
People do not know, these psychologists showed, why they are visible, leaving aside important determinants of behav-
are better able to fall asleep some nights, why they fear ior that are untapped (Crosby et al., 1980; Fazio & Dunton,
particular objects, why they are willing to accept greater 1997; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Greenwald & Banaji,
amounts of physical pain in the form of electric shocks, 1995; Wilson & Brekke, 1994)?
or why they prefer one item or sound to another. Furthermore, Such concerns are not new, and alternatives to standard
once people change their mind about issues such as busing, measures of social attitudes have been of interest for many
they misremember their previous attitude as matching their decades. For example, in the “bogus pipeline” technique
current one. Even people with the best of intentions may (Jones & Sigall, 1971), participants are led to believe that
be unable to provide accurate information about their own the researcher has a foolproof way of detecting the truth-
attitudes because they simply do not know. fulness of their answers by tapping into physiological
Looking at the same question from another angle, processes. This is typically accomplished by attaching
one can ask why psychologists should expect that people physiological sensors to participants and convincing them
ought to know their preferences in many or most situa- that these sensors function as lie detectors. If attitudes are
tions. Scientists do not make such assumptions about other solicited under such circumstances, it is assumed that the
aspects of human beings. They do not think that a good subject will not lie because of a belief that the experimenter
way to know about somebody’s blood pressure is to ask would ferret out the right answer anyway. A meta-analytic
them what it is; most people could not provide an honest review indicates that bogus pipeline procedures elicit more
and accurate answer even if they wished to. honest responses than do control conditions, particularly
Nisbett and Wilson’s statement (1977) was well ahead of when participants are asked to guess the physiological out-
its time, offered as it was before any of the attitude measures put of the “lie detector test” (Roese & Jamieson, 1993).
described here were invented. By now a significant amount That is, participants who were connected to an apparatus
of research and theorizing has focused on exactly the ques- that they believed was capable of determining their true
tions they posed: What can people know about their attitudes attitudes responded in a less socially desirable way than
and, more importantly, what can they not know? The interest participants who were not connected to such an appara-
in this epistemological question also originated because tus. Furthermore, participants who were presented with a
of data about the split between conscious and unconscious Likert-type scale asking them to report what they believed

CH10.indd 357 10/5/09 7:19:31 PM


363 Attitudes

the apparatus had recorded were even less likely to respond A growing repertoire of measures expanded the very
in a socially desirable manner. notion of what memory is. At least on the surface, the newly
However, self-presentation concerns are not the only discovered types of memory bore faint resemblance to the
reason to be interested in implicit measures (Nosek, 2007). familiar meaning of this construct, but these new measures
As noted earlier, the thornier problem of attitude measure- were revealing intriguing dissociations between different
ment remains the issue of measuring what is unknown to measures of memory. What the measures of conscious rec-
the respondent. The remainder of the tests described in this ollection were detecting seemed dull when probed by indi-
section, and the work that has commanded the greatest rect measures and vice versa. From such discoveries, the
attention by far, falls into the category of measures that same construct of memory came to be expanded to include
assume that respondents do not and cannot, by and large, varyingly named extensions such as unconscious memory,
have access to particular preferences they hold and act on. implicit memory, automatic memory, indirect memory, and
The assumption is that this state of affairs in the realm of procedural memory.
attitudes is similar to what is true of other mental faculties
Evaluative Priming
such as perception, attention, memory, and reasoning, all
of which are subject to veiled access. Starting in the 1980s, Gaertner and his collaborators (e.g.,
Dovidio, Evans, & Tyler, 1986; Gaertner & McLaughlin,
Measures of Implicit Memory as a Model 1983), Fazio and his colleagues (see Fazio et al., 1986),
for Measures of Implicit Attitudes and Chaiken and Bargh (1993; see Bargh et al., 1992;
Students of memory know that their field changed in sig- Duckworth, Bargh, Garcia, & Chaiken, 2002) developed and
nificant ways starting in the late 1970s. Evidence converged used a measure of evaluative (attitude) priming. Evaluative
from patients with striking memory disorders, as well as from priming tasks are based on the assumption that, if an object
the more mundane behavior of ordinary college students, (e.g., candy) elicits a positive evaluation, it should facilitate
showing the existence of entirely new forms of memory responses to other positive things such as “good” and “pleas-
never previously seen. If there was any doubt that the mind ant” because the two sets of stimuli are evaluatively con-
is only as knowable as the techniques or technologies avail- gruent. Likewise, if an object (e.g., Brussels sprouts) elicits
able to know it, these studies provided the evidence. For 100 a negative evaluation, it should facilitate responding to
years, since Ebbinghaus, the gramophone needle of mea- other negative items such as “bad” or “unpleasant” because
surement had been stuck in a single groove of repeatedly of the shared negativity. Importantly, priming effects should
asking one form of question: to recollect what had happened hold only in relevant domains. That is, evaluative priming
before. But beginning in the 1970s, even though they were should be seen in situations where participants are asked to
interested in the same thing (memory for an event), scien- decide whether something is “good” or “bad,” but facilita-
tists did not just ask for recollections of times past. Instead, tion should not be seen in other kinds of judgments (e.g.,
they asked what seemed like odd questions to understand “object” or “person”; see Wittenbrink, 2007).
memory: “Do you see this to be a word or nonword?” (lexi- Response latencies, that is, the time taken to respond to
cal decision task); “What, if anything, did you see flash by?” a word measured in milliseconds, provides an indirect mea-
(perceptual identification); “Can you complete this frag- sure of attitude strength. Participants see primes (attitude
ment to make a meaningful word?” (word-fragment com- objects such as candy or Brussels sprouts) for a short dura-
pletion); and even, “How much do you like this?” (Jacoby, tion (e.g., 200 ms). Primes are followed by targets (evalu-
1993; Richardson-Klavehn & Bjork, 1988; Roediger, 1990; ative words such as “good” or “bad”), and the participants
Roediger & McDermott, 1993; Schacter, 1987). then have the job of determining whether the target word
Following on the heels of substantial research using lex- describes the prime. If participants hold a strong associa-
ical decisions to understand semantic memory (Meyer & tion between the prime and the target word, they ought to
Schvaneveldt, 1971; Neely, 1977; Posner & Snyder, 1975), respond faster that the word describes the prime than if they
these new tasks transformed how episodic memory, or hold only a weak association between the two.
memory for events, was studied. If subjects had seen the Moreover, if evaluation is a strong driver of judgment,
information before, whether they consciously remembered the priming result should be obtained even when the judg-
seeing it or not, they were faster to say it was a word, more ment is itself nonevaluative, such as when the task the
likely to see it even though it whizzed by at high speed, subject performs does not involve a good-bad decision but
and more likely to use the word to fill in the blanks. These is, for example, a simple pronunciation task (see Bargh
tests served as indices of what was saved in memory and et al., 1996; Giner-Sorolla et al., 1999). Results obtained
whether the person was able to recollect it when asked, through priming techniques may be influenced by factors
“What do you remember?” such as attitude strength or accessibility, the representation

CH10.indd 358 10/5/09 7:19:33 PM


Measuring Attitudes: Conceptual Issues and Specific Techniques 364

of the prime (whether it is a picture or a word), and task bias in nonverbal behavior suggestive of racial discomfort
instructions (Wittenbrink, 2007). among a sample of Whites interacting with Blacks (Fazio
Psychologists have created several variations on et al., 1995). Furthermore, racial cues can prime an associ-
this basic priming paradigm. In some work (e.g., Giner- ation with violent objects. For example, White participants
Sorolla et al., 1999, Study 2), participants are exposed tended to misidentify harmless tools carried by Blacks as
to a prime and are then asked to pronounce a word that guns, though they made this mistake significantly less often
appears immediately afterward. In studies such as this, when examining White targets (Payne, 2001). Even people
participants are not required to make an explicit evalua- with the best of intentions are prone to errors such as this,
tive judgment. The crucial dependent variable is the length perhaps because stereotypes (e.g., Blacks are violent) are
of time participants take to pronounce the words, which is automatically activated in the presence of a relevant group
shorter when the word is evaluatively congruent with the member, regardless of one’s level of prejudice or stereo-
prime. In other variations, primes are presented in such a type endorsement (Devine, 1989). Evaluative priming is
way that participants are unaware of what they have seen not confined to the racial realm but also predicts social dis-
(either because the prime appeared for too short a time or tance from other stigmatized groups (e.g., obese people;
because it was immediately covered by another object on Bessenoff & Sherman, 2000), as well as nonintergroup
the screen). Nevertheless, even in these cases, the primes experiences such as anxiety during an interview (Spalding &
have been found to influence evaluative judgments (e.g., Hardin, 1999). As Wittenbrink (2007) points out, priming
Croizet, 1998; Greenwald, Klinger, & Liu, 1989; Otten & techniques generally predict such behaviors better than do
Wentura, 1999; Weinberger & Westen, 2008). self-report measures.
Consider a typical priming experiment: participants are Not all priming methods rely on presentations that are too
told that they will be taking part in a study on “word recog- quick for conscious processing. In the Affect Misattribution
nition and meaning,” and will be asked to perform several Procedure, participants view photographs of smiling or
word judgment tasks of increasing complexity (Fazio et al., scowling faces and then rate Chinese pictographs as either
1986). Participants are seated in front of a computer, and “more pleasant than average” or “less pleasant than aver-
their first task is to decide whether particular words are age.” Participants who saw scowling faces tended to
“good” or “bad” using unique keys on a keyboard to rate the pictographs less favorably even when they were
answer as quickly and accurately as possible. After com- specifically instructed that the photographs they had pre-
pleting this task, they are excused for a short break while viously seen might bias their responses (Payne, Cheng,
the experimenter selects 16 of the 70 words to which they Govorun, & Stewart, 2005). These findings demonstrate
have responded. These special 16 are chosen on the basis of that even conscious priming, which the participant has
each participant’s reaction time: The four words that they been told is a source of bias, can have a strong influence on
have most quickly judged to be good or bad are chosen attitudes, even attitudes toward unrelated objects.
as instances of strong primes. Likewise, the four that they
Implicit Association Test
were slowest to label good or bad serve as weak primes.
Participants then reenter the room and are given a sec- The IAT, like evaluative priming, estimates the strength
ond task. They will again see words to be judged as good of association between concepts (the attitude objects) and
or bad, but this time, each adjective will be preceded by attributes (Greenwald et al., 1998). When the attribute
a “memory word” (either 1 of the 16 surreptitiously cho- involved is the good-bad or pleasant-unpleasant dimen-
sen words or a nonsense string such as BBB). Participants sion, the resulting outcome is deemed to be a measure of
must pronounce the word or string aloud while making implicit attitude. When the attribute involved represents a
their judgment. The results are clear: Participants are faster stereotype (e.g., violent/nonviolent), the resulting outcome
to judge words as “good” or “bad” in the second task if is considered a measure of belief (see Banaji, 2001).
they are busy pronouncing a word of the same valence, Used prominently in the study of attitudes toward social
particularly when the “memory word” is a strong rather groups, an example of the IAT as a measure of attitudes
than a weak prime. A participant who has quickly judged toward people of different ages would unfold in the fol-
the word “cake” to be positive in the first task, for example, lowing manner (for details on constructing an IAT, see
is significantly faster to rate the word “delightful” as good Lane, Banaji, Nosek, & Greenwald, 2007): Participants
when pronouncing the word “cake.” would first classify pictures of faces by pressing one key
The magnitude of such priming effects seem to influence if the face is that of a young person and a different key if
a plethora of behaviors. For example, an implicit prefer- the face is that of an elderly person. Next, participants
ence for White over Black, as indicated by performance on would respond to various words by pressing one key if
tasks such as those described earlier, predicts the degree of the word is positive (e.g., joy, love) and a different key if the

CH10.indd 359 10/5/09 7:19:34 PM


365 Attitudes

word is negative (e.g., terrible, agony). In the following (Teachman, Marker, & Smith-Janik, 2008), in psychopaths
block of trials, participants would see either a word or a toward violence (Gray, MacCulloch, Smith, Morris, &
picture of a face. They would be asked to press one key if Snowden, 2003), and in adolescents toward self-harm
they see either a positive word or a young face and a differ- (Nock & Banaji, 2007). Researchers have also used the IAT
ent key if they see either a negative word or an elderly face. to study happiness (Walker & Schimmack, 2008), alcohol
In the last block of the IAT the pairing would be reversed; consumption (Ostafin & Palfai, 2006), smoking (Robinson,
now participants would be asked to press one key if they see Meier, Zetocha, & McCaul, 2005), and achievement
either a positive word or an elderly face and a different key (Brunstein & Schmitt, 2004), among other topics.
if they see either a negative word or a young face. The order Questions of validation have been addressed most
of blocks is counterbalanced across participants such that reassuringly though through studies of the relationship
some are first asked to pair good⫹young/bad⫹old, whereas between IAT scores and behaviors that satisfy the desire
others are first asked to pair good⫹old/bad⫹young. The for ecological validity. The IAT has been shown to predict
IAT score is obtained by subtracting mean reaction times a vaiety of such outcomes including the voting behaivor of
to the unexpected or counterstereotypical pairing (in this undecided voters (Arcuri, Castelli, Galdi, Zogmaister, &
case, good⫹old/young⫹bad) from mean reaction times to Amadori, 2008); political attitudes toward enlarging a U.S.
the opposite pairing. In this case, positive scores reflect a military base (Galdi, Arcuri, & Gawronski, 2008); actual
tendency to respond faster when young is paired with good suicide attempts (Nock, Park, Finn, Deliberto, Dour, &
and old is paired with bad. Greenwald, Nosek, and Banaji Banaji, in press); response to treatment for panic disor-
(2003) have offered a new scoring algorithm based on data der (Teachman, Marker, Smith-Janik, in press); adoles-
of thousands of participants. cents’ development of alcohol and drug addiction (Thrush,
To someone whose conscious attitude toward the Wiers, Ames, Grenard, Sussman, & Stacy; 20007; Wiers,
elderly is negative, the typical age attitude result on the Houben, & de Kraker, 2007); hiring of ethnic minorities
IAT should come as no surprise. Most people who have in an Arab vs. Swedish applicant pool (Rooth, 2009);
taken the test show some level of elderly/bad association; intent to change jobs (von Hippel, Brener, & von Hippel,
however, for the vast majority who do not sense in them- 2008); and nation-level gender differences in 8th grade
selves any negative attitude toward the elderly—especially science and math achievement (Nosek, et al, 2009).
those who are elderly themselves—the IAT result can be Among the questions of interest has been the relation-
surprising and not necessarily acceptable. In an attempt ship between measures of implicit and explicit attitudes.
to raise awareness about dissociations between explicit These two families of measures can be quite dissociated
and implicit attitudes and beliefs (discussed further in the from each other (the signature result from this area of
Attitude Dissociations section later in this chapter), sev- research), but the domain in which they are implemented
eral tests that often divulge such dissociations are avail- predicts the degree of association that will be obtained. In
able online at: http://implicit.harvard.edu. To date, more some domains, such as voting behavior close to an elec-
than 10 million tests have been administered. tion, implicit and explicit evaluations are substantially
Although it has been used as a measure of implicit correlated, but in many domains, such as intergroup atti-
attitudes, the IAT can easily be adapted to provide a mea- tudes, a two-factor solution offers the best fit (e.g., ethno-
sure of implicit beliefs. Such a test may involve comparison centrism; Cunningham, Nezlek, & Banaji, 2004). Nosek’s
of performance in male-female concepts with strong- extensive research on the topic of implicit-explicit conver-
weak attributes or Black-White concepts with scholar- gence and divergence (see Nosek, 2005, 2007; Nosek &
athlete attributes. Likewise, the IAT can be turned into a Smyth, 2007) shows that this relationship varies as a func-
measure of implicit self-attitude by measuring the asso- tion of self-presentation demands, evaluative strength of
ciation of self with the good-bad dimension (Greenwald the attitude, dimensionality (i.e., whether the measured
et al., 2002; see the section entitled, “The Attitude Toward attitude consists of two distinctive poles), and evaluative
Oneself“). distinctiveness (i.e., how much one perceives the attitude
In the years after its development, the IAT was used to to differ from the average attitude of the group).
study intergroup attitudes more than other types of evalu- The IAT is easily adaptable, and researchers have created
ations; however, it is increasingly visible in other research many variations. To overcome the limitation of relying on
programs, including those concerning consumer behavior two distinct categories (e.g., Black and White), the Go/No-
(Janiszewski, 1988; Maison, Greenwald, & Bruin, 2004; go Association Task (Nosek & Banaji, 2001) presents partic-
Shapiro, 1999) and attitudes of interest to clinical psycholo- ipants with one target item (e.g., Black faces) and numerous
gists. For example, the test has been used to study atti- distracter items (e.g., Latino, Asian, and White faces) to
tudes in clinical samples toward phobia-producing stimuli which they must inhibit a response. Other variants present

CH10.indd 360 10/5/09 7:19:34 PM


Measuring Attitudes: Conceptual Issues and Specific Techniques 366

participants with only one category (e.g., Black faces; based on the expectation that a participant will respond
Karpinski & Steinman, 2006) or ask subjects to categorize with the color name faster when they evaluate a particular
according to both category and attribute at the same time. In stimulus as positive (de Houwer, 2003).
the latter case, participants may see a picture of a face and Another test, the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (de
a word presented together in the middle of the screen Houwer, 2003), uses differently colored words to determine
and be asked to press one of four keys: one key if they see a participants’ evaluations of stimulus items. Instead of pairing
picture of a Black face paired with a positive word, another attributes with categories, this task utilizes white and colored
key if they see a picture of a White face paired with a nega- words. Participants are asked to categorize the white words
tive word, and so on (Bar-Anan, Nosek, & Vianello, 2009). based on their valence and the colored words based on their
Similarly, another variant presents stimulus items in this same color, the expectation being that participants will respond
way but includes neutral categories such as “furniture,” as more quickly when the white and colored words assigned to
well as more traditional categories (e.g., faces and positively the same key share valence. For example, a participant may
or negatively valenced words; Blanton, Jaccard, Gonzalez, & be asked to respond with one key whenever a positive white
Christie, 2006). In other variations, the number of blocks word or any blue word appears, and to respond with another
and trials has been reduced (Sriram & Greenwald, 2009; key when a negative white word or any green word appears.
Teige-Mocigemba, Klauer, & Rothermund, 2008) to further In this case, participants should be faster to categorize the
shorten the duration of the IAT and make it appropriate for word “joy” written in blue and the word “vomit” written in
inclusion in large-scale data-gathering exercises involving green, because “joy” is paired with positive white words and
multiple sites. Another variant of the IAT asks participants “vomit” is paired with negative white words.
to respond by moving a joystick, thus indexing unconscious Finally, another task based on response times engages
preference through automatic behaviors (Schnabel, Banse, & the participant physically. Using Evaluative Movement
Asendorpf, 2006). Finally, the IAT has been adapted for use Assessment (Brendl, Markman, & Messner, 2005),
with children as young as 6 (Baron & Banaji, 2006). researchers ask participants to categorize stimuli by mov-
ing a joystick toward or away from their own body. For
Other Response Latency Measures instance, participants may be required to pull a joystick
of Implicit Attitudes toward themselves if they see positive words and away
In addition to evaluative priming and the IAT, which are the from themselves if they see negative words. This measure
two most widely used measures of unconscious attitudes, allows researchers to measure attitudes toward several dif-
researchers have developed numerous other measures. The ferent objects using one scale that is centered on a neutral
first of these, developed in the late 1970s, is quite similar to point. That is, because the crucial dependent measure is a
a derivation of the IAT. Known as the Eriksen Flanker Task difference in reaction times (how long the participant took
(Eriksen & Schultz, 1979), this measure asks participants to push or pull the joystick in response to the same word),
to respond to a stimulus surrounded by distracter items. the true zero point reflects no difference in reaction time.
Congruent sets consist of items (e.g., arrows) pointing in
Criticisms of Response Latency Measures
the same direction, whereas incongruent sets contain dis-
tracter items pointing in different directions. Participants The measures described earlier have not been immune
are expected to respond faster to congruent rather than from criticism, and critics have posed three main clusters of
incongruent sets, indicating the orientations that they find challenges: psychometric properties, procedural features,
most related. and predictive validity. It has been argued that response
A second task adapts the original Stroop task to the latency measures do not necessarily correlate highly with
study of attitudes. Known as the Emotional Stroop Task each other (Bosson, Swann, & Pennebaker, 2000) and that
(e.g., Mogg, Mathews, & Weinman, 1989; Pratto & John, response latency measures lack a nonarbitrary zero point
1991), this measure requires participants to pronounce (Blanton & Jaccard, 2006). Yet others have questioned the
the color in which a word is written rather than saying the proper interpretation of the scores from such tests, draw-
word itself. Positive and negative words are presented, ing a distinction between the individual’s own attitude ver-
and the response latency to name the color in which the sus cultural learning (Karpinski & Hilton, 2001; Olson &
word is written is used as an index of attention resources. Fazio, 2004), and posed alternative explanations in terms
Participants are slower to respond with the color name of greater familiarity or greater salience as the determining
when undesirable traits are presented, indicating that such reason for the obtained outcome rather than attitude per
words require more attention than desirable trait labels se (Brendl, Markman, & Messner, 2001; Rothermund &
(Pratto & John, 1991). This task could, therefore, be used Wentura, 2004). These debates have enhanced an under-
to determine which of two stimuli a participant favors, standing of the IAT and other response latency measures

CH10.indd 361 10/5/09 7:19:35 PM


367 Attitudes

that share its properties. (Some definitive responses, as member “sometimes lets me borrow money from him,”
well as ongoing debates, can be found in Banaji, 2001; again implying that similar others are stably nice, whereas
Banaji, Nosek, & Greenwald, 2004; Dasgupta, McGhee, dissimilar others may only do nice things on occasion
Greenwald, & Banaji, 2000; Greenwald, Nosek, Banaji, & (Walton & Banaji, 2004).
Klauer, 2005; Greenwald, Nosek, & Sriram, 2006; Although this section provides an extensive list of implicit
Greenwald, Rudman, Nosek, & Zayas, 2006; Lane et al., behavioral measures of attitude currently in use, the list is
2007; Nosek & Hansen, 2008; Ottaway, Hayden, & Oakes, not complete. Other such measures include the name-letter
2001; and Rudman, Greenwald, Mellott, & Schwartz, effect (e.g., Koole, Dijksterhuis, & van Knippenberg, 2001;
1999.) Perhaps not surprisingly, these measures being so Nuttin, 1985), the stimulus response compatibility task
counter to the standard method of measuring attitudes, (Mogg, Bradley, Field, & de Houwer, 2003), the implicit
some have posed questions about the predictive validity of association procedure (Schnabel, Banse, & Asendorpf,
response latency measures. What can scientists learn about 2006), the single association test (Blanton et al., 2006), the
meaningful human behavior by knowing that individu- approach-avoid task (Rinck & Becker, 2007), the implicit
als respond to some words or pictures faster than others? relational assessment procedure (e.g., Barnes-Holmes,
Given the hundreds of studies that are now available using Hayden, Barnes-Holmes, & Stewart, 2008), and the word
such methods, a great deal is now known about the various association task (Stacy, Leigh, & Weingardt, 1997). All
forms of validity of the measures. Response latency mea- together, the development of such measures has been an
sures correlate with neural activation patterns, fall into line extensive preoccupation of scientists interested in attitudes
with expected “known-groups” differences in attitude, and and other aspects of social cognition.
predict nonverbal responses of friendliness, the choice of
a partner, performance ratings, and the treatment of others Measures of the Autonomic Nervous System
(Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009; Jost
et al., in press). The rapid blink of an eye, the rush of sweat to the palms,
and the hastening of a heartbeat are, according to some psy-
Other Indirect Measures of Attitudes chologists, useful indicators of a person’s attitude toward
Another commonly-used group of indirect attitude mea- the thing that is responsible for such bodily responses.
sures are linguistic in nature. Language reveals much about Blascovich and Mendes (this volume) thoroughly review
thoughts and emotions, and some of these revelations are common physiological measures and describe several ways
unconscious and unintended. In a thorough review of the rev- in which researchers have used these techniques to study
elations language provides about preferences, von Hippel, attitudes. This section elaborates on the ways in which sci-
Sekaquaptewa, and Vargas (2008) suggest that people’s entists have applied biological psychology to the study of
words may reveal more about themselves than they might preferences.
know. For example, the number of plural pronouns (we, us) In spite of the obvious advantage of stealth, physiologi-
as compared with the number of singular pronouns (I, he, cal measures did not fare well enough to become broadly
she) participants used when writing about relationships has used as a means of measuring attitudes for a variety of rea-
proved to be a strong predictor of their friendship quality sons, including the inability to distinguish positive from
(Agnew, van Lange, Rusbult, & Langston, 1998), and prim- negative responses. For many decades they played a more
ing people with plural pronouns causes them to feel greater minor role in understanding attitudes, and such techniques
interdependence (Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Gardner, have even been viewed as the poor cousins of more expen-
Gabriel, & Lee, 1999). Likewise, people may reveal their atti- sive measures of brain activity. However, because physi-
tudes through their choice of adjectives, relying on broader ological measures provide an intriguing glimpse into the
adjectives to describe attitude-consistent behaviors. For structure and functions of attitudes, this section describes
example, if a favored in-group member behaves positively, some of the more commonly used physiological techniques
people may say that the action is nice, which is perceived in the hopes of enhancing understanding of this important
to be a stable quality. However, if a disliked group member methodology.
performed the same action, individuals might label it “gen- Cacioppo and colleagues (Cacioppo, 1982; Cacioppo &
erous” (if the person has just offered them money) or “help- Sandman, 1978) used such measures quite early and exten-
ful” (if they have just opened a door for someone), implying sively to understand the nature of attitudes (Cacioppo, Petty,
that the outgroup member is not “nice” in general but per- Losch, & Kim, 1986; Cacioppo & Sandman, 1981; Ito &
forms only specific positive behaviors (Hamilton, Gibbons, Cacioppo, 2007), and Blascovich and Mendes have added
Stroessner, & Sherman, 1992). People may also refer to substantially to the continued application of these methods
the ingroup member as “nice” and say that the outgroup in many aspects of social cognition, including attitudes (see

CH10.indd 362 10/5/09 7:19:35 PM


Measuring Attitudes: Conceptual Issues and Specific Techniques 368

Blascovich & Mendes, this volume). Like other measures activity created in response to muscle contraction, with
of implicit attitudes, physiological recordings allow stronger contraction force resulting in higher measure-
researchers to gain access to attitudes that participants may ments (Hess, 2008).
be unwilling or unable to report. Unlike other techniques, From EMG, much has been learned about the uncon-
physiological measures also allow for continuous record- scious nature of attitudes. Participants in one study, for
ings and, therefore, may provide an index of attitude shifts example, showed more cheek area activity (associated with
over a brief time span (Cunningham, Packer, Kesek, & van smiling) when exposed to a smiling face, even when they
Bavel, 2008). did not consciously process the stimulus. Likewise, partici-
One of the earliest measures of physiological respond- pants showed greater brow activity in response to an angry
ing is electrodermal activity (EDA, also known as skin face as compared with a neutral-face baseline (Dimberg,
conductance response [SCR] or galvanic skin response). Thunberg, & Elmehed, 2000). Such facial expressions
EDA measures the amount of sweat produced by the can indicate positive (in the case of smiling) or negative
eccrine glands, which are found throughout the body but (in the case of frowning) attitudes. People continue to be
are heavily concentrated on the hands and feet (for more influenced by faces seen long enough to be processed con-
details on this method, see Mendes, 2008). Among the sciously; when viewing positively evaluated targets, partic-
first experiments to use EDA as an attitude measure dem- ipants engaged in more facial mimicry than when viewing
onstrated that participants showed greater EDA increases negatively evaluated targets. In fact, negative targets can
when interacting with a Black rather than a White experi- elicit opposing facial expressions (e.g., zygomaticus major
menter (Rankin & Campbell, 1955). Later researchers have activity, associated with smiling, in response to a sad
pointed out that physiological responses can be nonspecific expression; Likowski, Muhlberger, Seibt, Pauli, & Weyers,
and may indicate general levels of arousal; in Rankin and 2008). The relationship here is reciprocal; not only do peo-
Campbell’s (1955) study, for instance, it is not possible to ple mimic those they like, but they can facilitate liking by
determine whether increases in EDA signaled a positive copying others. In one study, participants who interacted
or negative evaluation (Krosnick et al., 2005; Mendes, with a mimicking confederate later reported more liking
2008). However, physiological changes can provide infor- of their interaction partner than did those who interacted
mation about particular attitudes in constrained settings. with a nonmimicking person (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999,
For instance, if the experimental situation is limited to fear, Study 2).
EDA activity may be used as an index of fear responding; Although EMG is a powerful measure because of its
in other situations, the same marker may simply indicate capacity to index microscopic activity that cannot be
greater arousal or attention to particular stimulus items. detected by the naked eye, other researchers have focused
Since the development of EDA measures, psycholo- on changes that are even more difficult to see, diving
gists have continued to adopt an increasing number of beneath the skin to explore the autonomic nervous system.
techniques used by physiologists. For instance, partici- In one study, researchers measured ventricular contractility
pants evaluated ideographs more favorably when they (the time from the beginning of the left ventricular contrac-
were presented during arm flexion than during arm exten- tion to the opening of the aortic valve of the heart), cardiac
sion (Cacioppo, Priester, & Berntson, 1993). It has also output (the amount of blood being pumped by the heart),
been shown that physical approach behaviors (e.g., pull- and total peripheral resistance (the overall amount of vaso-
ing a lever toward oneself) improved interracial attitudes constriction or vasodilation occurring in regions outside
compared with avoidance behaviors (e.g., pushing a lever the brain and heart) during interactions with expectancy-
away; Kawakami, Phills, Steele, & Dovidio, 2007). violating partners (e.g., an Asian confederate speaking with
Though EDA is often used as an index of arousal a Southern accent). They found that participants interact-
in response to a particular stimulus, it is a poor indica- ing with an expectancy-violating partner exhibited less
tor of valence. Individuals may show an increase in EDA ventricular contraction, lower cardiac output, and greater
because they are looking at something they really like or total peripheral resistance than did participants interact-
something they really do not like, and it is impossible to ing with a non-expectancy-violating confederate (Mendes,
differentiate these responses by examining EDA alone Blascovich, Hunter, Lickel, & Jost, 2007). These reactions
(Cunningham et al., 2008). For this reason, other measures are consistent with threat responses and indicate that par-
are necessary to accomplish such a task. Facial electro- ticipants interacting with unexpected others felt threatened
myography (EMG) can reveal negative or positive affect rather than challenged by the interaction. Participants whose
associated with a particular target, even when indepen- expectations had been violated also exhibited less positive
dent judges are unable to detect an evaluative response affect and rated their partners less positively than partici-
(see Krosnick et al., 2005). EMG measures electrical pants whose partners matched expectations, indicating that

CH10.indd 363 10/5/09 7:19:36 PM


369 Attitudes

people may hold more positive attitudes toward individuals that the N400 indicates the presence of semantically
whose behaviors are in line with their expectations. incongruent targets (Morris, Squires, Taber, & Lodge, 2003).
It is clear that measures of autonomic arousal can pro- Furthermore, evaluatively inconsistent stimuli also
vide a rich picture of attitudes when used and interpreted elicit LPPs of greater amplitude than evaluatively consis-
properly by experts who understand their virtues and tent stimuli. Participants were asked to accurately report or
limits, as is true of most modern measures of attitudes. misreport their attitudes. LPP amplitudes were higher when
Psychologists will most likely make greater use of physi- participants accurately reported their attitudes, suggesting
ological measures in conjunction with response latency that examining this waveform may provide a measure of
tests, as well as direct measures of brain activity as a means attitudes that participants are unable or unwilling to report
of studying attitudes in the future. (Crites, Cacioppo, Gardner, & Berntson, 1995). The LPP
waveforms that occur when participants respond to evalua-
tively incongruent stimuli are lateralized, and evidence sug-
Measures of Neural Activity
gests that this waveform is greater over the right, rather than
With the ability to measure the brain’s electrical activity the left, hemisphere (Cacioppo et al., 1996). The LPP can also
and blood flow/oxygenation levels, neuroscientists have be lateralized differently depending on the evaluation
looked directly at the boss of the autonomic nervous system. itself: Concepts rated as bad tended to elicit right-lateralized
Developments in social neuroscience have included atten- waveforms, whereas those rated as good elicited left later-
tion to understanding ordinary preferences, fear, and valence alization (Cunningham, Espinet, DeYoung, & Zelazo,
effects, all of which are relevant to understanding attitudes 2005). Positive and negative attitudes also elicit different
(see Lieberman, this volume). Together with behavioral patterns of EEG activation (Pizzagalli, Koenig, Regard, &
measures that have attempted to elaborate on the responses Lehmann, 1999).
participants give to direct questions, neural activity pro- Although ERPs have taught scientists much about atti-
vides yet another way of looking at dissociations between tudes, particularly because they offer good temporal reso-
conscious and less conscious attitudes. Because fMRI and lution, imaging via fMRI complements ERP research by
other technologies used to study the brain are relatively new, providing spatial resolution. The ability to view where
scientists’ understanding of the data presented here will in the brain activity is concentrated during specific tasks,
likely grow exponentially in the years to come. Thus, the possible because of fMRI, has produced a revolution in
findings described here should be taken as a starting place cognitive and social neuroscience. Because brain regions
for future work, not a final say on the role of neural activity that are more active require more oxygen than areas that
in evaluation. are relatively inactive, a greater volume of blood flows to
ERPs have been used for quite some time to determine the more active regions. fMRI technology measures and
the consistency and affective valence of attitudinal reports. records the difference in blood oxygenation levels in vari-
ERPs measure brain activity in response to specific events. ous brain regions. These measurements can be taken when
Notably, ERPs are capable of measuring rapid neural individuals are asked to consciously and deliberately reflect
changes, providing the only noninvasive measure to date on particular aspects of an attitude object, as well as when
of recording neural firing. Neuronal activity results in mea- participants are responding to stimulus items too quickly
surable electrical activity at the scalp, and ERPs correlate to reflect consciously on them (Cunningham & Zelazo,
this activity-specific stimuli or behavior. ERP waveforms 2007). Stanley, Phelps, and Banaji (2008) have offered a
contain multiple components, or deflections in either the three-tier model involving the amygdala, the anterior cin-
positive or negative direction (not valence), and the magni- gulated cortex (ACC), and the dorsolateral prefrontal cor-
tude of these deflections can provide information concern- tex (dlPFC) to describe the neural basis of implicit attitudes
ing the strength of the stimulus’s influence (Bartholow & (see Lieberman, this volume, for diagrams of brain regions
Amodio, 2008; Berger, 1929; Moruzzi & Magoun, 1949). involved in social cognition). According to this model, the
One such deflection is the P300 (e.g., a positive deflec- amygdala is involved in the automatic evaluation of social
tion occurring at a latency of about 300 ms). Judgments stimuli, whereas the ACC is involved in detection of implicit
concerning evaluatively inconsistent objects (e.g., expo- preferences and the dlPFC takes part in regulating them.
sure to a series of negative objects after exposure to posi- The amygdala is uniquely situated to modulate cogni-
tive traits) tend to elicit a larger amplitude P300-like wave tive input and automatic behaviors. It receives information
(Cacioppo, Crites, Berntson, & Coles, 1993). Such judg- from multiple neural structures, including low-level sen-
ments also elicit an N400 wave not found when partici- sory areas, as well as those regions responsible for memory
pants viewed evaluatively congruent stimuli, a result that and higher cognitive processes. Because of the direct route
replicates findings from the semantic literature, suggesting between the amygdala and the thalamus, the amygdala can

CH10.indd 364 10/5/09 7:19:38 PM


Origins of Attitudes 370

process information sooner than the cortical sensory areas how much their attitude had changed now that they were
(LeDoux, 1996). Output from the amygdala travels far permitted more time to come to a decision. Activity in
and wide (i.e., several inches!), to destinations as varied the bilateral amygdala, OFC, and right insula was related
as the brainstem and hypothalamus to early sensory corti- to attitude ratings for both evaluative and nonevaluative
ces and cortical association areas. Its projections into the judgments. Emotionality ratings were correlated with acti-
orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) allow individuals to compare vation in brainstem regions, areas that are associated with
expected rewards and punishments with the circumstances automatic processes. Their activation provides further sup-
in which they presently find themselves. If a conflict is port for the premise that aspects of evaluation function at
detected, the ACC kicks into gear to monitor the situation. unconscious levels (Cunningham, Raye, & Johnson, 2004).
The lateral PFC may also become involved, bringing its Given the coordination among different brain regions, it is
expertise in reprocessing and exerting conscious control impossible to locate attitudes in only one area. Forming,
(Cunningham, Zelazo, Packer, & van Bavel, 2007). expressing, and controlling attitudes requires cooperation
Indeed, attitude researchers have focused on the amyg- among different brain regions, none of which is capable of
dala in large part because of its important role in evalua- doing the job alone.
tion. This research indicates that greater amygdala activity As described earlier, researchers use a variety of tech-
is associated with the presentation of emotionally valenced niques to study automatic attitudes. These methods share
(e.g., fearful) faces conditioned with an aversive stimulus, several commonalities, foremost among which is that they
even when participants are not consciously aware of these do not seek a reflective response to a traditionally posed
stimuli (Morris, Ohman, & Dolan, 1998; Whalen et al., question. Furthermore, many measures of automatic atti-
1998; Williams et al., 2006). This important work suggests tudes make assumptions about the associative nature of
that brain activity, particularly in the amygdala, can pro- learning and memory, and rely on responses given quickly
vide information about evaluations of which the partici- to tap automatic cognition. De Houwer (2008) argues that
pant is unaware. Analyses of brain activity more generally, implicit measures have in common their ability to predict
and of the amygdala in particular, have provided evidence particular outcomes under certain conditions (e.g., circum-
about the malleability of attitudes (Harris & Fiske, 2006, stances under which participants cannot control the expres-
2007). The importance of the amygdala is so great that sion of a particular attitude). That is, measures of automatic
damage to this area severely compromises the ability to attitudes may be related through their functional properties,
acquire classically conditioned preferences (Davis, 1997). and measures may be automatic along particular dimen-
Other brain regions may respond to different aspects of sions but not others (de Houwer, 2006). The IAT, for exam-
evaluation. For example, although the amygdala is involved ple, can be considered automatic in that participants cannot
in automatic evaluation of social stimuli, it is the ACC that easily control their responses, but it cannot be considered
is implicated in the detection of implicit attitudes and the implicit in that participants are often aware of what each
dlPFC that may regulate them (Stanley et al., 2008). The OFC IAT is intended to measure, whereas linguistic measures of
is receiving specific attention in the study of attitudes; attitudes meet the implicit criterion on this dimension.
whereas the left posterior regions of the OFC are associated However, it is clear that the methods are not inter-
with determining the value of a particular object, medial changeable. Though implicit measures may have simi-
anterior areas of this region are involved in making deci- lar functions, they differ structurally and in the specific
sions based on that value (Cunningham, Kesek, & Mowrer, aspects of attitudes they tap (de Houwer, 2008). Although
in press). The ventral striatum, located just above the amyg- response latency measures offer information concerning
dala, may help people learn fear (Davis & Whalen, 2001). implicit associations, physiological and neuroscientific
Like LPPs, patterns of brain activation may be lateralized; methods provide extensive information on physical cor-
areas of the right inferior frontal cortex and anterior insula relates of particular attitudes. Thus, the decision of which
seem to be activated to a greater extent when processing measure to use depends on the type of study and the vari-
negative rather than positive information (Cunningham ables of interest, as well as increasingly on expertise and
et al., 2008). availability of resources.
Patterns of activation in other brain areas show that
evaluation can be unconscious. In one study, participants
were scanned while making evaluative (good or bad) or ORIGINS OF ATTITUDES
nonevaluative (abstract or concrete) judgments about rap-
idly presented stimuli. After scanning, participants were By the time one is old enough to read chapters such as
asked to reflectively rate each stimulus on several evalu- this one, the mind is stuffed with a near-infinite number
ative dimensions (e.g., positive or negative) and to report of attitudes toward everything imaginable, with tastes and

CH10.indd 365 10/5/09 7:19:38 PM


371 Attitudes

distastes ranging from Shropshire cheese to Schopenhauer. some fundamental questions about the origins of attitudes
But where do these attitudes come from and what do psy- have remained unattended for three reasons, and understand-
chologists know about their earliest forms? ing what these reasons are may be instructive.
Although acknowledging that people can acquire atti- First, practical considerations can interfere with the col-
tudes through potentially nonsocial sources such as classical lection of the necessary data. Scientists know little about the
conditioning (discussed later), social psychologists typi- manner in which infants and young children acquire pref-
cally assume that many preferences derive from different erences. Yet to understand anything of consequence one
parts of the social world, from the words and behaviors of must reach back to the earliest stages of development of
other beings, and from the events that unfold in the world. the organism for clues to its ontology. Whether it is the uni-
Individuals “get” attitudes from others directing infor- verse or the human body, the original or first state is crucial.
mation toward them explicitly (“Hey, wanna read some The least persuasive reason is the most likely for the absence
Schopenhauer together?”) or because the experiences that of such data. Adults prove to be more convenient samples
produce them are simply there for the taking (“I tasted some to study than infants and young children. A related reason
Shropshire cheese at Fromaggio’s and now I’m addicted.”). is that carving up the field as psychologists have, the study
The sources of attitudes may be agents in close proximity, of attitudes and social cognition has not been at the center of
such as associates, caregivers, friends, schools, and neigh- the developmentalists’ interest, just as developmental ques-
borhoods, or they may sit far away and reach distant indi- tions concerning social behavior have been largely absent
viduals through new technologies, such as television and the in the laboratories of social cognitionists, a situation that
Internet. From such sources people acquire attitudes toward may now be changing (see Olson & Dweck, 2008).
the shape of bodies, the design of clothes, and the beat of A second reason that the question of origins appears
music. People build preferences from blogs, chat rooms, to have been neglected is political. Examining the origins
and Twitter. The study of attitudes as it exists today provides of attitudes necessarily involves studying the connection
prima facie evidence of the vast and deep ways in which the between evolutionary and biological, as well as social,
social world presents, creates, imposes, cajoles, and sneaks forces. Though some psychologists have tackled the ques-
in attitudes of all forms all the time. tion of evolutionary influences on social behavior (e.g.,
One can learn social attitudes in many ways: from Pinker, 2002), many have been wary of considering such
parents (Sinclair, Dunn, & Lowery, 2005; Tenenbaum & explanations for topics such as group differences in math-
Leaper, 2002), peers (Poteat, 2007; van de Gaer, Pustjens, ematical ability or intelligence. Who can blame them, given
van Damme, & de Munter, 2007), and the media (Levina, the close association between the early analyses of herita-
Waldo, & Fitzgerald, 2000; Hargreaves & Tiggemann, bility and racial imperialism included among other politi-
2003), to name just a few sources. Of course, one’s cultural cally dubious uses of science?
tradition (Dunham, Baron, & Banaji, 2006; Hayes & Lee, William McGuire, writing in a precursor chapter on
2005; Sahar & Karasawa, 2005), the process of attitude this topic, notes that “[a] man of this writer ’s generation
contagion (Alexander, Piazza, Mekos, & Valente, 2001; considers the possibility that there may be a genetic com-
Cohen & Prinstein, 2006), and social learning (Bandura, ponent in attitude determination only with trepidation”
1977) all contribute to attitude formation as well. (McGuire, 1968, p. 161). Scientists should now be able to
To study attitudes, then, has been to study the processes overcome political resistance to pursuing intellectual mat-
of preference acquisition and change. Because this is the ters of importance so that such trepidation is legitimately
case, this chapter focuses on some of the lesser studied top- nonexistent. As psychology and other life sciences inter-
ics and approaches, as well as those that are in early stages act and even meld, integrative analyses of attitudes may
of growth, in the hope that their presence here will facili- also prosper. Such analyses will permit a more seamless
tate greater attention. Importantly, developmental mea- view of what is in the body and outside it, and they will
sures, especially those used with infants, are quite restricted surely reveal how people’s preferences have been shaped
because of infants’ limited capacities to express themselves. by the biological and cultural history of the human spe-
Such measures are thus open to multiple interpretations, cies, as well as the highly influential immediate situations
some of which are presented later. in which individuals find themselves.
The predominant understanding of attitudes concerns Such analyses should now proceed without their misuse
the structure of existing preferences and the processes of by eugenicists, a misuse in which psychology has played
attitude change in adult humans. Working with existing and no small role. In addition, psychologists can no longer turn
well-developed attitudes meant forfeiting analyses of their a blind eye to the fact that attitudes, like any other aspect
formation and change; thus, investigators often created new of human nature and culture, have an evolutionary his-
attitudes in the laboratory to have “clean” ones to dissect. But tory. Much can be learned from the open-mindedness of

CH10.indd 366 10/5/09 7:19:39 PM


Origins of Attitudes 372

the early commentaries in social psychology. Allport’s first scattered studies measuring the heritability factor of attitudes
chapter on attitudes in the 1935 handbook remarks on the suggest two conclusions. First, heritable attitudes seem to
favored status of the attitude concept by stating, “It is a be psychologically more robust and accessible (Bourgeois,
concept which escapes the ancient controversy concern- 2002; Crelia & Tesser, 1996; Tesser, 1993). Genetic influ-
ing the relative influence of heredity and environment” ences on attitudes may emerge earlier and seem to be more
(p. 798). This chapter treats it as such conceptually, in the consequential (e.g., people tend to prefer those who share their
hope that the next handbook will contain more solid evi- attitudes over those who do not, and this effect appears to be
dence about the origins of attitudes that can be obtained slightly stronger for attitudes that are more heritable). More
only by paying heed to analyses not only of adult humans heritable preferences may also be less prone to change; par-
but to other species and younger humans as well. ticipants in a group discussion showed less attitude change
A third reason for ignoring the question of origins is the when discussing evaluations estimated to be more heritable.
more understandable one of the lack of availability of meth- The influences of heritable attitudes appear earlier than pre-
ods. Recent research has shown just how much progress viously thought, showing up during early adolescence in
new techniques permit. Methods to understand the minds some studies (Abrahamson, Baker, & Caspi, 2002).
of adults and children using both behavioral and brain mea- Second, attitudes vary greatly in what is considered to be
sures will surely play a major role in new understandings of their heritability factor (e.g., Olson, Vernon, Harris, & Jang,
the attitude concept. In the time between Gregor Mendel’s 2001; Crelia & Tesser, 1996; Tesser, 1993). For example,
discoveries of the laws of heredity in 1866 and their redis- relatively high heritability coefficients have been calculated
covery at the beginning of the 20th century, as well as the for attitudes toward a variety of objects, including the death
sequencing of the human genome in 2003, a new era of penalty, jazz, apartheid, and censorship (Tesser, 1993).
understanding the role of heredity in the nature of prefer- Attitudes toward abortion are highly heritable, whereas atti-
ence has opened. tudes toward easy access to birth control are only weakly
Social psychology focuses on the social environment, heritable (Olson et al., 2001). Political attitudes appear to
and this will naturally lead scientists working in this field to be more heritable than political party identification (Alford,
be appropriately skeptical of any simple genetic reduc- Funk, & Hibbing, 2005), and conservatism seems to be
tionism in understanding attitudes and all things important. more highly heritable than religious attitudes (Abrahamson
They should continue to be unconvinced when it comes to et al., 2002). In general, 40% to 50% of the variability in
understanding attitudes for obvious reasons, including the ideological attitudes may be because of genetic factors
more general indictment that Lewontin provides in his (Alford et al., 2005; Bouchard et al., 2003; Carmen, 2007).
book The Triple Helix (2002, p. 17): “Any computer that This may be explained by the heritability of other cognitive
did as poor a job of computation as an organism does from and motivational factors that underlie political orientation
its genetic ‘program’ would immediately be thrown into (e.g., orientations toward uncertainty and threat; Jost, 2006,
the trash and its manufacturer would be sued by the pur- but see also Alford & Hibbing, 2007).
chaser.” The same can be said for theories of environmental Heritability research faces many difficulties. For one,
input as well, but because there is always the fascination no explanation exists for why some attitudes are or should
that the genetic code will explain everything, it is partic- be more heritable than others, nor do psychologists know
ularly important to be aware of the bias to overattribute what mechanism(s) might be responsible for transmitting
causal importance to genetic factors. some attitudes from parent to child. In addition, studies of
Nevertheless, it remains important to examine the bio- heritability have mostly used twin samples (e.g., Eaves,
logical and social roots of preferences. Naïve theories about Eysenck, & Martin, 1989), which present well-known inter-
the heritability of attitudes go in two opposing directions. pretational difficulties. For example, monozygotic twins
The visible preferences of children and parents are clearly are treated more similarly by significant others (Eaves
shaped by their different age cohorts, making it easy to see et al., 1989) and may, therefore, show a stronger resem-
differences in attitudes. “I am so not like my mother” is a blance because of environmental and not genetic factors.
routine expression, especially when speaking about tastes This may be true even for identical twins reared apart
in music, attire, and views of parenting. Just as much, gen- because of shared features that may elicit similar treatment
erational similarity also comes through: “He and his daugh- (e.g., because physical attractiveness is known to play a
ter both scrunch up their faces when they are served carrots; role in how one is treated, e.g., Dion, Berscheid, & Walster,
it must be genetic.” 1972). Furthermore, attitudes may incorrectly reveal a
It would be nice if the research evidence pointed a way high heritability coefficient when all members of a group
out, ruling clearly in favor or not in favor of the partial heri- have been exposed to the same environmental input. For
tability of attitudes, but that is not the current situation. A few instance, if all members of a particular geographic region

CH10.indd 367 10/5/09 7:19:40 PM


373 Attitudes

have consumed the same propaganda, they will likely show what they do know is restricted to a small number of
similar attitudes toward the object in question, leading to attitude domains. This situation appears to be changing,
a high heritability estimate despite the fact that the atti- with a more focused interest in understanding the minds of
tude was socially transmitted (Olson et al., 2001). Because infants and young children as they develop the ability for
genes can influence individuals’ responses to their environ- social cognition.
ment, genetic and environmental influences are difficult to Researchers typically use one of several measures when
separate (Eaves et al., 1989). studying infants. Looking time is among the most com-
mon, especially with infants who are not old enough to
reach for objects. Psychologists have used looking time as
Primates
an indication of the ability to differentiate different classes
To understand the attitudes people have and why, psycholo- of objects. For instance, if infants see nine female faces in
gists may look toward nonhuman primates, those closer to a row and then gaze longer at the tenth face if it is male,
humans in the ancestral chain, as well as those much further that is evidence that the infant recognizes the tenth face as
away. Understanding nonhuman primates alongside people different from the first nine. Psychologists have also used
provides yet another path toward deeper knowledge of looking time to index familiarity and preference. These
the origins of attitudes and preferences. Because attitudes, two constructs are difficult to differentiate among young
linked as they are to enabling survival, are so fundamental infants, who cannot give other indications of whether they
to the existence of every species, comparative studies may are looking longer at a particular object because it is famil-
also help those theorists whose primary task is to broadly iar or because they like it. Children who are old enough to
understand evolutionary processes that are psychological in grasp may provide evidence in favor of one or the other
nature. Exploring the social attitudes of primates is currently interpretation because they are likely to reach for the object
beyond the scope of this chapter because the research lies they prefer.
in disparate areas and needs concerted study and analysis Even in early infancy, the roots of mature social pref-
before review here. It also needs the expertise of those who erences are visible. A standard measure of preference is
are primatologists and other behavioral scientists, and such to examine the people or objects toward which a baby ori-
collaborations have recently begun to occur. For example, a ents. Such studies show that minutes after birth, infants
study with common marmoset monkeys showed that they show a preference for facelike configurations over equally
will provide food to genetically unrelated others, even if the complex but nonsocial patterns (Johnson, Dziurawiec,
others cannot reciprocate (Burkart, Fehr, Efferson, & van Ellis, & Morton, 1991; Mondloch et al., 1999; Valenza,
Schaik, 2007). In other research, Santos and colleagues have Simion, Cassia, & Umilta, 1996). Infants also prefer attrac-
shown that the complex mental operations that are involved tive over unattractive human (Ramsey, Langlois, Hoss,
in producing the effects of cognitive dissonance in humans Rubenstein, & Griffin, 2004) and animal (Quinn, Kelly,
can also be detected in capuchin monkeys (Egan, Santos, & Lee, Pascalis, & Slater, 2008) faces. These findings sug-
Bloom, 2007). Where humans obviously differ is in the abil- gest that the origins of the “what is beautiful is good” ste-
ity to be aware of preferences that are beneficial versus those reotype (Dion et al., 1972) are formed in early infancy.
that are harmful and make decisions that are opposed to the Surprisingly, infants also respond differently to sounds
dictates of simpler strategies of reproductive fitness. In fact, heard while in utero compared with more novel sounds. In
scholars writing about evolutionary theory have tended to one study, a group of pregnant women read Dr. Seuss’s
even-handedly analyze behaviors mindful of their long- delightful story The Cat in the Hat out loud, whereas
standing existence in the history of the species, as well as another group of pregnant women read a version where
the demands of culture and social environments in shaping the words “cat” and “hat” were replaced with “dog” and
them (e.g., Griskevicius, Goldstein, Mortensen, Cialdini, & “fog,” respectively. Newborns were able to listen to one
Kenrick, 2006; Schmitt, Realo, Voracek, & Allik, 2008; or the other version depending on how they sucked on a
Schwartz & Rubel, 2005). This work is highlighted here pacifier; for instance, if they sucked more vigorously,
because it is likely to become a shared space for collabo- a recording apparatus played a tape of a woman reading
ration between social psychology and primatology (e.g., the “cat in the hat” version, whereas if they sucked less
Mahajan, Martinez, Diesendruck, Banaji, & Santos, 2009). vigorously, the apparatus played a recording of the “dog
in the fog” version. Infants in both conditions altered
their sucking behavior to produce the more familiar story
Infants and Young Children
(DeCasper & Spence, 1986).
Psychologists know little about the social attitudes of As any parent knows, infants also show strong prefer-
infants and a bit more about such attitudes in children; ences for the face and voice of their primary caregivers

CH10.indd 368 10/5/09 7:19:42 PM


Origins of Attitudes 374

(Pascalis, de Schonen, Morton, Deruelle, & Fabre-Grenet, Results such as these have challenged existing views
1995). Though it is often assumed that newborns orient that attitudes emerge from protracted social learning in
toward women, research shows that 3- to 4-month-old which children converge on adult forms of attitudes over
infants whose primary caregiver is a male individual prefer a long period of development (Dunham & Banaji, 2008).
men’s faces (Quinn et al., 2002), suggesting the openness Instead, even the first studies show that complex attitudes,
of the mind to literally turn toward that which is famil- such as those toward social collectives, exist in adult-
iar. So far, only these two dimensions of preference—that like form in the earliest years in which such tests can be
toward faces, particularly attractive ones, and that toward administered. Other work shows that children, like adults,
sounds heard in utero—have been identified as requiring often prefer members of their own group even when the
no learning; they are present at birth (see Pascalis & Slater, group is not socially meaningful (e.g., groups that are dif-
2003; Slater, 2002). ferentiated based on the color of the shirt participants are
Though newborns do not distinguish among racial ingroup asked to wear; Baron & Carey, 2009; Bigler, Spears
and outgroup faces, infants as young as 3 months exhibit Brown, & Markell, 2001). This suggests a strong conti-
a marked preference for faces of same-race targets (Kelly nuity for attitudes—at least those related to intergroup
et al., 2005, 2007a). This preference grows stronger as infants evaluations—across development.
age; by 9 months, babies in one study were no longer able to Preference for the familiar is not limited to race. In fact,
distinguish faces of racial outgroup members (Kelly et al., accent may trump race in certain circumstances. In a series
2007b). Importantly, many experiments examining looking of studies, infants were permitted to grasp objects that
preference have focused on babies who had not been exposed were presented to them simultaneously by speakers of dif-
to racially diverse faces before seeing other-race faces in the ferent languages or accents. The results indicate an early
study (Bar-Haim, Ziv, Lamy, & Hodes, 2006; Kelly et al., preference for familiar sounds: 10-month-old Americans
2005). Thus, similar to a preference for faces of the same sex prefer English speakers to French speakers (80% reach for
as the primary caregiver, an own-race preference may indi- the toy offered by the English speaker), whereas French
cate a preference for familiar stimuli. babies show the opposite and symmetric preference for
Providing evidence of very early learning and prefer- their familiar sounds (Kinzler, Dupoux, & Spelke, 2007).
ence formation, these remarkable results suggest that later White children at age 5, showed a preference for a white
preferences for the familiar may have their roots in expe- other over a black other; however, when race and accent
riences that occur even before the first year of life. Yet, are explicitly confounded, such that the standard American
caution is needed when interpreting findings from studies English speaker is Black and the English speaker with
that use preferential looking as an indication of preference; French accent is White, accent proves more influential
after all, looking times can reflect alternative processes of than race and children prefer the Black speaker (speak-
surprise, intrigue, and expectancy violation. Bypassing this ing standard English) over the White speaker (speaking
limitation by using the Child IAT mentioned in the mea- English with a French accent; Kinzler, Shutts, DeJesus, &
surement section, some evidence suggests that 6-year-old Spelke, under review).
White children show a preference for Whites of the same Children’s preference for the familiar may be
magnitude as the preference found among 10-year-olds explained by one of psychology’s best-known findings:
and adults. Explicit race attitudes follow a quite different the mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968). Simple expo-
course, with the strongest ingroup preferences expressed sure to a neutral object can cause people to rate it more
at age 6, weaker at age 10, and nonexistent in the adult favorably, even when the exposure happens too quickly
sample (Baron & Banaji, 2006). for conscious processing. This effect may rely on the fact
That attitudes formed even at such a young age are that it is easier to process a familiar rather than an unfamiliar
socially constructed is poignantly seen in the following stimulus (Seamon, Brody, & Kauff, 1983). Supporting
result. The previous finding, if it reflects a mere prefer- this perspective, recent work has found that people show
ence for one’s own group, should be visible in children of more liking for objects that are preceded by primes
all social groups equally. Such is not the case, however, that facilitate processing and less liking for objects that
with the evidence showing that children from disadvan- are preceded by primes that inhibit easy processing
taged social groups (Hispanic and Black Americans) do (Reber, Winkielman, & Schwarz, 1998; Schwarz & Clore,
not show implicit ingroup preference, on average, when 2007; Winkielman, Schwarz, Fazendeiro, & Reber, 2003).
the comparison is their group versus the dominant group
Social Learning
(Dunham, Baron, & Banaji, 2008). Indeed, such is also
the case among adults; 40% to 50% of Blacks show a pro- Social learning theory (Bandura, 1977) continues to be the
White bias on the IAT (Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004). dominant account of how children acquire attitudes: by

CH10.indd 369 10/5/09 7:19:42 PM


375 Attitudes

observing others. Indeed, new evidence suggests that knowledge of the paradigms used in the study, suggesting
children may use this strategy to learn attitudes toward that not all conditioning effects are due to demand charac-
novel objects. In one set of studies, children preferred teristics (Baeyens, Eelen, & van den Bergh, 1990; Olson &
unfamiliar stimulus items (e.g., blicket, spoodle) chosen Fazio, 2001).
by another person of their same sexgender and age. For Of course, classical conditioning accounts for acquisition
example, 3-year-old girls who heard that an unfamiliar girl of both positive and negative attitudes, and can be used for
liked to play with blicket whereas an unfamiliar boy liked virtuous and nefarious purposes. Classical conditioning as
to play with spoodle said that they would rather play with a form of learning is understood quite well, based as it is
blicket (Shutts, Banaji, & Spelke, in press). Young children on data from several species, including humans, and using
also learn attitudes from older people, particularly their fear as the attitude that is induced. In classical conditioning
caregivers. As discussed earlier, part of this link may be models, people learn fear in much the same way as they may
because of genetics, especially for attitudes that are par- learn positive associations. For instance, what if every time
ticularly heritable. However, a social learning explanation participants heard a bell they received an electric shock?
also seems likely. They would learn to fear the bell, even on trials where it was
Researchers have found an association between chil- not immediately followed by physical pain. Such results are
dren’s and parents’ attitudes across multiple domains. For of great importance because fear is one of the most devastat-
example, mothers’ hostile attitudes predict the development ing emotions an organism can experience. At extreme levels,
of similar attitudes in their children (Raikkonen, Katainen, fearful attitudes can significantly impair daily functioning
Keskivaara, & Kelikangas-Jarvinen, 2000). Furthermore, (Rapaport, Clary, Fayyad, & Endicott, 2005).
parents’ attitudes toward racial minorities and nontraditional Unfortunately, it may be easier for humans to associate
women predict their children’s attitudes toward these targets fear with outgroup members, particularly those of another
(Sinclair, Dunn, & Lowery, 2005; Tenenbaum & Leaper, race. In one experiment, participants showed longer-
2002), and parental prejudice also influences the frequency lasting fear to racial outgroup faces that had previously
and quality of young children’s interracial interactions been paired with an electric shock. That is, when Whites
(Towles-Schwen & Fazio, 2001). In the only such study to saw a Black face and were then shocked, they showed
date, children’s implicit racial bias was found to be corre- a larger SCR (see the section entitled, “Measures of the
lated with that of their parents, particularly for those who Autonomic System”) than when they received an electric
are highly identified with their caregivers (Sinclair, Dunn, & shock paired with a White face. Blacks showed a similar pat-
Lowery, 2005). Thus, levels of prejudice may be transmitted tern; they, too, had a more persistent fear response to racial
from older family members to future generations. outgroup (in this case, White) faces. These findings suggest
that images of racial outgroup members may function as
Classical Conditioning prepared stimuli, or stimuli that are more readily associated
First introduced by Pavlov (1927), classical conditioning is with fear (Olsson, Ebert, Banaji, & Phelps, 2005).
among the most basic forms of learning. It refers to the pro- Recent research has supplemented this Pavlovian model
cess whereby a person develops positive or negative associ- of fear learning with evidence suggesting that people can
ations with a previously neutral object through association acquire an attitude of fear toward objects that have been
of inherently positive or negative attributes. For example, a associated with pain in others. That is, fear may be acquired
particular dress may gain positive associations through its through a social learning (Bandura, 1977) process. Partic-
pairing with an attractive model in advertisements, whereas ipants who view a video of another person receiving
African Americans may be subjected to negative evalu- shocks linked with particular images, for example, show
ations stemming, in part, from their negative portrayal in signs of fearing those images but not similar ones of a
the media (e.g., the repeated pairing of Black male indi- different color (Olsson, Nearing, & Phelps, 2007). This
viduals with violence). Attitude acquisition through clas- process may be evolutionarily adaptive; the ability to
sical conditioning remains a contentious issue, however, learn from others’ misfortunes may come in handy if one
in part because some researchers have found significant has seen a family member attacked by a bear and later
effects only in situations where participants were aware encounters another furry creature wandering in the woods.
that a conditioning paradigm was being used (see Olson & However, this process also has drawbacks, particularly if it
Fazio, 2001). Such results call into question the validity of is overactivated. For instance, learning to fear all middle-
some other findings by suggesting that the effects may be aged men after being robbed by one is not adaptive.
because of demand characteristics rather than classical con- Neural evidence suggests that classical and observa-
ditioning per se. However, evidence of classical condition- tional fear learning may not be as different as they first
ing can be present even when participants did not report appear. The amygdala, a brain region that is known for

CH10.indd 370 10/5/09 7:19:43 PM


The Attitude Toward Oneself 376

emotional processing and is particularly involved in fear, object toward which attitude is resoundingly positive,
is recruited both when subjects watch someone else receiv- that object is the self. Self-evaluations achieve consistency
ing shocks and when they receive shocks themselves during early adulthood and remain high before declining
(Olsson et al., 2007). This finding may explain why obser- after age 60 (Trzesniewski, Donnellan, & Robins, 2003).
vational learning is as effective as classically conditioned Additional variations across people and within the same
fear when the stimulus is consciously available (Olsson & person from circumstance to circumstance, perhaps even
Phelps, 2004). Notably, though amygdala activation was of moment to moment, have been investigated thoroughly,
comparable magnitude when participants were anticipat- making self-esteem a topic from which it is possible to
ing pain and when they actually received a shock, other learn much about the nature of attitudes.
brain regions were not similarly activated. The ACC As with the measurement of attitudes in general, attempts
and the anterior insula both exhibited greater activation to create new measures of implicit self-esteem have also
during the test than in the observation phase of the experi- grown in recent years. One such family of measures involves
ment (Olsson et al., 2007); thus, it appears that the amyg- looking at the degree to which one imbues the things and
dala responds to anticipation of pain differently than do people who have come to be associated with oneself with
other brain regions. positive regard (see Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). Known as
In summary, despite controversy surrounding the influence the name-letter effect, one technique measures the uncon-
of demand characteristics in classical conditioning exper- scious liking people show for the letters, and especially
iments, this paradigm remains a powerful tool to examine the initials, of their own name (developed by Nuttin, 1985;
the formation of various attitudes, particularly fear. From Krizan & Suls, 2008). Such preference can lead people to
studies of classical conditioning, psychologists know prefer products whose brand name starts with the same let-
that people can be taught to fear almost anything through ter as their own name (Brendl, Chattopadhyay, Pelham, &
association with a potent negative stimulus. Indeed, some Carvallo, 2005) and even to marry people whose first or
objects (e.g., images of racial outgroup members) are quite last name resembles their own (Jones, Pelham, Carvallo, &
easily associated with fear, an association that can be dif- Mirenberg, 2004). People are not only attracted to those
ficult to unlearn. who share their names but also to individuals with the same
Other stimulus items, in contrast, are quite easily associ- birth dates. One study showed that participants judged his-
ated with positive characteristics. One such target is the self. torical characters such as Rasputin less harshly when led to
believe that they shared a birthday with the unsavory char-
acter (Finch & Cialdini, 1989).
THE ATTITUDE TOWARD ONESELF Measures of implicit self-esteem look so different from
traditional (explicit) measures that the question of what
Human beings have a capacity that is the envy of every implicit measures might predict has acquired priority. In
chimpanzee—the ability to look into and evaluate the con- research with a clinical focus, measures of implicit self-
tents of their own minds. Remarkably, this capacity can be esteem have been shown to relate to narcissism (Zeigler-
turned on not only when evaluating all possible entities that Hill, 2006), symptoms of depression (Franck, de Raedt, &
exist in the world “out there” but also to evaluate one’s own de Houwer, 2007), and poor body image (Buhlmann,
worth along a subjectively meaningful good-bad continuum. Teachman, Gerbershagen, Kikul, & Rief, 2008). Such mea-
Self-esteem can be considered a primary attitude—an sures show more reliability than self-report questionnaires
evaluation of oneself. How good am I? Do I approve of what (Farnham, Greenwald, & Banaji, 1999) and may decrease
I have done? These questions can be posed and answered self-presentation concerns.
by engaging a conscious and third-person–like feeling for Although some researchers have found high correlations
oneself, an experience about which the self-esteem litera- between implicit and explicit self-esteem, most others report
ture has taught scientists a great deal (Baumeister, 1993; that two constructs are unrelated (see Dijksterhuis, Albers, &
Crocker & Major, 1989; Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000; Leary, Bongers, 2008; Greenwald & Farnham, 2000), and there are
1999, 2004; Major, Kaiser, & McCoy, 2003; Rosenberg, cases in which the two forms of self-attitude are sharply dis-
1965; Swann, Chang-Schneider, & Larsen McClarty, 2007; sociated. For example, stressful life events may influence
Twenge & Campbell, 2001). Swann and Bosson (this volume) implicit and explicit self-esteem differently, at least under
thoroughly review the self-esteem literature. This chapter some circumstances. Recent Asian American immigrants,
offers a brief glimpse into a perspective that views self- for instance, show a quick recovery in explicit self-esteem
esteem as an attitude. but persistently lowered implicit self-esteem after arriving in
The research findings detailed in the articles referenced the United States (Hetts, Sakuma, & Pelham, 1999). Thus,
earlier overwhelmingly demonstrate that if there is a single it seems safest to say that, on average, implicit and explicit

CH10.indd 371 10/5/09 7:19:43 PM


377 Attitudes

self-esteem are weakly correlated. As with other attitudes parenting linked to high self-esteem (de Hart, Pelham, &
in the intergroup context, where this weak correlation has Tennen, 2006). Self-esteem can also arise from the ways
also been found, the conclusion is that implicit and explicit in which people respond to contingencies of self-worth
forms of self-esteem are related but sufficiently unique to be (e.g., appearance, relative performance in competitions),
regarded as distinct. and early childhood experiences can determine responses
Unambiguously high self-esteem is associated with a to events in these domains (Crocker & Park, 2003). Self-
number of positive outcomes. Related to research concern- esteem is also malleable across situations; for example,
ing the social origins of self-esteem (e.g., Murray, Griffin, evaluative conditioning has been shown to increase
Rose, & Bellavia, 2006), positive self-regard is associated self-esteem across a number of studies (Baccus, Baldwin, &
with healthy social relationships and relationship satis- Packer, 2004; Dijksterhuis, 2004). The debate on whether
faction (e.g., Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 2000; Neyer & attitudes are stable or constructed lives on in debates about
Asendorpf, 2001), as well as positive evaluations by others self-attitudes, with evidence on both sides. On the one
(e.g., Robins, Hendin, & Trzesniewski, 2001). High self- hand, attitudes toward the self do show stability, as well as
esteem also predicts occupational success (e.g., Judge & individual differences that are reliable. On the other hand,
Bono, 2001), subjective well-being (e.g., Diener & Diener, however, self-esteem is critically influenced by people and
1995), and positive responses to failure (Di Paula & events in the social world, in predictable and lawful ways.
Campbell, 2002). From reading most of the work on self-attitudes, it is
Consistently low self-regard is associated with a num- easy to draw the conclusion that increasing positive self-
ber of negative outcomes, including depression and other regard is something to which all should strive. In con-
health problems (e.g., Franck et al., 2007; Roberts, Gotlib, & trast, high rather than low self-esteem can be the cause of
Kassel, 1996; Whisman & Kwon, 1993). People with low violence and aggression (Baumeister, Smart, & Boden,
self-esteem react more strongly to failures (Brown & 1996). Furthermore, high self-esteem is related to preferring
Dutton, 1995), experience a greater reduction in motiva- a novel ingroup to an equivalent outgroup member, sug-
tion after a lack of success (Greenwald & Farnham, 2000), gesting a link to discrimination (Gramzow & Gaertner,
and exhibit more anxiety during a confrontational inter- 2005; but see Brewer, 1999). These lines of research have
view (Spalding & Hardin, 1999). In fact, low self-esteem begun to question the value of the strong American belief
can prospectively predict depressive symptoms (Orth, in pumping up positive self-regard by showing that high
Robins, & Roberts, 2008), as well as criminal behavior and self-esteem may not be the panacea it is made out to be in
reduced economic prospects (Trzesniewski et al., 2006). the Western world.
Some outcomes may be preferentially linked with In conclusion, although psychologists have not tradi-
implicit self-esteem. For example, though depressed indi- tionally considered self-esteem an attitude, it does, indeed,
viduals tend to exhibit low explicit self-esteem, their fall into this category. Scientists have learned that self-
implicit self-esteem remains at levels comparable with a attitudes are likely to be robustly positive; that self-esteem,
nondepressed population (de Raedt, Schacht, Franck, & like other attitudes, has both implicit and explicit compo-
de Houwer, 2006). Though this finding is surprising in nents; and that these factors may differentially influence
light of cognitive theories of depression, de Raedt and col- behavioral outcomes. Self-esteem can vary within as well
leagues suggest that it is in line with findings suggesting as between individuals, and those people with high explicit
that depressed people do not lack positive self-schemas. self-esteem do not necessarily show correspondingly high
Perhaps implicit measures tap these underlying represen- levels of implicit self-esteem. The next section further
tations, which depressed individuals may not activate nor- addresses this topic of attitude dissociations.
mally. The dissociation is also evident in narcissism, which
is associated with high explicit paired with low implicit
self-esteem (Zeigler-Hill, 2006; but see also Campbell, ATTITUDE DISSOCIATIONS
Bosson, Goheen, Lakey, & Kernis, 2007).
Given the numerous benefits of high self-esteem, one This chapter, more than others on the topic of attitudes,
may wonder where this elixir comes from. Though peo- has focused on the implicit preferences that people can-
ple show robust and high self-esteem cross-culturally, not or will not report. Here, the topic of dissociations
this construct is clearly not invariable across individuals between conscious and less conscious forms of attitudes
and situations. Self-esteem is shaped by others’ favorable is addressed in greater detail, using the domain of inter-
(or unfavorable) opinions (e.g., Murray et al., 2006) and group attitudes as a case study simply because the bulk
people’s own upbringing, with overprotective parent- of the available research is focused there (see Yzerbyt &
ing linked to low implicit self-esteem and nurturing Demoulin, volume 2, and Dovidio & Gaertner, volume 2,

CH10.indd 372 10/5/09 7:19:44 PM


Attitude Dissociations 378

for a more comprehensive review of work conducted on signature result of modern research on two attitudinal sys-
intergroup relations). tems and has been reported in dozens of articles, making
As with human memory, implicit and explicit attitudes it impossible to refer to more than a handful of them (for
are assumed to be formed in different ways and expected reviews, see Fazio & Olson, 2003; Greenwald & Nosek,
to be susceptible to different influences; that is among the 2008; Hofmann, Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le, & Schmitt,
causes of their divergence (Craemer, 2007; Payne et al., 2005; Wilson et al., 2000). Among the underlying changes
2008). As beliefs and values about intergroup relations that such a result has silently produced is a shift in thinking
have changed, in particular, the social unacceptability of about the “prejudiced personality” as an aberrant and path-
expressing negativity toward groups other than one’s own, ological mental state to one that is ordinary and best under-
such attitudes have offered up a perfect place to observe stood as a continuum of individual difference (Dovidio &
disparities between expressed and elicited attitudes. Gaertner, 2004).
There is no better starting place than an article that Findings that are relevant to the question of dissocia-
appeared in 1989 and set off a flood of research on the tion do not come only from the intergroup literature, and
question of dissociations in automatic and controlled the dissociation (or lack thereof) in other domains has
intergroup attitudes. In her dissertation research, Patricia sometimes proved to be surprising. For example, as might
Devine showed that on a measure that elicited attitudes be expected, implicit and explicit attitudes toward abortion
through subliminal exposure of race primes, Whites who are highly correlated. In other cases, however, attitudes
consciously endorsed prejudicial views of Black Americans that might be expected to correlate are instead dissociated.
responded in essentially the same way as those who did There is little relationship, for instance, between whether
not. On other measures, where greater control over the atti- people say they prefer pants or skirts and the implicit pref-
tude expression was possible, the results looked quite dif- erence they show for one type of clothing over the other
ferent (Devine, 1989). Devine’s work was consistent with (Nosek, 2007).
existing proposals of aversive racism (Gaertner & Dovidio, From the basic finding that implicit and explicit prefer-
1986), which also spoke of a split between older forms of ences can diverge, several questions about the very nature of
prejudice that were explicit and blatant, and newer versions attitudes have emerged. Does the result really indicate two
that formed in response to shifts in American culture that (or more) systems of thought composed of separate rep-
precluded such expressions. As sociologists have shown, resentations, or do the methods of retrieving the attitude
values of fairness and equality have undermined endorse- merely tap different sides of the same underlying evalua-
ment of prejudice and discrimination, leading to a visible tion? How is such a dissociation to be computed? Is one of
shift in intergroup attitudes (Schuman, Steeh, Bobo, & these attitudes the “truer” one? How do the attitudes predict
Krysan, 1997). From such observations and the ability of behavior? For those who have argued that implicit mea-
a generation of methods to understand consciously inac- sures are not measures of attitude at all, this is an impor-
cessible intergroup attitudes, a wealth of data now exist on tant question. Explicitly stated attitudes have been studied
the dissociation between two fundamentally different forms for long enough that psychologists know something about
of attitudes that can be elicited from the same individual their nature—what goes into forming them, their malle-
toward the same group, depending on the probe that is used ability, their function. But what about implicit attitudes? If
(Dovidio, Kawakami, Smoak, & Gaertner, 2008; Fazio et al., they are so automatic, are they rigid and unbendable?
1986; Hofmann, Gschwendner, Nosek, & Schmitt, 2005). In most cases, a clear, simple, and often large differ-
When examining attitudes toward stigmatized groups, ence exists between the mean values of the implicit atti-
the data reveal stronger negative attitudes on implicit mea- tude and the explicit one. When these data are the focus,
sures than those expressed on explicit ones. In many cases, there is no question that a dissociation is evident. Its spe-
the effect size showing implicit group preference (usually cific nature takes on some interesting forms. For instance,
one’s own) can be two to three times the size obtained on White Americans report much less ingroup preference
self-report measures. Situational factors can influence the on explicit than on implicit measures; in contrast, Black
discrepancy between conscious and unconscious attitudes Americans report greater ingroup preference and lesser
and their different influences on behavior. For example, outgroup preference on explicit measures. This is not
different kinds of tests can predict different behaviors. uncommon among other stigmatized groups; the elderly
Implicit measures tend to predict nonverbal behaviors such show the same implicit preference for “young” over “old”
as leaning toward or away from one’s conversation partner, as their younger counterparts, and gay and lesbian individ-
whereas explicit questionnaires predict verbal behaviors uals do not show a strong preference for their own group;
(Dovidio, Kawakami, & Gaertner, 2002). The difference indeed, approximately 40% show a preference for straight
between explicit and implicit attitudes has been taken as a over gay (Jost et al., 2004; Nosek, Fitzsimons, & Kay,

CH10.indd 373 10/5/09 7:19:44 PM


379 Attitudes

2007). From such findings it is possible to see the different questions reflect a basic contrast between the assumptions
sources of influence that bear down on these two expres- concerning conscious and unconscious attitudes. Certainly,
sions of preference. research revealing neuroplasticity—that is, the ability of the
The question of dissociation takes on a different meaning brain to rewire and reconstruct itself to meet new demands
when attitudes toward targets other than stigmatized group or in response to new learning—suggests that change need
members are examined. Attitudes toward more neutral tar- not be consciously willed. It is in the nature of the demands
gets vary greatly depending on the domain of the attitude, made on the brain, on the new behaviors that are performed
but importantly, the correlation is hardly ever at zero or neg- that lead to change, regardless of whether such change is
ative. In many cases, the correlation between implicit and consciously willed.
explicit attitudes is substantial (see Nosek, 2005). In other In the early years of research on implicit attitudes,
words, the stronger the stated preference for X over Y, the there was, indeed, the sense that their lack of openness
stronger the implicit preference for X over Y. to conscious awareness and control must mean that they
Although strong positive correlations are obtained on would be rigid in response to change (Banaji, 2003). That
attitude topics such as Black-White, Coke-Pepsi, and cats- assumption turned out to be false. Evidence since the late
dogs, weak correlations are evident for topics that typically 1990s has accumulated at a rapid rate to show that implicit
do not represent intergroup contexts such as hot-cold and attitudes shift readily in response to contextual variables,
future-past (Nosek, 2007). Although self-presentation con- motivational states, and cognitive factors (see Blair, 2002;
cerns do play a role with those attitudes that are strongly Dasgupta, 2009; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). Such
susceptible to social desirability, measures tapping these evidence has found a home in theoretical models that have
attitudes tend to show lower implicit-explicit correlations. moved beyond the simple dual-process idea and probed
This is hardly the only factor accounting for the interrela- deeper into the nature of implicit social cognition, espe-
tionship, however (notice the low correlation for hot-cold cially the rapid responses that intergroup contexts elicit
and the strong correlation for Black-White). (Conrey et al., 2005; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006).
Other factors, such as the distinctiveness of the atti- For example, attitudes toward Black Americans varied
tude (i.e., perceived difference of one’s own attitude significantly when the background of the photo represented
compared with that of others), the degree of personal poor rather than middle-class neighborhoods (Wittenbrink,
experience with the attitude object, and the clarity of Judd, & Park, 2001). Furthermore, Black female individuals
two poles (where the two ends of the spectrum are true elicited more positive attitudes when thought of as female
opposites), influence the degree of correlation as well. rather than as Black (Mitchell, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003).
Furthermore, attitudes that are well elaborated and high Exposure to positive Black exemplars (Denzel Washington,
in importance produce stronger implicit-explicit correla- Martin Luther King, Jr.) and negative White exemplars
tions. From such data it is difficult to maintain a “sepa- (Timothy McVeigh, Jeffrey Dahmer) led to lower anti-
rate system” view of these two forms of attitude. How to Black bias (Dasgupta & Greenwald, 2001; see also Lowery
think about their association and dissociation will surely et al., 2001). White participants also exhibited less preju-
be sorted out in the future as new ways of dividing them dice toward Black targets when subliminally primed with
become available. pairings of Black-good and White-bad (Olson & Fazio,
2006), when participating in a session run by a Black exper-
imenter (Lowery et al., 2001), and when taking the IAT in
AUTOMATIC ATTITUDE MALLEABILITY the presence of a likable experimenter who was perceived
as holding egalitarian views (Sinclair, Lowery, Hardin, &
The dissociation between conscious and unconscious atti- Colangelo, 2005). Malleability is not limited to racial atti-
tudes is evident when one examines the literature on tudes; context also influences evaluations of everything
attitude change. Researchers working with explicit attitudes from cigarettes to Bill Clinton, Mike Tyson, chocolate, and
typically ask questions such as: What makes a message per- the beach (see Ferguson & Bargh, 2004; Ferguson, Bargh, &
suasive? What makes people resistant to persuasion? (see Nayak, 2005). Such work is difficult to conduct using
Albarracin & Vargas, this volume). Those studying implicit explicit measures because participants’ self-reported atti-
attitudes ask different questions: Can something that is tudes are quite sensitive to situational concerns.
inherently outside the purview of conscious awareness and The malleability of implicit attitudes extends to the neu-
control even be a candidate for thinking about change? ral level. Perceptual processing of fearful or threatening
If so, do the interventions differ from those that facilitate stimuli is associated with greater amygdala activation that
change? How do subtle changes in the environment influ- is attenuated by cognitive processing of these same stim-
ence automatic attitudes? The differences between these uli (Hariri, Mattay, Tessitore, Fera, & Weinberger, 2003).

CH10.indd 374 10/5/09 7:19:45 PM


Ideology 380

A series of shocking studies showed that outgroup members in a variety of ways, from the intuitive expectation that
perceived as cold and incompetent (e.g., the homeless) those who hold attitude X (“abortion is wrong”) are also
are processed differently in the medial prefrontal cortex, a likely to hold attitude Y (“my faith brings me great joy”) to
brain region heavily involved in social cognition, than are the constant clash of opinions that occur at the intersection
ingroup members (Harris & Fiske, 2006, 2007). Asking of the left versus right of the political spectrum.
participants to make individuating judgments about the This being the case, it may surprise readers to know that, in
targets (e.g., “Does this person like broccoli?”) dampened the 1960s, a decade when America seemed to be particularly
this effect, demonstrating that cognition can override auto- politicized, several prominent social scientists proclaimed that
matic neural responses. the “end of ideology” had come (Bell, 1960/2000; Converse,
One explanation for implicit attitude malleability comes 1964; Lipset, 1960; Shils, 1968). The basis for the claim
from those theorists who stress that measures of implicit came in the form of some good arguments and some flimsy
attitudes are not necessarily tapping only unconscious eval- ones: the minds of ordinary citizens (read: unsophisticated
uations (Conrey et al., 2005; Payne, Lambert, & Jacoby, types, unlike the elites) did not possess logically ordered
2002). In other words, the degree of possible cognitive con- attitudes; many Americans gave “I have no idea” answers to
trol in a particular social context should account for the flex- political questions, and when they did not, they seemed unable
ibility observed in such shifts. Another view sympathetic to to give accurate reasons for holding the attitudes. Attitudes
connectionist ways of reasoning accounts for the flexibility changed willy-nilly, including in response to the mere order
of implicit attitudes by thinking of them as large networks of of question presentation; little coherence existed between the
associative learning (based on direct experience, mass media, affective and cognitive components of attitude, and respon-
and significant others; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006; dents showed little consistency across attitudes that should be
Smith & DeCoster, 2000). Which aspects are activated related (McGuire, 1985).
depends on the match or goodness-of-fit between preexist- These arguments, and the evidence supporting them,
ing associations and the configuration of external inputs. formed the core of the position that ideology was overrated
Simple exposure or practiced associations between group and did not wield significant influence in most people’s
and attribute demonstrate the mechanism by which such mental lives. Some counterarguments were offered, how-
learning effects may occur, both in creating and in chang- ever. Situations, critics argued, have the power to moderate
ing attitudes. For example, simply seeing faces of one’s attitude expressions. And what if the measures are not sen-
own group may create negative outgroup attitudes. White sitive enough to reveal attitude consistencies? And (a radi-
participants who were exposed to White faces showed cal question!) what if the elites and the common folk are
more negative implicit and explicit attitude toward Blacks not so different, after all (Judd & Milburn, 1980; Milburn &
as compared with participants who did not see pictures of Judd, 1981)? Nevertheless, McGuire (1982) concluded his
faces. Though both implicit and explicit attitudes changed discussion about the situation by clearly siding with the
as a result of seeing White faces, the change in implicit atti- end-of-ideology position: “One can hope that these dialec-
tudes was somewhat weaker than the corresponding change tical confrontations of thesis with antithesis will arise to a
in the explicit ones (Smith, Dijksterhuis, & Chaiken, 2008). new and improved synthesis, though a puritanical observer
Though conscious and unconscious attitudes may change might wish that the workers would show more embarrass-
in the same direction, the magnitude of the change may not ment while waiting for it to emerge” (p. 90).
be equivalent. Such changes in intergroup attitudes can be Twenty-five years later, an answer emerged, less in the
influenced by ideology, the final topic of this chapter. form of a synthesis and more in the form of an out-and-out
challenge to the end-of-ideology position. Jost (2006), a
former student of McGuire’s, has argued that ideology is
IDEOLOGY alive and well, and very much a part of the lives of both
“ordinary” Americans and elites (see also Abramowitz &
If an attitude is a strand of feeling, then an ideology is a Saunders, 1998). Jost’s evidence comes from statistics
rope of intertwined attitudes and related fibers. Ideology about the number of individuals who regularly listen to
has been defined as “patterns or gestalts of attitudes” explicitly ideological talk radio and television shows, the
(Billig, 1984, p. 446), “an organization of opinions, atti- large number of American survey respondents (two thirds
tudes, and values—a way of thinking about man and soci- to three fourths) who identify themselves somewhere on the
ety” (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, liberal-conservative continuum, and the fact that ideologi-
1950, p. 2), and “an interrelated set of attitudes and values cal self-placement predicts many other attitudes, beliefs,
about the proper goals of society” (Tedin, 1987, p. 65). opinions, and behaviors (see also Jost, Federico, & Napier,
Expressions of attitudes indicate the presence of ideology 2009; Jost, Nosek, & Gosling, 2008).

CH10.indd 375 10/5/09 7:19:47 PM


381 Attitudes

Starting with these observations, Jost and colleagues racial bias (e.g., the IAT and explicit self-report scales;
have accumulated evidence revealing the presence of ide- Livingston, 2009).
ology in everyday life (see, for example, Jost, 2006, 2007; Though conservatives show a great deal of tolerance for
Jost, Federico, & Napier, 2009; Jost, Fitzsimons, & Kay, social inequality, they do not show the same level of accep-
2004; Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003a, 2003b, tance toward ambiguity and lack of structure. Liberals tend
Jost, Nosek, & Gosling, 2008). Through this work, they to be more open to new experiences (Jost, 2006) and novel
have also expounded on the left-right distinction and the stimulus items, including works of art (Wilson, 1973), for-
psychological differences that characterize the two attitu- eign films, and foreign travel. They engage in more cogni-
dinal positions. Given its novelty, Jost’s argument, as well tive complexity and have a lower need for closure than do
the broader accumulation of information about the network conservatives. And while liberals show higher preferences
of conscious and unconscious attitudes that constitute ide- for poetry (which is often ambiguous) and tattoos (which
ology, are worth examining. may reflect social change and the greater acceptability of
The simplest prediction states those who identify with modifying one’s body), conservatives show high opinions
left-wing ideology tend to adopt more liberal positions on of fraternities, sororities, sports utility vehicles, watching
specific issues, respond more favorably toward liberal can- television, and drinking alcohol (Jost et al., 2008).
didates, and vote for liberal politicians; the reverse should Perhaps stemming from a distaste of ambiguous situa-
be true for those on the right (e.g., Conover & Feldman, tions, conservatives tend to experience more fear of threat
1981; Jost, 2006; Kerlinger, 1984). The left and right ideo- and loss, a higher need for order, and a greater anxiety
logical stances are differentiated along two dimensions: concerning death than do many liberals (Jost et al., 2009).
those who identify as liberal tend to show less tolerance They are more likely to believe that the world is danger-
of inequality and more tolerance of change than those who ous (Duckitt, 2001), a belief that pairs with social confor-
identify as conservative (Jost, Fitzsimons, & Kay, 2004). mity to influence authoritarian attitudes. Authoritarianism,
The very definition of conservatism implies a desire to together with social dominance (Sidanius & Pratto, 2001),
keep things as they are, and indeed, liberals and conser- has been found to influence intergroup attitudes; thus, one’s
vatives think differently about modern social structures. political orientation can have a strong impact on one’s atti-
Individuals who see inequality as group based tend to iden- tudes and behaviors toward members of other groups
tify with their country’s more liberal party, whereas those (Duckitt, Wagner, du Plessis, & Birum, 2002). In line with
who see inequality as meritocracy based tend to identify this finding, conservatives tend to form more negative atti-
with more conservative ideologies (Jost, 2006; Napier & tudes toward people engaging in behavior they perceive to
Jost, 2008; Sibley & Wilson, 2007). be immoral (Haidt & Hersh, 2001). And, as expected from
The left-right divide also indicates a difference in ori- the very definition of what it means to be liberal and con-
entation toward the status quo, which right-leaning people servative, liberals tend to prefer social change more than
tend to support and left-leaning people tend to oppose (Jost their conservative counterparts (Jost et al., 2008).
et al., 2009). Conservatives are more likely to oppose poli- It is important to note that this research does not make
cies such as affirmative action, for example, and preju- claims about causality, which can be bidirectional. People
dice seems to play a large part in this stance despite the frequently form their political attitudes on the basis of
“principled” (race-neutral) objections conservatives say already-established beliefs about the social structure within
they are making (Federico & Sidanius, 2002; Sidanius, which they find themselves. The influence of attitudes can
Pratto, & Bobo, 1996). Rationalizations for inequality can go both ways, however; one’s attitudes toward inequality
partially account for the greater happiness of conserva- and the status quo can influence political affiliation, and
tives as compared with liberals; explaining inequality in political ideology can influence attitudes toward novel stim-
ways that do not challenge current social structures medi- ulus items. For example, liberals may favor foreign travel
ates the relationship between political orientation and because of their general openness to new experiences, and
subjective well-being, and increasing economic inequal- such travel likely provides the experiences that feed into the
ity widens the gap between liberals’ and conservatives’ further adoption of liberal attitudes.
happiness, perhaps because liberals are more troubled by Several scholars have taken exception to these broad
such disparity (Napier & Jost, 2008). Though conserva- characterizations of liberals and conservatives. Greenberg
tives sometimes argue that liberals are just oversensitive, and Jonas (2003) point out that extremism is not limited to
liberals are, in fact, more accurate at identifying preju- conservatives. However, liberal extremism is much less com-
dicial attitudes. When asked to determine behaviors that mon than extremism on the right side of the political scale.
are indicative of racism, liberals more accurately select Furthermore, though the exceptions pointed out by Greenberg
those that correlate highly with multiple measures of and Jonas—for example, political conservatives who appear

CH10.indd 376 10/5/09 7:19:48 PM


References 382

to be seeking change—do exist, they are exceptions that This chapter introduces work on the origins of attitudes
prove the general rule, and many examples of conservative for the first time. Research on nonhuman primates and
“change” are actually attempts to undo liberal changes (Jost, infants is just beginning, and the early returns indicate that
Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003a). it will teach us much about unique attitudes that charac-
Others have pointed out that conservatives may hold to terize human minds and that can grow only in complex
broader moral convictions than do liberals (e.g., Haidt & social environments while also supporting the idea of the
Graham, 2007). Liberals tend to explain their judgments of continuity of life. Research on young children has been
right and wrong by referencing autonomy, or the idea that surprising in revealing the presence of implicit intergroup
everyone has the right to do whatever they please as long as attitudes that are just as fully formed as in adults, even
they are not hurting anyone, and by the fairness/reciprocity though explicit attitudes vary across development. If the
principle, which states that it is right to treat others well. number of seemingly spontaneous symposia and confer-
Although conservatives also rely on these two ethics, three ences are an indication, research at the intersection of
others are also foundational to their thinking: ingroup/ social, cognitive, and developmental psychology will con-
loyalty (e.g., it is wrong to perform actions that offend tinue to generate interest and break down silly boundaries
one’s community), authority/respect, and purity/sanctity that currently separate areas of psychology.
(e.g., it is wrong to do things that are not in line with God’s The large remainder of the chapter focuses on the bread
plan for humans; Haidt & Hersh, 2001). and butter work of social psychologists who study attitudes,
Even more so than individual attitudes, the umbrella starting with the self, and looking outward to attitudes
status of ideology prompts the question of origins. Where toward social groups. Here the main lessons have been the
do particular orientations come from? Cultural influences dissociations between conscious and unconscious forms of
are an obvious input, as are early developmental influ- attitudes, as well as their malleability. That attitudes are
ences. For example, people who were highly reactive as not always consciously accessible or controllable, but nev-
4-month-old infants (e.g., those who exhibited strong ertheless guide behavior, has been a theme of much of the
emotional responses) tend to hold more positive attitudes work that is reviewed. Questions of how control may be
toward religion and less positive attitudes toward risk than asserted over attitudes that are consciously desired but out
others when they reached the age of 15 years. Low-reactive of reach and the ethics of changing less visible attitudes
infants, in contrast, grow into adolescents who hold favor- will require attention in the future. In this century, most of
able attitudes toward visiting new places and not worrying which lies ahead, psychologists have much to understand
excessively about unrealistic events (Kagan & Snidman, about the preferences people have, routinely acquire, and
2004). The highly influential constellation of attitudes that rely on to muddle through their ordinary lives. They will,
compromise ideology may originate in infancy and even no doubt, also speak about the extraordinary effects that
earlier, in the structures of one’s culture that were estab- even the most ordinary preferences create in shaping the
lished long before one’s birth. quality of lives. Indeed, what social psychologists have
learned about preferences has always been of general inter-
est because this simplest orientation to evaluate as good
SUMMARY and bad underlies so much of individual and group happi-
ness, safety, and progress.
Attitudes—the fundamental orientation to evaluate people,
other living beings, things, events, and ideas along a good-
bad dimension—have been studied with vigor through
much of the 20th century. This chapter focuses in some REFERENCES
depth on the question of measurement and the conceptual
issues it has raised in recent decades. It also attempted to Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J., Newcomb, T. M.,
Rosenberg, M. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1968). Theories of cognitive
show the rich and varied ways in which psychologists have consistency: A sourcebook. Chicago: Rand McNally.
probed attitudes, changing in the process the very way in Abrahamson, A. C., Baker, L. A., & Caspi, C. (2002). Rebellious teens?
which we understand this concept today. The most promi- Genetic and environmental influences on the social attitudes of adoles-
nent lessons from recent decades have been the benefit of cents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1392–1408.

a simpler definition, treating the concept alongside other Abramowitz, A. I., & Saunders, K. L. (1998). Ideological realignment in
the American electorate. Journal of Politics, 60, 634–652.
mental constructs especially memory, tapping its less con-
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N.
scious aspects, and allowing new methods to reveal its (1950). The authoritarian personality. Oxford: Harpers.
nature rather than abiding to a preconceived notion of what Agnew, C. R., van Lange, P. A. M., Rusbult, C. E., & Langston, C. A. (1998).
attitudes should be. Cognitive interdependence: Commitment and the mental representation

CH10.indd 377 10/5/09 7:19:48 PM


383 Attitudes

of close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayden, E., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2008).
74, 939–954. The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a response-
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1975). A Bayesian analysis of attribution pro- time and event-related-potentials methodology for testing natural ver-
cesses. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 261–277. bal relations. The Psychological Record, 58, 497–516.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit atti-
analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84, tudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood.
888–918. Psychological Science, 17, 53–58.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2009). Consequences of group member-
social behavior. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. ship on social categorization and inductive inference. Manuscript in
preparation.
Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (Eds.). (2005). The hand-
book of attitudes. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Barsalou, L. W. (2005). Continuity of the conceptual system across spe-
cies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 309–311.
Alexander, C., Piazza, M., Mekos, D., & Valente, T. (2001). Peers, schools,
and adolescent cigarette smoking. Journal of Adolescent Health, 29, Bartholow, B. D., & Amodio, D. M. (2008). Using event-related brain
22–30. potentials in socialpsychological research: A brief review and tutorial.
In E. Harmon-Jones & J. S. Beer (Eds.), Methods in the neurobiology
Alford, J. R., Funk, C. L., & Hibbing, J. R. (2005). Are political orienta-
of social and personality psychology (pp. 198–232). New York:
tions genetically transmitted? American Political Science Review, 99,
Guilford Press.
153–167.
Baumeister, R. F. (Ed.). (1993). Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard.
Alford, J. R., & Hibbing, J. R. (2007). Personal, interpersonal, and political
New York: Plenum Press.
temperaments. Annals of the American Academy of Political and
Social Science, 614, 196–212. Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened
egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem.
Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. A. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook
Psychological Review, 103, 5–33.
of social psychology (pp. 798–844). Worcester, MA: Clark University
Press. Bell, D. (1960/2000). The end of ideology: On the exhaustion of political
ideas in the fifties. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Anderson, N. H. (1971). Integration theory and attitude change.
Psychological Review, 78, 171–206. Bentler, P. M. (1980). Multivariate analysis with latent variables: Causal
modeling. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 419–456.
Anderson, N. H. (1981). Foundations of information integration theory.
St. Louis, MO: Academic Press. Berger, H. (1929). Uber das Elektrenkephalogramm das Menchen. Archiv
fur Psychiatrie, 87, 527–570.
Arcuri, L., Castelli, L., Galdi, S., Zogmaister, C. & Amadori, A. (2008).
Predicting the vote: Implicit attitudes as predictors of the future behavior Bessenoff, G. R., & Sherman, J. W. (2000). Automatic and controlled com-
of the decided and undecided voters. Political Psychology, 29, 369–387. ponents of prejudice toward fat people: Evaluation versus stereotype
activation. Social Cognition, 18, 329–353.
Baccus, J. R., Baldwin, M. W., & Packer, D. J. (2004). Increasing implicit
self-esteem through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 15, Bigler, R. S., Spears Brown, C., & Markell, M. (2001). When groups are
498–502. not created equal: Effects of group status on the formation of inter-
group attitudes in children. Child Development, 72, 1151–1162.
Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., & van den Bergh, O. (1990). Contingency awareness
in evaluative conditioning: A case for unaware affective-evaluative Billig, M. (1984). Political ideology: Social psychological aspects. In
learning. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 3–18. H. Tajfel (Ed.), The social dimension (Vol. 2, pp. 446–470). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitudes can be measured. In H. L. Roediger,
I. N. Nairne, & A. M. Suprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice.
Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 117–149). Washington, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 242–261.
DC: APA. Blanton, H., & Jaccard, J. (2006). Arbitrary metrics in psychology.
Banaji, M. R. (2003). The opposite of a great truth is also true. In J. Jost, American Psychologist, 61, 27–41.
D. Prentice, & M. R. Banaji (Eds.), The yin and yang of progress in Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Gonzalez, P. M., & Christie, C. (2006). Decoding
social psychology: Perspectivism at work (pp. 127–140). Washington, the implicit association test: Implications for criterion prediction.
DC: American Psychological Association. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 192–212.
Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2004). No place for Bogardus, E. S. (1925). Measuring social distances. Journal of Applied
nostalgia in science: A response to Arkes and Tetlock. Psychological Sociology, 9, 299–308.
Inquiry, 15, 279–310. Bogardus, E. S. (1931). Attitudes and the Mexican immigrant. In K. Young
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Oxford: Prentice-Hall. (Ed.), Social attitudes (pp. 291–327). New York: Henry Holt.
Bar-Anan, Y., Nosek, B. A., & Vianello, M. (2009). The sorting paired fea- Bosson, J. K., Swann, J. B., Jr., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking
tures task: A measure of association strengths. Experimental Psychology, the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the
56, 329–343. elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality 631–643.
of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and Bouchard, T., Segal, N., Tellegen, A., McGue, M., Keyes, M., & Krueger, R.
Social Psychology, 62, 893–912. (2003). Evidence for the construct validity and heritability of
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The auto- the Wilson-Patterson conservatism scale: A reared-apart twins study
matic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation of social attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 34,
with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 959–969.
32, 104–128. Bourgeois, M. J. (2002). Heritability of attitudes constrains dynamic social
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automatist of impact. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1063–1072.
being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. Brendl, C. M., Chattopadhyay, A., Pelham, B. W., & Carvallo, M. (2005).
Bar-Haim, Y., Ziv, T., Lamy, D., & Hodes, R. M. (2006). Nature and nurture Name letter branding: Valence transfers when product specific needs
in own-race face processing. Psychological Science, 17, 159–163. are active. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 405–415.

CH10.indd 378 10/5/09 7:19:48 PM


References 384

Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2001). How do indirect Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing
measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of
in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–766.
Psychology, 81, 760–773. Chaiken, S., & Bargh, J. A. (1993). Occurrence versus moderation of
Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2005). Indirectly measur- the automatic attitude activation effect: Reply to Fazio. Journal
ing evaluations of several attitude objects in relation to a neutral refer- of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 759–765.
ence point. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 346–368. Chaiken, S., & Eagly, A. H. (1983). Communication modality as a deter-
Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or out- minant of persuasion: The role of communicator salience. Journal of
group hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55, 429–444. Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 241–256.
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this ‘We’? Levels of col- Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic
lective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In
Social Psychology, 71, 83–93. J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–252).
Brinol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2005). Individual differences in attitude change. New York: Guilford Press.
In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social
of attitudes (pp. 575–615). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
Brown, J. D., & Dutton, K. A. (1995). The thrill of victory, the complexity Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The
of defeat: Self-esteem and people’s emotional reactions to success and perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality
failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 712–722. and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.
Brown, R. (1965). Social psychology. New York: Free Press. Cohen, G. L., & Prinstein, M. J. (2006). Peer contagion of aggression
Brunstein, J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2004). Assessing individual differences and health risk behavior among adolescent males: An experimental
in achievement motivation with the Implicit Association Test. Journal investigation of effects on public conduct and private attitudes. Child
of Research in Personality, 38, 536–555. Development, 77, 967–983.
Buhlmann, U., Teachman, B. A., Gerbershagen, A., Kikul, J., & Rief, W. Conover, S., & Feldman, P. J. (1981). The origins and meaning of liberal/
(2008). Implicit and explicit self-esteem and attractiveness beliefs conservative self-identifications. American Journal of Political
among individuals with body dysmorphic disorder. Cognitive Therapy Science, 25, 617–645.
and Research, 32, 213–225. Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & Groom, C. J.
Burkart, J. M., Fehr, E., Efferson, C., & van Schaik, C. P. (2007). Other- (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The
regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets quad model of implicit task preference. Journal of Personality and
provision food altruistically. Proceedings of the National Academy of Social Psychology, 89, 469–487.
Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 19762–19766. Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In
Cacioppo, J. T. (1982). Social psychophysiology: A classic perspective and D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent (pp. 206–261). London:
contemporary approach. Psychophysiology, 19, 241–251. Free Press of Glencoe.
Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., Berntson, G. G., & Coles, M. G. (1993). If Cooper, J. M. (2007). Cognitive dissonance: 50 years of a classical
attitudes affect how stimuli are processed, should they not affect the theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
event-related brain potential? Psychological Science, 4, 108–112. Craemer, T. (2007). An evolutionary model of racial attitude forma-
Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Attitudes to the tion: Socially shared and idiosyncratic racial attitudes. Annals of the
right: Evaluative processing is associated with lateralized late posi- American Academy of Political and Social Science, 614, 74–101.
tive event-related brain potentials. Personality and Social Psychology Crano, W. D., & Prislin, R. (2006). Attitudes and persuasion. Annual
Bulletin, 22, 1205–1219. Review of Psychology, 57, 345–374.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Losch, M. E., & Kim, H. S. (1986). Crelia, R. A., & Tesser, A. (1996). Attitude heritability and attitude rein-
Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate forcement: A replication. Personality and Individual Differences, 21,
the valence and intensity of affective reactions. Journal of Personality 803–808.
and Social Psychology, 50, 260–268. Crites, S. L., Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1995).
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorization: II. A late positive
determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differen- brain potential that varies as a function of attitude registration rather
tial effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, than attitude report. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
65, 5–17. 997–1013.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Sandman, C. A. (1978). Physiological differentiation Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-
of sensory and cognitive tasks as a function of warning, processing protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608–630.
demands, and reported unpleasantness. Biological Psychology, 6, Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2003). Seeking self-esteem: Construction, main-
181–192. tenance, and protection of self-worth. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney
Cacioppo, J. T., & Sandman, C. A. (1981). Psychophysiological function- (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 291–313). New York:
ing, cognitive responding, and attitudes. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & Guilford Press.
T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persuasion (pp. 81–104). Croizet, J. (1998). Unconscious perception of affective information
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and its impact on personality trait judgment. Current Psychology of
Campbell, D. T., Kruskal, W. H., & Wallace, W. P. (1966). Seating aggrega- Cognition, 17, 53–70.
tion as an index of attitude. Sociometry, 29, 1–15. Crosby, F., Bromley, S., & Saxe, L. (1980). Recent unobtrusive studies
Campbell, W. K., Bosson, J. K., Goheen, T. W., Lakey, C. E., & Kernis, M. H. of Black and White discrimination and prejudice: A literature review.
(2007). Do narcissists dislike themselves ‘deep down inside?’ Psychological Bulletin, 87, 546–563.
Psychological Science, 18, 227–229. Cunningham, W. A., Espinet, S. D., DeYoung, C. G., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005).
Carmen, I. (2007). Genetic configurations of political phenomena. Annals Attitudes to the right – and left: Frontal ERP asymmetries associated with
of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 614, 34–55. stimulus valence and processing goals. NeuroImage, 28, 8827–8834.

CH10.indd 379 10/5/09 7:19:50 PM


385 Attitudes

Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Diener, E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction
Banaji, M. R. (2003). Neural components of social evaluation. Journal and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 639–649. 653–663.
Cunningham, W. A., Kesek, A., & Mowrer, S. M. (in press). Distinct Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). I like myself but I don’t know why: Enhancing
orbito-frontal regions encode stimulus and choice valuation. Journal implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. Journal of
of Cognitive Neuroscience. Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 345–355.
Cunningham, W. A., Nezlek, J. B., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Implicit Dijksterhuis, A., Albers, L. W., & Bongers, K. C. A. (2008). Digging for
and explicit ethnocentrism: Revisiting the ideologies of prejudice. the real attitude: Lessons from research on implicit and explicit self-
Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1332–1346. esteem. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes:
Cunningham, W. A., Packer, D. J., Kesek, A., & van Bavel, J. J. (2008). Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 229–250). New York:
Implicit measures of attitudes: A physiological approach. In R. E. Petty, Psychology Press.
R. H. Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial
measures (pp. 485–512). New York: Psychology Press. reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11,
Cunningham, W. A., Raye, C. L., & Johnson, M. K. (2004). Implicit and 86–89.
explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional inten- Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good.
sity, and control in the processing of attitudes. Journal of Cognitive Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285–290.
Neuroscience, 16, 1717–1729. Di Paula, A., & Campbell, J. D. (2002). Self-esteem and persistence in
Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations: the face of failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
A social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive 711–724.
Sciences, 11, 97–104. Dovidio, J. F., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). New technologies for the direct and
Cunningham, W. A., Zelazo, P. D., Packer, D. J., & van Bavel, J. J. (2007). indirect assessment of attitudes. In J. M. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about
The iterative reprocessing model: A multi-level framework for attitudes questions: Inquiries into the cognitive bases of surveys (pp. 204–237).
and evaluations. Social Cognition, 25, 736–760. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Dasgupta, N. (2009). Mechanisms underlying the malleability of implicit Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2004). Aversive racism. In M. P. Zanna
prejudice and stereotypes: The role of automaticity and cognitive con- (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1–52). San
trol. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of stereotyping, prejudice, and Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.
discrimination (pp. 267–284). New York: Psychology Press. Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and
Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality
attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and and Social Psychology, 82, 62–68.
disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Smoak, N., & Gaertner, S. L. (2008).
81, 800–814. The nature of contemporary racial prejudice: Insights from implicit
Dasgupta, N., McGhee, D. E., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). and explicit measures of attitudes. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, &
Automatic preference for White Americans: Eliminating the familiarity P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures
explanation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 316–328. (pp. 165–192). New York: Psychology Press.
Davis, M. (1997). Neurobiology of fear responses: The role of the amygdala. Dovidio, J. R., Evans, N., & Tyler, R. B. (1986). Racial stereotypes: The
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurology, 9, 382–402. contents of their cognitive representations. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 22, 22–37.
Davis, M., & Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion.
Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 13–34. Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideol-
ogy and prejudice. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
DeCasper, A. J., & Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influ-
social psychology (pp. 41–113). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
ences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior &
Development, 9, 133–150. Duckitt, J., Wagner, C., du Plessis, I., & Birum, I. (2002). The psycho-
logical bases of ideology and prejudice: Testing a dual process model.
de Hart, T., Pelham, B. W., & Tennen, H. (2006). What lies beneath:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 75–93.
Parenting style and implicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 42, 1–17. Duckworth, K. L., Bargh, J. A., Garcia, M., & Chaiken, S. (2002). The auto-
matic evaluation of novel stimuli. Psychological Science, 13, 513–519.
de Houwer, J. (2003). The extrinsic affective Simon task. Experimental
Psychology, 50, 77–85. Dunham, Y., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). Invariance of intergroup attitudes
across the lifespan. Unpublished manuscript, Harvard University.
de Houwer, J. (2006). Using the implicit association test does not rule out
an impact of conscious propositional knowledge on evaluative condi- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). From American city
tioning. Learning and Motivation, 37, 176–187. to Japanese village: A cross-cultural investigation of implicit race atti-
tudes. Child Development, 77, 1268–1281.
de Houwer, J. (2008). Comparing measures of attitudes at the functional and
procedural level: Analysis and implications. In R. Petty, R. H. Fazio, Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The development of
& P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 248–253.
(pp. 361–390). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Orlando,
de Raedt, R., Schacht, R., Franck, E., & de Houwer, J. (2006). Self-esteem FL: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
and depression revisited: Implicit positive self-esteem in depressed Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1998). Attitude structure and function. In
patients? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1017–1028. D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and con- psychology (pp. 269–322). New York: McGraw-Hill.
trolled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Eaves, L. J., Eysenck, H. J., & Martin, N. G. (1989). Genes, culture and
56, 5–18. personality: An empirical approach. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? Egan, L. C., Santos, L. R., & Bloom, P. (2007). The origins of cognitive
The Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology dissonance: Evidence from children and monkeys. Psychological
Bulletin, 21, 1139–1150. Science, 18, 978–983.

CH10.indd 380 10/5/09 7:19:51 PM


References 386

Eriksen, C. W., & Schultz, D. W. (1979). Information processing in Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often
visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively fol-
Perception & Psychophysics, 25, 249–263. low from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and
Fabrigar, L. R., Petty, R. E., Smith, S. M., & Crites, S. L., Jr. (2006). Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Understanding knowledge effects on attitude-behavior consistency: Franck, E., de Raedt, R., & de Houwer, J. (2007). Implicit but not explicit
The role of relevance, complexity, and amount of knowledge. Journal self-esteem predicts future depressive symptomatology. Behaviour
of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 556–577. Research and Therapy, 45, 2448–2455.
Farnham, S. D., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1999). Implicit self- Galdi, S., Arcuri, L., & Gawronski, B. (2008). Automatic mental associa-
esteem. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity and social tions predict future choices of undecided decision-makers. Science,
cognition (pp. 230–248). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. 321, 1100–1102.
Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: Gaertner, S., & Bickman, L. (1971). Effects of race on the elicitation of
The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), helping behavior: The wrong number technique. Journal of Personality
Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 75–109). New York: and Social Psychology, 20, 218–222.
Academic Press.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In
Fazio, R. H. (2000). Accessible attitudes as tools for object appraisal: Their J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and
costs and benefits. In G. Maio & J. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: racism (pp. 61–89). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Functions of attitudes (pp. 1–36). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gaertner, S. L., & McLaughlin, J. P. (1983). Racial stereotypes:
Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations of varying
Associations and ascriptions of positive and negative characteristics.
strength. Social Cognition, 25, 603–637.
Social Psychology Quarterly, 46, 23–30.
Fazio, R. H., Chen, J., McDonel, E. C., & Sherman, S. J. (1982). Attitude
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). ‘I’ value freedom, but
accessibility, attitude-behavior consistency, and the strength of
‘we’ value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differ-
the object-evaluation association. Journal of Experimental Social
ences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10, 321–326.
Psychology, 18, 339–357.
Fazio, R. H., & Dunton, B. C. (1997). Categorization by race: The impact Gawronski, B. (2007). Social cognition (special issue: What is an atti-
of automatic and controlled components of racial prejudice. Journal of tude?). New York: Guilford Press.
Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 451–470. Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and proposi-
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). tional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and
Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731.
attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2007). Unraveling the process
Psychology, 69, 1013–1027. underlying evaluation: Attitudes from the perspective of the APE
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition model. Social Cognition, 25, 687–717.
research: Their meaning and uses. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist,
297–327. 46, 107–119.
Fazio, R. H., Powell, M. C., & Herr, P. M. (1983). Toward a process model Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, S. T., & Lindzey, G. (1998). The handbook of social
of the attitude-behavior relation: Accessing one’s attitude upon mere psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
observation of the attitude object. Journal of Personality and Social
Giner-Sorolla, R., Garcia, M. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The automatic
Psychology, 44, 723–735.
evaluation of pictures. Social Cognition, 17, 76–96.
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986).
Glaser, J. (1999). The relationship between stereotyping and prejudice:
On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and
Measure of newly formed automatic associations. Unpublished doc-
Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.
toral dissertation, Yale University; New Haven, CT.
Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1981). Direct experience and attitude-
behavior consistency. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we
social psychology (pp. 161–202). New York: Academic Press. trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions
about internet questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59, 93–104.
Federico, C. M., & Sidanius, J. (2002). Racism, ideology, and affirma-
tive action revisited: The antecedents and consequences of ‘principled Gramzow, R. H., & Gaertner, L. (2005). Self-esteem and favoritism toward
objections’ to affirmative action. Journal of Personality and Social novel in-groups: The self as an evaluative base. Journal of Personality
Psychology, 82, 488–502. and Social Psychology, 88, 801–815.
Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. (1997). Prejudice as self-image maintenance: Gray, N. S., MacCulloch, M. J., Smith, J., Morris, M., & Snowden, R. J.
Affirming the self through derogating others. Journal of Personality (2003). Violence viewed by psychopathic murders: Adapting a reveal-
and Social Psychology, 73, 31–44. ing test may expose those psychopaths who are most likely to kill.
Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects Nature, 423, 497–498.
of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and Gray-Little, B., & Hafdahl, A. R. (2000). Factors influencing racial com-
Social Psychology, 87, 557–572. parisons of self-esteem: A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin,
Ferguson, M. J., Bargh, J. A., & Nayak, D. A. (2005). After-affects: How 126, 26–54.
automatic evaluations influence the interpretation of subsequent, unre- Green, B. F. (1954). Attitude measurement. In G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook
lated stimuli. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 182–191. of social psychology. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Palo Alto, Greenberg, J., & Jonas, E. (2003). Psychological motives and political
CA: Stanford University Press. orientation—The left, the right, and the rigid: Comment on Jost et al.
Finch, J. F., & Cialdini, R. B. (1989). Another indirect tactic of (self-) (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 376–382.
image management: Boosting. Personality and Social Psychology Greenwald, A. G. (2004). The resting parrot, the dessert stomach,
Bulletin, 15, 222–232. and other perfectly defensible theories. In J. Jost, M. R. Banaji, &
Fishbein, M. (1963). An investigation of the relationship between beliefs D. A. Prentice (Eds.), The yin and yang of social cognition: Perspectives
about an object and the attitude toward that object. Human Relations, on the social psychology of thought systems (pp. 275–285). Washington,
16, 233–239. DC: American Psychological Association.

CH10.indd 381 10/5/09 7:19:52 PM


387 Attitudes

Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, Harmon-Jones, E. (1999). Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal
self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27. theory in social psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Association.
Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit atti- Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the
tudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological
109, 3–25. Science, 17, 847–853.
Greenwald, A. G., Brock, T. C., & Ostrom, T. M. (Eds.). (1968). Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2007). Social groups that elicit disgust are
Psychological foundations of attitudes. New York: Academic Press. differentially processed in mPFC. Social Cognitive and Affective
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Neuroscience, 2, 45–51.
Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality Hart, A. J., Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., &
and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038. Rauch, S. L. (2000). Differential response in the human amygdala to
Greenwald, A. G., Klinger, M. R., & Liu, T. J. (1989). Unconscious process- racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. Neuroreport, 11, 2351–2355.
ing of dichoptically masked words. Memory & Cognition, 17, 35–47. Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2005). The new unconscious.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring New York: Oxford University Press.
individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association Hayes, T. C., & Lee, M. R. (2005). The southern culture of honor and vio-
test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. lent attitudes. Sociological Spectrum, 25, 593–617.
Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Attitudinal dissociation: What Hess, U. (2008). Facial EMG. In E. Harmon-Jones & J. Beer (Eds.),
does it mean? In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Attitudes: Methods in the neurobiology of social and personality psychology
Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 65–82). Hillsdale, NJ: (pp. 70–91). New York: Guilford Press.
Lawrence Erlbaum. Hetts, J. J., Sakuma, M., & Pelham, B. W. (1999). Two roads to positive
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding regard: Implicit and explicit self-evaluation and culture. Journal of
and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algo- Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 512–559.
rithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216. Himmelfarb, S. (1993). The measurement of attitudes. In A. H. Eagly &
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Klauer, K. C. (2005). S. Chaiken (Eds.), The psychology of attitudes (pp. 23–88). Orlando,
Validity of the Salience Asymmetry Interpretation of the IAT: Comment FL: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
on Rothermund and Wentura (2004). Journal of Experimental Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M.
Psychology: General, 134(3), 420–425. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Sriram, N. (2006). Consequential valid- Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and
ity of the Implicit Association Test: Comment on the article by Blanton Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385.
and Jaccard. American Psychologist, 61(1), 56–61. Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Nosek, B. A., & Schmitt, M. (2005). What
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). moderates implicit-explicit consistency? European Review of Social
Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta- Psychology, 16, 335–390.
analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Hovland, C. I., Lumsdaine, A. A., & Sheffield, F. D. (1949). Experiments
Psychology, 97, 17–41. on mass communication. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Greenwald, A. G., Rudman, L. A., Nosek, B. A., & Zayas, V. (2006). Why Ito, T. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Attitudes as mental and neural states
so little faith? A reply to Blanton and Jaccard’s (2006) skeptical view of readiness: Using physiological measures to study implicit attitudes.
of testing pure multiplicative theories. Psychological Review, 113, In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes
170–180. (pp. 125–158). New York: Guilford Press.
Gregg, A. P. (2000). The hare and the tortoise: The origins and dynamics Jacoby, L. L. (1993). Unintended influences of perception and mem-
of explicit and implicit attitudes. Doctoral dissertation, Yale University; ory: Attention, awareness, and control. Canadian Psychology, 34,
New Haven, CT. 156–157.
Griskevicius, V., Goldstein, N. J., Mortensen, C. R., Cialdini, R. B., & Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., Lindsay, D. S., & Debner, J. A. (1992). Lectures
Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Going along versus going alone: When fun- for a layperson: Methods for revealing unconscious processes. In
damental motives facilitate strategic (non)conformity. Journal of R. F. Bornstein & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Perception without awareness
Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 281–294. (pp. 81–120). New York: Guilford Press.
Guttman, L. (1941). The quantification of a class of attributes: A theory and Janiszewski, C. (1988). Preconscious processing effects: The independence
method of scale construction. In P. Horst (Ed.), The prediction of personal of attitude formation and conscious thought. Journal of Consumer
adjustment (pp. 319–348). New York: Social Science Research Council. Research, 15, 199–209.
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of
have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 172–194.
Research, 20, 98–116. Johnson, B. T., Maio, G. R., & Smith-McLallen, A. (2005). Communication
Haidt, J., & Hersh, M. A. (2001). Sexual morality: The cultures and and attitude change: Causes, processes, and effects. In D. Albarracin,
emotions of conservatives and liberals. Journal of Applied Social B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes
Psychology, 31, 191–221. (pp. 617–669). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hamilton, D. L., Gibbons, P. A., Stroessner, S. J., & Sherman, J. W. (1992). Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’
Stereotypes and language use. In G. R. Semin & K. Fiedler (Eds.), preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline.
Language, interaction, and social cognition (pp. 102–128). Thousand Cognition, 40, 1–19.
Oaks, CA: Sage. Jones, E. E., & Sigall, H. (1971). The bogus pipeline: A new paradigm for
Hargreaves, D. A., & Tiggemann, M. (2003). Female ‘thin ideal’ media measuring affect and attitude. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 349–364.
images and boys’ attitudes toward girls. Sex Roles, 49, 539–544. Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Carvallo, M., & Mirenberg, M. C. (2004).
Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D. R. How do I love thee? Let me count the Js: Implicit egotism and inter-
(2003). Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful personal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87,
stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 494–501. 665–683.

CH10.indd 382 10/5/09 7:19:53 PM


References 388

Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, Kelman, H. C. (1958). Compliance, identification, and internalization:
61, 651–670. Three processes of attitude change. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2,
Jost, J. T. (2007). Coda—after ‘The end of the end of ideology.’ American 51–60.
Psychologist, 62, 1077–1080. Kelman, H. C. (1974). Attitudes are alive and well and gainfully employed
Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system jus- in the sphere of action. American Psychologist, 29, 310–324.
tification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious Kerlinger, F. N. (1984). Liberalism and conservatism: The nature and
bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25, 881–920. structure of social attitudes. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of
Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
Psychology, 60, 307–337. the United States of America, 104, 12577–12580.
Jost, J. T., Fitzsimons, G., & Kay, A. C. (2004). The ideological animal: Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., DeJesus, J., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Accent
A system justification view. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & trumps race in guiding children’s social preferences. Submitted for
T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of experimental existential psychol- publication.
ogy (pp. 263–283). New York: Guilford Press. Koole, S. L., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2001). What’s
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003a). Exceptions in a name: Implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of
that prove the rule—using a theory of motivated social cognition to Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 669–685.
account for ideological incongruities and political anomalies: Reply to Kraut, R., Olson, J., Banaji, M. R., Bruckman, A., Cohen, J., & Couper, M.
Greenberg and Jonas (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 383–393. (2004). Psychological research online: Report of board of scientific
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003b). Political affairs’ advisory group on the conduct of research on the Internet.
conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, American Psychologist, 59, 105–117.
129, 339–375. Krech, D., & Crutchfield, R. S. (1948). Theory and problems of social
Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its resur- psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
gence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspectives on Krizan, Z., & Suls, J. (2008). Are implicit and explicit measures of self-
Psychological Science, 3, 126–136. esteem related? A meta-analysis for the Name-Letter test. Personality
Jost, J. T., Rudman, L. A., Blair, I. V., Carney, D., Dasgupta, N., Glaser, J., and Individual Differences, 44, 521–531.
et al. (in press). The existence of implicit bias is beyond reasonable Krosnick, J. A., & Fabrigar, L. R. (in press). The handbook of question-
doubt: A refutation of ideological and methodological objections and naire design. New York: Oxford University Press.
executive summary of ten studies that no manager should ignore.
Krosnick, J. A., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2005). Attitude mea-
Research in Organizational Behavior.
surement. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The
Judd, C. M., & Kulik, J. A. (1980). Schematic effects of social attitudes on handbook of attitudes and attitude change (pp. 21–78). Mahwah, NJ:
information processing and recall. Journal of Personality and Social Lawrence Erlbaum.
Psychology, 38, 569–578.
Kruglanski, A. W., & Stroebe, W. (2005). The influence of beliefs and
Judd, C. M., & Milburn, M. A. (1980). The structure of attitude systems goals on attitudes: Issues of structure, function, and dynamics. In
in the general public: Comparisons of a structural equation model. D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of
American Sociological Review, 45, 627–644. attitudes (pp. 323–368). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluation Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2007).
traits—self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: IV. What we
emotional stability—with job satisfaction and job performance: know (so far) about the method. In B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz
A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 80–92. (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 59–102). New York:
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament. Guilford Press.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. LaPiere, R. T. (1934). Attitudes vs. actions. Social Forces, 13, 230–237.
Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Lavine, H., Thomsen, C. J., Zanna, M. P., & Borgida, E. (1998). On the
Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 774–788. primacy of affect in the determination of attitudes and behavior:
Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The Single Category Implicit The moderating role of affective-cognitive ambivalence. Journal of
Association Test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 398–421.
Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16–32. Leary, M. R. (1999). Making sense of self-esteem. Current Direction in
Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Public Psychological Science, 8, 32–35.
Opinion Quarterly, 24, 163–204. Leary, M. R. (2004). The self we know and the self we show: self-esteem,
Katz, D., & Braly, K. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one hundred college stu- self-presentation, and the maintenance of interpersonal relationships.
dents. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28, 280–290. In M. B. Brewer & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Emotion and motivation
Kawakami, K., Phills, C. E., Steele, J. R., & Dovidio, J. F. (2007). (Close) (pp. 204–224). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
distance makes the heart grow fonder: Improving implicit racial atti- LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpin-
tudes and interracial interactions through approach behaviors. Journal nings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Shuster.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 957–971. Levina, M., Waldo, C. R., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2000). We’re here, we’re queer,
Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Ge, L., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., et al. (2007a). we’re on TV: The effects of visual media on heterosexuals’ attitudes
Cross-race preferences for same-race faces extend beyond the African toward gay men and lesbians. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
versus Caucasian contrast in 3-month-old infants. Infancy, 11, 87–95. 30, 738–758.
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. Lewontin, R. (2002). The triple helix: Gene, organism, and environment.
(2007b). The other-race effect develops during infancy: Evidence of Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
perceptual narrowing. Psychological Science, 18, 1084–1089. Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflexion
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Gibson, A., Smith, M., and reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attribu-
et al. (2005). Three-month-olds, but not newborns, prefer own-race tional inference. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
faces. Developmental Science, 8, F31–F36. psychology (pp. 199–249). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

CH10.indd 383 10/5/09 7:19:56 PM


389 Attitudes

Likert, R. (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Archives Mendes, W. B. (2008). Assessing autonomic nervous system reactivity. In
of Psychology, 140, 1–55. E. Harmon-Jones & J. Beer (Eds.), Methods in the neurobiology of
Likowski, K. U., Muhlberger, A., Seibt, B., Pauli, P., & Weyers, P. (2008). social and personality psychology (pp. 118–147). New York: Guilford
Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes. Journal of Experimental Press.
Social Psychology, 44, 1065–1072. Mendes, W. B., Blascovich, J., Hunter, S. B., Lickel, B., & Jost, J. T.
Lindzey, G. (Ed.). (1954). Handbook of social psychology. Cambridge, (2007). Threatened by the unexpected: Physiological responses dur-
MA: Addison- Wesley. ing social interactions with expectancy-violating partners. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 698–716.
Lindzey, G., & Aronson, E. (Eds.). (1969). The handbook of social psy-
chology. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing
pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations.
Lindzey, G., & Aronson, E. (Eds.). (1985). The handbook of social psy-
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234.
chology. New York: Random House.
Lipset, S. M. (1960). Political man: The social bases of politics. Baltimore, Milburn, M. A., & Judd, C. M. (1981). Interpreting new methods in attitude
MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. structure research. American Sociological Review, 46, 675–677.

Livingston, R. W. (2009, February). What is “prejudice?” Assessing Milgram, S., Mann, L., & Harter, S. (1965). The lost-letter technique:
the accuracy and implications of folk notions of racism. Paper pre- A tool of social research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 29(3), 437.
sented at the annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. Hoboken, NJ: John
Psychology; Tampa, FL. Wiley & Sons.
Lord, C. G., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Attitude representation theory. In Mitchell, J. P. (2009). Social psychology as a natural kind. Manuscript
M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology submitted for publication.
(pp. 265–343). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Mitchell, J. P., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Contextual varia-
Lord, C. G., Lepper, M. R., & Mackie, D. (2008). Attitude prototypes tions in implicit evaluations. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
as determinants of attitude-behavior consistency. In R. H. Fazio & 132, 455–469.
R. E. Petty (Eds.), Attitudes: Their structure, function, and conse- Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Field, M., & de Houwer, J. (2003). Eye move-
quences (pp. 411–424). New York, NY: Psychology Press. ments to smoking-related pictures in smokers: Relationship between
Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence effects attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus
on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social valence. Addiction, 98, 825–836.
Psychology, 81, 842–855. Mogg, K., Mathews, A., & Weinman, J. (1989). Selective processing of
Lydon, J. E., & Zanna, M. P. (1990). Commitment in the face of adver- threat cues in anxiety states: A replication. Behaviour Research and
sity: A value-affirmation approach. Journal of Personality and Social Therapy, 27, 317–323.
Psychology, 58, 1040–1047. Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. L., Budreau, D. R., Maurer, D., Dannemiller, J. L.,
Madon, S., Guyll, M., Aboufadel, K., Montiel, E., Smith, A., Palumbo, P., Stephens, B. R., et al. (1999). Face perception during early infancy.
et al. (2001). Ethnic and national stereotypes: The Princeton trilogy revisited Psychological Science, 10, 419–422.
and revised. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 996–1010. Morris, J. P., Squires, N. K., Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2003). Activation
Mahajan, N., Martinez, M., Diesendruck, G., Banaji, M. R., & Santos, L. R. of political attitudes: A psychophysiological examination of the hot
(2009). The evolution of ingroup bias: Experiments on group biases in rhe- cognition hypothesis. Political Psychology, 24, 727–745.
sus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Unpublished manuscript, Yale University. Morris, J. S., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious
Maio, G. R., & Olson, J. M. (1995). Relations between values, attitudes, emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393, 467–470.
and behavioral intentions: The moderating role of attitude function. Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem reticular forma-
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, 266–285. tion and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalography & Clinical
Maio, G. R., & Olson, J. M. (2000). Why we evaluate: Functions of atti- Neurophysiology, 1, 455–473.
tudes. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Murchison, C. (1935). A handbook of social psychology. Worcester, MA:
Maison, D., Greenwald, A. G., & Bruin, R. H. (2004). Predictive validity of Clark University Press.
the Implicit Association Test in studies of brands, consumer attitudes, Murray, S. L., Griffin, D. W., Rose, P., & Bellavia, G. (2006). For better or
and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 405–415. worse? Self-esteem and the contingencies of acceptance in marriage.
Major, B., Kaiser, C. R., & McCoy, S. K. (2003). It’s not my fault: When Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 866–880.
and why attributions to prejudice protect self-esteem. Personality and Murray, S. L., Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1996). On creating value-
Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 772–781. expressive attitudes: An experimental approach. In C. Seligman,
McDougall, W. (1933). The nature of emotion. The Journal of Abnormal J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The psychology of values (pp.
and Social Psychology, 28, 245–258. 107–133). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
McGuire, W. J. (1960). Cognitive consistency and attitude change. Journal Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (2000). Self-esteem and the
of Abnormal Social Psychology, 60, 345–353. quest for felt security: How perceived regard regulates attachment pro-
McGuire, W. J. (1961). Resistance to persuasion conferred by active and cesses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 478–498.
passive prior refutation of the same and alternative counterarguments. Napier, J. L., & Jost, J. T. (2008). Why are conservatives happier than
Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 63, 326–332. liberals? Psychological Science, 19, 565–572.
McGuire, W. J. (1968). The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory:
G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity atten-
(pp. 136–314). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. tion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.
McGuire, W. J. (1982). Attitudes and attitude change. Unpublished manu- Neyer, F. J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2001). Personality-relationship transaction
script, Yale University. Permission granted by Claire McGuire. in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
McGuire, W. J. (1985). The nature of attitude and attitude change. In 1190–1204.
G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
(pp. 233–346). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

CH10.indd 384 10/5/09 7:19:56 PM


References 390

Nock, M. K., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). Assessment of self-injurious Attitude structure and function (pp. 11–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
thoughts using a behavioral test. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, Erlbaum Associates.
820–823. Ottaway, S. A., Hayden, D. C., & Oakes, M. A. (2001). Implicit attitudes
Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and and racism: Effects of word familiarity and frequency on the implicit
explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, association test. Social Cognition, 19, 97–144.
134, 565–584. Otten, S., & Wentura, D. (1999). About the impact of automaticity in the
Nosek, B. A. (2007). Implicit-explicit relations. Current Directions in Minimal Group Paradigm: Evidence from affective priming tasks.
Psychological Science, 16, 65–69. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 1049–1071.
Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The Go/No-go Association Task. Pascalis, O., de Schonen, S., Morton, J., Deruelle, C., & Fabre-Grenet, M.
Social Cognition, 19, 625–666. (1995). Mother ’s face recognition in neonates: A replication and an
Nosek, B. A., & Hansen, J. J. (2008). The associations in our heads belong extension. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 79–85.
to us: Searching for attitudes and knowledge in implicit evaluation. Pascalis, O., & Slater, A. (2003). The development of face processing in
Cognition & Emotion, 22, 553–594. infancy and early childhood: Current perspectives. Infant and Child
Nosek, B. A., & Smyth, F. L. (2007). A multitrait-multimethod valida- Development, 10, 1–2.
tion of the Implicit Association Test: Implicit and explicit attitudes are Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. New York: Oxford University Press.
related but distinct constructs. Experimental Psychology, 54, 14–29. Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. L., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality
Ranganath, K. A., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192.
attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, Payne, B. K., Burkley, M. A., & Stokes, M. B. (2008). Why do implicit
36–88. and explicit attitude tests diverge? The role of structural fit. Journal of
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala, A., Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 16–31.
Bar-Anan, Y., Bergh, R., et. al, (in press). National differences in Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. (2005). An inkblot
gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in sci- for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of
ence and math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293.
of Sciences.
Payne, B. K., Lambert, A. J., & Jacoby, L. L. (2002). Best laid plans:
Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: The name Effects of goals on accessibility bias and cognitive control in race-
letter effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 353–361. based misperceptions of weapons. Journal of Experimental Social
Olson, J. M., Goffin, R. D., & Haynes, G. A. (2007). Relative versus Psychology, 38, 384–396.
absolute measures of explicit attitudes: Implications for predicting Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic
diverse attitude-relevant criteria. Journal of Personality and Social and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: Westview Press.
Psychology, 93, 907–926.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on
Olson, J. M., Vernon, P. A., Harris, J. A., & Jang, K. L. (2001). The herita- responses to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral
bility of attitudes: A study of twins. Journal of Personality and Social routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology, 80, 845–860. 46, 69–81.
Olson, K. R., & Dweck, C. S. (2008). A blueprint for social cognitive Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persua-
development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 193–202. sion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York:
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2001). Implicit attitude formation through Springer-Verlag.
classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 12, 413–417. Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H., & Brinol, P. (Eds.). (2008). Attitudes: Insights
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extraper- from the new implicit measures. New York: Psychology Press.
sonal associations on the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.). (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents
IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 653–667. and consequences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). Reducing automatically activated Petty, R. E., Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. C. (Eds.). (1981). Cognitive
racial prejudice through implicit evaluative conditioning. Personality responses in persuasion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 421–433. Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Matching versus mismatching
Olsson, A., Ebert, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). The role of attitude functions: Implications for scrutiny of persuasive messages.
social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science, 309, 785–787. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 227–240.
Olsson, A., Nearing, K. I., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Learning fears by Petty, R. E., Wegener, D. T., & Fabrigar, L. R. (1997). Attitudes and atti-
observing others: The neural systems of social fear transmission. Social tude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 609–647.
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 3–11. Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E.,
Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). Learned fear of ‘unseen’ faces after Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., et al. (2000). Performance on indirect
Pavlovian, observational, and instructed fear. Psychological Science, measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of
15, 822–828. Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738.
Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Roberts, B. W. (2008). Low self-esteem pro- Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature.
spectively predicts depression in adolescence and young adulthood. New York: Viking.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 695–708. Pizzagalli, D., Koenig, T., Regard, M., & Lehmann, D. (1999). Rapid emo-
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement tional face processing in the human right and left brain hemispheres:
of meaning. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press. An ERP study. Neuroreport, 10, 2691–2698.
Ostafin, B. D., & Palfai, T. P. (2006). Compelled to consume: The Implicit Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In
Association Test and automatic alcohol motivation. Psychology of R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and Cognition: The Loyola
Addictive Behaviors, 20, 322–327. Symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ostrom, T. M. (1989). Interdependence of attitude theory and measure- Poteat, V. P. (2007). Peer group socialization of homophobic attitudes and
ment. In A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), behavior during adolescence. Child Development, 78, 1830–1842.

CH10.indd 385 10/5/09 7:19:57 PM


391 Attitudes

Pratkanis, A. R., Breckler, S. J., & Greenwald, A. G. (Eds.). (1989). Attitude in experimental social psychology (pp. 173–220). New York: Academic
structure and function. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Press.
Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention- Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the
grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality Implicit Association Test: Dissociating salience from associations.
and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 139–165.
Pratto, F., & Shih, M. (2000). Social dominance orientation and group con- Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., Mellott, D. S., & Schwartz, J. L. K.
text in implicit group prejudice. Psychological Science, 11, 515–518. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: Flexibility
Prislin, R., & Wood, W. (2005). Social influence in attitudes and attitude and generality of the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition, 17,
change. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The hand- 437–465.
book of attitudes (pp. 671–705). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sahar, G., & Karasawa, K. (2005). Is the personal always political?
Quinn, P. C., Kelly, D. J., Lee, K., Pascalis, O., & Slater, A. M. (2008). A cross-cultural analysis of abortion attitudes. Basic and Applied
Preference for attractive faces in human infants extends beyond con- Social Psychology, 27, 285–296.
specifics. Developmental Science, 11, 76–83. Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002).
Cognition, 13, 501–518.
Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference
for female. Perception, 31, 1109-1121. Raikkonen, K., Katainen, S., Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man
Keskivaara, P., & Kelikangas-Jarvinen, L. (2000). Temperament, be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits
mothering, and hostile attitudes: A 12-year-long longitudinal study. across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 3–12. 168–182.
Ramsey, J. L., Langlois, J. H., Hoss, R. A., Rubenstein, A. J., & Griffin, A. M. Schnabel, K., Banse, R., & Asendorpf, J. (2006). Employing automatic
(2004). Origins of a stereotype: Categorization of facial attractiveness approach and avoidance tendencies for the assessment of implicit
by 6-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 7, 201–211. personality self-concept: The Implicit Association Procedure (IAP).
Experimental Psychology, 53, 69–76.
Rankin, R. E., & Campbell, D. T. (1955). Galvanic skin response to Negro
and white experimenters. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Schuman, H., Steeh, C., Bobo, L., & Krysan, M. (1997). Racial attitudes
51, 30–33. in America: Trends and interpretations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Rapaport, M. H., Clary, C., Fayyad, R., & Endicott, J. (2005). Quality-of-
life impairment in depressive and anxiety disorders. American Journal Schwartz, S. H., & Rubel, T. (2005). Sex differences in value priorities:
of Psychiatry, 162, 1171–1178. Cross-cultural and multi-method studies. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 89, 1010–1028.
Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual flu-
ency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9, 45–48. Schwarz, N. (1996). Cognition and communication: Judgmental biases,
research methods, and the logic of conversation. Hillsdale, NJ:
Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of memory. In Lawrence Erlbaum.
M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology
(pp. 475–543). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews. Schwarz, N. (2007). Attitude construction: Evaluation in context. Social
Cognition, 25, 638–656.
Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Approach and avoidance in fear of spi-
Schwarz, N., & Bohner, G. (2001). The construction of attitudes. In
ders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38,
A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psy-
105–120.
chology (pp. 436–457). Oxford: Blackwell.
Roberts, J. E., Gotlib, I. H., & Kassel, J. D. (1996). Adult attachment
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments
security and symptoms of depression: The mediating roles of dysfunc-
of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states.
tional attitudes and low self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.
Psychology, 70, 310–320.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experi-
Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring
ences. In A. Kruglanski & E. Tory Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology:
global self-esteem: Construct validation of a single-item measure and
Handbook of basic principles (pp. 385–407). New York: Guilford
the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology
Press.
Bulletin, 27, 151–161.
Schwarz, N., & Sudman, S. (1996). Answering questions: Methodology
Robinson, M. D., Meier, B. P., Zetocha, K. J., & McCaul, K. D. (2005).
for determining cognitive and communicative processes in survey
Smoking and the Implicit Association Test: When the contrast cat-
research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
egory determines the theoretical conclusions. Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 27, 201–212. Seamon, J. G., Brody, N., & Kauff, D. M. (1983). Affective discrimina-
tion of stimuli that are not easily recognized: Effects of shadowing,
Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering.
masking, and cerebral laterality. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
American Psychologist, 45, 1043–1056.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 544–555.
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in nor- Seibt, B., Hafner, M., & Deutsch, R. (2007). Prepared to eat: How immedi-
mal human subjects. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of ate affective and motivational responses to food cues are influenced
Neuropsychology (pp. 63–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier. by food deprivation. European. Journal of Social Psychology, 37,
Roese, N. J., & Jamieson, D. W. (1993). Twenty years of bogus pipeline 359–379.
research: A critical review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, Shapiro, S. (1999). When an ad’s influence is beyond our conscious con-
114, 363–375. trol: Perceptual and conceptual fluency effects caused by incidental ad
Rooth, D-O. (2009). Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: exposure. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 16–36.
Real world evidence. Labour Economics, in press. Shavitt, S. (1989). Operationalizing functional theories of attitude. In
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Attitude struc-
NJ: Princeton University Press. ture and function (pp. 311–337). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Shavitt, S., & Nelson, M. (1999). The social identity function in per-
Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances son perception: Communicated meanings of product preferences. In

CH10.indd 386 10/5/09 7:19:57 PM


References 392

G. Maio & J. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: Function of attitudes Stacy, A. W., Leigh, B. C., & Weingardt, K. (1997). An individual-difference
(pp. 37–57). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. perspective applied to word association. Personality and Social
Sherif, C. W., Sherif, M., & Nebergall, R. E. (1965). Attitude and attitude Psychology Bulletin, 23, 229–237.
change: The social judgment-involvement approach. Philadelphia: Stanley, D., Phelps, E., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The neural basis of implicit
W. B. Saunders Company. attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 164–170.
Sherif, M., & Cantril, H. (1945). The psychology of ‘attitudes’: Part I. Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self-affirmation.
Psychological Review, 52, 295–319. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 5–19.
Sherman, S. J., Rose, J. S., Koch, K., Presson, C. C., & Chassin, L. (2003). Sudman, S., Bradburn, N., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Thinking about answers:
Implicit and explicit attitudes toward cigarette smoking: The effects of The application of cognitive processes to survey methodology. San
context and motivation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
22, 13–39. Swann, W. B., Jr., Chang-Schneider, C., & Larsen McClarty, K. (2007). Do
Shils, E. (1968). Ideology. In E. Shils (Ed.), International encyclopedia of people’s self- views matter? Self-concept and self-esteem in everyday
the social sciences (pp. 66–76). London: Macmillan. life. American Psychologist, 62, 84–94.
Shutts, K., Banaji, M. R., & Spelke, E. S. (in press). Social categories Teachman, B. A., Marker, C. D., & Smith-Janik, S. B. (2008). Automatic
guide young children’s preferences for novel objects. Developmental associations and panic disorder: Trajectories of change over the course
Science. of treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76,
Sibley, C. G., & Wilson, M. S. (2007). Political attitudes and the ideology 988–1002.
of equality: Differentiating support for liberal and conservative politi- Teachman, B., & Woody, S. (2003). Automatic processing in spider phobia:
cal parties in New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 36, Implicit fear associations over the course of treatment. Journal of
72–84. Abnormal Psychology, 112, 100–109.
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (2001). Social dominance: An intergroup theory Tedin, K. L. (1987). Political ideology and the vote. Research in
of social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cambridge University Micropolitics, 2, 63–94.
Press. Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., & Rothermund, K. (2008). Minimizing
Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., & Bobo, L. (1996). Racism, conservatism, method-specific variance in the IAT: A single block IAT. European
Affirmative Action, and intellectual sophistication: A matter of prin- Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 237–245.
cipled conservatism or group dominance? Journal of Personality and Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender schemas
Social Psychology, 70, 476–490. related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? A meta-analysis.
Sinclair, S., Dunn, E., & Lowery, B. S. (2005). The relationship between Developmental Psychology, 38, 615–630.
parental racial attitudes and children’s implicit prejudice. Journal of Tesser, A. (1993). The importance of heritability in psychological research:
Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 283–289. The case of attitudes. Psychological Review, 100, 129–142.
Sinclair, S., Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Colangelo, A. (2005). Social Thomas, W. I., & Znaniecki, F. (1918). The Polish peasant in Europe and
tuning of automatic racial attitudes: The role of affiliative motivation. America. Boston: Badger.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 583–592. Thurstone, L. L. (1928a). Attitudes can be measured. American Journal of
Sirken, M. G., Herrmann, D. J., Schechter, S., Schwarz, N., Tanur, J. M., & Sociology, 33, 529–554.
Tourangeau, R. (Eds.). (1999). Cognition and survey research. New Thurstone, L. L. (1928b). Scale construction with weighted observations.
York: Wiley. Journal of Educational Psychology, 19, 441–453.
Slater, A. (2002). Visual perception in the newborn infant: Issues and Thush, C., Wiers, R. W., Ames, S. L., Grenard, J. L., Sussman, S., &
debates. Intellectica, 34, 57–76. Stacy, A. W. (2007). Apples and oranges? Comparing indirect measures
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and of alcohol-related cognition predicting alcohol use in at-risk adoles-
cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underly- cents. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21, 587–591.
ing memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, Tourangeau, R. (2003). Cognitive aspects of survey measurement and
108–131. mismeasurement. International Journal of Public Opinion Research,
Smith, E. R., & Semin, G. R. (2004). Socially situated cognition: Cognition 15, 3–7.
in its social context. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of sur-
social psychology (pp. 53–117). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic vey response. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Press. Towles-Schwen, T., & Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the origins of racial atti-
Smith, M. B. (1947). The personal setting of public opinions: A study of tudes: Correlates of childhood experiences. Personality and Social
attitudes toward Russia. Public Opinion Quarterly, 11, 507–523. Psychology Bulletin, 27, 162–175.
Smith, M. B., Bruner, J. S., & White, R. W. (1956). Opinions and person- Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., Moffitt, T. E., Robins, R. W.,
ality. Oxford: Wiley. Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2006). Low self-esteem during adolescence
Smith, P. K., Dijksterhuis, A., & Chaiken, S. (2008). Subliminal exposure predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects
to faces and racial attitudes: Exposure to Whites makes Whites like during adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 42, 381–390.
Blacks less. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 50–64. Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., & Robins, R. W. (2003). Stability
Smith, J. R., Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (2006). Who will see me? The of self-esteem across the life span. Journal of Personality and Social
impact of type of audience on willingness to display group-mediated Psychology, 84, 205–220.
attitude-intention consistency. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Age and birth cohort differences
36, 1173–1197. in self-esteem: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Personality and Social
Spalding, L. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1999). Unconscious unease and self- Psychology Review, 5, 321–344.
handicapping: Behavioral consequences of individual differences in Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (1989). Unintended thought. New York:
implicit and explicit self-esteem. Psychological Science, 10, 535–539. Guilford Press.
Sriram, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2009). The Brief Implicit Association Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M., & Umilta, C. (1996). Face prefer-
Test. Experimental Psychology, 56, 283–294. ence at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 892–903.

CH10.indd 387 10/5/09 7:19:58 PM


393 Attitudes

van de Gaer, E., Pustjens, H., van Damme, J., & de Munter, A. (2007). Williams, L. M., Liddell, B. J., Kemp, A. H., Bryant, R. A., Meares, R. A.,
Impact of attitudes of peers on language achievement: Gender differ- Peduto, A. S., et al. (2006). Amygdala-prefrontal dissociation of sub-
ences. Journal of Educational Research, 101, 78–92. liminal and supraliminal fear. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 652–661.
Verplanken, B., Hofstee, G., & Janssen, H. J. W. (1998). Accessibility of Wilson, G. D. (1973). The psychology of conservatism. London: Academic
affective versus cognitive components of attitudes. European Journal Press.
of Social Psychology, 28, 23–35. Wilson, T. D., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and men-
von Hippel, W., Sekaquaptewa, D., & Vargas, P. T. (2008). Linguistic mark- tal correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations.
ers of implicit attitudes. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Brinol (Eds.), Psychological Bulletin, 116, 117–142.
Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 429–458). Wilson, T. D., & Hodges, S. D. (1992). Attitudes as temporary construc-
New York: Psychology Press. tions. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social
von Hippel, W., Brener, L., & von Hippel, C. (2008). Implicit prejudice judgments (pp. 37–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
toward injecting drug users predicts intentions to change jobs among Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual atti-
drug and alcohol nurses. Psychological Science, 19, 7–11. tudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.
Walker, S. S., & Schimmack, U. (2008). Validity of a happiness implicit Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. A., & Reber, R. (2003). The
association test as a measure of subjective well-being. Journal of hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative
Research in Personality, 42, 490–497. judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evalu-
Walton, G. M., & Banaji, M. R. (2004). Being what you say: The effect ation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217).
of essentialist linguistic labels on preferences. Social Cognition, 22, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
193–213. Wittenbrink, B. (2007). Implicit measures of attitudes. New York: Guilford
Webb, E. J., Campbell, D. T., Schwartz, R. D., & Sechrest, L. (1966). Press.
Unobtrusive measures: Nonreactive research in the social sciences. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (2001). Spontaneous prejudice
Oxford: Rand McNally. in context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes. Journal of
Wegener, D. T., & Carlston, D. E. (2005). Cognitive processes in atti- Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 815–827.
tude formation and change. In D. Albarracin, B. T. Johnson, & Wojciszke, B., Bazinska, R., & Jaworski, M. (1998). On the dominance
M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 493–542). Mahwah, of moral categories in impression formation. Personality and Social
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1251–1263.
Weinberger, J., & Westen, D. (2008). RATS, we should have used Clinton: Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal media-
Subliminal priming in political campaigns. Political Psychology, 29, tion of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of
631–651. Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 109–120.
Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Wyer, R. S., Jr., & Albarracin, D. (2005). Belief formation, organization,
Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expres- and change: Cognitive and motivational influences. In D. Albarracin,
sions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes
of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418. (pp. 273–322). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Whisman, M. A., & Kwon, P. (1993). Life stress and dysphoria: The role Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of
of self-esteem and hopelessness. Journal of Personality and Social Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1–27.
Psychology, 65, 1054–1060. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences.
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship of verbal American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.
and overt behavioral responses to attitude objects. Journal of Social Zaller, J. R., & Feldman, S. (1992). A simple theory of the survey response.
Issues, 25, 41–78. American Journal of Political Science, 36, 579–616.
Wiers, R. W., Houben, K., & de Kraker, J. (2007). Implicit cocaine asso- Zeigler-Hill, V. (2006). Discrepancies between implicit and explicit self-
ciations in active cocaine users and controls. Addictive Behaviors, 32, esteem: Implications for narcissism and self-esteem instability. Journal
1284–1289. of Personality, 74, 119–143.

CH10.indd 388 10/5/09 7:19:58 PM


Chapter 11

Attitudes and Persuasion


From Biology to Social Responses to Persuasive Intent

DOLORES ALBARRACÍN AND PATRICK VARGAS

This [attitude] concept is probably the most distinctive and indispensable concept in contemporary American social psychology.
—Allport (1935)

One of the authors of this chapter recently commented to the source of change. These basic claims about attitudes
a colleague, “If I read one more paper that opens with that are relatively uncontested but represent only a tiny fraction
Allport quote about attitude as the ‘most distinctive and of the extant research on attitudes and persuasion.
indispensable concept’ in social psychology, I’m probably Not surprisingly, the complex nature of attitudes has
going to scream.” The Allport Quote is used in empirical intrigued scholars for several millennia, often in the context
articles, book chapters, conference presentations, hand- of persuasion. Aristotle, for example, defined rhetoric as the
books, and lectures worldwide. Alas, here we are, open- “art of discovering, in a particular case, the available means
ing with The Quote. Why? Because Allport’s claim is as of persuasion.” In his view, speakers use their personal
profoundly true today as it was when he first wrote it more character to make their speech credible (ethos), stir emo-
than 70 years ago. tions in their audience (pathos), and prove a real or apparent
Despite being almost a cliché, The Quote can be used truth by means of persuasive arguments (logos). Because
to illustrate several important, basic points about attitudes. of this combination of power, emotion, and argumentation,
First, attitudes are evaluations: one of us expressed an Aristotle was quite disapproving of many of the persuasive
attitude—an intense dislike of clichés (which, forgive messages used by the sophists of his day. In modern times,
us for being hypocritical, may unintentionally emerge in social psychologists have studied similar phenomena but
our writing). Second, attitudes are important because we have often replaced disdain with admiration for the human
hold them toward a variety of different objects—people, ability to elicit compliance without a need for coercion.
places, things, ideas, actions, and so forth. Third, attitudes Politicians were on the radar for Aristotle and remained
continue to be at the center of social psychology because there for contemporary scientists interested in persuasion.
they are complex and difficult to study. We may like some Psychologists developed experimental methods for study-
aspects of an attitude object, but not others (The Quote, ing persuasion during the first part of the 20th century when
itself, is good; its overuse is bad). Fourth, attitudes keep events leading up to World War II made the experimen-
us interested because they may be measured and expressed tal study of persuasion a topic of paramount importance.
in a variety of ways—behaviorally, affectively, cogni- Following the practices of the Roman conglomerate, both
tively, implicitly, and explicitly. Fifth, attitudes are formed Lenin and Stalin set in motion political and social propa-
and changed in a variety of different ways, with varying ganda that controlled and unified an empire of otherwise
amounts of cognitive deliberation (e.g., repeated expo- divided nations that shared no cultural heritage. Millions
sure to stimuli may be outside of conscious awareness or of consistently designed posters from the Soviet era sur-
subject to careful cognitive deliberation) and awareness of vive as a testament of a brilliantly orchestrated persuasion

We thank Andrew Miller for invaluable assistance with this chapter. The research was facilitated by support from the National Institutes
of Health (grants K02-MH01861 and R01-NR08325).
394
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Defining Attitudes 395

machine. And, of course, the Soviet propaganda inspired Different individuals and social groups vary in their food
Hitler ’s ministers to systematically persuade German citi- preferences, their endorsement of women’s participation in
zens of various aspects of Nazi ideology, including the need political and work life, and their preferences for different
to isolate and exterminate “undesirable” ethnic groups. forms of art. Beyond a basic enjoyment of babies, national
Social psychologists started to methodically study per- symbols, and sweet tastes, there seems to be more attitude
suasion in an attempt to understand how to create American variance than invariance. Over the course of many decades,
propaganda that could be as effective as the Bolsheviks’ similar observations have fueled a quest for knowledge
and the Nazis’, though this time in the service of honorable about environmental influences on individuals’ likes and
values. During the 1950s and 1960s, Yale University dislikes. Moreover, in recent years, we have learned more
Professor Carl Hovland received government funding to about the interaction between environmental influences and
identify methods to strengthen and maintain American the biological and genetic bases that shape attitudes. This
troops’ morale. Some of these methods may still be used edition of the Handbook of Social Psychology arrives at a
on U.S. troops, but Hovland’s theory of persuasion has time when this realization is acute.
probably been more long lasting. According to Hovland Attitudes are implicit or explicit object evaluations and,
and Weiss (1951), a message is persuasive when it pro- therefore, have implications for all areas of social life. This
vides recipients with some concrete or symbolic reward. chapter describes attitudes as stemming from biological
For example, recipients are more persuaded by a credible, and environmental sources (cf. “nature” and “nurture”),
trustworthy, or attractive communicator because approval as depicted in Figure 11.1. Biological sources entail the
by such sources is intrinsically rewarding. genetic and biological bases of general patterns of affective
The theory of persuasion put forth by Hovland and his valence and arousal. Environmental sources may be either
collaborators was gradually replaced by theories of the active or passive, and influence affective, cognitive, and
cognitive processes that accompany persuasion. Attentive behavioral responses. A seemingly passive environment
message recipients are rarely passive consumers. They tend triggers cognitive processes that often, but not always,
to be cognitively active: processing and transforming infor- require little attention to or involvement with the attitude
mation, and generating their own take on it (Brock, 1967; object. A seemingly active environment can elicit those
Greenwald, 1968). A message may state that President same processes but entails awareness of influence intent
G. W. Bush was an effective commander-in-chief, but and may arouse corresponding defense mechanisms (e.g.,
critical message recipients may be overwhelmed by their resistance and reactance). For example, if you observe that
recollection of evidence to the contrary. Message recipients an object has positive attributes, this rarely leads you to
have been also known to engage in different forms of resist liking the object; however, receiving a call from a
processing that different psychologists term “systematic,” telemarketer who tries to persuade you to donate money
“peripheral,” “elaborative,” “associative,” and so on. These to the Atlantic Paranormal Society does tend to generate
persuasion processes are an important consideration in this resistance to persuasion. The cognitive processes elicited
chapter, as are the psychological evaluations that result when people are and are not aware of influence attempts
from mere observation of environmental events (e.g., an are the subjects of this chapter.
object that produces positive consequences).
The study of attitudes in persuasion and other contexts,
however, would be incomplete if we ignored the fact that
our evaluations are also shaped by biology. Many of the
methods to study these correlates have become more Biological Sources:
Personality, Genes, and
widely available in recent decades, and methods that did Neurotransmitters that Alter
not exist in prior decades can now be used to study atti- General Affective Arousal and Valence
tudes. This chapter also makes reference to lines of inquiry
related to the brain correlates of attitudes and connections
with the biological bases of personality as studied in the Attitudes
fields of personality psychology and behavior genetics.

Environmental Sources:
Cognitive and Affective Reactions to
DEFINING ATTITUDES Seemingly Passive and Active
Environments
There is no need of social psychological research to
appreciate the astonishing diversity of human attitudes. Figure 11.1 Sources of attitudes.
396 Attitudes and Persuasion

BIOLOGICAL SOURCES OF ATTITUDES: of psychological interests, monozygotic twins reared apart


HOW BIOLOGY AND GENES INFLUENCE obtained an average correlation of .41; monozygotic twins
AFFECTIVE VALENCE AND AROUSAL reared together averaged .49. On measures of social atti-
tudes, monozygotic twins reared apart obtained an average
There is a kernel of truth to the “snips and snails, and puppy correlation of .45; monozygotic twins reared together aver-
dogs’ tails” versus “sugar and spice, and everything nice” aged .43 (Bouchard et al., 1990). Thus, substantial evidence
nursery rhyme. Boys and girls are different; they obviously has been reported that attitudes may be influenced by biol-
differ biologically, and they also tend to differ attitudinally. ogy, but these studies address the issue at a molar level.
But to what extent do biological differences influence Next, we consider biological influences on attitudes at more
psychological and attitudinal differences? From the 1960s to molecular levels, and we begin with a consideration of atti-
the 1990s, many physicians and psychologists believed that tudinal structure and its relation to personality.
psychological sex differences were entirely socially con- The structure of an attitude can be understood using
structed, that people were “psychosexually neutral at birth,” models of affective feelings. A popular model of the struc-
(Diamond & Sigmundson, 1997, p. 298). The heartbreaking ture of affect and emotions incorporates the dimensions
case of David Reimer helped disprove that theory. of positive versus negative valence and high versus low
Reimer was born a boy who, at the age of 8 months, arousal (Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert, & Lang; 2001;
had his penis destroyed in a circumcision operation. As Russell, 2003; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985; for reviews, see
experts advised about the convenience of raising him Clore & Schnall, 2005; Schimmack & Crites, 2005). We
as a girl, Reimer ’s testes were removed at 22 months as may feel sad, angry, content, or excited, and these four
part of a male-to-female sex “reassignment” effort. For states vary not only in their negative or positive valence
years this case was reported in the popular media (using but also in their associated arousal (Russell, 2003). Arousal
pseudonyms Joan/John) as a success; “Joan” was said has proved to be an elusive term but generally comprises
to be successfully developing as a normal woman. “The autonomic activation and may be measured by changes in
effect[s] of such reports were widespread for theory and skin conductance, heart rate, or brain waves (see Bradley &
practice. Sociology, psychology, and women’s study texts Lang, 2007; Cacioppo, Berntson, & Crites, 1996). Feelings
were rewritten . . .” (Diamond & Sigmundson, 1997, p. 299) of anxiousness, tension, alertness, and excitement share
to suggest that genetic influences on psychological sex high autonomic activation or arousal (e.g., high heart and
differences were minimal. The truth, though, is that breathing rate), whereas feelings of sadness and content-
Reimer ’s childhood was far from normal. In terms of atti- ment share low autonomic activation or arousal (e.g., lower
tudes, young Joan was more like a John: s/he didn’t like heart and breathing rate; somnolence).
makeup, expressed a desire to shave, preferred boys’ toys Given that attitudes are valenced responses (positive vs.
to girls’, liked to dress up in men’s clothing, enjoyed play- negative), they can also be mapped onto a model with
ing soldier, and preferred hanging around other boys.1 valence and arousal as distinct dimensions. This model is
Of course, we do not intend to suggest that psychosexual graphically depicted in Figure 11.2. On the valence axis,
identity can be subsumed by the attitude concept, but this individuals may dislike or like a presidential candidate, and
case overwhelmingly suggests that important biological may dislike or like a particular food. Furthermore, attitudes
influences on attitudes exist. can be mapped onto the arousal dimension because they
Biological influences on attitudes may be dramatically vary in importance, confidence, or the degree to which
observed in unusual cases, such as Reimer’s, or where mono- they elicit strong emotional responses such as excitement
zygotic twins have been separated near birth. Monozygotic (see Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, 2000;
twins share the same genetic material, so when they have Fabrigar, MacDonald, & Wegener, 2005; Lang, Öhman, &
been raised apart from one another they provide an excel- Vaitl, 1988). For example, highly involving objects such
lent natural experiment for examining the influence of as abortion, God, and marijuana legalization often trigger
genes and environments on psychological characteristics. strong attitudes that are infused with feelings and con-
Such cases are quite rare, but a study of 100 monozygotic nect to other important attitudes, such as self-esteem (the
twins reared apart has been ongoing since 1979 (Bouchard, evaluations of oneself as a person; Eagly & Chaiken, 1993;
Lykken, McGue, Segal, & Tellegen, 1990). On measures Fabrigar et al., 2005). These attitudes are often reported as
extreme when individuals complete attitude scales (Judd &
Brauer, 1995), are held with high confidence (Abelson,
1 Atage 14, Reimer chose to resume living as a male, and at 32, he 1988), are easy to recall (Judd & Brauer, 1995), and are fer-
chose to reveal his identity to help others avoid similar suffering. vently defended against external attacks (Johnson, Maio, &
Tragically, Reimer committed suicide in 2004. Smith-McLallen, 2005; Petty, Tormala, & Rucker, 2004).
Biological Sources of Attitudes: How Biology and Genes Influence Affective Valence and Arousal 397

 

Valence Valence
   

Arousal
Arousal

 

Figure 11.2 Dimensions of valence and arousal. Figure 11.3 Hypothetical distribution of valence and arousal.

Adhering to a valence/arousal model does not require emotional meaning (Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Lee,
dogmatic adherence to a circumplex (see also Remington, 1999). Moreover, the amygdala is connected with the insula
Fabrigar, & Visser, 2000). For example, objects with and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and these three
extremely positive and negative valence are often important structures can collectively respond to both valence and
and generate high autonomic arousal (for a review, see arousal (Cunningham et al., 2004; Cunningham & Zelazo,
Bradley et al., 2001). In this case, high positive and nega- 2007; Rempel-Clower, 2007; Wright et al., 2008). The visual
tive valences are associated with high arousal, whereas cortex also appears to respond to both valence and arousal,
neutral valences are associated with low arousal. If one as judged from a functional magnetic resonance imaging
were to map objects onto independent valence and arousal study revealing greater activation when participants viewed
dimensions, they probably would not be evenly distrib- pleasant and unpleasant images, compared with neutral
uted among all four quadrants but rather in a U-type of images (Lang et al., 1998).
pattern (see Bradley et al., 2001; Remington et al., 2001;
Figure 11.3). Nevertheless, these two dimensions have Personality as the Biological Core of Stable
interesting applications in the attitude literature and con- Valence and Arousal Patterns
nect with stable response patterns that are grounded in biol-
ogy and genetics. Being stable patterns of feelings, thoughts, and behaviors
Interestingly, considerable research indicates that specific (Allport, 1937; Cattell, 1946; McCrae & Costa, 2008;
structures in the brain respond to stimuli in ways that sug- Mischel & Shoda, 2008), personality cuts across objects
gest that valence and arousal are not easy to separate. In and times, and may influence and be influenced by
particular, the amygdala. an almond-shaped group of specific attitudes. The general assumption, however, is
neurons located deep in the medial temporal lobes of the that personality is, in part, biologically driven, and it is not
brain, plays an important role in evaluation (Hamann, Ely, difficult to see how personality connects with the valence
Hoffman, & Kilts, 2002; Irwin et al., 1996). The amygdala and arousal dimensions in Figure 11.2. For example,
is particularly engaged during affective judgments of emo- Eysenck (Eysenck, 1967; Eysenck, Eysenck, & Educational
tional pictures, words, and odors (Cunningham, Raye, & and Industrial Testing Service, 1975) characterized person-
Johnson, 2004; Phan et al., 2004; Royet, Plailly, Delon- ality as linked to biological and genetic factors manifested
Martin, Kareken, & Segebarth, 2003), when the target in the three major traits of neuroticism, extraversion, and
objects are negative (vs. positive) stimuli (Cunningham, psychoticism. Neuroticism is the disposition to experience
Johnson, Gatenby, Gore, & Banaji, 2003; Morris et al., negative affect, and it appears to be relatively stable over
1996; Reekum et al., 2007), and when the stimuli have the lifetime (Eysenck, 1967; John & Srivastava, 1999).
398 Attitudes and Persuasion

This trait has been found to predict the onset of depres- Not surprisingly, traits predominantly related to arousal
sion and anxiety (Angst & Clayton, 1986; Boyce, Parker, (e.g., extraversion/introversion, impulsivity) are also related
Barnett, Cooney, & Smith, 1991; Hirschfeld et al., 1989; to valence and purportedly have a biological substrate. For
Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, 1993), and cor- example, extraversion comprises sociability and risk tak-
relates with negative attitudes toward a variety of objects. ing but also optimism and is contrasted with introversion,
For example, neuroticism reportedly correlates with feeling which comprises introspection and reservation but also pes-
dissatisfaction after eating chocolate (Müller, Dettmer, & simism (Taub, 1998). Eysenck (1967, 1975) conceptualized
Macht, 2008), sexual guilt in both men and women (r  .31 extraversion as a result of “cortical excitation” and
in both cases; p < .05; Heaven, Fitzpatrick, Craig, Kelly, & “inhibition” (see Pavlov, 1928), which was initially defined
Sebar, 2000), and women’s dissatisfaction with sexual as the degree of engagement of the ascending reticular acti-
experiences (r  .36; p < .05; Heaven et al.). Neuroticism vating system of the brain (Moruzzi & Magoun, 1949), and
also correlates with negative evaluations of medical treat- later as resulting from a limbic-arousal system, a mono-
ments and of physical symptoms (r  .14, p < .05; and amine-oxidase system, and a pituitary-adrenal system
r  .46, p < .001; Cox, Borger, Asmundson, & Taylor, (Eysenck, 1990a, 1990b). In summary, traits associated
2000), as well as negative attitudes toward working with with arousal (and valence) have been linked to various bio-
patients with AIDS (b  .21, p < .001; Knussen & Niven, logical systems that may also influence attitudes.
1999). Moreover, neuroticism has been linked to negative As a radical biological theorist of personality, Eysenck
attitudes toward work (for a review, see Tokar, Fischer, & (1967) devoted intense effort to precisely tie extraversion
Subich, 1998), beliefs that work interferes with personal to corresponding responses in a particular arousal system.
relationships (Mughal, Walsh, & Wilding, 1996), and per- For example, Eysenck proposed that deficits in arousal
ceived stress associated with work (Decker & Borgen, underlie extraverts’ search for external stimulation (see
1993: Spector & O’Connell, 1994). There are times, how- also Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985; Ludvigh & Happ, 1974;
ever, in which neuroticism correlates with positive atti- Shigehisa & Symons, 1973). Consistent with this hypoth-
tudes. For example, despite the reported guilt elicited by esis, past research has demonstrated that extraverts have
eating chocolate, neuroticism is associated with favorable lesser cerebral blood flow during resting states than do
attitudes toward chocolate (r  .27; p < .001; Müller introverts (Fischer, Wik, & Fredrikson, 1997; Mathew,
et al., 2008) as well as illicit substance use (r  .30; p < .001; Weinman, & Barr, 1984). Although individual differ-
Francis, 1996). Overall, these patterns suggest that neu- ences in extraversion have traditionally been linked to
roticism correlates with negative attitudes about all objects high experienced arousal, this research points to a corre-
except for those that are instrumental to improving nega- spondingly underactive brain during rest (but see Posner,
tive feelings (e.g., chocolate). Russell, & Peterson, 2005, for the relation between experi-
Although, by definition, neuroticism entails negative enced and physiological arousal).
affect and consequently negative attitudes, neuroticism One particularly fruitful approach to understanding the
can also entail high levels of anxiety (Eysenck, 1967). arousal dimension of affect and attitudes is Reinforcement
Hence, the higher arousal of neurotics could influence Sensitivity Theory (Gray, 1970, 1972, 1982, 1991; Gray &
attitude properties related to arousal. Up to now, however, McNaughton, 2000). According to the theory (Gray, 1991;
almost no research has related neuroticism to attitude Gray & McNaughton, 2000), different regulatory systems
extremity or arousal. One exception to the absence of underlie responses to reward and punishment: (1) the behav-
research in this area comes from an analysis of reactions ioral approach system (BAS), (2) the behavioral inhibition
to different types of advertisements. Over two studies, system (BIS), and (3) the fight-flight-freezing system. First,
Mooradian (1996) found that, as one might expect, pre- the BAS is engaged by positively valanced stimuli, rewards,
senting positive and negative ads yielded, respectively, or relief from punishment, and regulates approach behav-
more positive and negative attitudes toward the ads (asso- iors (Gray, 1991; Gray & McNaughton, 2000). Associated
ciations between neuroticism and attitude extremity for characteristics involve impulsivity, risk taking, and predis-
positive and negative ads, respectively,   .10 and .07; position for mania (Gray, 2000). Second, the BIS is engaged
p < .001). Although preliminary, these results suggest a need by conflicting goals leading to the need to inhibit one of
to further research the relations between neuroticism and these goals (Gray, 1991; Gray & McNaughton, 2000). This
negatively valenced attitudes, and how neuroticism relates system is presumably sensitive to punishment, nonreward,
to sensitivity to valence, as well as attitude extremity and novelty, and is involved in reducing negative or pain-
and importance. Even when neuroticism is more likely ful outcomes by inhibiting movement toward goals (Gray,
to implicate valence than arousal, the effects of arousal 1991; Gray & McNaughton, 2000). Associated psychopa-
should not be ignored. thology includes high anxiety, generalized anxiety disorder,
Biological Sources of Attitudes: How Biology and Genes Influence Affective Valence and Arousal 399

and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Gray & McNaughton, Genetics and Neurotransmitters


2000). Third, the fight-flight-freeze system regulates escape/
avoidance behavior and is engaged by aversive stimuli. Although pinpointing one gene that explains a particular
Associated characteristics and disorders include fear, avoid- behavioral pattern currently seems like an impossible
ant personality disorder, phobias, and panic attacks (Gray & endeavor, research has identified some important genetic
McNaughton, 2000). markers for the traits of neuroticism and impulsivity
Even though BAS and BIS closely align with approach to (for a summary, see Table 11.1). To begin, quantitative
positive stimuli and avoidance of negative stimuli, scholars genetic studies have shown that about 40% of the vari-
have debated whether the BAS system is related to only posi- ance in neuroticism has a genetic basis (Jang, Livesly, &
tive emotions and approach behavior, or instead to both pos- Vernon, 1996; Loehlin, 1992). Identifying specific genes
itive and negative emotions, as well as active approach for neuroticism, however, has been challenging, and
and avoidance. Empirical evidence supports both of these efforts have concentrated on genes related to the trans-
claims. On the one hand, self-reported BAS is associated port and reception of serotonin. The results from these
with greater left frontal cortical activity, approach behavior, attempts have not always been consistent and together
and positive affect, whereas self-reported BIS is associated suggest small associations. For example, two studies com-
with greater right frontal cortical activity and negative affect pared individuals with extremely high and low levels of
(Sutton & Davidson, 1997). On the other hand, arousal neuroticism. Although one of these studies revealed five
seems to be associated with asymmetric frontal activity that loci that differed across individuals with high and low
responds to both elation and anger (Harmon-Jones & Allen, levels of neuroticism (Fullerton et al., 2003), the other
1998; Hewig, Hagemann, Seifert, Naumann, & Bartussek, found no significant differences in these loci (Nash et al.,
2006). This finding suggests that approach can also involve 2004). A more recent study with eight DNA pools from
negative emotions. participants with extreme neuroticism scores provided a
More generally, impulsivity and particularly the hyper- whole-genome analysis of associations with neuroticism
active/impulsive form of attention-deficit/hyperactivity (Shifman et al., 2008). This second study found no single
disorder (ADHD) are linked to generalized low arousal, locus that accounted for more than 1% of the variance in
as judged by slower blood flow in the brain (Fischer et al., neuroticism but did identify a polymorphism of the
1997; Mathew et al., 1984) and reduced activity in fron- PDE4D gene (rs702543) associated with neuroticism (1%
tal and striatal regions that are responsible for impulse of the variance). Although this gene has been consistently
control (Amen & Carmichael, 1997; Lou, Henriksen, & implicated in major depression (Scott, Perini, Shering, &
Bruhn, 1984; Lou, Henriksen, Bruhn, Borner, & Nielsen, Whalley, 1991; Zhang et al., 2002), its low explanatory
1989; Rubia et al., 1999). At the same time, this low power in this study suggests that multiple loci likely sup-
baseline arousal (which is not always found, see e.g., port differences in neuroticism.
Zuckerman, 1990) may render a predisposition to experi- As abnormalities in the central serotoninergic system
ence arousal in the presence of external and, in particular, play a critical role in the cause of major depression and
interesting stimuli. High-sensation seekers, for example, anxiety disorders (Coccaro & Murphy, 1990; Maes &
manifest greater increases in both heart rate and ampli- Metzer, 1995), it is not surprising that neuroticism has
tude of event-related potentials than low-sensation seek- been linked to the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR).
ers in the presence of interesting stimuli (e.g., r  .67; The serotonin transporter influences serotoninergic
Pierson, Le Houezec, Fossaert, Dubal, & Jouvent,
1999; Smith, Perlstein, Davidson, & Michael, 1986;
Smith, Davidson, Smith, Goldstein, & Perlstein, 1989; Table 11.1 Summary of Trait Heritability and Associated Genes
Zuckerman, Simons, & Como, 1988; but see de la Pena, Neuroticism Impulsivity
1992, for a review of inconsistent findings). In this light,
% of trait 40% 40–60%
highly impulsive individuals may be highly aroused by explained by
certain stimuli, and hence their attitudes may be more heritability
extreme, more confident, or perceived as more important. Genes that PDE4D (phosphodiesterase D4 (dopamine transporter
Up to now, however, researchers have neither theorized have been 4 D gene) gene)
about nor researched the attitude strength implications identified as 5-HTTLPR (serotonin DRD2 and DRD4
related transporter gene) (dopamine receptor genes)
of individual differences in traits associated with sta-
DAT1 (dopamine
ble valence and arousal patterns. This mission seems transporter gene)
imperative for the upcoming generation of attitude CNTRF (ciliary
research. neurotrophic factor gene)
400 Attitudes and Persuasion

neurotransmission by affecting reuptake of synaptic toward novel and existing stimuli, but a study of these
serotonin, which varies among individuals and is abnor- associations will probably benefit from large samples and
mal in individuals who suffer from anxiety and depres- a multigene approach.
sion (Metzer & Arora, 1988; Owens & Nemeroff, 1994). Other genes also contribute to explaining impulsivity, even
Although this gene and dysregulation of the serotoninergic though their explanatory power is much lower than the 40%
system would seem to be excellent candidates for explain- to 60% estimated heritability of the trait. Comings, Saucier,
ing neuroticism, studies have failed to provide consistent and MacMurray (2002), for example, found that four dopa-
supportive evidence. Lesch et al. (1996) reported that dif- mine receptor genes contributed around 5% of the variance in
ferences in the 5-HTTLPR accounted for 3% to 4% of the novelty seeking. Along the same lines, DRD2 and DRD4 have
variability in neuroticism in two predominantly male sam- been associated with novelty seeking (Noble et al., 1998),
ples with extreme neuroticism scores. However, five subse- as well as drug and alcohol abuse disorders, ADHD, and
quent studies (Ball et al., 1997; Ebstein et al., 1997; Flory pathological gambling (Comings, Saucier, & MacMurray,
et al., 1999; Jorn et al., 1998; Nakamura et al., 1997) have 2002; Ebstein & Kotler, 2002). The DRD4 gene interacts
failed to replicate this finding, casting a shadow of doubt with parenting quality to influence the presence of ADHD
on whether the serotonin transporter gene is involved at in children (Sheese, Voelker, Rothbart, & Posner, 2007).
all. All in all, the PDE4D and 5-HTTLPR genes are likely The dopamine transporter gene, DAT1, has also been impli-
to play some role in negative affectivity and should be cated in the cause of ADHD, which frequently involves high
examined in relation to negative attitudes as well. impulsivity. Studies of affected children and their parents
Like neuroticism, an estimated 40% to 60% of the have identified a significant correlation between a 10-repeat
variance of impulsivity is inherited (Bouchard, 1994; allele of the DAT1 gene and ADHD (Cook et al., 1995; Daly,
Fulker, Eysenck, & Zuckerman, 1980; Loehlin, 1992; Jawj, Fitzgerald, & Gill, 1999; Gill, Daly, Heron, Hawi, &
Rietveld, Hudziak, Bartels, van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma, Fitzgerald, 1997; Waldman et al., 1998). Moreover, in some
2004), although the genetic influence is attenuated by studies (Waldman et al.,) but not in others (Farrow, Levy,
such factors as religious upbringing (Boomsma, de Geus, & Silberstein, 2005), this association was stronger for the
van Baal, & Koopmans, 1999). Furthermore, many specific hyperactive impulsive form of ADHD than for the solely
attitudes related to impulsivity have considerable herita- inattentive form. In summary, associations of the dopamine
bility. For example, high heritability has been observed in receptor and transporter genes with impulsivity support the
attitudes toward alcohol (51%; Perry, 1973), extreme and hypothesis of abnormal (accelerated) dopamine transport
dogmatic attitudes (more than 50%; Martin et al., 1986), as and reuptake in impulsivity (Barkley, 1998; Farrow et al.,
well attitudes toward roller coasters, practicing sports, the 2005; Swanson & Castellanos, 2002). The increased reup-
death penalty for murder, abortion on demand, and reading take presumably decreases the time for dopamine to act
books (Olson, Vernon, Harris, & Jang, 2001). Moreover, on the synapse and may explain the general finding of under-
attitudes with high heritability indexes are also highly active brains in ADHD and perhaps impulsivity more gen-
accessible, highly resistant to change, and more frequently erally (Farrow et al., 2005). Overall, fast dopamine reuptake
sought out in selecting attractive others (Olson et al.; may generate a preference for attitude objects that increase
Tesser, 1993). These attitude properties seem to align well dopamine production, including drugs, roller coasters, and
with the arousal dimension of affect and attitudes, indi- risky sexual situations.
cating that not only valence but also strength/arousal are Genes related to the general efficiency and health of
likely to be biologically based. neurons may also play a role in impulsivity. A large study
To date, the genetic basis of impulsivity has been at of patients with ADHD tested the premise of deficient
least partially established. Alleles of the dopamine recep- neurotrophic factors in this condition (Ribasés et al., 2008).
tor D4 (DRD4) gene are associated with the trait of nov- These factors constitute a family (i.e., the nerve growth
elty seeking, which correlates with impulsive sensation factor and the ciliary neurotrophic factor) that controls the
seeking (e.g., r  .15; Bailey, Breidenthal, Jorgensen, growth and proper development of nerves. Within this fam-
McCracken, & Fairbanks, 2007; Ebstein et al., 1996). The ily, one gene (CNTRF) is reportedly associated with both
long allele correlates with high levels of sensation seeking adult and childhood ADHD, and two genes (NTF3 and
and the short allele with low-to-moderate levels of this NTRK2) are reportedly associated with childhood ADHD
trait (Ebstein et al., 1996). However, this finding has been (Ribasés et al.). The effect sizes for these associations
replicated in about half of the studies that have tested this ranged from an odds ratio (OR) of 1.38 to 1.52, with addi-
association (Prolo & Licinio, 2002) and is yet to be con- tive effects yielding an OR of 2.04 (Ribasés et al., 2008),
nected with attitudes. Both the trait of novelty seeking and may in the future be examined for specific attitudes
and the gene itself could correlate with positive attitudes in addition to self-reported personality traits.
Inadverted Environmental Influences: Attitudes in Response to a Seemingly Passive Environment 401

Summary that their attitudes have actually changed (see Chartrand,


2005). Unfortunately, extant research is not so easily clas-
The personality traits of neuroticism and impulsivity/ sified. In some cases, people may be unaware of more than
extraversion influence the valence and arousal levels of one stage: People presented with subliminal stimuli are
chronic affective reactions. Both neuroticism and impulsivity obviously unaware of the presence of a stimulus and
have been associated with attitudes about specific objects, are presumably unaware of cognitive processes engaged
such as mostly negative attitudes for neuroticism and attitudes by the unseen stimulus, but it is unclear whether they lack
that are more reactive to external information for impulsivity. awareness of attitudinal change. This section reviews the-
Small correlations exist between neuroticism and the sero- ory and research addressing issues of attitude change when
tonin transporter gene, as well as between impulsivity and the people interact with a seemingly passive environment,
dopamine receptor and transporter genes. Despite their small when there is no obvious intent to persuade. “No obvious
size, these associations represent a critical first step in under- intent to persuade” may be reflected in a variety of differ-
standing how genes might influence attitude valence and ent ways, from subliminal persuasion to mere exposure to
arousal by exerting an influence on stable affective responses evaluative conditioning (EC) to direct experience with an
such as those encompassed by neuroticism and impulsivity. object. We begin with the most obvious instance of a seem-
ingly passive environment, where people are unaware of
the existence of a stimulus.
INADVERTED ENVIRONMENTAL
INFLUENCES: ATTITUDES IN RESPONSE Subliminal Persuasion and Priming
TO A SEEMINGLY PASSIVE ENVIRONMENT
Subliminal persuasion sounds like a sexy topic to the
Despite biological and genetic contributions, attitudes are layperson, but for psychologists, even assessing whether a
undeniable results of the environment even when we fail stimulus is subliminal engenders controversy. Colloquially,
to advert, or attend to, its influence. In 1957, in Ft. Lee, subliminal has been used to refer to unnoticed, disguised,
New Jersey, moviegoers watching William Holden and masked, and genuinely imperceptible stimuli (Pratkanis &
Kim Novak star in Picnic were ostensibly shown messages Greenwald, 1988). The term “subliminal” evokes the limen,
such as, “Eat popcorn,” and “Drink Coca-Cola,” during the defined as the point at which a stimulus evokes a sensation.
screening of the film (see McConnell, Cutler, & McNeil, Unfortunately, the concept of a point, or absolute threshold
1958). The presentation of these messages was said to have for the presence of some phenomenological sensation, is
been just 1/3,000th of a second, well below the threshold dubious. Thresholds tend to be normally distributed, and
of conscious visual perception (i.e., the limen). Although they vary both interpersonally and intrapersonally (Stevens,
the brief presentation made it impossible for the innocent 1951). Various approaches to operationalizing subliminal
moviegoers to have noticed the messages, James M. Vicary presentations have been suggested (e.g., signal detection
and Frances Thayer, architects of this early effort at sub- theory; Synodinos, 1988), but the contemporary consensus
liminal persuasion, claimed that the presentation of these has settled on simple self-report. A stimulus is considered
messages increased popcorn sales 58% and cola sales 18% subliminal when participants claim to be unable to per-
over the course of 6 weeks. ceive it, especially in the presence of incentives for correct
These early subliminal persuasion effects were, how- reports (Cheesman & Merikle, 1986; Fowler, Wolford,
ever, pure fiction. Attempts to replicate Vicary’s claims Slade, & Tassinary, 1981; Greenwald, 1992; Kihlstrom,
under controlled conditions were unsuccessful. In an inter- 1987, 1999; Merikle, 1988).
view published in June 1962 in Advertising Age, Vicary Until recently, evidence for subliminal persuasion had
admitted to making up the story to drum up publicity for been weak at best. Null effects (Champion & Turner, 1959; de
his marketing firm (Danzig, 1962). Despite the fraud in Fleur & Petranoff, 1959; George & Jennings, 1975; Greenwald,
Vicary’s claims of subliminal persuasion, it is clear that Spangenberg, Pratkanis, & Eskenazi, 1991; Merikle, 1988;
attitudes can be formed and changed outside of conscious Pratkanis, Eskenazi, & Greenwald, 1994; Smith &
awareness (Clore & Schnall, 2005; Kruglanski & Stroebe, Rogers, 1994; Vokey & Read, 1985), failures to replicate
2005; but see Cacioppo, Marshall-Goodell, Tassinary, & (e.g., Hawkins, 1970, a significant effect; Beatty & Hawkins,
Petty, 1992). A lack of awareness may be present at dif- 1989, no effect), and methodological problems (for reviews
ferent stages of the persuasion process. People may be see Moore, 1982, 1988; Pratkanis & Aronson, 1992) are
unaware of the presence of a persuasive stimulus, they legion. A meta-analysis of 23 studies obtained a nontrivial
may be unaware of cognitive processes that mediate atti- effect of subliminal advertising (r  .06; Trappey, 1996),
tude formation and change, and they may be unaware but this analysis must be treated with caution because it
402 Attitudes and Persuasion

included work with critical methodological problems (e.g., In other research, the subliminal presentation of a
treating disguised and unnoticed objects as “subliminal”). significant other ’s name has been shown to have variable
A trio of recent articles has demonstrated some effect of effects on achievement behavior depending on whether
subliminally presented stimuli on attitudes and behavior. In the other is close and values achievement (Fitzsimons &
one study, participants watched an 18-minute segment of Bargh, 2003). Specifically, when the word “dad” (vs.
an adult cartoon (The Simpsons). Experimental participants a control prime) was subliminally presented, individuals
were subliminally exposed to the word “thirsty” and a pic- who felt close to their dad and also believed their dad
ture of a can of Coca-Cola 12 times each for 33 ms at each to value achievement exhibited greater persistence and
presentation. Control participants were exposed to blank, quicker identification of achievement-related words in a
white screens at the same exposure rate and intervals as lexical decision task. In contrast, individuals who felt close
experimental participants. Before the manipulation, exper- to their father but felt no paternal pressure to achieve per-
imental and control participants reported being equally formed worse at these tasks (Shah, 2003).
thirsty; after the manipulation, experimental participants The behavioral effects of a subliminal prime seem to
reported being significantly more thirsty than control par- be contingent on a preexisting motivation to engage in
ticipants. In a follow-up study, the researchers eliminated this behavior. In one study, the masked, 33-ms presen-
the possibility of “thirsty” operating as a semantic prime tation of achievement primes increased willingness to
by using images of a sweaty athlete and a can of Coca- resume an achievement task only in participants with high
Cola. Similar results obtained (Cooper & Cooper, 2002). self-reported achievement motivation. The prime, how-
Other researchers experimentally manipulated thirst by ever, had reversed effects in participants with low self-
having all participants eat cookies and then having half of reported achievement motivation, who appeared to inhibit
the participants drink a glass of water (Strahan, Spencer, & achievement goals and performed worse in the presence
Zanna, 2002). Next, participants were subliminally (16 ms than in the absence of a prime (Hart & Albarracín, 2009).
exposures) presented with either thirst-related, or unrelated, Thus, these results provide evidence that personal dispo-
words via computer-mediated lexical decision task. There sitions interact with subliminal environmental stimuli to
was an effect of the thirst manipulation on self-reported produce changes in participants’ responses.
thirst, but contrary to Cooper and Cooper (2002), no effect There is also evidence of more indirect subliminal
of subliminal priming on self-reported thirst. However, in an persuasion by which subliminal exposure to a source can
ersatz taste test after the priming manipulation, thirsty partic- increase persuasion to a message later delivered by that
ipants who had also been primed with thirst drank the most source. In two studies, participants were either not exposed,
Kool-Aid. In a follow-up study, thirsty participants were subliminally exposed, or supraliminally exposed to a face
subliminally primed with either thirsty or neutral words, and that later turned out to be the source of a persuasive message
then shown advertisements for thirst-quenching and electro- (Weisbuch, Mackie, & Garcia-Marques, 2003). Participants
lyte-replacing beverages. Thirst-primed participants liked, who were subliminally exposed to the source were more
and took more coupons for, the thirst-quenching beverage persuaded by the arguments, and this effect was not medi-
compared with participants primed with neutral words. ated by explicit liking of the source. Moreover, participants
Conceptually similar results obtained when participants who who were supraliminally exposed to the source were also
were primed with a sad face, and expected to interact with more persuaded by the arguments, but this effect was medi-
others, preferred a mood-restoring music CD. In all cases, ated by explicit liking of the source and disappeared when
subliminal priming led to greater persuasion when partici- participants were informed that they had previously been
pants were motivated to engage in a related behavior. exposed to the source. Thus, Weisbuch et al. (2003) specu-
Other work showed that subliminal priming of brands lated that participants misattributed the positive responses
could influence preferences for branded items. Karremans, coming from the familiar source to the persuasive message.
Stroebe, and Claus (2006) also manipulated participant Subtle influences on attitudes and behaviors are some-
thirst and subliminally primed either a particular brand of times general rather than specific to an object. For example,
beverage or a neutral word. Again, thirsty participants were general action and inaction primes (e.g., “go” vs. “rest”)
most influenced by the priming manipulation. Thirsty influence the amount of motor or cognitive output
participants who received the neutral prime showed vir- irrespective of the type of behavior in question, with the
tually no preference for one beverage over another, but same stimuli producing trivial and important motor and
thirsty participants who received the brand prime pre- cognitive manifestations normally viewed as parts of dif-
ferred the primed brand. Thus, subliminal persuasion ferent systems (Albarracín, Handley, Noguchi, McCulloch,
seems to be most effective when a related motive is Li, Leeper et al., 2008; Hart & Albarracín, in press). A series
already aroused. of experiments by Albarracín and colleagues (Albarracı́n,
Inadverted Environmental Influences: Attitudes in Response to a Seemingly Passive Environment 403

Handly et al., 2008; Albarracı́n, Wang et al., 2009) exam- paradigm, and an unambiguous case of no obvious intent
ined the effects of instilling general action and inaction goals to persuade, Kunst-Wilson and Zajonc (1980) subliminally
using word primes such as “action” and “rest.” Some of presented 10 different polygons, 5 times each, to partici-
the experiments showed that the same stimuli, presented pants. The researchers informed participants that “slides
either subliminally or supraliminally, influenced motor would be shown on the screen at durations so brief that one
output, such as doodling on a piece of paper and eating, could not really see what was being presented . . .” (p. 557).
as well as cognitive output, such as recall and problem After the presentation of stimuli, participants were shown
solving. Two experiments supported the prediction that pairs of polygons, one old and one new, and asked to indi-
these diverse effects can result from the instigation of cate which one they had been shown earlier and which one
general action and inaction goals. Specifically, these last they preferred. Recognition of the polygons was just below
two studies confirmed that participants were motivated to chance (48%), but preference for old polygons was sub-
achieve active or inactive states and that attaining them stantially higher than chance (60%).
decreased the effects of the primes on behavior. Although Fifty years of mere exposure research has yielded an
these effects entailed a cognitive representation of action enormous amount of information, so here we describe just a
and inaction, they are clearly relevant to the arousal few important conclusions and the current state of the area.
dimension of attitudes and may, under certain conditions, In summarizing 208 empirical studies from 134 articles pub-
contribute to making stimuli more arousing. lished between 1968 and 1987, Bornstein (1989) obtained
In conclusion, despite accumulating evidence that a moderate effect size of .26. The effect emerges for all
subliminally presented stimuli can influence behavior (e.g., types of stimuli: auditory (r .24), ideographs (r  .22),
Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996), there remains no good evi- nonsense words/syllables (r .24), photographs (r  .37),
dence for subliminal persuasion operating as powerfully as meaningful words/names (r  .49), polygons (r  .41), and
Vicary imagined it. Subliminally presented stimuli are at best real people/objects (r  .20), except for abstract paintings,
a weak persuasive force. They seem to have their greatest drawings, and matrices (r  –.03). Moreover, the effect
influence when people are already predisposed toward tends to be stronger when a heterogeneous (r  .30) ver-
a related course of action (e.g., when a related motive is sus homogeneous (r  –.02) pool of stimuli are presented,
aroused), and when the opportunities for that action are when exposure times are shorter (especially less than 1 sec-
readily apparent and easily performed. Subliminally primed ond, r  .41), when stimuli are more complex (no effect
individuals who face physical impediments or psycho- size reported, but six of nine studies found more favorable
logical distractions to the related actions are (presumably) attitudes for complex stimuli compared with simple stim-
unlikely to show evidence of subliminal primes, and sim- uli), when the maximum number of stimulus presentations
ply priming a command (eat popcorn) does not seem likely is up to nine (r  .21), when there is some delay between
to cause a movie patron to get up, leave the ongoing film, stimulus exposure and evaluation (r  .22), and when the
and buy a box of popcorn. Moreover, most of the current participants are adults (r  .30) instead of children (aged
evidence on subliminal priming occurs in simple contexts, 12 or younger, r  .05).
when primes are presented alone on an otherwise blank Explanations of the processes underlying the mere
screen. Whether primes can be effectively embedded in a exposure effect have varied, with initial debate focusing
context where they compete for attention with other words on the extent to which higher-order cognition is involved.
and images remains largely an empirical question. Demand effects (Stang, 1974), belief formation (Grush,
1976), and a combination of positive habituation and bore-
dom (Berlyne, 1970) were all proposed as explanations
Mere Exposure
underlying mere exposure effects. However, after the sub-
Attitudes toward a stimulus may become more favorable liminal demonstration (Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980), in
with increasing frequency of exposure to the stimulus which participants could not possibly have intentionally
(Zajonc, 1968).2 In a classic example of the mere exposure thought about stimuli they had not even seen, delibera-
tive cognition about the stimuli was reduced to a sufficient
2 Notably,
but not necessary cause. Bornstein (1989) suggested that
attitude change caused by mere exposure is different
adding notions of implicit cognition to the habituation and
from attitude change caused by message repetition. In the mere
exposure paradigm, participants are repeatedly presented with boredom model would help account for nearly all of the
an attitude object and there is no obvious intent to persuade. major conclusions (noted earlier) in his meta-analysis. But
In the message-repetition paradigm, participants are repeatedly a more parsimonious contender came to light. Perceptual
provided with information about the attitude object, and thus the fluency—the notion that previously encountered stimuli are
intent to persuade is clear (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). easier to perceive and process (Jacoby, Kelley, Brown, &
404 Attitudes and Persuasion

Jasechko, 1989), and that this ease is misattributed as liking proposed that the mere exposure effect may be caused by a
(Bornstein & D’Agostino, 1994; Smith, 1998)—emerged simple conditioning effect.
as a likely candidate for explaining mere exposure effects.
More recent work, however, has cast some doubt on the Evaluative Conditioning
perceptual fluency/misattribution account, at least insofar
as it requires intentional cognitive processing. If liking Just as Pavlov’s dog came to salivate at the ring of a bell, so
is due to misattributed perceptual fluency, individuals can attitudes be formed via the repeated pairing of stimuli.
should make negative adjustments to their liking ratings Pavlovian conditioning occurs when a conditioned stim-
when they believe they have previously seen the stimuli ulus (CS; such as a bell) is consistently presented before
(Bornstein & D’Agostino, 1994). However, evidence is an unconditioned stimulus (US; such as food), and the CS
accumulating that the opposite holds true, that people pre- eventually provokes a response that was initially provoked
fer stimuli that they recognize (Anand & Sternthal, 1991; only by the US (such as salivation). EC may be procedur-
Brooks & Watkins, 1989; Fang, Singh, & Ahluwalia, ally similar to Pavlovian (classical) conditioning (Martin &
2007; Newell & Shanks, 2007; Szpunar, Schellenberg, & Levey, 1978, 1994; Levey & Martin, 1975), in which the
Pliner, 2004; Whittlesea & Price, 2001; but see Weisbuch US is an evaluatively valenced object and the CS is an
et al., 2003). Furthermore, repeated exposure to stimuli evaluatively neutral object. EC is said to occur when the
also increases liking of novel stimuli and has positive CS takes on the valence of the US. As noted later, how-
effects on mood (Monahan, Murphy, & Zajonc, 2000), nei- ever, EC also differs from classical conditioning in several
ther of which can be explained by the perceptual fluency/ important ways (de Houwer, Thomas, & Baeyens, 2001).
misattribution hypothesis but can be explained by a more In an attempt to explain the mere exposure effect,
generalized, automatic effect of fluency. Zajonc (2001) proposed that the repeated stimuli are the
The role of fluency in evaluative judgments has also conditioned stimuli and the lack of aversive experience
been proposed to be potentially independent of inten- throughout the experimental paradigm serves as a US.
tional information processing (Winkielman & Cacioppo, According to this account, novel stimuli elicit both
2001; Winkielman, Schwarz, Fazendeiro, & Reber, 2003). approach and avoidance responses. When no aversive
Winkielman and his colleagues have argued that percep- experience is paired with the CS, “avoidance and escape
tual or conceptual fluency, or both, may or may not be drop out, leaving only approach responses” (p. 226).
consciously recognized. According to the hedonic fluency However, Pickering and Gray (1999) noted that novel
model, any mental process that is fast and effortless engen- stimuli and stimuli indicating punishment or frustration
ders a positive affective response. Fluent processing, there- activate the BIS (i.e., anxiety), whereas reward and stimuli
fore, engenders positive affect, which is physiologically indicating relief from punishment activate the BAS. Thus,
detectable (Harmon-Jones & Allen, 2001; Winkielman & Monahan et al.’s (2000) single exposure condition should
Cacioppo, 2001) and may be transferred to evaluative have evoked anxiety and correspondingly more negative
judgments of previously seen stimuli, as well as novel explicit ratings of mood and novel stimuli. Contrary to this
stimuli and self-reported mood (Monahan et al., 2000). possibility, however, participants in the single-exposure
A meta-analysis of 90 studies examining the effect of per- condition did not rate the stimuli any more negatively
ceptual fluency on affective judgments obtained a medium than those in the control (no exposure) condition. The con-
effect size (Cohen’s d  .50). The effect was moderated by ditioning explanation for mere exposure effects requires
participants’ awareness of the experimental manipulation, further testing.
such that aware participants tended to discount fluency and EC effects are not large (de Houwer, Baeyens, & Field,
use other inputs to form attitudes, and the valence of the 2005) but seem to generalize across all five senses: visual
stimuli, such that neutral and positive stimuli produced (Levey & Martin, 1975), auditory (Eifert, Craill, Carey, &
larger fluency effects than negative stimuli (Warth, 2008). O’Connor, 1988), haptic (Hammerl & Fulcher, 2005), gus-
After 50 years of research on the mere exposure effect, tatory (Zellner, Rozin, Aron, & Kulish, 1983), and olfactory
the final word on explaining the effect may be near. The (Todrank, 1993). EC has also been demonstrated across
hedonic fluency model allows for both conscious and non- sensory modality, such as visual-auditory (Blair & Shimp,
conscious effects of perceptual fluency on evaluative judg- 1992), visual-olfactory (Hermans, Baeyens, & Natens,
ments. Repetition facilitates ease of processing, which, in 2000, cited in De Houwer et al., 2005), and visual-gustatory
turn, signals a positive state of affairs and induces gener- (Johnsrude, Owen, Zhao, & White, 1999). EC takes place
alized positive affect. This positivity may then influence regardless of the order in which the CS and US are paired:
evaluations of previously presented and novel stimuli, as when the CS is presented before the US (Levey & Martin,
well as a person’s mood. Recently, however, Zajonc (2001) 1975), when the CS and US are presented simultaneously
Inadverted Environmental Influences: Attitudes in Response to a Seemingly Passive Environment 405

(van Reekum, van den Berg, & Frijda, 1999), and when the also been used to propose an explanation for conflicting
US is presented before the CS (Stewart, Shimp, & Engle, results regarding contingency awareness and extinction (de
1987). Moreover, EC has been verified through observa- Houwer, 2007). EC effects driven by associative processing
tional learning (Baeyens, Eelen, Crombez, & De Houwer, might arise outside of contingency awareness, but those
2001), using both subliminal and supraliminal stimulus pre- driven by propositional processes would almost certainly
sentation (e.g., Dijksterhuis, 2004), with both explicit and be subject to contingency awareness. Similarly, EC effects
implicit attitude measures (Olson & Fazio, 2001, 2002), driven by propositional processes might be more susceptible
and both with and without contingency awareness (Olson & to extinction because participants are actively monitoring
Fazio, 2001, 2002; Pleyers, Corneille, Luminet, & Yzerbyt, stimulus presentation and forming rules about stimulus
2007). pairings. To date, however, these ideas are speculative, so
One of the ways in which EC differs from classical con- the topic remains an active and rich area of study.
ditioning is the hypothesized necessity of contingency
awareness. Classical conditioning effects are predicated on Implicit Attitudes
human learning and contingency awareness. If one does not
recognize that an electric shock always follows the ring- Implicit attitudes have been conceptualized as automatic,
ing bell, one should not develop a cringe response to the highly stable and resistant to change, developed over the
ringing bell. Current evidence suggests that EC occurs with long term, and largely because of socialization (e.g., Wilson,
awareness (e.g., Purkis & Lipp, 2001) and without aware- Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000). Consistent with such a con-
ness (Davey, 1994; Hammerl & Fulcher, 2005; Field & ceptualization, some research has found changes in explicit
Moore, 2005). Nierman (2008) meta-analyzed 50 EC stud- (more deliberate, conscious attitudes) but not implicit
ies and found mean-weighted effect sizes of d  .65 for attitudes (e.g., Gawronski & Strack, 2004). However, there
contingency-aware respondents and d  .27 for contingency- is also evidence that implicit attitudes can be changed
unaware respondents. As contingency awareness affects without a corresponding change in explicit attitudes (e.g.,
EC but does not completely eliminate it, EC may really Dasgupta & Greenwald, 2001; Karpinski & Hilton, 2001;
belong at the intersection of active and passive persuasion Olson & Fazio, 2006), and evidence that explicit attitudes
processes. A second way in which EC differs from classical change more slowly than implicit attitudes in the face of
conditioning is that classical conditioning disappears when counterattitudinal information (Rudman, Phelan, & Heppen,
the CS is no longer paired with the US (Hamm & Vaitl, 2007; Rydell & McConnell, 2006; Rydell, McConnell,
1996). In contrast, EC effects appear to resist extinction, Strain, Claypool, & Hugenberg, 2007). A meta-analysis of
even after 5 and 10 presentations of the CS without the US 126 studies examining correlations between the Implicit
(Baeyens, Crombez, van den Bergh, & Eelen, 1998; Diaz, Association Test and explicit attitude measures obtained
Ruiz, & Baeyens, 2005; but see Lipp & Purkis, 2006). an average r of .24 (Hoffmann, Gawronski, Gschwendner,
As with mere exposure effects, the mechanisms under- Le, & Schmitt, 2005). These dissociations hence suggest
lying EC effects are not yet entirely understood. Several that implicit and explicit attitudes are, indeed different, but
models have been put forth. One theory involves a holistic, the chapter on implicit attitudes (Banaji & Heiphetz, this
or Gestalt, principle in which the CS takes on the valence volume) can provide readers with much more information.
of the US simply by virtue of their spatiotemporal contigu- Differences between implicit and explicit attitudes
ity (Martin & Levey, 1978, 1994; Walther, Nagengast, & have also been observed in studies using neuroimaging.
Trasselli, 2005). Others have proposed two different sys- At least three structures appear to participate in automatic
tems at work for classical conditioning and EC (Baeyens & evaluations, namely the amygdala, the insula, and the orbito-
de Houwer, 1995; Baeyens, Eelen, & Crombez, 1995). frontal cortex (Bechara et al., 1999; Cunningham et al.,
According to this line of thought, classical conditioning is 2004; Hamann et al., 2002; Irwin et al., 1996; Kringelbach &
driven by an expectancy or signal learning system that initi- Rolls, 2004; Lane, Fink, Chau, & Dolan, 1997; Morris et al.,
ates preparatory responses (e.g., the cringe), whereas EC is 1996; Petrides, 2007; Phan et al., 2004; Reekum et al., 2007;
driven by a less sophisticated referential system that responds Rempel-Clower, 2007; Royet et al., 2003; Sergerie,
to paired events. The referential system is hypothesized to Lepage, & Armony, 2006; Taylor, Phan, Decker, & Liberzon,
automatically average the valence of the paired events. 2003; Wright et al., 2008). Correspondingly, the ACC is
Others have proposed that multiple cognitive processes, believed to be involved in controlled evaluations (Critchley,
both implicit (automatic, associative) and explicit (propo- 2005; Cunningham et al., 2003, 2004; Taylor et al., 2003).
sitions about stimulus contingencies), may be simultane- Activations of the ACC, involving both the ventral ACC
ously involved in EC effects (de Houwer et al., 2005; de (BA 24) and the dorsal ACC (BA 32), have been observed
Houwer, 2007). The multiple processes perspective has in functional neuroimaging studies that span a wide range of
406 Attitudes and Persuasion

cognitive contexts including selective attention and memory, in a restaurant. Explicit attitudes, typically measured by
and evaluation of stimuli (Cabeza & Nyberg, 1997; Egan self-report, are hypothesized to reflect the operation of a
et al., 2003; Hariri, Mattay, Tessitore, Fera, & Weinberger, superordinate reflective system that transforms associative
2003; Lane, Chua, & Dolan, 1999; Papez, 1937). There is evaluative responses into propositions (e.g., “I like lamb”)
also specific evidence of anterior cingulate activity while subject to syllogistic reasoning to appraise their legitimacy
evaluative (vs. nonevaluative) judgments of visual stimuli (see Albarracín, Hart, & McCulloch, 2006; Albarracín,
are requested (as well as activity in the temporal pole and the Noguchi, & Earl, 2006; Hart & Albarracín, 2009; Senay,
frontal operculum; Lane et al., 1997). Furthermore, a recent Albarracín, & Noguchi, in press). In many cases, initial,
study that carefully separated explicit evaluative judgments automatic evaluative responses will be deemed valid and
from implicit responses to positive and negative stimuli serve as the basis for explicit attitudes (i.e., implicit and ex-
revealed that the amygdala was involved in spontaneous plicit attitudes will be correlated); however, the engage-
responses, whereas the orbitofrontal cortex was involved in ment of reflective processing of propositional statements
explicit evaluative judgments (Wright et al., 2008). may produce implicit-explicit dissociations when the initial
These brain functional processes have also been inves- explicit evaluative response is deemed invalid.
tigated using training to produce automation in real time. Implicit attitude change, according to Gawronski and
Participants who practiced evaluating visual stimuli became Bodenhausen’s (2006) associative-propositional-evaluation
faster with time and were also more likely to make evalu- (APE) model, can occur when the evaluative associations
ative judgments spontaneously after the practice (Hong, for a given object change (as in EC research, reviewed
Albarracín, Wright, & Liu, 2008). Moreover, in a study earlier), and when contextual cues cause a different
using functional magnetic resonance imaging, practicing pattern of evaluative associations to come to mind. For
evaluation had effects on brain activation in various regions. example, implicit prejudice against African Americans
Specifically, there were posttraining increases in the activa- may decrease by exposing White participants to either
tion of brain regions associated with automatic evaluative admired African Americans and disliked Whites (vs. dis-
processing (e.g., the amygdala, the insula, and the orbito- liked African Americans and admired Whites; Dasgupta &
frontal cortex), regions associated with controlled evaluative Greenwald, 2001). Explicit attitude change, according to
processing (e.g., the temporal pole, the ACC, and the frontal the model, can occur when the evaluative associations for
operculum), and late visual regions (e.g., the posterior fusi- a given object change (see prior review of EC research),
form, the superior occipital lobe, and the parietal occipital when the propositions considered to be relevant change
lobe). Moreover, evaluation proceduralization was reflected (e.g., when new information about an object is learned;
by increased activity in areas associated with procedural Petty & Wegener, 1998), when people reassess their extant
learning (e.g., the striatal regions, the lateral cerebellum, the cognitions about an object (Wilson, Dunn, Kraft, & Lisle,
precuneus, and the inferior frontal cortices) and decreased 1989), and when efforts to achieve cognitive consistency
activity in areas associated with declarative learning (e.g., change (e.g., when one of two dissonant propositions is
the medial temporal lobe, the ventromedial prefrontal cor- rejected, or when an additional proposition helps resolve
tex, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex). Of all these the dissonance; Gawronski & Strack, 2004). Finally, this
regions, however, only evaluative regions correlated with APE model allows for influence attempts to interact with
the increase in evaluative speed that followed training. associative and propositional processing in a variety of dif-
In terms of purely psychological models, implicit ferent ways that are beyond the scope of this chapter.
and explicit attitudes are thought to reflect the operation The APE model is not the only model proposed to
of associative and propositional processes, respectively explain the formation of implicit and explicit attitudes.
(Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). Implicit attitudes, According to Bassili and Brown’s (2005) potentiated
typically measured using response-time tasks assess- recruitment framework, implicit and explicit attitudes both
ing the ease with which people can associate attitude emerge from interconnected microconcepts (“molecular
objects with positive or negative stimuli (but see Vargas, elements of knowledge that yield meaning when assembled
Sekaquaptewa, & von Hippel, 2007), are hypothesized to into networks with other microconcepts” [p. 552]). Which
reflect automatically retrieved affective responses that are microconcepts are activated, and hence which attitude is
linked to attitude objects. These responses are not assumed evoked, depends on four sources of potentiation: recent
to reflect personal endorsement, but only evaluative experience/information, context, spreading activation
thoughts that are linked with the object under consider- among microconcepts, and present cognitive activity. The
ation. Thoughts linked to an object may also be dependent potentiated recruitment framework posits that the primary
on the context; for example, “lamb” may be associa- difference between implicit and explicit attitudes is not
tively linked to “wool” on a cold day but to “dinner” that they are the result of different cognitive processes, but
Inadverted Environmental Influences: Attitudes in Response to a Seemingly Passive Environment 407

whether deliberative cognitive processing is involved in on implicit attitude measures and more careful processing
producing an output attitude. of attitude-relevant information. These findings suggested
The metacognitive model of attitudes (Petty, 2006; that the old explicit attitude continued to have an influence
Petty & Briñol, 2006; Petty, Briñol, & DeMarree, 2007; Petty, at the implicit level.
Wheeler, & Tormala, 2003) was proposed to account for The study of implicit attitudes takes us back to the
differences in implicit and explicit attitude change. Like issue of what aspects of the attitude and the environment
the associative-propositional evaluation and the potenti- enter awareness (Fazio & Olson, 2003; Vargas et al., 2007;
ated recruitment framework, this approach assumes that Vargas & von Hippel, & Petty, 2004; von Hippel, Sekaqua-
contemporary implicit measures tap automatic associative ptewa, & Vargas, 2008). Consider, for example, the implicit
evaluations, whereas traditional explicit measures rely formation of attitudes (where attitudes are formed outside of
on more deliberative information processing involving conscious awareness but are explicitly available). Evidence
attitude validity appraisals. According to the metacognitive has been reported that attitudes can be formed outside of con-
model, explicit attitudes show greater change than implicit scious awareness via exposure to value-charged information
ones when new evaluative associations are accepted (Betsch, Plessner, Schwieren, & Gütig, 2001). In one study,
and old evaluative associations are rejected, such as participants were instructed to focus on a series of advertise-
when one accepts the idea that classical music is good ments while the bottom of the screen flashed stock values.
and rejects the old, negative attitude as invalid. In con- Stocks with higher summative values were rated most highly
trast, implicit attitudes show greater change than explicit despite participants’ inability to consciously estimate sums.
ones when new evaluative associations are consciously In a follow-up study, participants formed attitudes either
rejected, such as when one learns that listening to classical implicitly or explicitly. Implicitly formed attitudes appeared
music causes brain damage, and then rejects that informa- to be based on a summative assessment of the stimuli,
tion. In this example, the brain damage message remains whereas explicitly formed attitudes appeared to be based on
associated with classical music, despite the conscious an averaging assessment of the stimuli (Betsch, Kaufmann,
rejection of that information, and this association should Lindow, Plessner, & Hoffmann, 2006).
be reflected in implicit measures. Thus, the metacognitive In the end, one of the most important puzzles related
model differs from the associative-propositional- to implicit attitudes, as well as priming, may concern how
evaluation model (but is similar to the potentiated recruit- we move from fragmentary environmental cues to a first-
ment framework) in that the formation of attitudes person propositional discourse (i.e., how we think and
depends not on associative versus propositional processes, talk to ourselves; Albarracín, Hart, & McCulloch, 2006;
but rather on the strength of the evaluative associations Albarracín, Noguchi, & Earl, 2006; Hart & Albarracín, in
and whether the associations are perceived to be valid. press; for similar points of view, see Hummel & Holyoak,
Moreover, the metacognitive model allows for both affec- 2005). On the one hand, the argument that propositional
tive (e.g., “I feel good about my attitude toward classical and associative processes are qualitatively different implies
music.”) and cognitive (e.g., “My attitude toward classi- a great gap between associating a presidential candidate
cal music is correct.”) validation of evaluative associa- with pleasant objects such as babies and learning that a
tions. The metacognitive model differs from other models presidential candidate supports pro-environment policies.
in that it allows for both attitudes and validity assessments On the other hand, the processes involved in understand-
to be stored in memory rather than constructed online as ing a verbal proposition are not diametrically different
proposed in the APE model. from those underlying simpler associations. For example,
The metacognitive model differs from other models in different word prime orders (e.g., “act-nice” vs. “nice-act”)
allowing for some degree of implicit-explicit ambivalence evoke different logical structures (a command vs. a com-
when either explicit or implicit attitudes (but not both) pliment) in an unintentional way (Albarracín, Noguchi, &
change. Petty, Tormala, Briñlol, and Jarvis (2006) induced Earl, 2006; for related findings, see Hart & Albarracín, 2009).
participants’ positive or negative attitudes toward initially From this point of view, propositions are understood by
neutral stimuli. Then, some participants were provided means of associative processes much like those observed
with new information that caused them to change their in simple automatic associations between an object (e.g., a
explicit attitudes toward the targets. These participants’ snake) and an attribute (e.g., bad).
new explicit attitudes were indistinguishable in direction
and reported ambivalence from those of participants who Direct Experiences
did not receive the new information. However, the recipi-
ents of new information did show evidence of implicit Having direct experience with an attitude object is perhaps
ambivalence, as manifested by more moderate responses the most basic, and obvious, way in which we form attitudes.
408 Attitudes and Persuasion

Direct experience is implicit in much of the research Bolton, 2002). When all else is equal, direct experience
reviewed in this section and appears to strongly influence should stimulate the use of a resulting summary attitude as
the likelihood that an attitude would be sufficiently arous- a basis for future behavior. However, direct experience
ing as to be activated when the object is encountered. In can sometimes provide mixed evidence about the desir-
one study, some participants were allowed to play with ability of an object (e.g., people are presented with neu-
examples of five types of puzzles (direct experience); how- trally valenced information, Reed et al., 2002, or products
ever, other participants were presented with examples of with standard qualities, Hoch & Ha, 1986). In these condi-
the types of puzzles already solved by another person (no tions, direct experience actually hinders attitude-behavior
direct experience  no action). All participants later indi- correspondence (Albarracín & McNatt, 2005; Glasman &
cated how interesting they found each type of puzzle. After Albarracín, 2006).
that, they were all allowed to play with the puzzles for up The ubiquity of broadband Internet has facilitated
to 15 minutes. The results from this study indicated that virtual simulations of experiences. For example, one
attitudes (the interest ratings) were stronger predictors of no longer has to actually get a haircut to experience the
actual behavior in the direct than indirect experience con- new look. Instead, one can purchase computer programs
dition (Regan & Fazio, 1977). that digitally alter one’s image in accordance with the
Fazio and his colleagues (1983) have suggested that planned hairstyle. In a study of virtual direct experience
direct experience produces high attitude-behavior correla- (Griffith & Chen, 2004), some products, such as movies
tions because attitudes formed from direct experience are and music, were easy to experience in a digital fashion.
easier to retrieve from permanent memory. Thus, direct Participants were simply presented with ads about these
experience may be analogous to repeatedly expressing products and did or did not view the product. Other prod-
or reporting one’s attitude. A meta-analysis of the behav- ucts, such as clothes and apparel, are more difficult to
ioral impact of recently formed attitudes (Glasman & experience virtually. Still, the researchers created movie
Albarracín, 2006; see also Kraus, 1995) confirmed this clips of the actual experience of, for example, view-
possibility. That is, direct experience and repeated expres- ing objects with a fictitious pair of sunglasses. Thus, for
sion/report of the attitude were correlated with faster these products as well, some conditions allowed for view-
reporting of attitudes (for direct experience, r  .60; for ing (virtual experience present), whereas others did not
repeated expression, r  .24; Glasman & Albarracín, (virtual experience absent). The results from this study
2006). Moreover, the influence of direct experience and indicated that when the experience was easy to digitalize,
attitude report on the attitude-behavior relation was medi- virtually experiencing and not experiencing the product
ated by response speed (from direct experience to response had different effects. Participants had more positive eval-
speed, r  .43; from response speed to the attitude-behavior uations and stronger intentions to buy the product after
correlation, r  .82). the virtual viewing than in the absence of virtual viewing.
Direct experience may also have a motivational effect, However, these effects disappeared when the products
leading people to form an attitude while the experi- were difficult to digitalize, in which case, attitudes and
ence develops. For one thing, direct experience does not purchasing intentions were the same with and without the
improve attitude-behavior correspondence when the bases virtual experience.
for attitudes and the behavior are different. For example,
an attitude based on experienced feelings, such as how Summary
much pleasure is associated with a video game (an affec-
tive experience), does not predict the use (or lack of use) Most of the processes studied by attitude researchers have
of this video game for career advancement or learning pur- concerned the influence of environmental information on
poses (an instrumental behavior; Millar & Tesser, 1986). attitudes, ranging from subliminal persuasion to the effects
Indeed, the attitude behavior correlation is r  .59 when of direct experience. Both implicit attitudinal responses
the experience with the attitude and the predicted behavior and explicit propositions about objects are affected when a
are similar (both instrumental or both affective), but person interacts with the surrounding world, and scholars
r  .39 when the two are dissimilar; Glasman & have attempted to understand the nature of these influ-
Albarracín, 2006). Second, as Hoch and Ha (1986; see also ences. Sometimes attitudes form through mere exposure,
Ha & Hoch, 1989; Wooten & Reed, 1998) and Albarracín with repeated presentation of a stimulus but little direct
and McNatt (2005) demonstrated, acquiring direct experi- contact with the object. Other times, attitudes form and
ence with the object is as important in guiding behavior change through direct and even virtual experience, and these
as receiving information that is unambiguous and con- attitudes appear to be highly consequential for future
sistent (i.e., evaluative diagnosticity; Reed, Wooten, & behavior.
Attitudes in Reaction to a Seemingly Active Environment 409

ATTITUDES IN REACTION TO A SEEMINGLY information, which supported participants’ preexisting


ACTIVE ENVIRONMENT attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors, and uncongenial informa-
tion, which challenged these tendencies. Although analy-
More than half of Americans believe that primates deserve ses indicated a moderate preference for congenial over
the same rights as human children, according to a survey uncongenial information (d  .36), this congeniality bias
commissioned by The Doris Day Animal League, a group was moderated by variables that affect the strength of
devoted to furthering the cause of animal rights. In 1999, defense and accuracy motivation. In support of the impor-
two thirds of Americans wanted Congress to consider a tance of defense motivation, the congeniality bias was
second impeachment trial against then-President Clinton, weaker when participants’ attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors
according to a survey commissioned by the right-wing were supported before information selection; when par-
Newsmax website. And two thirds of likely voters support ticipants’ attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors were not relevant
privatizing social security accounts, according to a sur- to important values or not held with conviction; when the
vey commissioned by the libertarian Cato Institute. All available information was low in quality; when partici-
of these findings have been presented as “facts” but were pants’ closed-mindedness was low; and when confidence
essentially purchased by the groups funding the surveys in the attitude, belief, or behavior was high. In support of
(Mooney, 2003). And the report on these suspect survey the importance of accuracy motivation, an uncongeniality
practices was published by an admittedly left-leaning peri- bias emerged when uncongenial information was relevant
odical, The American Prospect. to accomplishing a current goal.
Although environmental influences are often unde- Confidence in how one interacts with active environments
tected, people are often aware that information sources seems to be a powerful influence on information selec-
frequently spin the facts. Therefore, information recipients tion. History presents abundant examples that people who
are used to selecting information, questioning source strongly advocate and defend a given attitudinal position
credibility, and suspecting persuasive, if not overly manip- often change this position, becoming “converted” to points
ulative, intent (although we tend to do so most often when of view that are opposite to the ones they initially held.
we are faced with undesirable conclusions; see Hart et al., One reason for such changes is the degree to which indi-
2009; Kunda, 1990). Social psychologists have been pro- viduals perceive that they can defend their attitudes from
lific in tackling these phenomena in the context of selective attack (Albarracín & Mitchell, 2004; Albarracín, Wang, &
exposure, communicator-based persuasion, psychological Albarracín, in press). Presumably, people who are confident
reactance, forewarning of influence, resistance to persua- that their attitudes will survive future challenges are more
sion, and communication pragmatics. willing to examine evidence that both supports and contra-
dicts their prior attitudes (e.g., r between prior attitudes and
selection of consistent material  .35 for high trait defen-
Selective Exposure
sive confidence, from Albarracín & Mitchell, 2004, study
The availability of diverse information in an environment 3; see also J. Albarracín et al., in press). In contrast, people
does not guarantee that a person’s attitudes will be equally who doubt their defensive ability prefer proattitudinal infor-
diverse. A former United States vice president, for example, mation over materials that challenge their prior perspectives
reportedly requested that the television always be tuned to (e.g., r between prior attitudes and selection of consistent
a conservative news channel before he entered a hotel room material  .80 for low trait defensive confidence, from
(Dick Cheney’s Suite Demands, 2006). In selective expo- Albarracín & Mitchell, 2004, study 3; see also Albarracín,
sure research, the classic assumption to explain such reac- 2002; J. Albarracín et al., in press; see also Byrne, 1961;
tions is that people are motivated to defend their attitudes Olson & Zanna, 1982; for related views in other domains,
from challenges (e.g., Festinger, 1957; Olson & Stone, see Tesser, 2001). Although denial may in many ways be
2005). Selective exposure enables people to defend their a relatively primitive defense mechanism, avoiding coun-
attitudes by avoiding information likely to challenge them terattitudinal information preserves the attitudes of people
and seeking information likely to support them. Selectivity who doubt their defensive abilities (Albarracín & Mitchell,
of this type is known as a congeniality bias. 2004; J. Albarracín et al., in press).
A recent meta-analysis assessed whether exposure to
information is guided by defense or accuracy motives (Hart Source Credibility
et al., 2009; see also Noguchi, Durantini, Albarracín, &
Glasman, 2007). The studies examined information pref- Other demonstrations of reactions to a seemingly active
erences in relation to attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors in environment involve the effects of source credibility. Initial
situations that provided choices between congenial work on persuasion suggested that persuasion increases
410 Attitudes and Persuasion

in the presence of source reinforcers such as expertise, Other moderators of the effects of source credibility
trustworthiness, and likability (see Cialdini, 2001; Hovland & are involvement and amount of source information
Janis, 1959; Johnson, Maio, & Smith-McLalle, 2005). In available. For example, in one study, participants were
fact, the power of variables such as source credibility is such presented with an educational policy that would either
that the influence of experts facilitating behavioral interven- affect them or not. In addition, the source description,
tions to reduce HIV infection exceeds that of peers in almost which was either 1 page or 50 words in length, intro-
every population group (women and men, ethnic majority duced an expert in testing (high-credibility condition)
and minority; Durantini, Albarracín, Earl, & Mitchell, 2006; or an expert in fitness (low-credibility condition). As
e.g., behavior change mean d  .46 vs. .14 for experts vs. expected, the influence of the source depended on both
peers in adult samples). Moreover, these expertise effects involvement and description length. Brief source descrip-
are more stable across populations than the effects of the tions had greater impact than argument quality when
content of behavioral interventions (Albarracín, McNatt, involvement was low, but long descriptions had the same
et al., 2003; Albarracín, Gillette, et al., 2005; Albarracín, impact as the communication arguments when involve-
Durantini, & Earl, 2006) and are mediated by the creation ment was high. In other words, long source descriptions
of positive behavioral norms. For example, among female were processed as a persuasive argument, requiring high
and African American samples, source expertise correlated processing motivation (Pierro, Mannetti, Erb, Spiegel, &
an average r of .50 with social norms, and social norms cor- Kruglanski, 2005).
related an average r of .65 with behavior change. Despite considerable reliance on personal source
Source characteristics are often used as simple cues to information, the actual message presented by a source may
either accept or reject a communication without much scru- be more revealing than message recipients’ attributions
tiny of the offered arguments (Chaiken, 1987; Livingston, about source credibility. People believe that facial cues
2001; Petty & Wegener, 1998). That is, by applying the such as deviated gaze provide access to a source’s deception
heuristic that “experts can be trusted,” people can circumvent but are better at judging credibility in the absence of such
effortful decisions about the validity of the actual message cues (DePaulo, Stone, & Lassiter, 1985; Zuckerman,
arguments (Chaiken, 1987). This heuristic mode of process- DePaulo, & Rosenthal, 1981; Zuckerman & Driver, 1985).
ing requires an accessible heuristic and a readily available More useful information about deception is transmitted in
cue but can otherwise be applied under varying conditions speech errors and hesitations than in facial expressions.
of motivation and ability to process the communication Although very strong motivation to lie appears to pro-
(Chaiken, 1987; for a review, see Johnson et al., 2005). duce facial cues that experts at lie detection can identify
Despite the importance of heuristic influences of (DePaulo & Friedman, 1999), people who lack objective
source credibility, the processes triggered by a credible or information about a source are generally better off turning
trustworthy source turn out to be fairly complex (Petty & to the message for reliable credibility information.
Wegener, 1998). For example, source credibility has a much
stronger influence on attitudes when message recipients form Belief and Evaluations of the Content
attitudes toward new topics than when they change attitudes of Persuasive Messages
toward well-known topics (e.g., mean d  .73 and .33;
Kumkale, Albarracín, & Poyner-del Vento, in press). In addi- Propositional reasoning has been a classic topic for social
tion, the effect of source credibility depends on when the psychologists who study persuasion. For example, Hovland
information is presented. When the source is presented before (1959), McGuire (1968, 1985), and Wyer (1974) all con-
the message arguments, expertise can bias the way in which the tributed to the current understanding of the sequence of
arguments are processed (Heesacker, Petty, & Cacioppo, 1983; activities that take place when people process a persua-
Kumkale & Albarracín 2004; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986; sive message (see Albarracín, 2002; Johnson et al., 2005;
Petty & Wegener, 1998; see also Albarracín, Wallace, & Kruglanski & Stroebe, 2005; Wyer & Albarracín, 2005).
Glasman, 2004). Recipients are likely to dismiss arguments Hovland proposed that the processing of a persuasive mes-
coming from a noncredible source but attend to those coming sage involves three phases: (1) attention, (2) comprehen-
from a credible one (Kumkale & Albarracín, 2004; Petty & sion, and (3) acceptance of the message content. Attention
Cacioppo, 1986).In contrast, when presented after the mes- and comprehension concern learning or content retention,
sage arguments, the source can offer a heuristic for accepting whereas acceptance involves motivation (e.g., rewards) to
or rejecting the message. In this situation, the effect of the accept or reject the message conclusion.
source is fairly superficial and short lived because the source Hovland’s (1959) distinction of these three phases of
information is not integrated into the message representation influence was critical to analyze a number of phenomena.
(Kumkale & Albarracín, 2004). For example, extreme fear appeals increase attention to the
Attitudes in Reaction to a Seemingly Active Environment 411

message content but defeat themselves by inducing anxiety people presumably attend to a message and understand
(Janis & Milholland, 1954). Consistent with this idea, its arguments but also counterargue more than less intelli-
research on the effects of campaigns to reduce HIV risk gent people. Ultimately, these opposite influences predict
and improve attitudes toward protection has demonstrated that people of moderate intelligence should be more per-
greater efficacy when fear-inducing arguments are absent suadable than people of either low or high intelligence (see
rather than present (Albarracín, McNatt, et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005; Kruglanski & Stroebe, 2005; Wyer &
Albarracín, Gillette, et al., 2005; Earl & Albarracín, 2007). Albarracín, 2005).
Brain activity, as measured by event-related potentials, Wyer (1974) further elaborated McGuire’s (1972) model
suggests that fear-inducing messages cause people to avert using conditional probabilities (see Albarracín, 2002;
their attention away from the message (Ruiter, Kessels, Johnson et al., 2005; Kruglanski & Stroebe, 2005; Wyer &
Jansma, & Brug, 2006). Albarracín, 2005). In the new context, the probability of a
McGuire (1968, 1972) developed Hovland’s (1959) message’s influence is a function of the probability of
theorizing by capitalizing on a cognitive perspective that being influenced given that one receives the communi-
delineates a series of information-processing stages (see cation plus the probability of being influenced when one
Albarracín, 2002; Johnson et al., 2005; Kruglanski & does not receive it. That is,
Stroebe, 2005; Wyer & Albarracín, 2005). These stages
include: (1) exposure (i.e., E ), (2) attention (A). (3) PI  PR PI/R  PR' PI/R [3].
comprehension (C). (4) yielding (Y). (5) retention (R). and
(6) behavior (B). Messages can be effective only if pre- The probability of being influenced, assuming that one
sented to potential recipients, who themselves attend to receives the communication, can be rewritten as the probabi-
and comprehend the conclusions and arguments discussed lity of yielding. Yielding, in turn, depends on the prob-
in the message. Recipients must also agree with the con- ability of yielding when one successfully counterargues
clusion of the communication, retain this yielding over a the communication (PY /CA), as well as the probability of
period of time, and behave in the manner recommended by yielding when one fails to refute it (PY/CA'). Then, Equation
the communication. As a result, the success of the message 3 can be restated as follows:
can depend on its behavioral impact. That is,
PI  PR(PCA PY/CA  PCA'PY/CA)  PR' PI/R [4].
PI  PE  PA  PC  PY  PR  PB [1],
In this equation, the parenthetical term is a function of
where PI is the probability of influence of the persuasive the strength of the arguments in the message (see Petty &
message, and the remaining probabilities are associated Cacioppo, 1986). Stronger arguments should induce less
with the previously defined stages. counterarguing, and thus be more persuasive, but other
Later, McGuire (1968) generated a simpler version of factors may influence yielding and counterarguing as
his stage model (see Albarracín, 2002; Johnson et al., 2005; well. For example, the affect recipients experience for
Kruglanski & Stroebe, 2005; Wyer & Albarracín, 2005). reasons unrelated to the persuasive message can increase
With the methods available at the time, he observed or decrease yielding (see e.g., Albarracín & Wyer, 2001;
difficulties in measuring attention and comprehension Albarracín, 2002), making the message persuasive irre-
as separate processes, and synthesized these processes as spective of counterarguing (PY/CA  PY/CA').
well as exposure under the overall label of “reception” (R). Since 2000, researchers have tried to specify with greater
With this reduction, detail the processes involved in yielding (Albarracín, 2002;
Albarracín & Wyer, 2000, 2001; Brown & Albarracín, 2005;
PI  PR  PY [2], Crano & Prislin, 2005). In some formulations, the implica-
tions of both message-related and knowledge-based beliefs
where the probability of influence is a multiplicative and evaluations combine to form an attitude (A). This com-
function of the probability of receiving the message and the bination may be done in the manner postulated by Fishbein
probability of yielding to the message. This two-step model and Ajzen (1975; see Albarracín, Johnson, Fishbein, &
has important empirical implications. In particular, when Muellerleile, 2001; Ajzen & Fishbein, 2005). That is,
a situational or personal factor has the same influence on
both reception and yielding, the factor ’s influence should A  be [5],
be monotonic. In contrast, when a factor has opposite
influences on reception and yielding, the factor ’s influ- where A is attitude, b is a belief in an outcome or attribute,
ence should be curvilinear. For example, more intelligent and e is the evaluation of this outcome or attribute. This
412 Attitudes and Persuasion

attitude, together with other possible factors (e.g., social processes include message-learning approaches and
norms or perceptions of control; see Ajzen & Madden, 1986; expectancy value approaches, such as those described ear-
Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) then influences the recipients’ lier, as well as information integration theory (Anderson,
intention to perform the behavior (e.g., average r between 1971, 1981), and even cognitive dissonance theory. Low-
attitude and intention  .58; Albarracín et al., 2001). Later, effort processes include classical conditioning and EC,
this intention provides the basis for their future actions (see self-perception theory (Bem, 1972), and mere exposure.
also McGuire, 1985; e.g., average r between intention and In addition, the ELM has been used to organize different
behavior  .45; Albarracín et al., 2001). ways in which persuasion-related variables (source,
One question relevant to message processing is whether message, recipient, context) can impact attitude change
outcome-specific beliefs are formed before the outcomes (Petty & Wegener, 1998). First, persuasion-related vari-
are evaluated or afterward. Over three experiments, ables may serve as “arguments relevant to determining the
Albarracín and Wyer (2001) found that people who are merits of an object or position,” such as when an attractive
able to think about the arguments contained in the message source endorses a cosmetic product. The source’s attrac-
first form beliefs and evaluations of behavioral outcomes tiveness may serve as an argument supporting the efficacy
and then integrate the implications of these cognitions into of the product (Shavitt, Swan, Lowery, & Wänke, 1994).
their attitudes. In one of these studies, for example, the time Second, variables may also bias attitude-relevant informa-
taken to report outcome-specific beliefs and evaluations tion processing, such as when an attractive source causes
was analyzed as a function of presentation order and type people to interpret neutral information more favorably.
of reported cognition (beliefs vs. evaluations). Outcome Third, variables can serve as peripheral cues, such as when
beliefs and outcome evaluations were, on average, reported an attractive source is judged reason enough to buy a product
equally quickly, but evaluations were made more quickly (Cialdini, 1987). And fourth, variables can influence the
when beliefs had been reported beforehand than when they amount of cognitive elaboration in which people engage,
had not. Thus, these findings provided some support for such as when an attractive source causes people to attend
the hypothesis that outcome beliefs are formed before out- more carefully to the central arguments in a persua-
come evaluations (see also Gilbert, 1991). sive message (DeBono & Harnish, 1988; Puckett, Petty,
Details about the type and order of cognitions that Cacioppo, & Fisher, 1983). In general, the ELM framework
follow a persuasive message nicely complement classic has been an exceptionally powerful and robust approach to
findings from the elaboration likelihood model (ELM; the study of persuasion.
Petty & Cacioppo, 1981, 1986). The ELM is a theory about
how cognitive processes mediate the relation between Power, Authority, and Social Influence
persuasive stimuli and actual persuasion. According to this
model, message recipients’ cognitive responses to persua- Power has been conceptualized as the ability to influence
sive messages can vary along a continuum of elaboration another via control of resources or punishments (for
likelihood. When people are both motivated and able to reviews see Fiske, volume 2; French & Raven, 1959;
think carefully about the message (i.e., they engage in high Kelman, 1958; Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003; for
elaboration), they tend to scrutinize central issues, such as an alternate perspective see Turner, 2005). Although power
the quality of the arguments presented in a message. This may belong to the persuading agent, the message recipi-
processing is also known as the “central route” to persua- ent, or both, most research on the relation between power
sion. If people lack either motivation or ability (i.e., they and persuasion has focused on the power of the persuad-
engage in low elaboration), they tend to rely on factors ing agent (e.g., Festinger & Thibaut, 1951) and how that
other than their thoughts about the central issues, such power affects attitude change.
as characteristics of the person delivering the message. French and Raven (1959) differentiated five sources of
This processing is also known as the “peripheral route” power. Legitimate power reflects the recipient’s belief that
to persuasion. Both central and peripheral routes (or high the influencing agent has a right to direct behavior, as when
and low elaboration, respectively) can lead to persuasion, a police officer directs traffic. Referent power reflects the
but central route processing generally leads to stronger recipient’s liking of, or “feeling of oneness” (p. 161), with
attitudes that are more carefully integrated with extant the agent, exemplified by the power of an admired celebrity
cognitive structures, more resistant to subsequent persua- to influence purchasing decisions. Expert power reflects
sive attacks, and more likely to guide behavior. the recipient’s belief that the agent has superlative skills or
The ELM has been used to organize different knowledge, or both, as in a typical student–teacher relation-
approaches to studying attitude change into high- and ship. Reward and coercive power reflect the agent’s abil-
low-effort processes (Petty & Wegener, 1998). High-effort ity to administer rewards and punishments, respectively.
Attitudes in Reaction to a Seemingly Active Environment 413

A sixth source, informational power. is not derived from undergo training in skills (e.g., planning) that allow them
perceptions of the source but rather the information deliv- to overcome environmental obstacles than when they
ered by a source, such as the quality of the arguments do not (for audiences with African background, average
(Deutsch & Gerard, 1955; Raven, 1965). d  .58 vs. .16 when this training is and is not present;
Kelman (1958, 1961, 1974, 2006) proposed three Albarracín, Gillette, et al., 2005). These aspects, however,
processes of influence. Compliance is a form of pub- are not as important to the less deprived ethnic major-
lic acceptance in which recipients change their attitudes ity groups (for audiences with European background,
to gain a favorable (or prevent an unfavorable) response average d  .35 vs. .05 when this training is and is not
from the agent. Attitude change because of compliance is present; Albarracín, Gillette, et al., 2005). In addition,
unlikely to be permanent, being present only in front of the samples with lower social power change their behavior
agent and potentially producing private rejection (Brehm & more in response to expert than lay sources, presum-
Brehm, 1981). Identification may elicit private acceptance ably because professional experts are a door to resources
through the recipient’s desire to “establish or maintain a (Durantini et al., 2006). Specifically, when experts (e.g.,
positive self-defining relationship to another person or physicians, nurses, and psychologists) deliver a behavior-
a group,” (Kelman, 1958, p. 53). Attitude change because change program, the average behavior-change d values
of identification is likely to persist to the extent that the are .44 and .35 for predominantly African-background
relationship with the agent is salient. Internalization elicits and female audiences, respectively (Durantini et al.,
private acceptance because the recipient finds the ideas or 2006). In contrast, when lay community members (e.g.,
behaviors, or both, intrinsically appealing and consistent peer counselors) deliver a behavior-change program, the
with the recipient’s values. Attitude change because of average behavior-change d values are .14 and .15 for
internalization persists beyond the recipient’s relationship predominantly African-background and female audi-
with the agent. ences, respectively (Durantini et al., 2006).
Although relatively little work has been done on the Even the style of the message itself may be characterized
effect of the power of the message recipient, there has as powerful or powerless, independent of the quality or
been work on the influence of power on the power holder. substance of the arguments. A powerless message may
Drawing on research on BIS/BAS, and Higgins’s (1997, include tag questions (e.g., “isn’t that right?”), as well as
1999) work on regulatory focus, Keltner et al. (2003) verbal and nonverbal hesitations (e.g., “let’s see here” and
proposed that increased power activates approach-related “um”), which are absent in powerful messages. Important
tendencies (e.g., increased sensitivity to rewards, posi- for our analysis, powerless messages tend to evoke less
tive affect, automatic social cognition), and that decreased favorable attitudes than powerful ones (Areni & Sparks,
power activates avoidance tendencies (e.g., increased sen- 2005; Sparks & Areni, 2008).
sitivity to punishment, negative affect, controlled social Finally, power is closely related to authority, another
cognition). Message recipients who feel powerful may characteristic that can have a profound influence on attitu-
become more confident in their prior attitudes or become dinal and behavioral change. An authority is a figure who
more likely to agree with the message advocacy (Briñol, has acquired special status through experience, education,
Petty, Valle, Rucker, & Becerra, 2007). Recipients made special talents, or other means (Cialdini, 2001). An author-
to feel powerful before the persuasive attempt resisted ity may not have the ability to directly administer rewards
persuasion because they felt confident in their preexisting or punishments (e.g., the author of a book on astronomy)
ideas. In contrast, recipients made to feel powerful after a but may nevertheless be perceived as a legitimate source of
persuasive attempt were more persuaded because they felt information and behavioral guide. Milgram’s (1963, 1974)
confident in the new ideas engendered by the persuasive famous research on obedience to authority demonstrated
message. the profound impact of authorities on behavior, if not
The source of information people use as a basis for behavior attitudes (see also Bickman, 1974; Doob & Gross, 1968;
also appears to vary with the actual social power (access Hofling, Brotzman, Dalrymple, Graves, & Pierce, 1966;
to resources) of an audience (Albarracín, Durantini, & Lefkowitz, Blake, & Mouton, 1955). Despite much past
Earl, 2006; Albarracín, Gillette, et al., 2005; Albarracín, research in this area, relatively little work has been done
Kumkale, & Johnson, 2004; Durantini et al., 2006). For in recent years, and many of Kelman’s hypotheses about
example, the less power a population has (ethnic minor- power remain untested (Kelman, 2006). As operation-
ity, women, impoverished groups), the more important alizations of authority are relatively easy to create (e.g.,
skills and actual resource provision become (Albarracín, uniforms, titles; see Cialdini, 2001), the attitudinal impact
Gillette, et al., 2005). For example, ethnic minority audi- of authority is an area that should continue to attract
ences increase their condom use behavior more when they attention in the years to come.
414 Attitudes and Persuasion

Psychological Reactance somebody else’s rather than their own decisions. According
to predictions, participants who reported regret anticipated
Perhaps the most provocative statement about people’s greater regret about negative outcomes following defiance
attitudinal responses to a seemingly active environment than compliance. Nonetheless, these participants actually
comes from the theory of psychological reactance (Brehm, complied with the other person’s choices, whereas nonre-
1966; Brehm & Sensenig, 1966). According to this view, porters displayed the typical reactance effect. In summary,
when a person feels free to enact a given behavior, elimi- although cognitive mechanisms coexist with a pure inter-
nating or threatening to eliminate this freedom can instill personal effect, research to date has failed to substantiate a
psychological reactance. For example, constraints to freely role for anticipated regret.
choosing between A and B may arise if one is told to choose Cognitive mechanisms that have received support, how-
A as opposed to B. The reactance associated with a mild ever, include counterarguing and perceptions of source
constraint (on a single choice) may increase consideration credibility. Silvia (2006) investigated these processes by
of A as potentially better than B. The reactance associated manipulating the order of presentation of the coercion
with a high degree of constraint, however, may trigger (see earlier section on Source Credibility; Kumkale &
rejection of A over B. In Brehm and Sensenig’s (1966) Albarracín, 2004). Two persuasive messages were designed
classic study, participants were asked to choose between to persuade recipients of a choice of major. Half of the
pairs of paintings and were told that another person had participants were instructed to agree with the recommen-
written his or her choices of paintings. In the control dation at the beginning of the message and half at the end
condition, the note simply stated, “I prefer A (or B);” in the of the message. When the freedom-threatening statement
threat condition, the note stated, “I think we should both was at the beginning, the influence of the threat versus
choose A (or B).” In addition, the threat condition could control message was mediated by counterarguments
be about only one pair (low implication) or all pairs (high and perceptions that the source was untrustworthy and
implication). The results from this seminal study indicated inexpert. At the moment, it is not clear whether these cog-
persuasion in the threat conditions. When the note simply nitive responses comprise a motivated attempt to regain
stated a preference, then participants were likely to go along freedom or a cold inference based on an unfavorable
with it. In the threat conditions, however, the pattern dif- impression of the communicator. Given current atten-
fered. Participants were equally likely to choose A or B in tion to cold and hot reasoning processes, however, future
the low-implication condition (OR  .67) but more likely work is likely to provide further answers to this important
to choose B in the high-implication condition (OR  .47). question.
In summary, unambiguous reactance emerged when the Silvia (2006) also found a direct influence of the threat
choices on all pairs of paintings were constrained. on agreement when the coercive statement was at the end.
Considerable evidence has accumulated in support of However, interpretational ambiguities exist with a direct
psychological reactance. In particular, there is little defi- effect when only counterarguments are measured (“Were
ance when people reaffirm their freedom by channels other you criticizing the essay while you were reading it?”).
than resistance, including restoration of their freedom by Without requesting cognitive responses more broadly, it
another (Worchel & Brehm, 1971), prior exercise of their is unclear whether message recipients were consciously
freedom (Snyder & Wicklund, 1976), and exercise of aware of the persuasive intent, and if so, whether they
choice with respect to the form of compliance (Heilman & were aware of their reactance to it. In a study of organ
Garner, 1975). Nonetheless, it is possible that people’s defi- donation (Reinhart, Marshall, Feeley & Tutzauer, 2007),
ance is a public maneuver to avoid the impression of being loss-frame messages emphasizing the number of lost lives
easily manipulated (Brehm & Mann, 1975; Heilman & in the absence of donation were compared with gain-
Garner, 1975). This possibility has received mixed support, frame messages emphasizing the number of saved lives
going from finding similar effects of coercion on publicly in the presence of donation. In this study, both reported
and privately expressed decisions (Brehm & Mann, 1975) psychological reactance (“It irritates me that the message
to finding reduced reactance only when the manipulator told me how to feel about organ and tissue donation,”
is aware of other free choices by the actor (Baer, Hinkle, Reinhart et al., 2007, p. 251) and perceived persuasive
Smith, & Fenton, 1980). intent (“The creator of this message tried to manipulate me
Anticipated regret has also been implicated in defi- in ways I did not like,” Reinhart et al., 2007, p. 251)
ance of coercion, but there has been no support for this mediated the influence of framing on favorable reactions
possibility. Crawford, McConnell, Lewis, and Sherman to the message (a mixture of feelings about and per-
(2002) observed that anticipated regret could explain the ceived importance of the issue, and attitudes toward the
phenomenon if people expect greater regret from following message).
Attitudes in Reaction to a Seemingly Active Environment 415

It is nevertheless unlikely that reactance requires aware- Malone, 1993), then low involvement may produce a posi-
ness. Contrary to Reinhart et al.’s (2007) studies, which tive effect via identification, whereas high involvement
were conducted with undergraduates, other studies suggest may produce a negative effect via discounting. Following
that reactance may emerge with a lack of awareness. Studies Albarracín and Kumkale’s (2003) logic, future research may
conducted in the general population have recently uncovered reveal that further decreases in motivation or ability to think
reactance-type effects without any evidence of awareness of about this influence actually eliminates forewarning effects.
experienced reactance. For example, a field study conducted
by Albarracín, Durantini, Earl, Gunnoe, and Leeper, (2008) Resistance to Persuasion
showed that when health-promotion counseling was offered
by deemphasizing the likelihood of behavior change, enroll- Perceiving persuasive intent is generally sufficient for a
ment in the program was greater than when behavior change host of resistance processes to unfold. Audiences aban-
was emphasized (e.g., 60% vs. 41% enrollment). However, don the communicator and avoid similar messages in the
items measuring the degree to which the offer was imposing future (Festinger, 1964; Hart et al., 2009), and actively
failed to mediate the effect of the program introduction on counterargue the message in an attempt to resist its influ-
enrollment. Moreover, as being subliminally exposed to the ence (Sagarin & Cialdini, 2004; Wegener & Carlston,
name of a controlling significant other seems to elicit reac- 2005). Interestingly, the initial research on attempts to
tance (Chartrand, Dalton, & Fitzsimons, 2007), reactance counter an external influence had the objective of studying
may unfold in an automatic fashion. experimental participants who act contrary to the exper-
imenter ’s hypothesis (Weber & Cook, 1972). Masling
(1966) described this effect as a participant’s “screw you”
Forewarning of Influence Intent
reaction to the suspicion that the experimenter is attempt-
People who are forewarned of an upcoming influence ing to control their minds. In a classic set of two studies
attempt are sometimes more likely to resist this (Christensen, 1977), participants were induced to suspect,
influence (for more on influence, see Hogg, volume 2). For actually experience, or neither suspect nor experience
example, Dean, Austin, and Watts (1971, study 1) either psychological manipulation. In the suspicion condition,
did or did not inform participants that they would rate how the experimenter stated that psychological experiments
persuaded they were after receiving a message (i.e., a fore- often include procedures to manipulate behavior with-
warning of persuasive intent). The importance of the topic out the participants’ knowledge. In the experience condi-
(presidential elections vs. a health issue) varied, as did the tion, participants were asked to copy telephone numbers.
high or low status of the source. Interestingly, forewarning While participants wrote numbers, the experimenter told
decreased persuasion by the high-status source but not by them that fast copying was indicative of an obsessive-
the low-status source. compulsive personality disorder, which they later learned
A meta-analysis of the forewarning literature (Wood & was a calculated strategy to diminish their performance.
Quinn, 2003), however, has clarified the contribution of The third condition had neither instructions nor experi-
involvement to forewarning effects. In this meta-analysis, ence manipulations. Participants in these three conditions
there was an average shift in line with the expected message then underwent a verbal conditioning task (Taffel, 1955)
of d  .37, but the effect was highly variable. Three pooled during which the experimenter attempted to condition
studies indicated that forewarning produced resistance some responses by repeating “good.” The experimenter ’s
(a boomerang effect of the message) when involvement statements should normally increase the occurrence of the
was high but slight agreement when involvement was low. reinforced responses, but participants’ resistance may actu-
According to the authors, while one awaits a believed-to- ally eliminate this effect. As expected, participants were
be-persuasive message, both resisting and agreeing can conditioned only when participants did not have a recent
serve to reduce the threat posed by impending attacks manipulative experience.
against personal attitudes. This moderating effect raises The fascinating phenomenon of resistance to an
questions about the processes elicited by forewarning. external influence eventually received the attention of
Resistance may actively recruit counterarguments, thus social psychologists. For example, McGuire (1964)
requiring motivation to produce specific thoughts (Petty & directly manipulated the motivation to self-defend from a
Cacioppo, 1986). Alternatively, people may need involve- persuasive message by having participants counterargue
ment to reject the message without further elaboration. weak forms of the message before a subsequent stronger
For example, if identifying and then discounting the influ- attack. According to him, the process of counterarguing
ence each require increasing levels of ability (Albarracín should not only increase refutational ability but also the
& Kumkale, 2003; Gilbert, 1991; Gilbert, Tafarodi, & motivation to self-defend from the attack. Although the two
416 Attitudes and Persuasion

processes could not be disentangled in McGuire’s work, than those who were free to list any (positive, negative,
the refutational practice did, in fact, decrease vulnerability and/or neutral) thoughts. Findings indicated that partici-
to a subsequent attack. pants were persuaded regardless of what thoughts they
Recent work by Sagarin, Cialdini, Rice, and Serna listed, probably because the ad was difficult to refute.
(2002) has advanced our understanding of the role of However, participants who attempted to resist the message
motivation in defending our attitudes against influence (and failed) were more confident in their favorable attitude
attempts. Participants received training for discriminating toward the product than those who did not make an effort
between legitimate and illegitimate sources based on to resist persuasion (see also Schwarz et al., 1991). In other
whether the source has expertise in a particular domain. words, confidence was established in relation to prior reac-
This treatment was designed to leave participants aware tions to the message.
that there was a potential influence source, able to Research conducted by Albarracín, Cohen, and Kumkale
discriminate between legitimate and illegitimate sources, (2003) also suggests that message recipients consider the
and also willing to make this discrimination. As predicted, degree to which a prior message was persuasive. In this
the treatment increased persuasion for legitimate sources research, participants received a message that recom-
and decreased it for illegitimate sources. Other studies of mended either abstinence from or moderation in the use of
the same series confirmed that the perception of undue a new alcohol-type of product. After reading these mate-
manipulative intent decreased persuasion directly and rials, participants either tried the product or performed a
by mediating influences on specific counterarguments filler task before reporting their intentions to drink in the
of the message content. This conclusion was also consis- future. As predicted, participants who did not try the prod-
tent with independent findings that distrusting a source uct reported similar intentions to drink when they received
engenders automatic semantic associations that are the moderation message and when they received the absti-
incongruent with the source’s message (Schul, Mayo, & nence message (d  .03). In contrast, when participants tried
Burnstein, 2004). the product after receiving the message, recipients of the
Sources of influence may also be rejected when they are abstinence message had significantly stronger intentions to
perceived as being hypocritical. More than 1,500 inner-city drink than recipients of the moderation message (d  .76).
high-school students were assigned to different conditions of One interpretation of these results is that participants
an HIV prevention-intervention trial: The key conditions inferred their intentions after considering their trial behavior
were a teacher-led intervention and a student-led interven- vis-à-vis an external influence (the message recommenda-
tion (Fisher, Fisher, Bryan, & Misovich, 2002). Findings tion). Apparently, trying the product after a strong recom-
revealed that, compared with the teacher-led interven- mendation led participants to conclude that they truly liked
tion, the peer-led intervention was more effective at the the forbidden product. Another potential interpretation is
3-month follow-up but less effective at the 1-year follow- that the discrepancy made people anxious and thus more
up. Apparently, over the course of a year, the student inter- likely to resolve these feelings by justifying their drinking
ventionists were perceived as hypocritical because they behavior (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959). Although this par-
displayed some of the behaviors they had previously tried ticular interpretation seemed less plausible than the self-
to discourage in their peers. This perception of the source perception account based on additional data, both processes
as hypocritical presumably decreased the effectiveness of are relevant to situations in which the recipient’s behavior
the peer-led intervention. contradicts an earlier persuasive message.
Reactions to perceived influence intent are in many ways
similar to phenomena identified in the context of cogni-
Reflecting on Resistance and
tive dissonance. In Festinger and Carlsmith’s (1959) classic
Cognitive Dissonance
study, male participants worked for 1 hour on boring tasks
Another interesting facet of reacting against a seemingly such as turning spools on a board. Immediately after this task,
active environment is that the influence source provides participants were told that the experimenter was investigat-
an anchor for reflecting about attitudes. Rucker and Petty ing the effects of expectancies on performance. They were
(2002) presented participants with a strong ad promoting a further informed that they were in a control condition that
pharmaceutical product and instructed participants either did not receive any information before beginning the tasks.
to list negative thoughts (for an introduction to the tech- However, they learned that other participants were to receive
nique to induce biased thoughts, see Killeya & Johnson, information designed to create a positive expectation about
1998) or to simply list their thoughts about the message. the task. The positive expectations were ostensibly instilled
Presumably, participants who listed only negative thoughts by asking the participants to tell another student that the task
attempted to resist the communication to a greater extent was enjoyable. Importantly, participants were offered either
Attitudes in Reaction to a Seemingly Active Environment 417

$1 or $20 for providing a positive evaluation of the task to the Participants in the high-choice conditions showed vary-
other participant (actually an experimenter’s accomplice). ing degrees of attitude change. Those in the “tenseness”
Results of how enjoyable the task was to the partici- condition showed minimal attitude change; they, too, were
pants depended on the amount of money they received able to make an external attribution (to the placebo) for the
in exchange for lying. Participants who were paid $1 for physiological arousal induced by their dissonant behavior.
describing the experiment as enjoyable rated the tasks as Those in the “no side effect” condition showed moderate
more enjoyable than did participants who were paid $20. attitude change, consistent with other research on cognitive
Festinger and Carlsmith (1959) argued that participants dissonance. Finally, those in the “relaxation” condition
who lied experienced dissonance created by the cognitions showed a great deal of attitude change; they “showed an
“The tasks were boring” and “I told someone the tasks increased need to deal with their arousal by changing their
were enjoyable.” Those who were paid $20, however, had opinions” (Zanna & Cooper, 1974, p. 707).
an important consonant cognition in “I was paid a lot of
money to tell someone the tasks were enjoyable.” This
Communication Pragmatics
awareness of an environmental influence was sufficient to
reduce the dissonance magnitude and the associated influ- A final fascinating aspect of reactions to seemingly active
ence of the behavior on private reports. environments entails the use of the actual content of a
Cognitive dissonance theorists gave arousal and communication as a source of information about the influ-
perceived self-determination central roles in attitude change ence source. In a study conducted by Rosen, Cochran, and
(Olson & Stone, 2005), and highlight how awareness of a Musser (1990), participants read recommendation letters
reward’s influence can increase the level of arousal associ- about a job applicant, as well as the applicants’ (posi-
ated with an object. In particular, arousal measured with tive) self-descriptions. These self-descriptions positively
skin conductance is greater when participants undergo influenced impressions of the applicant only when the
typical dissonance manipulations and have no opportunity recommendation letters were favorable. In contrast, these
to reduce this dissonance (Croyle & Cooper, 1983; Elkin & descriptions negatively influenced these impressions
Leippe, 1986). This finding has received repeated support when the recommendation letters were unfavorable.
(see Harmon-Jones, Brehm, Greenberg, Simon, & Nelson, Presumably the self-descriptions were perceived as deceit-
1996) and is generally taken as an indication that affective ful or manipulative when they contradicted other informa-
feelings are necessary for the experience and consequences tion about the source of these self-descriptions.
of cognitive dissonance. Even more definitive, however, Wyer, Budesheim, and Lambert (1990) provided a sys-
has been evidence from studies using misattribution para- tematic analysis of communication pragmatics. In this
digms (Zanna & Cooper, 1974). For example, in one study, framework, people are supposed to spontaneously form
participants engaged in belief-discrepant behavior, and for impressions of communicators, even in the absence of
some, physiological measures of arousal were obtained instructions to do so. For this reason, the descriptions a
(Croyle & Cooper, 1983). When physiological measures speaker provides about another person often say more about
were obtained, participants’ beliefs were not affected by the speaker than the target person. Disparaging others, for
their behavior, presumably because they attributed their example, creates the impression that the speaker is intol-
experienced arousal to the elaborate measurement appara- erant and unfriendly. Interestingly, however, the content
tus. In contrast, when no such measures were obtained, par- of a communication has different pragmatic implications
ticipants supposedly attributed their feelings to the behavior depending on the source type. Gruenfeld and Wyer (1992)
and changed their beliefs accordingly. Participants were examined the influence of affirmations and denials in differ-
either instructed to write a counterattitudinal essay (low ent communication contexts. In this work, message sources
choice) or politely asked to write a counterattitudinal essay were manipulated to be either a newspaper or a volume that
(high choice). Also, all participants were given a placebo records archival knowledge. Both sources affirmed and
tablet. Some were informed that the pill would “produce a denied propositions that participants would normally assume
reaction of tenseness,” others that the pill would “produce to be untrue (e.g., the presidential candidate did not have an
a reaction of relaxation,” and others that the pill had no affair). When the message source was a newspaper, affirming
side effects (Zanna & Cooper, 1974, p. 705). Participants and denying statements were taken as informative, resulting
in the low-choice conditions showed minimal attitude in participants’ beliefs in both the affirmed and denied reports
change; for low-choice participants, writing the essay was (d for difference between affirmations and denials  .08).
not a counterattitudinal behavior because they were able to In contrast, when the ostensible source was an archive,
make an external attribution for their dissonant behavior only affirming statements informed beliefs (d for differ-
(e.g., “the experiment required that I write that essay”). ence between affirmations and denials  1.12). Presumably,
418 Attitudes and Persuasion

recipients believed newspapers to make statements for a Ajzen, I., & Madden, J. T. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior:
Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of
reason but archives to provide redundant information. In this
Experimental Social Psychology. 22, 453–474.
context, recipients believed that the newspaper denials were Albarracín, D. (2002). Cognition in persuasion: An analysis of information
attempts to manipulate public opinion. processing in response to persuasive communications. In M. P. Zanna
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp.
61–130). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Summary Albarracín, D., Cohen, J. B., & Kumkale, G. T. (2003). When persuasive
communications collide with behavior: Effects of post-message actions
A seemingly active environment can elicit attributions of on beliefs and intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
influence intent and, therefore, self-defense (e.g., resistance 29. 834–845.

and reactance theory). Many of these processes have been Albarracín, D., Durantini, M. R., & Earl, A. (2006). The effectiveness of
behavioral interventions to curb the HIV epidemic. Current Directions
studied in prior decades and have also received research in Psychological Science. 15, 73–78.
attention in recent years. Strong messages recommending Albarracín, D., Durantini, M. R., Earl, A., Gunnoe, J., & Leeper, J. (2008).
a behavior have been recently shown to influence attitudes Beyond the most willing audiences: A meta-intervention to increase
only in relation to the behavior of the recipient. The actual participation in HIV prevention intervention. Health Psychology. 27,
638–644.
arguments of a persuasive message, which were tradition-
Albarracín, D., Gillette, J., Earl, A., Glasman, L. R., Durantini, M. R., &
ally conceptualized as mere information, are now known Ho, M. H. (2005). A test of major assumptions about behavior change:
to provide pragmatic information as well. These and many A comprehensive look at HIV prevention interventions since the
more findings now provide a more complete picture of beginning of the epidemic. Psychological Bulletin, 131. 856–897.

social influence and persuasion mechanisms that should Albarracín, D., Handley, I., Noguchi, K., McCulloch, K. C., Li, H.,
Leeper, J., et al. (2008). Increasing and decreasing motor and cognitive
continue to emerge in the next decade. output: A model of general action and inaction goals. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 95. 510–523.
Albarracín, D., Hart, W., & McCulloch, K. C. (2006). Proposing what we
associate and associating what we propose: Comment on Gawronski
SUMMARY
and Bodenhausen. Psychological Bulletin. 132, 732–735.
Albarracín, D., Johnson, B. T., Fishbein, M., & Muellerleile, P. (2001).
Psychologists have made considerable progress under- Theories of reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom
standing environmental influences on attitudes and are use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 142–161.
now making strides toward understanding biological Albarracín, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (2005). Attitudes: Introduction
and genetic influences as well. This chapter summarizes and scope. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The
handbook of attitudes (pp. 3–20). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
some of the ways in which genes can influence general
Albarracín, D., & Kumkale, G. T. (2003). Affect as information in
affectivity (i.e., neuroticism and impulsivity), and there- persuasion: A model of affect identification and discounting. Journal
fore attitude valence and related arousal, but most of this of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 453–469.
work remains to be done. This chapter also reviewed Albarracín, D., Kumkale, G. T., & Johnson, B. T. (2004). Influences of
environmental influences, an area in which many impor- social power and resources on condom use decisions: A research
synthesis. AIDS Care. 16, 700–723.
tant and influential findings have accrued for more than
Albarracín, D., & McNatt, P. S. (2005). Change maintenance and decay of
a century of attitude research. The last decade has been the influences of past behavior: Anchoring attitudes on beliefs follow-
dedicated to elucidating some of the details of these pro- ing inconsistent actions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
cesses but has been relatively silent about the larger con- 31, 719–733.
nections between the processes triggered by seemingly Albarracín, D., McNatt, P. S., Klein, C., Ho, R., Mitchell, A., & Kumkale, G. T.
(2003). Persuasive communications to change actions: An analysis of
passive environments and reactions to persuasive intent. behavioral and cognitive impact in HIV prevention. Health Psychology,
We hope that future researchers will address these broad 22. 166–177.
questions, as well as the vast set of questions related to Albarracín, D., & Mitchell, A. L. (2004). The role of defensive confidence
gene-environment interactions. Both scholars and practi- in preference for proatttitudinal information: How believing that one is
strong can be a weakness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
tioners will benefit from continued progress in this classic 30, 1565–1584.
yet vibrant inquiry domain. Albarracín, D., Noguchi, K., & Earl, A. (2006). Joyce’s Ulysses and Woolf ’s
Jacob’s Room as the phenomenology of reasoning: Intentions and con-
trol as emergent of language and social interaction. Psychological
Inquiry, 17, 236–245.
Albarracín, D., Wallace, H. M., & Glasman, L. R. (2004). Survival and
REFERENCES
change of attitudes and other social judgments: A model of activation
and comparison. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
Abelson, R. P. (1988). Conviction. American Psychologist, 43. 267–275. psychology (Vol. 36, pp. 252–315). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on behavior. In Albarracín, J., Wang, W., & Albarracín, D. (in press). Defensive confidence
D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of in voting decisions: An analysis of the American National Election
attitudes (pp. 173–221). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Survey (Pilot Study). In ANES Group (Eds.), Political psychology.
References 419

Albarracín, D., Wang, W., & Leeper, J. (2009). Immediate increase in food Bem, D. J. (1972). Self perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
intake following exercise messages. Obesity. Obesity advanced online in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York:
publication. February 26, 2009; doi: 10.1033/oby. 2009.16. Academic Press.
Albarracín, D., & Wyer, R. S. (2000). The cognitive impact of past Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception
behavior: Influences on beliefs, attitudes, and future behavioral deci- and Psychophysics, 8, 279–286.
sions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 5–22. Betsch, T., Kaufmann, M., Lindow, F., Plessner, H., & Hoffmann, K.
Albarracín, D., & Wyer, R. S. (2001). Elaborative and nonelaborative (2006). Different principles of information integration in implicit and
processing of a behavior-related communication. Personality and explicit attitude formation. European Journal of Social Psychology.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 691–705. 36, 887–905.
Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook of social Betsch, T., Plessner, H., Schwieren, C., & Gütig, R. (2001). I like it
psychology (pp. 798–844). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. but I don’t know why: A value-account approach to implicit atti-
tude formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 27,
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation.
242–253.
New York: Henry Holt.
Bickman, L. (1974). The social power of a uniform. Journal of Applied
Amen, D. G., & Carmichael, B. D. (1997). High-resolution brain SPECT
Social Psychology, 4, 47–61.
imaging in ADHD. Annals of Clinical Psychology, 9, 81–86.
Blair, M. E., & Shimp, T. A. (1992). Consequences of an unpleasant
Anand, P., & Sternthal, B. (1991). Perceptual fluency and affect without
experience with music: A second-order negative conditioning perspec-
recognition. Memory & Cognition, 19, 293–300.
tive. Journal of Advertising, 21, 35–43.
Anderson, N. H. (1971). Integration theory and attitude change. Boomsma, D. I., de Geus, E. J. C., van Baal, G. C. M., & Koopmans, J. R.
Psychological Review. 78, 171–206. (1999). A religious upbringing reduces the influence of genetic factors
Anderson, N. H. (1981). Integration theory applied to cognitive responses on disinhibition: Evidence for interaction between genotype and envi-
and attitudes. In R. Petty, T. Ostrom, & T. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive ronment on personality. Twin Research, 2, 115–125.
responses in persuasion (pp. 361–397). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis
Angst, J., & Clayton, P. (1986). Premorbid personality of depressive, of research, 1968–1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265–289.
bipolar, and schizophrenic patients with special reference to suicidal Bornstein, R. F., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1994). The attribution and discount-
issues. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 27, 511–532. ing of perceptual fluency: Preliminary tests of a perceptual fluency/
Areni, C. S., & Sparks, J. R. (2005). Language power and persuasion. attributional model of the mere exposure effect. Social Cognition, 12,
Psychology and Marketing, 22,. 507–525. 103–128.
Baer, R., Hinkle, S., Smith, K., & Fenton, M. (1980). Reactance as a func- Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1994). Genes, environment and personality. Science,
tion of actual versus projected autonomy. Journal of Personality and 264. 1700–1701.
Social Psychology, 38, 416–422. Bouchard, T.J. Jr., Lykken, D.T., McGue, M., Segal, N., & Tellegen, A.
Baeyens, F., Crombez, G., Van den Bergh, O., & Eelen, P. (1998). Once in (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota
contact, always in contact: Evaluative conditioning is resistant to extinc- Study of Twins Reared Apart. Science, 250, 223–228.
tion. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy. 10, 179–199. Boyce, P., Parker, G., Barnett, B., Cooney, M., & Smith, F. (1991).
Baeyens, F., & De Houwer, J. (1995). Evaluative conditioning is a Personality as a vulnerability factor to depression. British Journal of
qualitatively distinct form of classical conditioning: A reply to Davey Psychiatry, 159, 106–114.
(1994). Behaviour Research and Therapy. 33, 825–831. Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001).
Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., & Crombez, G. (1995). Pavlovian associations Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture
are forever: On classical conditioning and extinction. Journal of processing. Emotion. 1, 276–298.
Psychophysiology. 9, 127–141. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Motivation and emotion. In
Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., Crombez, G., & De Houwer, J. (2001). On the role J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of
of beliefs in observational flavor conditioning. Current Psychology, psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 581–607). New York: Cambridge
20, 183–203. University Press.
Bailey, J. N., Breidenthal, S. E., Jorgensen, M. J., McCracken, J. T., & Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. San Diego, CA:
Fairbanks, L. A. (2007). The association of DRD4 and novelty seeking is Academic Press.
found in a nonhuman primate model. Psychiatric Genetics, 17, 23–27. Brehm, J. W., & Mann, M. (1975). Effect of importance of freedom and
Ball, D., Hill, L., Freeman, B., Eley, T. C., Strelau, J., Rienmann, R., attraction to group members on influence produced by group pressure.
et al. (1997). The serotonin transporter gene and peer-rated neuroti- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 816–824.
cism. NeuroReport, 8, 1301–1304. Brehm, J. W., & Sensenig, J. (1966). Social influence as a function of
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social attempted and implied usurpation of choice. Journal of Personality
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on and Social Psychology, 4, 703–707.
action. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 71, 230–244. Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance: A theory
Barkley, R. A. (1998, September). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. of freedom and control. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Scientific American, 279, 66–71. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Valle, C., Rucker, D. D., & Becerra, A. (2007).
Bassili, J. N., & Brown, R. D. (2005). Implicit and explicit attitudes: The effects of message recipients’ power before and after persuasion: A
Research, challenges, and theory. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 543–574). Mahwah, 93, 1040–1053.
NJ: Erlbaum. Brock, T. C. (1967). Communication discrepancy and intent to persuade
Beatty, S. E., & Hawkins, D. I. (1989). Subliminal stimulation: Some new as detriments of counterargument production. Journal of Experimental
data and interpretation. Journal of Advertising. 18, 4–8. Social Psychology, 3, 296–309.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A. R., & Lee, G. P. (1999). Different Brooks, J. O., & Watkins, M. J. (1989). Recognition memory and the mere
contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cor- exposure effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
tex to decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 5473–5481. Memory, and Cognition, 15, 968–976.
420 Attitudes and Persuasion

Brown, R. D., & Albarracín, D. (2005). Attitudes over time: Attitude Crano, W. D., & Prislin, R. (2005). Attitudes and persuasion. Annual
judgment and change. In A. Strathman & J. Joireman (Eds.), Review of Psychology, 57, 345–374.
Understanding behavior in the context of time: Theory, research, and Crawford, M. T., McConnell, A. R., Lewis, A. C., & Sherman, S. J. (2002).
applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Reactance, compliance, and anticipated regret. Journal of Experimental
Byrne, D. (1961). The repression-sensitization scale: Rationale, reliability, Social Psychology, 38, 56–63.
and validity. Journal of Personality, 29, 463–468. Critchley, H. D. (2005). Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective,
Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (1997). Imaging cognition: An empirical and cognitive integration. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493,
review of PET studies with normal subjects. Journal of Cognitive 154–166.
Neuroscience, 9, 1–26. Croyle, R. T. and Cooper, J. (1983). Dissonance arousal: Physiological
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Crites, S. L., Jr. (1996). Social neu- evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,
roscience: Principles of psychophysiological arousal and response. 782–791.
In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji,
Handbook of basic principles (pp. 72–101). New York: Academic M. R. (2003). Neural components of social evaluation. Journal of
Press. Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 639–649.
Cacioppo, J. T., Marshall-Goodell, B. S., Tassinary, L. G., & Petty, R. E. Cunningham, W. A., Raye, C. L., & Johnson, M. K. (2004). Implicit and
(1992). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: Classical condition- explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional inten-
ing is more effective when prior knowledge about the attitude stimu- sity, and control in the processing of attitudes. Journal of Cognitive
lus is low than high. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, Neuroscience, 16, 1717–1729.
207–233.
Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations:
Cattell, R. B. (1946). Description and measurement of personality. A social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive
Yonkers-on-Hudson, NY: World Book. Sciences, 11, 97–104.
Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, N., &
J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario Lang, P. J. (2000). Brain potentials in affective picture processing:
symposium (Vol. 3, pp. 143–177). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological
Erlbaum. Psychology, 52, 95–111.
Champion, J. M., & Turner, W. W. (1959). An experimental investigation of Daly, G., Jawj, Z., Fitzgerald, M., & Gill, M. (1999). Mapping susceptibility
subliminal perception. Journal of Applied Psychology. 43, 382–384. loci in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Preferential transmis-
Chartrand, T. L. (2005). The role of conscious awareness in consumer sion of parental alleles at DAT1, DBH and DRD5 to affected children.
behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 15(3), 203–210. Molecular Psychiatry, 4, 2192–2196.
Chartrand, T. L., Dalton, A. N., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2007). Nonconscious Danzig, F. (1962, September 17). Subliminal advertising—Today it’s just
relationship reactance: When significant others prime opposing goals. historic flashback for researcher Vicary. Advertising Age. 33, 72, 74.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 719–726. Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic
Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P. M. (1986). Distinguishing conscious from attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and
unconscious perception. Canadian Journal of Psychophysics. 40, disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
343–367. 81, 800–814.
Christensen, L. (1977). The negative subject: Myth, reality, or a prior Davey, G. C. L. (1994). Is evaluative conditioning a qualitatively distinct
experimental experiences effect? Journal of Personality and Social form of classical conditioning? Behaviour Research and Therapy. 32,
Psychology, 35, 392–400. 291–299.
Cialdini, R. B. (1987). Compliance principles of compliance profession- Dean, R. B., Austin, J. A., & Watts, W. A. (1971). Forewarning effect in
als: Psychologists of necessity. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. persuasion: Field and classroom experiments. Journal of Personality
Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 5, pp. and Social Psychology, 18, 210–221.
165–184). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. DeBono, K. G., & Harnish, R. J. (1988). Source expertise, source
Cialdini, R. B. (2001). Influence: Science and practice. Boston: Allyn & attractiveness, and the processing of persuasive information: A func-
Bacon. tional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 55,
541–546.
Clore, G. L., & Schnall, S. (2005). The influence of affect on attitude. In
D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of Decker, P. J., & Borgen, F. H. (1993). Dimensions of work appraisal:
attitudes (pp. 437–489). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Stress, strain, coping, job satisfaction, and negative affectivity. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 40, 470–478.
Coccaro, E. F., & Murphy, D. L. (1990). Serotonin in major psychiatric
disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. De Fleur, M. L., & Petranoff, R. M. (1959). A televised test of subliminal
persuasion. Public Opinion Quarterly. 23, 168–180.
Comings, D. E., Saucier, G., & MacMurray, J. P. (2002). Role of DRD2 and
other dopamine genes. In J. Benjamin, R. P. Ebstein, & R. H. Belmaker De Houwer, J. (2007). A conceptual and theoretical analysis of evaluative
(Eds.), Molecular genetics and the human personality (pp. 165–191). conditioning. Spanish Journal of Psychology. 10, 230–241.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. De Houwer, J., Baeyens, F., & Field, A. P. (2005). Associative learning
Cook, E. H., Stein, M. A., Krasowski, M. D., Cox, N. J., Olkon, D. M., of likes and dislikes: Some current controversies and possible ways
Kieffer, J. E., et al. (1995). Association of attention-deficit disorder and forward. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 161–174.
the dopamine transporter gene. American Journal of Human Genetics, de Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Association learning of
56, 993–998. likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evalua-
Cooper, J., & Cooper, G. (2002). Subliminal motivation: A story revisited. tive conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853–869.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 32, 2213–2227. de la Pena, V. H. (1992). Decoupling and Khintchine’s inequalities for
Cox, B. J., Borger, S. C., Asmundson, G. J. G., & Taylor, S. (2000). U-statistics. Annals of Probability, 20, 1877–1892.
Dimensions of hypochondriasis and the five-factor model of personal- DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1999). Nonverbal communication. In
ity. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 99–108. D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social
References 421

psychology (4th ed., Vol. II, pp. 3–40). Oxford: Oxford University Eysenck, H. J. (1990b). Genetic and environmental contributions to indi-
Press. vidual differences: The three major dimensions of personality. Journal
DePaulo, B. M., Stone, J. I., & Lassiter, G. D. (1985). Deceiving and of Personality, 58, 245–261.
detecting deceit. In B. R. Schlenker (Ed.), The self and social life (pp. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual dif-
323–370). New York: McGraw-Hill. ferences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum.
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and Eysenck, H. J., Eysenck, S. G., & Educational and Industrial Testing
informational social influences upon individual judgement. Journal of Service. (1975). EPQ (Eysenck personality questionnaire). San Diego,
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629–636. CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
Diamond, M., & Sigmundson, H. K. (1997). Sex reassignment at birth: Fabrigar, L. R., MacDonald, T. K., & Wegener, D. T. (2005). The structure
Long term review and clinical implications. Archives of Pediatrics and of attitudes. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The
Adolescent Medicine, 151, 298–304. handbook of attitudes (pp. 79–124). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Díaz, E., Ruiz, G., & Baeyens, F. (2005). Resistance to extinction of human Fang, X., Singh, S., & Ahluwalia, R. (2007). An examination of different
evaluative conditioning using a between-subjects design. Cognition & explanations for the mere exposure effect. Journal of Consumer
Emotion. 19, 245–268. Research, 34, 97–103.
Dick Cheney’s suite demands. (2006). Retrieved October 6, 2008, from Farrow, M., Levy, F., & Silberstein, R. (2005). Neurobiology of ADHD.
http://www.thesmokinggun.com/archive/0322061cheney1.html. In C. Stough (Ed.), Neurobiology of exceptionality (pp. 143–197).
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). I like myself but I don’t know why: Enhancing New York: Springer.
implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. Journal of Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition
Personality and Social Psychology. 86, 345–355. research: Their meaning and uses. Annual Review of Psychology. 54,
Doob, A. N., & Gross, A. E. (1968). Status of frustrator as an inhibitor of 297–327.
horn-honking responses. Journal of Social Psychology, 76, 213–218. Fazio, R. H., Powell, M. C., & Herr, P. M. (1983). Toward a process model
Durantini, M. R., Albarracín, D., Earl, A., & Mitchell, A. L. (2006). of the attitude-behavior relation: Accessing one’s attitude upon mere
Conceptualizing the influence of social agents of change: A meta- observation of the attitude object. Journal of Personality and Social
analysis of HIV prevention interventions for different groups. Psychology, 44, 723–735.
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 212–248. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row,
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Peterson.
Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision and dissonance. Stanford, CA:
Earl, A., & Albarracín, D. (2007). Nature, decay and spiraling of the effects Stanford University Press.
of fear-inducing arguments and HIV counseling and testing: A meta- Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced
analysis of the short- and long-term outcomes of HIV-prevention compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203–210.
interventions. Health Psychology, 26, 494–506. Festinger, L., & Thibaut, J. (1951). Interpersonal communication in small
Ebstein, R. P., Gritsenko, I., Nemanov, L., Frisch, A., Osher, Y, & Belmaker, groups. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 92–99.
R. H. (1997). No association between the serotonin transporter gene Field, A. P., & Moore, A. C. (2005). Dissociating the effects of attention
regulatory region polymorphism and the tridimensional personality and contingency awareness on evaluative conditioning effects in the
questionnaire (TPQ) temperament of human avoidance. Molecular visual paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 217–243.
Psychiatry, 3, 449–451. Fischer, H., Wik, G., & Fredrikson, M. (1997). Extraversion, neuroti-
Ebstein, R. P., & Kotler, M. (2002). Personality, substance abuse, and genes. cism and brain function: A PET study of personality. Personality and
In J. Benjamin, R. P. Ebstein, & R. H. Belmaker (Eds.), Molecular Individual Differences, 23, 345–352.
genetics and the human personality (pp. 151–163). Washington, DC: Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitudes, intention, and
American Psychiatric Press. behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Rearing, MA:
Ebstein, R. P., Novick, O., Umansky, R., Priel, B., Osher, Y., Blaine, D., Addison-Wesley.
et al. (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism Fisher, J. D., Fisher, W. A., Bryan, A. D., & Misovich, S. J. (2002).
associated with the human personality trait of novelty seeking. Nature Information-motivation-behavioral skills model-based HIV risk
Genetics, 12, 78–80. behavior change intervention for inner-city high school youth. Health
Egan, G., Silk, T., Zamarripa, F., Williams, J., Federico, P., Cunnington, R., Psychology, 21, 177–186.
et al. (2003). Neural correlates of the emergence of consciousness of Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of you: Nonconscious
thirst. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, pursuit of interpersonal goals associated with relationship partners.
15241–15246. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 148–163.
Eifert, G. H., Craill, L., Carey, E., & O’Connor, C. (1988). Affect Flory, J. D., Manuck, S. B., Ferrell, R. E., Dent, K. M., Peters, D. G., &
modification through evaluative conditioning with music. Behaviour Muldoon, M. F. (1999). Neuroticism is not associated with the serotonin
Research and Therapy, 26, 321–330. transporter (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. Molecular Psychiatry, 4, 93–96.
Elkin, R. A., & Leippe, M. R. (1986). Physiological arousal, dissonance, Fowler, C. A., Wolford, G., Slade, R., & Tassinary, L. (1981). Lexical access
and attitude change: Evidence for a dissonance—arousal link and with and without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
a “don’t remind me” effect. Journal of Personality and Social General. 110, 341–362.
Psychology, 51, 55–65. Francis, L. J. (1996). The relationship between Eysenck’s personality
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, factors and attitude towards substance use among 13–15-year-olds.
IL: Thomas. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 633–640.
Eysenck, H. J. (1975). The structure of social attitudes. British Journal of French, J. R. P., Jr., & Raven, B. H. (1959). The bases of social power.
Social and Clinical Psychology, 14. 323–331. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 150–167). Ann
Eysenck, H. J. (1990a). Biological dimensions of personality. In Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.
L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research Fulker, D. W., Eysenck, S. B. G., & Zuckerman, M. (1980). The genetics of
(pp. 244–276). New York: Guilford Press. sensation seeking. Journal of Personality Research, 14, 261–281.
422 Attitudes and Persuasion

Fullerton, J., Cubin, M., Tiwari, H., Wang, C., Bomhra, A., Davidson, S., Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Hoffman, J. M., & Kilts, C. D. (2002). Ecstasy
Miller, S. et al. (2003). Linkage analysis of extremely discordant and and agony: Activation in the human amygdala in positive and negative
concordant sibling pairs identifies quantitative-trait loci that influences emotion. Psychological Science, 13, 135–141.
variation in the human personality trait neuroticism. American Journal Hamm, A. O., & Vaitl, D. (1996). Affective learning: Awareness and aver-
of Human Genetics, 72, 879–890. sion. Psychophysiology, 33, 698–710.
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional Hammerl, M., & Fulcher, E. P. (2005). Reactance in affective-evaluative
processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit learning: Outside of conscious control? Cognition and Emotion, 19,
attitude change. Psychological Bulletin. 132, 692–731. 197–216.
Gawronski, B., & Strack, F. (2004). On the propositional nature of cog- Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D. R.
nitive consistency: Dissonance changes explicit, but not implicit atti- (2003). Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful
tudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 40, 535–542. stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 494–501.
George, S. G., & Jennings, L. B. (1975). Effect of subliminal stimuli on Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. B. (1998). Anger and frontal brain activity:
consumer behavior: Negative evidence. Perceptual & Motor Skills. 41, EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative
847–854. affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist, 1310–1316.
46, 107–119. Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, & J. J. B. (2001). The role of affect in the mere
Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can’t not believe exposure effect: Evidence from psychophysiological and individual
everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, differences approaches. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
65, 221–233. 27, 889–898.
Gill, M., Daly, G., Heron, S., Hawi, Z., & Fitzgerald, M. (1997). Harmon-Jones, E., Brehm, J. W., Greenberg, J., Simon, L., & Nelson, D. E.
Confirmation of association between attention deficit hyperactiv- (1996). Evidence that the production of aversive consequences is not
ity disorder and a dopamine transporter polymorphism. Molecular necessary to create cognitive dissonance. Journal of Personality and
Psychiatry, 2, 311–313. Social Psychology, 70. 5–16.
Glasman, L. R., & Albarracín, D. (2006). Forming attitudes that predict Hart, W., & Albarracín, D. (in press). The effects of chronic achievement
future behavior: A meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation. motivation and achievement primes on the activation of achieve-
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 778–822. ment and fun goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion- Hart, W., & Albarracín, D. (2009). What I was doing vs. what I did: Verb
extraversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8, 249–266. aspect influences memory and future actions. Psychological Science,
Gray, J. A. (1972). The psychology of fear and stress. New York: 20, 238–244.
McGraw-Hill. Hart, W., Albarracín, D., Eagly, A. H., Lindberg, M., Lee, K. H., &
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the Brechan, I. (2009). Feeling validated vs. being correct: A meta-
functions of the septo-hippocampal system. New York: Clarendon analysis of exposure to information. Psychological Bulletin, 135,
Press/Oxford University Press. 555–588.
Gray, J. A. (1991). The neuropsychology of temperament. In J. Strelau & Hawkins, D. (1970). The effects of subliminal stimulation on drive
A. Angleitner (Eds.), Explorations in temperament: International level and brand preference. Journal of Marketing Research. 7,
perspectives on theory and measurement (pp. 105–128). New York: 322–326.
Plenum Press. Heaven, P. C. L., Fitzpatrick, J., Craig, F. L., Kelly, P., & Sebar, G. (2000).
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). Fundamentals of the septo- Five personality factors and sex: Preliminary findings. Personality,
hippocampal system. In J. A. Gray & N. McNaughton (Eds.), The and Individual Differences, 28, 1133–1141.
neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo- Heesacker, M., Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1983). Field depen-
hippocampal system (2nd ed., pp. 204–232). Oxford: Oxford University dence and attitude change: Source credibility can alter persuasion
Press. by affecting message-relevant thinking. Journal of Personality, 51,
Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response to per- 653–666.
suasion, and attitude change. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock, & Heilman, M. E., & Garner, K. A. (1975). Counteracting the boomerang:
T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 147– The effects of choice on compliance to threats and promises. Journal of
170). New York: Academic Press. Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 911–917.
Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed. Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D.
American Psychologist. 47, 766–779. (2006). The relation of cortical activity and BIS/BAS on the trait level.
Greenwald, A. G., Spangenberg, E. R., Pratkanis, A. R., & Eskenazi, J. Biological Psychology, 71, 42–53.
(1991). Double-blind tests of subliminal self-help audiotapes. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist,
Psychological Science. 2, 119–122. 52, 1280–1300.
Griffith, D. A., & Chen, Q. (2004). The influence of virtual direct experi- Higgins, E. T. (1999). Promotion and prevention as a motivational duality:
ence VDE on online ad message effectiveness. Journal of Advertising, Implications for evaluative processes. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope
33, 55–69. (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 503–525).
Gruenfeld, D. H., & Wyer, R. S. (1992). Semantics and pragmatics of social New York: Guilford Press.
influence: How affirmations and denials affect beliefs in referent prop- Hirschfeld, R. M., Klerman, G. L., Lavori, P., Keller, M. B., Griffith, P., &
ositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 38–49. Coryell, W. (1989). Premorbid personality assessments of first onset of
Grush, J. E. (1976). Attitude formation and mere exposure phenomena: A major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 345–350.
nonartifactual explanation of empirical findings. Journal of Personality Hoch, S. J., & Ha, Y. W. (1986). Consumer learning: Advertising and the
and Social Psychology, 33, 281–290. ambiguity of product experience. Journal of Consumer Research, 13,
Ha, Y. W., & Hoch, S. J. (1989). Ambiguity, processing strategy, and 221–233.
advertising-evidence interactions. Journal of Consumer Research, 16, Hoffmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005).
354–360. A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association
References 423

test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Kelman, H. C. (2006). Interests, relationships, identities: Three central
Psychology Bulletin. 31, 1369–1385. issues for individuals and groups in negotiating their social environ-
Hofling, C. K., Brotzman, E., Dalrymple, S., Graves, N., & Pierce, C. M. ment. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 1–26.
(1966). An experimental study in nurse–physician relationships. Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach,
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143, 171–180. and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.
Hong, L., Albarracín, D., Wright, P., & Liu, Y. (2008). Proceduralization Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J.
of evaluations. Unpublished manuscript, University of Florida, (1993). A longitudinal twin study of personality and major depression
Gainesville, FL. in women. Archives in General Psychiatry, 50, 853–862.
Hovland, C. I. (1959). Reconciling conflicting results derived from Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science. 237, 1445–1452.
experimental and survey studies of attitude change. American Kihlstrom, J. F. (1999). The psychological unconscious. In L. A. Pervin, &
Psychologist, 14, 8–17. O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research
Hovland, C. I., & Janis, I. L. (1959). Personality and persuasibility. New (2nd ed., pp. 424–442). New York: Guilford Press.
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Killeya, L. A., & Johnson, B. T. (1998). Experimental induction of biased
Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W. (1951). “The influence of source credibility systematic processing: The directed thought technique. Personality
on communication effectiveness.” Public Opinion Quarterly. 15, and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 17–33.
635–650. Knussen, C., & Niven, C. A. (1999). HIV/AIDS and health care workers:
Hummel, J. E., & Holyoak, K. J. (2005). Relational reasoning in a neurally Contact with patients and attitudes towards them. Psychology and
plausible cognitive architecture. Current Directions in Psychological Health, 14, 367–378.
Science, 14, 153–157. Kraus, S. J. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: A meta-
Irwin, W., Davidson, R. J., Lowe, M. J., Mock, B. J., Sorenson, J. A., & analysis of the empirical literature. Personality and Social Psychology
Turski, P. A. (1996). Human amygdala activation detected with echo- Bulletin. 21, 58–75.
planar functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroReport, 7, Kringelbach, M. L., & Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy
1765–1769. of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and
Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C., Brown, J., & Jasechko, J. (1989). Becoming famous neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology, 72, 341–372.
overnight: Limits on the ability to avoid unconscious influences of the Kruglanski, A. W., & Stroebe, W. (2005). The influence of beliefs and
past. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 326–338. goals on attitudes: Issues of structure, function, and dynamics. In
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vernon, P. A. (1996). Heritability of the big D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of
five personality dimensions and their facets: A twin study. Journal of attitudes (pp. 323–368). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Personality, 64, 577–591. Kumkale, G. T., & Albarracín, D. (2004). The sleeper effect in persuasion:
Janis, I. L., & Milholland, W. (1954). The influence of threat appeals A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 143–172.
on selective learning of the content of a persuasive communication. Kumkale, G. T., & Albarracín, D., & Poyner-del Vento, P. (in press). The
Journal of Psychology, 37, 75–80. effects of source credibility in the presence or absence of prior atti-
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big five trait taxonomy: tudes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology.
History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological
O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd Bulletin, 108, 480–498.
ed., pp. 102–139). New York: Guilford Press. Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of
Johnson, B. T., Maio, G. R., & Smith-McLallen, A. (2005). Communication stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science. 207, 557–558.
and attitude change: Causes, processes, and effects. In D. Albarracín, Lane, R. D., Chua, P. M., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Common effects of emo-
B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. tional valence, arousal and attention on neural activation during visual
617–669). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. processing of pictures. Neuropsychologia, 37, 989–997.
Johnsrude, I. S., Owen, A. M., Zhao, W. V., & White, N. M. (1999). Lane, R. D., Fink, G. R., Chau, P. M-L., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Neural
Conditioned preference in humans: A novel experimental approach. activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses.
Learning and Motivation, 30, 250–264. NeuroReport, 8, 3969–3972.
Jorn, A. F., Henderson, A. S., Jacomb, P. A., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., Fitzsimmons, J. R., Cuthbert, B. N., Scott, J. D.,
Rodgers, B., et al. (1998). An association study of a functional Moulder, B., et al. (1998). Emotional arousal and activation of the
polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene with personality and visual cortex: An fMRI analysis. Psychophysiology. 35, 199–210.
psychiatric symptoms. Molecular Psychiatry, 2, 224–226.
Lang, P. J., Öhman, A., & Vaitl, D., (1988). The international affective
Judd, C. M., & Brauer, M. (1995). Repetition and evaluative extremity. In picture system [photographic slides]. Gainesville, FL: The Center for
R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
consequences: Ohio State University series on attitudes and persua-
Lefkowitz, M., Blake, R. R., & Mouton, J. S. (1955). Status factors in
sion (Vol. 4, pp. 43–71). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
pedestrian violation of traffic signals. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Psychology, 51, 704–706.
test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81, 774–788.
Lesch, K. P., Bengel, D., Hells, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Pertri, S.,
Karremans, J. C., Stroebe, W., & Claus, J. (2006). Beyond Vicary’s fanta- et al. (1996). Association of anxiety related trains with a polymorphism
sies: The impact of subliminal priming and brand choice. Journal of in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274,
Experimental Social Psychology. 42, 792–798. 1527–1531.
Kelman, H. C. (1958). Compliance, identification, and internalization: Three Levey, A. B., & Martin, I. (1975). Classical conditioning of human ‘evalu-
processes of attitude change. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, 51–60. ative’ responses. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 4, 205–207.
Kelman, H. C. (1961). Processes of opinion change. Public Opinion Lipp, O. V., & Purkis, H. M. (2006). The effects of assessment type on
Quarterly, 25, 57–78. verbal ratings of conditional stimulus valence and contingency judg-
Kelman, H. C. (1974). Attitudes are alive and well and gainfully employed ments: Implications for the extinction of evaluative learning. Journal of
in the sphere of action. American Psychologist, 29, 310–324. Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 32, 431–440.
424 Attitudes and Persuasion

Livingston, R. W. (2001). What you see is what you get: Systematic vari- Monahan, J. L., Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Subliminal mere
ability in perceptual-based social judgment. Personality and Social exposure: Specific, general, and diffuse effects. Psychological Science,
Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1086–1097. 11, 462–466.
Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Genes and environment in personality development. Mooney, C. (2003). John Zogby’s creative polls and a closer look at his
Sage series on individual differences and development. Thousand methods. The American Prospect, 14, 29–33.
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Mooradian, T. A. (1996). Personality and ad-evoked feelings: The case for
Lou, H. C., Henriksen, L., & Bruhn, P. (1984). Focal cerebral hypoper- extraversion and neuroticism. Journal of the Academy of Marketing
fusion in children with dysphasia and/or attention deficit disorder. Science, 24. 99–109.
Archives of Neurology, 41, 825–829. Moore, T. E. (1982). Subliminal advertising: What you see is what you get.
Lou, H. C., Henriksen, L., Bruhn, P., Borner, H., & Nielsen, J. B. (1989). Journal of Marketing. 46, 38–47.
Striatal dysfunction in attention deficit and hyperkinetic disorder. Moore, T. E. (1988). The case against subliminal manipulation. Psychology &
Archives of Neurology, 46, 48–52. Marketing. 5, 297–316.
Ludvigh, E. J., & Happ, D. (1974). Extraversion and preferred level of sen- Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young,A. W., Calder,A. J.,
sory stimulation. British Journal of Psychology, 65, 359–365. et al. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to
Maes, M., & Metzer, H. Y. (1995). The serotonin hypothesis of major depres- fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383, 812–815.
sion. In F. E. Bloom & D. J. Kupfer (Eds.), Psychopharmacology: The Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation
fourth generation of progress. New York: Raven Press. and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Martin, I., & Levey, A. B. (1978). Evaluative conditioning. Advances in Neurophysiology. 1, 455–473.
Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1, 57–102. Mughal, S., Walsh, J., & Wilding, J. (1996). Stress and work performance: The
Martin, I., & Levey, A. B. (1994). The evaluative response: Primitive but role of trait anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 685–691.
necessary. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 32, 301–305. Müller, J., Dettmer, D., & Macht, M. (2008). The attitudes to chocolate
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C, Jardine, R., Feingold, L. R., & questionnaire: Psychometric properties and relationships to dimensions
Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Transmission of social attitudes. Proceedings of of eating. Appetite, 50, 499–505.
the National Academy of Sciences USA, 83, 4364–4368. Nakamura, T., Muramatsu, T., Ono, Y., Matsushita, S., Higuchi, S.,
Masling, J. (1966). Role-related behavior of the subject and psycholo- Mizushima, H., et al. (1997). Serotonin transporter gene regula-
gist and its effect upon psychological data. Nebraska Symposium on tory region polymorphism and anxiety-related traits in the Japanese.
Motivation, 14, 67–103. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 74, 544–545.
Mathew, R. J., Weinman, M. L., & Barr, D. L. (1984). Personality and regional Nash, M. W., Huezo-Diaz, P., Williamson, R. J., Sterne, A., Purcell, S.,
cerebral blood flow. British Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 529–532. Hoda, F., et al. (2004). Genome-wide linkage analysis: A composite
McConnell, J. V., Cutler, R. L., & McNeil, E. B. (1958). Subliminal stimu- index of neuroticism and mood-related scales in extreme selected sib-
lation: An overview. American Psychologist. 13, 229–242. ships. Human Molecular Genetics, 13, 2137–2182.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2008). The five-factor theory of personality. Newell, B. R., & Shanks, D. R. (2007). Recognizing what you like:
In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of person- Examining the relation between the mere-exposure effect and recogni-
ality theory and research (pp. 159–181). New York: Guilford Press. tion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 103–118.
McGuire, W. J. (1964). Introducing resistance to persuasion: Some Nierman, A. J. (2008). Evaluative conditioning in the visual domain:
contemporary approaches. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimen- A meta-analysis. Unpublished manuscript, University of Kansas,
tal social psychology (pp. 191–229). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Lawrence, KS.
McGuire, W. J. (1968). Personality and attitude change: An informa- Noble, E. P., Ozkaragoz, T. Z., Ritchie, T. L., Zhang, X., Belin, T. R., &
tion-processing theory. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock, & T. M. Sparkes, R. S. (1998). D2 and D4 dopamine receptor polymorphisms
Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 171–196). and personality. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 81, 257–267.
New York: Academic Press. Noguchi, K., Durantini, M. R., Albarracín, D., & Glasman, L. R. (2007).
McGuire, W. J. (1972). Attitude change: The information-processing Who is exposed to which health promotion programs? A meta-analysis
paradigm. In C. G. McClintock (Ed.), Experimental social psychology of motivations underlying enrollment and retention in HIV prevention
(pp. 108–141). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. interventions. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 955–975.
McGuire, W. J. (1985). Attitudes and attitude change. In W. J. McGuire Olson, J. M., & Stone, J. (2005). The influence of behavior on attitudes. In
(Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 173–257). San Diego, CA: D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of
Academic Press. attitudes (pp. 223–271). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Merikle, P. M. (1988). Subliminal auditory messages: An evaluation. Olson, J. M., Vernon, P. A., Harris, J. A., & Jang, K. L. (2001). The herita-
Psychology and Marketing. 5, 355–372. bility of attitudes: A study of twins. Journal of Personality and Social
Metzer, H. Y., & Arora, R. C. (1988). Genetic control of serotonin uptake in Psychology, 80, 845–860.
blood platelets: A twin study. Psychiatric Research, 24, 263–269. Olson, J. M., & Zanna, M. P. (1982). Repression-sensitization differences
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal in responses to a decision. Journal of Personality, 50, 46–57.
and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378. Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2001). Implicit attitude formation through
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority. New York: Harper & Row. classical conditioning. Psychological Science. 12, 413–417.
Millar, M. G., & Tesser, A. (1986). Effects of affective and cognitive focus Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2002). Implicit acquisition and manifestation
on the attitude-behavior relation. Journal of Personality and Social of classically conditioned attitudes. Social Cognition. 20, 89–104.
Psychology 51, 270–276. Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). Reducing automatically activated
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (2008). Toward a unified theory of personality: racial prejudice through implicit evaluative conditioning. Personality
Integrating dispositions and processing dynamics within the cognitive- and Social Psychology Bulletin. 32, 421–433.
affective processing system. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin Owens, M. J., & Nemeroff, C. B. (1994). Role of serotonin in the patho-
(Eds.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 159–181). physiology of depression: Focus on the serotonin transporter. Clinical
New York: Guilford Press. Chemistry, 40, 288–295.
References 425

Papez, J. W. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Posner, J., Russell, J., & Peterson, B. (2005). The circumplex model of affect:
Neurology and Psychiatry, 38, 725–743. An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive develop-
Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Experimental psychology and psycho-pathology in ment, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17,
animals. Lectures on conditioned reflexes: Twenty-five years of objec- 715–734.
tive study of the higher nervous activity (behaviour) of animals (pp. Pratkanis, A. R., & Aronson, E. (1992). Age of propaganda: The everyday
47–60). New York: Liverwright Publishing. use and abuse of persuasion. New York: W. H. Freeman/Times Books/
Perry, A. (1973). The effect of heredity on attitudes toward alcohol, ciga- Henry Holt & Co.
rettes, and coffee. Journal of Applied Psychology, 58. 275–277. Pratkanis, A. R., Eskenazi, J., & Greenwald, A. G. (1994). What you
Petrides, M. (2007). The orbitofrontal cortex: Novelty, deviation from expect is what you believe (but not necessarily what you get): A test of
expectation, and memory. In G. Schoenbaum, J. A. Gottfried, the effectiveness of subliminal self-help audiotapes. Basic & Applied
E.A. Murray, & S. J. Ramus (Eds.), Linking affect to action: Critical Social Psychology. 15, 251–276.
contributions of the orbitofrontal cortex (pp. 33–53). Hoboken, NJ: Pratkanis, A. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (1988). Recent perspectives on uncon-
Wiley. scious processing: Still no marketing applications. Psychology and
Petty, R. E. (2006). A meta-cognitive model of attitudes. Journal of Marketing. 5, 337–353.
Consumer Research. 33, 22–24. Prolo, P., & Licinio, J. (2002). DRD4 and novelty seeking. In
Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2006). A metacognitive approach to “implicit” J. Benjamin, R. P. Ebstein, & R. H. Belmaker (Eds.), Molecular
and “explicit” evaluations: Comment on Gawronski and Bodenhausen genetics and the human personality (pp. 91–107). Washington, DC:
(2006). Psychological Bulletin. 132, 740–744. American Psychiatric Publishing.
Puckett, J. M., Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Fisher, D. L. (1983). The
Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (2007). The meta-cognitive
relative impact of age and attractiveness stereotypes on persuasion.
model (MCM) of attitudes: Implications for attitude measurement,
Journal of Gerontology. 38, 340–343.
change, and strength. Social Cognition. 25, 657–686.
Purkis, H. M., & Lipp, O. V. (2001). Does affective learning exist in the absence
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic
of contingency awareness? Learning and Motivation, 32, 84–99.
and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown.
Raven, B. H. (1965). Social influence and power. In I. D. Steiner & M.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion:
Fishbein (Eds.), Current studies in social psychology (pp. 371–382).
Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York:
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Springer-Verlag.
Reed II, A., Wooten, D. B., & Bolton, L. E. (2002). The temporary
Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z. L., Briñol, P., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (2006). Implicit
construction of consumer attitudes. Journal of Consumer Psychology,
ambivalence from attitude change: An exploration of the PAST model.
12, 375–388.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 90, 21–41.
Reekum, C. M., Urry, H. L., Johnstone, T., Thurow, M. E., Frye, C. J.,
Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. (2004). An attitude strength
Jackson, C. A., et al. (2007). Individual differences in amygdala and
perspective on resistance to persuasion. In M. R. Banaji, J. T. Jost, &
ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity are associated with evalu-
D. Prentice (Eds.), The yin and yang of social cognition: Festschrift
ation speed and psychological well-being. Journal of Cognitive
for William McGuire (pp. 37–51). Washington, DC: American
Neuroscience, 19, 237–248.
Psychological Association.
Regan, D. T., & Fazio, R. H. (1977). On the consistency of attitudes
Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Attitude change: Multiple roles
and behavior: Look to the method of attitude formation. Journal of
for persuasion variables. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, and G. Lindzey
Experimental and Social Psychology, 13, 28–45.
(Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 323–390). Boston:
McGraw-Hill. Reinhart, A. M., Marshall, H. M., Feeley, T. H., & Tutzauer, F. (2007). The
persuasive effects of message framing in organ donation: The mediat-
Petty, R. E., Wheeler, S. C., & Tormala, Z. L. (2003). Persuasion and
ing role of psychological reactance. Communication Monographs, 74,
attitude change. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of
229–255.
psychology: Personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 353–382).
Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Remington, N. A., Fabrigar, L. R., & Visser, P. S. (2000). Reexamining
the circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., Ho, S. H., Britton, J. C., & Liberzon, I.
Psychology, 79, 286–300.
(2004). Neural correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: A
trial-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 21, 768–780. Rempel-Clower, N. L. (2007). Role of orbitofrontal cortex connections in
emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1121, 72–86.
Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1999). The neuroscience of personality.
In L. A. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Ribasés, A., Hervás, J., Ramos-Quiroga, R., Bosch, A., Bielsa, X.,
Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 277–299). New York: Guilford Gastaminza, M., et al. (2008). Association study of 10 genes encoding
Press. neurotrophic factors and their receptors in adult and child attention-
deficit/hyperactivity. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 935–945.
Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Erb, H. P., Spiegel, S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2005).
Informational length and order of presentation as determinants Rietveld, M. J. H., Hudziak, J. J., Bartels, M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., &
of persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, Boomsma, D. I. (2004). Heritability of attention problems in children:
458–469. Longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to 12. Journal of
Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 45, 577–588.
Pierson, A., Le Houezec, J., Fossaert, A., Dubal, S., & Jouvent, R. (1999).
Frontal reactivity and sensation seeking an ERP study in skydivers. Rosen, S., Cochran, W., & Musser, L. M. (1990). Reactions to a match
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, versus mismatch between an applicant’s self-presentational style and
23, 447, 463. work reputation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 117–129.
Pleyers, G., Corneille, O., Luminet, O., & Yzerbyt, V. (2007). Aware Royet, J. P., Plailly, J., Delon-Martin, C., Kareken, D. A., & Segebarth, C.
and (dis)liking: Item-based analyses reveal that valence acquisition (2003). fMRI of emotional responses to odors: Influence of hedonic valence
via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency and judgment, handedness, and gender. Neuroimage, 20, 713–728.
awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C. R.,
and Cognition, 33, 130–144. Simmons, A., et al. (1999). Hypofrontality in attention deficit
426 Attitudes and Persuasion

hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: A study Shigehisa, T., & Symons, J. R. (1973). Effect of intensity of visual stimulation
with functional MRI. American Psychiatric Association, 156, on auditory sensitivity in relation to personality. British Journal of
891–896. Psychology, 64, 205–213.
Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2002). Failing to resist persuasion: Silvia, P. (2006). Reactance and the dynamics of disagreement: Multiple
Consequences for attitude strength. Paper presented at the Meeting of paths from threatened freedom to resistance to persuasion. European
the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL. Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 673–685.
Rudman, L. A., Phelan, J. E., & Heppen, J. B. (2007). Developmental Smith, B. D., Davidson, R. A., Smith, D. L., Goldstein, H., & Perlstein, W.
sources of implicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology (1989). Sensation seeking and arousal: Effects of strong stimulation on
Bulletin. 33, 1700–1713. electrodermal activation and memory task performance. Personality
Ruiter, R. A., Kessels, L. T. E., Jansma, B. M., & Brug, J. (2006). Increased and Individual Differences, 10, 671–679.
attention for computer-tailored health communications: An event- Smith, B. D., Perlstein, W. M., Davidson, R. A., & Michael, K. (1986).
related potential study. Health Psychology, 25, 300–306. Sensation seeking: Differential effect of relevant, novel stimulation on elec-
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of trodermal activity. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 445–452.
emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172. Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in
Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Understanding implicit and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 48, 813–838.
explicit attitude change: A systems of reasoning analysis. Journal of Smith, E. R. (1998). Mental representation and memory. In D. T. Gilbert,
Personality and Social Psychology. 91, 995–1008. S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., Strain, L. M., Claypool, H. M., & (pp. 269–322). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Hugenberg, K. (2007). Implicit and explicit attitudes respond dif- Smith, K. H., & Rogers, M. (1994). Effectiveness of subliminal mes-
ferently to increasing amounts of counterattitudinal information. sages in television commercials: Two experiments. Journal of Applied
European Journal of Social Psychology. 37, 867–878. Psychology. 79, 866–874.
Sagarin, B. J., & Cialdini, R. B. (2004). Creating critical consumers: Snyder, M. L., & Wicklund, R. A. (1976). Prior exercise of freedom and
Motivating receptivity by teaching resistance. In E. S. Knowles & reactance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 120–130.
J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion (pp. 259–282). Mahwah,
Sparks, J. R., & Areni, C. S. (2008). Style versus substance: Multiple
NJ: Erlbaum.
roles of language power in persuasion. Journal of Applied Social
Sagarin, B. J., Cialdini, R. B., Rice, W. E., & Serna, S. B. (2002). Dispelling Psychology, 38, 37–60.
the illusion of invulnerability: The motivations and mechanisms of
Spector, P. E., & O’Connell, B. J. (1994). The contribution of personality
resistance to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
traits, negative affectivity, locus of control and type A to the subsequent
83, 526–541.
reports of job stressors and job strains. Journal of Occupation and
Schimmack, U., & Crites, S. L., Jr. (2005). The structure of affect. In Organizational Psychology, 67, 1–11.
D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of
Stang, D. J. (1974). Methodological factors in mere exposure research.
attitudes (pp. 397–435). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychological Bulletin, 81, 1014–1025.
Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatka, H., &
Stevens S. S. (1951). Handbook of experimental psychology. Oxford: Wiley.
Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the
availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Stewart, E. W., Shimp, T. A., & Engle, R. W. (1987). Classical conditioning
61, 195–202. of consumer attitudes: Four experiments in an advertising context.
Journal of Consumer Research. 14, 334–351.
Schul, Y., Mayo, R., & Burnstein, E. (2004). Encoding under trust and dis-
trust: The spontaneous activation of incongruent cognitions. Journal of Strahan, E. J., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna, M. P. (2002). Subliminal priming
Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 668–679. and persuasion: Striking while the iron is hot. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology. 38, 556–568.
Scott, A. L., Perini, A. F., Shering, P. A., & Whalley, L. J. (1991) In-patient
major depression: Is rolipram as effective as amitriptyline? European Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry:
Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 40, 127–129. A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition sys-
tems. Psychological Science, 8, 204–210.
Senay, I., Albarracín, D., & Noguchi, K. (in press). Motivating goal-directed
behavior through introspective self-talk: The role of the interrogative Swanson, J. M., & Castellanos, F. X. (2002). Biological bases of ADHD—
form of simple future tense. Psychological Science. Neuroanatomy, genetics, and pathophysiology. In P. S. Jensen &
Sergerie, K., Lepage, M., & Armony, J. L. (2006). A process-specific func- J. R. Cooper (Eds.), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: State
tional dissociation of the amygdala in emotional memory. Journal of of the science—best practices (pp. 7–1–7–20). Kingston, NJ: Civic
Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1359–1367. Research Institute.

Shah, J. (2003). Automatic for the people: How representations of signifi- Synodinos, N. E. (1988). Review and appraisal of subliminal perception
cant others implicitly affect goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and within the context of signal detection theory. Psychology and
Social Psychology, 84, 661–681. Marketing. 5, 317–336.

Shavitt, S., Swan, S., Lowery, T. M., & Wänke, M. (1994). The interac- Szpunar, K. K., Schellenberg, E. G., & Pliner, P. (2004). Liking and memory
tion of endorser attractiveness and involvement in persuasion depends for musical stimuli as a function of exposure. Journal of Experimental
on the goal that guides message processing. Journal of Consumer Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 370–381.
Psychology. 3, 137–162. Taffel, C. (1955). Anxiety and the conditioning of verbal behavior. Journal
Sheese, B. E., Voelker, P. M., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2007). of Abnormal Psychology, 51, 496–501.
Parenting quality interacts with genetic variation in dopamine recep- Taub, J. M. (1998). Eysenck’s descriptive and biological theory of
tor D4 to influence temperament in early childhood. Development and personality: A review of construct validity. International Journal of
Psychopathology, 19, 1039–1046. Neuroscience, 94, 145–197.
Shifman, S., Bhomra, A., Smiley, S., Wray, N. R., James, M. R., Martin, N. G., Taylor, S. F., Phan, K. L., Decker, L. R., & Liberzon, I. (2003). Subjective
et al. (2008). A whole genome association study of neuroticism using rating of emotionally salient stimuli modulates neural activity.
DNA pooling. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 302–312. NeuroImage, 18, 650–659.
References 427

Tesser, A. (1993). The importance of heritability in psychological research: Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual atti-
The case of attitudes. Psychological Review, 100, 129–142. tudes. Psychological Review. 107, 101–126.
Tesser, A. (2001). On the plasticity of self-defense. Current Directions in Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the
Psychological Science, 10, 66–69. face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits posi-
Todrank, J. (1993). Evaluative conditioning of face picture CSs to odor tive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 989–1000.
USs. Dissertation Abstracts International, 54(3-B). Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. A., & Reber, R. (2003). The
Tokar, D. M., Fischer, A. R., & Subich, L. M. (1998). Personality and hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative
vocational behavior: A selective review of the literature, 1993–1997. judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evalu-
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 53, 115–153. ation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Trappey, C. (1996). A meta-analysis of consumer choice and subliminal
advertising. Psychology and Marketing. 13, 517–530. Wood, W., & Quinn, J. M. (2003). Forewarned and forearemed? Two meta-
analytic syntheses of forewarnings of influence appeals. Psychological
Turner, J. C. (2005). Explaining the nature of power: A three-process theory.
Bulletin, 129, 119–138.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 1–22.
Wooten, D. B., & Reed II, A. (1998). Informational influence and the
van Reekum, C. M., van den Berg, H., & Frijda, N. H. (1999). Cross-modal
ambiguity of product experience: Order effects on the weighting of evi-
preference acquisition: Evaluative conditioning of pictures by affective
dence. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 7, 79–99.
olfactory and auditory cues. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 831–836.
Worchel, S., & Brehm, J. W. (1971). Direct and implied social restoration
Vargas, P. T., Sekaquaptewa, D., & von Hippel, W. (2007). Armed only
of freedom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18,
with paper and pencil: “Low-tech” measures of implicit attitudes. In
294–304.
W. Bernd & S. Norbert (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp.
103–124). New York: Guilford Press. Wright, P., Albarracín, D., Brown, R. D., Li, H., He, G., & Liu, Y.
(2008). Dissociated responses in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cor-
Vargas, P. T., von Hippel, W., & Petty, R. E. (2004). Using partially struc-
tex to bottom-up and top-down components of emotional evaluation.
tured attitude measures to enhance the attitude-behavior relationship.
NeuroImage, 39, 894–902.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 30, 197–211.
Wyer, R. S. (1974). Cognitive organization and change: An information-
Vokey, J. R., & Read, J. D. (1985). Subliminal messages: Between the devil
processing approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
and the media. American Psychologist. 40, 1231–1239.
Wyer, R. S., & Albarracín, D. (2005). Belief formation, organization,
von Hippel, W., Sekaquaptewa, D., & Vargas, P. (2008). Linguistic markers and change: Cognitive and motivational influences. In D. Albarracín,
of implicit attitudes. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes
Attitudes: Insights from the new wave of implicit measures (pp. (pp.273–322). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
429–458). New York: Psychology Press.
Wyer, R. S., Budesheim, T. L., & Lambert, A. J. (1990). Cognitive repre-
Waldman, I. D., Rowe, D. C., Abramowitz, A., Kozel, S. T., Mohr, J. H., sentations of conversations about persons. Journal of Personality and
Sherman, S. L., et al. (1998). Association and linkage of the dopamine Social Psychology, 58, 218–238.
transporter gene and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children:
Heterogeneity owing to diagnostic subtype and severity. American Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of
Journal of Human Genetics, 63, 1767–1776. Personality and Social Psychology. 9, 1–27.

Walther, E., Nagengast, B., & Trasselli, C. (2005). Evaluative conditioning Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current
in social psychology: Facts and speculations. Cognition & Emotion. Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 224–228.
19, 175–196. Zanna, M. P., & Cooper J. (1974). Dissonance and the pill: An attribution
Warth, M. (2008). Beiläufige Werbewirkung als Anwendungsbeispiel approach to studying the arousal properties of dissonance. Journal of
des Fluency-Konzepts: Meta-Analyse und Kosten-Nutzen-Analyse. Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 703–709.
Unpublished master ’s thesis, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Zellner, D. A., Rozin, P., Aron, M., & Kulish, C. (1983). Conditioned
Germany. enhancement of human’s liking for flavor by pairing with sweetness.
Learning and Motivation, 14, 338–350.
Weber, S. J., & Cook, T. D. (1972). Subject effects in laboratory research:
An examination of subject roles, demand characteristics, and valid Zhang, H. T., Huang, Y., Jin, S. L., Frith, S. A., Suvarna, N., Conti, M.,
inference. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 273–295. et al. (2002). Antidepressant-like profile and reduced sensitivity to
rolipram in mice deficient in the PDE4D phosphodiesterase enzyme.
Wegener, D. T., & Carlston, D. E. (2005). Cognitive processes in atti-
Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 587–595.
tude formation and change. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, &
M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 493–542). Mahwah, Zuckerman, M. (1990). The psychophysiology of sensation seeking.
NJ: Erlbaum. Journal of Personality, 58, 313–345.
Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and non-
Weisbuch, M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2003). Prior source
verbal communication of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
exposure and persuasion: Further evidence for misattributional pro-
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 1–59). San Diego, CA:
cesses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 691–700.
Academic Press.
Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Price, J. R. (2001). Implicit/explicit memory ver- Zuckerman, M., & Driver, R. E. (1985). Telling lies: Verbal and nonverbal
sus analytic/nonanalytic processing: Rethinking the mere exposure correlates of deception. In W. A. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.),
effect. Memory and Cognition, 29, 234–246. Multichannel integration of nonverbal behavior (pp. 129–147).
Wilson, T. D., Dunn, D. S., Kraft, D., & Lisle, D. J. (1989). Introspection, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
attitude change, and attitude-behavior consistency: The disruptive Zuckerman, M., Simons, R. F., & Como, P. G. (1988). Sensation seek-
effects of explaining why we feel the way we do. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), ing and stimulus intensity as modulators of cortical, cardiovascular,
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 287–343). and electrodermal response: A cross-modality study. Personality and
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Individual Differences, 9, 361–372.
Chapter 12

Perceiving People
C. NEIL MACRAE AND SUSANNE QUADFLIEG

Your face, my thane, is as a book where men may read strange matters.
—Shakespeare (Macbeth, Act I, Scene 5: Durband, 1985)

The human face (and to a lesser extent body) is with- recently has this work captured the attention of experimental
out rival as a stimulus in the person perception process. social psychologists. A surprising paradox may account for
Daily experience attests that even the briefest of glances this oversight—the paradox of the missing persons (see
at an individual is sufficient to furnish a wealth of per- Gilbert & Hixon, 1991). Despite person perception constitut-
son-relevant information. From minimal visual cues, one ing a central area of inquiry in social psychology for decades,
can quickly decode the sex, age, emotional status, gaze two elements have surprisingly been absent in explorations of
direction, and sometimes even the intentions of a target. this topic—persons and perception. That is, despite the prior-
Moreover, if the person is familiar (e.g., Elvis Presley), ity given to perceptual processes in contemporary accounts
identity-relevant knowledge also rapidly comes to mind of face processing (Bruce & Young, 1986; Haxby et al.,
(e.g., singer, actor, American, You Ain’t Nothin’ but a 2000, 2002), work in social psychology has tended to over-
Hound Dog). Such is the dexterity with which perceivers look the importance of these operations (but see Zebrowitz,
can access person knowledge following the registration 1997), focusing instead on the cognitive and behavioral
of facial cues, Haxby, Hoffman, and Gobbini (2000) have aspects of person construal (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000).
argued that, “face perception may be the most developed This oversight can arguably be traced to a couple of fac-
visual perceptual skill in humans” (p. 223). tors. First, the dominant theoretical (and empirical) pursuit
Of course, that the face occupies a pivotal position in in social–cognitive research has been to explicate the man-
social cognition is not a recent revelation. More than 2,000 ner in which category-based knowledge (i.e., stereotypes)
years ago, Cicero (106–43 BCE) noted that “all action is of affects impressions, memories and actions, outcomes that
the mind and the mirror of the mind is the face.” Endorsing tap the products of postperceptual operations (Bodenhausen
this prescient observation, contemporary researchers have & Macrae, 1998; Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990).
sought to unravel the mysteries of human face processing Second, a favored technique for exploring person perception
and shed insight on the processes that support person under- has been to present participants with triggering verbal stimuli
standing. Although social psychologists have made consider- (e.g., category labels), a strategy that necessarily obscures the
able contributions to this literature (see Zebrowitz, 1997), for role played by perceptual operations in the detection, clas-
the most part, work on face processing has been conducted sification, and identification of social targets (but see Blair,
by cognitive psychologists and neuroscientists. As a result of Judd, & Chapleau, 2004; Cloutier, Mason, & Macrae, 2005;
their endeavours, much is now known about the mental and Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Livingston & Brewer, 2002; Macrae,
neural operations that support face processing, the nature of Bodenhausen, & Milne, 1995; Maddox & Gray, 2002;
person identification, and the extent to which person recogni- Quinn &Macrae, 2005). Yet these operations patently exert a
tion is compromised following insult to the brain (see Bruce significant influence on the course and products of the
& Humphreys, 1994; Farah, Wilson, Drain, & Tanaka, 1998; person-perception process (Bruce & Young, 1998).
Haxby et al., 2000; Haxby, Hoffman, & Gobbini, 2002). The primary objective of the current chapter is therefore
Notwithstanding the significant strides that cognitive to provide an overview of how contemporary research in
psychologists and neuroscientists have taken toward delin- social psychology, cognitive psychology, and cognitive neu-
eating the operations that underpin person perception, only roscience can inform understanding of critical components
428
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Decoding Person Cues 429

of the person perception process. In this respect, our excited by the mere passing of pimply boys, couples falling
treatment of person perception will be unashamedly literal; in love or drifting apart, divorced dads trying valiantly to
we will focus on persons and perception. In so doing, we amuse their estranged kids, or bewildered senior citizens
hope our approach will not further muddy already murky trying to get to grips with the self-service ticket device.
waters. As Gilbert (1998) observed in this very chapter But how is it that one comes to know these things about
in a previous edition of the Handbook, “Social psycholo- other people? What does it take to discern that someone is
gists have studied person perception for more than half a male or female, excited or confused, estranged or in love?
century, and for more than half a century have bemoaned Research exploring the perceptual determinants of person
the deficiencies of the phrase” (pp. 89–150). The prob- understanding provides answers to these questions.
lem can be summarized as follows. Although the object
of inquiry is unquestionable (i.e., people), the processes Types of Person Cues: Faces and Bodies
through which knowledge about the object can be acquired in Motion
(i.e., perception) has seemed less relevant, at least to social
psychologists. As a result, the term person perception has In recent years, accumulating evidence has demonstrated
often been applied to refer to how the hidden goals, beliefs, that the human visual system is exquisitely adapted for
and intentions that guide human behavior are identified, the perception of conspecifics. Not only do human faces
but rarely to elucidate the operations that enable people to attract most of our attention when we perceive other people
recognize inverted faces in poorly illuminated restaurants. (Fletcher-Watson, Findlay, Leekam, & Benson, 2008), but
Accordingly, an extensive social-psychological literature we also possess the capacity to identify a virtually unlimited
now charts how people identity and decode the myriad fac- number of individuals as we navigate our social environ-
tors (e.g., dispositions, intentions, beliefs) that shape social ments and relationships (Haxby et al., 2000). The building
interaction (Gilbert, 1998). For the most part, however, blocks of these abilities appear early in life. From birth,
this work considers the perception of personality, not the infants display a preference for facelike stimuli over other
perception of people. visual patterns, and during the first 3 months of life, they
Given that even highly sophisticated judgments or attri- progressively hone their face-processing skills (Morton &
butions about others are fueled by inputs that derive from Johnson, 1991; Simion, Leo, Turati, Valenza, & Dalla
the workings of early perceptual operations, our goal in Barba, 2007). Between 3 and 7 months of age, infants gain
this chapter is to consider the perceptual processes through the ability to distinguish the face of their mother from the
which person understanding emerges. Hence we attempt faces of strangers and begin to categorize people accord-
to elucidate how perceptual operations contribute toward ing to sex and emotional expression (Nelson, 2001). In
the abstraction of both variant (e.g., emotional expression, addition, from 4 months onward, infants develop a tem-
gaze direction) and invariant (e.g., sex, identity) aspects of plate for perceiving the prototypical configuration of faces
person knowledge from available physical cues (Bruce & (i.e., two lateralized eyes over a centralized nose over a
Young, 1986; Burton, Bruce, & Johnston, 1990; Haxby mouth), as evidenced by the emergence of the face inver-
et al., 2000, 2002). In this respect, following a description sion effect (FIE; Fagan, 1972).
of the different classes of person knowledge that can be The FIE, a phenomenon that is also reliably observed
extracted from facial and bodily cues, we switch attention among healthy adults, refers to the observation that when
to a consideration of the basic processing operations and faces and objects are inverted (i.e., rotated through 180
associated neural structures that support person percep- degrees), recognition performance decreases more dramati-
tion. In so doing, our objective is to furnish an integrative cally for the former than the latter stimuli (Yin, 1969). This
account of contemporary research exploring the dynamics effect has been attributed to the different types of informa-
of person construal. tion that can be extracted from available facial cues. During
processing, not only do perceivers extract information relat-
ing to single facial features (e.g., mouth, nose, eyes, ears,
DECODING PERSON CUES chin, cheekbones—feature-based information), they also
encode information pertaining to the spatial arrangement
Consider for a moment the last time you stood in line to or configuration of facial features (i.e., configural informa-
secure tickets at the cinema. Besides thinking about which tion; see Maurer, Le Grand, & Mondloch, 2002; Rossion &
movie to view or whether or not to purchase popcorn and Gauthier, 2002). After faces are inverted, whereas the pro-
a fizzy drink, one’s colleagues in the queue can be a con- cessing of single features proceeds as normal, the extraction
stant source of entertainment. For example, depending on of configural (i.e., relational) information is compromised.
the time of day, one is likely to see groups of teenage girls Because inversion impedes recognition performance for
430 Perceiving People

faces more severely than other classes of object (Maurer When, however, the stimuli are shown in motion, naïve
et al., 2002), the FIE has been taken as evidence that person observers can readily recognize a human form and asso-
recognition is reliant on the extraction of configural infor- ciated movements (Blake & Shiffrar, 2007). This percep-
mation from facial cues. tion of PL motion remains remarkably robust even when
The observation that face recognition is highly depen- dynamic noise (i.e., additional lights) is added to the
dent on configural information gains further support from display (Hiris, Humphrey, & Stout, 2005). Mirroring
a related memorial phenomenon, the face composite effect the effects reported with static faces and bodies, the abil-
(FCE). This effect derives from the demonstration that ity to detect motion is impaired when PL animations are
when the top and bottom halves of a face comprise dif- inverted (Hiris, Krebeck, Edmonds, & Stout, 2005). PL
ferent people (e.g., top half  your mother; bottom half  animations are of theoretical interest because they convey
your aunt), participants experience difficulty reporting patterns of motion with little or no form-related informa-
the identity of the person portrayed in the top half of the tion and the visual details that usually accompany person
face (Hole, 1994). It is assumed that the FCE occurs perception (e.g., clothing, emotional expression, facial
because the extraction of configural information prompts attractiveness). As such, they give an important insight
participants to fuse the two parts of the composite stimu- into the impact that motion cues alone exert on the process
lus into a novel person, thereby impairing their ability to of person construal. Given the role that motion cues play
recognize the identity of each of the constituent elements. in the person perception process, it is unsurprising to learn
Corroborating this viewpoint, recognition performance that the ability to detect PL depictions of human movement
is enhanced when the processing of configural informa- emerges early in life and remains intact across the lifespan.
tion is impaired by misaligning the top and bottom halves At 4 months of age, infants prefer looking at a sequence of
of the novel composite face (Young, Hellawell, & Hay, dots that depict human motion rather than random move-
1987). ment (Fox & McDaniel, 1982). In the elderly, the ability to
It is important to note that not only faces appear to enjoy perceive human motion from PL animations is relatively
a privileged status during stimulus processing; so, too, stable despite significant age-related deficits in percep-
do human bodies. Compared with other classes of objects, tual processing (Gilmore, Wenk, Baylor, & Stuve, 1992;
human bodies capture a disproportionate amount of atten- Norman, Payton, Long, & Hawkes, 2004).
tion during stimulus processing (Downing, Bray, Rogers, & Taken together, these lines of research tell an emerging
Childs, 2004). Like faces, bodies are constituted of a set tale. Perceiving others offers a wealth of visual information
of common elements (e.g., two arms, two legs, a trunk) that is potentially relevant for both person understanding
that are arranged in a typical configuration (e.g., arms and social interaction. Usually a mere glance is sufficient
located in the upper and legs located in the lower half of for a perceiver to extract information about a person’s
the trunk). Hence, similar to face recognition, body identi- facial features and expression, hairstyle, body shape,
fication requires individuals to make fine-grained percep- height and posture, gaze direction, head–body orienta-
tual distinctions based on the shape and configuration of tion, and facial and bodily movements. In combination,
various parts. In this respect, it is interesting to note that, these static forms and dynamic motion cues can be used to
like person identification, stimulus inversion impairs body infer conceptual “person knowledge” about other social
recognition (i.e., the recognition of static body postures), agents. Such knowledge can be variable across situations
thus revealing that core aspects of person perception (i.e., (e.g., when inferences concern a person’s emotional state)
face and body recognition) rely on the extraction of con- or stable across different processing contexts (e.g., infer-
figural information from available stimulus cues (Reed, ences regarding a person’s identity). Attention now turns
Stone, Bozova, & Tanaka, 2003; Reed, Stone, Grubb, & to a consideration of these classes of person knowledge
McGoldrick, 2006). and how they fuel the person perception process.
As primary sources of person-related knowledge, both
faces and bodies convey additional information when they Extracting Variant Person Knowledge
are in motion. Studies employing animated point-light (PL)
displays have demonstrated that socially relevant informa- Many person inferences are dependent on the situation in
tion can readily be gleaned from people’s patterns of move- which they emerge. Most prominently, for example, deter-
ment. In work of this kind, movement is portrayed via the mining a person’s direction of attention or emotional state is
relative motion of a small number of PL markers that are a dynamic, context-specific problem. In addition, evaluating
positioned on the head and the joints of the body. Importantly, the person’s personality and attractiveness can also be shaped
when presented as static images, the depicted assem- by the detection of specific contextual cues. The following
bly of lights appears entirely meaningless to perceivers. paragraphs summarize what is currently known about
Decoding Person Cues 431

how perceivers extract variant person knowledge about rapidly according to sex (Campbell, Wallace, & Benson,
others. 1996; Macrae, Hood, Milne, Rowe, & Mason, 2002).
Faces with direct gaze and neutral expression are also rated
Social Attention as more attractive when sexually relevant for the perceiver
Direction of gaze (in combination with head–body direc- and more likeable (regardless of sexual relevance) than faces
tion) is a crucial medium through which people transmit with averted gaze (Mason, Tatkow, & Macrae, 2005). One
important social information. According to Baron-Cohen possibility is that effects of this kind are elicited by direct
(1995), detecting the presence of eyes and determining eye contact capturing a perceiver ’s attention and delaying
where they are looking is one of the primary objectives of his or her disengagement from a face (Baron-Cohen, 1995;
the brain (see Brothers, 1990). As Langton, Watt, and Bruce Senju & Hasegawa, 2005; von Grünau & Anston, 1995).
(2000) reported, “Humans and many other species tend to Aside from the processing benefits emerging from mutual
look at things in their environment that are of immediate eye contact, averted gaze also plays a significant role in social
interest to them. You might be the recipient of another ’s cognition. When a target displays averted gaze, perceivers
gaze, for instance, because you are a potential meal, a mate can readily follow the direction of the eyes to ensure that both
or simply because you are someone with whom they would self and other are attending to the same object (or person) in
like to interact” (pp. 51–52). Given the informational value space, a phenomenon referred to as joint attention. Although
of gaze cues, it makes good sense that people should be infants reliably establish joint attention from around 9 months
sensitized to eye gaze in others. As gaze direction signals (Cleveland & Striano, 2007), even at 3 months they display a
the appearance and relative importance of objects in the reflexive gaze-triggered orienting response (Hood, Willen, &
environment (e.g., friends, food, speeding vehicles), con- Driver, 1998). This orienting response has been explored
siderable adaptive advantages can be gleaned from an extensively in adults. In the favored experimental para-
information-processing system that is finely tuned to gaze digm, participants are presented with gaze cues (i.e., averted
direction and interpretation. Importantly, the structure of eyes) that orient attention either “toward” or “away from”
the human eye facilitates just such a task. Comprising a targets that appear at peripheral locations. Participants’ task
small dark area (i.e., the pupil and iris) surrounded by is simply to respond to the cued or noncued targets. Using
a region of white (i.e., the sclera), the eye is well adapted to this type of methodology, an extensive literature has dem-
signal gaze direction, hence social attention (see Frischen, onstrated that congruency between the gaze cue and target
Bayliss, & Tipper, 2007). Practically speaking, gaze cues location facilitates responding, thereby revealing that gaze
can readily signal whether an object of significance has cues trigger reflexive shifts in visual attention (Driver et al.,
appeared in the immediate environment (e.g., the boss) 1999). Interestingly, although faces that correctly signal tar-
or if one is the target of another person’s attention (i.e., get location via gaze cues are perceived to be more trustwor-
direct gaze). Although perceivers are highly accurate at thy than faces that look in the opposite direction, reflexive
decoding the gaze direction of others, this ability can be shifts in attention are not modulated by the trustworthi-
compromised when the eyes are inverted in facial images, ness of the cue providers (Bayliss & Tipper, 2006). That is,
a finding that suggests configural operations support the spatial cueing and person evaluation are decoupled.
processing of gaze cues (Jenkins & Langton, 2003). Recently, the relationship between attentional cueing
The ability to decode and interpret gaze cues emerges and the interplay of gaze, head and bodily orientation has
early in life. Almost from birth, infants show a fascination been targeted in a number of investigations. This work has
with eyes over other regions of the face (Farroni, Csibra, demonstrated that gaze-triggered shifts in attention are more
Simion, & Johnson, 2002; Morton & Johnson, 1991) and, pronounced when gaze and head orientation signal differ-
by the age of 4 months, infants can discriminate direct from ent locations than when the cues are congruent (Hietanen,
averted gaze (Vecera & Johnson, 1995). By the time they 1999). Similar results have been observed when gaze and
are between the ages of 9 and 18 months, young children bodily orientation signal different locations (Hietanen,
begin to read the eyes in terms of goal direction, attending 2002). Taken together, these findings suggest that inferring
immediately to the eyes when the intentions of an actor are another person’s direction of attention involves the inte-
ambiguous (Phillips, Baron-Cohen, & Rutter, 1992). Among grated perception of eye, head, and body position, although
adults, it has been demonstrated that, under conditions of gaze cues play a dominant role in signalling the appearance
direct gaze, the processes supporting person perception are of objects in the environment (Frischen et al., 2007).
optimized. Consequences of mutual gaze include that faces
Emotional Status
with direct compared with averted gaze are more likely to
be remembered (Hood, Macrae, Cole-Davies, & Dias, 2003; People’s attempt to make sense of their immediate social
Mason, Hood, & Macrae, 2004) and to be categorized more environment is also tightly associated with the goal of
432 Perceiving People

understanding the emotional states of others. Is the dentist of the jaw (Bassili, 1979). In this respect, even quite subtle
smiling because she is friendly or nervous? Why is the facial motion has been shown to enhance the identification
man on the park bench crying? Many studies suggest that of facial emotions (Ambadar, Schooler, & Cohn, 2005;
perceivers routinely use a range of static and dynamic cues Bould & Morris, 2008).
(i.e., facial and bodily) to infer the emotional states of other Recent data have also demonstrated that the perception
social agents. Although it appears to be generally the case of basic facial expressions can be modulated by contex-
that judging emotional intensity relies strongly on motion tual cues. Specifically, identification is shaped both by the
cues (Atkinson, Dittrich, Gemmell, & Young, 2004), accu- context in which facial expressions are encountered and
rate identification of a specific emotion appears to depend the bodily posture that accompanies emotional expression
critically on the structural combination of facial and bodily (Aviezer et al., 2008; Ekman, 1965). Put simply, facial
features. For instance, extensive research has demon- expressions are categorized most rapidly when they are
strated that humans are able to recognize at least seven paired with emotionally congruent body postures (Meeren,
basic expressions of emotion from facial expressions (i.e., van Heijnsbergen, & de Gelder, 2005; Van den Stock,
happiness, fear, surprise, anger, sadness, disgust, and con- Righart, & de Gelder, 2007). In addition, although per-
tempt) with high accuracy and consistency (Ekman, 2003). ceivers can read emotions from static postures at a level
Although some facial features are particularly salient for above chance (Coulsen, 2004; Ekman, 1965; Wallbott,
the detection of specific emotions (e.g., wide eyes for sur- 1998), bodily cues in motion significantly enhance emo-
prise, knitted eyebrows for sadness, smile for happiness, tion recognition (Atkinson et al., 2004; Chouchourelou,
frown for anger), the ability to decode emotional expres- Matsuka, Harber, & Shiffrar, 2006; Clark, Bradshaw,
sions commonly requires the integration of several facial Field, Hampson, & Rose, 2005; Pollick, Lestou, Ryu, &
features (Ellison & Massaro, 1997). For example, gaze Cho, 2002). Even the motion of isolated body parts, such
direction appears to play a prominent role in emotion rec- as the manner in which an arm is moved while drinking,
ognition. In particular, expressions of anger are perceived waving, or lifting, can be sufficient to infer the emotional
more accurately, categorized more quickly, and rated states of others (Pollick et al., 2002; Pollick, Paterson,
as more intense when eye gaze is directed “toward” than Bruderlin, & Sanford, 2001). In particular, research indi-
“away from” the perceiver. Interestingly, the opposite is cates that the bodily movements that accompany anger
true for expressions of fear (see Adams & Kleck, 2003; or elation tend to be faster, more energetic, and spatially
Sander, Grandjean, Kaiser, Wehrle, & Scherer, 2006). expansive than those that signal passive emotions such
Findings such as these suggest that the perception of facial as contempt, sadness, and boredom (Camras, Sullivan, &
emotions is influenced by the potential behavioral intent Michel, 1993; Montepare, Koff, Zaitchik, & Albert, 1999;
of the cue provider to approach or avoid the perceiver. Wallbott, 1998). In addition, exaggerated motion cues are
According to this line of reasoning, whereas direct gaze generally associated with the perception of elevated emo-
is thought to enhance the perception of approach-oriented tional intensity across diverse kinds of emotions (Atkinson
emotions such as anger and joy, averted gaze is assumed et al., 2004).
to enhance the perception of avoidance-oriented emotions
Personality Traits
such as fear and sadness (Adams & Kleck, 2005).
Further evidence speaking to the idea that perceivers It is well established that people are highly attuned to
integrate information from several facial features for effi- drawing personality inferences when perceiving others. An
cient emotion recognition comes from studies investigating outer appearance that signals the possession of a certain
the mechanisms that subserve the decoding of emotional characteristic or trait can have far-reaching consequences
expressions. Perceivers have been shown to exhibit both a in many domains of everyday life. Take facial character-
face inversion and a face composite effect for emotion rec- istics, for example. It has been shown that assessments of
ognition, thereby suggesting that the processing of config- dominance are related to cadets’ level of attainment (i.e.,
ural information contributes to the interpretation of facial rank) in the military (Mueller & Mazur, 1996), judgments
expressions (Durand, Gallay, Seigneuric, Robichon, & of competence are predictive of people’s political voting
Baudouin, 2007). Beyond the integration of static features decisions (Todorov, Mandisodza, Goren, & Hall, 2005),
to determine the emotions of others, specific motion cues and ratings of power-related traits in chief executive offi-
also seem to play an important role for emotion perception. cers are associated with company profits (Rule & Ambady,
For example, whereas anger seems to be conveyed by a 2008). Beyond face-based inferences, increased body
downward movement of the forehead and a compression height—a feature that is tightly associated with social
of the mouth, surprise is associated with a rapid upward dominance (Roberts & Herman, 1986; Young & French,
movement of the brow coupled with a downward movement 1998)—has been demonstrated to be positively related to
Decoding Person Cues 433

increased income and occupational success (Judge & obvious importance to perceivers may be gleaned from
Cable, 2004). Finally, wearing revealing clothes, a signal facial cues with particular rapidity (Bar, Neta, & Linz,
of immodesty and apparent promiscuity, has been shown 2006). Some recent work has also suggested that faces
to cause laypeople, judges, police officers, and prosecu- may automatically be screened for trustworthiness, regard-
tors alike to hold victims of rape to be responsible for their less of the goals or intentions of the perceiver (Engell,
assault (Lennon, Johnson, & Schulz, 1999). These brief Haxby, & Todorov, 2007; Todorov, Baron, & Oosterhof,
examples reveal that, despite conventional societal wis- 2008; but see Santos & Young, 2005). As in other domains
dom that is it inappropriate to judge a book by its cover, of face processing, facial inversion impedes the extraction of
social perceivers rarely heed this advice. personality-related information, a finding that further
Numerous lines of research (Albright, Kenny, & Malloy, underscores the importance of configural information to
1988; Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992; Behling & Williams, core aspects of person perception (Santos & Young, 2008).
1991; Borkenau & Liebler, 1992; Kenny, Horner, Beyond facial features, initial work using PL displays
Kashy, & Chu, 1992; Norman, & Goldberg, 1966; Secord, has revealed that inferences regarding a person’s extra-
Dukes, & Bevan, 1954) have provided compelling evi- version, warmth, and trustworthiness can also be drawn
dence that trait judgments can readily be drawn from a from patterns of body movement alone (e.g., Heberlein,
person’s physiognomy (i.e., facial features), outer appear- Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio, 2004; Heberlein & Saxe,
ance (i.e., clothing), or demeanor (i.e., posture, walking 2005; Montepare & Zebrowitz-McArthur, 1988). Further
style). Of particular importance, people tend to agree on research is required, however, to clarify how quickly these
the traits and qualities they attribute to others, a phenom- motion-based inferences are drawn and whether they occur
enon that has been termed consensus at zero acquaintance. in a mandatory fashion similar to facial evaluations.
Remarkably, this consensus can even be observed cross-
Physical Attractiveness
culturally, suggesting that some trait inferences may have
a universal basis (Albright et al., 1997; Secord & Bevan, It has long been established that the ascription of personal-
1956). Notwithstanding this observation, however, some ity traits can also be influenced by a person’s attractive-
traits are more inferable than others and elicit higher con- ness. Socialized on a diet of Hollywood fare, one quickly
sensus across perceivers (Funder & Colvin, 1998; Kenny learns that attractive people live in elegant houses, drive
et al., 1992; Norman & Goldberg, 1996). For example, expensive cars, and lead unblemished and perfect lives. Put
while judging a person’s extraversion and conservatism simply, that which is beautiful is also healthy, wealthy, and
from appearance cues elicits high consensus across per- wise (Dion, Berscheid, & Walster, 1972). Tempting though
ceivers, judgments of neuroticism or openness reveal little it may be to source this belief to the shallower facets of
or no agreement (Beer & Watson, 2008). contemporary society, in reality even the ancient Greeks
Given people’s ability to infer traits from person cues, equated beauty with the possession of positive qualities.
an obvious question arises. Exactly which features do peo- Generally speaking, the benefits of looking good are plenti-
ple use to guide their judgments? At present this issue is ful. Attractive people are seen to possess more desirable
poorly understood, although it is apparent that the selection characteristics and are treated more positively than their
of features is clearly trait-dependent. For example, people less attractive counterparts, a phenomenon that has been
are deemed to be highly extraverted when they have a labeled the beauty-is-good stereotype (Dion et al., 1972;
round face, stylish hair, wear extravagant clothing, show Eagly, Ashmore, Makhijani, & Longo, 1991). Even under
a friendly expression, display frequent and rapid body move- conditions in which no explicit evaluation of an individual
ments, and walk in a relaxed manner (Borkenau & Liebler, is required, heightened attractiveness elicits feelings of pos-
1992). To elucidate further the process of trait ascription fol- itivity (Olson & Marshuetz, 2005; van Leeuwen & Macrae,
lowing person perception, recent studies have explored the 2004). Coupled with beautiful people attracting bundles of
persistence of these inferences and the extent to which positivity, unattractive persons are commonly viewed and
they can be drawn under impoverished viewing conditions. evaluated in an unfavorable manner. For example, they are
This work has demonstrated that a mere 100 ms exposure deemed to be less sociable, altruistic, and intelligent than
to an unfamiliar face is sufficient for participants to draw their attractive counterparts (Griffin & Langlois, 2006).
likeability, trustworthiness, competence, and aggressive- The force of these stereotypes can be clearly quantified
ness inferences about the target that are similar to those across the life span. Attractive kids elicit more attention
generated under longer viewing times (Willis & Todorov, and affection and are punished less severely than unattract-
2006). For threat—but not intelligence—judgments, ive children (Dion, 1972; Langlois, Ritter, Casey, & Sawin,
the same effect has been reported for exposure times as 1995). In adulthood, heightened attractiveness is related
brief as 39 ms, indicating that personality traits that have to increased mating opportunities (Epstein, Klinkenberg,
434 Perceiving People

Scandell, Faulkner, & Claus, 2007), better employment are considered to be more attractive when this luminance
prospects (Dubois & Pansu, 2004), advantageous work difference is increased (as is often artificially done by
evaluations (Hamermesh & Parker, 2005), increased earn- applying cosmetics on the lips and eyes), male faces are
ing potential (Frieze, Olson, Russell, 1991), and longev- rated as more attractive when the luminance difference is
ity (Henderson & Anglin, 2003). As Langlois et al. (2000) decreased (Russell, 2003). Further components of facial
concluded on the basis of an integrative meta-analysis of attractiveness include a healthy skin and dental appear-
such research findings, “The effects of facial attractiveness ance, a pleasant expression, youthfulness, and good
are robust and pandemic . . . attractiveness is a significant grooming (Deffenbacher, Vetter, Johanson, & O’Toole,
advantage for both children and adults in almost every 1998; Jones, Little, Burt, & Perrett, 2004; Rhodes, 2006;
domain of judgment, treatment, and behavior” (p. 404). Willis, Esqueda, & Schacht, 2008).
But what exactly does heightened attractiveness actually Regardless of the specific cues that signal facial beauty,
mean? In addressing this issue, much attention has been research has indicated that in adulthood the perception
dedicated to uncover whether stable facial and bodily mark- of facial attractiveness occurs rapidly. Even when faces
ers of physical beauty can be identified. Although recent are presented for less than 110 ms, participants are able
work provides evidence that beyond shared standards of to judge the attractiveness of a target (Locher, Unger,
facial beauty, personal taste also significantly impacts judg- Sociedade, & Wahl, 1993; Olson & Marshuetz, 2005).
ments of attractiveness (Hönekopp, 2006), it appears that Supporting this ability is the extraction of configural infor-
such judgments are partially consistent across sex, culture, mation from faces (Bruce & Young, 1986). Participants
race, and even age (Langlois et al. 2000; Rhodes, 2006; required to evaluate the attractiveness of the top half of
Thornhill & Gangestad, 1999). Already newborns prefer a face were found to report increased rating of attractive-
looking at attractive rather than unattractive faces and young ness when this portion of the stimulus was aligned with an
children agree with adults about which faces are attractive attractive compared with an unattractive bottom half,
(see Ramsey, Langlois, Hoss, Rubenstein, & Griffin, 2004; an effect that indicates that attractiveness judgments entail
Slater, Bremner, Johnson, Sherwood, Hayes, & Brown, the integration of facial features into a unique whole
2000). Findings such as these suggest that beliefs about (Abbas & Duchaine, 2008).
attractiveness are far from idiosyncratic and may even be In addition to facial cues, bodily information also con-
partially innate (i.e., not attributable to the assimilation of tributes to assessments of attractiveness. Although little is
cultural standards of beauty). known about the precise manner in which facial and bodily
Specific facial attributes that have been consistently linked cues influence overall assessments of beauty, initial evi-
to increased attractiveness are bilateral facial symmetry and dence suggests that these cues contribute independently to
the extent to which a face conforms to a spatially aver- people’s judgments (Peters, Rhodes, & Simmons, 2007).
aged prototype of a population (Rhodes, 2006; Thornhill & The importance of bodily attractiveness becomes most
Gangestad, 1999). When facial symmetry is ampli- apparent when considering perceptions of the overweight.
fied experimentally, attractiveness generally increases, as Men and women who deviate from societal standards
long as the manipulation does not introduce structural of bodily attractiveness by being overweight are seen as
abnormalities (Perrett, Burt, Penton-Voak, Lee, Rowland, & flawed not only in their appearance but also in terms of
Edwards, 1999; Rhodes, Proffitt, Grady, & Sumich, their personality (DeJong & Kleck, 1986; Hebl & Turchin,
1998). Similarly, individual faces can be made more (or 2005). Participants ascribe more negative traits (e.g., lazy,
less) attractive by moving their configurations closer to incompetent, boring, undisciplined) to overweight than aver-
(or further from) an average same-sex template, and faces age-weight individuals (Brochu & Morrison, 2007; Puhl,
that naturally lie closer to this template are deemed to be Moss-Racusin, Schwartz, & Brownell, 2008), an outcome
more attractive than less typical faces (Rhodes, 2006). In that precipitates serious discrimination in health, work, and
addition, sexual dimorphism (i.e., high femininity in female educational settings (Puhl & Brownell, 2001).
faces and high masculinity in male faces) has been associ- Interestingly, body-shape inferences are strongly depen-
ated with increased facial attractiveness. Sexually dimor- dent on cultural influences, such that increased body fat is
phic features in males correspond to thick brow ridges, considered to be attractive in some societies (Anderson,
thin lips, a large square jaw and small eyes, whereas they Crawford, Nadeau, & Lindberg, 1992). In current Western
are constituted by a small lower facial area, fuller lips, and societies, however, men are deemed to be most attractive
smaller chins in females (Rhodes, 2006). Finally, the lumi- when they are tall, muscular with broad chests, low in body
nance difference between the darker regions of the eyes and fat, and possess a general air of physical fitness (Fan, Dai,
mouth and the surrounding lighter skin has been identified Liu, & Wu, 2005; Hönekopp, Rudolph, Beier, Liebert, &
as a marker of facial attractiveness. Whereas female faces Müller, 2007; Weeden & Sabini, 2005). Women, in contrast,
Decoding Person Cues 435

are considered to be to be most attractive when they possess with membership in these groups (Brewer, 1988; Fiske &
a small waist-to-hip ratio (WHR; a measure of body shape, Neuberg, 1990). Compared with men, women are believed
computed by dividing a person’s waist circumference to be less intelligent, competent, and ambitious. However,
by his or her hip circumference) and a body mass index if they are communal and conform to traditional female
(BMI; a measure of body weight computed by dividing a roles, women are deemed to be wonderful (Rudman, 2005).
person’s weight in kilograms by the square of his or her Similarly, African Americans are stereotypically assumed to
height in meters) at the lower end of the normal range be lazy, criminal, and uneducated but also musical and ath-
(Furnham, Swami, & Shah, 2006; Singh, 1993; Weeden & letic (Devine & Elliot, 1995), whereas Asian Americans are
Sabini, 2005). The contribution of breast size to percep- considered to be intelligent, industrious, conservative, and
tions of attractiveness of women remains open to debate. shy (Lin, Kwan, Cheung, & Fiske, 2005). In addition, ste-
Although some data suggest that female attractiveness is reotypes about age groups suggest that lovable, dependent,
associated with large breasts (Singh & Young, 1995), other and weak infants will turn into belligerent, moody, and irre-
studies reveal the opposite effect (Furnham & Swami, sponsible adolescents before they suffer physical and mental
2007). Additional research suggests that larger breasts decline including tiredness, forgetfulness, and self-pity by
enhance ratings of attractiveness only when they are paired the time they reach old age (Angus & Reeve, 2006; Perdue &
with a body possessing a low WHR (Furnham, Dias, & Gurtman, 1990; Zebrowitz & Montepare, 2000).
McClelland, 1998). Of course, conceptions of ideal breast As consensual beliefs about individuals based on
size have fluctuated dramatically in history and depend knowledge of the groups to which they belong, stereotypes
critically on the sex of the perceiver (Furnham, Hester, & are ingrained in the very fabric of society (Fiske, 1998;
Weir, 1990; Mazur, 1986). For these reasons, it is unlikely Lippmann, 1922), are acquired in early childhood (Hill &
that breast size in a reliable marker of female attractiveness Flom, 2007; Poulin-Dubois, Serbin, Eichstedt, Sen, & Beissel,
across different perceivers (Furnham & Swami, 2007). 2002), and are notoriously resistant to modification or
Beyond mere body shape, a person’s posture and manner change. Given that the indiscriminate application of ste-
of movement have also been associated with attractive- reotypes often promotes judgmental inaccuracy, societal
ness. For example, a slumped posture is known to reduce inequality, and intergroup conflict (Fiske, 1998), major
ratings of physical attractiveness (Osborn, 1996), whereas research efforts have centered on elucidating the mechanisms
in women a feminine walking style with swaying hips and in that support social stereotyping. In this respect, three sequen-
men a masculine pattern of movement with swaggering tial steps have been identified: (a) social categorization—
shoulders increases perceived attractiveness (Johnson & perceivers’ propensity to classify others according to
Tassinary, 2007). salient categorical markers (Allport, 1954; Brewer, 1988;
Fiske & Neuberg, 1990), (b) stereotype activation—
the extent to which associated stereotypic knowledge is
Extracting Invariant Person Knowledge
accessible in a given situation, and (c) stereotype appli-
Contrary to the extraction of variant person knowledge, cation—whether this information serves as the basis
some pivotal inferences about others result in relatively of person understanding (Devine, 1989; Gilbert & Hixon,
stable representations regardless of the situation-specific 1991, Kunda & Sinclair, 1999).
person cues that may be present. Such stable representa- Naturally, a perceiver ’s propensity to classify others
tions, for example, target categorical person knowledge according to meaningful social groups depends on the ease
such as a person’s sex or race but also his or her identity. with which this information can be extracted from available
visual cues. Although some group memberships are made
Categorical Knowledge visible through choice (e.g., sports fans dressed in the col-
To balance the perceptual demands of individuated (i.e., ors of their team), others are identifiable through unavoid-
person-specific) processing with the goal of making sense able visual markers. Critically, the “big three” categories
of others rapidly and efficiently, perceivers regularly rely in person perception (Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg,
on categorical knowledge structures to simplify the person 1990)—sex, race, and age—can easily be gleaned from
perception process. As a result, impressions and evalua- salient biological cues. Unsurprisingly, therefore, these
tions of others are influenced not only by a person’s spe- categories dominate people’s perceptions of others
cific appearance but also by the social categories to which across a range of social situations (van Knippenberg &
he or she belongs and the stereotypes associated with these Dijksterhuis, 2000).
groups. Dominant stereotypes in Western society, for exam- Notwithstanding the fact that a person’s face and body
ple, target a person’s sex, race, and age and typically com- offers numerous clues to his or her sex, the relative contri-
prise beliefs regarding the traits and behaviors associated bution of these cues to the process of sex categorization is
436 Perceiving People

still poorly understood. Recent evidence on sex judgments Thus, it is perhaps not surprising that recent data suggest that
suggests that when perceivers are directly requested to body shape typically guides people’s sex judgments more
report the sex of a target, hair cues alone are sufficient strongly than motion cues (Johnson & Tassinary, 2005).
to perform this task with both accuracy and rapidity (Goshen- Much like sex, perceivers are also remarkably adept at
Gottstein & Ganel, 2000; Macrae & Martin, 2007; Martin & extracting a person’s age (George & Hole, 1995, 2000).
Macrae, 2007). Even when cues from hairstyle, makeup, Again, however, the relative contribution of various age-
and facial hair are minimized, however, sex categorization specifying features to person categorization remains open to
remains a relatively easy task (Burton, Bruce, & Dench, debate (Burt & Perrett, 1995; George & Hole, 1995, 1998).
1993). This ability to distinguish the sexes is likely a con- From birth to early adolescence, faces are characterized
sequence of the structural changes in facial appearance that by a forward-sloping forehead, larger lower-placed eyes,
occur during puberty. Whereas testosterone stimulates the a smaller and less protruding chin, and lighter skin. Facial
growth of the jaw, cheekbones, and brow ridges in men, aging, in contrast, is characterized by leathery, crinkled,
estrogen inhibits this development in women and contrib- open-pored, and blemished skin; lengthened ears and nose;
utes to an increase in lip size (Thornhill & Møller, 1997). As dropped eyelids; a less angular jaw; a stronger double chin;
a result of these structural differences, the isolated presen- and thinned, graying hair (Berry & McArthur, 1986; Bruce &
tation of certain adult face features can be sufficient for per- Young, 1998). Surprisingly, age perception is relatively unim-
ceivers to identify sex at levels above chance, with the eye paired by facial inversion, suggesting that isolated category-
and brow region being most predictive of sex, followed by specifying features (e.g., skin texture) may be sufficient to
the mouth and the nose (Brown & Perrett, 1993; Roberts & drive age categorization (George & Hole, 2000). Beyond
Bruce, 1988). Furthermore, at least among Caucasians, facial cues, human development triggers changes elsewhere
male faces postpuberty tend to be darker and redder than in the body. Compared with adults, infants are obviously
female faces (Jablonski & Chaplin, 2000), a difference in shorter and chubbier and possess disproportionately large
skin tone that perceivers also successfully exploit to classify heads and short limbs. As such, children are easily identi-
targets according to sex (Hill, Bruce, & Akamatsu, 1995; fied on the basis of their body shape. Complementing these
Nestor & Tarr, 2008). In summary, although isolated facial static feature-based cues, facial and bodily motion can also
features are sufficient to determine a person’s sex, research signal a person’s age (Berry, 1990; Montepare & Zebrowitz-
has indicated that sex categorization may often rely on the McArthur, 1988). Whereas young children are characterized
integration of several features (Baudoin & Humphreys, by a toddling walk, adults have a style that involves swaying
2006; Bruce et al., 1993; Brown & Perrett, 1993; Roberts & hips, bent knees, swinging arms, and an increased walking
Bruce, 1988; Schyns, Bonnar, & Gosselin, 2002). pace, markers that all diminish as one ages. Using these
Faces, of course, are not the only marker of sex; men age-related differences in human motion, perceivers seem
and women can also be differentiated on the basis of bodily to be able to establish the relative vintage of an individual
cues. Apart from primary markers such as breasts, body (i.e., young or old; see Montepare & Zebrowitz-McArthur,
shape can also be used to identify the sex of individuals 1988), although they remain relatively poor at judging a
(Johnson & Tassinary, 2005). After puberty, females tend target’s absolute age from motion cues alone.
to deposit more fat on the hips than men so on average they Although the biological markers that signal a person’s
end up with a lower WHR. In addition, the average ratio age and sex have received considerable empirical attention,
between hip width and body height is larger for females exploration of the features that drive racial categorization
than males, and because men have a greater shoulder width, has been less commonplace, probably because the iden-
ratios of shoulder to hip width can also signal a person’s tity of the critical category-specifying cue seems obvious.
sex (Pollick, Kay, Heim, & Stringer, 2005). Taken still Without much thought, skin tone indicates whether a per-
further, men and women can also be differentiated on the son is of African, Asian, or Caucasian descent (Maddox,
basis of their patterns of facial and bodily movement, espe- 2004). Beyond color cues, however, the shape of facial
cially when viewed from the front (Hill, Jinno, & Johnston, features also plays a prominent role in race categorization.
2003; Hill & Johnston, 2001; Kozlowski & Cutting, 1977; For example, Asians are associated with the possession of
Pollick et al., 2005). It is worth noting, however, that sex almond eyes, and individuals of African descent are rec-
classification from motion cues has been shown to be quite ognized on the basis of nose shape and hair texture (Blair,
malleable and subject to adaptation based on one’s prior Judd, Sadler, & Jenkins, 2002).
viewing history (Jordon, Fallah, & Stoner, 2006). In addi- Triggered by readily available person cues, the effects of
tion, perceivers fail to extract sex-specific cues from dynamic stereotyping on person perception can be pervasive and per-
arm movements; despite the fact that computational neural nicious. For instance, corroborating traditional age-related
networks are able to perform this task (Pollick et al., 2002). stereotypes, faces of elderly individuals are rated as less
Decoding Person Cues 437

energetic and likeable than younger adults (Ebner, 2008). Intriguingly, once a face has been categorized as
Similarly, people are believed to be stronger and happier belonging to a certain group, the social categorization itself
when they display a youthful gait rather than when their can in turn influence the perception of a face (e.g., Corneille,
gait resembles that of an elderly person (Montepare & Huart, Becquart, & Bredart, 2004; Huart, Corneille, &
Zebrowitz-McArthur, 1988). In addition, even across age Becquart, 2005). When, for example, racially ambiguous
and racial groups, faces with childlike qualities such as faces are labeled as Black, participants judge skin tone
large eyes, a small nose, thin eyebrows, and a small chin to be darker than when the identical faces are labeled as
are believed to be submissive, dependent, naïve, and par- White (Levin & Banaji, 2006). Similarly, when the group
ticularly lovable (Berry & McArthur, 1986; Zebrowitz & membership of faces is made obvious through the addition
Montepare, 1992; Zebrowitz, Montepare, & Lee, 1993). of stereotypical African American rather than Hispanic
Intriguingly, these effects extend into adulthood, such that hairstyles, identical faces are perceived to be darker and
babyfaced individuals are less likely to be convicted of to contain deeper eyes and a wider mouth (MacLin &
intentional wrongdoing in both real and simulated trials than Malpass, 2003). Findings such as these demonstrate the
persons with mature facial features (Berry & Zebrowitz- complex influence that categorical forces can exert on
McArthur, 1988; Zebrowitz & McDonald, 1991). the products of the person perception process.
What is perhaps most striking about the stereotyping A second line of research has demonstrated that even
process is that it often occurs without perceivers’ intention after person categorization has successfully occurred,
or conscious awareness (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). stereotype activation and application do not necessarily
Given the observation that stereotyping is readily elicited occur. For example, although social categorization can
upon perceiving others, it has been argued that stereotype still be accomplished following the removal of a dominant
contents are automatically and unavoidably activated dur- category-specifying cue (e.g., hairstyle for sex judgments),
ing the person perception process (e.g., Allport, 1954; stereotype activation is significantly reduced under these
Bargh, 1999; Brewer, 1988; Chen & Bargh, 1994; Devine, conditions (Martin & Macrae, 2007). Additionally, manip-
1989; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). Accumulating evidence has ulating the facial maturity of male and female faces reveals
suggested, however, that the link between person percep- that only mature-faced males and babyfaced females are
tion and stereotype activation is not as rigid as was initially judged in accordance with sex stereotypes (e.g., women
assumed (Allport, 1954; Bargh, 1999). For instance, the are warmer and less powerful than men). For males and
magnitude of category-based person perception is sensitive females equivalent in facial maturity or mature-faced
to perceivers’ immediate processing goals. Passive regis- women and babyfaced men, stereotypic judgments are typ-
tration of a face or encoding a face with a perceptual pro- ically weakened or even reversed (Friedman & Zebrowitz,
cessing objective in place (e.g., dot detection) fails to elicit 1992). In a similar vein, in the context of simulated elec-
category activation (Livingston & Brewer, 2002; Macrae, tions, it has been shown that, in line with gender stereo-
Quinn, Mason, & Quadflieg, 2005; Quinn & Macrae, 2005; types, male candidates are preferred when the politically
Wheeler & Fiske, 2005). It seems to be the case that active relevant topic demands masculine characteristics (e.g.,
semantic encoding of a face is necessary to trigger person competitiveness), whereas females are preferred when
categorization (Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, Thorn, & the topic calls for feminine qualities (e.g., sociability).
Castelli, 1997). Under such processing conditions, however, Interestingly, for gender-atypical candidates (i.e., femi-
the extraction of category-specifying facial cues has been nine men and masculine women) these effects are reversed
shown to be a highly efficient, unintentional process that is (Lammers, Gordijn, & Otten, 2008). There is further evi-
resistant to stimulus degradation and impoverished viewing dence that stereotype activation and application are sensi-
conditions (Cloutier, Mason, & Macrae, 2005; Cloutier & tive to the extent to which individuals possess facial features
Macrae, 2007; Macrae et al., 2005; Santos & Young, 2005). that are deemed to be typical of the groups to which they
Extending this line of investigation, recent evidence has belong (Livingston & Brewer, 2002; Locke, Macrae, &
demonstrated that person categorization is a dynamic pro- Eaton, 2005; Maddox & Gray, 2002). Faces with more
cess in which multiple social categories can simultaneously Afrocentric features have been shown to elicit stronger
be activated until a stable (i.e., dominant) construal emerges activation and application of the African American stereo-
(Macrae & Martin, 2007; Martin & Macrae, 2007). For type than faces with less prototypical features (Blair et al.,
example, on their way to identifying targets correctly accord- 2002; Maddox, 2004). This form of feature-based stereo-
ing to sex, perceivers have been shown to entertain simulta- typing has also been observed in criminal-sentencing
neously both categorical solutions (i.e., male and female) for decisions, such that targets with Afrocentric features are
atypical targets (i.e., long-haired males and short-cropped punished most severely (Blair et al., 2004; Eberhardt,
females; see Freeman, Ambady, & Rule, 2008). Davies, Purdie-Vaughns, & Johnson, 2006).
438 Perceiving People

Not only does the appearance of the cue provider changes in their appearance (e.g., new haircut, aging, change
modulate stereotyping, so, too, can the circumstances or of clothing). Indeed, it has been demonstrated that for
context under which individuals are encountered (Barden, familiar faces, changes in viewpoint, illumination, and dis-
Maddux, Petty, & Brewer, 2004; Macrae et al., 1995; tance from the viewer cause little in the way of difficulty,
Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 2001). For example, viewing although such changes significantly diminish recognition
an Asian woman performing a typical female activity (e.g., performance for newly learned faces (Hancock, Bruce, &
applying cosmetics) or a typical Asian activity (e.g., eating Burton, 2000).
with chopsticks) was sufficient to prompt either the sex or Despite these observations of increased perceptual flu-
the race of the target respectively to dominate the categori- ency for familiar faces, it is not entirely clear how person
zation process (Macrae et al., 1995). Similarly, stereotype perception changes after targets become familiar. For
activation is amplified when an African American target is example, the extraction of featural and configural informa-
encountered in an expected rather than unexpected setting tion supports the processing of both familiar and novel faces
(e.g., street corner vs. outside a church; see Wittenbrink (Collishaw & Hole, 2000). The only reports of qualitative
et al., 2001). Capturing a similar effect, when clothing differences in face processing with regard to familiarity
assigns stereotype-congruent or incongruent roles toward comes from studies that have explored the relative salience
a Black individual (i.e., dressed as a criminal or a lawyer of different parts of the face (Ellis, Shepherd, & Davies,
in prison), the typical pattern of racial stereotyping can be 1979; Young, Hay, McWeeny, Flude, & Ellis, 1985). This
reversed (Barden et al., 2004). Finally, an encounter with work suggests that for the recognition of newly learned
an Asian assistant during an experiment has been shown to faces, external features such as hairstyle, and outer facial
trigger stereotype activation only when participants were contours are important, whereas the recognition of familiar
not required to perform a concurrent mental activity (i.e., faces is supported by internal features, notably the eyes,
digit rehearsal), even though the assistant’s race could be mouth, and nose (Haig, 1986; O’Donnell & Bruce, 2001;
reported after the experiment was completed (Gilbert & Sadr, Jarudi, & Sinha, 2003; Schyns et al., 2002). Further
Hixon, 1991). Collectively, these findings indicate that evidence suggests that familiar targets can also be recognized
although category-based perception of others is often done on the basis of their head, face, or body movements—that is,
unintentionally (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000), the acti- by the way they smile, move their arms, or walk (Cutting &
vation and application of stereotypes on the basis of these Kozlowski, 1977; Hill & Johnston, 2001; Hill & Pollick,
categories is nevertheless moderated by a variety of factors, 2000; Lander, Chuang, & Wickham, 2006; Stevenage,
including the appearance of the target, the goals of the per- Nixon, & Vince, 1999; Troje, Westhoff, & Lavrov, 2005).
ceiver, and the context in which a person is encountered. The relative importance of motion cues for person identifi-
cation increases as a function of a viewer’s visual experience
Person Identity with the target of interest (O’Toole, Roark, & Abdi, 2002).
Besides drawing inferences about strangers on the basis Under nonoptimal viewing conditions, motion cues can even
of their idiosyncratic appearance or social group member- enhance recognition performance (Knappmeyer, Thornton, &
ships, person perception often comprises the recognition Bülthoff, 2003; Lander & Chuang, 2005), although it is
of familiar individuals (e.g., friends, colleagues, family worth noting that movement-based identification appears to
members). Surprisingly, however, the process of person be viewpoint-dependent. To identify a friend by his or her
identification has not loomed large in theoretical accounts gait, for example, is easier from frontal or half-profile views
of person perception (Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, than from profile views (Jokisch, Daum, & Troje, 2006).
1990). Instead, most work on this topic has been under- Apart from integrating form and motion cues to establish
taken by cognitive psychologists and neuroscientists (e.g., visual familiarity, researchers assume that person recogni-
Bruce & Young, 1986; Haxby et al., 2000). Integral com- tion requires an additional sense of felt familiarity and the
ponents of person recognition include the perception of a spontaneous activation of biographical knowledge about
familiar appearance, an emotional response that is asso- the target in question (e.g., Lynden is a psychologist who
ciated with the person, and the spontaneous retrieval of comes from New Zealand). The assumption of felt famil-
person-specific semantic knowledge (Gobbini & Haxby, iarity derives from patient studies demonstrating that the
2007). Appearances become familiar as individuals meet co-occurrence of visual and emotional familiarity can be dis-
across a range of situations, under various viewing con- sociated. That is, patients suffering from prosopagnosia—
ditions, and with different portions of the face and body a disorder leaving them unable to recognize familiar
in view. Through repeated exposure, perceivers come to faces—display increased skin conductance to familiar but
recognize others under a varied set of viewing conditions, not unfamiliar faces, thereby demonstrating intact (albeit
over long distances, within crowds, and even after partial covert) emotional recognition of known others (De Haan,
Decoding Person Cues 439

Bauer, & Greve, 1992; Tranel & Damasio, 1985). In direct when the face of a stranger reminds one of a good friend,
contrast, patients suffering from Capgras’s syndrome do traits descriptive of the friend are readily attributed to the
not show any skin conductance differences in response unknown other (White & Shapiro, 1987). Similarly, when
to familiar or unfamiliar faces (Ellis, Young, Quayle, & exposed to photographs of a few short-faced professors
De Pauw, 1997). Interestingly, these individuals continue who were known to be fair and a few long-faced profes-
to recognize familiar others but deny their authenticity, sors who were considered to be unfair, college students
claiming that relatives or friends have been replaced by a subsequently judged an unknown short-faced professor
duplicates (Capgras & Reboul-Lachaux, 1923; Hirstein & to be more reasonable than an unknown colleague, even
Ramachandran, 1997). Driving this effect is a lack of felt though they were unaware of the relationship between
familiarity (e.g., emotional recognition) when known tar- facial length and fairness (Hill, Lewicki, Czyzewska, &
gets are encountered. This observed double dissociation Schuller, 1990). Physical resemblance to self also seems to
between visual and emotional familiarity suggests that influence the person perception process. Recent evidence
both processes contribute to person identification in an suggests that ratings of trust are elevated when strangers
important way. resemble self (DeBruine, Jones, Little, & Perrett, 2008).
Arguably the hallmark of person identification is the Finally, of course, sometimes targets are encountered on
retrieval of specific facts or details about known targets. An numerous occasions yet no specific target-related knowl-
extensive literature confirms that prior experience with a edge is acquired (i.e., the woman one sees on the bus every
target influences person perception such that, once detected, morning). What happens, then, when these familiar yet
familiar individuals trigger the spontaneous retrieval of unknown others are perceived? Perceivers seem to regard
biographical knowledge. For example, when asked to judge perceptually familiar people as more likeable compared with
the traits of target individuals from photographs, perceiv- unfamiliar targets (Moreland & Zajonc, 1982), an effect that
ers’ responses continue to be influenced by learning expe- is likely driven by the misattribution of positive affect
riences from the past (e.g., information associated with elicited by increased perceptual fluency (Winkielman &
the faces), even though the precise target-specific details Cacioppo, 2001). Recent work also suggests that percep-
have been forgotten (Uleman, Blader, & Todorov, 2005). tually familiar individuals tend to be construed in an even
Similarly, affective person-related knowledge gleaned from stronger stereotype-related manner than unfamiliar others
minimal information (i.e., reading that someone engages in (Smith et al., 2006), perhaps reflecting perceivers’ need to
a disgusting behavior) is spontaneously reactivated when make sense of frequently encountered targets.
the associated face is encountered on a subsequent occasion
(Todorov, Gobbini, Evans, & Haxby, 2007). In the Eye of the Beholder
Under certain circumstances, however, the spontaneous
retrieval of person-related knowledge seems to be impaired It has long been acknowledged that person perception is
even though visual and emotional familiarity with an indi- influenced by not only the attributes of the cue provider
vidual has been established. For example, the so-called but also the characteristics of the perceiver. That is, the
butcher-on-the-bus phenomenon refers to the observation eye of the beholder modulates the perception of attentional
that when one encounters a known target in an atypical set- focus, emotion, personality, attractiveness, and social group
ting, one may acknowledge that the target is familiar yet membership. For example, compared with males, females
be unable to bring any target-specific knowledge to mind display stronger reflexive gaze cueing effects (Frischen
because of the power of the unfamiliar context (Mandler, et al., 2007), especially when the gaze cues are provided by
1980; Yovel & Paller, 2004). A similar dissociation of personally familiar targets (Deaner, Shepherd, & Platt,
familiarity and person knowledge has been reported in 2007). In addition, for individuals high in trait anxiety,
some patients suffering from memory loss. Although these gaze cueing is facilitated when the cue provider displays
patients are able to judge the familiarity of others, such is a fearful rather than neutral expression (Mathews, Fox,
their impaired ability to access associated semantic knowl- Yiend, & Calder, 2003). These findings suggest that even
edge that person identification is severely compromised low-level processing operations—such as reflexive gaze-
(De Haan, Young, & Newcombe, 1991). triggered shifts in visual attention—are modulated by the
On occasion, unfamiliar targets are deemed to be familiar characteristics of the perceiver.
because they resemble friends, family, or celebrities in In a similar vein, despite the classic demonstration that
their appearance or mannerisms. A consequence of this individuals from all corners of the world can identify basic
similarity can be that these strangers are ascribed the emotions at levels better than chance (Ekman, 1999), there
same traits, attributes, and characteristics as the targets is also evidence that emotion recognition is enhanced when
they resemble (Chen & Andersen, 1999). For example, perceivers judge individuals from either their own culture
440 Perceiving People

(Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002) or cultures with which they cognition has extended to a consideration of how person
are familiar (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2003). The accuracy perception is implemented in the brain. Complementing
of emotion recognition is also profoundly influenced research exploring the mental processes that underlie per-
by the sex and age of perceivers. Women appear to be son processing, this work has sought to illuminate the neural
more accurate at recognizing emotions than men (Hall & operations that support the perception of conspecifics. To
Matsumoto, 2004), and as adults get older, they display elucidate the neural networks underlying person perception,
decrements in their ability to identify emotions from visual a wide range of neuroscientific methods have been used (see
cues (Ruffman, Henry, Livingstone, & Phillips, 2008). Table 12.1 for a short description of these methods).
Intriguingly, a perceiver ’s own emotional state also influ- Data obtained through the use of these approaches has
ences recognition performance such that congruent emo- provided significant insights into the cognitive, evaluative,
tions are identified most readily in others (Niedenthal, and neural processes that support core aspects of person
Halberstadt, Margolin, & Innes-Ker, 2000). perception. Before considering these insights, however,
Unsurprisingly, judging the attractiveness of others a brief neuroanatomical orientation may be useful. At a
is also dependent on the characteristics and status of global level, the brain is subdivided into four structures:
the perceiver. For example, a person’s preference for specific the frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes (see
personality traits can elevate the attractiveness of faces that Figure 12.1). Anterior refers the front of the brain, whereas
appear to display the desirable characteristics in question posterior refers to the rear. The terms superior and inferior
(see Little, Burt, & Perrett, 2006). When familiar others mean up and down, respectively. Structures situated close
are the targets of interest, attractiveness is moderated by to the midline of the brain (i.e., where the two hemispheres
how much one likes and respects the to-be-judged target meet) are termed medial structures; those located toward
(Kniffin & Wilson, 2004; Paunonen, 2006). In addition, the side of the brain are termed lateral structures. Finally,
hormonal changes in men and women can modulate rat- dorsal regions reside toward the top of the brain (i.e.,
ings of the attractiveness of potential partners (Jones et al., above the forebrain axis), and ventral regions are located
2008; Welling et al., 2008). toward the bottom (i.e., below the forebrain axis).
Finally, perceiver-related factors also modulate the con-
sequences of category-related processing. Most notably,
Table 12.1 Neuroscientific Methods at a Glance
perceivers show better memory for own-race than other
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): A noninvasive
race faces (e.g., Meissner & Brigham, 2001), an effect that
imaging technique measuring brain activity by recording oxygen levels
seems to arise because categorical face processing reduces in the brain’s blood vessels.
both the search for unique, individuating characteristics in Positron emission tomography (PET): An imaging technique used in
other-race faces and the motivation to process these stimuli conjunction with the injection of small amounts of radioactive tracers
(Bernstein, Young, & Hugenberg, 2007). In addition, the to study brain anatomy and brain activity.
link between person perception and stereotype activation is Magnetoencephalography (MEG): A noninvasive imaging technique
moderated by preexisting prejudiced beliefs (Devine, 1989). measuring the magnetic fields produced by electrical activity in the brain.

Compared with prejudiced persons, egalitarian individuals Electroencephalography (EEG): The noninvasive recording of
electrical brain activity produced by placing electrodes on the scalp.
show diminished stereotype activation from available
Event-related potential (ERP): The noninvasive recording of electrical
category-specifying cues (Johns, Cullum, Smith, & Freng,
brain activity following the presentation of a stimulus by placing
2008). Even factors such as a perceiver ’s hormonal status electrodes on the scalp.
can modulate the activation of gender stereotypes (Macrae, Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): A noninvasive
Alnwick, Milne, & Schloerscheidt, 2002). Taken together, method to stimulate cells in the brain via rapidly changing magnetic
this body of work confirms that the manner in which people fields. Depending on the kind of stimulation, an increased or decreased
excitability of cells can be evoked.
construe others is shaped not only by the available visual
(e.g., facial, bodily) cues but also by a host of perceiver- Intracranial cell recordings: Recordings of the electrical activity of cells
via electrodes implanted in the brain.
related factors that moderate the course and products of the
Brain lesion studies: Assume that the function of an area of the brain
person perception process (see Zebrowitz, 2006). can be inferred from the behaviors that people can no longer perform
after their brain was damaged in a certain region (i.e., after they had a
circumscribed brain lesion).
PERSON PERCEPTION IN THE BRAIN
The Neuroanatomy of Person Perception
Given the insight that perceptual operations exert a signifi-
cant influence on the detection, classification, evaluation, A primary objective of neuropsychological investiga-
and identification of conspecifics, recent work in social tions has been to map cognitive functions onto precise
Person Perception in the Brain 441

Dorsal

Dorsal
Stream
Parietal Thalamus
Lobe
(LGN)
Frontal
Posterior Lobe Anterior
Occipital
Lobe Temporal Striate
Lobe Cortex
Ventral
Stream
Ventral
Figure 12.2 Visual pathways in the human brain.
Figure 12.1 The human brain.

neuroanatomical regions. Unsurprisingly, the neuroanatomy objects and friends from strangers and to trigger applicable
of person perception is tied closely to the structures that social category memberships. Complicating matters some-
support basic visual processing. Visual processing com- what, facial and bodily features and their relationships
mences when light-sensitive photoreceptor cells in the eye change both slowly (i.e., over time through aging) and rap-
are stimulated and the resultant signal is transmitted to the idly (i.e., changes in emotional expressions or actions), and
brain along the optic nerve. The majority of fibers in the their perception depends crucially on the conditions under
optic nerve terminate in the lateral geniculate nucleus which they are encountered (e.g., distance from observer,
(LGN) from where the visual information is then projected lighting conditions). Despite these complicating factors, an
to the primary visual cortex (also called the striate cortex) extensive literature has documented how a circumscribed
in the occipital lobe of the brain. Accordingly, this path- network of regions responds following the perception of
way is termed the primary geniculostriate visual pathway. faces and bodies.
From the primary visual cortex, information is distributed
through visual-association cortices (also called extrastri- The Neural Correlates of Face Perception
ate cortices) onto two visual pathways, often referred to as That there may be a neural network dedicated to face pro-
the dorsal and ventral visual streams (Goodale & Milner, cessing was first suggested by observations of prosopag-
1992). Both streams analyze the incoming visual signal but nostic patients. Following damage to the temporal cortex,
with regard to divergent properties. Whereas the ventral although these patients were adept at identifying objects
processing stream plays a dominant role in the identifica- they were demonstrably impaired at recognizing familiar
tion and recognition of stimuli and extends anatomically faces (Hecaen & Angelergues, 1962). Further evidence
toward the inferior temporal lobe; the dorsal processing for a dedicated face-processing system was garnered from
stream is implicated in the localization of stimuli and coor- single-cell recording studies in nonhuman primates, which
dination of visually guided actions and extends thoughout revealed that cells in the superior temporal sulcus (STS)
the posterior parietal cortex. Importantly, the two path- respond selectively to faces (Perrett, Rolls, & Caan, 1982).
ways are not completely independent, thereby ensuring Since these early studies, accumulating evidence suggests
that information about both the identity and location of an that a distributed neural system of multiple, bilateral brain
object (including another person) can be extracted from regions underlies face processing. According to Haxby and
the available visual signal (Schwarzlose, Swisher, Dang, & colleagues (2000; Gobbini & Haxby, 2007), for example,
Kanwisher, 2008; see Figure 12.2). brain regions involved in face perception can be divided
Mirroring the detection of cheeseburgers, tables, and into two distinct neural subsystems: a core system of
penguins, the perception of social agents recruits neural brain regions that subserve the visual analysis of faces
networks dedicated to the processing of visual information. and an extended system of brain regions that support the
It is worth noting, however, that person perception poses construal of person knowledge (see Figure 12.3). On the
some interesting challenges for the visual system. Human basis of the coordinated collaboration of the regions within
faces and bodies are complex three-dimensional stimuli these subsystems, diverse aspects of face perception can be
comprising multiple features that vary in shape and sur- accomplished.
face characteristics (i.e., texture, spatial frequency, lumi- Cortical regions comprising the core system of face percep-
nance, and color distribution). The particular configuration tion include the occipital face area (OFA) and the fusiform face
of these features can be used to distinguish people from area (FFA), both located in the ventral extrastriate cortex,
442 Perceiving People

IPS

Striate & pSTS


Extrastriate
Cortices OFA AMY OFC
FFA
aTL

CORE NEURAL SYSTEM: EXTENDED NEURAL SYSTEM:


OFA: featural analysis of faces IPS: spatially directed attention Figure 12.3 Brain regions implementing face
perception. AMY  amygdala; aTL  anterior
FFA: configural analysis of faces aTL: person knowledge temporal lobe; FFA  fusiform face area; IPS 
pSTS: analysis of facial movements OFC: facial attractiveness intraparietal sulcus; OFA  occipital face area;
AMY: person evaluation OFC  orbitofrontal cortex; pSTS  posterior
superior temporal sulcus.

as well as the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS). halves of a face only when the bottom half of the face
Although all three of these regions analyze the visual is also identical), thereby indicating that FFA activity is
appearance of faces, they do so differentially (Haxby et al., associated with the integration of face parts into holistic–
2000). In particular, whereas the OFA and FFA contribute configural wholes (Kanwisher & Yovel, 2006). That the
mainly to the encoding of facial features and their struc- FFA is not only involved in the configural processing of
tural configuration, the pSTS is involved in the percep- faces was demonstrated in a study in which participants
tion of changeable aspects of facial appearance (i.e., facial were required to complete successive discrimination tasks
movements) coming from the head, eyes, and lips. How on face pairs that differed in either their individual com-
do we know, however, that the OFA, FFA, and pSTS have ponents (i.e., nose, mouth, eyes) or the configuration of
different functional contributions toward face processing these parts (i.e., the spacing among face parts; Yovel &
when they all reliably activate in neuroimaging studies when Kanwisher, 2004). In both conditions, a similar FFA response
participants view faces compared with other common was observed indicating that the FFA is also involved in the
objects or nonsense (control) stimuli (Gauthier et al., representation of face parts. FFA activity can be reduced,
2000; Haxby et al., 1999; Ishai, Schmidt, & Boesiger, however, when participants are asked to pay particular
2005; Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1997; Yovel & attention to a changeable aspect of the face, notably gaze
Kanwisher, 2005)? direction (Hoffman & Haxby, 2000). These findings sug-
With regard to the OFA, it has been noted that acti- gest that the FFA does not play a central role in all aspects
vation in this region is similarly strong to upright and of face perception but rather in the perception of invariant
inverted faces (Haxby et al., 1999; Yovel & Kanwisher, aspects of facial appearance. In line with this assumption,
2005) suggesting that disruptions to configural processing the FFA has been found to play a major role in encoding
do not interfere with the processing of facial information structural information that contributes to the analysis of
that is undertaken in this area. Given that the perceptual sex and race-related facial properties (Cloutier, Turk, &
analysis of facial features can still be accomplished for Macrae, 2007; Golby, Gabrieli, Chiao, & Eberhardt, 2001;
inverted faces and that temporally disrupting activity in the Kim et al., 2006; Ng, Ciaramitaro, Anstis, Boynton, &
OFA using repetitive transmagnetic stimulation (rTMS) Fine, 2006).
has been demonstrated to interfere with the accurate per- In contrast, paying particular attention toward eye gaze
ception of face parts but not their configuration (Pitcher, in static faces—or actually perceiving movements of the
Walsh, Yovel, & Duchaine, 2007), it has been concluded eyes or the mouth in dynamic stimuli—has consistently
that activity in the OFA mainly reflects the visual analysis been found to increase activation in the pSTS (Campbell
of facial features. et al., 2001; Cloutier et al., 2007; Hoffman & Haxby,
In contrast, the FFA has been reported to show both face- 2000; Puce, Allison, Bentin, Gore, & McCarthy, 1998;
inversion (i.e., an elevated blood oxygen level–dependent Schuermann et al., 2005). As a convergence zone for the
response for upright than inverted faces) and face com- dorsal and ventral visual streams, the pSTS is thought to
posite effects (i.e., an adaptation across two identical top be well equipped to integrate form and motion information
Person Perception in the Brain 443

emanating from a person’s face to encode changeable Richardson, Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2004). Interestingly,
aspects of person knowledge. increased amygdala responsivity toward emotional faces
Thus, through the combined efforts of the OFA, FFA, can persist under conditions of reduced attention or aware-
and pSTS the representation of specific individual faces ness, such as when faces are shown at ignored locations
can be accomplished (Giese & Poggio, 2003; Rossion, (A. K. Anderson, Christoff, Panitz, De Rosa, & Gabrieli,
2008). The visual analysis of faces undertaken in this core 2003), when they are masked (Whalen et al., 1998),
neural network provides information that can be used to or when they are presented in the blind field of patients
generate broader person-related inferences, such as judg- with damage to the primary visual cortex (Pegna, Khateb,
ing or interpreting the attractiveness, identity, personality, Lazeyras, & Seghier, 2005). These findings have raised the
emotional status, and gaze direction of other social agents. question of how visual information can reach the amyg-
To compute these inferences, additional brain regions dala despite attenuated inputs or even destruction of the
are recruited (i.e., extended system) that work in con- primary visual cortical pathway. It is now believed that
cert with the core face-processing system (Haxby et al., the amygdala can receive “quick and dirty” visual input
2000). Interestingly, although researchers agree as to from retinal projections through the superior colliculus and
which regions comprise the core face-processing system, the pulvinar nucleus (LeDoux, 1996; Morris, Öhman, &
less conformity has been achieved with regard to the areas Dolan, 1999; Williams, Palmer, Liddell, Song, & Gordon,
that constitute the extended system. To date, the follow- 2006). Converging evidence for this viewpoint comes
ing regions have been deemed to comprise part of this from studies demonstrating that rapid emotional face pro-
network. cessing can be driven by coarse visual input based on low
An important region contributing to the processing spatial frequency information contained within faces. For
of eye gaze is the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), located on example, when fearful faces are presented either intact or
the lateral surface of the parietal lobe. In tandem with the filtered such that they contain only low spatial frequency
pSTS, the IPS is involved in the processing of gaze cues information, amygdala activity is observed for both types
that signal the direction of social attention. Neuroimaging of stimuli. In contrast, only intact faces trigger activation of
data show that IPS activity is increased when faces dis- FFA. What this implies is that emotional processing in the
play averted gaze (Hoffman & Haxby, 2000; Pelphrey, amygdala is independent of the face-processing opera-
Singerman, Allison, & McCarthy, 2003; Wicker, Michel, tions that occur in extrastriate visual cortex (Vuilleumier &
Henaff, & Decety, 1998), suggesting that this region sup- Pourtois, 2007). Collectively these studies suggest that
ports inferences about the spatial location of other people’s although emotional face processing typically relies on the
attention. interplay of regions in the core face-processing system and
Observing another person’s face also provides crucial the amygdala, it can nevertheless be preserved in a rudi-
information about what the person is feeling. Following mentary way even when the complex structural encoding
discrete patterns of brain damage, two intriguing types of of faces is compromised.
patients have been identified. Whereas some individuals It is worth noting, however, that the vast majority of
display a deficiency in face recognition yet a spared capac- investigations of emotional face processing have explored
ity to read facial expressions of emotion, others exhibit the perceptions of fearful expressions. This may be somewhat
opposite effects (e.g., impaired emotional processing and problematic because recent work suggests that the neural
intact face recognition; see Posamentier & Abdi, 2003). network that implements fear perception does not neces-
Informed by such double dissociations, it has been sug- sarily generalize to other emotions (Morris et al., 1996).
gested that partly separable neural networks subserve the In particular, the critical role of the amygdala may be
detection and analysis of facial affect and the structural restricted to facial expressions that signal danger or fear.
encoding of facial features. Indeed, neuroimaging stud- Patients with bilateral amygdala damage have been found
ies have revealed that, compared with neutral faces, emo- to be selectively impaired at recognizing fearful expres-
tional faces elicit not only an increased response in core sions (Adolphs et al., 2005). In contrast, the perception of
regions of the face-processing system but also a concomi- disgust appears to be selectively related to activation in the
tant increase in activity in the amygdala (Vuilleumier & insula (Calder, Keane, Manes, Antoun, & Young, 2000;
Pourtois, 2007). M. L. Phillips et al., 1997). Further research is therefore
The amygdala is an almond-shaped mass of nuclei required to elucidate the functional contribution of distinct,
located deep within the medial temporal lobes. If damaged, emotion-specific activations with the implementation of
the normal enhancement of FFA activation toward emo- face processing more generally.
tional faces disappears, strongly suggesting a direct modu- Further underscoring the pivotal status of the amyg-
latory influence of the amygdala on the FFA (Vuilleumier, dala to social–cognitive functioning, recent research has
444 Perceiving People

documented that this structure also plays a pivotal role in (Macrae et al., 2005; Quinn & Macrae, 2005), effects that
core aspects of trait ascription. Faces that are considered have been mirrored by the patterning of activity in the
to be particularly untrustworthy elicit an elevated amyg- amygdala. Whereas amygdala activity tracks with racial
dala response (Engell et al., 2007; Todorov et al., 2008; status (i.e., exhibits increased responses for faces that do
Winston, Strange, O’Doherty, & Dolan, 2002) and patients not match the perceiver ’s own race) when participants are
with damage to this structure consider trustworthy faces (as making socially meaningful judgments of the targets such
rated by healthy individuals) to look quite untrustworthy as judging their age or sex (e.g., Hart et al., 2000; Ronquillo
(Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio, 1998). Interestingly, prosop- et al., 2007; Wheeler & Fiske, 2005), activity in this region
agnostic patients who are unable to perceive the identity is insensitive to target race when faces are processed at a
of faces report normal judgments of trust (Todorov & superficial visual level (i.e., during a dot detection task or
Duchaine, 2008). These data suggest that mirroring the a perceptual matching task; Cunningham et al., 2004; Phelps
separable networks for the analysis of facial affect and et al., 2000; Wheeler & Fiske, 2005). Intriguingly, however,
the structural encoding of facial features, the extraction the more individuals associate their racial ingroup with
of identity-related information and trait knowledge from positivity and the outgroup with negativity as measured by
facial cues may also be dissociable. Additionally, these the Implicit Association Test (IAT), the stronger amygdala
data, taken together with work on the processing of emo- activity differentiates between different race faces even
tional faces, imply that activity in the amygdala seems to under mere perceptual processing conditions (Cunningham
reflect higher vigilance toward potentially challenging et al., 2004; Phelps et al., 2000). These findings suggest
(i.e., fearful or untrustworthy) interaction partners rather that if perceivers consider race to be a particularly impor-
than encoding specific emotional states or personality tant dimension for construing others, amygdala activity is
traits in others. insensitive to the prevailing encoding context.
Beyond trait judgments, explorations of facial attrac- Despite recent advances in understanding the neural
tiveness have revealed that the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) underpinnings of social categorization, the exact func-
is a reliable contributor to important aspects of person per- tional contribution of the amygdala to race-based person
ception. Activity in this area tracks with the relative facial perception remains unclear. For example, is this structure
attractiveness of faces, even when participants’ task is responding to lower-level perceptual differences between
simply to classify the stimuli according to sex (O’Doherty faces or differences in the features that signal the relative
et al., 2003; Winston, O’Doherty, Kilner, Perrett, & Dolan, trustworthiness of individuals who differ from self in terms
2007). Interestingly, however, whereas for heterosexual of race? What appears to be apparent is that the observed
women and homosexual men attractive male faces elicit amygdala activation is unlikely to reflect processes of ste-
stronger OFC activation than attractive female faces, in reotype activation or application given that none of the
heterosexual men and homosexual women, attractive studies required participants to draw stereotypic infer-
female faces evoke stronger OFC activation than attractive ences while perceiving the faces nor were they able to
male faces. These findings suggest that the OFC responds demonstrate that a relationship exists between participants’
not only to the attractiveness of a face per se but particu- explicit stereotypic beliefs (as tapped into by the Modern
larly to attractive faces that are sexually relevant for a par- Racism Scale) and the magnitude of amygdala response to
ticular perceiver (Ishai, 2007; Kranz & Ishai, 2006). These outgroup members (Cunningham et al., 2004; Phelps et al.,
findings support previous data showing that the OFC plays 2000; Wheeler & Fiske, 2005). Furthermore, although it
a crucial role in the evaluation of sensory signals and their is well established that faces with more Afrocentric fea-
reward value to guide a perceiver ’s responses to his or her tures elicit stronger activation of the African American
environment (for a review, see Rolls, 2000). stereotype than faces with less prototypical features (Blair
Preliminary research has also explored the neural struc- et al., 2002), a recent neuroimaging study failed to find dif-
tures supporting the extraction of categorical information ferences in amygdala activation toward light-skinned and
from facial cues. Whereas behavioral researchers have dark-skinned Black faces (Ronquillo et al., 2007).
made noticeable progress at elucidating how each of the Alternatively, it has been argued that the amygdala may
three aspects of the stereotyping process (social categoriza- be involved in rapid group-based automatic affective evalu-
tion, stereotype activation, stereotype application) unfold, ations (Amodio, 2008). Patient data suggest, however, that
neuroimaging studies have focused almost exclusively on automatic category-related evaluative associations remain
exploring the first step, the social categorization of others. normal even when recruitment of the amygdala is impos-
Interestingly, behavioral findings suggesting that passively sible because of brain damage (Phelps, Cannistraci, &
viewing a face or making a superficial perceptual judg- Cunningham, 2003). In sum, the precise contribution of
ment is insufficient to reliably trigger social categorization the amygdala to social categorization requires further
Person Perception in the Brain 445

The Neural Correlates of Body Perception


empirical attention. Underscoring this recommendation is
the observation that for age rather than race judgments, the Like the human face, the human body and its associated
amygdala seems to display a larger response to faces of parts are a rich source of socially relevant information
ingroup (i.e., faces of the perceiver ’s own age group) than about others. Although the neural network supporting body
outgroup members (i.e., faces that are either significantly perception has received less attention than the correspond-
younger or older than the perceiver; Wright et al., 2008). ing face-processing system, evidence has recently emerged
This observation challenges the generalized assumption revealing that cortical regions specialized for visual body
that amygdala activity merely signals a higher vigilance perception can be dissociated from areas involved in face
toward outgroup members. or object processing. Cells responding selectively to the
Finally, beyond the amygdala, one of the most impor- shapes of monkey and human bodies or body parts were first
tant regions in the extended face-processing system is the demonstrated by single-cell recording studies targeting the
anterior temporal lobe (aTL). In contrast to the conflict- temporal cortex of monkeys (Desimone, Albright, Gross, &
ing findings that have been reported in the OFA and FFA Bruce, 1984; Wachsmuth, Oram, & Perrett, 1994). Since
(Kanwisher & Yovel, 2006), converging evidence suggests the collection of additional data on the neural correlates
that the aTL plays a pivotal role in the detection of facial of body perception in humans, it has become apparent
identity (see Gobbini & Haxby, 2007). Neuroimaging that, like face processing, a core and an extended neural
investigations have repeatedly demonstrated that the per- system subserving body perception can be distinguished
ception of familiar faces, either because they are personal (see Figure 12.4). The core neural system, which is dedi-
acquaintances or famous celebrities, is associated with cated to the visual analysis of the human body and its
elevated activity in the aTL (Kriegeskorte, Formisano, parts, receives inputs from early visual areas that deal with
Sorger, & Goebel, 2007; Leveroni et al., 2000; Sugiura form and motion processing. An extended neural network
et al., 2001). Most tellingly, following damage to this of regions then uses the outputs of the core system to draw
structure, the ability to identify familiar faces deteriorates body-based inferences (Giese & Poggio, 2003; Lange &
dramatically (Evans, Heggs, Antoun, & Hedges, 1995; Lappe, 2006; Peelen & Downing, 2007).
Gainotti, Barbier, & Marra, 2003). It has been speculated The core neural system of body perception comprises
that the crucial contribution of the aTL toward person per- the extrastriate body area (EBA), the fusiform body area
ception is based on its representations of specific semantic (FBA), and the pSTS. The EBA is located bilaterally in the
and biographical knowledge about others that is accessed posterior inferior temporal sulcus in close proximity but
when a familiar face is encountered (Gobbini & Haxby, slightly superior to the OFA (Peelen & Downing, 2005a;
2007; Haxby et al., 2000). Additionally, a weaker amyg- Spiridon, Fischl, & Kanwisher, 2006). Activity in this area
dala response has been noted to accompany the percep- is strongly and selectively enhanced to a variety of static
tion of familiar than unfamiliar faces. It has been argued images of headless human bodies and body parts com-
that this lack of activation reflects a lower level of vigi- pared with images of faces and objects or their constituent
lance toward familiar others than strangers (Gobbini & elements (Downing, Chan, Peelen, Dodds, & Kanwisher,
Haxby, 2007). 2006; Downing, Wiggett, & Peelen, 2007; Peelen & Downing,

Parietal
Cortex

PMC
Striate & pSTS
Extrastriate
Cortices EBA AMY
FBA

CORE NEURAL SYSTEM: EXTENDED NEURAL SYSTEM:


Figure 12.4 Brain regions implementing body
EBA: featural analysis of bodies AMY: person evaluation
perception. AMY  amygdala; EBA  extrastriate
FBA: configural analysis of bodies PMC & Parietal Cortex: action body area; FBA  fusiform body area; PMC  pre-
interpretation & personality motor cortex; pSTS  posterior superior temporal
pSTS: analysis of body movements inference
sulcus.
446 Perceiving People

2005a; Pourtois, Peelen, Spinelli, Seeck, & Vuilleumier, When stimulated with body images of varying completeness
2007). Importantly, the increased response in the EBA (i.e., fingers, hands, arms, torsos, whole bodies), the EBA
generalizes to line drawings, stick figures, silhouettes, and responds more specifically toward body parts than the
PL animations of bodies, indicating that EBA activity is FBA, whereas the FBA displays a relative bias for more
elicited irrespective of the specific visual features of body- complete images of the body (Taylor, Wiggett, & Downing,
related images (Downing, Jiang, Shuman, & Kanwisher, 2007). On the basis of these findings, it has been suggested
2001; Peelen, Wiggett, & Downing, 2006). Furthermore, that the EBA deals with the encoding and representation of
when a temporal inactivation of the EBA is induced body parts, whereas the FBA processes the configuration
by rTMS, body and body-part recognition is impaired of body parts into wholes, hence constructs a holistic rep-
(Urgesi, Berlucchi, & Aglioti, 2004; Urgesi, Calvo-Merino, resentation of the body (Peelen & Downing, 2007).
Haggard, & Aglioti, 2007). Although the EBA and FBA play a prominent role
Although the EBA is thought to be involved in the in the encoding of static bodies and their associated parts,
visual analysis of human bodies and their parts, the exact the pSTS is implicated in the processing of bodily related
functional significance of this region remains open to motion (Blake & Shiffrar, 2007; Giese & Poggio, 2003;
debate. On the one hand, EBA activation has been found Michels, Lappe, & Vaina, 2005; Puce & Perrett, 2003). When
to adapt more strongly to succeeding images depicting pSTS activity is disrupted, either temporarily following
relatively similar than dissimilar body postures, thereby rTMS or more permanently because of a stroke, a person’s
suggesting that it may play an important role in encoding ability to perceive bodily motion is significantly impaired
and visually analyzing static “snapshots” of bodies and (Grossman, Batelli, & Pascual-Leone, 2005; Saygin, 2007).
their parts (Downing, Peelen, Wiggett, & Tew, 2006). In contrast to activity in the EBA and FBA, which increases
On the other hand, however, the right EBA in particular toward bodies relative to objects regardless of whether they
has also been associated with the encoding of the ori- are stationary or not, activity in the pSTS increases more to
entation of bodies and their parts such that activity is the former than latter stimuli only when they are in motion
increased when bodily information is presented in an (Grossman & Blake, 2002; Saxe, Xiao, Kovacs, Perrett, &
allocentric (i.e., consistent with looking at someone Kanwisher, 2004). Robust and selective pSTS activation
else’s body) compared with an egocentric perspective has also been observed toward complex human motion in
(i.e., consistent with looking at one’s own body; Chan, movie sequences (Beauchamp, Lee, Haxby, & Martin, 2002;
Peelen, & Downing, 2004; Saxe, Jamal, & Powell, Hasson, Nir, Levy, Fuhrmann, & Malach, 2004; Pelphrey,
2006). Finally, it is still a matter of debate whether the Mitchell, Mckeown, Goldstein, Allison, & McCarthy,
EBA is involved in the body-based identification of indi- 2003), toward PL-animated bodily movements (Grèzes
viduals as originally suggested (Downing et al., 2001; et al., 2001; Grossman et al., 2000; Grossman, Blake, &
Urgesi, Candidi, Ionta, & Aglioti, 2007, but see Kable & Kim, 2004; Pyles, Garcia, Hoffman, & Grossman, 2007),
Chatterjee, 2006). Perceiving one’s own body parts and and toward bodily silhouettes depicted with implied move-
those of familiar others has been shown to increase ment (Peuskens, Vanrie, Verfaillie, & Orban, 2005).
activity not in the EBA but rather in the aTL (Chan et al., Whether pSTS activation toward human bodily move-
2004). This finding corroborates work on face processing ments can be reliably distinguished from activation toward
demonstrating that the aTL represents specific semantic facial movements is still a matter of debate. During the
and biographical knowledge about others. Given the rela- passive viewing of face versus body movements, over-
tive paucity of work on this topic, however, additional lapping but discrete foci of activation in the pSTS have
data are required to increase understanding of whether been shown (Pelphrey, Morris, Michelich, Allison, &
and how the EBA and the aTL contribute to the percep- McCarthy, 2005; Wheaton, Thompson, Syngeniotis,
tion of identity-specific bodily cues. Abbott, & Puce, 2004). In contrast, when subjects have
A second body-selective area that is anatomically dis- been asked to search for the same type of motion pattern
tinct from the EBA has been located in the posterior fusi- in bodily and facial movements, no significant differ-
form gyrus adjacent to, and overlapping with, the FFA ence in the loci of activation in pSTS has been reported
(Peelen et al., 2006). Similar to the EBA, the FBA responds (Thompson, Hardee, Panayiotou, Crewther, & Puce,
selectively to whole headless bodies and body parts, as 2007). Thus, under which processing conditions pSTS
well as schematic, stick figure, and PL-animated depic- activity distinguishes between facial and bodily move-
tions of the body (Peelen & Downing, 2005b; Peelen et al., ments is an issue that requires clarification.
2006; Schwarzlose, Baker, & Kanwisher, 2005). Despite Similar to the extended face-processing network,
their similar response properties during body perception, additional brain regions support the perception of bodies
activity in EBA and FBA can nevertheless be dissociated. (Decety & Grèzes, 1999; de Gelder, 2006; Rizzolatti,
Person Perception in the Brain 447

Fogassi, & Gallese, 2001). For example, perceiving whole One strategy through which people can understand and
bodies or body parts conveying various emotional states interpret the behavior of others is to use their own “motor
(such as anger, happiness, and fear) evokes an elevated knowledge” as a guiding framework (i.e., motor simula-
response in the core body perception system (i.e., EBA, tion). That is, incoming bodily related visual information
FBA and pSTS—see Gallagher & Frith, 2004; Grèzes, from others can be mapped onto one’s own motor repre-
Pichon, & de Gelder, 2007; Hadjikhani & de Gelder, sentation of the action in question (Decety & Grèzes, 1999;
2003; Pichon, de Gelder, & Grèzes, 2007). Importantly, Rizzolatti et al., 2001). As it turns out, the PMC and parietal
modulation of this system is accompanied by amygdala cortex are involved in both the perception and execution
activity in response to emotionally expressive body pos- of action (Grèzes, Armony, Rowe, & Passingham, 2003;
tures or movements (de Gelder, Snyder, Greve, Gerard, & Grèzes & Decety, 2001; Iacoboni et al., 1999; Molnar-
Hadjikhani, 2004; Gallagher & Frith, 2004; Hadjikhani & de Szakacs, Iacoboni, Koski, & Mazziotta, 2005; Rizzolatti &
Gelder, 2003; Peelen, Atkinson, Andersson, & Vuilleumier, Craighero, 2004). Providing further evidence that action
2007; Pichon et al., 2007). More specifically, the magni- perception is subserved by motor simulation is the obser-
tude of emotional modulation in the core system appears to vation that activity in the critical frontoparietal network is
be directly related to a concomitant response in the amyg- elevated when perceivers observe actions that can be easily
dala, such that individuals displaying a stronger amygdala mapped onto their own motor representations. Specifically,
response toward emotional bodies also show stronger individuals show stronger activity in premotor and parietal
modulation in the core system subserving body perception cortices during the perception of actions they regularly per-
(Peelen et al., 2007). Surprisingly, however, the amygdala form themselves compared with actions they rarely enact
does not appear to be strictly necessary for the normal rec- (Calvo-Merino, Glaser, Grèzes, Passingham, & Haggard,
ognition of bodily expressions. In contrast to data obtained 2005; Calvo-Merino, Grèzes, Glaser, Passingham, &
for emotional face processing, bilateral amygdala lesions Haggard, 2006).
do not seem to interfere with the successful identification of Although humans are highly adept at using bodily cues
emotions from expressive bodily cues (Adolphs & Tranel, to draw rapid inferences about the actions of conspecif-
2003; Atkinson, Heberlein, & Adolphs, 2007). This finding, ics, they can also use this information to infer the person-
however, does not address the issue of whether the amyg- ality of others. Such trait judgments have received less
dala is implicated in the automatic appraisal of emotionally experimental attention than action-related inferences; nev-
expressive bodily cues. It has recently been demonstrated ertheless, there is preliminary evidence to suggest that per-
that compared with nonemotional stimuli, emotional body sonality judgments from body cues (i.e., whether a person
images presented in the blind field of a patient with dam- is friendly or outgoing) are also associated with activity
age to the striate cortex activate the pSTS, even though the in the frontoparietal network. Convergent evidence from
patient is unaware of the presented body posture (de Gelder & lesion and neuroimaging studies suggests that the frontal
Hadjikhani, 2006). Similar to work on facial expressions, operculum, located in the left PMC, and the left inferior
these findings suggest that emotional body postures may parietal cortex contribute to personality judgments from
also be processed through subcortical visual pathways that body cues (Heberlein et al., 2004; Heberlein & Saxe,
include the amygdala (de Gelder, 2006). 2005).
In contrast to the perception of emotions from bodily
cues, perceiving others in action (i.e., during the exhibition The Temporal Character of Person Perception
of intentional motor behavior) has been tightly linked to
activation in the premotor and parietal cortices. Activity in Aside from exploring the neuroanatomy of person per-
each of these areas increases selectively during action per- ception (i.e., the “where” question), scientific attention
ception compared with the perception of static displays of has also targeted the temporal aspects of this process (i.e.,
bodies or their parts (Buccino et al., 2001; Chong, Williams, the “when” question). Recordings of event-related elec-
Cunnington, & Mattingley, 2008; Saygin, Wilson, Hagler, trical potentials (ERPs) or event-related magnetic fields
Bates, & Sereno, 2004; Wheaton et al., 2004). The func- (ERMFs) are powerful and widely used methods to inves-
tional significance of this frontoparietal network seems to tigate the time course of visual categorization in the human
lie in retrieving the meaning of actions performed by other brain. Numerous studies using these techniques have
social agents. Studies applying rTMS have demonstrated revealed that pictures of faces or bodies elicit distinct elec-
that the temporal deactivation of the frontoparietal net- trical and magnetic activity that distinguishes them from
work significantly impairs people’s ability to discriminate other classes of visual object (de Gelder, 2006; Rossion &
and understand actions (Pobric & Hamilton, 2006; Urgesi, Jacques, 2008). Importantly, these measures can address
Candidi, et al., 2007). the question how early in the visual processing stream
448 Perceiving People

faces and bodies can be discriminated from other types of Lending support to this viewpoint, previous research has
stimuli. suggested that the N170 is particularly sensitive to infor-
mation coming from the region of the eyes. For example,
The Time Course of Face Perception when presented in isolation, static eyes elicit a larger N170
Recordings of ERPs and ERMFs show enhanced responsiv- than static whole faces (Bentin et al., 1996; Puce, Smith, &
ity to faces relative to a variety of other objects with peaks Allison, 2000). In addition, when schematic faces are pre-
at approximately 100 ms (Herrmann, Ehlis, Muehlberger, & sented with a pair of small objects substituted for the eyes,
Fallgatter, 2005; Liu, Harris, & Kanwisher, 2002; Pegna, no N170 effect is observed (Bentin, Golland, Flevaris,
Khateb, Michel, & Landis, 2004), 170 ms (Bentin, Allison, Robertson, & Moscovitch, 2006). It has therefore been
Puce, Perez, & McCarthy, 1996; Eimer & McCarthy, 1999; proposed that face-selective and eye-selective neurons con-
Itier & Taylor, 2004a), and 250 ms (Bentin & Deouell, tribute to the emergence of the N170, with eye-selective
2000; Schweinberger, Huddy, & Burton, 2004) after stimu- neurons playing a prominent role in specific task contexts,
lus onset. In ERP studies, the positive potential that peaks such as when configural processing is disrupted and fea-
at 100 ms (the “P100”) after the onset of a face is typi- tural strategies need to be adopted (Itier et al., 2007) or
cally observed at electrodes near the medial occipital pole when eyes provide valuable information about objects in
(Eimer, 2000) and is thought to be generated by activity in the environment (Puce et al., 2000).
visual areas located in the striate and extrastriate cortices Whether the N170 is modulated by the expressed emo-
(Di Russo, Martinez, Sereno, Pitzalis, & Hillyard, 2002). tionality of a face remains open to debate. Whereas some
The P100 is known to be particularly sensitive to low-level studies have reported an emotion-based modulation of the
properties of visual stimuli, such as luminance, contrast, N170 (Batty & Taylor, 2003; Blau, Maurer, Tottenham, &
color, or spatial frequency. Increased P100 amplitude for McCandliss, 2007; Sprengelmeyer & Jentzsch, 2006),
faces compared with objects has not been consistently others have failed to reveal such an effect (Ashley,
observed and is notably absent in studies that carefully Vuilleumier, & Swick, 2004; Eimer & Holmes, 2002;
control the low-level visual properties of the stimuli. As Eimer, Holmes, & McGlone, 2003). What has been reli-
such, the P100 is thought to be sensitive to low-level cues ably shown, however, is that a frontocentral positivity
that may be associated with, but are by no means exclusive as early as 120 ms after stimulus onset and a later more
to, faces (Rossion & Jacques, 2008). broadly distributed positivity beyond 250 ms are modu-
Compared with the P100, most ERP studies find a much lated by emotional facial expressions (Eimer & Holmes,
more robust face-related effect in the negative potential 2002; Holmes, Vuilleumier, & Eimer, 2003; Vuilleumier &
that peaks at occipitotemporal sites around 170 ms (the Pourtois, 2007; Williams et al., 2006). Previous work
“N170”) after stimulus onset. Source localization studies which has reported sustained positive ERP deflections
have usually identified brain regions in the core system that beyond 250 ms after stimulus onset for nonfacial emo-
gives rise to this N170 effect (Deffke et al., 2007; Itier & tional stimuli suggest that these late effects are not face-
Taylor, 2004b). In healthy individuals, a larger N170 has specific (Eimer & Holmes, 2007). In contrast, the early
been reported for faces compared with other objects. In (i.e., 120 ms) face-specific ERP component indicates that
addition, the N170 emerges for a diverse range of facial some information about the affective significance of faces
stimuli, including schematic, sketched, painted, drawn, can be extracted rapidly and before the perceptual analy-
and photographed faces, thus indicating that the effect sis differentiating faces from other objects is fully com-
is unlikely to be driven by low-level visual properties of pleted in the core neural system as indexed by the N170
faces (Sagiv & Bentin, 2001; Rossion & Jacques, 2008). (Vuilleumier & Pourtois, 2007). Again, further research
Interestingly, patients who are selectively impaired at rec- is required to establish whether comparable ERP effects
ognizing facial configurations display no face-specific are triggered by different emotional expressions (Eimer,
N170 effect (Bentin, DeGutis, D’Esposito, & Robertson, Holmes, & McGlone, 2003) or whether diverse emotional
2007; Eimer & McCarthy, 1999; Kress & Daum, 2003). expressions give rise to distinct modulations of ERP wave-
Of theoretical interest, the N170 is substantially forms (Ashley et al., 2004; Batty & Taylor, 2003; Pourtois,
increased (although delayed) when faces are inverted Grandjean, Sander, & Vuilleumier, 2004; Williams et al.,
(Bentin et al., 1996; Itier, Latinus, & Taylor, 2006). Quite 2006).
why this effect emerges is unclear. Some recent data sug- Although the occipitotemporal N170 appears to be
gest that, following inversion, there is greater recruitment of an early, reliable index differentiating face from object
eye-selective neurons in addition to face-selective neurons perception, the component is insensitive to differences
and that this increase promotes the generation of the N170 in facial familiarity (Bentin & Deouell, 2000; Herzmann,
inversion effect (Itier, Alain, Sedore, & McIntosh, 2007). Schweinberger, Sommer, & Jentzsch, 2004; Tanaka, Curran,
Implications for Social-Psychological Treatments of Person Perception 449

Porterfield, & Collins, 2006), face category (Caldara, body or body part configurations relative to possible
Thut, Michel, Bovet, & Renault, 2003; Ito, Thompson, & configurations has also been found to elicit ERP differences
Cacioppo, 2004; Mouchetant-Rostaing, Giard, Bentin, as early as 190 ms after stimulus onset (Overney, Michel,
Aguera, & Pernier, 2000; Mouchetant-Rostaing & Giard, Harris, & Pegna, 2005). Finally, a negative occipitotemporal
2003), or facial attractiveness (Schacht, Werheid, & ERP component related to the detection of the familiar form
Sommer, 2008; Werheid, Schacht, & Sommer, 2007). of a human figure from PL animations has been observed
These findings suggest that the N170 reflects the process- around 180 to 200 ms after the appearance of a stimulus
ing of faces with regard to their common structural proper- (Hirai, Fukushima, & Hiraki, 2003; Jokisch, Daum, Suchan, &
ties rather than the representation of a unique face. Troje, 2005). Collectively, these findings suggest that similar
Convergent data on reliable sex-, age-, and race-related to the face-specific N170, the N190 indexes the form-related
ERP effects are rare. Initial evidence suggests that at cen- structural processing of the human body and its parts and is
tral scalp locations around 150 to 250 ms after the appear- likely to be generated by the EBA, FBA, or both.
ance of a face, category-related processing effects emerge There is also preliminary empirical evidence that
(Mouchetant-Rostaing et al., 2000; Mouchetant-Rostaing & perceiving instrumental body actions performed with and
Giard, 2003; Ito et al., 2004). Further research is required without an emotional expression elicits faster processing of
to illuminate whether category-related ERP differences can emotional actions at parietooccipital sites as early as 110 ms
be demonstrated when low-level properties of the stimuli after stimulus onset (van Heijnsbergen, Meeren, Grèzes, &
and people’s processing goals are controlled. Interestingly, de Gelder, 2007). A study investigating the simultaneous per-
a reliable ERP component that responds toward facial ception of emotional expressions from facial and body cues
familiarity has been observed. Facial familiarity elicits a has revealed that as early as 110 ms after stimulus onset, a
negative potential that emerges at inferior temporal sites larger occipital positivity is elicited toward face–body pairs
around 250 ms (i.e., the “N250”) after stimulus onset. This with emotionally matching (i.e., fearful facial expression
component is sensitive to different types of familiar faces, and fearful body posture) rather than mismatching expres-
including famous faces, personally known faces, and exper- sions (i.e., angry facial expression and fearful body posture;
imentally learned faces (Herzmann et al., 2004; Tanaka Meeren et al., 2005). Similar to insights gained for emotional
et al., 2006). Finally, only few ERP studies have targeted face processing, these results suggest that emotional body
the processing of facial attractiveness. Although early ERP cues are processed even before the structural body analysis
markers of facial attractiveness have been reported, repli- as indexed by the N190 has been completed.
cable ERP differences between attractive and unattractive Finally, only a few studies have targeted the temporal
faces seem to occur relatively late in the processing stream dynamics underlying the perception of body motion and
between 400 and 600 ms after the onset of a face (Schacht action understanding. Thus far, it has been demonstrated that
et al., 2008; Werheid et al., 2007). the perception of whole bodies depicted with implied motion
relative to stationary bodies (i.e., running vs. standing) is
The Time Course of Body Perception associated with an increased ERP positivity at occipitopa-
Complementing work on face processing, researchers rietal electrodes at 260 ms after stimulus onset, an effect
have recently begun to investigate the temporal dynamics that cannot be attributed to low-level visual differences
of body and body part perception. When the perception of between the stimuli (Lorteije et al., 2006). In addition, when
photographic images of headless whole bodies as well as observed body motion comprises a meaningful action that
body silhouettes and stick figures is compared with the ends either erroneously or correctly, unexpected action slips
perception of pictures displaying faces and objects, a body- are marked by a larger ERP positivity at parietal electrode
specific negative component peaking at occipitotemporal sites around 300 ms after the error was perceived (De Bruijn,
electrode sites at 190 ms (N190) after stimulus onset has Schubotz, & Ullsperger, 2007). Further research is neces-
been observed (Thierry et al., 2006). What these data sug- sary to replicate and extend the role of these ERP indices
gest is that there is approximately a 20-ms difference for the with respect to action perception and understanding.
categorization of visual inputs as comprising faces or bod-
ies (Gliga & Dehaene-Lambertz, 2005). In a similar vein,
intracranial cell recording studies have revealed highly IMPLICATIONS FOR SOCIAL-
selective responses to headless bodies in the EBA starting PSYCHOLOGICAL TREATMENTS
at around 190 ms (Pourtois et al., 2007) and hand-selective OF PERSON PERCEPTION
responses in mainly occipitotemporal areas emerging 200
to 250 ms after stimulus onset (McCarthy, Puce, Belger, & Despite the priority given to perceptual processes in contem-
Allison, 1999). The perception of anatomically impossible porary accounts of face recognition (e.g., Bruce & Young,
450 Perceiving People

1986; Haxby et al., 2000, 2002), work in social cognition (1954) seminal writings, researchers have sought to
has tended to overlook the importance of these operations demonstrate the cognitive benefits that accrue from the adop-
(but see Zebrowitz, 1997), focusing instead on the cogni- tion of a categorical conception toward others. Although
tive and behavioral aspects of person construal (Macrae & the reported effects are many, the findings can be summa-
Bodenhausen, 2000). As noted at the beginning of the rized succinctly and easily—categorical thinking stream-
chapter, this oversight can be traced to a couple of factors. lines most aspects of person perception, including decision
First, the dominant empirical pursuit in social–cognitive making, memorial functioning, attentional processing, and
research has been to investigate person construal at the stages impression formation (see Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000).
of stereotype activation and application (Bodenhausen & Given these economizing effects, categorical thinking has
Macrae, 1998; Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; been characterized as an indispensable cognitive tool in the
Kunda & Spencer, 2003; Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000), person perception process (Bodenhausen & Lichtenstein,
stages that take place after a target has been detected 1987; Bodenhausen & Wyer, 1985; Gilbert & Hixon, 1991;
and categorized. Second, a favored technique for eliciting Macrae, Milne, & Bodenhausen, 1994; Pendry & Macrae,
categorical thinking has been to present participants with 1994). As Gilbert and Hixon (1991) have put it, “the ability
verbal stimuli (e.g., category labels, forenames), a meth- to understand new and unique individuals in terms of old
odology that necessarily obscures the role played by per- and general beliefs is certainly among the handiest tools in
ceptual operations in the registration, classification, and the social perceiver ’s kit” (p. 509). In no sense does con-
recognition of social targets (but see Blair et al., 2004; temporary work on face processing dispute this viewpoint;
Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Livingstone & Brewer, 2002; clearly the application of generic knowledge structures
Macrae et al., 1995, 1997; Maddox & Gray, 2002). Yet, simplifies otherwise complex and effortful aspects of the
as we have shown, these operations patently exert a sig- person perception process. What is questionable, however,
nificant influence on the course and myriad products of the is whether cognitive economy is the only determinant of
person perception process (Bruce & Young, 1998). people’s preference for category-based solutions to the
Underlying people’s understanding of others are per- puzzle of person construal (Allport, 1954). As an extensive
ceptual operations that abstract both variant (e.g., emo- literature in face processing has demonstrated, early per-
tional expression, gaze direction) and invariant (e.g., sex, ceptual processes (and their associated products) also play
identity) aspects of person knowledge from available facial an important contributory role to the generation of cate-
cues (Bruce & Young, 1986; Burton, Bruce, & Johnston, gorical thinking (Blair et al., 2004; Livingstone & Brewer,
1990; Haxby et al., 2000, 2002, Tarr & Gauthier, 2000). 2002; Mason & Macrae, 2004).
In this respect, it is interesting to note that contemporary Several lines of evidence point to perceptual process-
models of face processing confront the same challenge ing as an important determinant of categorical thinking.
as social–cognitive approaches to person perception— First, perceivers can extract category-cueing material from
namely, to establish how and when perceivers construe tar- faces more rapidly and accurately than identity-triggering
gets (e.g., Albert Einstein, Uncle Dave) as unique entities information. Second, at least for intact stimuli, the opera-
(i.e., person individuation) rather than instances of generic tions that extract category-cueing information demonstrate
social groupings (i.e., person categorization). Interestingly, a resistance to manipulations that impair face processing,
the solutions reached by the respective fields are quite such as stimulus orientation and degradation (Maurer
distinct, with each emphasizing the importance of differ- et al., 2002; Valentine, 1988). This observation is important
ent operations in the information-processing stream (e.g., because it highlights the perceptual efficiency of categori-
Bruce & Young, 1986; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). Whereas cal thinking. Even when perceivers are presented with stim-
social–cognitive research underscores the importance uli in suboptimal conditions, the face-processing system is
of memorial operations (e.g., category-based vs. person- still capable of extracting categorical knowledge in a rapid
based retrieval) in the person perception process (Fiske & and accurate manner. Third, category activation is sensitive
Neuberg, 1990; Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000), work in to the typicality of group members. In particular, categori-
face processing emphasizes the perceptual operations cal thinking is moderated by the extent to which individu-
through which people abstract categorical and identity- als possess physical features that are deemed to be typical
related information about others (Bruce & Young, 1986; of the groups to which they belong (see Blair et al., 2004;
Farah et al., 1998; Haxby et al., 2000, 2002). Livingstone & Brewer, 2002; Maddox & Gray, 2002).
Dominating both research and theorizing in social psy- What the aforementioned findings suggest is that,
chology for almost 5 decades has been the assumption given appropriate triggering cues, category-based respond-
that category-based responding drives person perception ing may be the predominant social–cognitive outcome
(Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). Since Allport’s at all stages of the person perception process. Before the
Person Perception: One Process or Several? 451

cognitive efficiencies that accrue from the application of circumstances (e.g., goal-based processing) and for certain
category-based knowledge structures in memory (Allport, targets (e.g., familiar or important others). In this way,
1954; Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Macrae & perceivers are able to negotiate the complex demands
Bodenhausen, 2000), perceptual operations may already be imposed by everyday social interaction (Brewer, 1988;
preferentially extracting category-cueing information from Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Kunda & Spencer, 2003; Macrae &
faces, especially in challenging task environments. Little Bodenhausen, 2000).
wonder, therefore, that categorical thinking should exert Notwithstanding the inherent flexibility of the face-
such a potent influence on person construal. The process- processing system (Bruce & Young, 1986, 1998; Haxby
ing architecture that supports social–cognitive functioning et al., 2000, 2002; Tarr & Gauthier, 2000), why is it that per-
(Haxby et al., 2000, 2002) would appear to be biased ceivers so readily adopt a categorical stance when interact-
toward category-based responding at all stages of the per- ing with others? Aside from the cognitive economies that
son perception process. In this respect, person construal accrue from this mode of thought (Macrae & Bodenhausen,
may simply reflect the mind’s evolved strategy of impos- 2000), perceptual factors also likely contribute to this pref-
ing a categorical solution on a broad range of problems in erence for categorical thinking. Generally speaking, clas-
perception and cognition (Harnad, 1987). sifying a person by category is less perceptually taxing
Given the perceptual efficiency of category-based con- than individuating that same target from other members
strual, does this imply that the products of the person per- of the group (Tarr & Cheng, 2003; Tarr & Gauthier, 2000).
ception process are inevitably dominated by that which This ease of perceptual processing is undoubtedly impor-
can be computed most easily—categorical judgments? In tant when one can use generic categorical representations
addressing this important issue, two observations are wor- in memory to guide cognition and action (Bodenhausen &
thy of mention. The ultimate benefit of categorical thinking Macrae, 1998; Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990;
lies in the fact that preexisting knowledge structures can be Kunda & Spencer, 2003). It is perhaps no accident,
used to guide information processing and response genera- therefore, that perceivers are most likely to think categori-
tion, especially when other sources of person knowledge are cally about individuals who belong to groups for which
absent, ambiguous, or difficult to acquire (Allport, 1954; strong beliefs and expectances are held (e.g., race and
Bodenhausen & Macrae, 1998; Brewer, 1988; Fiske & sex stereotypes—Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990);
Neuberg, 1990; Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). It is unsur- groups that are characterized by the possession of salient
prising, therefore, that categorical construal is triggered by perceptual features (e.g., skin tone, hairstyle).
basic featural cues (Blair et al., 2004; Livingstone & Brewer,
2002) and is more resistant to disruption than person-based
construal. Such functional properties would be expected of PERSON PERCEPTION: ONE PROCESS
a process that streamlines information-processing and OR SEVERAL?
response generation. Ease of categorical construal, how-
ever, does not extend to all processing contexts and social Humans are remarkably skilled at understanding other
targets. Indeed, there are conditions under which individu- social agents. From only a few visual cues, people are
ated responses reliably dominate people’s outputs (Fiske & able to formulate detailed impressions of others (Brewer,
Neuberg, 1990; Kunda & Spencer, 2003). 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990), identify the sex, emo-
Any object, including a person, can be identified at tional status, and identity of conspecifics (Bruce & Young,
multiple levels of abstraction (e.g., woman vs. one’s cur- 1986); and infer the hidden internal states (e.g., goals,
rent partner). It is assumed, however, that objects are first intentions) that give rise to purposive behavior (Baron-
perceptually identified at what is termed the entry level of Cohen, 1995; Gallagher & Frith, 2004). When one con-
categorical representation (Jolicoeur, Gluck, & Kosslyn, siders the challenges inherent in daily social exchange,
1984). This is the level at which a name can be generated these social–cognitive abilities are striking. So why
or matched most rapidly to an object. Importantly, for some are social perceivers so adroit at understanding others?
social targets, particularly highly familiar or relevant oth- In social cognition, two basic processes are believed
ers, person recognition is believed to be automatized at the to subserve person perception—categorization and
individual or exemplar-based level (Tanaka, 2001; Turk, individuation (see Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). At least as
Rosenblum, Gazzaniga, & Macrae, 2005). Thus, although operationalized in the social–cognitive literature, catego-
category-based responding may be the least demand- rization and individuation are typically indexed through
ing option at all stages of the person perception process, the differential products of memory retrieval (Kunda &
the face-processing system nevertheless retains the flex- Spencer, 2003; Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000; Macrae et al.,
ibility to generate person-based responses under specific 1999), a strategy that gives credence to the viewpoint that
452 Perceiving People

person construal is guided by the operation of two distinct 2000; Ganel & Goshen-Gottstein, 2002; Rossion, 2002).
cognitive processes. As Ganel and Goshen-Gottstein (2002) have reported,
Before the retrieval of information from long-term “as for the processing of sex and identity . . . results . . .
memory, however, a great deal of social–cognitive pro- indicate that these facial dimensions are processed by a
cessing has already taken place. Most notably, perceivers single system at both a functional level of description and at
have resolved the perceptual puzzle of identifying social a neuroanatomical level (p. 865)”. This viewpoint does not
agents from available visual cues. Capturing, as they do, imply, however, that classic dual-process models in social
different solutions or outcomes to the problem of person cognition are necessarily incorrect in their assertion that cat-
construal (e.g., group member vs. unique entity), categori- egorization and individuation are distinct cognitive processes
zation and individuation also operate at these early stages (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). Such a statement may be accurate
of person perception (Bruce & Young, 1986; Haxby et al., when one considers later stages in the person perception pro-
2000, 2002; Tarr & Cheng, 2003; Tarr & Gauthier, 2000). cess, stages in which perceivers organize target-relevant infor-
Importantly, however, rather than reflecting the operation mation in memory, allocate attention to current processing
of dual cognitive processes, during the early perceptual concerns, and generate target-related inferences (Macrae &
stages of person construal, categorization and individua- Bodenhausen, 2000; Mason & Macrae, 2004). What is
tion are believed to index different products or outcomes of needed is additional research that specifies the precise
a common processing mechanism (see Mason & Macrae, nature of the underlying perceptual, cognitive, and neural
2004). Such a viewpoint is consistent with recent models of operations that guide person construal at all stages of the
object recognition (Tarr & Cheng, 2003; Tarr & Gauthier, person perception process.
2000). Tarr and Cheng (2003), for example, have proposed
that recognition across all object categories (e.g., birds,
cars, people) is supported by a single processing system SUMMARY
that is tuned by a combination of experience and instruction
(Diamond & Carey, 1986; Haxby et al., 2000, 2002; To negotiate the intricacies of everyday interaction, perceiv-
Tanaka, 2001; Tarr & Gauthier, 2000). Within this system, ers require information-processing routines that simplify
regions of ventral temporal cortex are engaged when peo- the complexities of the person perception process. To gain
ple make subordinate-level (i.e., individuated) judgments a comprehensive understanding of the process and products
about stimuli for which they have acquired prior percep- of person construal, integrative theorizing and experimen-
tual experience. This includes, but is not restricted to, faces tation is required. Although neuroscience research provides
(Tarr & Cheng, 2003; Tarr & Gauthier, 2000). valuable information about the temporal characteristics
If a single (albeit modular) system is sufficient to recog- and neuroanatomy of person perception, it has little to say
nize all types of objects at varying levels of specificity, it about the precise stimulus cues that people use to construe
seems reasonable to conclude that categorization and indi- others, the extent to which the process of construal is mod-
viduation may reflect different social–cognitive products of ulated by processing goals and individual differences, and
this system. Adopting such a viewpoint, Mason and Macrae the degree to which early visual operations guide the gen-
(2004) have argued that categorization (i.e., category-based eration of people’s postperceptual products, such as impres-
representations) and individuation (i.e., person-based repre- sions and memories. Social cognition research, however,
sentations) are perceptual solutions to the problem of per- speaks directly to these fundamentally important questions.
son construal that simply make different demands on the Only by using a range of complementary techniques and
distributed neural network that subserves face processing approaches, we suspect, will it be possible to elucidate the
(see Haxby et al., 2000, 2002). Specifically, as individua- richness of the person perception process.
tion relies on the extraction of more complex information
(i.e., configural information) from the face than categoriza-
tion (Diamond & Carey, 1986; Farah et al., 1998; Maurer
et al., 2002; Rossion & Gauthier, 2002), greater activity REFERENCES
is observed in regions of ventral temporal cortex during
the former than latter construal. Together with findings Abbas, Z.-A., & Duchaine, B. (2008). The role of holistic processing in
from brain imaging investigations (Dubois et al., 1999; judgments of facial attractiveness. Perception, 37, 1187–1196.
Mason & Macrae, 2004; Rossion, Schiltz, Robaye, Pirenne, & Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the
processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14,
Crommelink, 2001), recent behavioral research also sug- 644–647.
gests that categorization and individuation are realized Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze
through a single processing mechanism (see Calder et al., on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 3–11.
References 453

Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., & Baudoin, J.-Y., & Humphreys, G. W. (2006). Configural information in
Damasio, A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition gender categorisation. Perception, 35, 531–540.
after amygdala damage. Nature, 433, 68–72. Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2006). Predictive gaze cues and person-
Adolphs, R., & Tranel, D. (2003). Amygdala damage impairs emotion ality judgments: Should eye trust you? Psychological Science, 17,
recognition from scenes only when they contain facial expressions. 514–520.
Neuropsychologia, 41, 1281–1289. Beauchamp, M. S., Lee, K. E., Haxby, J. V., & Martin, A. (2002). Parallel
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1998). The human amygdala in visual motion processing streams for manipulable objects and human
social judgment. Nature, 393, 470–474. movements. Neuron, 4, 149–159.
Albright, L., Kenny, D. A., & Malloy, T. E. (1988). Consensus in personal- Beer, A. & Watson, D. (2008). Personality judgment at zero acquaintance:
ity judgments at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Agreement, assumed similarity, and implicit simplicity. Journal of
Psychology, 55, 387–395. Personality Assessment, 90, 250–260.
Albright, L., Malloy, T. E., Dong, Q., Kenny, D. A., Fang, X., Winquist, L., & Behling, D. U., & Williams, E. A. (1991). Influence of dress on perception
Yu, D. (1997). Cross-cultural consensus in personality judgments. of intelligence and expectations of scholastic achievement. Clothing
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 558–569. and Textiles Research Journal, 9, 1–7.
Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & McCarthy, G. (1996).
Addison-Wesley. Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 551–565.
Ambadar, Z., Schooler, J., & Cohn, J. F. (2005). Deciphering the enigmatic
face: The importance of facial dynamics in interpreting subtle facial Bentin, S., DeGutis, J. M., D’Esposito, M., & Robertson, L. C. (2007).
expression. Psychological Science, 16, 403–410. Too many trees to see the forest: Performance, event-related potential,
and functional magnetic resonance imaging manifestations of integra-
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior
tive congenital prosopagnosia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19,
as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis.
132–146.
Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.
Bentin, S., & Deouell, L. (2000). Structural encoding and identification
Amodio, D. M. (2008). The social neuroscience of intergroup relations.
in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanism. Cognitive
European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 1–54.
Neuropsychology, 17, 35–54.
Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., De Rosa, E., & Gabrieli, J. D.
Bentin, S., Golland, Y., Flevaris, A., Robertson, L. C., & Moscovitch, M.
(2003). Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial
(2006). Processing the trees and the forest during initial stages of
signals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5627–5633.
face perception: Electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive
Anderson, J. L., Crawford, C. B., Nadeau, J., & Lindberg, T. (1992). Was Neuroscience, 18, 1406–1421.
the Duchess of Windsor right? A cross-cultural review of the socioecol-
Bernstein, M. J., Young, S. G., & Hugenberg, K. (2007). The cross cat-
ogy of ideals of female body shape. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13,
egory effect: Mere social categorization is sufficient to elicit an own-
197–227.
group bias in face recognition. Psychological Science, 18, 706–712.
Angus, J., & Reeve, P. (2006). Ageism: A threat to “aging well” in the 21st Berry, D. S. (1990). What can a moving face tell us? Journal of Personality
century. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 25, 137–152. and Social Psychology, 58, 1004–1014.
Ashley, V., Vuilleumier, P., & Swick, D. (2004). Time course and specific- Berry, D. S., & McArthur, L. Z. (1986). Perceiving character in faces:
ity of event-related potentials to emotional expressions. NeuroReport, The impact of age-related craniofacial changes on social perception.
15, 211–216. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 3–18.
Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Berry, D. S., & Zebrowitz-McArthur, L. (1998). What’s in a face? Facial
Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point- maturity and the attribution of legal responsibility. Personality and
light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746. Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 23–33.
Atkinson, A. P., Heberlein, A. S., & Adolphs, R. (2007). Spared ability Blair, I. V., Judd, C. M., & Chapleau, K. M. (2004). The influence of
to recognise fear from static and moving whole-body cues following Afrocentric facial features in criminal sentencing. Psychological
bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2772–2782. Science, 15, 674–679.
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., Blair, I. V., Judd, M., Sadler, M. S., & Jenkins, C. (2002). The role of
Moscovitch, M., & Bentin, S. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Afrocentric features in person perception: Judging by features and cat-
Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological egories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 5–25.
Science, 19, 724–732.
Blake, R., & Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual
Bar, M., Neta, M., & Linz, H. (2006). Very first impressions. Emotion, 6, Review of Psychology, 58, 47–73.
269–278.
Blau, V. C., Maurer, U., Tottenham, N., & McCandliss, B. D. (2007).
Barden, J., Maddux, W. W., Petty, R. E., & Brewer, M. B. (2004). Contextual The face-specific N170 component is modulated by emotional facial
moderation of racial bias: The impact of social roles on controlled and expression. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3, 1–13.
automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Bodenhausen, G. V., & Lichtenstein, M. (1987). Social stereotypes and
Psychology, 87, 5–22. information-processing strategies: The impact of task complexity.
Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster: The case against controllability Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 871–880.
of automatic stereotype effects. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual Bodenhausen, G. V., & Macrae, C. N. (1998). Stereotype activation and
process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press. inhibition. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 11,
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT pp. 1–52). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Press/Bradford. Bodenhausen, G. V., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1985). Effects of stereotypes on
Bassili, J. N. (1979). Emotion recognition: The role of facial movement and decision making and information-processing strategies. Journal of
the relative importance of upper and lower areas of the face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 267–282.
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2049–2058. Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1992). Trait inferences: Sources of validity
Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2003). Early processing of the six basic facial at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
emotional expressions. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 613–620. 62, 645–657.
454 Perceiving People

Bould, E., & Morris, N. (2008). Role of motion signals in recognizing Capgras, J. M. J., & Reboul-Lachaux, J. (1923). L’illusion des “sosies”
subtle facial expressions of emotion. British Journal of Psychology, dans un d’elire systématisé chronique. Bulletin in Society and Clinical
99, 167–189. Medicine Mental, 11, 6–16.
Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual-process model of impression formation. In Chan, A. W., Peelen, M., & Downing, P. (2004). The effect of viewpoint
R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 1, on body representation in the extrastriate body area. NeuroReport, 15,
pp. 1–36). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 2407–2410.
Brochu, P. M., & Morrison, M. A. (2007). Implicit and explicit prejudice Chen, S., & Andersen, S.M. (1999). Relationships from the past in the
toward overweight and average-weight men and women: Testing their present: Significant-other representations and transference in inter-
correspondence and relation to behavioral intentions. Journal of Social personal life. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 147, 681–706. Psychology (Vol. 31, pp. 123–190). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for integrating pri- Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1994). Nonconscious behavioral confirmation
mate behaviour and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in processes: The self-fulfilling consequences of automatic stereotype
Neuroscience, 1, 27–51. activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 541–560.
Brown, E., & Perrett, D. I. (1993). What gives a face its gender? Perception, Chong, T. T. J., Williams, M. A., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J. B.
22, 829–840. (2008). Selective attention modulates inferior frontal gyrus activity
during action observation. NeuroImage, 40, 298–307.
Bruce, V., Burton, A. M., Hanna, E., Healey, P., Mason, O., Coombes, A.,
Fright, R., & Linney, A. (1993). Sex discrimination: How do we tell the Chouchourelou, A., Matsuka, T., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2006). The
difference between male and female faces? Perception, 22, 131–152. visual analysis of emotional actions. Social Neuroscience, 1, 63–74.
Clark, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D.
Bruce, V., & Humphreys, G. (1994). Recognizing faces and objects. Visual
(2005). The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light
Cognition, 1, 141–180.
displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception, 34, 1171–1180.
Bruce, V., & Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. British
Cleveland, A., & Striano, T. (2007). The effects of joint attention on
Journal of Psychology, 77, 305–327.
object processing in 4- and 9-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and
Bruce, V., & Young, A. (1998). In the eye of the beholder: The science of Development, 30, 499–504.
face perception. New York: Oxford University Press.
Cloutier, J., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Who or what are you? Facial orienta-
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., tion and person construal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37,
et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal 1298–1309.
areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. European Journal of Cloutier, J., Mason, M. F., & Macrae, N. C. (2005). The perceptual deter-
Neuroscience, 13, 400–404. minants of person construal: Reopening the social-cognitive toolbox.
Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (1995). Perception of age in adult Caucasian Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 885–894.
male faces: Computer graphic manipulation of shape and colour infor- Cloutier, J., Turk, D. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Extracting variant and
mation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 259, invariant information from faces: The neural substrates of gaze detec-
137–143. tion and sex categorization. Social Neuroscience, 3, 69–78.
Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Dench, N., (1993). What’s the difference Collishaw, S. M., & Hole, G. J. (2000). Featural and configurational pro-
between men and women? Evidence from facial measurement. cesses in the recognition of faces of different familiarity. Perception,
Perception, 22, 153–176. 29, 893–909.
Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Johnston, R. A. (1990). Understanding face Corneille, O., Huart, J., Becquart, E., & Bredart, S. (2004). When memory
recognition with an interactive activation model. British Journal of shifts toward more typical category exemplars: Accentuation effects in
Psychology, 81, 361–380. the recollection of ethnically ambiguous faces. Journal of Personality
Caldara, R., Thut, G., Michel, C. M., Bovet, P., & Renault, B. (2003). Face and Social Psychology, 86, 236–250.
versus non-face object perception and the “other-race” effect: A spatio- Coulsen, M. (2004). Attributing emotion to static body postures:
temporal event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, Recognition accuracy, confusions, and viewpoint dependence. Journal
515–528. of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 117–139.
Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N., & Young, A. W. (2000). Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., &
Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Banaji, M. R. (2004). Separable neural components in the processing
Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1077–1078. of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15, 806–813.
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their
(2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the
with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249. Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.
Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. Deaner, R. O., Shepherd, S. V., & Platt, M. L. (2007). Familiarity accentu-
(2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in ates gaze cueing in women but not men. Biology Letters, 3, 64–67.
action observation. Current Biology, 16, 1–6. De Bruijn, E. R. A., Schubotz, R. I., & Ullsperger, M. (2007). An event-
Campbell, R., MacSweeney, M., Surguladze, S., Calvert, G., McGuire, P., related potential study on the observation of erroneous everyday actions.
Suckling, J., et al. (2001). Cortical substrates for the perception of Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 278–285.
face actions: An fMRI study of the specificity of activation for seen DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Social
speech and for meaningless lower-face acts (gurning). Cognitive Brain perception of facial resemblance in humans. Archives of Sexual
Research, 12, 233–243. Behavior, 37, 64–77.
Campbell, R., Wallace, S., & Benson, P. J. (1996). Real men don’t look Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the percep-
down: Direction of gaze affects sex decision on faces. Visual Cognition, tion of human actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 172–178.
3, 393–412. Deffenbacher, K. A., Vetter, T., Johanson, J., & O’Toole, A. J. (1998). Facial
Camras, L. A., Sullivan, J., & Michel, G. (1993). Do infants express dis- aging, attractiveness, and distinctiveness. Perception, 27, 1233–1243.
crete emotions? Adult judgments of facial, vocal, and body actions. Deffke, I., Sander, T., Heidenreich, J., Sommer, W., Curio, G., Trahms, L., &
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 171–186. Lueschow, A. (2007). MEG/EEG sources of the 170-ms response
References 455

to faces are co-localized in the fusiform gyrus. NeuroImage, 35, Dubois, S., Rossion, B., Schiltz, C., Bodart, J.-M., Michel, C., Bruyer, R., &
1495–1501. Crommelinck, M. (1999). Effect of familiarity on the processing of
de Gelder, B. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotional body lan- human faces. NeuroImage, 9, 278–289.
guage. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 242–249. Durand, K., Gallay, M., Seigneuric, A., Robichon, F., & Baudouin. J.-Y.
de Gelder, B., & Hadjikhani, N. (2006). Non-conscious recognition of (2007). The development of facial emotion recognition: The role of
emotional body language. NeuroReport, 17, 583–586. configural information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
97, 14–27.
de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., & Hadjikhani, N.
(2004). Fear fosters flight. A mechanism for fear contagion when Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Longo, L. C. (1991).
perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of the What is beautiful is good, but . . . : A meta-analytic review of research
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110,
16701–16706. 109–128.
De Haan, E. H. F., Bauer, R. M., & Greve, K. W. (1992). Behavioural and Eberhardt, J. L., Davies, P. G., Purdie-Vaughns, V. J., & Johnson, S. L.
physiological evidence for covert face recognition in a prosopagnosic (2006). Looking deathworthy: Perceived stereotypicality of Black
patient. Cortex, 28, 77–95. defendants predicts capital-sentencing outcomes. Psychological
Science, 17, 382–386.
De Haan, E. H. F., Young, A. W., & Newcombe, F. (1991). A dissocia-
tion between the sense of familiarity and access to semantic infor- Ebner, N. C. (2008). Age of face matters: Age-group differences in ratings
mation concerning familiar people. European Journal of Cognitive of young and old faces. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 130–136.
Psychology, 3, 51–67. Eimer, M. (2000). Attentional modulations of event-related brain potentials
DeJong, W., & Kleck, R. E. (1986). The social psychological effects of sensitive to faces. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 103–116.
overweight. In C. P. Herman, M. P. Zanna, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Eimer, M., & Holmes, A. (2002). An ERP study on the time course of emo-
Physical appearance, stigma, and social behavior (pp. 65–87). tional face processing. NeuroReport, 13, 427–431.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Eimer, M., & Holmes, A. (2007). Event-related potential correlates of emo-
Desimone, R., Albright, T. D., Gross, C. G., & Bruce, C. (1984). Stimulus- tional face processing. Neuropsychologia, 45, 15–31.
selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque. The Eimer, M., Holmes, A., & McGlone, F. (2003). The role of spatial attention
Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 2051–2062. in the processing of facial expression: An ERP study of rapid brain
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and con- responses to six basic emotions. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral
trolled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Neuroscience, 3, 97–110.
56, 5–18. Eimer, M., & McCarthy, R. A. (1999). Prosopagnosia and structural encod-
Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? ing of faces: Evidence from event-related potentials. NeuroReport, 10,
The Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology 255–259.
Bulletin, 21, 1139–1150. Ekman, P. (1965). Differential communication of affect by head and body
Di Russo, F., Martinez, A., Sereno, M. I., Pitzalis, S., & Hillyard, S. A. cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 726–735.
(2002). Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked Ekman, P. (1999) Facial expressions. In T. Dalgleish & T. Power (Eds.) The
potential. Human Brain Mapping, 15, 95–111. Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (pp. 301–320). Sussex, England:
Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and are not special: An Wiley.
effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed. New York: Times Books.
115, 107–117. Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural
Dion, K. (1972). Physical attractiveness and evaluation of children’s specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological
transgressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, Bulletin, 128, 203–235.
207–213. Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Universals and cultural differ-
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. ences in recognizing emotions. Current Directions in Psychological
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285–290. Science, 12, 159–164.
Downing, P. E., Bray, D., Rogers, J., & Childs, C. (2004). Bodies capture Ellis, H., Shepherd, J., & Davies, G., (1979). Identification of familiar and
attention when nothing is expected. Cognition, 93, B27–B38. unfamiliar faces from internal and external features: Some implications
Downing, P. E., Chan, A. W., Peelen, M. V., Dodds, C. M., & Kanwisher, N. for theories of face recognition. Perception, 8, 431–439.
(2006). Domain specificity in visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16, Ellis, H. D., Young, A. W., Quayle, A. H., & De Pauw, K. W. (1997).
1453–1461. Reduced autonomic responses to faces in Capgras delusion. Proceedings
Downing, P. E., Jiang, Y., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences, 264,
area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science, 293, 1085–1092.
2470–2473. Ellison, J. W., & Massaro, D. W. (1997). Featural evaluation, integration,
Downing, P. E., Peelen, M. V., Wiggett, A. J., & Tew, B. D. (2006). The role and judgment of facial affect. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
of the extrastriate body area in action perception. Social Neuroscience, Human Perception and Performance, 23, 213–226.
1, 52–62. Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness
Downing, P. E., Wiggett, A. J., & Peelen, M. V. (2007). Functional mag- decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala.
netic resonance imaging investigation of overlapping lateral occipito- Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1508–1519.
temporal activations using multi-voxel pattern analysis. The Journal of Epstein, J., Klinkenberg, W. D., Scandell, D. J., Faulkner, K., & Claus, R. E.
Neuroscience, 27, 226–233. (2007). Perceived physical attractiveness, sexual history, and sexual
Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron- intentions: An Internet study. Sex Roles, 56, 23–31.
Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial ori- Evans, J. J., Heggs, A. J., Antoun, N. & Hedges, J. R. (1995). Progressive
enting. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–540. prosopagnosia associated with selective right temporal lobe atrophy.
Dubois, M., & Pansu, P. (2004). Facial attractiveness, applicants’ qualifica- A new syndrome? Brain, 118, 1–13.
tions, and judges’ expertise about decisions in preselective recruitment. Fagan J. (1972). Infants’ recognition memory for faces. Journal of
Psychological Reports, 95, 1120–1134. Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 453–476.
456 Perceiving People

Fan, J., Dai, W., Liu, F., & Wu, J. (2005). Visual perception of male body George, P. A., & Hole, G. J. (2000). The role of spatial and surface cues in
attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society London B, 272, the age-processing of unfamiliar faces. Visual Cognition, 7, 485–509.
219–226. Giese, M. A., & Poggio, T. (2003). Neural mechanisms for the recognition
Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Drain, H. M., & Tanaka, J. W. (1998). What is of biological movements. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 179–192.
special about face perception? Psychological Review, 105, 482–498. Gilbert, D. T. (1998). Ordinary personology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.,
detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy pp. 89–150). New York: McGraw-Hill.
of Sciences, 99, 9602–9605.
Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking. Activation
Fiske, S. T. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice and discrimination. In and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and
D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.
psychology (pp. 357–411). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Gilmore, G. C., Wenk, H. E., Baylor, L. A., & Stuve, T. A. (1992). Motion
Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression forma- perception and aging. Psychology and Aging, 7, 654–660.
tion, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of
information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances Gliga, T., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2005). Structural encoding of body and
in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 1–74. face in human infants and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
17, 1328–1340.
Fletcher-Watson, S., Findlay, J. M., Leekam, S. R., & Benson, V. (2008).
Rapid detection of person information in a naturalistic scene. Gobbini, M. I., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Neural systems for recognition of
Perception, 37, 571–583. familiar faces. Neuropsychologia, 45, 32–41.
Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Chiao, J. Y., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2001).
human infants. Science, 218, 486–487. Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-
race faces. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 845–850.
Freeman, J. B., Ambady, N., & Rule, N. (2008). Will a category cue attract
you? Motor output reveals dynamic competition across person con- Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate pathways for perception
strual. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 673–690. and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 20–25.
Friedman, H., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (1992). The contribution of typical sex Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Ganel, T. (2000). Repetition priming for familiar
differences in facial maturity to sex role stereotypes. Personality and and unfamiliar faces in a sex-judgment task: Evidence for a common
Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 430–438. route for the processing of sex and identity. Journal of Experimental
Frieze, I. H., Olson, J. E., & Russell, J. (1991). Attractiveness and income Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1198–1214.
for men and women in management. Journal of Applied Social Grèzes, J., Armony, J. L., Rowe, J., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Activations
Psychology, 21, 1039–1057. related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: An
Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of atten- fMRI study. NeuroImage, 18, 928–937.
tion: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental
Psychological Bulletin, 133, 694–724. simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-
Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1988). Friends and strangers: analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 1–19.
Acquaintanceship, agreement, and the accuracy of personality judg- Grèzes, J., Fonlupt, P., Bertenthal, B., Delon-Martin, C., Segebarth, C., &
ment. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 55, 149–158. Decety, J. (2001). Does perception of biological motion rely on specific
Furnham, A., Dias, M., & McClelland, A. (1998). The role of body weight, brain regions? NeuroImage, 13, 775–785.
waist-to-hip ratio, and breast size in judgments of female attractive- Grèzes, J., Pichon, S., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Perceiving fear in dynamic
ness. Sex Roles, 34, 311–326. body expressions. NeuroImage, 35, 959–967.
Furnham, A., Hester, C., & Weir, C. (1990). Sex difference in preference Griffin, A. M., & Langlois, J. H. (2006). Stereotype directionality and
for specific female body shapes. Sex Roles, 22, 743–754. attractiveness stereotyping: Is beauty good or is ugly bad? Social
Furnham, A., & Swami, V. (2007). Perception of female buttocks and Cognition, 24, 187–206.
breast size in profile. Social Behavior and Personality, 35, 1–8.
Grossman, E. D., Battelli, L., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Repetitive TMS
Furnham, A., Swami, V., & Shah, K. (2006). Body weight, waist-to-hip over STSp disrupts perception of biological motion. Vision Research,
ratio and breast size correlates of ratings of attractiveness and health. 45, 2847–2853.
Personality and Individual differences, 41, 443–454.
Grossman, E. D., & Blake, R. (2002). Brain areas active during visual per-
Ganel, T., & Goshen-Gottstein, Y. (2002). The perceptual integrality of ception of biological motion. Neuron, 35, 1167–1175.
sex and identity of faces: Further evidence for the single-route hypoth-
Grossman, E. D., Blake, R., & Kim, C. Y. (2004). Learning to see bio-
esis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
logical motion: Brain activity parallels behavior. Journal of Cognitive
Performance, 28, 854–867.
Neuroscience, 16, 1669–1679.
Gainotti, G., Barbier, A., & Marra, C. (2003). Slowly progressive defect in
recognition of familiar people in a patient with right anterior temporal Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, J., &
atrophy. Brain, 126, 792–803. Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological
motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 711–720.
Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Dissociable neural pathways for
the perception and recognition of expressive and instrumental gestures. Hadjikhani, N., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Seeing fearful body expressions
Neuropsychologia, 42, 1725–1736. activates the fusiform cortex and amygdala. Current Biology, 13,
2201–2205.
Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Moylan, J., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., &
Anderson, A. W. (2000). The fusiform “face area” is part of a net- Haig, N. D. (1986). Exploring recognition with interchanged facial fea-
work that processes faces at the individual level. Journal of Cognitive tures. Perception, 15, 235–247.
Neuroscience, 12, 495–504. Hall, J. A., & Matsumoto, D. (2004). Gender differences in judgments of
George, P. A., & Hole, G. J. (1995). Factors influencing the accuracy of age multiple emotions from facial expressions. Emotion, 4, 201–296.
estimates of unfamiliar faces. Perception, 24, 1059–1073. Hamermesh, D. S., & Parker, A. (2005). Beauty in the classroom:
George, P. A., & Hole, G. J. (1998). The influence of feature-based informa- Instructors’ pulchritude and putative pedagogical productivity.
tion in the age processing of unfamiliar faces. Perception, 27, 295–312. Economics of Education Review, 24, 369–376.
References 457

Hancock, P. J. B., Bruce, V., & Burton, A. M. (2000). Recognition of Hiris, E., Humphrey, D., & Stout, A. (2005). Temporal properties in masking
unfamiliar faces. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 330–337. of biological motion. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 435–443.
Harnad, S. (Ed.). (1987). Categorical perception: The groundwork of cog- Hiris, E., Krebeck, A., Edmonds, J., & Stout, A. (2005). What learn-
nition. New York: Cambridge University Press. ing to see arbitrary motion tells us about biological motion percep-
Hart, A. J., Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., & tion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Rauch, S. L. (2000). Differential response in the human amygdala to Performance, 31, 1096–1106.
racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. NeuroReport, 11, 2351–2355. Hirstein, W., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1997). Capgras’ syndrome: A novel
Hasson, U., Nir, Y., Levy, I., Fuhrmann, G., & Malach, R. (2004). probe for understanding the neural representation of the identity and
Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. familiarity of persons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.
Science, 303, 1634–1640. Series B. Biological Sciences, 264, 437–444.
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. A. (2000). The distributed Hoffman, E., & Haxby, J. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and
human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception.
4, 223–233. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 80–84.
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. A. (2002). Human neural Hole, G. (1994). Configurational factors in the perception of unfamiliar
systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological faces. Perception, 23, 65–74.
Psychiatry, 51, 59–67. Holmes, A., Vuilleumier, P., & Eimer, M. (2003). The processing of emotional
Haxby, J., Ungerleider, L. G., Clark, V. P., Schouten, J. L., Hoffman, E. A., & facial expression is gated by spatial attention: Evidence from event-
Martin, A. (1999). The effect of face inversion on activity in human related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 174–184.
neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron, 22, 189–199. Hönekopp, J. (2006). Once more: Is beauty in the eye of the beholder?
Heberlein, A. S., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2004). Cortical Relative contributions of private and shared taste to judgments of
regions for judgments of emotions and personality traits from point- facial attractiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
light walkers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1143–1158. Perception and Performance, 32, 199–209.
Heberlein, A. S., & Saxe, R. R. (2005). Dissociation between emotional Hönekopp, J., Rudolph, U., Beier, L., Liebert, A., & Müller, C. (2007).
and personality judgments: Convergent evidence from functional neu- Physical attractiveness of face and body as indicators of physical fit-
roimaging. NeuroImage, 28, 770–777. ness in men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 106–111.
Hebl, M. R., & Turchin, J. M. (2005). The stigma of obesity: What about Hood, B. M., Macrae, C. N., Cole-Davies, V. & Dias, M. (2003). Eye
men? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 267–275. remember you! The effects of gaze direction on face recognition in
children and adults. Developmental Science, 6, 69–73.
Hecaen, H., & Angelergues, R. (1962). Agnosia for faces (prosopagnosia).
Archives of Neurology, 7, 24–32. Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of
visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9, 131–134.
Henderson, J. J. A., & Anglin, J. M. (2003). Facial attractiveness predicts
longevity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 351–356. Huart, J., Corneille, O., & Becquart, E. (2005). Face-based categorization,
Herrmann, M. J., Ehlis, A. C., Muehlberger, A., & Fallgatter, A. J. context-based categorization, and distortions in the recollection of gen-
(2005). Source localization of early stages of face processing. Brain der ambiguous faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41,
Topography, 18, 77–85. 598–608.

Herzmann, G., Schweinberger, S. R., Sommer, W., & Jentzsch, I. (2004). Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., &
What’s special about personally familiar faces? A multimodal approach. Rizzolatí, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science,
Psychophysiology, 41, 688–701. 286, 2526–2528.

Hietanen, J. K. (1999). Does your gaze direction and head orientation shift Ishai, A. (2007). Sex, beauty and the orbitofrontal cortex. International
my visual attention? NeuroReport, 10, 3443–3447. Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 181–185.

Hietanen, J. K. (2002). Social attention orienting integrates visual infor- Ishai, A., Schmidt, C. F., & Boesiger, P. (2005). Face perception is medi-
mation from head and body orientation. Psychological Research, 66, ated by a distributed cortical network. Brain Research Bulletin, 67,
174–179. 87–93.

Hill, H., Bruce, V., & Akamatsu, S. (1995). Perceiving the sex and race of Itier, R. J., Alain, C., Sedore, K., & McIntosh, A. R. (2007). Early face pro-
faces: The role of shape and colour. Proceedings of the Royal Society cessing specificity: It’s in the eyes! Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 261, 367–373. 19, 1815–1826.

Hill, S. E., & Flom, R. (2007). 18- & 24-month-olds’ discrimination of Itier, R. J., Latinus, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2006). Face, eye and object early
gender-consistent and inconsistent activities. Infant Behavior and processing: What is the face specificity? NeuroImage, 29, 667–676.
Development, 30, 168–173. Itier, R. J., & Taylor, M. J. (2004a). N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal dif-
Hill, H., Jinno, Y., & Johnston, A. (2003). Comparing solid-body with ferences between object and face processing using ERPs. Cerebral
point-light animations. Perception, 32, 561–566. Cortex, 14, 132–142.
Hill, H., & Johnston, A. (2001). Categorizing sex and identity from the Itier, R. J., & Taylor, M. J. (2004b). Source analysis of the N170 to faces
biological motion of faces. Current Biology, 11, 880–885. and objects. NeuroReport, 15, 1261–1265.
Hill, H., & Pollick, F. E. (2000). Exaggerating temporal differences Ito, T. A., Thompson, E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). Tracking the timecourse
enhances recognition of individuals from point light displays. of social perception: The effects of racial cues on event-related brain
Psychological Science, 11, 223–228. potentials. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1267–1280.
Hill, T., Lewicki, P., Czyzewska, M., & Schuller, G. (1990). The role of Jablonski, N. G., & Chaplin, G. (2000). The evolution of human skin color-
learned inferential encoding rules in the perception of faces: Effects ation. Journal of Human Evolution, 39, 57–106.
of nonconscious self-perpetuation of a bias. Journal of Experimental Jenkins, J., & Langton, S. R. H. (2003). Configural processing in the per-
Social Psychology, 26, 350–371. ception of eye-gaze direction. Perception, 32, 1181–1188.
Hirai, M., Fukushima, H., & Hiraki, K. (2003). An event-related poten- Johns, M., Cullum, J., Smith, T., & Freng, S. (2008). Internal motivation
tials study of biological motion perception in humans. Neuroscience to respond without prejudice and automatic egalitarian goal activation.
Letters, 344, 41–44. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1514–1519.
458 Perceiving People

Johnson, K. L., & Tassinary, L. G. (2005). Perceiving sex directly and Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. J. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind
indirectly. Meaning in motion and morphology. Psychological Science, and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework
2005, 16, 890–897. for stereotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129,
Johnson, K. L., & Tassinary, L. G. (2007). Compatibility of basic social 522–544.
perceptions determines perceived attractiveness. Proceedings of the Lammers, J., Gordijn, E. H., & Otten, S. (2008). Iron ladies, men of steel:
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, The effects of gender stereotyping on the perception of male and
5246–5251. female candidates are moderated by prototypicality. European Journal
Jokisch, D., Daum, I., Suchan, B., & Troje, N. F. (2005). Structural encoding of Social Psychology, DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.505.
and recognition of biological motion: Evidence from event-related poten- Lander, K., & Chuang, L. (2005). Why are moving faces easier to recog-
tials and source analysis. Behavioural Brain Research, 157, 195–204. nize? Visual Cognition, 12, 429–442.
Jokisch, D., Daum, I., & Troje, N. F. (2006). Self-recognition versus rec- Lander, K., Chuang, L., & Wickham, L. (2006). Recognizing face iden-
ognition of others by biological motion: Viewpoint-dependant effects. tity from natural and morphed smiles. The Quarterly Journal of
Perception, 35, 911–920. Experimental Psychology, 59, 801–808.
Jolicoeur, P., Gluck, M. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1984). Pictures and names: Lange, J., & Lappe, M. (2006). A model of biological motion percep-
Making the connection. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 243–275. tion from configural form cues. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26,
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Perrett, D. I., Little, A. C., Feinberg, D. R., & 2894–2906.
Law Smith, M. J. (2008). Effects of menstrual cycle phase on face pref- Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., &
erences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 78–84. Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and
Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2004). When facial theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390–423.
attractiveness is only skin deep. Perception, 33, 569–576. Langlois, J. H., Ritter, J. M., Casey, R. J., & Sawin, D. B. (1995). Infant
Jordon, H., Fallah, M., & Stoner, G. R. (2006). Adaptation of gender attractiveness predicts maternal behaviors and attitudes. Developmental
derived from biological motion. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 738–739. Psychology, 31, 464–472.
Judge, T. A., & Cable, D. M. (2004). The effect of physical height on Langton, S. R. H., Watt, R. J., & Bruce, V. (2000). Do the eyes have it?
workplace success and income: Preliminary test of a theoretical model. Cues to the direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 428–441. 4, 50–59.
Kable, J., & Chatterjee, A. (2006). Specificity of action representations LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of
in occipitotemporal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, emotional life. New York: Simon and Schuster.
1498–1517. Lennon, S. J., Johnson, K. K. P., & Schulz, T. L. (1999). Forging linkages
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face between dress and law in the U.S., part I: Rape and sexual harassment.
area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face per- Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 17, 144–156.
ception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–4311. Leveroni, C. L., Seidenberg, M., Mayer, A. R., Mead, L. A., Binder, J. R., &
Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: A cortical region Rao, S. M. (2000). Neural systems underlying the recognition of
specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of familiar and newly learned faces. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20,
the Royal Society B, 361, 2109–2128. 878–886.
Kenny, D. A., Horner, C., Kashy, D. A., & Chu, L. (1992). Consensus at Levin, D. T., & Banaji, R. (2006). Distortions in the perceived light-
zero acquaintance: Replication, behavioral cues, and stability. Journal ness of faces: The role of race categories. Journal of Experimental
of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 88–97. Psychology: General, 135, 501–512.
Kim, J. S., Yoon, H. W., Kim, B. S., Jeun, S. S., Jung, S. L., & Choe, B. J. Lin, M. H., Kwan, V. S. Y., Cheung, A., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Stereotype
(2006). Racial distinction of the unknown facial identity recogni- content model explains prejudice for an envied outgroup: Scale of
tion mechanism by event-related fMRI. Neuroscience Letters, 397, Anti-Asian American stereotypes. Personality and Social Psychology
279–284. Bulletin, 31, 34–47.
Knappmeyer, B., Thornton, I. M., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2003). The use of Lippmann, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace
facial motion and facial form during the processing of identity. Vision Jovanovitch.
Research, 43, 1921–1939. Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2006). What is good is beautiful:
Kniffin, K., & Wilson, D. S. (2004). The effect of non-physical traits on Face preference reflects desired personality. Personality and Individual
the perception of physical attractiveness: Three naturalistic studies. Differences, 41, 1107–1118.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 88–101. Liu, J., Harris, A., & Kanwisher, N. (2002). Stages of processing in face
Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker perception: An MEG study. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 910–916.
from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, Livingston, R. W., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). What are we really priming?
575–580. Cue-based versus category-based processing of facial stimuli. Journal
Kranz, F., & Ishai, A. (2006). Face perception is modulated by sexual pref- of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 5–18.
erence. Current Biology, 16, 63–68. Locher, P., Unger, R., Sociedade, P., & Wahl, J. (1993). At first glance:
Kress, T., & Daum, I. (2003). Event-related potentials reflect impaired face Accessibility of the physical attractiveness stereotype. Sex Roles, 28,
recognition in patients with congenital prosopagnosia. Neuroscience 729–743.
Letters, 352, 133–136. Locke, V., Macrae, C. N., & Eaton, J. L. (2005). Is person categorization
Kriegeskorte, N., Formisano, E., Sorger, B., & Goebel, R. (2007). modulated by exemplar typicality. Social Cognition, 23, 417–428.
Individual faces elicit distinct patterns in human anterior tempo- Lorteije, J. A. M., Kenemans, J. L., Jellema, T., van der Lubbe, R. H. J.,
ral cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, de Heer, F., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (2006). Delayed response to ani-
20600–20605. mate implied motion in human motion processing areas. Journal of
Kunda, Z., & Sinclair, L. (1999). Motivated reasoning with stereotypes: Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 158–168.
Activation, application, and inhibition. Psychological Inquiry, 10, MacLin, O. H., & Malpass, S. (2003). The ambiguous-race face illusion.
12–22. Perception, 32, 249–252.
References 459

Macrae, C. N., Alnwick, K. A., Milne, A.B., & Schloerscheidt, A. M. (2002). Michels, L., Lappe, M., & Vaina, L. M. (2005). Visual areas involved
Person perception across the menstrual cycle: Hormonal influences on in the perception of human movement from dynamic form analysis.
social-cognitive functioning. Psychological Science, 13, 532–536. NeuroReport, 16, 1037–1041.
Macrae, C. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: Thinking Molnar-Szakacs, I., Iacoboni, M, Koski, L., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2005).
categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 93–120. Functional segregation within pars opercularis of the inferior frontal
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Milne, A. B. (1995). The dissection gyrus: Evidence from fMRI studies of imitation and action observa-
of selection in person perception: Inhibitory processes in social stereo- tion. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 986–994.
typing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 397–407. Montepare, J., Koff, E., Zaitchik, D., & Albert, M. (1999). The use of body
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., Thorn, T. M. J., & movements and gestures as cues to emotions in younger and older
Castelli, L. (1997). On the activation of social stereotypes: The mod- adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 133–152.
erating role of processing objectives. Journal of Experimental Social Montepare, J. M., & Zebrowitz-McArthur, L. (1988). Impressions of people
Psychology, 33, 471–489. created by age-related qualities of their gaits. Journal of Personality
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Schloerscheidt, A. M., & Milne, A. B. and Social Psychology, 55, 547–556.
(1999). Tale of the unexpected: Effective function and person perception. Moreland, L. R., & Zajonc, B. R. (1982). Exposure effects in person per-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 200–213. ception: Familiarity, similarity, and attraction. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 18, 395–415.
Macrae, C. N., Hood, B. M., Milne, A. B., Rowe, A. C., & Mason, M. F.
(2002). Are you looking at me? Eye gaze and person perception. Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W.,
Psychological Science, 13, 460–464. Calder, A. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1996). A differential neural response in
the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature,
Macrae, C. N., & Martin, D. (2007). A boy primed Sue: Feature-based pro-
383, 812–815.
cessing and person construal. European Journal of Social Psychology,
37, 793–805. Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the
right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National
Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 1680–1685.
energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 37–47. Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-
process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98,
Macrae, C. N., Quinn, K. A., Mason, M. F., & Quadflieg, S. (2005).
164–181.
Understanding others: The face and person construal. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 686–695. Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., & Giard, M. H. (2003). Electrophysiological
correlates of age and gender perception on human faces. Journal of
Maddox, K. B. (2004). Perspectives on racial phenotypicality bias. Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 900–910.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 383–401.
Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M. H., Bentin, S., Aguera, P. E., & Pernier, J.
Maddox, K. B., & Gray, S. A. (2002). Cognitive representations of Black (2000). Neurophysiological correlates of face gender processing in
Americans: Reexploring the role of skin tone. Personality and Social humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 303–310.
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 250–259.
Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance in West Point cadets
Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: the judgment of previous occurrence. predicts military rank 20 years later. Social Forces, 74, 823–850.
Psychological Review, 87, 252–271.
Nelson, C. A. (2001). The development and neural bases of face recogni-
Martin, D., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). A face with a cue: Exploring the tion. Infant and Child Development, 10, 3–18.
inevitability of person categorization. European Journal of Social
Nestor, A., & Tarr, M. J. (2008). The segmental structure of faces and its
Psychology, 37, 806–816.
use in gender recognition. Journal of Vision, 8, 1–12.
Mason, M. F., Hood, B. M., & Macrae, C. N. (2004). Look into my eyes:
Ng, M., Ciaramitaro, V. M., Anstis, S., Boynton, G. M., & Fine, I. (2006).
Gaze direction and person memory. Memory, 12, 637–643.
Selectivity for the configural cues that identify the gender, ethnicity, and
Mason, M. F., & Macrae, C. N. (2004). Categorizing and individuating oth- identity of faces in human cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy
ers: The neural substrates of person perception. Journal of Cognitive of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 19552–19557.
Neuroscience, 16, 1785–1795. Niedenthal, P. M., Halberstadt, J. B., Margolin, J., & Innes-Ker, A. H.
Mason, M. F., Tatkow, E. P., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). The look of love: Gaze (2000). Emotional state and the detection of change in facial expression
shifts and person perception. Psychological Science, 16, 236–239. of emotion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 211–222.
Mathews, A., Fox, E., Yiend, J., & Calder, A. (2003). The face of fear: Norman, J. F., Payton, S. M., Long, J. R., & Hawkes, L. M. (2004). Aging and
Effects of eye gaze and emotion on visual attention. Visual Cognition, perception of biological motion. Psychology and Aging, 19, 219–225.
10, 823–835. Norman, W. T., & Goldberg, L. R. (1966). Raters, ratees, and randomness
Maurer, D., Le Grand, R., & Mondloch, C. (2002). The many faces of con- in personality structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
figural processing. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 255–260. 4, 681–691.
Mazur, A. (1986). U.S. trends in feminine beauty and overadaptation. The O’Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M., & Dolan, R. J.
Journal of Sex Research, 22, 281–303. (2003). Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in
McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Belger, A., & Allison, T. (1999). facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41, 147–155.
Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. II: Response O’Donnell, C., & Bruce, V. (2001). Familiarisation with faces selectively
properties of face-specific potentials generated in occipito-temporal enhances sensitivity to changes made to the eyes. Perception, 30, 755–764.
cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 431–444. Olson, I. R., & Marshuetz, C. (2005). Facial attractiveness is appraised in a
Meeren, H. K. M., van Heijnsbergen, C. C. R. J., & de Gelder, B. (2005). glance. Emotion, 5, 498–502.
Rapid perceptual integration of facial expression and emotional body Osborn, D. R. (1996). Beauty is as beauty does? Makeup and posture
language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the effects on physical attractiveness judgments. Journal of Applied Social
United States of America, 102, 16518–16523. Psychology, 26, 31–51.
Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigat- O’Toole, A. J., Roark, D. A., & Abdi, H. (2002). Recognizing mov-
ing the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. ing faces: A psychological and neural synthesis. Trends in Cognitive
Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3–35. Sciences, 6, 261–266.
460 Perceiving People

Overney, L. S., Michel, C. M., Harris, I. M., & Pegna, A. J. (2005). Cerebral Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact
processes in mental transformations of body parts: Recognition prior to in goal detection. Evidence from normal infants and children with
rotation. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 722–734. autism or mental handicap. Development and Psychopathology, 4,
Paunonen, S. V. (2006). You are honest, therefore I like you and find you 375–383.
attractive. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 237–249. Pichon, S., de Gelder, B., & Grèzes, J. (2007). Emotional modulation of
Peelen, M. V., Atkinson, A. P., Andersson, F., & Vuilleumier, P. (2007). visual and motor areas by dynamic body expressions of anger. Social
Emotional modulation of body-selective visual areas. Social Cognitive Neurosciences. EPub May 2007.
and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 274–283. Pitcher, D., Walsh, V., Yovel, G., & Duchaine, B. (2007). TMS evidence for
the involvement of the right occipital face area in early face processing.
Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2005a). Within-subject reproducibility of
Current Biology, 17, 1568–1573.
category-specific visual activation with functional MRI. Human Brain
Mapping, 25, 402–408. Pobric, G., & Hamilton, A. F. (2006). Action understanding requires the
left inferior frontal cortex. Current Biology, 16, 524–529.
Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2005b) Selectivity for the human body in
the fusiform gyrus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 93, 603–608. Pollick, F. E., Kay, J. W., Heim, K., & Stringer, R. (2005). Gender recogni-
tion from point-light walkers. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2007). The neural basis of visual body
Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1247–1265.
perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 636–648.
Pollick, F. E., Lestou, V., Ryu, J., & Cho, S.-B. (2002). Estimating the effi-
Peelen, M. V., Wiggett, A. J., & Downing, P. E. (2006). Patterns of fMRI ciency of recognizing gender and affect from biological motion. Vision
activity dissociate overlapping functional brain areas that respond to Research, 42, 2345–2355.
biological motion. Neuron, 49, 815–822.
Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001).
Pegna, A. J., Khateb, A., Lazeyras, F., & Seghier, M. L. (2005). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51–B61.
Discriminating emotional faces without primary visual cortices
Posamentier, M. T., & Abdi, H. (2003). Processing faces and facial expres-
involves the right amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 24–25.
sions. Neuropsychology Review, 13, 113–143.
Pegna, A. J., Khateb, A., Michel, C. M., & Landis, T. (2004). Visual recog-
Poulin-Dubois, D., Serbin, L. A., Eichstedt, J. A., Sen, M. G., & Beissel, C. F.
nition of faces, objects, and words using degraded stimuli: Where and
(2002). Men don’t put on make-up: Toddlers’ knowledge of the gen-
when it occurs. Human Brain Mapping, 22, 300–311.
der stereotyping of household activities. Social Development, 11,
Pelphrey, K. A., Mitchell, T. V., McKeown, M. J., Goldstein, J., Allison, T., & 166–181.
McCarthy, G. (2003). Brain activity evoked by the perception of human
Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004).
walking: controlling for meaningful coherent motion. The Journal of
Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fear-
Neuroscience, 23, 6819–6825.
ful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 619–633.
Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., Michelich, C. R., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. Pourtois, G., Peelen, M., Spinelli, L., Seeck, M., & Vuilleumier, P. (2007).
(2005). Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in poste- Direct intracranial recording of body-selective responses in human
rior temporal cortex: An fMRI study of eye, mouth and hand move- extrastriate visual cortex. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2621–2625.
ments. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1866–1876.
Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1998).
Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2003). Temporal cortex activation of humans viewing eye and mouth move-
Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: the influence of ments. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 2188–2199.
context. Neuropsychologia, 41, 156–170.
Puce, A., & Perrett, D. (2003). Electrophysiology and brain imaging of
Pendry, L. F., & Macrae, C. N. (1994). Stereotypes and mental life: The biological motion. Philosophical Transactions Royal Society London.
case of the motivated but thwarted tactician. Journal of Experimental Biological Sciences, 358, 435–445.
Social Psychology, 30, 303–325.
Puce, A., Smith, A., & Allison, T. (2000). ERPs evoked by viewing facial
Perdue, C. W., & Gurtman, M. B. (1990). Evidence for the automaticity of movements. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 221–239.
ageism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 199–216.
Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2001). Bias, discrimination, and obesity.
Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., Lee, K. J., Rowland, D. A., & Obesity Research, 9, 788–805.
Edwards, R. (1999). Symmetry and human facial attractiveness. Puhl, R. M., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Schwartz, M. B., & Brownell, K. D.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 295–307. (2008). Weight stigmatization and bias reduction: Perspectives of over-
Perrett, D. I., Rolls, E. T., & Caan, W. (1982). Visual neurones responsive weight and obese adults. Health Education Research, 23, 347–358.
to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, Pyles, J. A., Garcia, J. O., Hoffman, D. D., & Grossman, E. D. (2007).
47, 329–342. Visual perception and neural correlates of novel “biological motion.”
Peters, M., Rhodes, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2007). Contributions of the face Vision Research, 47, 2786–2797.
and body to overall attractiveness. Animal Behavior, 73, 937–942. Quinn, K. A., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). Categorizing others: The dynamics
Peuskens, H., Vanrie, J., Verfaillie, K., & Orban, G. A. (2005). Specificity of person construal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
of regions processing biological motion. European Journal of 88, 467–479.
Neuroscience, 21, 2864–2875. Ramsey, J. L., Langlois, J. H., Hoss, R. A., Rubenstein, A. J., & Griffin, A. M.
Phelps, E., Cannistraci, C., & Cunningham, W. (2003). Intact performance (2004). Origins of a stereotype: Categorization of facial attractiveness
on an indirect measure of race bias following amygdala damage. by 6-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 7, 201–211.
Neuropsychologia, 41, 203–208. Reed, C. L., Stone, V., Bozova, S., & Tanaka, J. (2003). The body inversion
Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., effect. Psychological Science, 14, 302–308.
Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Performance on Reed, C. L., Stone, V. E., Grubb, J. D., & McGoldrick, J. E. (2006).
indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Turning configural processing upside down: Part and whole body pos-
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738. tures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., Performance, 32, 73–87.
Calder, A. J., et al. (1997). A specific neural substrate for perceiving Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual
facial expressions of disgust. Nature, 389, 495–498. Reviews of Psychology, 57, 199–226.
References 461

Rhodes, G., Proffitt, F., Grady, J. M., & Sumich, A. (1998). Facial Saxe, R., Xiao, D. K., Kovacs, G., Perrett, D. I., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). A
symmetry and the perception of beauty. Psychonomic Bulletin and region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed
Review, 5, 659–69. intentional actions. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1435–1446.
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Saygin, A. P. (2007). Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary
Reviews of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. for biological motion perception. Brain, 130, 2452–2461.
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mech- Saygin, A. P., Wilson, S. M., Hagler, D. J., Bates, E., & Sereno, M. I.
anisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature (2004). Point-light biological motion perception activates human pre-
Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–670. motor cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 6181–6188.
Roberts, J. V., & Herman, C. P. (1986). The psychology of height: An Schacht, A., Werheid, K., & Sommer, W. (2008). The appraisal of
empirical review. In C. P. Herman, M. P. Zanna, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), facial beauty is rapid but not mandatory. Cognitive, Affective, and
Physical appearance, stigma, and social behavior (pp. 113–140). Behavioural Neuroscience, 8, 132–142.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schuermann, M., Hesse, M. D., Stephan, K. E., Saarela, M., Zilles, K.,
Roberts, T., & Bruce, V. (1988). Feature saliency in judging the sex and Hari, R., & Fink, G. R. (2005). Yearning to yawn: The neural basis of
familiarity of faces. Perception, 17, 829–840. contagious yawning. NeuroImage, 24, 1260–1264.
Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, Schwarzlose, R. F., Baker, C. I., & Kanwisher, N. (2005). Separate face and
10, 284–294. body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. The Journal of Neuroscience,
Ronquillo, J., Denson, D. F., Lickel, B., Lu, Z.-L., Nandy, A., & Maddox, K.B. 25, 11055–11059.
(2007). The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity: An Schwarzlose, R.F., Swisher, J.D., Dang, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2008). The
fMRI investigation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, distribution of category and location information across object-selective
39–44. regions of visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Rossion, B. (2002). Is sex categorisation from faces really parallel to face Scienes, 105, 4447–4452.
recognition? Visual Cognition, 9, 1003–1020.
Schweinberger, S. R., Huddy, V., & Burton, A. M. (2004). N250r: A face-
Rossion, B. (2008). Constraining the cortical face network by neuroimag- selective brain response to stimulus repetitions. NeuroReport, 15,
ing studies of acquired prosopagnosia. NeuroImage, 40, 423–426. 1501–1505.
Rossion, B., & Gauthier, I. (2002). How does the brain process upright Schyns, P. G., Bonnar, L., & Gosselin, F. (2002). Show me the fea-
and inverted faces? Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, tures! Understanding recognition from the use of visual information.
1, 62–74. Psychological Science, 13, 402–409.
Rossion, B., & Jacques, C. (2008). Does physical interstimulus variance Secord, P. F., & Bevan, W. (1956). Personalities in faces: III. A cross cul-
account for early electrophysiological face sensitive responses in the tural comparison of impressions of physiognomy and personality in
human brain? Ten lessons on the N170. NeuroImage, 39, 1959–1979. faces. Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 283–288.
Rossion, B., Schiltz, C., Robaye, L., Pirenne, D., & Crommelinck, M. Secord, P. F., Dukes, W. F., & Bevan, W. (1954). Personalities in faces: I.
(2001). How does the brain discriminate familiar and unfamiliar faces: An experiment in social perceiving. Genetic Psychology Monographs,
A PET study of face categorical perception. Journal of Cognitive 49, 231–279.
Neuroscience, 13, 1019–1034.
Senju, A., & Hasegawa, T. (2005). Direct gaze captures visuospatial atten-
Rudman, L. A. (2005). Rejection of women? Beyond prejudice as antipa-
tion. Visual Cognition, 12, 127–144.
thy. In J. F. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. A. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of
prejudice (pp. 106–120). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Simion, F., Leo, I., Turati, C., Valenza, E., & Dalla Barba, B. (2007). How
face specialization emerges in the first months of life. Progress in
Ruffman, T., Henry, J. D., Livingstone, V., & Phillips, L. H. (2008). A meta-
Brain Research, 164, 169–185.
analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: Implications for
neuropsychological models of aging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of female physical attractive-
Reviews, 32, 863–881. ness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 65, 293–307.
Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2008). The face of success. Inferences
from chief executive officers’ appearance predict company profits. Singh, D., & Young, R. K. (1995). Body weight, waist-to-hip ratio, breasts
Psychological Science, 19, 109–111. and hips: Role in judgments of female attractiveness and desirability
Russell, R. (2003). Sex, beauty, and the relative luminance of facial fea- for relationships. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 483–507.
tures. Perception, 32, 1093–1107. Slater, A., Bremner, G., Johnson, S. P., Sherwood, P., Hayes, R., & Brown, E.
Sadr, J., Jarudi, I., & Sinha, P. (2003). The role of eyebrows in face recog- (2000). Newborn infants’ preference for attractive faces: The role of
nition. Perception, 32, 285–293. internal and external facial features. Infancy, 1, 265–274.
Sagiv, N., & Bentin, S. (2001). Structural encoding of human and sche- Smith, E. R., Miller, D. A., Maitner, A. T., Crump, S. A., Garcia-Marques, T., &
matic faces: Holistic and part-based processes. Journal of Cognitive Mackie, D. M. (2006). Familiarity can increase stereotyping. Journal
Neuroscience, 13, 937–951. of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 471–478.
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2006). Spiridon, M., Fischl, B., & Kanwisher, N. (2006). Location and spatial pro-
Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial file of category-specific regions in human extrastriate cortex. Human
expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Brain Mapping, 27, 77–89.
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470–480. Sprengelmeyer, R., & Jentzsch, I. (2006). Event related potentials and the
Santos, I. M., & Young, A. W. (2005). Exploring the perception of social perception of intensity in facial expressions. Neuropsychologia, 44,
characteristics in faces using the isolation effect. Visual Cognition, 12, 2899–2906.
213–247. Stevenage, S. V., Nixon, M. S., & Vince, K. (1999). Visual analysis of gait
Santos, I. M., & Young, A. W. (2008). Effects of inversion and negation on as a cue to identity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 513–526.
social inferences from faces. Perception, 37, 1061–1078. Sugiura, M., Kawashima, R., Nakamura, K., Sato, N., Nakamura, A.,
Saxe, R., Jamal, N., & Powell, L. (2006). My body or your body? The Kato, T., et al. (2001). Activation reduction in anterior temporal cor-
effect of visual perspective on cortical body representations. Cerebral tices during repeated recognition of faces of personal acquaintances.
Cortex, 16, 178–182. NeuroImage, 13, 877–890.
462 Perceiving People

Tanaka, J. W. (2001). The entry point of face recognition: evidence for van Heijnsbergen, C. C. R. J., Meeren, H. K. M., Grèzes, J., & de Gelder, B.
face expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 130, (2007). Rapid detection of fear in body expressions, an ERP study.
534–543. Brain Research, 1186, 233–241.
Tanaka, J. W., Curran, T., Porterfield, A. L., & Collins, D. (2006). Activation van Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Social categorization and
of preexisting and acquired face representations: The N250 event- stereotyping: A functional perspective. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone
related potential as an index of face familiarity. Journal of Cognitive (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology, 11, (pp. 105–144).
Neuroscience, 18, 1488–1497. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Tarr, M. J., & Cheng, Y. D. (2003). Learning to see faces and objects. van Leeuwen, M. L., & Macrae, C. N. (2004). Is beautiful always good?
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 23–30. Implicit benefits of facial attractiveness. Social Cognition, 22,
Tarr, M. J., & Gauthier, I. (2000). FFA: A flexible fusiform area for 637–649.
subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise. Nature Vecera, S. P., & Johnson, M. H. (1995). Gaze detection and the cortical pro-
Neuroscience, 3, 764–769. cessing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults. Visual Cognition,
Taylor, J. C., Wiggett, A. J., & Downing, P. E. (2007). fMRI analysis of 2, 59–87.
body and body part representations in the extrastriate and fusiform body von Grünau, M., & Anston, C. (1995). The detection of gaze direction: A
areas. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98, 1626–1633. stare-in-the-crowd effect. Perception, 24, 1297–1313.
Thierry, G., Pegna, A. J., Dodds, C., Roberts, M., Basan, S., & Downing, P. Vuilleumier, P., & Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain
(2006). An event-related potential component sensitive to images of mechanisms during emotion face perception: Evidence from functional
the human body. NeuroImage, 32, 871–879. neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia, 45, 174–194.
Thompson, J. C., Hardee, J. E., Panayiotou, A., Crewther, D., & Puce, A. Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J.
(2007). Common and distinct brain activation to viewing dynamic (2004). Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical acti-
sequences of face and hand movements. NeuroImage, 37, 966–973. vation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7,
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends in 1271–1278.
Cognitive Sciences, 3, 452–460.
Wachsmuth, E., Oram, M. W., & Perrett, D. I. (1994). Recognition of
Thornhill, R., & Møller, A. P. (1997). Developmental stability, disease objects and their component parts: responses of single units in the tem-
and medicine. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical poral cortex of the macaque. Cerebral Cortex, 5, 509–522.
Society, 72, 497–548.
Wallbott, H. G. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal
Todorov, A., Baron, S. G., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Evaluating face trust- of Social Psychology, 28, 879–896.
worthiness: A model based approach. Social Cognitive and Affective
Weeden, J., & Sabini, J. (2005). Physical attractiveness and health in
Neuroscience, 3, 119–127.
Western societies: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 635–653.
Todorov, A., & Duchaine, B. (2008). Reading trustworthiness in faces
without recognizing faces. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 395–410. Welling, L. L. M., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Smith, F. G., Feinberg,
D. R., Little, A. C., & Al-Dujaili, E. A. S. (2008). Men report stronger
Todorov, A., Gobbini, M. I., Evans, K. K., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). attraction to femininity in women’s faces when their testosterone levels
Spontaneous retrieval of affective person knowledge in face percep- are high. Hormones & Behavior, 54, 703–708.
tion. Neuropsychologia, 45, 163–173.
Werheid, K., Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2007). Facial attractiveness
Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences
modulates early and late event-related brain potentials. Biological
of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308,
Psychology, 76, 100–108.
1623–1626.
Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., &
Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1985). Knowledge without awareness: An
Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expres-
autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. Science, 228,
sions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. The
1453–1454.
Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418.
Troje, N. F., Westhoff, C., & Lavrov, M. (2005). Person identification from
Wheaton, K. J., Thompson, J. C., Syngeniotis, A., Abbott, D. F., & Puce, A.
biological motion: Effects of structural and kinematic cues. Perception &
(2004).Viewing the motion of human body parts activates different
Psychophysics, 67, 667–675.
regions of premotor, temporal, and parietal cortex. NeuroImage, 22,
Turk, D. J., Rosenblum, A. C., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). 277–288.
Seeing John Malkovich: The neural substrates of person categoriza-
tion. NeuroImage, 24, 1147–1153. Wheeler, M. E., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Controlling racial prejudice.
Psychological Science, 16, 56–62.
Uleman, J. S., Blader, S. L., & Todorov, A. (2005). Implicit impressions. In
R. R. Hassin, J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious White, G. L., & Shapiro, D. (1987). Don’t I know you? Antecedents and
(pp. 362–392). New York: Oxford University Press. social consequences of perceived familiarity. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 23, 75–92.
Urgesi, C., Berlucchi, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2004). Magnetic stimulation
of extrastriate body area impairs visual processing of nonfacial body Wicker, B., Michel, F., Henaff, M.-A., & Decety, J. (1998). Brain regions
parts. Current Biology, 14, 2130–2134. involved in the perception of gaze: A PET study. NeuroImage, 8,
221–227.
Urgesi, C., Calvo-Merino, B., Haggard, P., & Aglioti, S. M. (2007).
Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals two cortical pathways for Williams, L. M., Palmer, D., Liddell, B. J., Song, L., & Gordon, E. (2006).
visual body processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 8023–8030. The “when” and “where” of perceiving signals of threat versus non-
Urgesi, C., Candidi, M., Ionta, S., Aglioti, S. M. (2007). Representation threat. NeuroImage, 31, 458–467.
of body identity and body actions in extrastriate body area and ventral Willis, M. S., Esqueda, C. W., & Schacht, R. N. (2008). Social perceptions
premotor cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 30–31. of individuals missing upper front teeth. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
Valentine, T. (1988). Upside-down faces: A review of the effect of inver- 106, 423–435.
sion on face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 471–491. Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind
Van den Stock, J., Righart, R., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Body expressions after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17, 592–598.
influence recognition of emotions in the face and voice. Emotion, 7, Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the
487–494. face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits
References 463

positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, Yovel, G., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Face perception: Domain specific, not
989–1000. process specific. Neuron, 44, 747–748.
Winston, J. S., O’Doherty, J., Kilner, J. M., Perrett, D. I., & Dolan, R. J. Yovel, G., & Kanwisher, N. (2005). The neural basis of the behavioral
(2007). Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. face-inversion effect. Current Biology, 15, 2256–2262.
Neuropyschologia, 45, 195–206. Yovel, G., & Paller, K. A. (2004). The neural basis of the butcher-on-the-
Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). bus phenomenon: When a face seems familiar but is not remembered.
Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trust- NeuroImage, 21, 789–800.
worthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 277–283. Zebrowitz, L. A. (1997). Reading faces: Window to the soul? Boulder,
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (2001). Spontaneous prejudice CO: Westview Press.
in context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Zebrowitz, L. A. (2006). Finally, faces find favor. Social Cognition, 24,
Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 815–827. 657–701.
Wright, C. I., Negreira, A., Gold, A. L., Britton, J. C., Williams, D., & Zebrowitz, L. A., & McDonald, S. M. (1991). The impact of litigants’
Feldman Barrett, L. (2008). Neural correlates of novelty and face–age babyfacedness and attractiveness on adjudications in small claims
effects in young and elderly adults. NeuroImage, 42, 956–968. courts. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 603–623.
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (1992). Impressions of babyfaced
Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental
males and females across the lifespan. Developmental Psychology, 28,
Psychology, 81, 141–145.
1143–1152.
Young, T. J., & French. L. A. (1998). Heights of U.S. presidents: A trend
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (2000). “Too young, too old”:
analysis for 1948–1996. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 321–322.
Stigmatizing adolescents and elders. In T. K. Heatherton, R. E.
Young, A. W., Hay, D. C., McWeeny, K. H., Flude, B. M., & Ellis, A. W. Kleck, M. R. Hebl, & J. G. Hull (Eds.), The psychology of stigma
(1985). Matching familiar and unfamiliar faces on internal and external (pp. 334–373). New York: Guilford Press.
features. Perception, 14, 737–746. Zebrowitz, L. A., Montepare, J. M., & Lee, H. K. (1993). They don’t all
Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., & Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational infor- look alike: Individuated impressions of other racial groups. Journal of
mation in face perception. Perception, 16, 747–759. Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 85–101.
Chapter 13

Nonverbal Behavior
NALINI AMBADY AND MAX WEISBUCH

In a much-cited essay, the experimental psychologist and natural selection, over the course of a single lifetime via
vision scientist Nicholas Humphrey (1976) argued that socialization, or over the course of minutes in moving from
our success as a species is due to our social intellect and one social interaction to another, social adaptation is heav-
that our brain and intellect have evolved for social process- ily grounded in nonverbal behavior.
ing. Our success and survival, according to Humphrey, are Evidence for the role of nonverbal behavior in social
due not to our technology or ability to create and use tools adaptation comes from several sources. While humans do not
but rather to our ability to function as social beings. He develop linguistic capabilities until well into their first year
argued that the function of our superior intellect is to create of life, at birth humans respond to and produce nonverbal
and preserve social bonds and community: Indeed, there is behavior. For instance, newborns are able to imitate certain
increasing acknowledgment across disciplines that human facial expressions (Meltzoff & Moore, 1983; Sagi &
cognition evolved, in part, to cope with increasing social Hoffman, 1976). Shortly after birth and well before lan-
demands (Cosmides & Tooby, 1992; Dunbar, 1998; Taylor guage development, infants learn to produce and interpret
et al., 2000). a range of psychologically meaningful nonverbal behav-
In this chapter, we argue that the ability to communicate iors (Walker-Andrews, 2008). After the development of
nonverbally is at the core of this social intellect. The social language, nonverbal sensitivity only increases such that
intellect involves the abilities to infer others’ motives, inten- following infancy children continue to be socialized via
tions, character traits, and emotions and, in turn, to commu- the nonverbal behavior of caregivers and peers (Feinman,
nicate one’s own motives, intentions, character traits, and 1982; Walden & Ogan, 1988). Not only does nonverbal
emotions. Underlying these specific abilities is the broad, behavior serve as the starting point for social cognition in
generalized ability to understand nonverbal behavior and humans, but it also is the communicative means by which
to use nonverbal behavior to communicate. we are connected to our evolutionary ancestors. Across
Nonverbal behavior and communication are hence foun- the animal kingdom but especially in the primates, the
dational to social perception, cognition, interaction, and importance of nonverbal behavior to social life cannot be
behavior. Beyond evolutionary adaptations to social life underestimated—many scholars regard nonverbal commu-
that occur on the scale of millions of years, humans exhibit nication as the social glue that supports primate affiliation
the extraordinary ability to adapt to their social context over and alliances (Hauser, 1996; Preston & de Waal, 2002).
much shorter timescales; this adaptation is also undergirded The ontogenetic and evolutionary primacy for the role
by nonverbal behavior. Such short-term social adaptiveness of nonverbal behavior in social cognition suggests that the
lies at the heart of many influential social-psychological production and perception of nonverbal behavior should
theories, including theories of affiliation (Baumeister & occur early in cognitive processing, even among adults.
Leary, 1995), theories of social influence (Deutsch & The machinery is certainly there to allow for such early
Gerard, 1955; Heider, 1958), and theories of prejudice and processing. For example, as compared with semantic
ingroup bias (Dovidio & Gaertner; Yzerbyt & Demoulin, (verbal) stimuli, image-based stimuli are more efficiently
volume 2), to name a few. Although spoken language can and enduringly matched to conceptual categories (Paivio,
be deployed to solve some problems of social complexity, 1971; Paivio & Csapo, 1973; Seifert, 1997). Recent models
nonverbal communication is the speediest, most effortless, of cognition suggest that much thought occurs via per-
and historically developed means for adapting to the social ceptual rather than semantic representations (Barsalou,
world. Whether occurring over millions of years through 1999). And the production of nonverbal behavior stands

464
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Nonverbal Production 465

in contrast to the production of verbal behavior in terms warmth, dominance, or immediacy. Both levels of nonverbal
of the consumption of cognitive resources: Whereas pro- behavior are reviewed in this chapter, but more emphasis is
ducing verbal behavior by speaking or writing is resource placed on macro-level behavior. This emphasis reflects the
consuming, stopping nonverbal behavior is resource con- literature and the idea that macro-level behaviors are often
suming (Richards & Gross, 2000; Richeson & Shelton, more likely to exhibit validity and to generalize across peo-
2003). The ontogenetic, evolutionary, and cognitive- ple, cultures, and time (Ambady, Bernieri, & Richeson, 2000;
processing primacy of nonverbal behavior has important Weisbuch, Slepian, Clarke, Ambady, & Veenstra-Vander
consequences for social psychology, many of which are Weele, in press; Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997).
reviewed in this chapter. In this chapter, we first discuss the production of
nonverbal behavior, including theories regarding non-
verbal behavior. We address several issues, includ-
Nonverbal Behavior
ing cross-species displays of nonverbal behavior, the
The purpose of this chapter is to provide a thorough automaticity and control of nonverbal expression, and
review of the existing literature on nonverbal behavior and the role of contextual and cultural cues in the production of
nonverbal communication. In doing so, it is important to nonverbal behavior. We also consider nonverbal behavior
define and clarify several constructs. in infancy and the role of individual differences in nonver-
Nonverbal behavior has been defined broadly as any bal production. Finally, we suggest that nonverbal behav-
behavior that is not linguistic (DePaulo & Friedman, ior undergirds basic cognitive processing and thinking.
1998). However, this broad definition includes activities We then turn our attention to the perception of nonverbal
such as lifting weights, driving a car, and using a hammer, behavior, focusing on automaticity and control in nonver-
and these activities are normally not what social psychol- bal perception. We examine how nonverbal perceptiveness
ogists mean when they use the term “nonverbal behavior.” might reflect basic social intelligence. We conclude by
The broad definition also runs the risk of including just describing the critical role that nonverbal behavior plays in
about any behavior that can be conceived, which in turn social influence.
creates a concept that may not be useful. Instead, nonver-
bal behavior is here defined as perceptible non-linguistic
behavior that is not instrumental to manipulating the
NONVERBAL PRODUCTION
physical state of the world. By excluding the manipula-
tion of nonsocial objects, our definition of nonverbal
From impression management and leadership to socializa-
behavior includes the subtle facial expressions, body lan-
tion and culture, the production of nonverbal behavior is
guage, social touching, vocal acoustics, and interpersonal
the foundation for many social processes. Although this
distance that are normally intended by the term within
idea may be counterintuitive for a species in which lan-
psychology. Nonverbal communication refers to the send-
guage is vital for survival and for scholars accustomed
ing and receiving of thoughts and feelings via nonverbal
to communicating research findings in words, nonverbal
behavior.
communication is the only means of social organization in
Traditionally, nonverbal behavior research has been
other species and is the primary way that preverbal infants
divided into encoding (the production and communication
exert social influence. Behavior that serves as the architec-
of nonverbal behavior) and decoding (the recognition and
ture of social life for our ancestral cousins and for our own
interpretation of nonverbal behavior). Because the terms
early development might reasonably underlie many social-
“encoding” and “decoding” suggest deliberate action on
psychological processes.
the part of the encoder and the decoder and because recent
work points to the automaticity of these processes, we use
the terms nonverbal production for encoding and nonver- Theories on the Production of Nonverbal
bal perception for decoding. Behavior
Another traditional distinction within the literature
Theories of Nonverbal Leakage
on nonverbal behavior is that between micro-level and
macro-level nonverbal behaviors. Micro-level nonverbal Perhaps the most prominent theory of nonverbal produc-
behaviors are individual behaviors, sometimes referred to tion, initially offered by Charles Darwin, resembles what
as “cues” such as smiles, eyebrow raises, forward leans, many people today probably think: Nonverbal behavior
and finger tapping. Macro-level nonverbal behaviors gen- reveals emotion. Indeed, although the history of theories on
erally refer to constellations of behavior that are imbued nonverbal communication dates back at least to Confucius
with broader psychological meaning, such as displays of (Knapp, 2006), most modern theories are roughly built on
466 Nonverbal Behavior

evolutionary principles, so it seems appropriate to start nonverbal influence theories suggest that all forms of non-
with Darwin. In his book The Expression of the Emotions in verbal communication, including emotional expression,
Man and Animals, Darwin (1872) describes the many sim- primarily support the interests of the expresser. Nonverbal
ilarities (and differences) between nonverbal expressions expressions, whether facial, vocal, or otherwise, are said
in humans and those in other animals. One important pos- to be signals that influence conspecifics’ actions. This
tulate in this book was that biological mechanisms associ- influence can occur either because the nonverbal display
ated with emotions directly influence nonverbal behavior. signals something about the upcoming behavior of the
For example, Darwin writes, “terror causes the body to expresser or because the display itself has direct affec-
tremble. The skin becomes pale, sweat breaks out, and tive consequences for perceivers. Signals that do not
the hair bristles” (p. 90). Paul Ekman, Sylvan Tomkins, influence others’ behavior cannot contribute to survival
and Carroll Izard were heavily influenced by Darwin’s and/or reproduction; hence, these signals are not selected.
approach and argued that subcortical brain structures asso- For example, threatening facial and vocal expressions
ciated with emotion directly caused particular patterns of would not have remained in existence if these did not pro-
facial expression (e.g., Ekman & Friesen, 1969a). By this voke appeasement in conspecifics (Fridlund, 1994; Owren,
view, specific facial expressions are directly caused by spe- Rendall, & Bachorowski, 2005). By the same token, only
cific emotions, and this relationship is universal. Hence, to the extent that threatening displays reliably predicted a
the same facial expression should index the same emotions powerful attack should perceivers have evolved appease-
across cultures. ment to the display. Consequently, an evolutionary arms
Unlike Darwin, who simply sent questionnaires to col- race emerges between expressers and perceivers. On the
leagues around the world (“do people in your area raise one hand, perceivers must learn to discriminate between
their eyebrows in fear?”), Ekman and his colleagues trav- reliable and unreliable nonverbal predictors of behavior.
eled to different cultures, asked individuals in these cul- On the other hand, expressers benefit if the display gains
tures to pose emotion expressions, and asked individuals in what they want even (and perhaps especially) when it does
still other cultures to identify the emotions. For example, not predict their behavior. Thus, as perceivers are increas-
they traveled to Papua, New Guinea, to visit a group of ingly able to discriminate between reliable and unreliable
people who had never been exposed to other cultures. signals (over evolutionary time), expressers should alter
Ekman (1971) provided some of these individuals with these signals.
scenarios (e.g., a dead pig on the ground) and videotaped Models of nonverbal influence do not stipulate a one-
them as they expressed the emotion they would feel in that to-one relationship between internal states and nonverbal
circumstance. The facial expressions in these videotapes behavior. In fact, Fridlund’s (1994) influential model sug-
were remarkably similar to American emotion expressions, gests that it would be detrimental to an expresser if others
and Americans were able to correctly identify the emo- could always “see” the expresser’s internal state (see also
tion in these expressions. Likewise, the tribesmen in New Hauser, 1996). More conservatively, Owren and colleagues
Guinea were able to identify the emotions of Americans. (2005) argue that a relationship between internal state and
A recent meta-analysis examined cross-cultural facial expression will exist at times, if only because an internal
expressions in 162 samples, with picture sets and rat- goal prompts the influence attempt in the first place.
ers from a great number of countries ranging from New Thus, according to one model, nonverbal behavior func-
Guinea to Malaysia to Germany to Ethiopia. Supportive of tions to reveal, and according to another model, nonver-
universality in facial emotion, in only 3% of these cross- bal behavior functions to influence. Both models are built
cultural samples was even a single emotion recognized at on Darwinian principles, and both posit that nonverbal
rates below chance (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). These behavior is a powerful tool for understanding human social
findings are consistent with the broader theory—that evolution. Hence, a good place to begin to understand the
specific emotions directly cause corresponding and hard- utility and ubiquity of nonverbal behavior is with a com-
wired facial expressions. parison of humans with our primate relatives.

Theories of Nonverbal Influence


Nonverbal Cues in Humans and Other Species
The second major perspective regarding the produc-
tion of nonverbal behavior is not restricted to emotion All primates (including humans) share certain needs,
but extends to all aspects of social influence and com- and all primates are social creatures, surviving in
munication. Although this perspective and the nonverbal groups. Moreover, human brain structures bear consider-
leakage perspective share the premise that evolutionary able similarity to those of other primates, particularly in
forces guide the production of nonverbal behavior, only the subcortical areas. These primitive brain areas play an
Nonverbal Production 467

important role in human and nonhuman behavior alike. In meta-analysis of 74 studies, the nonverbal behaviors
this section, the production of nonverbal behavior is exam- exhibiting a meaningful relationship with verticality
ined with respect to similarities and differences between were increased facial expressiveness, postural expansion,
humans and other creatures. Specifically, human displays decreased interpersonal distance from other people, and
of dominance and affiliation are compared with those of louder voice. Note that postural expansion (making one-
other primate species. self appear larger), decreased interpersonal distance, and
louder voice are also dominance cues among apes. In other
Social Status and Dominance domains, cross-species similarity is modulated by the
Although there are far too many nuances in social behavior emergence of language. One illustrative example regards
to provide a complete description with only two factors, eye gaze. Submissive nonhuman primates hold gaze less
status (or potency) and affiliation (or warmth) consistently than dominant creatures. Among humans the relationship
emerge as the two underlying factors in studies of social is more complex: Submissive humans hold gaze less while
perception (Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007; Rosenthal, Hall, speaking than while listening, whereas dominant people
DiMatteo, Rogers, & Archer, 1979). So it is no coincidence do not (Exline, Ellyson, & Long, 1975).
that nonverbal behavior plays an important role in vertical Despite the existence of some apparent overlap between
(status) and horizontal (affiliation) social relations across humans and other primates with respect to nonverbal dom-
species. inance displays, it is an oversimplification to claim that
The role of nonverbal dominance is so important among all primates express dominance similarly. For example,
some primate species that it at times eclipses real physical whereas macaques display submissiveness with a stereo-
dominance in demarking social structure. Consequently, typic facial expression that includes bared teeth (domi-
leading primatologists make use of the terms real domi- nant macaques never display this face), this same facial
nance to describe when one ape physically dominates expression is not always associated with submissiveness in
another (as in a fight) and formal dominance to describe chimps. Likewise, the relationship between facial expres-
the nonverbal rituals associated with dominant and sub- siveness and dominance appears to be unique to humans.
missive animals (de Waal, 1982). Normal vertical inter- Nonetheless, striking similarities appear between human
actions among chimpanzees always illustrate formal dominance displays and dominance displays of other pri-
dominance but sometimes do not illustrate real dominance. mate species, as highlighted with respect to postural expan-
Formal dominance may be observed when chimps greet sion, interpersonal distance, and vocal volume.
one another, especially after a conflict. In these interac- What functions might explain the ubiquity of domi-
tions, lower-status chimps repeatedly bow and emit a nance displays? Many scholars have argued that dominance
sound called a “pant-grunt” while timidly looking up at the displays reduce the need for actual physical aggression
more dominant ape. At the same time, the higher-status ape or more destructive displays of dominance. For example,
makes itself look bigger by stretching and standing its hair Nelson (1984) observed more than 600 encounters in which
on end (Tomasello & Call, 1997). an intruding pigeon attempted to take the territory of a male
Across primates, making oneself look bigger consis- pigeon—in not one of these instances did Nelson observe
tently emerges as a dominant greeting. Beyond stretching an injury, and in the vast majority, territorial males won
and putting hair slightly on end, alpha males often walk conflicts against intruders in the absence of fighting. A sin-
around in an exaggerated manner, as if they are bigger gle type of display, the neck stretch, was highly effective in
than they actually are. More generally, access to food, that intruders fled away in response to this display on 43%
water, space, and sexual partners is highly but imperfectly of the occurrences. Neck stretches are not common domi-
correlated with nonverbal indicators of dominance such nance displays in humans, but this example helps to illus-
as interpersonal distance and movement, withdrawal and trate that all species have some form of dominance display
approach, and staring versus gaze avoidance (Bernstein, and these displays can function to help individuals avoid
1981). Finally, primate dominance signals include non- risk (in having to exert real power) and can help the group
linguistic vocal acoustics, such as high-frequency calls to avoid harm.
signal subordinate status (de Waal, 1988; Hauser, 1993;
Affiliation
Hayaki, 1990).
With this in mind, a recent meta-analysis provided Given the hierarchical nature of most primate social struc-
a thorough investigation of human dominance displays ture, vertical relations are clearly important. However, so
(Hall, Coats, & Smith-LeBeau, 2005). This meta-analysis are horizontal relations. Social exclusion typically meant
included studies that measured the different nonverbal death for our ancestors and still means as much for many
cues exhibited by people varying in verticality. In this of our primate cousins (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Leary,
468 Nonverbal Behavior

volume 2). Social bonding, friendships, and coalitions are which individuals’ nonverbal behavior is synchronized
the glue that holds people together in social groups. As during interaction is measureable and is related to apparent
with the vertical dimension of social relations, this hori- affiliative motives (e.g., Bernieri, Reznick, & Rosenthal,
zontal dimension is supported by nonverbal behavior in 1988).
both human and nonhuman primates.
Issues in Examining Nonverbal
Without a doubt, touch and grooming are the most impor-
Cues Across Species
tant affiliative cues among nonhuman primates. In many
ape groups, grooming is as valuable a commodity as food Scholars of both human and animal behavior note that non-
and it can be predicted on the basis of relationship status verbal communication is complicated for several reasons.
(de Waal, 1982; Furuichi, 1989; Gouzoules & Gouzoules, First, individual nonverbal cues only rarely have decon-
1987; Nishida, 1987; Seyfarth, 1980). Grooming is also textualized meaning. Just as a human smile may indicate
associated with forming bonds. Previously groomed entities disparagement when it is flashed during an insult, ingratia-
are especially likely to help their former groomers in con- tion when it is shown while confessing to a misdeed, or
flicts (Hemelrijk, 1994; Schino, 2007; Seyfarth & Cheney, joy when it is shown on learning of a promotion, a chim-
1984), to provide food to their former groomers (de Waal, panzee’s outstretched arm may be used to ask for food, to
1982), and to share body warmth with former groomers garner support, or to suggest forgiveness (de Waal, 1982).
in the cold (Cheney & Seyfarth, 2007). Moreover, getting Despite the existence of the context-general cues reviewed
groomed helps to reduce stress (Gust, Gordon, Brodie, & in the preceding sections, a great deal of growth in under-
McClure, 1994). standing nonverbal communication may be accomplished
Although grooming is clearly the most predominant by examining homologues in the context-specific meaning
affiliative behavior in nonhuman primates, there are other of nonverbal cues. For example, do the meanings of non-
important affiliative nonverbal cues. For example, most verbal behavior in humans and in chimps both change
primate species exhibit identifiable cues that are reliably when moving from an interaction with kin to an interaction
used to recruit allies. Baboons emit certain vocaliza- with a higher-status individual? This type of analysis may
tions to indicate their support for individuals involved in reveal even greater similarity among species than previ-
a conflict—announcing they will intercede if necessary ously thought.
(Cheney & Seyfarth, 2007). And among chimpanzees, A second issue regards the importance of facial expres-
two facial expressions are reliably associated with affilia- sions in nonverbal communication across primates. The
tive behavior: the relaxed, open-mouth face (“playface”), continuity between nonverbal behavior in humans and that
which increases affiliative behavior and play, and the in the great apes (chimps, bonobos, gorillas, and orangutans)
silent, bared-teeth face (“fear grin”), which also increases is highlighted by nearly identical sets of facial muscles
affiliation (Waller & Dunbar, 2005). In general, affilia- (Burrows, Waller, Parr, & Bonar, 2006) such that the activa-
tive nonverbal displays among primates function to help tion of similar facial muscles appears to produce roughly the
create and maintain affiliations and horizontal group same expressive facial appearance in both species (Waller,
structure. Vick, Parr, Bard, & Pasqualini, 2006). Facial expressions
Although humans do not typically groom one another serve important social functions among primates—even
for hours on end, we do touch one another. Indeed, physi- among monkeys, who have much less muscular control over
cal touch is an important mechanism for the provision of their faces than humans and the great apes (Hauser, 1996).
social support and the acknowledgment of interpersonal A particularly interesting (and given the findings, cruel)
relationship. Touch increases with interpersonal intimacy investigation illustrates the importance of facial expres-
(Guerrero & Anderson, 1991), and interactions involv- sion in monkeys. Izard (1971) removed rhesus monkeys
ing touch appear to be more intimate (Burgoon, Buller, from their social group, lesioned the eighth cranial nerve
Hale, & de Turck, 1984). In the absence of touch, close (rendering control over facial expression impossible, save
interpersonal distance often signals affiliation (Burgoon, eye movement), and returned them to the social group.
1991; Mehrabian, 1969). Beyond touch and interper- These individuals subsequently experienced fewer and
sonal distance, affiliation is thought to be illustrated by a lower-quality social interactions, dropped in dominance
constellation of nonverbal cues sometimes labeled “non- rank, and were involved in more conflict. Unfortunately,
verbal involvement” (Edinger & Patterson, 1983) and similar interpersonal difficulties are experienced by indi-
sometimes labeled “nonverbal immediacy” (Anderson, viduals with certain physical and neurological diseases.
1985). The particular constellation said to account for For instance, patients with Parkinson’s disease experience
immediacy typically includes touch, close interpersonal “facial masking” and gradually lose control over their
distance, gaze, and forward lean. Finally, the degree to facial expressions. Even physicians who should know
Nonverbal Production 469

better underestimate Parkinson’s patients’ sociability and Primates can exhibit strategic nonverbal behaviors—for
overestimate their neuroticism (Tickle-Degnen & Lyons, example, de Waal (1982) described a chimpanzee named
2004), highlighting the corresponding importance of facial Yeroen who endured a shallow wound after a relatively
expression in humans. tame tussle with another chimp named Nikkie. Initially, it
In sum, nonverbal cues clearly play a role in structuring was puzzling to the research group that Yeroen wobbled
social life for nonhuman primates. Dominance displays in pitifully despite the lack of damage to his body. It soon
some primates are more central to social hierarchy than are became clear, however, that Yeroen would only limp when
actual exhibitions of power (aggression), and such displays within the visual field of Nikkie—outside of Nikkie’s
appear to save lives and increase the survival likelihood of visual field, Yeroen walked normally. Although it is some-
individual animals. Moreover, many similarities appear times difficult to interpret chimpanzee behavior, in this
between human and nonhuman primate displays of nonverbal instance it appears that Yeroen altered his nonverbal behav-
dominance. Similarly, affiliation displays play a key role in ior (gait) in a strategic move, suggesting that this behavior
maintaining and revealing social cohesion among both human was regulated. Likewise, not all nonverbal behaviors are
and nonhuman primates. Although the evidence for homolo- “automatic.”
gous affiliative cues is tenuous at best, there are loosely Still, nonverbal behavior is foundational to understand-
related homologues. Finally, despite differences in the mean- ing the occurrence and consequences of automatic cogni-
ing of particular facial expressions between chimpanzees and tive and behavioral processes as they occur in the natural
humans, the two species appear to have nearly identical facial world. Beyond the growing influence of the Internet, social
musculature and expressive appearance. The implications of interaction always includes nonverbal behavior and this
these similarities are yet to be realized, but it is likely that they behavior has important social consequences: People place
will reveal more similarities than differences in the produc- considerable emphasis on nonverbal behavior in draw-
tion of nonverbal behavior among primate species. ing inferences about others, perhaps more emphasis than
More broadly, the study of nonverbal behavior provides on verbal behavior (Argyle, Alkema, & Gilmour, 1971;
an important bridge for comparative studies involving Argyle, Salter, Nicholson, Williams, & Burgess, 1970).
humans and other primates. Although primatologists have People (and perhaps Yeroen) could thus enjoy substan-
been interested in social interaction for decades, they often tial social gains by controlling nonverbal behavior in
rely on linguists for their social theories concerning humans. ways that create desired impressions. To understand self-
Likewise, most mainstream social-psychological articles do presentation, then, is to understand automatic versus con-
not include references to other primates; if they do, it is usu- trolled processes, and to understand these processes in a
ally only in passing to note that humans are also animals. social context is to understand automaticity in nonverbal
The bridge provided by nonverbal behavior is wide enough communication.
for researchers in many areas of social psychology to look
Types of Automatic Nonverbal Behavior
toward primatology. For example, a great deal of research
focuses on the if, when, why, and how in the use of decep- The early part of the 20th century bore witness to a small
tion in primates (Hauser, 1996). Clearly, such effects would group of researchers examining the spontaneity of non-
be informative to social psychologists interested in self- verbal behavior. For example, Landis (1924) observed the
presentation and impression management, and these psy- nonverbal behavior of unknowing participants who were
chologists may impart some wisdom to the primatologists. forced to sniff ammonia, look at pornographic materials,
The use of grooming in most primates, of sexual touching and cut off the heads of live rats. Some participants even had
in bonobos, and of the “playface” in chimpanzees to form firecrackers dropped underneath their chairs. Fortunately
alliances and social friendships and to gain compliance for modern-day participants, such experiments are gener-
should be of interest to social psychologists involved in rela- ally no longer possible, at least not in the United States.
tionships research, research on social influence, and group Despite a lack of access to these rather abusive means of
dynamics. For those researchers who understand that humans eliciting spontaneous nonverbal behavior, contemporary
are socially intelligent primates, the examination of our researchers have made a great deal of progress in under-
primate cousins’ nonverbal behavior should be invaluable standing the many nuances of automaticity (Dijksterhuis,
to understanding certain aspects of human social behavior. this volume). One way to simplify the complexity of this
topic is to break it into four types of “conditional auto-
maticity” (Bargh, 1994). A lack of controllability refers
Automaticity and Control in Nonverbal Behavior
to processes that cannot be consciously regulated even if
That we share certain behavioral patterns with other pri- one tries, a lack of intentionality refers to processes that
mates does not imply that this behavior is automatic. begin without our conscious permission, efficiency refers
470 Nonverbal Behavior

to processes that do not consume cognitive resources, and roughly pose emotional faces (Mazur, 2005). Moreover,
awareness refers to subjective knowledge of the process or symbolic gestures (e.g., the “peace” sign) are rarely unin-
its behavioral influence. tentional. Nonetheless, for every nonverbal behavior we
control there is another that cannot be simultaneously con-
Unintentional and Uncontrollable Nonverbal Behav- trolled, suggesting that at any point in (social) time people
iors Perhaps the strongest form of automatic behavior is exhibit spontaneous nonverbal behavior.
behavior that cannot be stopped or initiated consciously.
Most studies of automatic social behavior include behav- Efficiency in Nonverbal Behavior Social interaction
iors that can be consciously initiated or that can be con- places various demands on cognitive resources (Ambady &
sciously stopped. For example, although thinking about Gray, 2002; Gilbert, Jones, & Pelham, 1987). In conversa-
elderly people automatically activated slower walking in tion, we have to (1) keep track of what the other person
one study (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996), walking slowly is saying, (2) keep what we want to say in mind until we
is something that healthy adults can do or stop doing if speak, and (3) generate spoken responses, all of which
they try. In contrast, less than 10% of the population can consume cognitive resources. Within and beyond the con-
produce the raising and pulling together of the brows that versation, people engage in active social perception pro-
occurs spontaneously with fear (DePaulo, 1992), blushing cesses, such as attending to others’ nonverbal behavior to
appears to be uncontrollable (Castelfranchi & Poggi, 1990; evaluate the impression one is creating, keeping an eye
Leary, Britt, Cutlip, & Templeton, 1992; Timms, 1980), on the broader social environment, trying to avoid biases
and some argue that the Duchenne smile (Ekman, in forming judgments, and engaging in perspective taking
Davidson, & Friesen, 1990), especially the contraction of to facilitate the flow of conversation. These many demands
the outer strands of the eye muscles, cannot normally be con- would seem to require us to be social superheroes, and yet
sciously controlled (Frank, Ekman, & Friesen, 1993). And without much conscious effort we are able accomplish
although most people can consciously control the direc- meaningful and smooth-flowing social interaction.
tion of their gaze when so inclined, this conscious control One asset in our superhero arsenal may be that nonver-
is short-circuited in the presence of another’s gaze—one’s bal behavior can proceed spontaneously, without the use
own gaze uncontrollably orients in the direction of another’s of cognitive resources, whereas stopping it or changing it
gaze (Driver et al., 1999). Of special importance for self- takes effort. For example, in one study, suppressing non-
presentation and social influence, these difficult-to-control verbal expressions during an enjoyable film reduced mem-
behaviors have substantial social consequences. For exam- ory for the film itself (Richards & Gross, 2000). Moreover,
ple, people like those who display Duchenne as opposed people who chronically suppress nonverbal behavior
to non-Duchenne smiles (Frank et al., 1993) and blushing exhibited worse memory than did nonsupressors (Richards &
appears to ameliorate self-presentational damage created Gross, 2000).
by untoward behavior (Leary et al., 1992). Efforts at control are probably most pronounced in
Although only a few individual nonverbal behaviors are situations in which it is important to create a particular
normally outside of conscious control, others may often be impression. For example, White people with negative
initiated unintentionally. Indeed, involuntary and voluntary associations toward Black people may feel compelled
facial actions appear to be innervated by different neurons to effortfully control their nonverbal behavior. Indeed,
(Ekman, 1984; Fridlund, 1994; Rinn, 1984). Moreover, Richeson and Shelton (2003) demonstrated that White
certain stimuli evoke stereotypic facial expressions even people who held negative implicit associations toward
when presented subliminally, short-circuiting the possi- Black people suppressed nonverbal behavior (reducing
bility of intentionality (Dimberg, Thunberg, & Elmehed, limb, bodily, and eye movement) during an interaction
2000). And people spontaneously respond to various posi- with a Black person and subsequently exhibited height-
tive and negative stimuli with the expected facial expres- ened interference on a Stroop task that indexes reductions
sions (e.g., Ekman et al., 1990). Thus, facial expressions in attentional control. Similarly, Black people exhibited
may often be uncontrollably elicited. more control (trying to appear more engaged) and more
From the discussion so far, it should be clear that cer- of a deficit in executive function in interacting with mem-
tain nonverbal behaviors are always uncontrollable and bers of another race than with members of their own race
unintentional and others can often be elicited without our (Richeson, Trawalter, & Shelton, 2005; Shelton, Richeson, &
conscious volition. Yet many nonverbal behaviors are con- Salvatore, 2005).
trollable under some circumstances. At any moment, we Together, these results suggest that nonverbal communi-
can easily change the way we walk, how close we stand to cation normally proceeds effortlessly and that consciously
others, and our posture, and even untrained children can stopping or altering nonverbal behavior requires resources
Nonverbal Production 471

from a limited pool of cognitive or self-regulatory resources. 2003). For instance, individuals who were prevented from
Perhaps contributing to these effects is the extra effort that gesturing showed worse memory on a task compared with
people have to spend just to consciously identify their non- those allowed to gesture, suggesting that inhibiting non-
verbal behavior, as described in the next section. verbal behavior functions as a cognitive load (Goldin-
Meadow & Wagner, 2005).
Awareness of Nonverbal Behavior The poet Robert For scholars interested in automatic and controlled
Burns (1853) wrote, “Oh wad some power the giftie gie us processes, nonverbal behavior provides a unique play-
To see oursel’s as others see us!” The ability to see ourselves ground. The production of nonverbal behavior has socially
as others do is particularly challenging in the domain of meaningful consequences that have existed for millions of
nonverbal behavior. We cannot see our own facial expres- years. Human evolution planned for social interaction, and
sions, perceive our bodily movements and gestures, or hear the many automatic social processes that we share with
our own voices as others do (Ekman & Friesen, 1969b). less cortically advanced species should be observable in
People may be aware that they are expressing joy in their nonverbal behavior. The implication is that a sound way
face, that they are gesturing with their arms, or that their to understand socially situated automatic behavior is via
voice has escalated several octaves, but more fine-grained examining nonverbal behavior. Yet not all socially adap-
awareness is difficult. Accordingly, recent research suggests tive behavior need be automatic, as described in the next
that most people exhibit comparatively little awareness of section.
their nonverbal behavior. For example, people appear to be
moderately accurate in estimating how much they smile Context and Culture in the Production
but are not good at estimating behaviors such as nodding, of Nonverbal Behavior
gazing, gesturing, and self-touch (Hall, Horgan, & Carter,
2002; Hall, Murphy, & Schmid-Mast, 2007). People who do not behave differently with their spouses
More broadly, Barr and Kleck (1995) examined the than with their bosses are people who will soon be divorced
relationship between facial expressiveness reported by or fired. The power of the situation is obvious when con-
participants and that reliably judged by raters. Participants sidering the contextualized nature of overt behaviors such
reported being more facially expressive than they appeared as romantic advances versus promotion requests. Many
to observers, even when they had been previously told to situations are defined partly by the nonverbal behaviors
attend to their own facial expressions; when later shown appropriate therein such that the most socially success-
film of their own facial responses, the vast majority ful impression managers are those who effortlessly adapt
admitted that they appeared much less expressive than and display those appropriate nonverbal behaviors. More
they remembered. In replication of these effects, people broadly, examining nonverbal behavior across cultures not
who were trying to maintain a neutral facial expression only permits inferences about the universality of certain
while tasting a disgusting drink overestimated the extent nonverbal behaviors but also provides substantial informa-
to which disgust was revealed (Gilovich, Savitsky, & tion about culture itself.
Medvec, 1998). On balance, it seems that people are gen-
Contextual Control of Nonverbal Behavior
erally unaware of their nonverbal behavior.
The efficiency with which nonverbal behavior adapts to the
Summary: Automaticity and Control social context is rather striking. Indeed, the social environ-
in Nonverbal Behavior ment automatically and with great speed initiates changes
In sum, as with most social activity, the production of in facial expression and paraverbal behavior (Dimberg
nonverbal behavior is neither fully automatic nor fully et al., 2000; Moody, McIntosh, Mann, & Weisser, 2007;
controlled. Unlike most social activity, however, some Neumann & Strack, 2000). This fact can be observed in any
nonverbal behaviors are nearly impossible to consciously movie theater—smiles and laughter immediately follow
start or stop and nonverbal behavior is often initiated and funny scenes, and startle responses occur immediately
executed without intention. Moreover, people are surpris- following terrifying scenes. Hence, nonverbal behavior
ingly unaware of the form that their nonverbal behavior reflects its social context.
takes. Nearly every study to date shows that nonverbal
Although some nonverbal behaviors can be controlled, behavior is different when others are present (vs. absent;
particularly in the service of impression management and cf. DePaulo, 1992). Perhaps most well known is a study
meeting social norms (DePaulo, 1992), the control of non- in which participants viewing a pleasant videotape
verbal behavior appears to be taxing and reduces capacities alone exhibited little smiling as compared with (a) par-
for the performance of other tasks (Richeson & Shelton, ticipants who were told that a friend was watching the
472 Nonverbal Behavior

same video or (b) participants for whom a friend was humans show a remarkable nonverbal ability to efficiently
actually present (Fridlund, 1991). Notably, self-reported and enduringly adapt to social situations.
emotion did not differ among conditions, suggesting that
Culture and the Production of Nonverbal Behavior
the between-group differences in nonverbal behavior
were expressive, not experiential. Similarly, Cole (1986) As numerous anthropological accounts attest, culture
demonstrated that children smiled at an experimenter in exerts a profound impact on nonverbal behavior. This is
response to a clearly disappointing gift but did not do so clearly true with respect to gestures. Some of the earliest
in the absence of an audience. The effect of social context empirical work on nonverbal behavior demonstrated that
is not always to increase expressiveness but rather to fit Sicilian and Lithuanian immigrants exhibited distinct ges-
established norms. For example, Kleck and colleagues tures that disappeared as individuals adapted to American
(1976) administered painful electric shock to participants culture (Efron, 1941). The cultural specificity of gestures
and observed attenuated nonverbal expressivity with an is still apparent, of course. For example, walking hand in
audience. hand may be reserved for lovers or parent–child relations
Beyond the clear impact of any audience, the contex- in the West, but in China and other countries, friends often
tualized nature of nonverbal behavior is evident in the hold hands. It is also well known that the “a-ok” gesture in
diverging nonverbal behaviors directed toward bosses ver- America is an obscene sexual gesture elsewhere. Hence,
sus peers, friends versus strangers, and experts versus non- gestures are often completely culturally specific.
experts (e.g., Buck, Losow, Murphy, & Costanzo, 1992; A more controversial topic is the nonverbal expression
Fugita, Hogrebe, & Wexley, 1980; Montepare & Vega, of emotion. With respect to spontaneous emotion expres-
1988; Steckler & Rosenthal, 1985). Situation-specific sions, Ekman (1971) observed highly positive correlations
goals also contextualize nonverbal behavior. For example, between American and Japanese participants’ patterns of
people are especially likely to smile, nod, and gaze at others facial response to neutral and pleasant videos. Similar find-
when trying to impress or gain favor (Godfrey, Jones, & ings have been observed in at least 10 other studies, sup-
Lord, 1986; Lefebvre, 1973; Rosenfeld, 1966). porting universality (Matsumoto, 2006). Yet Russell (1994)
The contextualization of some types of nonverbal suggests that in many studies, spontaneous facial expres-
behavior appears to be universal. For instance, across cul- sions only exhibit cross-cultural reliability with respect to
tures, people use a type of speech characterized by high general positivity and negativity—not discrete emotions.
pitch, variable intonation, and other paraverbal devices Two recent studies provided more conflicting evidence:
(“baby talk”) when talking to infants and children (as Matsumoto and Willingham (2006) demonstrated cross-
opposed to normal functioning adults (Ferguson, 1964). cultural stability in emotion expression among Olympic
People also use this baby talk with foreigners, the elderly, medal winners, whereas Naab and Russell (2007) demon-
developmentally delayed adults, and pets (Caporael, 1981; strated little such stability between a preliterature culture
DePaulo & Coleman, 1986, 1987). and the United States. It does seem clear that certain spon-
Although it may be tempting to conclude from this taneous facial movements are universally associated with
research that increased conscious control must account certain feeling states, although those feeling states may be
for the influence of the social situation on nonverbal discrete emotions or diffuse affect.
behavior (DePaulo & Friedman, 1998), many of the The study of cultural similarity in spontaneous emotion
reviewed findings might be interpreted as biologically expressions is supplemented by a much larger literature on
prepared or functional responses to social life that either deliberately posed emotion expressions. In most of these
are hardwired (e.g., responses to higher-status individuals) studies, photographers take pictures of people in differ-
or have through learning become automatic (DePaulo & ent cultures posing each of several emotions. People from
Friedman, 1998). For example, although people exhibit other cultures then rate the emotion that each facial con-
predictably negative nonverbal behavior toward stig- figuration expressed. In general, there is clear evidence
matized individuals, this effect occurs primarily for for cross-cultural similarity in deliberately posed emotion
highly prejudiced expressers, suggesting a learned but expressions (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). Yet these pro-
habitual response (Chaikin, Sigler, & Derlega, 1974; totypical facial expressions are supplemented by culture-
Dovidio, Kawakami, Johnson, Johnson, & Howard, 1997; specific patterns. Facial expressions of fear (for example)
Harris, Moniz, Sowards, & Krane, 1994; King, Shapiro, bear considerable similarity across cultures, but subtle dif-
Hebl, Singletary, & Turner, 2006). ferences also exist—people within the culture are better
In short, contextual influences on nonverbal behavior able to recognize fear than are people outside of the culture
may sometimes reflect conscious control but often reflect (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). Indeed, as opposed to neu-
the automatic influence of the environment. More generally, tral facial expressions, emotional facial expressions appear
Nonverbal Production 473

to reveal culture; in one study, nationality judgments of To spend time with infants is to know that they come
Japanese and Japanese Americans were more accurate equipped with impressive vocal capacities. Crying is typi-
when the posers exhibited emotional expressions (Marsh, cally evident at or within a few minutes of birth and when
Elfenbein, & Ambady, 2003). In short, cultural similarities it is not there could be trouble. Crying has some clear social
in emotion expressions appear to be supplemented by non- functions. For example, infants cry in response to other
verbal accents in such expression. infants’ cries, but this same pattern does not occur when
the eliciting stimuli are synthetic cries, the cries of an older
Summary: Nonverbal Behavior in Context infant, or recordings of infants’ own cries (Martin & Clark,
(and Culture) 1982; Sagi & Hoffman, 1976; Simner, 1971). These find-
Nonverbal behavior is nothing if not contextually driven, even ings illustrate that crying serves a social function beyond a
if a few universal action tendencies exist. Whether because simple response to a noxious stimulus. Likewise, if undif-
of spontaneous or controlled processes, nonverbal behavior ferentiated displeasure is all that fueled crying, newborns
reflects conformity to situational constraints. The efficiency should not exhibit acoustically differentiated cries for pain ver-
with which such nonverbal adaptation occurs, the specificity sus anger, as they do (Lester & Boukydis, 1992; Zeskind &
and duration of context-specific nonverbal behavior, and the Collins, 1987). Crying is socially meaningful behavior.
existence of culturally specific patterns of nonverbal activity Crying is not the only nonverbal communication skill
all attest to the special role of nonverbal behavior in assimi- displayed by newborns—meaningful facial gestures emerge
lating to the group and to particular others. quickly. Within 42 minutes of birth, many newborns imi-
One of the more fascinating things about nonverbal tate nonverbal behaviors such as mouth opening and tongue
behavior is that, to the extent that the same contexts and protrusion; this capacity is especially strong within 12 to
cultures elicit the same nonverbal behavior, such behav- 21 days after birth, and it can occur a full day after infants
ior helps define the context. In fact, the way that cultural observe a novel facial expression (Meltzoff & Moore,
anthropologists and primatologists understand social 1977, 1983, 1994). Within a few months of birth, infants
situations is by observing nonverbal behaviors—those exhibit spontaneous smiling to human faces, spontane-
behaviors help define the situation, and in some cases, ous anger to the removal of a reward, and other nonverbal
the culture (Molinsky, Krabbenhoft, Ambady, & Choi, emotion expressions (Alessandri, Sullivan, & Lewis, 1990;
2005). Thus, nonverbal behavior is crucial not only for an Campos, Campos, & Barrett, 1989; Oster, 1978; Sullivan &
understanding of contextual and cultural effects but also Lewis, 2003). These responses may help infants meet a
for defining the social-psychological situation. Broadly, need, whether that need be affiliation or acquisition of
then, situational control of nonverbal behavior is benefi- a rewarding stimulus.
cial for individuals, as well as for the group. The flexibility Other difficulties faced by parents may also be attributed
of nonverbal behavior vis-à-vis the situation is clearly an to infant nonverbal communication. For example, putting
efficient means of social assimilation that is so important an infant in a car seat is not always an easy task for parents,
that it emerges before perhaps any other social ability. yet the movements of infants at these times are not com-
pletely random efforts to frustrate adults. Infants use vari-
ous gestures to attract attention and to make requests, and
Nonverbal Behavior in Infancy
still other specific body movements are associated with
An understanding of the early stages of life provides a great positive or negative affect (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988;
deal of information about the natural processes central to Legerstee, Corter, & Kineapple, 1990). For example, posi-
social psychology. Much has been learned about infants’ tive affect is often communicated with open-handed arm
adaptation to and development within the social world, and extensions, whereas negative affect is often communicated
this development typically involves nonverbal behavior. with closed hands and arms extended at the sides. Around
As explained here, nonverbal behavior represents the sole 1 year, more pleasant “offering” gestures emerge, and by
means by which young infants engage the social world. 14 to 16 months toddlers have a broad range of request,
attribute, and pointing gestures (Acredolo & Goodwyn,
Nonverbal Development: Findings 1988; Masur, 1983). Note here the continued develop-
Anyone who has spent time with infants recognizes that ment of gestures following the advent of verbal speech. In
newborns are not sponges who passively take in informa- fact, gestures predict changes in language and facilitate the
tion. Babies have needs, and when those needs are not met, learning of language (Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, 2005).
they let us know. One important obstacle for infants, then, After the first year of life, children begin to learn dis-
is how to let us know what they need or want. The only way play rules and to adjust their expressions accordingly.
for them to accomplish this is through nonverbal behavior. Although not definitively in response to a display rule,
474 Nonverbal Behavior

infants begin to reduce negative gestural responses as early speech. At the end of his excited speech, Dean drew a fist
as 1 year of age (Blake, McConnell, Horton, & Benson, high up in the air and as he vocalized the sound “byah!”
1992). By 24 months, toddlers can deliberately use facial he quickly moved the fist down and forward. This moment
expressions to elicit support from caregivers (Buss & Kiel, was televised repeatedly in the United States, was ridi-
2004), although it can be difficult for them to do (joy is the culed in the media, and many political pundits attributed
most reliable controlled expression; Fridlund, Ekman, & the demise of Dean’s campaign to that moment.
Oster, 1987). The ability to situationally alter nonverbal Impressions of others are often dominated by nonver-
expressions appears to improve with age. In Cole’s (1986) bal behavior (Argyle et al., 1970, 1971), a fact that may
study, 10-year-olds were better at concealing their negative have undermined Howard Dean’s political ambitions.
facial emotion than were 4-year-olds. Moreover, many scholars believe that individual differ-
ences in nonverbal communication are predictive of more
Nonverbal Development: Theory and Implications general social intelligence such that nonverbal behavior
A variety of developmental theories attempt to account both indexes and contributes to likely social success.
for early nonverbal expressions—differentiation theories, for
Expressiveness
example, emphasize that infants move from expressing
relatively diffuse affect to more differentiated discrete The two most-studied individual differences in nonverbal
emotions (Izard, 1991; Sroufe, 1996). One recent theory communication are nonverbal expressiveness and gender.
offers a fascinating and compelling explanation of how Nonverbal expressiveness is the extent to which nonverbal
we develop from undifferentiated emotion expressers into behavior reveals feelings (e.g., Buck, 1984). The current
discrete emotion expressers. This sociocultural internaliza- review highlights the facets of nonverbal expressiveness
tion model (Holodynski & Friedlmeier, 2006) draws on the most relevant to social adaptation (for a complete review,
robust finding that caregivers mimic their children’s non- see Riggio, 1992, 2006). For example, individual differ-
verbal behavior, and vice versa (Malatesta & Izard, 1984). ences in nonverbal expressivity emerge earlier than ver-
The idea is that caregivers selectively mimic their children’s bal behavior, coincide with our initial social adaptation in
emotional behavior and in exaggerated form. Parents’ selec- the world, and are stable throughout childhood (Kagan,
tive and exaggerated mimicry then rubs back off on the Snidman, & Arcus, 1998). Expressivity also seems to be
children, who may reduce their unmimicked expressions stable within families and across generations (Halberstadt,
and alter their mimicked expressions to better approximate Fox, & Jones, 1993). Nonverbal expressiveness is criti-
the parent’s expression. Over time, this process can shape cal to social success, at least within a culture: The mood
expression—diffuse affect becomes organized into specific of nonverbally expressive people is likely to spread
facial configurations of discrete emotions. This process may throughout a social setting (Friedman & Riggio, 1981;
help explain how nonverbal accents (Marsh et al., 2003) are Sullins, 1991), expressiveness appears to be more impor-
transmitted within a culture. tant than even physical attractiveness in first impressions
Strangely enough, the world of the infant is one that (Friedman, Riggio, & Casella, 1988), and people tend to be
most of us have difficulty remembering, even though we especially satisfied with expressive physicians, teachers,
all once lived there (e.g., Usher & Neisser, 1993). Perhaps and athletes (Abrami, Leventhal, & Perry, 1982; DiMatteo,
this has to do, in part, with most of our experiences in that 1979; Rejeski & Lowe, 1980). Moreover, nonverbal
world being nonverbal. Our faces, nonlinguistic voices, expressiveness in one arena can carry over to other arenas.
and bodies were our only means of communicating our Thus, people who are socially expressive in their nonverbal
love, distaste, and interest for various aspects of the world. behavior also tend to be more expressive in their personal
It thus seems reasonable that this early nonverbal experi- webpages (Weisbuch, Ivcevic, & Ambady, 2009). The
ence shapes later social adaptation, temperament, cogni- causal impact of expressiveness on individual and group
tion, and behavior—even if we can’t remember that early life may help to explain broader differences in behavior,
experience it perhaps helps to form unique personalities. such as those that occur with gender.
Indeed, individual differences in nonverbal behavior may
Gender
have important predictive value, a topic we turn to next.
Most people believe that substantial nonverbal differ-
ences exist between men and women. Lay beliefs hold, for
Individual Differences in the Production
instance, that women speak more softly, gaze more, and
of Nonverbal Behavior
smile more than do men (Briton & Hall, 1995). According
In the 2004 American presidential campaign, candidate to one provocative theory, women nonverbally behave
Howard Dean exhibited a great deal of excitement in one in the way that weak and low-status people do and this
Nonverbal Production 475

behavior helps to account for other gender differences (Barsalou, 1999). Over the last decade, a new model of
(Henley, 1977). To what extent are these assumptions cognition has begun to compete with the existing view
about gender differences supported? that cognitive representation is largely semantic. By the
The relationship between gender and nonverbal domi- older view, the category “elderly” would be defined by a
nance is not simple. For example, women are more likely feature list that might include “gray hair,” “slow,” “calm-
than men to exhibit some nonverbal behaviors empirically ing,” and so on. By the newer “grounded cognition” view
associated with low status, such as a softer voice, but also (often described as “embodied cognition”), cognitive repre-
some nonverbal behaviors empirically associated with high sentations are composed of perceptual information, includ-
status, such as facial expressiveness. Still, women and men ing the motor information involved in nonverbal behavior
clearly exhibit different nonverbal behaviors, and many (Barsalou, 1999). “Elderly,” for example, would be defined
of these confirm stereotypes about nonverbal behavior. by visual neurons active with grayness, motor neurons
In comparison with men, women smile more, gaze more, active with slow movement, limbic system neurons active
are more facially expressive, exhibit more expressive ges- with calmness, and so on. Activation of the elderly person
tures, touch people more, and maintain smaller interper- category is just the activation of these neurons; likewise,
sonal distances (Hall, 1984). Of course, these effects have activation of these (e.g., “slow” motor) neurons makes it
many nuances—for example, differences in smiling are likely that the category (“elderly”) will become active.
only existent during social interaction (LaFrance, Hecht, & According to this view, category activation is just the acti-
Levy-Paluck, 2003). Women and men also differ consider- vation of modality-specific neurons, including motor neu-
ably in their movement—from point light displays alone, in rons. Nonverbal behavior can thus be part of thinking.
which an actor walks in the dark with lightbulbs affixed to
Grounding Social Experience
the joints, perceivers can detect whether that actor is male
in Nonverbal Behavior
or female (Kozlowski & Cutting, 1977). This difference
appears to occur because masculine men exhibit a “swag- According to theories of grounded cognition, a pattern of
ger,” with lots of shoulder movement and little hip move- neuronal activation corresponds to both category activation
ment, whereas feminine women exhibit a “sway,” with lots and category perception. These theories suggest a critical
of hip movement but little shoulder movement (Johnson & role for movement, or the activation of motor neurons, in
Tassinary, 2005). category activation. Indeed, people categorize objects most
quickly when making gesturing motions that simulate the
Summary: Nonverbal Behavior and movement they normally make when handling that object
Individual Differences (Barsalou, Niedenthal, Barbey, & Ruppert, 2003; Tucker &
In general, nonverbal behavior is an important component Ellis, 1998). For example, they are fastest to identify a
of personality that is not limited to expressiveness and gen- faucet when moving their hand in a faucet-turning motion.
der. Extraversion and self-monitoring, for example, have And when making responses with gentle finger grasping,
important nonverbal components. One of the more well- people are faster to identify objects that require gentle
studied topics in all of psychology, infant temperament, motor grasping (a grape) than those that need gross motor
is based on nonverbal reactions, suggesting the centrality grasping (a hammer; Tucker & Ellis, 2001). Similarly,
of nonverbal behavior to the study of personality. Indeed, when motor components of a category are suppressed, so
infant temperament predicts personality at 10 to 12 years is the perception of that category. Participants forced to
of age (Kagan & Snidman, 2004). Moreover, the facial hold a pencil sideways between their lips and their teeth
expressions of 18-month-olds were reliable predictors of (preventing smiles) were slower than other participants to
the Big Five personality traits 2 years later, even after con- detect a change in another’s emotional facial expression
trolling for relational variables such as attachment at 18 (Niedenthal, Brauer, Halberstadt, & Innes-Ker, 2001).
months (Abe & Izard, 1999). In short, nonverbal behavior Moreover, people prevented from moving their faces are
appears to play an important role in personality. slow to identify emotion expressions in general (Stel &
van Knippenberg, 2008). Finally, feelings of pride after
success are inhibited among people forced to slump versus
Thinking Nonverbally
those forced to sit upright (Stepper & Strack, 1993).
The production of nonverbal behavior clearly plays an If smiling produces happiness, then we should like
important role in social adaptation. But might basic social objects more when we smile in their presence. Indeed,
cognition also be based on nonverbal behavior? Research attitude change toward a counterattitudinal essay was
on cognitive and perceptual processing is increasingly strongest among participants whose expressions had
informed by the view that people think nonverbally been contorted into smiles (vs. frowns) as they wrote
476 Nonverbal Behavior

counterattitudinal essays (Rhodewalt & Comer, 1979). As influencing introspective judgments, memory, and attitudes.
compared with smiling participants, frowning participants Although a grounded cognition account offers an engaging
do not like cartoons as much (Strack, Martin, & Stepper, explanation of these effects, other theories can explain at
1988), and participants with a disgust expression thought least portions of them. For example, the ideomotor theory
odors smelled particularly bad (Kraut, 1982). These findings of perception (Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001) explains how the
provide initial evidence that motor representations involv- perception of behavior produces the same behavior in one-
ing the face are involved in attitudinal representations. self, and vice versa. Likewise, the direct effects of nonverbal
Other nonverbal movements are involved in representa- behavior on subjective evaluations of emotion and attitudes
tions of liking and disliking. For example, in one set of stud- can be explained by the idea that people treat their nonver-
ies, participants were instructed to nod or shake their heads bal behaviors as metaperceptual cues to internal states (e.g.,
(to test a pair of headphones) while listening to an argument. Brinol & Petty, 2003). In sum, the evidence clearly suggests
They then stopped moving their heads and indicated the an important role for nonverbal behavior in social cognition,
extent to which they agreed with the argument. Participants but a grounded cognition account is not the only theory for
who nodded agreed with the argument to a greater extent describing this role. The next decade should provide evi-
than did participants who shook their heads (Wells & Petty, dence that disambiguates the relevant models.
1980). Likewise, eye gaze may cause rather than sim-
ply reflect preferences. For example, Shimojo, Simion, Summary: Nonverbal Production
Shimojo, and Scheier (2003) found that before a preferential
decision, the pattern of gaze between two faces eventually The production of nonverbal behavior is clearly part of
shifted toward the face that was later preferred. Moreover, social adaptation. In this way, humans are not unlike other
manipulations of gaze direction caused predictable changes primates who use nonverbal behavior to maintain social
in preferences. Indeed, experimental studies have demon- structure and to promote affiliation. In adapting to the
strated that, for heterosexual individuals, increased gaze social world and setting the foundation for later social cog-
toward opposite sex partners causes increased romantic nition, infants rely heavily on nonverbal behavior for get-
attraction (e.g., Kellerman, Lewis, & Laird, 1989). ting their needs met. As adults, nonverbal production skills
More evidence that the production of nonverbal behav- developed over the millennia and early in individual lives
ior facilitates cognitive processing comes from work on aid in prompting adaptation to social groups. Such adapta-
learning. Children who gesture more during instruction tion plays an important role and helps in socialization and
learn more than those who gesture less (Goldin-Meadow & acculturation. Finally, and surprisingly, the production of
Wagner, 2005). nonverbal behavior even plays an important role in basic
The evidence thus far suggests an important role for cognitive and perceptual processes.
nonverbal behavior in basic and social perception, although
a great deal more work remains to be done to explore these NONVERBAL FOUNDATIONS
relationships. Beyond social perception, emotion, and atti- OF SOCIAL PERCEPTION
tudes, other effects of nonverbal behavior on social cog-
nition are likely. For example, given substantial evidence Clearly, the production of nonverbal behavior is central to
for mood-congruent memory, smiling should and does various social-psychological processes. But the other side of
promote memory for emotion-congruent material (Laird, the nonverbal equation—nonverbal perception—is perhaps
Wagener, Halal, & Szegda, 1982; Riskind, 1983). In gen- even more important to social adaptation. As with the pro-
eral, nonverbal behavior plays a foundational role in the duction of nonverbal behavior, the perception of nonverbal
perception and expression of affective phenomena ranging behavior is foundational. Much learning early in life occurs
from emotion to attitudes to memory. The grounded cogni- via others’ nonverbal behavior, suggesting that the foundation
tion perspective implies that nonverbal behavior plays a of our social knowledge is nonverbal. Indeed, adult social
broader role in social cognition as well. perception and social judgment rely heavily on nonverbal
behavior. Meaningful nonverbal behavior provides some of
Grounded Cognition and Nonverbal
the earliest input to social perception and permits rapid and
Behavior: Redux
efficient social judgments (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992).
The traditional view of expressive behavior, articulated in
the most prominent theories of nonverbal behavior (Ekman, Nonverbal Perception Processes
1999; Fridlund, 1994), is that internal states (emotions, inten-
tions) cause nonverbal expressions. Yet it also appears to be The social cognition juggernaut of the 1980s and early
the case that nonverbal expressions can cause internal states, 1990s relied heavily on the use of written experimental
Nonverbal Foundations of Social Perception 477

materials. The field of social cognition built on the Vogeley, 2008; Wild, Erb, & Bartels, 2001). Hence,
established base of cognitive psychology and followed its neural pathways for processing unattended nonver-
methods, often using abstract, verbal, and written stimuli. bal expressions and producing unintentional nonverbal
While this era provided a rich and exciting knowledge behavior appear to be largely subcortical and shared with
base, it mostly overlooked the importance of social ecol- other mammals and may account, in part, for meaningful
ogy in basic cognitive processes (McArthur & Baron, pre-conscious responses to emotion expressions.
1983). By the late 1990s, an increasing number of stud- Automatic processing of nonverbal behavior is not lim-
ies were demonstrating important differences between the ited to emotion. The extraction of information from non-
processes that helped to disambiguate written descriptions verbal behavior proceeds in a relatively automatic fashion
of people and the processes that helped to disambiguate in many domains. For example, limitations imposed
images of real people (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). In on cognitive processing resources do not interfere with
the last decade, increasing emphasis has been placed on (1) recognition of social relationship type (e.g., friends vs.
examining real behavior (Baumeister, Vohs, & Funder, strangers) given strictly nonverbal information, (2) recog-
2007). To that end, it has become increasingly important to nition of attitudes based on nonverbal behavior, and (3)
understand the processes that play a role in the perception accuracy in nonverbal person perception (Ambady & Gray,
of nonverbal behavior. In particular, the next two sections 2002; Gilbert & Krull, 1988; Patterson & Stockbridge,
review the automatic processes involved in nonverbal 1998). Findings such as these suggest that the processing
perception and how various nonverbal cues contribute to of nonverbal behavior is often free of cognitive resources
person perception. (i.e., is often automatic).
Perhaps the strongest evidence that nonverbal behavior
Automatic Processing of Nonverbal Behavior is processed automatically comes from research on gaze
Are our impressions and conclusions about others driven following. Even if people think that they can resist the urge
by spontaneous processes that escape our awareness? Or to look where others’ look, research has shown that atten-
are they the result of conscious deliberation? These are tion is obliged to move in the direction of others’ gaze.
fundamental questions that highlight the role of nonver- Indeed, specific neurons code for eye gaze direction (e.g.,
bal behavior in social processing, judgment, and behavior Calder et al., 2007) and are involved in the orienting of
(Bargh & Pietromonaco, 1982; Bargh, 1994; Devine, 1989; attention (Rafal, 1996). Within 3 months of birth, infants’
Winter & Uleman, 1984). saccadic eye movements appear to follow the dynamic gaze
of caretakers (Farroni, Massaccesi, Pividori, & Johnson,
Automaticity in Nonverbal Perception: Nonconscious 2004; Hood, Willen, & Driver, 1998), and this gaze fol-
Processing Substantial evidence indicates that facial lowing becomes especially strong by 18 months (Brooks &
expressions are processed and elicit meaningful Meltzoff, 2002, 2005). In adulthood, others’ eye gaze may
responses prior to perceivers’ conscious recognition of those be the only visual cue that, placed centrally, reflexively
expressions (Murphy & Zajonc, 1993; Ravaja, Kallinen, directs attention toward the periphery. The presentation of
Saari, & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, 2004; Rotteveel, de a gaze cue in the center of the screen rapidly shifts atten-
Groot, Geutskens, & Phaf, 2001; Stapel, Koomen, & tion in the direction of the gaze (Friesen & Kingstone,
Ruys, 2002; Strahan, Spencer, & Zanna, 2002; 1998). Even when participants are explicitly told that a
Winkielman, Berridge, & Wilbarger, 2005). Evidence target will be in the opposite direction of the eye gaze
from neuroscience details how some of this processing image and even when the target typically is in the oppo-
may occur. For example, the human amygdala responds site direction, participants still exhibit a speedy attention-
to emotional facial expressions even when those facial orienting effect in the gazed-at direction, suggesting that
expressions are presented subliminally (e.g., Hariri, this effect cannot be suppressed (Driver et al., 1999). Thus,
Tessitore, Mattay, Fera, & Weinberger, 2002; Liddell another’s gaze immediately orients one’s attention, and
et al., 2005; Ohman, 2002; Whalen et al., 1998, 2004). this orienting can only be consciously suppressed after a
The path from the subcortical visual areas to the amyg- half second or so—immediate gaze following seems to be
dala may represent the more “ancient” route to emo- obligatory.
tional understanding found in several species, including
nonmammals (Adolphs, 2006). Moreover, the somato- Automaticity in Nonverbal Mimicry: Goal-Dependent
sensory system common to mammals is often activated Automaticity A great deal of evidence shows that peo-
in response to emotion expressions, a finding that some ple unintentionally (and often without awareness) mimic
scholars regard as evidence for emotional contagion at the nonverbal behavior of others (Chartrand & Bargh,
the neural level (e.g., Schilbach, Eickhoff, Mojzisch, & 1999; Neumann & Strack, 2000). In one paradigm, slides
478 Nonverbal Behavior

of happy and angry faces elicited facial muscle responses If mirror neurons and nonverbal mimicry are driven by
that corresponded to the presented slides (Dimberg, 1982, affiliation motives, they may be taken offline when mim-
1990). Facial pain appears to elicit expressions of facial icry would actually disrupt social interaction. In one study,
pain in viewers, foot tapping and nose rubbing during for example, individuals were seated facing one another and
social interaction appear to prompt the same in interac- participated in a cooperative task (Tiedens & Fragale, 2003).
tional partners, and infants exhibit facial imitation shortly One of these individuals was a confederate who exhibited
after birth (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; Meltzoff & Moore, nonverbal dominance or submission via postural expansion
1977; Vaughan & Lanzetta, 1980). In conversation, peo- or constriction. Participants’ behavior revealed complemen-
ple appear to imitate accents, vocal tone, and vocal speed tarity rather than mimicry. Thus, over the course of the inter-
(Giles & Powesland, 1975; Neumann & Strack, 2000; action with a dominant, expansive confederate, participants
Webb, 1969). And while it is well known that yawning exhibited increasing postural constriction and such responses
is contagious among humans (Provine, 1986) even dogs contributed to positive evaluations of the interaction.
appear to catch human yawns (Joly-Mascheroni, Senju, In general, meaningful nonverbal behavior is often pro-
& Shepherd, 2008). The effects of nonverbal mimicry cessed nonconsciously and without cognitive resources.
do not end at the behavior itself, but nonverbal mimicry is People clearly have automatic mechanisms in place for
related to emotion in a process referred to as emotion con- speedy adaptation to the social environment, as communi-
tagion (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994). cated via responses to nonverbal behavior. Of course, such
Indeed, evidence now suggests considerable overlap flexibility occurs in responses to real social environments,
in the neurons responsible for certain motor movements the importance of which is reviewed in what follows.
and perceiving the same movements in others; this is true
Configural and Featural Processes in Nonverbal
for both monkeys and humans (Rizzolatti & Craighero,
Perception and Judgment
2004). These neurons have been labeled “mirror neurons”
and are thought by some to account for nonverbal mimicry Although political critics and pornographers might disagree,
(Gallese & Goldman, 1998). human bodies have heads and human heads have bodies.
Regions above and below the neck are crucial to social
Moderated Effects of Nonverbal Mimicry The exis- life, yet just as initial studies on social perception often used
tence of neurons dedicated to simulating others’ actions a completely disembodied context, contemporary studies
suggests something special about nonverbal mimicry be- have used partially disembodied stimuli in an effort to create
yond simple perception. Given the importance of group a solid scientific basis for future research. For example, the
life and hence social bonding to our species, it could be last decade has witnessed a tremendous amount of research
that these “special” neurons are dedicated to simulating on how the human face is processed perceptually and cog-
the behavior of and thus to understanding our fellow group nitively (Macrae, & Quadflieg, this volume; Zebrowitz,
members (e.g., Preston & de Waal, 2002). 2006). And just as moving from written materials to natu-
Recent work is consistent with this idea. For example, ralistic faces has forced a rewriting and rethinking of some
Weisbuch and Ambady (2008a) observed that nonverbal previously “known” phenomena (e.g., Quinn & Macrae,
emotion contagion only occurred when the social perceiver 2005), moving from bodiless faces to a full-human context
shared group membership with the social target. Others may produce some revisions to what has been learned about
have observed increased imitation for ingroup face rubbing social perception from studies of human faces. In this sec-
and facial expressions of liked faces; when shown happy tion, we describe existing theory and research that models
or angry images of then-President Ronald Reagan, people whole-person perceptual processes.
who did not like Reagan did not exhibit emotion conta-
gion (Likowski, Muhlberger, Seibt, Pauli, & Weyers, 2008; Feature-Based Perception of the Whole Person Social
McHugo, Lanzetta, Sullivan, Masters, & Englis, 1985; psychologists have often used a lens model (Brunswik,
Yabar, Johnston, Miles, & Peace, 2006). Indeed, when 1956) to explore the nonverbal cues that contribute to
people want to bond or have been socially excluded, they social judgment. Studies that use this model to understand
are especially likely to exhibit nonverbal mimicry toward nonverbal behavior first measure characteristics of social
ingroup members (Lakin & Chartrand, 2003; Lakin, targets (typically via self-report). Then, those social targets
Chartrand, & Arkin, 2008). Hence, the evidence appears to engage in a videotaped task and, based on the generated
be strong that nonverbal mimicry is especially responsive videotapes, judges code the targets for various behaviors
to the ingroup. For this reason, it seems likely that affili- (smiling, talking time, etc.). Finally, social perceivers
ation goals underlie the simulation of others’ nonverbal provide macro-level ratings for the targets on relevant
behavior. dimensions (e.g., personality). The resulting analyses
Nonverbal Foundations of Social Perception 479

describe the nonverbal cues associated with particular environment in which they have evolved—both over the
target characteristics and the nonverbal cues used by social course of one’s life, and over generations. According to
perceivers in judging those particular target characteristics. this theory, perceiving is for doing in that perception is
Such an analysis can be informative with respect to both the for extracting the actionable properties of things or the
nonverbal cues that social perceivers use in general and properties of things that are relevant to well-being. These
the nonverbal cues that accurate social perceivers use. self-relevant properties are described as affordances. For
In one example, groups of three previously unacquainted example, lowered eyebrows might afford danger if the
college students were asked to converse for about 15 min- eyebrow-lowering individual is moving toward the self,
utes on topics of their choosing while seated in an informal is yelling loudly, and exhibits other facial bodily motions
waiting room. The conversations took place a week after indicative of aggressive intent. The key point for the cur-
participants had provided self-reported personality ratings. rent purposes is that affordances are said to be readily
On the basis of silent videotapes, judges coded the non- communicated over multiple modalities and most effec-
verbal behavior of the targets and social perceivers pro- tively via the higher-order patterning (or “configuration”)
vided personality ratings of the targets (Gifford, 1994). The of cues, rather than single cues in isolation (Zebrowitz &
eight measured personality traits were then analyzed with Collins, 1997). McArthur and Baron (1983), in a position
a lens model. For example, social perceivers’ ratings of consistent with Gibson’s, suggested that social percep-
dominance were based on head orientation, trunk orienta- tion also proceeds in an ecologically adaptive manner.
tion, self-touching, gesturing, and many more cues. While They argued that perceivers extract invariants, or stable
perceivers correctly used leg extension and gesturing, the social properties, from higher-order patterns in the social
majority of cues they used were not valid indicators of stimulus array. This social-ecological approach either
dominance. Moreover, they missed several important cues, implicitly or explicitly anticipated several findings with
such as “left leg lean.” Most traits showed similar patterns respect to the perception and judgment of nonverbal
whereby social perceivers correctly used several nonverbal behavior.
cues but also failed to use many correct cues and overused
nondiagnostic cues. Various other lens model analyses have Multichannel Facilitation and Interference in Nonverbal
examined the role of particular nonverbal cues in social Perception The social-ecological approach suggests that
judgment, and in general the results suggest widespread modalities combine to inform social perception and judg-
use of nonverbal cues in judgment, even when those cues ment. If so, then judgment should be especially fast when
are not diagnostic (e.g., Gifford, Ng, & Wilkinson, 1985). several modalities impart the same information and are
Yet such featural analyses have several important limi- consistent. Moreover, judgment should be impaired or
tations. One unfortunate consequence of the study of dis- slowed when the to-be-judged modality imparts informa-
crete nonverbal cues is that while some of these cues may tion that is inconsistent with a second modality.
prove to be widespread across cultures and time in relation Multimodal facilitation is clearly demonstrated with
to psychological states and traits (e.g., emotion expres- infant identification of emotion expression. Walker-
sions), many may prove to be culturally specific, lending Andrews and colleagues (for a review, see Walker-Andrews,
to eventual difficulties in replication and in the production 2008) have demonstrated, for example, that 3-month-old
of scientific laws (Gergen, 1973). Complicating matters infants can recognize facial expressions of emotion but
further is that even within a culture the production of non- only when accompanied by a corresponding vocal expres-
verbal cues in one setting, such as a group conversation sion. Only later do infants develop the ability to recognize
(e.g., Gifford, 1994), may differ markedly from the pro- facial expressions in the absence of other communicative
duction of nonverbal cues in another setting, such as read- channels. At least with regard to emotion recognition, then,
ing a standard statement aloud (e.g., Borkenau & Liebler, developing humans require some degree of consistency to
1992). Hence, the importance of specific nonverbal cues learn the emotional meaning of any particular nonverbal
to social judgment may be both domain and situation spe- cue. Multimodal facilitation is also illustrated in observa-
cific (Ambady et al., 2000). Finally, lens models to date tions of patients. In one case, occipitotemporal damage
have disregarded the importance of configural processing was responsible for a patient’s visual agnosia, which pre-
by focusing on specific cues rather than on the interaction vented her from consciously recognizing emotion in the
of those cues (Zebrowitz & Collins, 1997). face (de Gelder, Pourtois, Vroomen, & Bachoud-Levi,
2000). Yet remarkably, exposure to facial expressions of
Configural Processes in Nonverbal Perception: emotion influenced her ability to recognize emotions in the
Ecological Theory One influential perceptual theory voice, demonstrating an automatic multimodal influence
(Gibson, 1979), holds that perceptual processes adapt to the in nonverbal judgment.
480 Nonverbal Behavior

Among adults, multimodal interference is illustrated one study used difficult-to-identify facial expressions and
by the so-called McGurk effect: When people are asked presented them alone, within a short “movie” in which
to identify vocal utterances and to ignore images of tar- the expresser began with a neutral expression (“dynamic”
gets moving their lips, they are unable to do so. These condition), or within an interrupted movie in which each
participants often misidentify the vocal utterance (“ba”) frame was separated by visual noise (Ambadar, Schooler, &
in the direction of the lip movement (“ga”; McGurk & Cohn, 2005). The single-frame and interrupted-movie con-
MacDonald, 1976). Hence, in perceiving a paraverbal ditions yielded significantly lower accuracy than did the
cue, a facial cue can interfere. One particularly fascinat- dynamic movie condition.
ing demonstration of multimodal interference used posed Thus, while features can be extracted from faces, higher-
facial expression pictures. These facial expressions were order patterns are clearly important to social perceivers
seamlessly photo-edited onto bodies that expressed a dif- encountering real nonverbal expressions. This idea can be
ferent emotion. For example, a prototypically angry face illustrated with respect to the combination of eye gaze and
was placed on a body that was holding a dirtied pair of emotion expression. Joy and anger appear to be consider-
underwear; a prototypically disgusted face was placed on a ably more intense and easier to identify when combined
body that was holding a fist in the air (as if about to strike with direct than with averted gaze, whereas the opposite
a blow). Although participants were instructed to ignore is true for sadness and fear (Adams & Kleck, 2003, 2005).
the body and focus on the face in making their emotion Adams and colleagues argue that congruence in motiva-
decision, they were unable to do so. Thus, disgust facial tional meaning (approach vs. avoidance) speeds responses
expressions were correctly identified 91% of the time whereas incongruence slows responses. For example, both
when presented alone, but when presented on bodies hold- joy and direct gaze signal approach, whereas both fear and
ing fists in the air, these faces were incorrectly judged to be averted gaze signal avoidance; hence, both of these combi-
angry by 87% of participants (Aviezer et al., 2008). nations should be and are processed especially quickly.
Finally, meaningful physiological states may be best
communicated via multiple channels. Although research Configuring Multiple Dimensions of Social Percep-
drawing a connection between physiological patterns and tion Just as higher-order patterns of nonverbal cues and
nonverbal behavior has typically examined physiological modalities inform social perception, ecological principles
responses to facial expressions, one recent study examined suggest that nonverbal patterns may combine with other
physiological and nonverbal responses to social interac- social categories and cues to produce affordances in social
tion (Weisbuch, Seery, Ambady, & Blascovich, 2009). The perception.
cardiovascular patterns indicating threat and challenge Gender, for example, clearly moderates the meaning
could be predicted by social perceivers’ judgments but of nonverbal behaviors. Crying babies are perceived as
only when judgments of facial and vocal confidence were considerably more angry when they are boys (Condry &
considered together. Condry, 1976). During a marital dispute, silence on the
In general, and consistent with social-ecological prin- part of husbands is considered loving whereas silence on
ciples, social perception processes appear to have adapted the part of wives is considered a sign of hostility (Gaelick,
to the dynamic information inherent to whole beings rather Bodenhausen, & Wyer, 1985). And happy faces are most
than to isolated parts of those humans. quickly and accurately categorized when those faces are
female (Hugenberg & Sczesny, 2006). Another social
Configural and Dynamic Processing of Nonverbal category (race) also moderates the meaning of nonverbal
Channels Although human heads do not normally behaviors. Although negative facial expressions automati-
express emotions when they are physically separated from cally elicit negative affect in social perceivers (Dimberg
their larger body, research suggests that facial emotion is et al., 2000; Murphy & Zajonc, 1993; Whalen et al., 1998),
perceived in accordance with the broader principles of eco- this effect is reversed when the expresser is an outgroup
logical theories. Specifically, the configural pattern among member (Weisbuch & Ambady, 2008a). In other research,
expressive facial features is important to the identification White social perceivers more readily perceived anger in
of nonverbal expressions. Removing these configural rela- Black faces than in White faces (and joy in White than
tions by inverting the images greatly increases the time nec- in Black faces), especially to the degree that White per-
essary to make an emotion judgment (the same is true for ceivers exhibited implicit prejudice or had a self-protection
body expressions; Calder & Jansen, 2005; Stekelenburg & goal (Hugenberg, 2005; Hugenberg & Bodenhausen, 2003;
de Gelder, 2004). In addition, supportive of an ecological Maner et al., 2005). At the neural level, Chiao and colleagues
account is that dynamic facial expressions appear to reveal (2008) found that the amygdala response of Japanese
emotion better than static facial expressions. For example, and American participants to facial fear was greater when
Nonverbal Foundations of Social Perception 481

the observed faces were members of their own culture 45-minute-old neonates imitate open-mouth and tongue
relative to members of the other culture. In general, the pat- protrusion displays (Meltzoff & Moore, 1983). There is
tern of findings in this section suggest that it is the mean- also the phenomenon of crying contagion described ear-
ing of the expression to the perceiver (the affordance)—not lier: On hearing another infant in the nursery cry, others
the expression on its own—that dictates the response. cry as well (Martin & Clark, 1982; Sagi & Hoffman, 1976;
Simner, 1971). And scholars agree that infants are able
Summary: Implications for Social Perception and to discriminate and not just imitate nonverbal displays of
Social Judgment emotion in the first few months of life (Flom & Bahrick,
There is substantial evidence that social perceivers nor- 2007; Walker-Andrews, 1997). For example, infants respond
mally form judgments based on the unique configuration differently to approving and disapproving vocalizations
of the nonverbal elements. Although these effects do not even when those vocalizations are spoken in a foreign lan-
rule out the possibility that single features provide impor- guage (Fernald, 1993).
tant information about the meaning of nonverbal behavior, As their minimal language skills increase, children
in real life nonverbal judgments proceed configurally and appear to be more and not less sensitive to nonverbal
are enhanced by dynamic movement. In other words, there behavior. For example, as infants grow from 3 to 7 months
appears to be substantial support for social-ecological the- of age, their skills at decoding nonverbal emotional dis-
ories with respect to the perception of nonverbal behavior. plays become increasingly sophisticated (e.g., Flom &
Nonetheless, when important multimodal or dynamic cues Bahrick, 2007). And infants begin to recognize body
are absent, single features may prove to be especially language and movement between 4 and 6 months of age
influential. (Fox & McDaniel, 1982). Indeed, nonverbal perception
skills increase throughout childhood and adolescence
(Rosenthal et al., 1979). In short, extremely young humans
Judgments of Nonverbal Behavior: Surprising
can clearly extract meaningful information from nonver-
Social Insight
bal behavior. To highlight the power of adult analogues to
The ability to judge nonverbal behavior may be fundamen- this skill, the next section details how nonverbal judgment
tal to human intelligence. is robust to many challenges.
Since the early 1980s, some scholars have argued for
Social Intelligence: How Low Can You Go?
the “social brain hypothesis,” which posits that the devel-
opment of the human brain is a direct consequence of the The 4-minute mile, the first flight to the moon, and the
complexities of human social life. In fact, the size of peaceful revolution of Gandhi were astounding feats that
the neocortex is correlated with social group size in cross- highlighted humans’ physical, mental, and spiritual capaci-
species comparisons (Dunbar, 1992, 1995; Gittleman, 1986; ties. Of course, it would not have been astounding to dem-
Marino, 1996) but not with other (nonsocial) candidate onstrate that people can run on two legs, that people can
causes of larger brains, such as ranging area and diffi- create, or that people can adhere to a moral code—even
culty of extracting food from the environment (Dunbar, though each of these latter capacities is rather astound-
1998). Human intelligence may thus be a by-product ing when compared with the abilities of other species.
of the need to interact in and keep track of an expansive Similarly, it would hardly be astounding to demonstrate
social group. For this reason, an understanding of how that people can make accurate social judgments if given
people achieve accuracy in social judgment is crucial to enough information, even if this capacity is astounding.
understanding the human psyche. To that end, social judg- By restricting the amount of physical information provided
ment relies heavily on nonverbal behavior both for what to social perceivers, it is possible to examine and perhaps
it communicates directly and for how it alters spoken highlight the human capacity for social intelligence.
content.
Temporally Limited Nonverbal Judgment The greatest
Social Intelligence: Early Development amount of research on degraded social judgment has
If people are to adapt to the social world early in life, it been accomplished in the domain of “thin slices” or brief
must be in the absence of linguistic understanding. We are observations of behavior. A meta-analysis of these stud-
born equipped with tools that enable such social adaptation ies revealed that social judgments made from less than
and perhaps form the basis for nonverbal judgments later in 30 seconds of strictly nonverbal exposure accurately pre-
life. Nonverbal sensitivity in humans begins prenatally, as dicted criterion variables with a rather large effect size (Ambady
people are born sensitive to the prosody of their mother’s & Rosenthal, 1992). Moreover, only negligible increases in
voice (DeCasper & Fifer, 1980). And as noted earlier, even accuracy were seen when exposure time was increased
482 Nonverbal Behavior

from 30 seconds to larger increments. Notably, provision Bradshaw, Field, Hampson, & Rose, 2005; Cutting &
of the verbal channel did not reliably enhance judgment Kozlowski, 1977; Johnson, Gill, Reichman, & Tassinary,
and in many cases detracted from judgment. Despite the 2007; Kozlowski & Cutting, 1977; Norman, Payton, Long, &
time limit and the elimination of verbal information, social Hawkes, 2004). Another technique, “content filtering,”
judgment operated efficiently and effectively. removes high-frequency sounds from speech such that it
It is possible that the robustness of social intelligence is not possible to derive words from the speech but vocal
might be limited to more easily observable domains, such prosody and other vocal parameters remain. From con-
as extraversion. Indeed, with self-report and peer report as tent-filtered speech, social perceivers’ judgments reliably
criteria measures, several research groups observed accu- predict job performance of managers and telephone opera-
racy in extraversion judgments based on brief exposure tors (Ambady, Krabbenhoft, & Hogan, 2006; Hecht &
to participants who varied only in nonverbal behavior LaFrance, 1995) and malpractice suits against vocalizing
(Borkenau & Liebler, 1992, 1993; Gangestad, Simpson, surgeons (Ambady et al., 2002). Although people are not
DiGeronimo, & Biek, 1992). Yet even in judging extra- good at detecting deception in general (as explained later),
version people exhibited surprising social intelligence. they are better than average when given only content-
For example, Borkenau and Liebler (1992) simply asked filtered speech (Zuckerman et al., 1981).
their social targets to read a weather report aloud to a video In sum, across both temporal and spatial restrictions of
camera in an otherwise empty room, yet social perceivers information, perceivers exhibit surprising social intelli-
exhibited accuracy in their extraversion judgments. gence as long as they have access to some nonverbal infor-
More importantly, accuracy in judgments from nonver- mation. Interestingly, people are often not aware of their
bal thin slices is hardly limited to extraversion. Among the ability to make accurate judgments, especially from subtle
more interesting variables that can be predicted via naïve cues, and their confidence in and predictions of their abil-
observers’ nonverbal thin-slice judgments are sexual ori- ity and their actual performance in this domain are not cali-
entation (Ambady, Hallahan, & Conner, 1999), sexual pro- brated (Patterson, Foster, & Bellmer, 2001; Rule, Ambady,
miscuity (Gangestad et al., 1992), racial biases (Richeson & Adams, & Macrae, 2008).
Shelton, 2005), trial judges’ expectations for trial outcome
Social Intelligence: Specific Domains
(Blanck, Rosenthal, & Cordell, 1985), and status within a
company (Hall & Friedman, 1999). Equally interesting is Intelligence in nonverbal judgment is especially notewor-
a study in which thin-slice judgments were based on targets thy with respect to deception detection, emotion recogni-
behaving in a context different from the outcome measure. tion, and individual differences. Each area provides unique
Specifically, judgments based on less than 1 minute of visual information about the importance and likely development
exposure to an informal conversation predicted the intelli- of social intelligence, and it is perhaps for this reason that
gence (IQ and peer reports of intelligence) of social targets these topics have attracted scholarly interest.
(Murphy, Hall, & Colvin, 2003).
In general, research on temporal limitations demon- Deception and Deception Detection Deception detection
strates that major reductions in the amount of physical is a particularly interesting area of research with respect
information available to social perceivers do little to stifle to social intelligence. Individuals can exhibit social intel-
social judgment ability, as long as nonverbal behavior is ligence by successfully deceiving others, whereas social
available. perceivers can exhibit social intelligence by successfully
detecting deception. Results of a large meta-analysis sug-
Spatially Limited Nonverbal Judgment In some work gest that social perceivers exhibit just-above-chance accu-
on social intelligence, special efforts are made to reduce racy in detecting deception (about 54%; Bond & DePaulo,
the amount of spatial information available to social per- 2006). When provided with only nonverbal (silent video)
ceivers. The most famous of these manipulations uses min- information, accuracy slips closer to chance (51%). These
iature light-emitting objects placed on social targets’ major effects suggest that social perceivers are not particularly
joints (e.g., elbows and knees). In a darkened space, social intelligent with respect to identifying lies. Although it is
targets perform some activity, often walking, such that possible that certain people are especially adept at detect-
their motion is visible but a great deal of important social ing deception, Bond and DePaulo (2008) demonstrated
information (e.g., the face) is not. In these studies on bio- that the standard deviation of judges’ true ability is less
logical motion, the light fixtures do not appear to constitute than 1%. And expertise, experience, and formal train-
a human until they begin to move. Given movement, social ing do not seem to improve lie detection accuracy. In a
perceivers are able to reliably determine identity (friend vs. meta-analysis of the literature, Aamodt and Custer (2006;
stranger), emotion, gender, sexuality, and behavior (Clarke, see also Vrij, 1993; Vrij & Semin, 1996) found that
Nonverbal Foundations of Social Perception 483

“professional lie catchers” (e.g., police officers, detectives, emotion: The patient was not better than chance at detecting
judges, secret service agents, and parole officers) were no visually presented squares versus circles, authentic ver-
more accurate at detecting deception than were students sus jumbled faces, male versus female faces, or positive
and other citizens. The professionals had an average accu- versus negative scenes. This patient exhibited more acti-
racy rate (56%) only slightly higher than that of novices vation in the right amygdala while viewing emotional ver-
(54%). Hence, it is unlikely that certain types of people are sus nonemotional faces, suggesting a neural pathway for
a great deal better than others at detection accuracy. Given emotion recognition.
the hypothesis that humans must have evolved social intel- Recognizing others’ emotion may be important for
ligence, why are people not better at detecting deception various reasons. Emotion expressions may provide imme-
from verbal or nonverbal cues? diate and dense information about (1) the current social
One explanation is that deception is a special domain. environment (Dimberg, 1997), (2) information about the
While perceivers benefit by making accurate assessments, expressive individual’s likely behavior (Owren et al.,
actors benefit by being able to deceive their counterparts. 2005), and (3) information relevant for demonstrating
Indeed, nonverbal influence theories generally describe a empathy and securing social ties (Preston & de Waal,
one-upmanship throughout the ages of human evolution 2002). “Affective blindsight” and the hundreds of studies
where, as social perceivers are increasingly able to dis- on the recognition of emotion from different channels of
criminate signal from noise in nonverbal behavior, it is communication suggest an important role for the recogni-
increasingly important for social actors to increase noise tion of nonverbal emotion in social adaptation. The sections
or reduce signal so as to accomplish their goals more often that follow illustrate the robustness of emotion recognition.
(Owren et al., 2005). Indeed, few nonverbal cues distin-
guish liars from truth-tellers, and those that do are gener- Emotion Recognition Across Cultures Emotion recog-
ally associated with small effect sizes (with the exception nition across cultures, where one might expect some error
of vocal immediacy and pupil dilation; DePaulo et al., due to substantial differences in accepted behavior, is sur-
2003). prisingly high. In one meta-analysis of 162 cross-cultural
Yet it is important to note that the bulk of research on studies (noted earlier), 99.6% of the studies demonstrated
deception detection comes from carefully controlled labo- that people detect emotion from posed pictures at above-
ratory studies, when the liar’s motivation to be success- chance levels, with only five studies indicating chance-
ful may be minimal. In a meta-analysis of the literature, level detection for even a single emotion (Elfenbein &
DePaulo and colleagues (2003) examined whether the cues Ambady, 2002). Although emotion recognition is gener-
to deception become more transparent during “high-stakes” ally lower from vocal than from facial samples, people
lies, when the liar has more motivation to be successful. also exhibit cross-cultural accuracy in detecting posed vo-
Their analysis revealed that when liars are more motivated cal emotion (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). In one study, for
to succeed, they become tenser; specifically, they use less example, actors emotionally enunciated sets of syllables
eye contact and a higher-pitched voice. Similar results constructed by a trained linguist to be language neutral.
were reported by Mann, Vrig, and Bull (2004) in a study Perceivers from nine countries (n > 31), speaking seven
of people’s behavior during real-life high-stakes situa- different languages then guessed which emotion was illus-
tions, including murder, rape, and arson suspects undergo- trated by each vocalization (Scherer, Banse, & Wallbott,
ing police interrogations. It is as yet unclear whether this 2001). Even the worst-performing country (Indonesia) cor-
greater transparency during higher-stakes situations reli- rectly identified each emotion at higher than chance, and
ably results in greater accuracy on the part of perceivers. on average cross-cultural accuracy (corrected for chance)
was a little higher than 60%.
Emotion Recognition Whereas social perceivers do not The evidence with respect to the recognition of
exhibit particular intelligence in deception detection, emo- spontaneous emotions across cultures is a bit more ambig-
tion recognition is a different story. Even some blind people uous, with some researchers reporting cross-cultural
can detect, at rates above chance, the emotion expression recognition and others reporting no cross-cultural recogni-
on a face presented to the blind visual field (de Gelder, tion (cf. Matsumoto & Willingham, 2006; Naab & Russell,
Vroomen, Pourtois, & Weiskrantz, 1999; Pegna, Khateb, 2007). Perhaps the most conservative way to summarize
Lazeyras, & Seghier, 2004). For example, one patient this literature is to say that spontaneous emotions are more
could not consciously detect movement, colors, or a strong difficult to identify than are posed emotions. This makes
light source yet was able to guess (at above-chance rates) sense since posed emotions are prototypes of the sponta-
the emotion expression on faces presented visually (Pegna neous emotions. Posed emotions evoke considerable uni-
et al.). Notably, these effects appeared to be specific to versal agreement, suggesting that social intelligence in
484 Nonverbal Behavior

the domain of emotion is guided by biologically prepared nonverbal expressions (i.e., facial expression, body move-
emotion prototypes. Speculatively, these prototypes may ment, and vocal tone). Specifically, this study used the
help to support intelligence in emotion recognition, whereas profile of nonverbal sensitivity (PONS; Rosenthal et al.,
spontaneous displays built on these prototypes may diverge 1979), which presents respondents with 2-second video
on the basis of culture (cf. Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). clips of an actress and asks respondents to indicate the
behavior illustrated by the actress (e.g., talking to a cashier
Emotion Recognition with More Subtle Stimuli In- vs. scolding a child). These are edited clips in which body
credibly, people can identify most of the basic emotion parts or aspects of the audio track have been eliminated or
facial expressions (joy, sadness, anger, and disgust) in altered. Third graders’ PONS accuracy was approximately
only 12 to 25 ms (McAndrew, 1986). People can also 58% for the body, whereas fourth graders’ accuracy was
reliably identify emotions from only body postures, whole- approximately 68%. Accuracy in nonverbal judgment
body movement, and even just arm movements (Atkinson, continues to improve in a linear fashion after sixth grade,
Dittrich, Gemmel, & Young, 2004; Pollick, Paterson, with “jumps” occurring from sixth grade to junior high and
Bruderlin, & Sanford, 2001). Touch also appears to convey from high school to college and with accuracy leveling off
emotion. In one study, anger, disgust, fear, gratitude, love, around age 25.
and sympathy could be differentially identified by perceiv-
ers who were touched on the arm or who viewed another Impaired Social Intelligence In the last decade, schol-
individual being touched on the arm (Hertenstein, Keltner, ars across disciplines have recognized the importance of
App, Bulleit, & Jaskolka, 2006). And although odor pro- intelligence in nonverbal judgment for normal social func-
duction is only debatably nonverbal communication, it tioning. Specifically, autism spectrum disorders, includ-
seems relevant to note that social perceivers could identify ing Asperger’s syndrome, appear to be undergirded by a
mood by smelling underarm pads worn by participants who lack of intelligence in nonverbal judgment (e.g., Baron-
had viewed mood-inducing movies (Chen & Haviland- Cohen, 2005). People with autism spectrum disorders are
Jones, 2000; see also Levine & McBurney, 1986). able to discriminate identity among faces but have much
If intelligence in nonverbal judgment reaches its nadir more difficulty discriminating among the negative facial
with identifying deception, it may reach its apex with iden- expressions (Ashwin, Chapman, Colle, & Baron-Cohen,
tifying emotion. Several basic emotions can be univer- 2006) and in identifying mental states from eye gaze cues
sally identified (across cultures) by sight or sound. Even (Baron-Cohen, Joliffe, Mortimore, & Robertson, 1997).
extremely subtle stimuli, such as those expressed for only Indeed, deficits in these areas are now viewed fundamental
12 to 25 ms or those expressed via body posture, can be to autism spectrum disorders rather than simple concomi-
reliably identified. Even partially blind people exhibit tants (Baron-Cohen, 2005). Abnormalities in the neural
visual emotion recognition. network known as the “social brain” (which consists of
the medial, inferior frontal, and superior temporal cortices
Intelligence in Nonverbal Judgment: Individual and the amygdala) may be responsible for these deficits
Differences (Brothers & Ring, 1992; Golan, Baron-Cohen, Hill, &
Between the apex of emotion recognition and the nadir of Golan, 2006).
deception detection lay considerable individual differences Similarly, research with schizophrenic patients reveals
in nonverbal judgment ability. that intelligence in nonverbal judgment may be fundamen-
tal to this disease. Various studies have demonstrated that,
Superior Social Intelligence Several groups are as compared with other psychiatric patients, schizophrenic
known for their impressive social intelligence. First, individuals have difficulty interpreting facial expressions in
across cultures, nonverbal channels, and ages, women particular and nonverbal behavior in general (e.g., Cutting,
appear to make more accurate nonverbal judgments than 1981; Sergi & Green, 2003). Moreover, such difficulty is
men (Hall & Andrzejewski, 2008; Rosip & Hall, 2004). associated with lower functional status (Penn, Spaulding,
These gender differences hold throughout childhood and Reed, & Sullivan, 1996; Sergi, Rassovsky, Nuechterlein, &
adulthood. Green, 2006), suggesting an important role for nonverbal
Second, nonverbal judgment appears to improve with judgment abilities in the development of schizophrenia.
age, up to a certain point. Rosenthal and colleagues used Finally, a recent meta-analysis demonstrated that people
an uncommonly age-diverse sample to demonstrate devel- who engage in antisocial behaviors, as well as those who
opmental changes in nonverbal judgment. This large-scale typically show a lack of empathy or remorse (e.g., psycho-
study used a heavily validated measure for examin- paths) show a specific deficit in the ability to recognize
ing accuracy in interpersonal judgment from particular fearful expressions (Marsh & Blair, 2007). Such findings
Nonverbal Social Influence 485

are perhaps unsurprising because the accurate perception (the face; Ekman & Friesen, 1969b; Rosenthal & DePaulo,
of fearful expressions likely contributes to the develop- 1979). If so, the social perceiver who picks up (“eaves-
ment of more prosocial behavior (Dadds et al., 2006). drops”) on vocal emotion may not be well appreciated by
In general, clinical deficits in psychosocial functioning individuals who are trying to conceal their true feelings. In
are associated with reductions in social intelligence, as one study, individuals who were especially good at pick-
indicated by judgment in nonverbal behavior. Nonverbal ing up on negative emotion in the voice (but not the face)
judgment skills may underlie several disorders that until were especially disliked by their supervisors and team-
recently had been characterized as disorders in cognitive mates in an organization (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002).
processing. This is an especially important area of research Other studies have also demonstrated the negative inter-
that would benefit from experimental and longitudinal personal ramifications of eavesdropping. For example,
designs to assess causality. eavesdropping among students was associated with poor
interpersonal functioning, as rated by teachers (Rosenthal &
Intelligence in Social Judgment: Social Impact DePaulo, 1979). Moreover, eavesdroppers appeared to
If superior social intelligence helps individuals to survive create especially uncomfortable social interactions with
and thrive, superior skill should be related to more positive strangers (Puccinelli & Tickle-Degnen, 2004).
outcomes. Intelligence in nonverbal judgment is related to Because of difficulties in causal interpretations, this is
several positive social outcomes. As compared to people an area of research that could benefit from experimental
with less intelligence in nonverbal judgment, those with methods. Training in nonverbal judgment skills—both
more social intelligence are more popular, report less lone- general and channel specific—followed by interpersonal
liness, receive better raises, and tend to attain higher status interaction would permit causal analyses of the extent to
(Byron, Terranova, & Nowicki, 2007; Hall & Halberstadt, which intelligence in nonverbal judgment causes enhanced
1994; Nowicki & Duke, 1994; Pitterman & Nowicki, popularity and psychosocial functioning.
2004; Rosenthal et al., 1979; for a meta-analysis, see Hall,
Summary: Judgments of Nonverbal Behavior
Andrzejewski, & Yopchick, in press).
At first glance, these findings suggest that much can Much like running on two legs, social intelligence is often
be gained from heightened intelligence in nonverbal taken for granted. Yet social intelligence is a remarkable
judgment. Yet most studies on this topic suffer from inter- feat of evolution that is perhaps responsible for the abilities
pretive issues—it could be the case, for example, that as of humans to live in groups, to function interpersonally, and
a consequence of becoming popular or high status, indi- to promote the interests of their genes within a social envi-
viduals attend more to others’ nonverbal behaviors and ronment. For these reasons, intelligence in nonverbal judg-
enhance their nonverbal judgments. The causal role of ment may have played an important role in the survival of
nonverbal judgment in social functioning thus remains the human species. A number of empirical findings support
unclear. Moreover, skill at nonverbal judgment does this inference. First, there is a universal ability to decode
seem to depend on the channel of communication being affective facial and vocal expressions. Second, people
judged. People who make accurate judgments from the need little time to come to relatively accurate conclusions
face are not necessarily equally good at making judg- about personality traits on the basis of nonverbal behav-
ments from the voice, and people who are good at judg- ior. Third, people need only limited information from the
ing emotion are not necessarily good at judging deception body, face, or voice to come to accurate conclusions about
(e.g., Buck, 1976; Hall, 2001). Recent work suggests the many characteristics of others. Fourth, intelligence deficits
intriguing possibility that specific nonverbal skills might in nonverbal judgment appear to play an important role in
be associated with superior performance on specific out- the definition of psychiatric abnormality, including autism
comes. For instance, Marsh and colleagues (2007) found and schizophrenia, suggesting that nonverbal judgment is
that accuracy at judging fear expressions predicted proso- crucial for social adaptation. And finally, intelligence in
cial tendencies better than did accuracy at judging other nonverbal judgment is associated with affiliation.
emotions.
Moreover, in some cases, the ability to pick up on emo-
tions from nonverbal behavior is actually associated with NONVERBAL SOCIAL INFLUENCE
negative outcomes. In particular, people may really want
to keep some of their feelings secret. Although they may Intelligence in conscious nonverbal judgment is clearly
be successful regulating their facial expressions at these important to social adaptation. However, the many auto-
times, vocal and bodily expressions are thought to be more matic processes involved in the perception of nonverbal
difficult to control—hence, they may “leak” true feelings behavior also allow for efficient social adaptation in the
486 Nonverbal Behavior

absence of conscious judgment. This capacity implies an Indirect Nonverbal Influence


important role for nonverbal behavior in social influence.
Indirect nonverbal influence occurs when the beliefs and
Most social influence articles use verbal communication
attitudes of the self change after perceiving another’s non-
as the medium of influence (Weisbuch & Ambady, 2008c).
verbal responses to some stimuli. Among animals and from
Conformity is often manifested by individuals speaking
childhood to adulthood, there is impressive continuity in
aloud (“Line A is shorter”) or by written statements about
the power of indirect nonverbal influence.
what most people believe (“78% of people like choco-
late”). Persuasion research almost exclusively employs
Nonhuman Animals
written or spoken arguments, partly due to conceptual-
izations of what counts as persuasion. In both domains, Long before there was human language, organisms were
researchers occasionally examine the role of nonverbal responding to conspecifics’ facial expressions, body lan-
cues, but beliefs are still transmitted via verbal behavior. guage, gestures, and nonlinguistic vocalizations (Darwin,
And in these cases, the examination of nonverbal cues is 1872). Judging by the diversity of organisms that exhibit
folded into frameworks based on verbal communication meaningful responses to nonverbal behavior, indirect non-
and these cues are treated as moderator variables (Burgoon, verbal influence may be universal in the animal kingdom.
Dunbar, & Segrin, 2002). As but one of many examples, chukar partridge chicks only
Yet nowhere in most definitions of social influence eat food items that their mother has pointed at with a partly
is there a requirement that beliefs be expressed verbally. opened bill (Avital & Jablonka, 2000). Among fish, min-
At the broadest level, social influence can be conceptual- nows are sensitive to the “fright behavior” (e.g., dashing
ized as the influence of one person’s expressed beliefs on or freezing) of other minnows such that perceiver min-
another’s personal beliefs, and beliefs may be expressed nows who have observed another minnow’s fright behavior
nonverbally, through facial expressions, body language, in response to an object come to avoid that object as well
paraverbal behavior, and subtle touch. If only because of (Mathis, Chivers, & Smith, 1996). And meerkats are sen-
the ubiquity of nonverbal communication, there is tre- sitive to nonlinguistic vocalizations, such that one meerkat
mendous potential for nonverbal social influence. That vocalization may produce one type of group behavior
is, people enact a constant stream of nonverbal behavior (harassment of an otherwise predatory cobra) whereas
but even the most talkative people spend a great deal of another slightly different vocalization might produce another
time not speaking or writing. It is impossible to not behave group response (e.g., flight; Avital & Jablonka, 2000).
nonverbally—even the absence of movement may be con- Among nonhuman primates, parents’ nonverbal
sidered nonverbal behavior, as when a first date leads a responses to various animals influence the formation of
nervous person to remain perfectly still. Speaking with predator–nonpredator categorization schemes of young
a monotone voice is likely to compel inferences of dull- vervet monkeys (Cheney & Seyfarth, 1990). Moreover,
ness, a lack of facial expression often leads inferences of baboons develop dislike for particular colors of banana
aloofness, and nonverbal stiffness often leads to inferences after observing other baboons’ negative facial and
of anxiety (Banse & Scherer, 1996; LeDoux, 2000; Tickle- bodily responses to these bananas (Jouventin, Pasteur, &
Degnen & Lyons, 2004). Any social activity that can be Cambefort, 1976). Likewise, rhesus monkeys develop a
imagined includes a constant stream of nonverbal behavior, fear of snakes after observing conspecifics’ fear responses
whereas only some of these activities include verbal behav- to snakes (e.g., Mineka, Davidson, Cook, & Keir, 1984).
ior. Thus, we must be exposed to more nonverbal than ver- And our nearest nonhuman ancestor—the chimpanzee—is
bal behavior. The anthropologist Ray Birdwhistell (1970) sensitive to emotional facial expressions and body lan-
claimed that people only speak for about 10 to 11 minutes guage (Nakayama, 2004; Parr, Waller, & Vick, 2007).
per day. Although it is difficult to be precise, scholars have Social influence in the animal kingdom always occurs
estimated that within dyadic interaction, there is twice in the absence of language. That does not ensure that
as much nonverbal as verbal behavior (Birdwhistell, such influence is always “nonverbal,” since much animal
1970; Knapp, 1978). Nonverbal behavior is—almost by learning may occur via explicit behaviors and physical
definition—more prevalent than verbal behavior. The manipulations. Nonetheless, the main point is that indirect
widespread prevalence of nonverbal behavior suggests nonverbal influence is common to a diversity of animals
nonverbal social influence may be widespread. Although with whom we share much evolutionary history.
this potential has only recently been the subject of formal
Childhood
examination in social psychology, the following sections
review two forms of nonverbal influence by integrating lit- As with other animals, if young infants are to be socialized
eratures across disciplines. in their first year or so, it must be in the absence of language.
Nonverbal Social Influence 487

Shortly after birth and through the first year, infants have reported disliking the taste of the drink more following
impressive nonverbal perception skills (reviewed earlier) this manipulation (Baeyens, Vansteenwegen, de Houwer, &
coupled with a nearly complete absence of linguistic skills. Crombez, 1996). These studies thus provide initial evi-
It appears that the early emergence of nonverbal percep- dence for the special power of nonverbal behavior in social
tion permits social influence via nonverbal behavior. influence—in particular the role of indirect nonverbal
Specifically, infants adopt evaluations toward objects by influence.
observing others’ nonverbal behavior. In the presence of In summary, nonverbal communication is necessary
an adult, especially a caretaker, infants reference the care- for the transmission of beliefs in the first few months of
taker’s facial and vocal expressions to determine the value life and appears to play an important role in the forma-
or danger inherent to a nearby object or potential behavior tive years even after language has been acquired. Hence,
(Klinnert, 1984; Klinnert, Emde, Butterfield, & Campos, nonverbal social influence may indeed be one of the pri-
1986; Mumme & Fernald, 2003; Repacholi & Meltzoff, mary mechanisms through which culture is transferred to
2007; Sorce, Emde, Campos, & Klinnert, 1985). This children’s minds.
phenomenon is referred to as social referencing or, here,
Adults
“nonverbal social referencing.” One illustration of this
effect is that a toy is especially likely to be approached if Only recently have the nonverbal influence processes seen
presentation of the toy is simply coupled with others’ posi- in children and apes also been observed in human adults. In
tive facial expressions. This is social influence in that the one recent study (Weisbuch & Ambady, 2009), female
infant’s beliefs about the toy (whether implicit or explicit) participants were exposed to a series of brief and silent
have been influenced by the beliefs expressed via the video clips featuring actresses of varying body size. As
nonverbal behavior of another individual. the actresses’ body sizes (barely) increased, they received
One set of studies in particular demonstrates sophistica- either increasingly positive nonverbal behavior from
tion in how nonverbal social influence contributes to infant other characters (pro-heavy condition) or increasingly
belief and behavior. In these studies, infants watched two negative nonverbal behavior from the other characters
experimenters—one experimenter performed an action (pro-slim condition). Females in the pro-slim condition
toward a toy, while the second experimenter (“emoter”) reported desiring a slimmer body size and admiring slim
directed a negative or neutral nonverbal expression at the women more than did females in the pro-heavy condition.
first experimenter. Infants took longer to touch the toys, Remarkably, separate groups of participants could not con-
touched them for a shorter period, and were less likely to sciously identify the (pro-heavy or pro-slim) patterns in
imitate the first experimenter when the emoter had (vs. these clips even when they were provided with a substan-
had not) expressed anger toward the first experimenter’s tial monetary award. Hence, indirect and nonverbal social
behavior (Repacholi & Meltzoff, 2007). Although it is dif- influence continues to exert an (automatic) social influence
ficult to measure beliefs in infants, these findings strongly among adults.
suggest an indirect and nonverbal social influence. Other research supports a similar conclusion regarding
Following infancy into toddlerhood and preschool, nonverbal influence, although through a slightly differ-
nonverbal social referencing is used extensively by the ent mechanism. For example, in one study, one member
developing child (Feinman, 1982; Walden & Ogan, 1988). of a dyad was manipulated to be especially happy just
Perhaps of greater interest is that the social influence of before the dyad inspected a product together. Participants
nonverbal behavior may be stronger than the social influ- interacting with a happy (and silent) dyad partner liked
ence of verbal behavior in 3- to 6-year-old children. In one the product more than those interacting with a neutral
study, for example, 3- to 6-year-old children observed a mood dyad partner. Crucially, these effects were mediated
videotaped social interaction between a White adult and by nonverbal emotional contagion (the amount of smil-
a Black adult (Castelli, de Dea, & Nesdale, 2008). The ver- ing passed from the dyad partner to the participant during
bal and nonverbal behavior of the White adult was varied product inspection). Indeed, when nonverbal exposure
orthogonally. Children’s attitudes toward the Black adult was blocked (via a translucent screen), no attitude change
were more positive when the nonverbal behavior of the occurred (Howard & Gengler, 2001; see also Ramanathan &
White adult (toward the Black adult) was positive (vs. neg- McGill, 2007; Tanner, Ferraro, Chartrand, Bettman, & van
ative). In a second study, these effects generalized to atti- Baaren, 2008).
tudes toward other Black adults. No similar effect emerged Research more commonly labeled “evaluative condi-
for verbal behavior. In a different study, similar results were tioning” also supports the role of nonverbal social influ-
observed among children who had observed the pairing of ence in belief acquisition. Findings in this domain are
drink ingestion with a negative facial expression—children especially noteworthy in that they model a process that
488 Nonverbal Behavior

is likely to happen often in the “real world”: the devotion Compliance in Nonverbal Social Influence:
of limited conscious resources to nonverbal emotional Behavioral Confirmation
behavior in the presence of other objects. For example, Behavioral confirmation occurs when the expectations
happy facial expressions presented subliminally and just of another are spontaneously confirmed by the target.
before a novel symbol facilitate positive evaluations of that For example, if a White interviewer expects poor perfor-
novel symbol, as compared with when the symbol is pre- mance from a Black interviewee, the White interviewer
ceded by an angry subliminal expression (e.g., Murphy & will exhibit negative nonverbal behavior and the Black
Zajonc, 1993). These effects occur whether evaluations of interviewee will, as a consequence, perform more poorly
the symbols are measured with self-report evaluations or (Word, Zanna, & Cooper, 1974). Meta-analyses con-
facial electromyography (Rotteveel et al., 2001). In addi- ducted by Harris and Rosenthal (1985) revealed that to the
tion, subliminal happy expressions facilitate drinking extent that the sender maintained (with the receiver) eye
among thirsty people (Winkielman et al., 2005), sublim- contact (r ⫽ 0.33), little interpersonal distance (r ⫽ 0.45),
inal sad faces make music seem gloomy (Strahan et al., a high frequency of smiles (r ⫽ 0.29), and a fast speech
2002), subliminal facial expressions influence ratings of rate (r ⫽ 0.48), the receiver was likely to be influenced
other people (Stapel et al., 2002), and subliminal happy and confirm the sender’s positive expectations. Nonverbal
faces can increase positive evaluations of television pro- behavioral confirmation occurs in many domains ranging
grams (Ravaja et al., 2004). Moreover, happy faces paired from job interviews (Word et al., 1974), to courtrooms
with self-relevant behavior facilitate positive evaluations (Blanck et al., 1985), to experimenter–participant interac-
associated with the self (Baccus, Baldwin, & Packer, tions (Rosenthal, 1966). Expectations communicated non-
2004). And when perceivers are exposed to faces looking verbally are so influential as to exert cross-species effects.
at objects, the displayed facial emotion influences evalua- For example, experimenters’ expectations can nonverbally
tions of the objects (Bayliss, Frischen, Fenske, & Tipper, influence rats’ maze-running and Skinner-box performance
2007). Although many of these latter findings are often (Rosenthal & Lawson, 1964) and at least one horse’s count-
considered an aspect of “conditioning” rather than social ing ability (“Clever Hans”; Pfungst, 1911).
influence, it is not clear—given the role of human stimuli— Participants’ attitudes and beliefs can also be influ-
that these effects should not also be considered social enced by nonverbally communicated expectations. For
influence. example, trial judges influence jury verdicts via nonver-
bal behavior directed at the jury (Halverson, Hallahan,
Direct Nonverbal Influence in Adults Hart, & Rosenthal, 1997; Hart, 1995). In one study, Hart
(1995) took videotaped footage of judges’ final instruc-
In the studies reviewed thus far, the impact of the nonverbal tions to actual juries and showed this footage to mock
behavior was indirect—it was more about the relationship jurors who had just observed evidence from a separate
between a nonverbal behavior and a second stimulus than (but real) trial. Judges who expected “innocent” (vs.
the relationship between the nonverbal behavior and the “guilty”) verdicts in the original trials were especially
self. Direct nonverbal influence occurs when the nonver- likely to elicit “innocent” verdicts from the mock jurors.
bal behavior of another individual directed toward the self Because the judges gave identical (impartial) verbal
influences the beliefs and attitudes of the self. Indirect non- instructions to jurors, the nonverbal behavior of judges
verbal influence effects are perhaps analogous to persua- directed at the (mock) jurors must have influenced the
sion and conformity, whereas direct nonverbal influence jurors’ beliefs about a third party. The judges elicited
effects may be more analogous to compliance. For example, compliance to their expectations via nonverbal behavior.
among chimps, the “silent, bared-teeth” display, perhaps On the whole, it appears that compliance can be gained
similar to human smiles, elicits affiliative behavior in per- via nonverbal influence.
ceivers of the expression, and the “relaxed, open-mouth”
face seems to reduce aggression and increase interest in
Compliance in Nonverbal Social Influence:
“play” among conspecifics that perceive the facial expres-
Dominance Displays
sion (Waller & Dunbar, 2005). And wolf pack leaders are
thought to be especially likely to lead the pack in a hunt Individuals influence others and gain their compliance via
after being nuzzled by other wolves in their pack (e.g., dominance-related nonverbal behavior. This is perhaps an
Mech, 1970). Among humans, two types of direct nonver- artifact of our genetic relationship with other primates. For
bal influence have received the most attention from social example, young monkeys isolated since birth exhibit a fear
psychologists: behavioral confirmation and dominance response when shown a picture of an older male monkey
displays. making a threat display (Sackett, 1966). Although humans
References 489

sometimes direct such anger or dominance displays at The area of nonverbal behavior has been criticized for
others, nonverbal dominance expressions more commonly a lack of theoretical development. Part of the problem is
observed in humans include patterns of gestures (Hall that, as documented in this chapter, nonverbal behavior
et al., 2005), gaze (Ellyson & Dovidio, 1985), and pos- is ubiquitous in so many domains of behavior. And within
tural expansion (Tiedens & Fragale, 2003). The expres- each domain, specific theoretical axioms hold true. Indeed,
sion of these dynamic cues gains behavioral compliance theories have been developed regarding specific domains
(submissiveness) among humans (e.g., Tiedens & Fragale, of nonverbal behavior. For instance, elegant theories
2003) and other primates (Cheney & Seyfarth, 1990). For have been generated and tested regarding rapport (Tickle-
example, stared-at drivers are more likely to flee an inter- Degnen & Rosenthal, 1990), nonverbal social influence
section, leaving this territory to the staring pedestrian (Weisbuch & Ambady, 2008b), and social-ecological per-
(Ellsworth, Carlsmith, & Henson, 1972). Likewise, a “dom- ception (McArthur & Baron, 1983). Recently, more com-
inance stare” directed at people on an elevator speeded exit prehensive theories have been generated that attempt to
time of elevator riders (Elman, Schulte, & Bukoff, 1977). cross and bridge the different domains of nonverbal behav-
In each case, compliance occurred as a consequence of ior (e.g., Patterson, 2006). We anticipate much progress in
nonverbal dominance directed toward the self. theoretical development in this area in the near future.
The idea that responses to nonverbal dominance might We began this chapter by citing Humphrey’s (1976)
include enduring changes to explicit beliefs about the self assertion that our intellect developed to solve social, as
was presaged by Dovidio and Ellyson (1985), who sug- opposed to cognitive, problems. In the same essay, he
gested the following: asserted that “Experimental psychologists in Britain have
tended to regard social psychology as a poor country
Messages concerning dominance and relative status may be cousin of their subject—gauche, undisciplined, and slightly
communicated and accepted without conscious awareness by absurd” (p. 308). More than 20 years have passed since
the sender . . . . Interactants may come to explain their behav- this claim, and in those years social psychology has made
ior with self-attributions that are consistent with their status impressive strides. Indeed, experimental psychologists
relationship. (p. 146) and neuroscientists (in Britain and beyond) now appreci-
Hence, a social influence approach to nonverbal domi- ate the fundamental contributions of social psychology as
nance displays need not be limited to examining how they explore topics that have long been central to social
nonverbal dominance cues moderate the impact of a verbal psychologists who study nonverbal behavior—topics such
message (Carli, LaFleur, & Loeber, 1995; Lee & Ofshe, as how we perceive and understand the emotions, disposi-
1981). tions, goals, and intentions of other people and how we
Given the tremendous capacity of the human mind to communicate our emotions, dispositions, goals, and inten-
process information automatically, the nonconscious sen- tions to them.
sitivity of the human mind to nonverbal behavior, and the
ubiquity of nonverbal behavior, a socially situated human
mind should be vulnerable to an enormous amount of non-
REFERENCES
verbal social influence occurring outside awareness of that
influence and even outside awareness of the nonverbal
Aamodt, M. G., & Custer, H. (2006). Who can best catch a liar? A meta-
behavior itself (Weisbuch & Ambady, 2008b). analysis of individual differences in deception detection. Forensic
Examiner, Spring 2006, 6–11.
Abe, J. A., & Izard, C. E. (1999). A longitudinal study of emotion expres-
SUMMARY sion and personality relations in early development. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 566–577.
If many of our social cognitive structures are formed early Abrami, P. C., Leventhal, L., & Perry, R. P. (1982). Educational seduction.
Review of Educational Research, 52, 446–464.
in life to enable nonverbal behavior, if the basis of social
Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal
life in primates is rooted in nonverbal behavior, and if infants. Child Development, 59, 450–466.
social perception, cognition, judgment, relations, and Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the
influence are heavily informed by nonverbal processes, it processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14,
seems reasonable to conclude that nonverbal behavior is 644–647.
an important foundation for social adaptation in humans. Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on
the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 3–11.
We hope we have illustrated the critical role that nonverbal
Adolphs, R. (2006). Perception and emotion: How we recognize facial
behavior plays in influencing basic social-psychological expression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15,
processes. 222–226.
490 Nonverbal Behavior

Allessandri, S. M., Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (1990). Violation of Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social
expectancy and frustration in early infancy. Devlopmental Psychology, behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on
26, 738–744. action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244.
Ambadar, Z., Schooler, J. W., & Cohn, J. F. (2005). Deciphering the Bargh, J. A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information process-
enigmatic face: The importance of facial dynamics in interpreting ing and social perception: The influence of trait information presented
subtle facial expressions. Psychological Science, 16, 403–410. outside of conscious awareness on impression formation. Journal of
Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 437–449.
of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behav- Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). The empathizing system: A revision of the 1994
ioral stream. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social version of the mind-reading system. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund (Eds.),
psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 201–271). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Origins of the social mind (pp. 468–492). New York: Guilford Press.
Ambady, N., & Gray, H. M. (2002). On being sad and mistaken: Mood Baron-Cohen, S., Joliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997).
effects on the accuracy of thin-slice judgments. Journal of Personality Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high
and Social Psychology, 83, 947–961. functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Conner, B. (1999). Accuracy of judgments of Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.
sexual orientation from thin slices of behavior. Journal of Personality Barr, C. L., & Kleck, R. E. (1995). Self–other perception of the intensity of
and Social Psychology, 77, 538–547. facial expressions of emotion: Do we know what we show? Journal of
Ambady, N., Krabbenhoft, M. A., & Hogan, D. (2006). The 30-sec. sale: Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 608–618.
Using thin slices to evaluate sales effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain
Psychology, 16, 4–13. Sciences, 22, 577–609.
Ambady, N., LaPlante, M. A., Nguyen, T., Rosenthal, R., Chaumeton, N., & Barsalou, L. W., Niedenthal, P. M., Barbey, A., & Ruppert, J. (2003). Social
Levinson, W. (2002). Surgeons’ tone of voice: A clue to malpractice embodiment. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and moti-
history. Surgery, 132, 5–9. vation (Vol. 43, pp. 43–92). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire
as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.
Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Anderson, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy in interpersonal communi- Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as
cation. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel integra- the science of self-reports and finger movements: Or, whatever hap-
tions of nonverbal behavior (pp. 1–36). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. pened to actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science,
Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language 396–403.
development. Human Development, 28, 40–49. Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Affective
Argyle, M., Alkema, F., & Gilmour, R. (1971). The communication of evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and
friendly and hostile attitudes by verbal and nonverbal signals. European emotional expression. Cognition, 104, 644–653.
Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 385–402. Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseu-
Argyle, M., Salter, V., Nicholson, H., Williams, M., & Burgess, P. (1970). dosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process
The communication of inferior and superior attitudes by verbal and in mother–infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social
nonverbal signals. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, Psychology, 54, 243–253.
9, 230. Bernstein, I. S. (1981). Dominance: The baby and the bathwater. Behavioral
Ashwin, C., Chapman, E., Colle, L., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Impaired and Brain Sciences, 4, 419–458.
recognition of negative basic emotions in autism: A test of the amyg- Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and context. Philadelphia: University
dala theory. Social Neuroscience, 1, 349–363. of Pennsylvania Press.
Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmel, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Blake, J., McConnell, S., Horton, G., & Benson, N. (1992). The gestural
Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in repertoire and its development over the second year. Early Development
point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746. and Parenting, 1, 127–136.
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., Blanck, P. D., Rosenthal, R., & Cordell, L. D. H. (1985). The appearance of
Moscovitch, M., & Bentin, S. (2008). Angry, disgusted or afraid? justice: Judges’ verbal and nonverbal behaviour in criminal jury trials.
Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Stanford Law Review, 38, 89–164.
Science, 19, 724–732.
Bond, C. F., Jr., & DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judg-
Avital, E., & Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal traditions: Behavioral inheri- ments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 214–234.
tance in evolution. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press. Bond, C. F., Jr., & DePaulo, B. M. (2008). Individual differences in judging
deception: Accuracy and bias. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 477–492.
Baccus, J. R., Baldwin, M. W., & Packer, D. J. (2004). Increasing implicit
self-esteem through classical conditioning. Psychological Science, 15, Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1992). Trait inferences: Sources of validity
498–502. at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
62, 645–657.
Baeyens, F., Vansteenwegen. D., de Houwer, J., & Crombez, G. (1996).
Observational conditioning of food valence in humans. Appetite, 27, Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1993). Convergence of stranger ratings of
235–250. personality and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and mea-
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emo- sured intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,
tion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 546–553.
614–636. Brinol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2003). Overt head movements and persuasion:
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, effi- A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
ciency, intention, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & 84, 1123–1139.
T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–40). Briton, N. J., & Hall, J. A. (1995). Beliefs and female and male nonverbal
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. communication. Sex Roles, 32, 79–90.
References 491

Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. M. (2002). The importance of the eyes: How Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The
infants interpret adult looking behaviour. Developmental Psychology, perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality
38, 958–966. and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. M. (2005). The development of gaze following Chen, D., & Haviland-Jones, J. (2000). Human olfactory communication
and its relationship to language. Developmental Science, 8, 535–543. of emotion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 771–781.
Brothers, L., & Ring, B. (1992). A neuroethological framework for the repre- Cheney, D., & Seyfarth, R. (1990). How monkeys see the world: Inside
sentation of other minds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 107–118. the mind of another species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psycho- Cheney, D., & Seyfarth, R. (2007). Baboon metaphysics. Chicago:
logical experiments. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. University of Chicago Press.
Buck, R. (1976). Human motivation and emotion. New York: Wiley. Chiao, J. Y., Iidaka, T., Gordon, H. L., Nogawa, J., Bar, M., Aminoff, E.,Sadato,
Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford Press. N., & Ambady, N. (2008). Cultural specificity in amygdala response to
fear faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2167–2174.
Buck, R., Losow, J. I., Murphy, M. M., & Costanzo, P. (1992). Social
facilitation and inhibition of emotional expression and communication. Clarke, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 962–968. (2005). The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light
displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception, 34, 1171–1180.
Burgoon, J. K. (1991). Relational message interpretations of touch, con-
versational distance, and posture. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, Cole, P. M. (1986). Children’s spontaneous control of facial expression.
233–259. Child Development, 57, 1309–1321.

Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., Hale, J. L., & de Turck, M. (1984). Relational Condry, J., & Condry, S. (1976). Sex differences: A study of the eye of the
messages associated with nonverbal behaviors. Human Communication beholder. Child Development, 47, 812–819.
Research, 10, 351–378. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social
Burgoon, J. K., Dunbar, N. E., & Segrin, C. (2002). Nonverbal influence. In exchange. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The
J. P. Dillard & M. Pfau (Eds.), The persuasion handbook: Developments adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture
in theory and practice (pp. 445–473). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. (pp. 163–228). New York: Oxford University Press.

Burns, R. (1853). The complete poetical works of Robert Burns. Cutting, J. (1981). Judgement of emotional expression in schizophrenics.
New York: D. Appleton. British Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1–6.

Burrows, A. M., Waller, B. M., Parr, L. A., & Bonar, C. J. (2006). Muscles Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognition of friends by
of facial expression in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Descriptive, their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the
comparative, and phylogenetic contexts. Journal of Anatomy, 208, Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.
153–167. Dadds, M. R., Perry, Y., Hawes, D. J., Merz, S., Riddell, A. C., Haines, D. J.,
et al. (2006). Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in child
Buss, K. A., & Kiel, E. J. (2004). Comparison of sadness, anger, and
psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 280–281.
fear facial expressions when toddlers look at their mothers. Child
Development, 75, 1761–1773. Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals.
London: John Murray.
Byron, K., Terranova, S., & Nowicki, S. (2007). Nonverbal emotion recog-
nition and salespersons: Linking ability to perceived and actual success. DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 2600–2619. prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.
Calder, A. J., Beaver, J. D., Winston, J. S., Dolan, R. J., Jenkins, R., Eger, E., DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and self-representation.
et al. (2007). Separate coding of different gaze directions in the supe- Psychological Bulletin, 111, 203–243.
rior temporal sulcus and inferior parietal lobule. Current Biology, 17, de Gelder, B., Pourtois, G., Vroomen, J., & Bachoud-Levi, A. C. (2000).
20–25. Covert processing of faces in prosopagnosia is restricted to facial
Calder, A. J., & Jansen, J. (2005). Configural coding of facial expressions: expressions: Evidence from cross-modal bias. Brain and Cognition,
The impact of inversion and photographic negative. Visual Cognition, 44, 425–444.
12, 495–518. de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., Pourtois, G., & Weiskrantz, L. (1999). Non-
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emergent themes conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex.
in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. NeuroReport, 10, 3759–3763.
Developmental Psychology, 25, 394–402. DePaulo, B. M., & Coleman, L. M. (1986). Talking to children, foreigners,
Caporael, L. R. (1981). The paralanguage of caregiving: Babytalk to the and retarded adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,
institutionalized aged. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 945–959.
40, 876–884. DePaulo, B. M., & Coleman, L. M. (1987). Verbal and nonverbal commu-
Carli, L., LaFleur, S. J., & Loeber, C. C. (1995). Nonverbal behavior, gen- nication of warmth to children, foreigners, and retarded adults. Journal
der, and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, of Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 75–88.
1030–1041. DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Nonverbal communication.
Castelfranchi, C., & Poggi, I. (1990). Blushing as discourse: Was Darwin In D. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social
wrong? In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness and embarrassment: psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 3–40). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Perspectives from social psychology (pp. 230–254). Cambridge, DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton,
England: Cambridge University Press. K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin,
Castelli, L., de Dea, C., & Nesdale, D. (2008). Learning social attitudes: 129, 74–118.
Children’s sensitivity to the nonverbal behaviors of adult models Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and
during interracial interactions. Personality and Social Psychology informational social influences upon individual judgment. Journal of
Bulletin, 34, 1504–1511. Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629–636.
Chaikin, A., Sigler, E., & Derlega, V. (1974). Nonverbal mediators Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and con-
of teacher expectancy effects. Journal of Personality and Social trolled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology, 30, 144–149. 56, 5–18.
492 Nonverbal Behavior

de Waal, F. B. M. (1982). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Henson, A. (1972). The stare as a
apes. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. stimulus to flight in human subjects: A series of field experiments.
de Waal, F. B. M. (1988). The communicative repertoire of captive bono- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 302–311.
bos (Pan paniscus) compared to that of chimpanzees. Behaviour, 106, Ellyson, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1985). Power, dominance, and nonverbal
183–251. behavior: New York: Springer-Verlag.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior express- Elman, D., Schulte, D. C., & Bukoff, A. (1977). Effects of facial expression
way: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In M. P. and stare duration on walking speed: Two field experiments. Journal of
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. Nonverbal Behavior, 2, 93–99.
1–40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Exline, R. V., Ellyson, S. L., & Long, B. (1975). Visual behavior as
DiMatteo, M. R. (1979). A social-psychological analysis of physician– an aspect of power role relationships. In P. Pliner, L. Krames, &
patient rapport: Toward a science of the art of medicine. Journal of T. Alloway (Eds.), Nonverbal communication of aggression (pp.
Social Issues, 35, 13–33. 21– 52). New York: Plenum Press.
Dimberg, U. (1982). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial Farroni, T., Massaccesi, S., Pividori, D., & Johnson, M. H. (2004). Gaze
expressions. Psychophysiology, 19, 643–647. following in newborns. Infancy, 5, 39–60.
Dimberg, U. (1990). Facial electromyography and emotional reactions. Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer
Psychophysiology, 27, 481–494. Quarterly, 28, 445–470.
Dimberg, U. (1997). Psychophysiological reactions to facial expres- Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American
sions. In U. Segerstrale & P. Molnar (Eds.), Nonverbal commu- Anthropologist, 66, 103–114.
nication: Where nature meets culture (pp. 47–60). Mahwah, NJ: Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to
Erlbaum. vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development,
Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial 64, 657–674.
reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11, Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions
86–89. of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive
Dovidio, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1985). Patterns of visual dominance. Science, 11, 77–83.
In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, and Flom, R., & Bahrick, L. E. (2007). The development of infant discrimi-
nonverbal behavior (pp. 129–149). New York: Springer. nation of affect in multimodal and unimodal stimulation: The role of
Dovidio, J., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A. intersensory redundancy. Developmental Psychology, 43, 238–252.
(1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510–540. human infants. Science, 218, 486–487.
Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron- Frank, M. G., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1993). Behavioral markers and
Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial recognizability of the smile of enjoyment. Journal of Personality
orienting. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–540. and Social Psychology, 64, 83–93.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in Fridlund, A. J. (1991). Sociality of solitary smiling: Potentiation by an implicit
humans. Journal of Human Evolution, 20, 469–493. audience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 229–240.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1995). Neocortex size and group size in primates: A test Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view.
of the hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 28, 287–296. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Fridlund, A. J., Ekman, P., & Oster, H. (1987). Facial expressions of emo-
Anthropology, 6, 178–190. tion. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and
communication (2nd ed., pp. 143–223). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Edinger, J. A., & Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal involvement in social
control. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 30–56. Friedman, H. S., & Riggio, R. E. (1981). Effect of individual differences
in nonverbal expressiveness on transmission of emotion. Journal of
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and environment. New York: King’s Crown Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 96–104.
Press.
Friedman, H. S., Riggio, R. E., & Casella, D. F. (1988). Nonverbal skill,
Ekman, P. (1971). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions personal charisma, and initial attraction. Personality and Social
of emotion. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation Psychology Bulletin, 14, 203–211.
(pp. 207–283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive ori-
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. Scherer & enting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin and
P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 319–344). Mahwah, NJ: Review, 5, 490–495.
Erlbaum.
Fugita, S. S., Hogrebe, M. C., & Wexley, K. N. (1980). Perceptions of
Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dagleish & M. Power (Eds.), deception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 637–643.
Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 301–320). Sussex, England:
Furuichi, T. (1989). Social interaction and the life history of female Pan
John Wiley & Sons.
paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. International Journal of Primatology, 10,
Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Duchenne’s smile: 173–197.
Emotional expression and brain physiology II. Journal of Personality
Gaelick, L., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Wyer, R. S. (1985). Emotional com-
and Social Psychology, 58, 342–353.
munication in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969a). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Psychology, 49, 1246–1265.
Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation the-
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969b). Nonverbal leakage and clues to ory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 493–501.
deception. Psychiatry, 32, 88–105. Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., DiGeronimo, K., & Biek, M. (1992).
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural Differential accuracy in person perception across traits: Examination of
specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological a functional hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Bulletin, 128, 203–235. 62, 688–698.
References 493

Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality Hall, J. A., & Friedman, G. B. (1999). Status, gender and nonverbal behavior:
and Social Psychology, 26, 309–320. A study of structured interactions between employees of a company.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1082–1091.
Wilmington, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1994). “Subordination” and sensitivity
Gifford, R. (1994). A lens-mapping framework for understanding the encod- to nonverbal cues: A study of married working women. Sex Roles, 31,
ing and decoding of interpersonal dispositions in nonverbal behavior. 149–165.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 398–412. Hall, J. A., Horgan, T. G., & Carter, J. D. (2002). Assigned and felt status in
Gifford, R., Ng, C. F., & Wilkinson, M. (1985). Nonverbal cues in the relation to observer-coded and participant-reported smiling. Journal of
employment interview: Links between applicant qualities and inter- Nonverbal Behavior, 26, 63–81.
viewer judgments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 729–736. Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., & Schmid-Mast, M. (2007). Nonverbal
Gilbert, D. T., Jones, E. E., & Pelham, B. W. (1987). Influence and infer- self-accuracy in interpersonal interaction. Personality and Social
ence: What the active perceiver overlooks. Journal of Personality and Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1675–1685.
Social Psychology, 52, 861–870. Halverson, A. M., Hallahan, M., Hart, A. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1997).
Gilbert, D. T., & Krull, D. S. (1988). Seeing less and knowing more: The Reducing the biasing effects of judges’ nonverbal behavior with simpli-
benefits of perceptual ignorance. Journal of Personality and Social fied jury instruction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 590–598.
Psychology, 54, 193–202. Hariri, A. R., Tessitore, A., Mattay, V. S., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D. R.
(2002). The amygdala to emotional stimuli: A comparison of faces and
Giles, H., & Powesland, P. F. (1975). Speech styles and social evaluation.
scenes. NeuroImage, 17, 317–323.
London: Academic Press.
Harris, M. J., Moniz, A. J., Sowards, B. A., & Krane, K. (1994). Mediation
Gilovich, T., Savitsky, K., & Medvec, V. H. (1998). The illusion of trans-
of interpersonal expectancy effects: Expectancies about the elderly.
parency: Biased assessments of others’ ability to read one’s emotional
Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, 36–48.
states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 332–346.
Harris, M. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Mediation of interpersonal expec-
Gittleman, J. H. (1986). Carnivore brain size, behavioural ecology and
tancy effects: 31 meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 363–386.
phylogeny. Journal of Mammalogy, 67, 23–26.
Hart, A. J. (1995). Naturally occurring expectation effects. Journal of
Godfrey, D. K., Jones, E. E., & Lord, C. G. (1986). Self-promotion is
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 109–115.
not ingratiating. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50,
106–115. Hatfield, J. T., Cacioppo, J. C., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional conta-
gion. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J. J., & Golan, Y. (2006). The “reading
the mind in the films” task: Complex emotion recognition in adults Hauser, M. D. (1993). The evolution of primate vocalizations: Effects of
with and without autism spectrum conditions. Social Neuroscience, 1, phylogeny, body weight, and social context. American Naturalist, 142,
111–123. 528–542.
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Wagner, S. M. (2005). How our hands help us learn. Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA:
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 234–241. MIT Press.
Gouzoules, S., & Gouzoules, H. (1987). Kinship. In B. B. Smuts, Hayaki, H. (1990). Social context of pant-grunting in young chimpanzees.
D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, & T. T. Struhsaker In T. Nishida (Ed.), The chimpanzees of the Mahale mountains (pp.
(Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 299–305). Chicago: University of 189–206). Tokyo: Tokyo University Press.
Chicago Press. Hecht, M. A., & LaFrance, M. (1995). How (fast) can I help you? Tone
Guerrero, L. K., & Anderson, P. A. (1991). The waxing and waning of rela- of voice and telephone operator efficiency in interactions. Journal of
tional intimacy: Touch as a function of relational stage, gender, and Applied Social Psychology, 25, 2086–2098.
touch avoidance. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York:
147–165. John Wiley & Sons.
Gust, D. A., Gordon, T. P., Brodie, A. R., & McClure, H. M. (1994). Effect Hemelrijk, C. K. (1994). Support for being groomed in long-tailed
of a preferred companion in modulating stress in adult female rhesus macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Animal Behaviour, 48, 479–481.
monkeys. Physiology and Behavior, 55, 681–684. Henley, N. M. (1977). Body politics: Power, sex, and nonverbal communi-
Halberstadt, A. G., Fox, N. A., & Jones, N. A. (1993). Do expressive moth- cation. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
ers have expressive children? The role of socialization in children’s Hertenstein, M. J., Keltner, D., App, B., Bulleit, B. A., & Jaskolka, A. R.
affect expression. Social Development, 2, 48–65. (2006). Touch communicates distinct emotions. Emotion, 6, 528–533.
Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy Holodynski, M., & Friedlmeier, W. (2006). Development of emotions and
and expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. emotion regulation. New York: Springer.
Hall, J. A. (2001). The PONS test and the psychometric approach to mea- Hood, B., M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult’s eyes trigger
suring interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9,
Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 143–160). 131–134.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Howard, D. J., & Gengler, C. (2001). Emotional contagion effects on
Hall, J. A., & Andrzejewski, S. A. (2008). Who draws accurate first product attitudes. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 189–201.
impressions? Personal correlates of sensitivity to nonverbal cues. In Hugenberg, K. (2005). Social categorization and the perception of facial
N. Ambady & J. Skowronski (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 87–106). affect: Target race moderates the response latency advantage for happy
New York: Guilford Press. faces. Emotion, 5, 267–276.
Hall, J. A., Andrzejewski, S. A., & Yopchick, J. E. (in press). Psychosocial Hugenberg, K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2003). Facing prejudice: Implicit
correlates of interpersonal sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of prejudice and the perception of facial threat. Psychological Science,
Nonverbal Behavior. 14, 640–643.
Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & Smith-LeBeau, L. (2005). Nonverbal behav- Hugenberg, K., & Sczesny, S. (2006). On wonderful women and see-
ior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. ing smiles: Social categorization moderates the happy face response
Psychological Bulletin, 131, 898–924. latency advantage. Social Cognition, 24, 516–539.
494 Nonverbal Behavior

Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. P. G. Bateson & Leary, M. R., Britt, T. W., Cutlip, W. D., & Templeton, J. L. (1992). Social
R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303– 317). blushing. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 446–480.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of
Iverson, J. N., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for Neuroscience, 23, 155–184.
language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367–371. Lee, M. T., & Ofshe, R. (1981). The impact of behavioral style and status
Izard, C. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Meredity. characteristics on social influence: A test of two competing theories.
Izard, C. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum Press. Social Psychology Quarterly, 44, 73–82.
Johnson, K. L., & Tassinary, L. G. (2005). Meaning in motion and mor- Lefebvre, L. M. (1973). An experimental approach to the use of ingratia-
phology. Psychological Science, 16, 890–897. tion tactics under homogenous and heterogeneous dyads. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 427–445.
Johnson, K. L., Gill, S., Reichman, V., & Tassinary, L. G. (2007). Swagger,
sway, and sexuality: Judging sexual orientation from body motion Legerstee, M., Corter, C., & Kineapple, K. (1990). Hand, arm, and facial
and morphology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, actions of young infants to a social and nonsocial stimulus. Child
321–334. Development, 61, 774–784.
Joly-Mascheroni, R. M., Senju, A., & Shepherd, A. J. (2008). Dogs catch Lester, B. M., & Boukydis, C. F. Z. (1992). No language but a cry.
human yawns. Biology Letters, 4, 446–448. In H. Papousek, U. Jurgens, & M. Papousek (Eds.), Nonverbal
communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp.
Jouventin, P., Pasteur, G., & Cambefort, J. P. (1976). Observational learn-
174– 191). New York: Cambridge University Press.
ing of baboons and avoidance of mimics: Exploratory tests. Evolution,
31, 21–28. Levine, J. M., & McBurney, D. H. (1986). The role of olfaction in social
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament. perception and behavior. In C. P. Herman, M. P. Zanna, & E. T.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Higgins (Eds.), Physical appearance, stigma, and social behavior (pp.
179– 217). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kagan, J., Snidman, N., & Arcus, D. (1998). Childhood derivatives of high
and low reactivity in infancy. Child Development, 69, 1483–1493. Liddell, B. J., Brown, K. J., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M. J., Das, P., Peduto, A.,
et al. (2005). A direct brainstem–amygdala–cortical “alarm” system for
Kellerman, J., Lewis, J., & Laird, J. D. (1989). Looking and loving: subliminal signals of fear. NeuroImage, 24, 235–243.
The effects of mutual gaze on feelings of romantic love. Journal of
Research in Personality, 23, 145–161. Likowski, K. U., Muhlberger, A., Seibt, B., Pauli, P., & Weyers, P. (2008).
Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes. Journal of Experimental
King, E. B., Shapiro, J. R., Hebl, M. R., Singletary, S. L., & Turner, S.
Social Psychology, 44, 1065–1072.
(2006). The stigma of obesity in customer service: A mechanism
for remediation and bottom-line consequences of interpersonal Macrae, C. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: Thinking
discrimination. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 579–593. categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51,
93–120.
Kleck, R. E., Vaughan, R. C., Cartwright-Smith, K. B., Vaughan, K. B.,
Colby, C. Z., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1976). Effects of being observed on Malatesta, C. Z., & Izard, C. E. (1984). Emotion in adult development.
expressive, subjective, and physiological responses to painful stimuli. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 1211–1218. Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Robertson, T. E., Hofer, B.,
Klinnert, M. D. (1984). The regulation of infant behavior by maternal facial Neuberg, S. L., et al. (2005). Functional projection: How fundamental
expression. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 447–465. social motives can bias interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 88, 63–78.
Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social
referencing: The infant’s use of emotional signals from a friendly adult Mann, S., Vrij, A., & Bull, R. (2004). Detecting true lies: Police officers’
with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22, 427–432. ability to detect suspects’ lies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89,
137–149.
Knapp, M. L. (1978). Nonverbal communication in social interaction.
New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Marino, L. (1996). What can dolphins tell us about primate evolution?
Knapp, J. L. (2006). An historical overview of nonverbal research. In Evolutionary Anthropology, 5, 81–86.
V. Manusov & M. L. Patterson (Eds.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal Marsh, A. A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2007). Deficits in facial affect recogni-
communication (pp. 3–20). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. tion among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and
Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 454–465.
from a dynamic point light display. Perception and Psychophysics, 21, Marsh, A. A., Elfenbein, E. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Nonverbal “accents”:
575–580. Cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. Psychological
Kraut, R. E. (1982). Social presence, facial feedback, and emotion. Journal Science, 14, 373–376.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 853–863. Marsh, A. A., Kozak, M. N., & Ambady, N. (2007). Accurate identifica-
LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Levy-Paluck, E. (2003). The contingent tion of fear facial expressions predicts prosocial behavior. Emotion, 7,
smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological 239–251.
Bulletin, 129, 305–334. Martin, G. B., & Clark, R. D. (1982). Distress crying in neonates: Species
Laird, J. D., Wagener, J. J., Halal, M., & Szegda, M. (1982). Remembering and peer specificity. Developmental Psychology, 18, 3–9.
what you feel: The effects of emotion on memory. Journal of Masur, E. F. (1983). Gestural development, dual-directional signaling, and the
Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 646–658. transition to words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12, 93–109.
Lakin, J., L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral Mathis, A., Chivers, D. P., & Smith, R. J. F. (1996). Cultural transmission
mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, of predator recognition in fishes: Intraspecific and interspecific learn-
334–339. ing. Animal Behaviour, 51, 185–201.
Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. (2008). I am too just like you: Matsumoto, D. (2006). Culture and nonverbal behavior. In V. Manusov &
Nonconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social M. L. Patterson (Eds.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal communica-
exclusion. Psychological Science, 19, 816–822. tion (pp. 219–235). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Landis, C. (1924). Studies of emotional reactions. I: A preliminary study of Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2006). The thrill of victory and the
facial expression. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7, 325–341. agony of defeat: Spontaneous expressions of medal winners of
References 495

the 2004 Athens Olympic games. Journal of Personality and Social Nishida, T. (1987). Development of social grooming between mother and
Psychology, 91, 568–581. offspring in wild chimpanzees, Folia Primatologica, 50, 109–123.
Mazur, A. (2005). Biosociology of dominance and deference. Lanham, Norman, J. F., Payton, S. M., Long, J. R., & Hawkes, L. M. (2004). Aging
MD: Rowman & Littlefield. and perception of biological motion. Psychology and Aging, 19,
McAndrew, F. T. (1986). A cross-cultural study of recognition thresh- 219–225.
olds for facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Cross-Cultural Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual differences in the nonver-
Psychology, 17, 211–224. bal communication of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal
McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward an ecological theory of Accuracy scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35.
social perception. Psychological Review, 90, 215–238. Ohman, A. (2002). Automaticity and the amygdala: Nonconscious
McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. responses to emotional faces. Current Directions in Psychological
Nature, 264, 746–748. Science, 11, 62–65.
Oster, H. (1978). Facial expression and affect development.
McHugo, G. J., Lanzetta, J. T., Sullivan, D. G., Masters, R. D., & Englis, B. G.
In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect (pp.
(1985). Emotional reactions to a political leader’s expressive displays.
43–76). New York: Plenum Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1513–1529.
Owren, M. J., Rendall, D., & Bachorowski, J. A. (2005). Conscious and
Mech, L. D. (1970). The wolf: The ecology and behavior of an endan-
unconscious emotion in nonlinguistic vocal communication. In L. F.
gered species. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Barrett, P. Niedenthal, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and con-
Mehrabian, A. (1969). Significance of posture and position in the com- sciousness (pp. 185–204). New York: Guilford Press.
munication of attitude and status relationships. Psychological Bulletin,
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processing. New York: Holt,
71, 359–372.
Rinehart, and Winston.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual
Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery
gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.
or dual coding? Cognitive Psychology, 5, 176–206.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M., & Vick, S. J. (2007). New developments in under-
facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709. standing emotional facial signals in chimpanzees. Current Directions
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the repre- in Psychological Science, 16, 117–122.
sentation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 83–99. Patterson, M. L. (2006). The evolution of theories of interactive behav-
Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., & Keir, R. (1984). Observational ior (pp. 21–40). The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication.
conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Psychology, 93, 355–372. Patterson, M. L., Foster, J. L., & Bellmer, C. D. (2001). Another look at
Molinsky, A. L., Krabbenhoft, M. A., Ambady, N., & Choi, Y. S. (2005). accuracy and confidence in social judgments. Journal of Nonverbal
Cracking the nonverbal code. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Behavior, 25, 207–219.
36, 380–395. Patterson, M. L., & Stockbridge, E. (1998). Effects of cognitive demand
Montepare, J. M., & Vega, C. (1988). Women’s vocal reactions to intimate and judgment strategy on person perception accuracy. Journal of
and casual male friends. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 253–263.
14, 103–113. Pegna, A. J., Khateb, A., Lazeyras, F., & Seghier, M. L. (2004).
Moody, E. J., McIntosh, D. N., Mann, L. J., & Weisser, K. R. (2007). More Discriminating emotional faces without primary visual cortices
than mere mimicry? The influence of emotion on rapid facial reactions involves the right amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 24–25.
to faces. Emotion, 7, 447–457. Penn, D., Spaulding, W., Reed, D., & Sullivan, M. (1996). The relation-
Mumme, D., & Fernald, A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: Learning ship of social cognition toward behavior in chronic schizophrenia.
from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. Child Schizophrenia Research, 20, 327–335.
Development, 74, 221–237. Pfungst, O. (1911). Clever Hans (the horse of Mr. von Osten): A contri-
Murphy, N. A., Hall, J. A., & Colvin, C. R. (2003). Accurate intelligence bution to experimental, animal, and human psychology (C. L. Rahn,
assessments in social interactions: Mediators and gender effects. Trans.). New York: Holt.
Journal of Personality, 71, 465–493. Pitterman, H., & Nowicki, S. (2004). A test of the ability to identify emo-
Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: tion in human standing and sitting postures: The Diagnostic Analysis of
Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Nonverbal Accuracy-2 Posture Test (DANVA2-POS). Genetic, Social,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739. and General Psychology Monographs, 130, 146–162.
Naab, P. J., & Russell, J. A. (2007). Judgments of emotion from spontane- Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001).
ous facial expressions of New Guineans. Emotion, 7, 736–744. Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51–B61.
Nakayama, K. (2004). Observing conspecifics scratching induces a conta- Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and prox-
gion of scratching in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Journal of imal bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–72.
Comparative Psychology, 118, 20–24. Provine, R. R. (1986). Yawning as a stereotyped action pattern and releas-
Nelson, D. A. (1984). Communication of intentions in agonistic con- ing stimulus. Ethology, 72, 109–122.
texts by the pigeon guillemot, Cepphus columba. Behaviour, 88, Puccinelli, N. M., & Tickle-Degnen, L. (2004). Knowing too much about
145–188. others: Moderators of the relationship between eavesdropping and
Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). Mood contagion: The automatic trans- rapport in social interaction. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28,
fer of mood between persons. Journal of Personality and Social 223–243.
Psychology, 79, 211–223. Quinn, K. A., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). Categorizing others: On the dynam-
Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Innes-Ker, A. H. ics of person construal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
(2001). When did her smile drop? Facial mimicry and the influences 88, 467–479.
of emotional state on the detection of change in emotional expression. Rafal, R. (1996). Visual attention: Converging operations from neurology
Cognition and Emotion, 15, 853–864. and psychology. In A. F. Kramer, M. G. Coles, & G. D. Logan (Eds.),
496 Nonverbal Behavior

Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention. Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. expressions? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological
Ramanathan, S., & McGill, A. L. (2007). Consuming with others: Social Review, 115, 102–141.
influences on moment-to-moment and retrospective evaluation of Sackett, G. P. (1966). Monkeys reared in isolation with pictures as visual
experience. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 506−524. input: Evidence for an innate releasing mechanism. Science, 154,
Ravaja, N., Kallinen, K., Saari, T., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (2004). 1468–1473.
Suboptimal exposure to facial expressions when viewing video mes- Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn.
sages from a small screen: Effects on emotion, attention, and memory. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 120–131.
Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., & Wallbott, H. G. (2001). Emotion inferences
Rejeski, W. J., & Lowe, C. A. (1980). The role of ability and effort in attri- from vocal expression correlate across languages and cultures. Journal
butions for sport achievement. Journal of Personality, 48, 223–244. of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 76–92.
Repacholi, B. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Emotional eavesdrop- Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S. B., Mojzisch, A., & Vogeley, K. (2008). What’s
ping: Infants selectively respond to indirect emotional signals. Child in a smile? Neural correlates of facial embodiment in social interaction.
Development, 78, 503–521. Social Neuroscience, 3, 37–50.
Rhodewalt, F., & Comer, R. (1979). Induced-compliance attitude change: Once Schino, G. (2007). Grooming and agonistic support: A meta-analysis of
more with feeling. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 35–47. reciprocal altruism. Behavioral Ecology, 18, 115–120.
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: Seifert, L. S. (1997). Activating representations in semantic memory:
The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Different benefits for words and pictures. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 79, 410–424. Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1106–1121.
Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). When prejudice does not pay:
Sergi, M. J., & Green, M. F. (2003). Social perception and early visual
Effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychological
processing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 59, 233–241.
Science, 14, 287–290.
Sergi, M. J., Rassovsky, Y., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Green, M. F. (2006).
Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2005). Thin slices of racial bias. Journal
Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual pro-
of Nonverbal Behavior, 29, 75–86.
cessing on functional status in schizophrenia. American Journal of
Richeson, J. A., Trawalter, S., & Shelton, J. N. (2005). African-Americans’ Psychiatry, 163, 448–454.
implicit racial attitudes and the depletion of executive function after
Seyfarth, R. M. (1980). The distribution of grooming and related behav-
interracial interactions. Social Cognition, 23, 336–352.
iours in and among adult female vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour,
Riggio, R. E. (1992). Social interaction skills and nonverbal behavior. 28, 798–213.
In R. S. Feldman (Ed.), Applications of nonverbal behavioral theories
and research (pp. 3–30). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1984). Grooming, alliances, and recipro-
cal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature, 308, 541–543.
Riggio, R. E. (2006). Nonverbal skills and abilities. In V. Manusov & M. L.
Patterson (Eds.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication Shelton, J. N., Richeson, J. A., & Salvatore, J. (2005). Expecting to the tar-
(pp. 79–96). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. get of prejudice: Implications for interethnic interactions. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1189–1202.
Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial expression: A review
of the neurological and psychological mechanisms for producing facial Shimojo, S., Simion, C., Shimojo, E., & Scheier, C. (2003). Gaze bias
expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 52–77. both reflects and influences preference. Nature Neuroscience, 12,
1317–1322.
Riskind, J. H. (1983). Nonverbal expressions and the accessibility of life expe-
rience memories: A congruence hypothesis. Social Cognition, 2, 62–86. Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant.
Developmental Psychology, 5, 136–150.
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual
Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal
Rosenfeld, H. M. (1966). Approval-seeking and approval-inducing func- emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-
tions of verbal and nonverbal responses in the dyad. Journal of olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195–200.
Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 597–605. Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development. New York: Cambridge
Rosenthal, R. (1966). Experimenter effects in behavioral research. University Press.
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Stapel, D. A., Koomen, W., & Ruys, K. I. (2002). The effects of diffuse
Rosenthal, R., & DePaulo, B. M. (1979). Sex differences in eavesdropping and distinct affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
on nonverbal cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 60–74.
273–285. Steckler, N. A., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Sex differences in nonverbal and
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. verbal communication with bosses, peers, and subordinates. Journal of
(1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication. Baltimore: Johns Applied Psychology, 70, 157–163.
Hopkins University Press. Stekelenburg, J. J., & de Gelder, B. (2004). Temporal ventriloquism: Sound
Rosenthal, R., & Lawson, R. (1964). A longitudinal study of the effects of modulates the flash-lag effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
experimenter bias on the operant learning of laboratory rats. Journal of Human Perception and Performance, 30, 513–518.
Psychiatry Research, 2, 61–72. Stel, M., & van Knippenberg, A. (2008). The role of facial mimicry in the
Rosip, J. C., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Knowledge of nonverbal cues, gender, recognition of affect. Psychological Science, 19, 984–985.
and decoding accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 267–286. Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emo-
Rotteveel, M., de Groot, P., Geutskens, A., & Phaf, R. H. (2001). Stronger tional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social
suboptimal than optimal priming? Emotion, 1, 348–364. Psychology, 64, 211–220.
Rule, N., Ambady, N., Adams, R. B., Jr., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating
Accuracy and awareness in the perception and categorization of male conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feed-
sexual orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, back hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
1019–1028. 768–777.
References 497

Strahan, E. J., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna, M. P. (2002). Subliminal priming Weisbuch, M., & Ambady, A. (2008a). Affective divergence: Automatic
and persuasion: Striking while the iron is hot. Journal of Experimental responses to others’ emotions depend on group membership. Journal
Social Psychology, 38, 556–568. of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1063–1079.
Sullins, E. S. (1991). Emotional contagion revisited: Effects of social com- Weisbuch, M., & Ambady, N. (2008b). Non-conscious routes to build-
parison and expressive style on mood convergence. Personality and ing culture: Nonverbal components of socialization. Journal of
Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 166–174. Consciousness Studies, 15, 159–183.
Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (2003). Contextual determinants of anger Weisbuch, M., & Ambady, A. (2008c). Nonverbal social influence: A
and other negative expressions in young infants. Developmental theory and a review. Under review.
Psychology, 39, 693–705. Weisbuch, M., & Ambady, N. (2009). Unspoken cultural influence:
Tanner, R. J., Ferraro, R., Chartrand, T. L., Bettman, J. R., & van Baaren, R. Exposure to and influence of nonverbal bias. Journal of Personality
(2008). Of chameleons and consumption: The impact of mimicry on and Social Psychology, 96, 1104–1119.
choice and preferences. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 754–766. Weisbuch, M., Ivcevic, Z., & Ambady, N. (2009). On being liked on the
Taylor, S. E., Cousino-Klein, L., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., web and in the “real world”: Consistency in first impressions across
Gurung, R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to personal webpages and spontaneous behavior. Journal of Experimental
stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Social Psychology, 45, 573–576.
Review, 107, 411–429. Weisbuch, M., Seery, M. D., Ambady, N., & Blascovich, J. (2009). The
Tickle-Degnen, L., & Lyons, K. D. (2004). Practitioners’ impressions of face and the voice of threat: The nonverbal communication of visceral
patients with Parkinson’s disease: The social ecology of the expressive states during social interaction. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33,
mask. Social Science and Medicine, 58, 603–614. 141–148.
Tickle-Degnen, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1990). The nature of rapport and its Weisbuch, M., Slepian, M. L., Clarke, A., Ambady, N., &
nonverbal correlates. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 285–293. Veenstra-Vander Weele, J. (in press). Behavioral stability across
Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power moves: Complementarity in time and situations: Nonverbal versus verbal consistency. Journal of
dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality Nonverbal Behavior.
and Social Psychology, 84, 558–568. Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overt head movements
Timms, M. W. H. (1980). Treatment of chronic blushing through paradoxi- on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic
cal intention. Behavioral Psychotherapy, 8, 59–61. and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230.
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford Whalen, P. J., Kagan, J., Cook, R. G., Davis, F. C., Kim, H., Polis, S.,
University Press. McLaren, D. G., et al. (2004). Human amygdala responsivity to masked
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and fearful eye whites. Science, 306, 2061.
components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., &
Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846. Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expres-
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during sions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal
visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8, 769–800. of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418.
Usher, J. A., & Neisser, U. (1993). Childhood amnesia and the begin- Wild, B., Erb, M., & Bartels, M. (2001). Are emotions contagious? Evoked
nings of memory for four early life events. Journal of Experimental emotions while viewing emotional faces: Quality, quantity, time course,
Psychology: General, 122, 155–165. and gender differences. Psychiatry Research, 102, 109–124.
Vaughan, K. B., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1980). Vicarious instigation and conditioning Winkielman, P., Berridge, K. C., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2005). Unconscious
of facial expressive and autonomic responses to a model’s expressive dis- affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence con-
play of pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 909–923. sumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality and Social
Vrij, A. (1993). Credibility judgments of detectives: The impact of nonver- Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121–135.
bal behavior, social skills, and physical characteristics on impression Winter, L., & Uleman, J. S. (1984). When are social judgments made?
formation. Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 601–610. Evidence for the spontaneousness of trait inferences. Journal of
Vrij, A., & Semin, G. R. (1996). Lie experts’ beliefs about nonverbal indi- Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 237–252.
cators of deception. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20(1), 65–80. Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal media-
Walden, T., & Ogan, T. (1988). The development of social referencing. tion of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of
Child Development, 59, 1230–1240. Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 109–120.
Walker-Andrews, A. S. (1997). Infants’ perception of expressive behaviors: Yabar, Y., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Peace, V. (2006). Implicit behavioral
Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological Bulletin, mimicry: Investigating the impact of group membership. Journal of
121, 437–456. Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 97–113.
Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2008). Intermodal emotional processes in infancy. Zebrowitz, L. A. (2006). Finally, faces find favor. Social Cognition, 24,
In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland, & L. Feldman-Barrett (Eds.), Handbook 657–701.
of emotions (pp. 364–375). New York: Guilford Press. Zebrowitz, L. A., & Collins, M. A. (1997). Accurate social percep-
Waller, B. M., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2005). Differential behavioral effects of tion at zero acquaintance: The affordances of a Gibsonian approach.
silent bared teeth display and relaxed open mouth display in chimpan- Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 204–233.
zees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology, 111, 129–142. Zeskind, P. S., & Collins, V. (1987). Pitch of infant crying and caregiver responses
Waller, B. M., Vick, S. J., Parr, L. A., Bard, K. A., & Pasqualini, M. C. (2006). in a natural setting. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 501–504.
Intramuscular electrical stimulation of facial muscles in humans and Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and non-
chimpanzees: Duchenne revisited and extended. Emotion, 6, 367–382. verbal communication of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
Webb, J. T. (1969). Subject speech rates as a function of interviewer in experimental social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 2–60). San Diego, CA:
behavior. Language and Speech, 12, 54–67. Academic Press.
Chapter 14

Mind Perception
NICHOLAS EPLEY AND ADAM WAYTZ

People seem to have access to the workings of their own process by which people intuitively explain other people’s
minds but not to the workings of other minds. Philosophically behavior to arrive at impressions of others’ stable disposi-
speaking, this is supposed to create a problem for us. tions and enduring personalities. Intentional actions reveal
Because we do not have access to other minds, we cannot more about someone’s enduring dispositions than acci-
confidently conclude that other people have minds at all. dental actions, meaning that inferences about others’
But most of us do not speak philosophically and therefore mental states are often at the heart of the person percep-
have no trouble talking at length about other people’s tion process. But before an ordinary perceiver can decide
desires, intentions, goals, attitudes, beliefs, and emotions. which mental states are responsible for a given action, an
Solving this other minds problem at all seems to be no prob- ordinary perceiver needs to at least implicitly determine if
lem at all. People worry about whether others like them or another agent has a mind in the first place, and then deter-
not, find them trustworthy or not, or find them attractive or mine that agent’s state of mind in the second place. Interest
not. People wonder whether others are being truthful in these first two components of the person perception pro-
or deceptive, whether others are motivated by greed or gen- cess has now attracted so much attention, from so many
erosity, and whether others behaved intentionally or acci- converging disciplines, in domains that go so far beyond
dentally. And people infer their own future mental states basic attributional questions that it necessitates its own
when making decisions designed to create future versions treatment in this handbook. Instead of having only one
of themselves that are happy, such as whether to marry or chapter addressing how ordinary people understand others,
save for retirement. Once formed, the ability to think about this handbook therefore has two (see also Macrae &
other minds is so proficient that other minds appear almost Quadflieg, this volume). This chapter covers how people
everywhere that people look, from pets that become loving infer that other agents have mental states and then how
and considerate, to technological gadgets that become obsti- people decide what those mental states might be. That
nate and vindictive, to gods that have goals and plans for chapter will cover, in part, how ordinary perceivers extract
one’s life. If reasoning about other minds is a problem, it information from more specific features of persons—
resembles an addiction more than a conundrum. especially their face and body—to form impressions of
Inferences about mental states are often at the very center other people’s more stable and enduring identities.
of social thought and behavior, and social psychologists Because people are capable of attributing minds to
have been working for a long time to understand how, nonhuman agents such as other animals, gadgets, or gods,
and how well, people make them. All previous versions of and are also capable of denying mental states to other
the Handbook of Social Psychology except the first have humans and treating them like animals or objects, we have
therefore included chapters describing how people under- dropped the term person from our title and instead adopted
stand one another (Bruner & Tagiuri, 1954; Gilbert, 1998; Wegner ’s (2002) term of mind perception. Although people
Ross & Fletcher, 1985; Tagiuri, 1969)—what earlier authors seem readily able to solve the classic philosophical version
called “person perception” and what Gilbert called “ordi- of the other minds problem, the philosophical version cap-
nary personology.” These chapters organize the dominant tures the main theme of all current research on mind per-
research tradition on causal attribution that addresses the ception and therefore is the central guide for this chapter.

We thank Mark Alicke, Ian Apperly, Susan Fiske, Daniel Gilbert, Bertram Malle, Jason Mitchell, Ara Norenzayan, Emily Pronin, Rebecca
Saxe, Jacquie Vorauer, and Daniel Wegner for helpful comments on an earlier draft.

498

CH14.indd 498 12/22/09 3:39:35 PM


Mind Perception Versus Person Perception 499

People do not have direct information about others’ mental the broader host of traits, dispositions, and capacities that
states and must therefore base their inferences on what- people might attribute to others. People’s attempts to
ever information about others’ mental states they do have understand other agents can be organized conceptually
access to. This requires a leap from observable behavior into three questions: (1) Does it have a mind?, (2) What
to unobservable mental states that is so common and rou- state might that mind be in?, and (3) Which states of mind
tine that people often seem unaware that they are making are responsible for the agent’s behavior? Research on mind
a leap (Ross & Ward, 1996). The sections of this chapter perception focuses on the first two questions, whereas
explain how mind perception differs from the more gen- much of the work in person perception has focused on the
eral study of person perception (Mind Perception versus processes of integration, correction, and discounting that
Person Perception), why people reason about other minds guide the third question. In this way, work on mind percep-
(Why Mind?), when they are likely to do so (Activating tion may be considered a kind of preattributional process,
Mind Perception), how they acquire the capacity for mind identifying the kinds of causes that might explain or predict
perception (Acquiring Mind Perception), how they use this another ’s behavior. Teenagers, for instance, are capable of
capacity (Using Mind Perception), how minds are applied intentional deception whereas toddlers are not, and parents
to a wide range of targets (Targets of Mind Perception), trying to explain their child’s behavior do well to know the
and why mind perception matters to social psychology difference.
(Consequences of Mind Perception). Mind perception is also something of a preattribu-
tional process because the presence versus absence of
mind is the defining feature of people’s intuitive distinc-
MIND PERCEPTION VERSUS tions between people and nonpeople. The psychological
PERSON PERCEPTION line between human and nonhuman—us and them—is
among the most critical distinctions that any person or cul-
Although other minds cannot be directly observed, they ture can make, but it is also among the most difficult to
can be easily defined. People intuitively represent other objectively define (Farah & Heberlein, 2007). A growing
minds in two broad sets of psychological capacities— body of literature, which we review later, suggests that
conscious experience and intentional agency (Gray, Gray, & this distinction is intuitively made on the relative presence
Wegner, 2007). Conscious experience involves metacogni- versus absence of mind. People can experience conscious
tive capacities, including secondary emotions (e.g., regret, emotions such as shame or pride, contempt or compas-
rage, sympathy, pride, or joy; Demoulin et al., 2004; sion, and suffering or joy, whereas objects or technological
Leyens et al., 2003), conscious awareness of one’s envi- agents are less able to do so. And most people think other
ronment, and basic psychological states (e.g., fear, hunger, humans are capable of intentional actions, self-control,
thirst, or pain). Intentional agency is the capacity to engage and deliberate planning, whereas most people generally
in reasoned action, self-control, strategic planning, or goal- believe that nonhuman animals are less capable. Mind per-
directed behavior and therefore to possess conscious pref- ception is therefore central to phenomena such as anthro-
erences, beliefs, and explicit knowledge. Agents—entities pomorphism, whereby people treat nonhuman agents
that act—are attributed these mental capacities in varying as humanlike (Epley, Waytz, & Cacioppo, 2007), and to
degrees along a continuum rather than as a dichotomy, the inverse process of dehumanization, whereby people
with agents being seen as having more or less of a capacity treat other people like animals or objects (Bandura,
rather than all or none of it. Agents can vary along these Underwood, & Fromson, 1975; Haslam, 2006; Leyens et al.,
two dimensions quite independently; some have a high 2003). Ordinary perceivers, after all, are capable of treating
degree of both intentional agency and experience (e.g., the their pets as people and their enemies as animals. Without
self), whereas others having a high degree of experience perceiving minds, people are not “perceiving people.”
but little agency (e.g., a baby), high agency but little expe- In contrast, research on mind perception is also broader
rience (e.g., God), or little agency or experience (e.g., a than existing research on person perception because it
dead person; Gray et al., 2007; cf. Gray & Wegner, 2008). expands the scope of agents considered by ordinary per-
Social psychological research on mind perception bor- ceivers. “Social” agents include any entity that acts inter-
rows much from its intellectual foundations in person per- dependently with others, but research in “social cognition”
ception but differs from classic work because it is both has almost exclusively addressed how people think about
narrower and broader than the research in person percep- other people (Kwan & Fiske, 2008). This is unfortunate
tion from which it grew. It is narrower because it focuses because people seem readily able to attribute humanlike
only on inferences about others’ minds—what is often mental states to all sorts of agents. The stock market can
called “mentalizing” (Frith & Frith, 2003)—rather than “flirt with 10,000” (Morris, Sheldon, Ames, & Young,

CH14.indd 499 12/22/09 3:39:36 PM


500 Mind Perception

2007), one’s crashing computer can seem to have a “mind actions merely upon observing an action (Apperly, Riggs,
of its own” (Waytz, Morewedge, Epley, Monteleone, Simpson, Samson, & Chiavarino, 2006), but instead do so
Gao, & Cacioppo, 2009), and one’s dog can be a loyal when mental states are most likely to be useful. Given that
and caring companion (Serpell, 2003). The vast majority our species seems to have acquired a universal capacity to
of people living today—and nearly all who have lived in reason about other minds that is shared only in the dim-
past centuries—believe in an omniscient God (or set of mest capacity by any other species (Hare, 2007; Herrmann,
gods) whose goals, intentions, and desires can be observed Call, Hernández-Lloreda, Hare, & Tomasello, 2007; Saxe,
in the world around them (Guthrie, 1993). Mental states 2006), people must get something especially useful out
render an agent’s behavior understandable and predictable of mind perception to warrant all of the additional neu-
(Baron-Cohen, 1995; Dennett, 1987; Hebb, 1946; Heider, rons. This turns out to be wrong. People do not just get
1958), whether those agents are people or not. Research something useful, they get at least three things useful—the
on mind perception shifts the focus of attention from the abilities to comprehend others’ actions, to understand oth-
target being perceived to the person perceiving and thereby ers’ communication, and to coordinate one’s own behavior
shifts the focus from a specific target to more domain- with others’ behavior. These benefits matter not only for
general psychological processes involved in mental state explaining why people think about others’ mental states,
attributions. but also for predicting when people are most likely to acti-
The emergence of this particular chapter in the Handbook vate their capacity for mind perception to reason about
of Social Psychology therefore marks an evolution that has other minds.
gained both breadth and depth on the problem of how people
think about themselves and others—a change that repre- Comprehension
sents a collective focusing on topics that have proven of
deep importance across disciplines. Flanking the progress Spend even 5 minutes watching a foreign film without sub-
in social psychology and picking up dramatic speed have titles and you will recognize that uncertainty about one’s
been advances in developmental psychology regarding the environment is inherently aversive. “Human nature abhors
acquisition and uniqueness of mind perception, in clinical a lack of predictability and absence of meaning” (Gilovich,
psychology regarding perturbations in the mind perception 1991, p. 9), putting uncertainty, ambiguity, and unpredict-
process, in neuroscience about the biological architecture ability all on the short list of life’s most unpleasant expe-
of mind perception, and in anthropology about the impli- riences that people try to overcome whenever possible
cations and consequences of mind perception across cul- (Cohen, Stotland, & Wolfe, 1955; Ellsberg, 1961). White
tures. Social psychology serves as the hub for this research (1959) suggested that the desire to understand, predict, and
because it bridges the study of unique populations to the behave competently in one’s environment was the primary
more general population, translates reductionist labora- motive guiding all of psychological development, whereas
tory work into general mechanisms that govern behavior in Kagan (1972) listed understanding and comprehension
everyday life, and identifies psychological processes that as one of four basic motivations and Fiske (2004) listed
can predict and explain variability across cultures and levels it as one of five. Although the number appears open to
of analyses. A chapter on mind perception is therefore per- debate, the inclusion of understanding and comprehension
fectly suited for the Handbook of Social Psychology, and as a basic human motivation is not.
the surging interest in mind perception across multiple dis- When a billiard ball rolls along a pool table after being
ciplines makes this a good time for the first. struck by another ball, nobody thinks much about the
causal sequence of events. It is an entirely different matter
when a billiard ball rolls without being struck by another
WHY MIND? ball. Self-propelled agents cannot be completely explained
by forces outside the agent and instead must be explained by
Articulating why mind perception is important to social forces inside the agent—maybe magnetism, maybe
psychologists does not explain why it seems important demons, maybe mind? Understanding self-propelled move-
to everyone else. The world is full of invisible things that ment requires an explanation of why the agent starts and
most people spend no time thinking about, from quarks to stops, the intensity of the action, the direction of the action,
dark matter to what it looks like inside one’s own intestinal and the nature of the action. The metaphysical language of
tract. Thinking is for doing (Fiske, 1992; James, 1890), and mind provides just the kind of satisfactory answers needed
the amount of time spent thinking about invisible minds to explain self-propelled action from almost any kind of
must be for something. People do not automatically attri- agent, perhaps explaining why people seem so readily
bute higher-order mental states such as beliefs to others’ inclined to explain others’ actions in terms of underlying

CH14.indd 500 12/22/09 3:39:36 PM


Why Mind? 501

mental states rather than mitigating environmental forces Mental states such as beliefs, desires, and intentions
(Gilbert & Malone, 1995). Desires and goals describe provide explanations for otherwise random and seemingly
why an agent starts and stops, and their strength describes senseless actions. This does not mean that the mental states
the intensity of action, whereas beliefs, attitudes, knowl- people use to make sense of their world will always seem
edge, motivation, and emotions describe the direction and sensible to observers, such as when New Orleans Mayor
nature of the action. The resulting language of intention- Ray Nagin explained the unexpectedly horrible disaster of
ality combines these mental states with behavioral capacities Hurricane Katrina in 2005 as an indication that “God is
of skill and ability (Malle & Knobe, 1997) in a way that angry at America [for] being in Iraq under false pretenses
“ties together the cause–effect relations” (Heider, 1958, [and is] upset at Black America, too” (Martel, 2006, p. A4).
p. 100). Although all behavior is caused by underlying Nevertheless, it does help to explain why people are likely
physical properties, whether the machinery of a technolog- to use their mind perception abilities so widely.
ical agent or the neural firings of a sentient one, the com- All of this mentalizing can seem a bit silly to grown-ups
plexity of those physical properties runs into “the problem who believe they have long stopped believing in imaginary
of combinatorial explosion” (Dennett, 1987) as a perceiver things. Some people with harder noses than most have tried
moves from explaining a self-propelled billiard ball to a hard to avoid it but have inevitably failed. Donald Hebb
self-propelled human. The language of intentions and men- (1946, p. 88), for instance, noted that a 2-year effort at the
tal states avoids this altogether by using the same terms to Yerkes Primate laboratory to avoid using mental states to
explain and understand all seemingly self-propelled action, describe chimpanzees resulted in nothing more than an
or to predict the behavior of those agents. “endless series of specific acts in which no order or mean-
That these concepts of mind are imprecise is precisely ing could be found,” whereas by using “anthropomorphic
irrelevant for their functional value in gaining a sense of concepts of emotion and attitude one could quickly and
understanding and predictability across a range of agents. easily describe the peculiarities of individual animals.”
Adopting what Dennett (1987) calls an “intentional stance” Behaviorists likewise disavowed all talk of mentalizing
by thinking of others in terms of mental states is a simple and discussion of unobservable cognitive states (Skinner,
and wide-ranging approximation that serves as a practical 1957), but they ultimately fell short of their promises by
guide for the intuitive psychologist, providing seemingly never being able to come up with an alternative language
adequate reasons that explain actions and events (Malle, that either they or others were willing to use (Chomsky,
1999). At the least, it works markedly better than an 1959). Mental states turn others’ actions into something
explanatory system without mental states. meaningful, orderly, and seemingly comprehensible, an
Consider, for instance, the classic film clip of geomet- outcome that is deeply satisfying to perceivers so deeply
rical shapes created by Heider and Simmel (1944). The motivated to understand.
film depicts three geometrical shapes moving inside, out-
side, and around a square. The objects move as a group
Communication
but in imperfect, apparently self-propelled fashion. Like
a blurry picture coming into focus, the initial movements Mental states enable understanding not only of what other
of the objects are unclear but quickly take on a coherent agents do but also of what others say. Communication
and organizing set of mental states that makes sense of inherently involves getting information from one person’s
the otherwise random movements (big square is moving head to that of another to achieve shared understanding,
around . . . and seems unhappy with little square . . . and is a task that at least implicitly requires considering another
now chasing him around the house . . . ). All but 1 of the person’s desires, beliefs, intentions, and knowledge (Clark &
34 participants who initially watched the film described it Wilkes-Gibbs, 1986; Fussell & Krauss, 1992; Isaacs & Clark,
using clearly anthropomorphic mental states, and for good 1987; Schober, 1993; Wilkes-Gibbs & Clark, 1992). Norms
reason: of conversation (Grice, 1975) dictate that people follow the
“principle of optimal design,” sharing only relevant infor-
As long as the pattern of events shown in the film is perceived
mation that accounts for the knowledge shared by both
in terms of movements as such, it presents a chaos of juxta-
speakers and listeners. From a speaker ’s perspective, know-
posed items. When, however, the geometrical figures assume
personal characteristics so that their movements are per-
ing what is relevant for communication requires thinking
ceived in terms of motives and sentiments, a unified structure about what listeners know and what they do not. Fewer
appears. . . . But motives and sentiments are psychological details, for instance, are required when giving driving direc-
entities. . . . They are “mentalistic concepts,” so-called inter- tions to a local than to a visitor, and people do indeed alter
vening variables that bring order into the array of behavior their communication to match the apparent knowledge of a
mediating them. (Heider, 1958, pp. 31–32) listener (Lau, Chiu, & Hong, 2001).

CH14.indd 501 12/22/09 3:39:36 PM


502 Mind Perception

From a listener ’s perspective, understanding communi- that evolutionary psychologists consider the acquisition
cation requires clarifying the ambiguity inherent in almost of this capacity to be one of the most likely reasons the
all spoken language based on the speaker ’s presumed human brain is three times larger than that of our nearest
intentions and goals. The same comment about one’s “nice primate relative (Herrmann et al., 2007).
work,” “great haircut,” or “brilliant remark” could be inter- Reasoning about other minds can be useful for coordi-
preted as a genuine compliment from a friend or a sarcas- nating action to achieve a wide variety of goals. Within
tic barb from a foe, depending on the speaker ’s presumed groups and societies, for instance, knowing “who knows
intention. Identical words can therefore communicate what” enables effective decision making. Instead of requir-
different meanings, and knowledge about the underlying ing that each person know the same information within
intention is required for information to be communicated each group, simply remembering what every other person
effectively. Notice, for instance, the importance of a jour- in the group (or society) knows enables group members to
nalist’s intentions when interpreting the following (genuine) identify appropriate experts when necessary. Such “trans-
newspaper headlines: “Prostitutes appeal to Pope,” “Miners active memory” (Wegner, 1986) can enable groups to make
refuse to work after death,” “Red tape holds up new decisions more quickly and efficiently (Ren, Carley, &
bridges,” and “Kids make nutritional snacks.” Argote, 2006; Zhang, Hempel, Han, & Tjosvold, 2007) and
Because communication relies so heavily on mental generally improves group performance on interdependent
state inferences, language use and mind perception are tasks by giving individuals access to more knowledge than
close traveling companions in the psychological literature. they could possess alone (Austin, 2003; Lewis, Lange, &
When people fail to communicate effectively, it is often Gillis, 2005; Moreland & Argote, 2003; Wegner, 1995).
because of a failure of mind perception to know what In negotiation and conflict resolution, accurately identify-
needs to be said or to understand what was just said rather ing the other side’s preferences can enable more desirable
than a more basic failure to know how to use a given lan- outcomes for both parties (Elfenbein, Foo, White, Tan, &
guage (Keysar & Barr, 2002). And people who are slow to Aik, 2007; Galinsky, Maddux, Gilin, & White, 2008;
develop language, namely, deaf children whose parents do Neale & Bazerman, 1983; Thompson & Hastie, 1990),
not use sign language, are also developmentally delayed whereas incorrectly identifying the other side’s prefer-
in their abilities to reason about others’ mental states (de ences and motives can lead to less desirable outcomes
Villiers, 2005). Whether the ability to reason about mental (Epley, Caruso, & Bazerman, 2006). And in almost any
states is necessary for language to develop (Baron-Cohen, interdependent social relationship where achieving one’s
1995; Bloom, 2000; Hare, 2007), or the other way around own goals depends on others, mind perception is centrally
(de Villiers, 2005; Gernsbacher & Frymiare, 2006), is involved in decisions about whether to trust another per-
unclear. What is clear is that the ability to reason about son and cooperate with them or to distrust and compete
mental states is a major benefit when communicating with with them (Lee, 2008; McCabe, Smith, & LePore, 2000;
others, and this benefit is likely one of the reasons that Rilling, Sanfey, Aronson, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2004).
people think about others’ mental states so readily. Social interaction is often likened to a game of chess, and
accurately using mental states to predict others’ behavior
enables people to play well.
Coordination
Perhaps the most basic interpersonal goal facilitated by
Understanding one’s own and others’ behavior is good, mind perception, however, is to affiliate and connect with
being able to communicate with others is better, but being desirable others (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). This goal
able to use this understanding and communication to requires people to think about others’ existing impressions
predict others’ behavior is the best. Understanding why of the self, to behave in ways that are likely to maintain
another agent performs a particular action is a prerequi- those favorable impressions, and to consider how one’s
site for predicting it, and mind perception enables the own behavior is likely to be interpreted by others (Snyder,
ordinary perceiver to reason about mental states that are 1974). For instance, in the Machiavellian classic How
positively correlated with subsequent behavior. In a world to Win Friends and Influence People (1936/1981), Dale
of interdependent relationships where people are trying to Carnegie advised readers to “try honestly to see things
cooperate and compete, help and hurt, and persuade and from the other person’s point of view” (p. 175) and to
deceive, being able to reason one step back about medi- “talk in terms of the other person’s interests” (p. 98).
ating mental states allows people to stay one step ahead Indeed, increasing the sense of shared interests among
of others’ subsequent behavior. Mind perception does not people also increases social connection and liking (Brewer,
give people magical powers of foresight that operate with 1979; Byrne, 1971; Tajfel, 1981). Considering another ’s
perfect acuity, but it provides enough predictive validity goals, intentions, emotions, and beliefs can lead people to

CH14.indd 502 12/22/09 3:39:37 PM


Activating Mind Perception 503

identify those who are likely to be good targets for affiliation weapons, etc.) that is necessary for survival. Herrmann et al.
and to tune their behavior so that it is consistent with a (2007) suggest that rampant mind perception and social
target’s presumed beliefs to increase the similarity and con- intelligence are critical for living successfully in large
nection (Galinsky, Ku, & Wang, 2005; Sinclair, Huntsinger, groups and societies, evidenced by the strong correlation
Skorinko, & Hardin, 2005). Being finely attuned to others’ between social complexity and brain size among nonhu-
mental states can indeed be a fine way to win friends and man primates (Dunbar, 2003).
influence people, and being oblivious to them is a fine way It is not that such evolutionary explanations are likely to
to end up alone (Davis, 1983; Galinsky et al., 2005). Given be wrong, but rather that they are extremely difficult
that social isolation was tantamount to a death sentence to test. We have therefore focused on more proximal expla-
for most of human evolutionary history (Case & Williams, nations. In particular, knowledge about one’s own and
2004), it is perhaps no surprise that normally developed others’ mental states seems critical for explaining behav-
humans possess the apparently universal capacity for mind ior, for communicating effectively, and for coordinating
perception (Baron-Cohen, 1995). actions. Although the ability to think about other minds and
Finally, mind reading is beneficial for coordination in a get beyond one’s own limited perspective may be among
slightly different interpersonal context—coordinating one’s the most useful capacities that evolution has built into the
current behavior and goals with one’s future self. People human brain, this does not mean that it is used to solve
think about themselves at some point in the future in much all of our problems, at all times, and in all circumstances.
the same way that they think about others (Pronin, Olivola, & Capacities are not to be mistaken for tendencies. The pri-
Kennedy, 2008; Pronin & Ross, 2006), using the same mary uses of mind perception matter because they provide
psychological processes that enable people to reason about insight into when people are likely to activate this capacity
mental states in others (Buckner & Carroll, 2007). People and when they are not.
marry, divorce, accept jobs, and save for retirement at least
partly based on beliefs about what will make their future
minds feel happy and contented. Although people often ACTIVATING MIND PERCEPTION
make the same kinds of mistakes when thinking about
their own future mental states (Wilson & Gilbert, 2005) The benefits of mind perception are inherently social in
as they do when thinking about others’ mental states (e.g., nature, enabling interdependent actions across the gap
Epley, 2008; Keysar & Barr, 2002; Nickerson, 1999; Van between self and others, as well as the gap between the
Boven & Loewenstein, 2003), there are also many benefits present and the future. Getting into the mind of another
to strategic planning and preparation that may ultimately agent is therefore most beneficial when interdependence
make it worth the occasional costs of regret and wasted exists between self and others or when one’s current deci-
resources. sions are directly related to one’s future reality. As a result,
situational and dispositional factors that increase interde-
pendence also increase attempts to get beyond one’s own
Summary: Why Mind?
current perspective to consider the minds of others.
Ultimately, the only relevant outcomes for the mindless Power is perhaps the clearest determinant of social
machinery of natural selection are survival and sexual interdependence. Those in positions of power are indepen-
reproduction, and the capacities that enable people to live dent of others because their own outcomes are unaffected
long enough to have many viable offspring are likely those by others’ actions, whereas those without power are inher-
passed from one generation to the next. It is not hard to ently interdependent because their outcomes are controlled
tell a story about how a keen mind-reading ability would by others’ actions (Fiske, 1993). Independence is not an
increase the likelihood of both survival and sex (Nichols & ancillary feature of social power; it is the defining feature
Stich, 2003). Guthrie (1993), for instance, suggests that (Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003). And as one would
people have a systematic bias toward seeing humanlike predict given the benefits of mind perception, people
mental states in other agents—even nonhuman agents— in positions of power are less likely to consider others’
because the consequences of failing to detect an intentional mental states than are people in positions without power.
agent in one’s environment exact a greater cost on one’s The powerful, for instance, are less likely than the
reproductive fitness than mistakenly identifying an inten- relatively powerless to attend to other people amid social
tional agent that is not there. Mithen (1996) suggests that interactions (Erber & Fiske, 1984; Neuberg & Fiske, 1987;
the attribution of humanlike mental states to nonhuman Ruscher & Fiske, 1990), are less able to accurately identify
stimuli has been adaptive in providing a useful analogy to others’ interests and attitudes following a social interac-
reason about both the natural world and technology (tools, tion (Keltner & Robinson, 1997), are less likely to report

CH14.indd 503 12/22/09 3:39:37 PM


504 Mind Perception

trying to see things from the other person’s perspective in other minds than do children without siblings (Jenkins &
everyday life (Galinsky, Magee, Inesi, & Gruenfeld, 2006), Astington, 1996; Lewis, Freeman, Kyriakidou, Maridaki-
are less likely to experience empathy and compassion for Kassotaki, & Berridge, 1996; McAlister & Peterson, 2007;
the suffering of another person (Van Kleef et al., 2009), Perner, Ruffman, & Leekam, 1994). Beyond the local cul-
and are more likely to treat others as mindless objects ture of one’s family, the interdependent nature of one’s
to achieve their own goals (Gruenfeld, Inesi, Magee, & broader ethnic or national culture influences mind per-
Galinsky, 2008). In one simple demonstration, participants ception. Markus and Kitayama (1991, p. 229) note that
were asked to draw the letter E on their forehead (as a “the reciprocal interdependence with others that is the sign
measure of self-focused attention, Hass, 1984). Those who of the interdependent self . . . requires inhibiting the
had just reflected on a time that they were in a high-power ‘I’ perspective and processing instead from the ‘thou’
position were less likely to draw the E in an other-focused perspective. . . . The requirement is to ‘read’ the other ’s
manner so that it could be read from an observer ’s per- mind and thus to know what the other is thinking or feel-
spective compared with those who had just reflected on ing.” Collectivist cultures in which the interdependent
a time they had been in a low-power position (Galinsky self-concept dominates, such as in Southeast Asia, there-
et al., 2006). This does not mean that people in a position fore tend to produce individuals who are also more likely
of power are unable to consider others’ mental states if to consider other minds in social interactions compared
they are specifically motivated to do so (e.g., Overbeck & with members of independent cultures, such as the United
Park, 2006), but rather that possessing independence from States and Western Europe, who focus more heavily on the
others is likely to diminish people’s tendency to use their self (Cohen & Gunz, 2002; Leung & Cohen, 2007). When
mind perception abilities. interpreting spoken instructions, for instance, members of
Other factors that increase interdependence support this collectivist cultures tend to consider the speakers’ inten-
more general conclusion as well. Those who are especially tions and visual perspective more readily than do members
motivated to connect with others, for instance, are more of individualist cultures (Wu & Keysar, 2007). Like any
likely to attend to others’ emotions (Pickett, Gardner, & other behavioral tendency, thinking about other minds may
Knowles, 2004), are more likely to mimic others’ behavior become more habitual in an environment that reinforces
in a social interaction (Cheng & Chartrand, 2003; Lakin & people for doing so.
Chartrand, 2003), and are more likely to attribute mental All of these results suggest that activating one’s capacity
states to nonhuman agents such as electronic gadgets and for mind perception can be moderated by the interdepen-
pets (Epley, Akalis, Waytz, & Cacioppo, 2008). People are dent nature of the social context. This should not be taken
more likely to seek information about others’ mental states as evidence, however, that people automatically think
when they expect to meet them in the future than when about other minds in interdependent contexts. Monitoring
they do not (Douglas, 1984, 1990) and are more likely to others’ thoughts requires attention and mental effort, and
accommodate another ’s perspective when effective com- people can consider others’ perspectives only after they
munication is important (Higgins, McCann, & Fondacaro, have acquired a perspective themselves. People can there-
1982; Higgins & Rholes, 1978; Krauss & Fussell, 1991; fore fail to consider others’ mental states because they are
Zajonc, 1960). The actions of those perceived to be a direct so absorbed in their own. In negotiations between indi-
threat to the self—and hence more interdependent on one’s viduals or groups, for instance, explicitly asking people
own actions—are perceived to be more intentional than to consider the other side’s thoughts and preferences can
the actions of those who are not perceived to be a threat significantly alter people’s behavior and the outcomes of
(Kozak, Czipri, Felczak, & Correll, 2009). Even thinking the negotiation, suggesting that negotiators who were not
about money, the presence of which enables independence explicitly instructed were also not naturally considering
from others (Vohs, Mead, & Goode, 2006), decreases the others’ perspective to the same degree. Sometimes such
tendency to adopt another ’s perspective in social interaction perspective taking can improve negotiation outcomes
(Mead, Caruso, Baumeister, & Vohs, 2009). Mind percep- because it highlights potential integrative solutions (e.g.,
tion is critical in interdependent contexts, and those contexts Galinsky, Maddux, et al., 2008; Neale & Bazerman, 1983;
indeed appear to activate one’s mind perception abilities. Thompson & Hastie, 1990), and sometimes it can hinder
Situational influences can alter the tendency to engage negotiations because it highlights selfish and conflicting
in mind perception from one moment to the next, but motives in others (Epley et al., 2006). Regardless of the
developmental and cultural influences can create longer- direction, both effects arise because considering others’
lasting changes from one person to another. Having sib- thoughts activates information that was not activated
lings increases a child’s need to consider other minds, and already when people were absorbed in their own perspec-
children with siblings appear better able to reason about tive. These results simply serve as a reminder that others’

CH14.indd 504 12/22/09 3:39:38 PM


Acquiring Mind Perception 505

mental states are inherently invisible, and that people of errors committed by adults when their capacity is taxed,
are unlikely to see them unless they are actively looking differing more in the degree of error rather than in the kind
for them. of error. Understanding how the ability to reason about
Although increasing the degree of interdependence is mental states comes into being over the course of years has
the most widely studied moderator of activating the capac- been fruitful for understanding how this capacity functions
ity to consider other minds, it is not the only one. For over the course of moments in full-grown adults.
instance, considering the meaning of an event (i.e., why
an event happened) makes the agent performing the event A Theory of Mind
seem more mindful compared with focusing on low-level
details of the event (i.e., how an event happened; Kozak, Tracking the development of any capacity requires a con-
Marsh, & Wegner, 2006). Likewise, increasing the appar- crete understanding of the capacity being tracked and how
ent unpredictability of an agent increases attention to the to measure it. Mind perception involves making infer-
agent’s mental states (Barrett & Johnson, 2003; Epley, ences about one’s own and others’ mental states by pos-
Waytz, Akalis, & Cacioppo, 2008; Waytz, Morewedge, iting unobservable properties such as intentions, desires,
et al., 2009). These additional findings are consistent with goals, beliefs, and secondary emotions to serve as media-
the main benefits of mind perception discussed in the pre- tors between people’s sensory input and their subsequent
vious section, suggesting that people are more likely to action. This inferential process is usually referred to as a
think about other minds when explaining, understanding, theory of mind for two main reasons, “first, because such
or predicting others’ actions. [mental] states are not directly observable, and second,
The results discussed in this section provide some because the system can be used to make predictions, specif-
insight into when people are likely to think about other ically about the behavior of other organisms” (Premack &
minds but not into how people do so once the capacity Woodruff, 1978, p. 515). Considerable confusion and incon-
is activated. Understanding how people solve the other sistency has plagued the use of the term “theory of mind”
minds problem requires first looking beyond the traditional because it has been used at some times too narrowly by
boundaries in social psychology to understand how people including only theory-based inferences about other minds
acquire the capacity to reason about other minds over the and at other times too broadly by including social cogni-
course of development and then identifying tasks that tive inferences that do not involve mind perception per se
reveal the mechanisms enabling people to make inferences (e.g., dispositional inference). We therefore use “theory
about others’ mental states. We take both of these steps in of mind” to refer only to a person’s intuitive belief that
turn over the next two sections. other agents do indeed have minds and that this belief con-
tains some knowledge about how minds work.
Understanding what a theory of mind would entail is
ACQUIRING MIND PERCEPTION easier than knowing when someone is using it. Premack
and Woodruff (1978) first raised this issue when consid-
Like many desirable human capacities, such as the ability ering whether chimpanzees have a theory of mind. They
to effectively use language, tools, or basketballs, people demonstrated, for instance, that a chimpanzee watching a
are not born with the ability to overcome the other minds person perform a goal-directed action (e.g., struggling to
problem but instead develop the ability over time. Social get a banana out of a locker) would consistently select a
psychologists are not usually excited about explaining tool that would enable the person to achieve a presumed
variability in a person’s behavior across time, largely goal (e.g., a key) when given the choice between a rele-
because they are more interested in explaining variability vant and an irrelevant tool (e.g., a key or a hose attached
in a person’s behavior across situations at a single point in to a water spigot). Three commentators were unimpressed
time. But in this case, longitude informs latitude. The lon- and independently suggested a different method that has
gitudinal trajectory of mind perception across time pro- dominated research in this area ever since—testing cases
vides insight into variability in mind perception across in which a target’s mental states differ from a perceiver ’s
situations at any one point in time. Reasoning about some own mental states (Bennett, 1978; Dennett, 1978; Harman,
mental states (intentions and goals) seems to be easy, 1978). Dennett, for instance, noted that simple associations
whereas reasoning about other mental states seems to be and learned experience could create behavior that looked
difficult (mistaken beliefs and differing perspectives). The the same as using a theory of mind. Because true beliefs
speed with which these capacities develop in children can tend to correspond with reality, this covariance makes it
help to identify which is which. Many errors committed difficult to determine whether agents are responding to
by children as their capacity develops are the same kinds their own beliefs about reality or to what they know about

CH14.indd 505 12/22/09 3:39:38 PM


506 Mind Perception

another agent’s beliefs. To identify that perceivers are actions (e.g., an experimenter reaching for a new toy after
using a theory of mind would require testing their ability reaching repeatedly in the same way for another toy) than
to reason from another agent’s false beliefs—or another ’s to novel biological action directed toward the same goal
belief that differs from one’s own belief. More than simply (e.g., an experimenter reaching for the same toy after
providing a suggestion, Dennett also provided a method repeatedly reaching for it using a different behavioral
of experimentation (as did Harman, albeit less explicitly). grasp; Woodward, 1998). Children as young as 7 months
What if the key that normally opened the locker had been even imitate another person’s goal-directed reach regard-
moved to a new location by an evil experimenter, who then less of whether the goal was achieved but do not mimic
swapped a useless key in its usual place? Would the original accidental or apparently unintentional reaching (Hamlin,
experimenter look in the location where the key is usually Hallinan, & Woodward, 2008; see also Carpenter, Akhtar, &
kept, and where the original experimenter therefore believes Tomasello, 1998; Meltzoff, 1995). By 2 years of age, chil-
it to be? Or would the original experimenter look in the new dren use an agent’s intentions to encode their actions, such
location, where the perceiver now knows it to be? as using an artist’s intentions when providing names for
This procedure has come to be known as the false belief drawings (Preissler & Bloom, 2008).
task (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985; Wimmer & Selectively attending to other agents does not, however,
Perner, 1983). It is generally considered one of the criti- give insight into other agents’ mental experience without
cal markers of a fully developed theory of mind because it the additional capacity of sharing their attentional perspec-
requires using many of the most sophisticated elements of tive. Most adults know that if you want to find out what
mind perception—postulating unobservable mental states, people are thinking you should pay attention to where they
using those states to predict behavior that would differ are looking. Indeed, people not only think about objects
from the way an objective observer would respond, and they are led to look at (Grant & Spivey, 2003) but also
using those mental states (in humans) to explain the tar- tend to look at objects they are thinking about (Rayner,
get’s mistaken action. Solving the false belief task requires, 1998; Tanenhaus, Magnuson, Dahan, & Chambers, 2000;
at least theoretically, the recognition that others’ mental Tanenhaus, Spivey-Knowlton, Eberhard, & Sedivy, 1996).
states may differ fundamentally from one’s own and that Children seem to implicitly understand this as early as
behavior is determined by others’ beliefs independent of 2 months, when they look preferentially at a person’s eyes
one’s own beliefs. Armed with a new method and a clear compared with other aspects of the face (Maurer, 1985), and
goal, psychologists raced to discover when this theory of by 2 years they can almost perfectly orient themselves in
mind develops in children, who can solve theory of mind the direction of another ’s gaze (Baldwin, 1991, 1993). The
tasks and who cannot, and what factors alter the ability to eyes may not provide a window into a person’s soul, but
solve these tasks. The resulting research produced a flurry they certainly provide a window into that person’s mind by
of results and a blizzard of controversy. Once everything developing joint attention between a target and a perceiver.
settled, the following firm conclusions remained. With these foundational abilities of attending to social
agents, representing desires and goals, and acquiring joint
Developing a Theory of Mind attention in place, theory of mind begins to develop with
Cross-Age Development Reasoning about other minds more sophistication. Between 18 months and 2 years, chil-
first requires paying some attention to the agents that might dren begin to identify intentions from an actor ’s speech
possess minds. This begins early, around 3 months of age, (Baldwin & Moses, 1996) and begin using mental states
when children start finding the common targets of mind (e.g., “want”) in their own speech (Bartsch & Wellman,
perception especially interesting and show preferential 1995; Bretherton & Beeghly, 1982; Wellman, 1993).
attention toward other people (Legerstee, 1991), animate Children around this time also develop a more complete
objects (Crichton & Lange-Küttner, 1999, and biological understanding of the concept “desire” and its implica-
motion (Bertenthal, Proffitt, & Cutting, 1984). This hyper- tions, understanding that people behave in accordance with
sensitivity to agency remains a critical component of mind their desires and are unhappy if they do not get what they
perception for the rest of people’s lives (Barrett, 2000). want (Bartsch & Wellman, 1995). By age 3, children also
After attending to agents that might possess a mind, develop a more complete understanding of intention, differ-
observers need to develop a tendency to interpret and entiating intended from unintended action (Shultz, 1991),
understand those agents’ actions in terms of mental states and between 4 and 5 years children understand intentions
such as desires and intentions (e.g., she likes that or he as separate from desires and as separate from the outcomes
wants to leave). Around 6 months of age, children appear of intentional acts (Astington, 1993; Moses, 1993).
to distinguish between goal-directed action and simple A fully sophisticated theory of mind, however, involves
biological motion, attending longer to novel goal-related not only the recognition that other agents have mental

CH14.indd 506 12/22/09 3:39:39 PM


Acquiring Mind Perception 507

states that predict and explain behavior—children seem appears to stem from difficulties inhibiting one’s own
to overcome the philosophical version of the other minds present perspective or knowledge (Carlson, Moses, & Hix,
problem before they are out of diapers—but also the added 1998; Carlson & Moses, 2001; Zaitchik, 1991), a difficulty
ability to recognize that others’ mental states may differ that leads to conceptually identical egocentric biases in
from one’s own. Differing motivations, goals, intentions, judgment among full-grown adults (Birch & Bloom, 2007;
beliefs, or even vantage point may lead people to perceive, Epley, Keysar, Van Boven, & Gilovich, 2004; Keysar &
evaluate, and understand the same stimulus in different Barr, 2002). Although this ability to reason about others’
ways. A fully developed theory of mind enables perspec- perspectives may not develop quite as quickly as many
tive taking—the ability to reason about others’ mental parents would like, the basic components of a fully devel-
states independent of one’s own. Piaget (1959) recognized oped theory of mind are already in place by the time most
that this ability to overcome egocentrism marked the major children are off to kindergarten.
difference between children and adults:
Cross-Species Development Human infants acquire the
We have endeavored to show . . . that thought in the child is ability to reason about other minds over the course of years,
egocentric, i.e., that the child thinks for himself without trou-
but Homo sapiens acquired it over the course of millennia.
bling to make himself understood nor to place himself at the
Notable similarities and differences in mind perception
other person’s point of view. . . . If this be the case, we must
expect childish reasoning to differ very considerably from
exist between humans and their closest relatives, similari-
ours, to be deductive and above all less rigorous. (p. 1) ties that bear testament to a common evolutionary ancestry
and differences that may help to explain notable gaps in
Various measures suggest that children, at least in terms behavioral capacities. Premack and Woodruff (1978) ques-
of their theory of mind, start looking more like adults tioned whether chimpanzees have a theory of mind, and
around the age of 4 or 5 (Deutsch & Pechmann, 1982; the answer has proven elusive ever since. Although such
Flavell, 1986; Sonnenschein & Whitehurst, 1984). Most attempts have long been plagued by allegations of ram-
extensively studied at this age are children’s abilities to pant anthropomorphism on the part of researchers rather
reason about false beliefs. Following the suggestions by than actual abilities on the part of primates (Asquith, 1986;
Dennett (1978) and others described earlier, Wimmer and Darwin, 1872/2002; Hebb, 1946; Povinelli & Vonk, 2003),
Perner (1983) developed the false belief task in which par- there are now good reasons to believe that chimpanzees
ticipants are asked to predict how someone with a false and some other great apes possess a rudimentary theory of
belief is likely to act. In the typical false belief task, partic- mind that is similar to that of a developing human (Call &
ipants watch a story in which one character (Sally) places an Tomasello, 2008). Like young infants, chimpanzees pref-
object (a key) in a drawer and then leaves the room. While erentially attend to social agents and can track (albeit not
Sally is away, her sister (Ann) walks into the room and perfectly) another agent’s eye gaze to monitor that agent’s
moves the key to a cupboard on the other side of the room. attention (Povinelli & Eddy, 1996a). Chimpanzees also
Sally then returns to the room, and participants predict have some understanding of goals. Like human infants
where Sally will look for the key. The obvious answer to (Behne, Carpenter, Call, & Tomasello, 2005), chimpanzees
any adult is that Sally will look where she believes the key differentiate between intentional and accidental behavior,
to be, namely, in the drawer where she put it. This is not responding differently, for instance, when a person fails to
obvious, however, to a 3-year-old, who is likely to predict give them food because of an inability to do so versus an
that Sally will look where the child knows the object to be, unwillingness to do so (Call, Hare, Carpenter, & Tomasello,
namely, in the cupboard. Children start to solve this partic- 2004). Chimpanzees also respond differently to accidental
ular problem correctly around the age of 4 and do so more versus intentional actions (Call & Tomasello, 1998), such
reliably around the age of 5 in cultures around the world as when another chimpanzee steals food versus receives
(Callaghan et al., 2005; Perner, Leekam, & Wimmer, 1987; the same food innocently (Jensen, Call, & Tomasello,
Perner & Wimmer, 1985). 2007). Chimpanzees also imitate an intentional action
The development of the ability to reason about false even when the action has not been completed (Myowa-
beliefs in others mirrors the development of the ability to Yamakoshi & Matsuzawa, 2000; Tomasello & Carpenter,
attribute false beliefs to the self. Three-year-olds who learn 2005). Chimpanzees will hand a person an object they
a fact during an experimental session, for instance, tend appear to be reaching for intentionally, for instance, but
to claim that they have always known that fact, whereas not one that the person is “reaching” for only accidentally
4- and 5-year-olds report that they did not know the fact (Warneken & Tomasello, 2006).
before the session (Taylor, Esbensen, & Bennett, 1994). More advanced capacities also emerge among chimpan-
This difficulty in reasoning about false beliefs in others zees, most notably the recognition that other agents possess

CH14.indd 507 12/22/09 3:39:39 PM


508 Mind Perception

beliefs and knowledge. Chimpanzees not only track others’ of which other sophisticated human capacities develop—
eye gaze but also communicate in gestures that suggest from imagining the future, to reconstructing the past, to
an understanding that other agents’ knowledge is at least using symbolic language (Hare, 2007). Science may not
partly a function of their visual perspective (Povinelli & end up identifying mind perception as the critical human
Eddy, 1996b). Chimpanzees, for instance, are more likely capacity that moved us as a species from savannahs to sky-
to hide or take food from others when they are not look- scrapers, but it is not a bad bet.
ing or when others’ vision is occluded (Hare, Call, &
Methods of Acquisition
Tomasello, 2006; Melis, Call, & Tomasello, 2006).
Although chimpanzees have these basic building blocks The trajectory through which people develop a theory of
in place for a fully functioning theory of mind, they do mind does not explain how people acquire it, but under-
not seem able to understand false beliefs or recognize that standing this process of acquisition provides insight into
others’ evaluations of a stimulus might differ fundamen- how adults in everyday life are likely to use their mind per-
tally from their own (for reviews, see Call & Tomasello, ception faculties. Before psychological science required
2008; Hare, 2007; Penn & Povinelli, 2007). Chimpanzees calculations to make assertions, many philosophers noted
fail nonverbal versions of false belief tasks that 5-year-old that the easiest way to solve the other minds problem was
humans readily pass (Call & Tomasello, 1999), even when to use one’s own mental experiences as a guide or anal-
the task is competitive and rewards are clearly contingent ogy (e.g., Hume, 1739/1958; Kant, 1781/1953; Russell,
on performance (Hare, Call, & Tomasello, 2001). 1948; Smith, 1759/1976). Common to all of these propos-
Such a divergence in mind perception could arise either als is that understanding others’ mental states first requires,
because humans have acquired more general intelligence as Mill (1872/1974) suggested, understanding “my own
over evolutionary time or because humans have acquired case”—understanding how one’s own mind works by
more social intelligence in particular. Emerging evidence noting covariance between one’s own behavior and one’s
suggests that evolution may have favored particular over inner experience. Theories about other minds may develop
general. Consider one particularly ambitious set of experi- out of childhood experience with one’s own mind follow-
ments comparing a sample of more than 100 2.5-year-old ing basic principles of inference—that agents should have
humans with the same number of adult chimpanzees (and minds like one’s own to the extent that they appear similar
32 more distantly related orangutans for good measure; on relevant dimensions.
Herrmann et al., 2007). No meaningful difference emerged Almost immediately after birth, human infants mimic an
among these groups in performance on technical or physi- adult’s facial expression, head movements, and hand ges-
cal tasks, such as the ability to track a reward after its loca- tures (Meltzoff & Moore, 1997). Such mimicry occurs quite
tion changed, the ability to understand and use numerical automatically through the rest of life (Chartrand, Maddux, &
quantities, or several measures of the ability to understand Lakin, 2005). This provides a mechanism for develop-
causal relations (including using tools). Humans and chim- ing a theory of mind because people tend to use their own
panzees solved approximately 68% of these tasks correctly, actions and behaviors as information when making infer-
and orangutans solved 59% correctly. Large differences ences about their own experiences, thoughts, and inten-
emerged, however, on the three major tests of social tions. Adults, for instance, find cartoons to be funnier when
intelligence, including the ability to learn by mimicking holding a pen in their mouth that forces a smile (Strack,
others, the ability to communicate through gestures that Martin, & Stepper, 1988), find persuasive messages even
accommodate another ’s attentional state, and two theory more compelling when nodding their heads up and down
of mind tests (following an actor ’s gaze and understand- (Wells & Petty, 1980), and rate stimuli more favorably
ing an actor ’s intention following an unsuccessful action). when moving their arms in an approach-oriented posture
Children solved these social tasks correctly nearly 75% of than when moving them in an avoidance-oriented pos-
the time, whereas chimpanzees and orangutans did so 33% ture (Cacioppo, Priester, & Berntson, 1993; for a review,
and 36% of the time, respectively. Although still a matter see Semin & Smith, 2008). Others’ emotions and experi-
of speculation, these authors suggest that this heightened ence are contagious, at least in part, because humans from
social intelligence serves “as a kind of ‘bootstrap’ for the the early moments of life find themselves quite naturally
distinctively complex development of human cognition mimicking others’ actions (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson,
in general” (Herrmann et al., 2007, p. 1360). Our nearest 1994; Meltzoff & Brooks, 2001).
primate relatives have the building blocks necessary for Many systematic mistakes that young children make
mind perception but not the full ability to reason about oth- when reasoning about others suggest that young children
ers’ mental states independent of one’s own. This seem- do indeed assume that others’ mental states are the same
ingly unremarkable difference may be the one ability out as one’s own. For instance, if children are shown a pencil

CH14.indd 508 12/22/09 3:39:39 PM


Acquiring Mind Perception 509

box containing candy, 3-year-olds mistakenly report that on false belief tasks (Perner et al., 1994). Children whose
they knew the contents of the box all along before opening parents discuss mental state terms early in development
it and that others will know the actual contents of the box also understand false beliefs more quickly (Dunn, Brown,
as well, whereas older children correctly note they believed Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991).
the pencil box would contain pencils rather than candy and Acquiring the ability to reason about other minds occurs
that others will also be mistaken (Astington & Gopnik, in a relatively predictable pattern, incorporating two broad
1988; Gopnik & Astington, 1988). These mistakes are not sources of information—first-person experience from the
random but instead are systematically egocentric. Children inside and third-person observation from the outside. Of
predict that other agents will know what “I” know and these, first-person experience appears primary in develop-
behave accordingly. When children are able to understand ment, with the basic mechanisms that would enable first-
themselves and the workings of their own mental states, person experience of others’ inner mental states present
they readily apply these mental states to others (Harris, already at birth (or at least as close to birth as new moth-
2002; Meltzoff, 2007; Piaget & Inhelder, 1956). The sym- ers are willing to let scientists get). The mistakes made
metrical nature of theory of mind development for perceiv- by young children, as well as by nonhuman primates, are
ing oneself and for perceiving others suggests that children consistent with an early egocentric view of the world that
may use knowledge about the self to understand others’ is later supplanted by recognition that first-person expe-
minds (Harris, 1991; Johnson, 1988) or that the same theo- rience may be misleading and that one’s egocentric per-
ries and perceptions that guide self-understanding also ception may be inappropriate as an analogy for others.
guide the understanding of others (Gopnik & Meltzoff, A fully developed theory of mind therefore employs
1994). As we discuss later, it is likely that both of these acquired information about the agent being perceived
processes are operating. To at least some degree, acquir- and information acquired from a host of external sources and
ing information about one’s own experiences and resulting observations about how particular minds work. How these
mental states serves as a likely guide to acquiring informa- two sources of information create a fully developed theory
tion about others. of mind, and the precise degrees to which first-person per-
If infants begin their education about others’ mental ception versus third-person observation are involved, is
states by assuming that other minds are similar to one’s own actively debated (Goldman, 2008; Nichols & Stich, 2003)
(Meltzoff, 2007), then they earn their interpersonal doctor- and discussed in more detail in the next section. What
ates by recognizing the subtle and nuanced ways in which seems clear at the moment is that both sources of infor-
their own perceptions are unique. Later transitions in theory mation appear necessary to explain the entire existing pat-
of mind development appear to involve acquiring informa- tern of evidence from early infancy to later childhood, and
tion from external observation and through social learning attempts to reduce this complexity into a single source of
that suggests one’s own egocentric perspective is an imper- information so far have favored parsimony over reality.
fect analogy for reasoning about other minds (Gopnik,
1996; Gopnik, Slaughter, & Meltzoff, 1994; Gopnik & Summary: Acquiring Mind Perception
Wellman, 1994). Culture provides one influential source of
such information, and variance in theories of mind appear Understanding the development of mind perception is rel-
to stem at least partly from external knowledge conferred to evant for social psychologists not because of the timeline
young children over the course of development, rather it produces but rather because of the stairway it suggests.
than simply through egocentric experience. Children living The precise time at which some capacity can be detected
in rural cultures, for instance, acquire different theories is at least partly a function of the precision of the measure
about nonhuman animal minds compared with children liv- to track it. As those measures become more sophisticated
ing in urban cultures, cross-cultural variance that appears (or as the tasks get easier), the dates at which capacities
better explained by differences in socialization than by dif- emerge become increasingly earlier. This may lead, by
ferences in first-person simulation (Anggoro, Waxman, & extrapolation, to the feeling that developmental psychol-
Medin, 2008; Lillard, 1998; Shweder & Bourne, 1984). ogists will eventually show that an infant can write The
Similarly, experience that one’s own mental states may differ Brothers’ Karamazov if only given the right kind of pen-
from others is gained more quickly when more other minds cil. One recent study using a nonverbal version of the false
are present to learn from. Family members have a critical belief task, for instance, reports that children as young as
influence on theory of mind development, suggesting a 15 months may be predicting others’ behavior based on the
process of apprenticeship whereby children learn about other person’s presumed beliefs (Onishi & Baillargeon,
mental states from close others. Recall that children with 2005), instead of at four to five years old as observed in
siblings, for example, show reliably better performance linguistic versions of the false belief task.

CH14.indd 509 12/22/09 3:39:40 PM


510 Mind Perception

The more important point is to notice the pattern in This example illustrates both major pieces of information
which capacities develop and appear to build on one people can use when reasoning about other minds, as well
another across different measures. Infants do not enter the as the process underlying mind perception that is observed
world with an intuitive understanding of how minds work, over the course of years in children and over the course
but they develop it quickly. The process that adults go of moments in adults. Understanding another ’s differing
through when reasoning about other minds does not liter- mental states, for the young and the old alike, may first
ally recapitulate the developmental trajectory they followed require overcoming one’s own.
to acquire that capacity, but there are notable similarities. These two sources of information—one’s own mental
Adults tend to naturally mimic other people’s behavior, are states or experience and differentiating information about
relatively generous in attributing mental states to nonhu- other minds, such as causal theories or stereotypes—have
man agents, reliably use simple mental state concepts such been the backbone of psychological theories designed to
as intentions and desires to explain others’ behavior but explain the underlying mechanisms that enable mind per-
less reliably use more complicated concepts such as knowl- ception. Simulation theories have posited that people rea-
edge and beliefs, have an easy time inferring that others’ son about other minds by using themselves to simulate
preferences are similar to their own, and have relatively and reason about others’ mental states, whereas inferential
more difficulty reasoning about cases when others’ prefer- theories—usually encompassed by the umbrella term theory
ences are different than one’s own. Parents are consider- theory—have posited that people reason about other minds
ably more adept than their children at mind perception, but like intuitive scientists, using a theory of how minds work
they do not seem to outgrow their childhood tendencies so to make inferences about others’ mental states independent
much as they effortfully overcome them when engaging of one’s own psychological perspective.
in mind perception. Of course, acquiring a capacity and The debate between simulation and theory theory
using it in everyday life are different things. How adults accounts of mind perception began like many in psycho-
use their fully formed capacity for mind perception, how logical science, with proponents staking out extreme posi-
the surrounding environment enables or disables people’s tions that mind readers use only one source of information
ability to reason about other minds, and how mind percep- or the other. The years of accumulating research have not
tion shapes social life are precisely the kinds of questions been kind to strict adherents of either position. This debate
social psychologists care the most about. The rest of the is therefore resolving like many others in psychological
chapter provides at least a partial answer to each one. science, with hybrid models suggesting that people use
both simulation and theory-driven inference in varying
degrees, depending on the situational context and the
USING MIND PERCEPTION perceiver ’s processing capacity or motivational state.
These theoretical accounts have morphed so extremely
Oliver Sacks (2003) wrote a vivid account of what it is and complexly over time that any attempt to characterize
like to go blind later in life and unintentionally provided a them invariably misses subtleties across them. None of the
revealing example of everyday mind perception. In this evidence presented in the following paragraphs is com-
account, Sacks described two extreme outcomes following pletely consistent with one theoretical proposition and
adult-onset blindness, one in which people lose their sense completely inconsistent with the other, and the empirical
of the visual world altogether and another in which they tussle has produced many glancing blows without a single
develop a heightened sense of the visual world through knockout. Our approach is therefore to review evidence at
extremely vivid and detailed mental imagery. Sacks was least broadly consistent with each general mechanism and
describing a person from this latter vivid imagery group then to describe recent integrative accounts that we believe
whose carpentry skills had become so proficient that he was are likely to be more comprehensive, more accurate, and
able to replace all of the roof gutters on his house single- more useful for understanding how people reason about
handedly. This man reported that his neighbors were other minds in everyday life.
understandably startled to see a blind man doing carpen-
try work on his roof, but that they were especially startled Mechanisms of Mind Perception
when they saw him working in the dark of night.
Simulation Theories
This heightened startle comes from the reflexive ease
of imagining oneself on the roof in the dark of night. But The intuitive solution to the other minds problem is to use
startles are fleeting, and this reflexive egocentrism is sub- one’s own mind to simulate another ’s mind. As Russell
sequently replaced by the recognition that darkness is (1948, p. 483) argued, “The behavior of other people is
no trouble for a man without functioning eyes to see it. in many ways analogous to our own, and we suppose that

CH14.indd 510 12/22/09 3:39:40 PM


Using Mind Perception 511

it must have analogous causes.” This argument from that one’s own thoughts precede one’s behavior can lead
analogy has such a long history (Hume, 1739/1958; Mill, to the causal inference that thoughts cause actions, even
1872/1974; Ryle, 1949; Wittgenstein, 1953) that phi- if the actual neural sequence of events occurs in precisely
losophers defending it in the 1960s were already feeling the opposite direction (Wegner, 2002). This resulting expe-
somewhat apologetic—“To some it may seem like I am rience of intentional causation and free will can then be
flogging a dead horse” (Slote, 1966, p. 341). After all, used to infer that others’ behavior is caused by their inner
Kant (1781/1953, p. A353) had nearly two centuries earlier thoughts as well.
called the tendency for analogical reasoning about others A classic objection to the argument from analogy is
to be “obvious.” that people can viscerally imagine all sorts of experiences that
Psychologists in the 1960s were just getting started, they have never directly experienced themselves, such as the
however, in refining a descriptive account of the process pain of having a nail driven into one’s eye or the terror of
by which one’s own mental experience might be used to being buried alive. These simulation accounts, however,
solve the other minds problem and were doing so in the do not require that people simply project their own past
face of attacks from alternative models that were decid- experience onto others but rather that people possess the
edly less “obvious.” Floyd Allport had introduced the term ability to imagine their experiences in another state and
“social projection” in 1924 to describe the positive correla- use the output of that simulation as a proxy for others’
tion between students’ reports of their own cheating behavior mental states (Goldman, 2008). The ability to imagine an
and their estimates of others’ cheating (Allport, 1924; see event through fantasy and pretense without actually hav-
also Katz & Allport, 1931), but formal accounts of how ing to experience it is surely on the list of humans’ most
the mechanism underlying such projection might operate desirable capacities (Hegarty, 2004), enabling people to
did not emerge until considerably later. The culminating efficiently understand and learn about events without
efforts that appear to have sparked subsequent research taking the time or risking the danger of living through
occurred in 1986, when Heal, Gordon, and Humphrey all of them.
independently refined philosophy’s argument from analogy Of course, simulations are useful only when they are
by proposing that adults understand others’ mental states good analogues for the target or system to be understood.
through a process of first-person simulation (or what Heal Giving drugs to rats to simulate their effects on humans
called “replication”). By this account, people need not makes reasonable sense; giving drugs to robots for the
acquire any abstract or inferential theory of mind but can same purpose does not. People should therefore naturally
instead understand others’ mental states by imagining what use their own experience and simulation abilities to reason
they would think, desire, or intend if they were in the same about others who appear similar on relevant attributes to
situation and then projecting the results of this simula- the self and should be less inclined to do so to reason about
tion onto the target (Harris, 1992). “We could . . . imagine others who seem different from the self. In the presence of
what it’s like to be [others],” Humphrey wrote (1986, a person or agent perceived to be similar to oneself, and in
pp. 71–72), “because we know what it’s like to be ourselves the absence of any other individuating information, using
[and] make sense of [others’] behavior by projecting what one’s own introspective experience as a guide may not only
I know about my mind into them.” This approach essen- be a perfectly rational thing to do (Krueger & Acevedo,
tially turns the philosophical version of the other minds 2005), it may be the only possible thing to do (Dawes &
problem on its head—people can reason about others’ mental Mulford, 1996). Although this moderating factor of simi-
states precisely because they have first-person access to larity was not an explicit feature in original accounts of
their own mental states. simulation theories, it was at least implied by the logic of
The term simulation is no accident; it specifies when this the theory itself. Simulation may therefore be most use-
process is likely to be used and when it is not. Simulations fully characterized as just one tool or heuristic that people
are useful for understanding a system when the actual have at their disposal for reasoning about other minds, one
experience is impractical or imprudent. The likely behav- that is likely to be used more extensively in some contexts,
ior of a skyscraper in a windstorm, for instance, can be overridden in others, or sometimes avoided entirely.
understood by building a small-scale model and turning At least four classes of findings are commonly cited as
on a fan, or the likely effects of a medical treatment on evidence for a simulation-based mechanism. First, mind
humans can be understood by giving the treatment to rats perception exhibits systematic biases consistent with a simu-
instead. In the absence of any direct perceptual informa- lation mechanism. Chief among these are that inferences
tion about another person’s mental states, people may opt to about others’ mental states—including goals, intentions,
simulate another’s mental states by using one’s own mental emotions, attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge—are often
states. For instance, the consistent introspective experience egocentrically biased (see Alicke, Dunning, & Krueger,

CH14.indd 511 12/22/09 3:39:41 PM


512 Mind Perception

2005, for a review). Consider just a few examples. In quences of time into their evaluations is widely documented
general, people use their own knowledge as a guide to in people’s predictions of their own and others’ future
others’ knowledge, leading people to believe that others’ feelings and preferences (Ratner, Kahn, & Kahneman, 1999;
attitudes and beliefs are relatively similar to their own Read & Loewenstein, 1995; Wilson & Gilbert, 2003), as
(Alicke & Largo, 1995; Babcock & Loewenstein, 1997; well as in the recollection of mental experiences such as pain
Krueger & Clement, 1994; Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). and suffering (Ariely, 1998; Fredrickson & Kahneman, 1993;
People who own an object, for instance, tend to value it Redelmeier & Kahneman, 1996; Schreiber & Kahneman,
more than people who do not (Kahneman, Knetsch, & 2000). Simulations are often insensitive to the actual time
Thaler, 1990), and owners therefore tend to assume that that characterizes extended human experience, and people’s
nonowners will value the object more than nonown- assessments of other agents’ mental states often appear to be
ers actually do (Loewenstein & Adler, 1995; Van Boven, relatively insensitive to time as well.
Dunning, & Loewenstein, 2000). Conceptually identical Third, people are often egocentric when considering other
egocentric biases also emerge when people evaluate dis- agents’ mental states, but not universally so. Both behav-
tant versions of themselves in the future and past, with ioral and neuroimaging evidence suggest that the perceived
people inferring their preferences, beliefs, and attitudes similarity between oneself and a target moderates the extent
in the future and past based on those mental states in the to which people use themselves as a guide to others. In one
present (Eich, Reeves, Jaeger, & Graff-Radford, 1985; experiment, for instance, Columbia University students
Fischhoff, 1975; Goethals & Reckman, 1973; Hawkins & predicted the preferences of a University of California
Hastie, 1990; Loewenstein, O’Donoghue, & Rabin, 2003; at Berkeley student. The more similar participants felt to
McFarland & Ross, 1987; see also Bernstein, Atance, UC Berkeley students in general, the more they relied on
Meltzoff, & Loftus, 2007). Anyone who has been confident their own preferences to estimate the preferences of a parti-
of never being hungry again immediately after eating a big cular UC Berkeley student (Ames, 2004b). In a more recent
meal has directly experienced the ease with which one’s neuroimaging experiment, neural regions associated with
current state can be used as a guide to one’s future states self-referential thoughts (especially the ventral medial pre-
(Gilbert, Gill, & Wilson, 2002; see Loewenstein, 2005, for frontal cortex) were activated when participants reasoned
a review). This egocentric “curse of knowledge” (Camerer, about the mental states of a person perceived to be similar
Loewenstein, & Weber, 1989) also emerges in people’s to themselves but not when they reasoned about a person
tendency to overestimate the extent to which others can perceived to be different (Mitchell, Macrae, & Banaji,
identify their hidden emotions, preferences, and inten- 2006). More broadly, the extent to which a target looks
tions (Cameron & Vorauer, 2008; Gilovich, Savitsky, & like or moves at the same pace as a human moderates the
Medvec, 1998; Keysar & Barr, 2002; Kruger, Epley, extent to which people attribute humanlike mental states to
Parker, & Ng, 2005) and in their tendency to use private both human and nonhuman targets, at least partly explain-
self-evaluations as a guide for how they will be evaluated ing why hummingbirds and sloths seem relatively mind-
by others (Chambers, Epley, Savitsky, & Windschitl, 2008; less compared with more humanly paced horses or lions
Gilovich, Kruger, & Medvec, 2002; Kenny & DePaulo, (Morewedge, Preston, & Wegner, 2007). Not only does
1993). Simulations produce outputs dependent on fea- perceived similarity appear to increase the tendency to
tures of the simulating mechanism itself, and all of these use oneself as a source of simulation, but the opposite
results suggest that attempts to understand others’ men- occurs as well: Being explicitly asked to simulate another
tal states depend critically on self-centered inputs from person’s perspective and put oneself in another ’s shoes
one’s own mental states. increases the perceived similarity between self and other
Second, simulations can be revealed not only in the (Davis, Conklin, Smith, & Luce, 1996; Galinsky et al.,
systematic features that are present in the outputs of mind 2005), the amount of empathy viscerally experienced
perception but also in the features that are absent. Mental for another person (Batson, 1998), and the amount of
simulations differ in several ways from actual experience self-referential thought engaged (Ames, Jenkins, Banaji,
(Gilbert & Wilson, 2007), but one especially distinctive & Mitchell, 2008; Davis et al., 2004). Adopting another ’s
way is that simulations are relatively timeless. Whereas perspective even leads people to behave in ways that
experience occurs in real time, mental simulations unfold are consistent with the stereotypes associated with a
in an instant. Try, for example, to imagine how happy you particular target (Galinsky, Wang, & Ku, 2008). People
would feel spending a week on vacation in Hawaii. If we seem to egocentrically simulate others’ mental experience
were welcoming you back to this sentence only moments when they perceive others to be similar, and they become
later rather than a week later, you get the point. Indeed, more similar to others once they have simulated others’
people’s inability to accurately incorporate the conse- mental experience.

CH14.indd 512 12/22/09 3:39:41 PM


Using Mind Perception 513

Finally, people appear to simulate others’ experience These mirror neurons are more active when others are
not only in their mind but in their whole bodies as well. As performing intentional actions (e.g., reaching toward food)
already mentioned, the tendency to mimic others’ behavior than when performing unintentional actions (e.g., the same
can be observed as soon as mothers allow psychologists reaching behavior in the absence of food; Fogassi et al.,
to examine their infants (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977, 1983, 2005; Umilta et al., 2001).
1994). Adults maintain this tendency, mimicking others’ More indirect evidence for a highly complex mirror neu-
actions such as yawning (Platek, Critton, Myers, & Gallup, ron system in humans comes from less invasive techniques
2003) or face rubbing (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Those of functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroen-
who are dispositionally inclined to engage in perspective cephalograms showing similar activation for performing
taking (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999), or who are especially versus observing intentional actions (Buccino, Binkofski, &
motivated to form a social connection with another per- Riggio, 2004; Buccino et al., 2001; Decety et al., 1997;
son (Lakin & Chartrand, 2003; Lakin, Chartrand, & Arkin, Iacoboni et al., 2001) and for experiencing versus observing
2008), are also the most likely to mimic others’ behavior. emotions (Jabbi, Swart, & Keysers, 2007). This mirror
For instance, those who are not in a committed romantic system is involved not only in simulating the actions
relationship are more likely to mimic an attractive opposite- of human agents but also in simulating the actions of non-
sex partner in an interaction than those who are in a com- human agents such as robots and simple shapes (Gazzola,
mitted relationship (Karremans & Verwijmeren, 2008). Rizzolatti, Wicker, & Keysers, 2007; Wheatley, Milleville, &
This mimicry not only makes others’ behaviors contagious Martin, 2007). Because simulating others’ mental states
but makes their moods, emotions, and attitudes conta- requires matching representations of the self with repre-
gious as well (Neumann & Strack, 2000; for reviews, see sentations of others, these mirror neurons may provide
Hatfield et al., 1994; Semin & Smith, 2008). Disabling a the neural architecture for this exact matching function in
person’s ability to mimic another ’s behavior also seems to mind reading and serve as the mechanism that enables empa-
impair one’s ability to recognize the mental states associ- thy with others (Ferrari et al., 2006; Gallese & Goldman,
ated with that behavior. In one experiment, for instance, 1998; Gallese, Keysers, & Rizzolatti, 2004; Rizzolatti,
participants asked to bite down on a pen were less able to Fogassi, & Gallese, 2001).
detect changes in others’ emotional expressions than those Discovery of the mirror neuron system is recent, and
who could move their mouths freely (Niedenthal, Brauer, enthusiasm for potential implications is vastly outpac-
Halberstadt, & Innes-Ker, 2001). Participants in another ing empirical support for them (Saxe, 2005; Southgate &
experiment were less able to accurately identify happi- Hamilton, 2008). Many implications of the mirror neuron
ness in another ’s facial expression when biting on a pen system for broader social functioning remain highly specu-
that specifically disabled the ability to smile (Oberman, lative, and their direct links to mind perception have not yet
Winkielman, & Ramachandran, 2007; see also Niedenthal, been demonstrated. Adult macaques, for instance, do not
Barsalou, Winkielman, Krauth-Gruber, & Ric, 2005). appear to engage in overt behavioral mimicry even though
Mental states can be produced by actions, and copying they possess mirror neurons (although infant macaques
others’ actions provides a plausible mechanism for copy- mimic in the first days of life; Ferrari et al., 2006), nor
ing their mental states. is there strong evidence that macaques can represent oth-
Recent neuroscientific evidence suggests that such ers’ mental states. Research establishing the connections
mimicry may even occur without overt behavioral imita- among mirror neurons, mimicry, and mind perception will
tion. We doubt that Hume (1739/1958, p. 365) was think- likely fill psychology journal pages for years to come.
ing literally when he wrote that “the minds of men are Nevertheless, these emerging findings in neuroscience,
mirrors to one another,” but it appears that mimicry may along with long-standing evidence of egocentric biases
indeed result from specialized neural circuitry that mir- in judgment, moderation of self-referential reasoning by
rors others’ actions (see Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004, for perceived similarity, and behavioral evidence of mim-
a review). Because virtually all humans object to having icry, make it clear that people at least sometimes under-
probes inserted into their brains, direct evidence of these stand others’ mental states through a process of simulation.
“mirror neurons” comes at this point only from macaques When Slote (1966) worried that other philosophers would
in whom a nontrivial percentage of cells (roughly 10% to perceive him as “beating a dead horse,” it was not because
15%) in the premotor cortex become active both when they simulation in mind perception had been conclusively dem-
are performing an action (e.g., reaching for food) and when onstrated but rather because its validity as a logical form
they are observing the same action performed by others of reasoning had been so widely and thoroughly attacked
(Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996; Keysers by other philosophers (Plantinga, 1966). Psychological
et al., 2003; Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Gallese, & Fogassi, 1996). research has not so much revived this particular dead

CH14.indd 513 12/22/09 3:39:41 PM


514 Mind Perception

horse as shown that its appeal to ordinary perceivers as a This proposition is old news to social psychologists.
psychological mechanism for knowing other minds was Theories of person perception have long proposed the
never in much doubt. same idea—that people understand an actor ’s intentions
and the behavioral dispositions that are likely to result by
Theory Theories either implicitly or explicitly performing a causal analy-
An account of mind perception based only on simulation sis of the person’s behavior based on individuating or cat-
seems to echo Piaget’s assessment that people possess only egorical knowledge. Indeed, the entire history of research
a “protoplasmic consciousness unable to make any distinc- on causal attribution in social psychology posits theory-
tion between the self and things” (1929, p. 235). But Piaget driven processes of inference to make sense of oneself
was talking about 2-year-olds. What makes adult mind and others (Bruner & Tagiuri, 1954; Gilbert, 1998; Jones,
readers remarkable is not their ability to perceive similari- 1990; Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). Understanding other
ties between themselves and others but rather their ability people’s momentary mental states, as well as their endur-
to perceive differences. The developmental trajectory ing dispositions, requires attention to a person’s behavior
described earlier is marked by impressive changes in the across time and situations in comparison with other people’s
ability to recognize that others’ mental states are different behavior. These causal attribution theories proposed that
from one’s own, that the self is distinct from others, and people attend to covariation between an actor ’s behavior
that people’s overt behaviors may be deceptive and there- and its consequences to reveal underlying intentions, in
fore inconsistent with their underlying beliefs. As adults, which unconventional or unique behaviors (i.e., noncom-
the self is defined by features that make it different from mon effects) were the most likely to reveal unique and
others (e.g., needs Sports Illustrated, has fishing poles, informative intentions about the person (Jones & Davis,
likes sauerkraut) rather than by the features that make 1965). Stable behavioral dispositions that distinguish one
it similar to others (e.g., needs oxygen, has arms, likes person from another are revealed by a more complicated
money; Kihlstrom et al., 1988; Markus & Sentis, 1982). test (a three-way analysis of variance, in fact) that considers
If the self is a tool that is used to simulate similar others’ the extent to which a person’s behavior occurs only in the
mental states, then it might not come in handy as often as presence of a particular stimulus or entity (its distinctive-
one might suspect. ness), occurs consistently over time in the presence of the
One way to explain these changes is that people learn same stimulus or entity (its consistency), and is similar to
how other minds work over time and acquire an intui- others’ behavior in the same circumstance (its consensus;
tive understanding of psychology, just as a scientist learns Kelley, 1967, 1973).
about a topic under study by testing hypotheses and attend- As with simulation accounts, several classes of findings
ing to available evidence (Churchland, 1984). People do are broadly consistent with this theory-driven account of
not understand others’ behavior on these theory-driven mind perception. First, when introspective signals are weak
accounts by imagining themselves in another ’s shoes and or ambiguous, people appear to reason about their own men-
simulating the mental states that would result—an egocen- tal states by using the same process of theoretical inference
trically based process. Rather, they use an intuitive theory that they use to reason about others (Bem, 1972; Gopnik,
of mind that explains behavior in terms of underlying men- 1993; Karniol, 2003; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977; Ryle, 1949).
tal states—an allocentrically based process (Flavell, 1988; People presume, for instance, a consistent correspondence
Gopnik, 1993; Leslie, 1987; Perner, 1991). People learn between a person’s beliefs and their resulting behavior (e.g.,
that their mothers are to be trusted but their politicians are she said she loved me because she really loves me), and
not, and they use their existing knowledge about these tar- decades of research shows that people are inclined to draw
gets to predict their goals, intentions, preferences, or other this correspondent inference even when alternate causal
mental states rather than using an egocentrically based explanations are available (e.g., a preceding plea to “just
simulation. The content of these theory-driven inferences tell me you love me”; Gilbert & Jones, 1986).
can come through observations of others’ actions, interpre- People under certain conditions exhibit this correspon-
tations of one’s own actions, or culturally conferred stereo- dence bias when observing their own behavior as well
types and associations that form the basis of knowledge (Gilbert & Gill, 2000). If, for instance, people are induced
about the relationship between mental states and behavior. through subtle experimental manipulation to behave in a
Understanding the mental states of other agents is no dif- way that is inconsistent with their previously expressed
ferent on this account than any other kind of inferential rea- (but easily forgotten) attitudes, people change their own
soning that posits invisible forces such as goals or gravity attitudes to be consistent with their behavior. People make
to explain observable actions such as fighting or falling the same kinds of inferences about another person’s under-
(Baker, Tenenbaum, & Saxe, 2008). lying beliefs (Bem & McConnell, 1970). Because people’s

CH14.indd 514 12/22/09 3:39:42 PM


Using Mind Perception 515

inferences about the causes of their own actions sometimes Integrative Accounts: Mind Perception as
follow the same set of logical inferences made about Inductive Inference
others, it is reasonable to presume that mental state infer-
ences are often based on the same process of theoretical Whenever opposing sides of a scientific dispute generate
inference. “The way the mind seems to its owner is the so much evidence and so little resolution, it is a good guess
owner’s best guess at its method of operation, not a revealed that participants in the debate are talking past each other
truth” (Wegner, 2005, p. 33). If putting oneself in one’s own rather than to each other. Classic debates in psychology
shoes requires some theoretical guesswork, then surely about nature versus nurture, motivation versus cognition,
putting oneself in another person’s shoes does so as well. and dissonance versus self-perception invariably end not
Second, adults’ ability to reason about the conse- by one side beating the other into submission but rather by
quences of others’ false beliefs provides evidence consis- delineating territory to understand the domains in which
tent with a theory-driven account of mind perception (for each set of forces is likely to operate. Luckily for progress
reviews, see Malle, 2005; Saxe, 2008). Children’s ability in understanding mind perception, the entrenched either–
to explain another ’s actions in terms of underlying mental or debates about a single underlying mechanism are giv-
states (e.g., a false belief) seems to precede their ability to ing way to calls for integration (e.g., Apperly, 2008; Keysers
accurately predict another ’s behavior based on false beliefs &Gazzola, 2007; Mitchell, 2006) and even a few sugges-
(Amsterlaw, 2006), suggesting that an intuitive under- tions for how to do so (Ames, 2004a; Epley, Morewedge, et
standing of mental states precedes the ability to predict the al., 2004; Keysar & Barr, 2002; Malle, 2005; Mussweiler,
behavioral consequences of them. Among adults, neuroim- 2003; Nichols & Stich, 2003; Saxe, 2008; Zaki & Ochsner,
aging suggests regions specifically dedicated to reasoning in press). People appear able to use both simulation and
about others’ beliefs. Although early evidence suggested a theory-driven strategies for solving the other minds prob-
wider network of regions involved in theory-based mental lem. The trick for psychologists is to understand when
inferences, accumulating evidence suggests that the tem- people adopt one, another, or some combination of both.
poroparietal junction bilaterally is the most selectively One promising integration is to recognize that reasoning
active region for solving false belief tasks in particular about other minds represents a more basic process of induc-
(Saxe, 2008). This region is also selectively active when tion, whereby people reason about unobservable mental
people listen to a story focused on another person’s inner states in others by starting from an inductive base of readily
thoughts (e.g., a belief) compared with other inner attri- accessible knowledge. Like any process of induction, men-
butes such as their bodily states (e.g., hunger or sick- tal state reasoning should be guided by the basic cognitive
ness) or their outer appearance (e.g., tall or thin; Saxe & operations of knowledge activation and subsequent appli-
Powell, 2006, but see Mitchell, 2008, for an alternative cation (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Higgins, 1996). This latter
interpretation). application stage includes attempts to correct or adjust an
Finally, people do not rely on egocentric simulations intuitive response in light of less accessible information also
when reasoning about others who are different from them; activated at the time of judgment. Such correction processes
instead, they rely on individuating or categorical informa- are notoriously difficult, requiring mental effort (i.e., exec-
tion (Krueger, 1998; Vorauer, Hunter, Main, & Roy, 2000). utive control) to integrate information, test alternatives, or
For instance, asking people to write about the ways in inhibit a highly accessible default (Gilbert, Pelham, & Krull,
which they are different from the average MBA student 1988). As a result, inductive inferences are consistently
increases the use of stereotypes about the average MBA biased in the direction of the most readily accessible infor-
student to predict this target’s preferences, and diminishes mation. Such dual process accounts have dominated theo-
the use of one’s own preferences (Ames, 2004a, 2004b). The ries of human judgment in social psychology because they
number of articles reporting the pervasive influence of ste- have proven fruitful in explaining judgments that seem to
reotypes in social judgments is now so extensive that it is be based on a combination of intuitive and reflective
more easily weighed than counted, and receives consider- responses, as well as the systematic biases in judgment that
able treatment in other chapters of this handbook (Dovidio & result (e.g., Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Epley, Keysar, et al.,
Gaertner, volume 2; Fiske, volume 2; Macrae & Quadflieg, 2004; Gilbert & Malone, 1995; Kahneman & Frederick,
this volume; Yzerbyt & Demoulin, volume 2). Although 2002; Lieberman, Gaunt, Gilbert, & Trope, 2002).
stereotypes are not exclusively used to make mental state Dual-process models appear useful for reconciling
inferences, they are the kinds of representations that can when people are likely to use their own mental states and
be used to make theoretical predictions about them and are experiences as a guide to others’ momentary and transitory
therefore consistent with theory-driven accounts of mind mental states rather than using their developed theory of
perception. mind based on known attributes about others. In particular,

CH14.indd 515 12/22/09 3:39:42 PM


516 Mind Perception

several findings suggest that egocentric simulation can be over the course of years in young children, and it suggests
a default mechanism for predicting others’ mental states, that older children adopt a more sophisticated method of
activated automatically and only subsequently adjusted reasoning about minds than young children.
to accommodate for perceived differences between self This same shift appears to take place over the course
and others. The reason is that one’s own perspective is of moments in adults. Adults do not seem to outgrow
not simply a source of analogy for other minds but is also their tendency to simulate others’ experience; rather, they
the source through which people construct and understand develop the ability to subsequently overcome and suppress
reality (Asch, 1952; Krech & Crutchfield, 1948). People it when clearly required to do so. Performance on theory of
see the world through their own eyes, experience it through mind tasks in children, for instance, is at least partly
their own senses, and have ready access to their own determined by the simultaneously developing capacity
knowledge and attitudes. These inputs are distinctly per- to inhibit default responses (Carlson, Moses, & Claxton,
spective bound, and they influence how people understand 2004). In false belief tasks with adults, evidence of ego-
and interpret their perceptions or how people imagine and centric anchoring emerges when more subtle prediction
construe events when direct perceptual inputs are absent measures are used, such as indicating the likelihood that
(e.g., when imagining hypothetical or future events com- a target will look in a given location rather than simply
pared with directly observing another ’s behavior). These being asked where the target will look (Birch & Bloom,
top-down influences work automatically and reflexively, 2007). Reasoning about others’ differing (false) beliefs
enabling a person to understand an otherwise ambiguous also appears to require more effortful cognitive processing
action at the time that an event is encoded but leaving no than reasoning about others’ similar (true) beliefs (Apperly,
trace of its effects. As a result, people tend to be naïve Back, Samson, & France, 2008), and decreasing people’s
realists, assuming that their own perceptions are an objec- tendency to engage in effortful thought (e.g., by putting
tive reflection of the way the world is rather than merely a them in a happy mood) increases egocentric biases in false
reflection of the way the world appears to them (Ross & belief tasks (Converse, Lin, Keysar, & Epley, 2008).
Ward, 1996). Several other findings are consistent with this egocen-
If mental states are produced by people’s subjective tric default and correction account. For instance, the ego-
interpretation of reality, then one’s own assessment of centric biases described earlier in which people tend to use
the real world is a sensible and rational starting point for their mental states as a guide to others do not show evidence
reasoning about others’ mental states (Dawes & Mulford, of complete egocentrism, but are moderated in a direction
1996; Krueger & Acevedo, 2005). But adults also learn over that suggests an effortful and deliberate attempt to incorpo-
the course of development that their own perceptions may rate information about how others’ mental states might dif-
be a bad stopping point in social judgment because oth- fer from one’s own. People who own an item, for instance,
ers’ perspectives may differ from their own and that their know that buyers are likely to value it less than they do, and
resulting mental states can differ (Epley, Morewedge, & buyers likewise know that owners value an item more than
Keysar, 2004; Gilbert & Gill, 2000). In the language of they do themselves (Van Boven et al., 2000). These distinc-
dual-process models, simulation may be an automatic tions show evidence of using one’s own mental states and
process necessary to characterize an event, whereas using evaluations as a guide to others, as well as using theoretical
one’s theory of mind is a controlled process of correction inferences about others’ differing mental states that make
or adjustment of an egocentric anchor that requires effort- their own evaluations unique. These results simultaneously
ful attention, deliberate activation, and intentional control. provide evidence of egocentric anchoring in the strong cor-
Consider, for instance, the paradigmatic test of mental relations between self and other and evidence of theory-
state reasoning—the false belief task: If Sally places her driven correction in the mean differences between self and
key in a drawer and her sister, Ann, then moved it to the other (McFarland & Miller, 1990).
cupboard while Sally is at school, then where will Sally More revealing, however, is that egocentric biases—
look when she comes back to look for her key? In this task, such as those stemming from the “curse of knowledge”—
one’s own knowledge differs from the target’s knowledge, increase when people are asked to respond quickly, and
and people have to rely on what the target knows to predict when they are especially inclined to accept default responses
Sally’s behavior. Young children appear to use their own in judgment (e.g., when they are nodding their heads com-
knowledge to simulate Sally’s behavior, predicting that she pared with shaking them from side to side), but decrease
will look in the cupboard. Older children appear to over- when people are motivated to engage in additional effort-
come their own knowledge and predict that Sally will look ful thought by financial incentives for accuracy (Epley,
where she believes the key to be. This potential shift from Keysar, et al., 2004). People also appear to interpret others’
simulation to theory-driven prediction appears to take place spoken language egocentrically and only subsequently

CH14.indd 516 12/22/09 3:39:42 PM


Targets of Mind Perception 517

accommodate another ’s differing perspective. Participants Summary: Using Mind Perception


in one experiment, for instance, were asked by an experi-
People appear to use multiple strategies to solve the other
mental confederate to move objects around a vertical grid
minds problem, not only using the self as a source of anal-
(Keysar, Barr, Balin, & Brauner, 2000). Some objects
ogy or simulation but also using theory-driven inferences
could be seen only from the participant’s own perspective,
about how particular minds work. However, determining
whereas others were mutually visible to the participant and
whether recent attempts to integrate these two sources of
the confederate. On critical trials, the confederate provided
information into a more general theoretical model are suc-
an ambiguous instruction that could refer to two objects,
cessful is unfortunately a task for the future rather than for
one hidden from the confederate and one mutually observ-
the present. At present, the rapidly accumulating evidence
able. Results showed that participants tended to look first
from varying domains suggests that simple solutions to the
at the hidden object suggested by an egocentric interpre-
other minds problem are likely to explain some phenomena
tation of the instruction and only subsequently to look at
but not others, and that a full theory of how people make
the mutually observable object suggested by knowledge
inferences about unobservable mental states will have to
of the other person’s perspective. Highlighting how this
attend to the complexities of everyday life more than to the
process in adults is similar to that in children, an experiment
dictates of Occam’s razor. If social psychologists are to be
comparing children and their parents using the same eye-
a part of this solution, it will require looking far beyond
tracking paradigm demonstrated that children and adults
our usual interests to consider how this process develops
did not differ in the speed with which they interpreted an
and how it varies across species. It will require going far
instruction egocentrically (after correcting for baseline dif-
beyond the typical behavioral methods that have defined
ferences) but did differ in the speed with which they cor-
the discipline to integrate emerging discoveries in neuro-
rected that egocentric interpretation (Epley, Morewedge,
science. It will require retaining a focus on key phenomena
et al., 2004). Adults may not end up solely using their own
in everyday life and using highly controlled experiments to
perspective as a guide to others’ mental states, but they
shed light on phenomena that people outside the discipline
may well begin there.
care about. But most of all, it will require transcending
Although people can sometimes appear to be using a
the natural tendency to reason about problems from our
combination of simulation and theory-driven mechanisms
own disciplinary perspective in order to integrate a dizzy-
to make inferences about other minds, they are not nec-
ing array of findings emerging far beyond the traditional
essarily using them in equal degrees across all situations.
bounds of social psychology and identify a common lan-
Factors that increase the tendency to use the self as an
guage that everyone can understand to discuss it. Despite
inductive anchor in prediction should therefore moder-
being the central focus of frenzied activity in psychology
ate the extent to which people rely on simulation-based
for decades, and in philosophy for centuries, the enormous
mechanisms centered on one’s own perceptions and beliefs
existing literature on how people solve the other minds
versus more theory-driven inferences based on one’s devel-
problem in everyday life does not, as yet, provide a com-
oped theory of mind. Perceived similarity to the self is the
prehensive solution. It provides a call to action.
most obvious determinant. As already mentioned, people
use themselves as a source of information when reasoning
about similar others, but rely more heavily on stereotypes
and categorical information about others who seem to TARGETS OF MIND PERCEPTION
be different from them. When stereotypes about a target
are highly accessible, self-relevant information may not be Understanding how a process functions is important not
used. We believe it is telling that most demonstrations simply for curiosity’s sake, but also for application’s
of egocentric biases consistent with a simulation account sake. Social psychologists care about the mechanisms
involve reasoning about an unspecified or ambiguous tar- that enable mind perception because they care even more
get (e.g., the average American, another participant in this about how people apply this capacity to understand other
experiment, or a fictional character in a story), whereas minds in everyday life and the consequences that result.
most demonstrations consistent with theory-based infer- The mechanisms of mind perception allow people to “see”
ences come from contexts in which categorical informa- other minds, but they do not tell us what people will
tion about a target is highly relevant and clearly specified see when they think about those minds. Humans and other
(e.g., the target’s gender, race, or ethnicity). Without spe- animals can appear mindful or mindless, hurricanes can be
cific information about other minds, simulation is not only seen to reveal the mind of God or as random acts of nature,
a reasonable strategy for inductive inference—it may be and at times a computer may seem to have a mind of its
the only possible strategy. own. The basic mechanisms of mind perception discussed

CH14.indd 517 12/22/09 3:39:43 PM


518 Mind Perception

in the previous section appear to operate in a relatively to ordinary perceivers. People tend to rely on introspective
similar fashion across various targets, from evaluations of access to their own mental states when understanding their
one’s own mental states, to the evaluation of other people’s own mind, adopting an “inside” perspective, but rely more
mental states, to the evaluation of nonhuman agents such heavily on observed or recalled behavior when reasoning
as animals, gadgets, and gods. Research investigating how about others, adopting more of an “outside” perspective
people assess the mental states of these various targets can (for a review, see Pronin, in press).
be divided into three broad categories—own versus other For instance, people tend to use their intentions more
minds, close versus distant other minds, and human versus heavily when inferring their own enduring traits and dis-
nonhuman minds. Although the mechanisms that enable positions than they do when inferring others’ traits and
mind perception across each of these distinctions appear dispositions. People may think that they are conscientious
to be the same, the way in which they are applied and the for merely planning to throw a birthday party, but need to
outcomes that result are anything but. see an actual party to infer that another person is equally
conscientious (Kruger & Gilovich, 2004). Explanations of
one’s own behavior also tend to focus more on unobserv-
Own Versus Other Minds
able reasons (mental states such as beliefs, intentions, and
The philosophical version of the other minds problem motives), whereas explanations of others’ behavior tends
makes a critical distinction between the seemingly direct to focus on past behavior (causal histories that explain
way people experience their own mental states from the the progression of one event to another with less focus
inside and the indirect way people experience others’ on inner mental states and experiences; Malle, Knobe, &
mental states from the outside. People experience their own Nelson, 2007). People also consult their current intentions
minds directly, whereas other minds are inherently invis- when predicting their own future behavior (e.g., such as
ible and must be inferred using the “outside” mechanisms their good intentions to donate to a charity) but tend to rely
of simulation and theory-driven inferences. John Locke more heavily on general behavioral base rates or obvious
(1690/1975) suggested that this implied a kind of sixth external influences when predicting others’ behavior (e.g.,
sense of “introspection” that allows people to perceive such as how often people normally donate in such char-
their own mental states, and René Descartes (1637/1998) ity drives or how difficult it would be to donate; Buehler
was so convinced that his sixth sense operated truthfully et al., 1994; Epley & Dunning, 2000; MacDonald & Ross,
that he staked both his own existence, as well as God’s, on 1999). And because of the general preference for intro-
it. Introspection provides an intimate look into the deep- spection when thinking about oneself, people prefer inside
est parts of ourselves, apparently revealing truths about our information over outside observation when predicting their
mental processes that simply cannot be identified any other future emotional reactions that would enable introspective
way. C. S. Lewis (1952, p. 33) described the common senti- simulation. When women in one experiment wanted to
ment best when he argued that the self was the only thing predict, for instance, how much they would enjoy “speed-
“in the whole universe which we know more about than dating” a man, they preferred to know information about
we could learn from external observation . . . In this case we the man’s identity more than to know how much another
have, so to speak, inside information; we are in the know.” woman liked her speed-date, even though the latter infor-
Both Descartes and Lewis were wrong. Descartes mation produces more accurate predictions than the former
was wrong because introspection does not operate truth- (Gilbert, Killingsworth, Eyre, & Wilson, 2009).
fully but rather interpretively, making sense of the brain’s Notice that the major cause of the asymmetry in these
operation and sensory experiences more than directly findings is not so much between self and other as between
reporting on the causes of one’s judgments and behavior an inside approach to prediction and explanation that relies
(Bargh & Morsella, 2008; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977; heavily on introspective experience and an outside approach
Wegner, 2002; Wegner & Bargh, 1998). Lewis was wrong that reasons more heavily from observed or presumed
because a reliance on introspection to predict one’s own behavior (Jones & Nisbett, 1972; see also Kahneman &
behavior from the inside perspective can sometimes be Lovallo, 1993; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). As a result,
less calibrated than predictions based on information this same asymmetry in attention to introspective mental
observed from an outside perspective (Bass & Yammarino, states arises across other forms of psychological distance,
1991; Buehler, Griffin, & Ross, 1994; Epley & Dunning, not simply between self and other but also between self
2000, 2006; Koehler & Poon, 2006; MacDonald & Ross, in the present and self in the future. Some evidence does
1999; Risucci, Tortolani, & Ward, 1989). Although both indeed suggest that people represent future versions of
Descartes and Lewis were wrong about the actual validity of themselves in ways that are similar to how they represent
introspection, they were right about its apparent validity others, again using introspective experience more when

CH14.indd 518 12/22/09 3:39:43 PM


Targets of Mind Perception 519

evaluating themselves or making decisions in the present explanations of behavior. People tend to explain their own
but focusing more on observable attributes of the situation behavior by appealing to more complicated mental states
and context when evaluating themselves in the future or pre- of beliefs and knowledge—more complicated and late-
dicting their future behavior (Pronin et al., 2008; Pronin & developing concepts in one’s theory of mind—whereas
Ross, 2006). they explain others’ action by appealing to the more sim-
Although the mechanisms that enable mind percep- plistic mental concepts of wants and desires (Malle et al.,
tion are designed to help ordinary perceivers narrow the 2007). People also recognize more complex relations
gap between the observability of one’s own mental states between their own mental states and behavior than they
and the observability of others’ mental states, these results appear to do when explaining others’ behavior. When vot-
make it clear that they are unlikely to close the gap entirely. ing in an election, for instance, people can recognize that
Empathizing with another ’s pain, for instance, generates the correspondence between their belief and actual voting
affective reactions consistent with experiencing pain but may be complicated—that they can vote for a candidate
not the intense sensory stimulation of actually experienc- not because they truly like the candidate (an approach
ing pain (Singer et al., 2004). This suggests that the other motivation) but rather because they actively dislike the
minds problem might pose something of a problem after opposing candidate (an avoidance motivation). Even when
all: If introspection vividly illuminates the workings of people experience this disconnect between their thoughts
one’s own mind, it may in many ways seem “brighter” than and their behavior, they nevertheless assume a simple cor-
the minds of others that are viewed less directly through the respondent relationship in others’ behavior—that others
mechanisms of simulation and theory-driven inference. If vote for a candidate because they like that particular candi-
people cannot see others’ mental states as easily as they date (Miller & Nelson, 2002).
can perceive their own, then they may indeed believe that This can, under certain circumstances, create a state of
others have less mind than they have themselves. pluralistic ignorance whereby most people assume that their
Numerous findings converge on this “lesser minds” own mental states (attitudes, beliefs, intentions, or goals)
problem. The most direct comes from studies showing that differ from the majority of others in a group despite identi-
people believe they possess more mentally complex traits cal overt behavior (Allport, 1924). People may, for instance,
(e.g., “analytic,” “imaginative,” and “sympathetic”) than privately feel that norms of political correctness have gone
others do (Haslam & Bain, 2007; Haslam, Bain, Douge, too far or that lenient drinking norms on campus are a
Lee, & Bastian, 2005), possess more complicated moral real cause for concern, but they follow these norms none-
sentiments than others do (Epley & Dunning, 2000; Heath, theless because they interpret others’ identical behavior in
1999; Kahn, 1958; Miller, 1999), and are therefore more following the norm to indicate that others are actually
likely to be influenced by these secondary emotional in favor of the norm (Miller & McFarland, 1987, 1991;
states and moral sentiments than others are (Epley & Prentice & Miller, 1993; Van Boven, 2000). If introspection
Dunning, 2000; Hsee & Weber, 1997; Koehler & Poon, enables the observation of complex relationships between
2006; McFarland & Miller, 1990; Miller & McFarland, one’s mental states and one’s behavior, its absence leaves
1987, 1991; Sabini, Cosmas, Siepmann, & Stein, 1999; relatively simple relationships between mental states and
Van Boven, 2000). People also report that they are better behavior in others, and relatively simpler minds as well.
able to reason objectively about the external world and Some of these tendencies are almost certainly self-
are therefore less biased in their judgment than the more satisfying and may therefore appear to stem from the
simplistic reasoning of others (Pronin, Gilovich, & Ross, motivation to think well of oneself rather than from the dif-
2004), are better able to resist persuasive appeals and mass ferential access to one’s own versus others’ mental states
media attempts to influence their judgment (Davison, 1983; and experience. Believing oneself to be more thoughtful,
Perloff, 1993; see also Pronin, Berger, & Molouki, 2007), emotionally complex, or behaviorally sophisticated than
and are more psychologically responsive to the demands others is surely better than more thoughtless, simpler,
of a situation than are others who are seen as responding or mindlessly reflexive. But these are not the only infer-
more mindlessly on the basis of stable and enduring traits ences that introspection enables. It can also lead people
(Kammer, 1982; Nisbett, Caputo, Legant, & Marecek, to think that their fear of embarrassment is more intense
1973). People even report that they are more capable mind and crippling than others’ (Van Boven, Loewenstein, &
readers than others, having more insight into other people’s Dunning, 2005), that their experience of disgust is more
“true selves” than others have into their own “true selves” likely to keep them from behaving charitably than others’
(Pronin, Kruger, Savitsky, & Ross, 2001). less intense experience (Pronin et al., 2008), that they are
Beyond these self-reported capacities, evidence that being judged more harshly for a self-deprecating blun-
others seem to have “lesser minds” emerges from people’s der than they actually are (Epley, Savitsky, & Gilovich,

CH14.indd 519 12/22/09 3:39:44 PM


520 Mind Perception

2002; Savitsky, Epley, & Gilovich, 2001), that their own Most evidence consistent with this conclusion comes
negative intentions have caused more harm than have from differences in representations of, and mental capaci-
another person’s negative intentions (Pronin, Wegner, ties attributed to, ingroup versus outgroup members.
McCarthy, & Rodriguez, 2006), or that their own futures are Compared with more distant outgroup members, ingroup
more unpredictable than others’ futures (Pronin & Kugler, members are seen as more capable of experiencing second-
2007). More important, increasing access to others’ mental ary emotions that require higher-order mental states such
states and intentions, or decreasing access to one’s own, as love, hope, contempt, or resentment (Demoulin et al.,
reliably diminishes the distinctions people make between 2004; Gaunt, Leyens, & Demoulin, 2002; Leyens et al., 2000)
their own minds and other minds (e.g., Epley & Dunning, and as more likely to experience the moral sentiments nec-
2000; Kruger & Gilovich, 2004; Pronin, in press; Savitsky essary for prosocial actions such as empathy and compas-
et al., 2001). Access to one’s own mental states may cer- sion (Opotow, 1990; Staub, 1989). These mental states are
tainly, at times, be self-satisfying, but self-satisfaction is far the ones that people perceive to be uniquely human mental
from a completely satisfying explanation for these results. capacities (Leyens et al., 2000), and denying these capaci-
More powerful, it appears, is the asymmetry between ties in others strips them of their minds, as well as their
the direct experience of one’s own mind versus the indi- humanity (Haslam, 2006). Indeed, members of outgroups
rect experience of other minds, a gap that no amount of can often be represented as animals both through spoken
simulation or theory-driven inference can close entirely. metaphors and through artistic depictions (Goff, Eberhardt,
When people perceive an object to be dim because they Williams, & Jackson, 2008; Viki et al., 2006). This denial
are wearing dark sunglasses, they may fail to fully recog- of mind to distant outgroup members, especially in capaci-
nize the influence of the interpretive lens and conclude that ties related to agency (e.g., competence) or experience
the object is dimmer than it actually is (Rock & Nijhawan, (e.g., interpersonal warmth), is the most prevalent for
1989). This problem seems to confront mind readers as the most distant outgroups such as drug addicts and the
well. People can perceive others’ mental states, but they do homeless (Harris & Fiske, 2006).
so through the darkened lens provided by mechanisms of Liking for a target has closely related effects on the
simulation and theory-driven inference rather than through attribution of mental capacities, with well-liked individuals
the relatively bright light of introspection. being seen as possessing the full catalogue of mental
capacities compared with those of relatively disliked indi-
viduals, who have relatively lesser minds. People are more
Close Versus Distant Other Minds
likely to adopt the perspective of a liked target than that
Not all other minds are created equal, nor are all other of a disliked target (Frantz & Janoff-Bulman, 2000), con-
minds equally “other.” The relative invisibility of others’ strue their actions in a higher level of abstraction focused
mental states appears to increase as the gap between self on the meaning of an action rather than in its low-level
and other becomes greater across social distance and as behavioral attributes (Kozak et al., 2006), and believe that
other minds are understood from an outside perspective liked targets have more well-developed mental capacities
focused on behaviors and actions rather than on an inside (e.g., the ability to experience emotions and intentionally
perspective focused in mental states and experiences. control their actions) than those of disliked targets (Kozak
As the gap between oneself and others grows larger, so et al., 2006). In romantic relationships, feeling close to
does the difficulty of bridging that gap to recognize similar another person is related to the feeling of empathic under-
mental states in others. standing (e.g., Kahn, 1970; Noller & Ruzzene, 1991). This
Many relative differences in how mind readers under- increased perception of mental similarity and self–other
stand their own mental capacities compared with those overlap increases prosocial actions toward a relationship
of others are mimicked in how people compare the minds of partner (Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991), as well as
close versus distant others. As already discussed, perceived the strength and length of the romantic relationship
similarity increases the tendency to use one’s own mental (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992), even when the perceived
states as a guide to others’ beliefs, attitudes, and prefer- similarity in preferences, motives, and beliefs is illusory
ences (Ames, 2004a, 2004b; Krueger, 1998; Mitchell et al., (Lemay & Clark, 2008; Murray, Holmes, Bellavia, Griffin, &
2006), thereby enabling people to use their sixth sense of Dolderman, 2002).
introspection and their powers of simulation when reason-
ing about others who are close to the self compared with Human Versus Nonhuman Minds
those more distant from it. The mental capacities attributed
to distant others therefore tend to be dimmer than those If the gap between one’s own mind and other people’s
attributed to close others. minds is large, the gap between one’s own mind and

CH14.indd 520 12/22/09 3:39:44 PM


Targets of Mind Perception 521

nonhuman minds is immense. But as with other human subsequent judgment and behavior (Chartrand, Fitzsimons, &
minds, not all nonhuman minds are created equal. The Fitzsimons, 2008). The extent to which such inferences
determinants that induce people to perceive mental states are guided by mechanisms of simulation or theory-driven
in other humans—from self-propelled action to interde- inferences, however, is unclear.
pendence to causal uncertainty—operate with nonhumans
Technological Agents
as well, as do the basic mechanisms that enable the per-
ception of mental states and behavioral traits (Gosling, Anyone who has noticed a snarl in the grill of a sports car
Kwan, & John, 2003; Kwan, Gosling, & John, 2008). has also noticed engineers’ attempts to humanize technol-
These determinants can make some nonhuman agents ogy (Welsh, 2006; for a broader review, see Reeves &
appear more mindful than others. The consequences of Nass, 1996). Advances in artificial intelligence have
perceiving mental states in these agents are also qualita- produced robots that can convey emotion (Breazeal &
tively similar to mind perception with human agents—they Aryananda, 2002), can recognize emotions and social cues
increase feelings of social connection and support, enable (Breazeal, 2003), and can even imitate human actions and
understanding and apparent predictability, and serve as behave interdependently (Breazeal & Scassellati, 2002).
potent explanations for observed behavior. Because the These factors can give technological agents the appearance
biological differences between humans and nonhumans of both mindful agency and experience that would make
are so transparently obvious, we focus not on the perceived them seem like moral agents capable of both perform-
absence of mind in nonhuman agents but rather on factors ing harm and experiencing harm from others (Floridi &
that increase the perceived presence of mind. The aware- Sanders, 2004). Social cues that increase the tendency to
ness that people can anthropomorphize nonhuman agents, think of other people’s mental states—such as the presence
attributing humanlike mental states and capacities to them, of eyes and directed gaze, goal-directed motion, and self-
is at least as old as the sixth century B.C. (Lesher, 1992) propelled motion—also increase the likelihood of attrib-
and has been a topic of heated debate in intellectual dis- uting humanlike mental states to technological agents or
course ever since (Broadhurst, 1963; Darwin, 1872/2002; behaving toward these agents as if they had mental states
Feuerbach, 1873/2004; Guthrie, 1993; Hebb, 1946; Hume, (Guajardo & Woodward, 2004; Johnson, Booth, & O’Hearn,
1757/1957; Pepperberg, 2002; Povinelli & Vonk, 2003; 2001; Woodward, 1998). In one study, the degree of human
Ruskin, 1923). Understanding the psychological processes resemblance in a computerized poker partner (e.g., the pres-
that enable such anthropomorphism, however, is only just ence of a face) was positively related to the partner ’s per-
beginning (for reviews, see Epley et al., 2007; Kwan & ceived intelligence (Koda & Maes, 1996; see also Moreale &
Fiske, 2008; Mitchell, Thompson, & Miles, 1997). Watt, 2004; Nass, Moon, Fogg, Reeves, & Dryer, 1995). In
another, a computer-generated agent (specifically, a human-
Biological Agents
like face) appeared more trustworthy and persuasive when
The gap between human and nonhuman minds may appear it matched the participant’s ethnicity than when it did not
great, but the factors that increase the perception of mental match (Nass, Isbister, & Lee, 2000). These findings suggest
states across nonhuman agents are largely identical. People that it is relatively easy to attribute at least some humanlike
are more likely to anthropomorphize animals that are simi- mental states to technological agents and to behave toward
lar to humans in either morphology or motion (e.g., Eddy, these agents as if they indeed possess minds (Reeves &
Gallup, & Povinelli, 1993; Morewedge et al., 2007), and Nass, 1996). This tendency has actually posed something
this tendency is moderated by cultural beliefs and practices of an unexpected problem to survey researchers using com-
related to biological agents (e.g., Asquith, 1996; Medin & puters to collect data, not because people find it difficult to
Atran, 2004; Waxman & Medin, 2007). Factors that use such technology but rather because people sometimes
increase interdependence with a nonhuman agent, such respond in a more socially desirable fashion when complet-
as the motivation to understand and explain an animal’s ing a questionnaire on a computer than when they do so on
behavior or to establish social connection (Epley, Akalis, a piece of paper (Lautenschlager & Flaherty, 1990).
et al., 2008), or the appearance of interdependent and
Supernatural Agents
socially responsive action from the agent itself also increase
the tendency to perceive mental states in nonhuman ani- The perception of anthropomorphic mental states extends
mals (Johnson, 2003). Some evidence demonstrates that not only to materially present agents but also to meta-
stereotypes and categorical information about animals, physical agents such as gods and ghosts. The default
such as the belief that “Rottweilers are vicious,” is accessed representation of God in virtually all religions is deeply
to make inferences about these nonhuman animals (Kwan anthropomorphic, complete with mental states of inten-
et al., 2008) and can act as a prime to influence a person’s tions, goals, purpose, and emotions (Guthrie, 1993). People

CH14.indd 521 12/22/09 3:39:44 PM


522 Mind Perception

likewise report that ghosts are capable of suffering (Hinde, own beliefs as when reasoning about God’s beliefs but that
1999), capable of being internally motivated (Lillard, consistent differences emerged when reasoning about the
1998), and capable of controlling the actions of humans “average American’s” beliefs across a series of issues. As
(Parish, 1991, 1994), despite recognizing the absence of with inferences about other people, people use their own
biological states such as hunger or thirst (Bering, 2002). mental states as a guide to others unless they have explicit
Religious belief appears to come so naturally to people cues to suggest that their own mental states are likely to be
partly because it also appears to be a natural by-product of fundamentally different from others’ mental states.
the near-universal capacity for mind perception that leads Even when people are not wholly egocentric thinking
ordinary perceivers to identify intentions, goals, and pur- about the minds of religious agents, attributes of the self
poses to physical and natural events in the environment often influence evaluations of God’s internal states. In one
(e.g., Atran & Norenzayan, 2004; Barrett, 2000; Bloom, study, Catholic students with particularly high self-esteem
2004; Boyer, 2003; Gilbert, Brown, Pinel, & Wilson, 2000; saw God as more accepting, loving, saving, forgiving,
Guthrie, 1993). People acquire more specific beliefs from and approving, whereas low self-esteem participants saw
various religious traditions as they age, much like people God as more restricting, controlling, strict, and demand-
acquire specific beliefs about other people, but the psycho- ing (Benson & Spilka, 1973). In another, people who
logical foundation for belief in supernatural agents appears reported being particularly lonely also perceived God to
to be acquired in lockstep with the development of one’s be less helpful and more wrathful (Schwab & Petersen,
theory of mind. 1990). Conceptually similar egocentric tendencies emerge
Children through 14 years of age, for instance, are when people evaluate the mental states of other people as
likely to hold highly anthropomorphic representations of well. Those who are afraid, for instance, perceive a threat-
God (Heller, 1986; Landy, 2001; Mudge, 1923). Adults are ening target to be angrier than do those who are not, and
less anthropomorphic but continue to hold subtle anthro- men interested in dating a woman tend to perceive her as
pomorphic representations that may even conflict with the more sexually aroused than do men not interested in dating
theological tenets of their reported religious beliefs. For (Maner et al., 2005).
instance, Christianity dictates that God is not embodied in As with mind perception more generally, motivations
space or time (is omnipresent), has complete knowledge that increase interdependence—such as the motivation for
(is omniscient), and has unlimited power (is omnipotent). social connection with others—also appear to influence
Christian participants in one study explicitly reported religious representations. Religious agents are generally
believing that God indeed possessed these properties con- perceived to be strong sources of social connection and
sistent with theological teachings. When asked to recall support (Burris, Batson, Altstaedten, & Stephens, 1994;
God’s behavior in various experimental scenarios, however, Kirkpatrick, 1999; Luhrmann, 2004), and those induced to
these same participants reported that God was in only one feel lonely or isolated report believing in God more than
place at a time, had been mistaken, and had limited power those who are not induced to feel lonely (Epley, Akalis,
(Barrett & Keil, 1996). All of these inferences were biased et al., 2008). People who feel socially disconnected
memories of the actual stories consistent with an anthro- more routinely, such as those who are recently divorced
pomorphic representation of God rather than a theological (Cain, 1988), lonely (Rokach & Brock, 1998), or single
representation. Conceptually identical results emerged in a (Granqvist & Hagekull, 2000), also show an increased ten-
similar study with Hindu participants (Barrett, 1998). dency to represent God as a socially responsive and sup-
Such egocentric biases also emerge when religious portive agent. Experiencing social loss from the death of
believers are asked to report God’s beliefs on important a close other also increases the likelihood of forming a
social issues, such as abortion, the death penalty, or sup- connection with God (McIntosh, Silver, & Wortman, 1993;
port for legalizing same-sex marriages or marijuana. In Spilka, Hood, & Gorsuch, 1985; Wuthnow, Christiano, &
one series of experiments (Epley, Converse, Delbosc, Kozlowski, 1980).
Monteleone, & Cacioppo, in press), the correspondence This research suggests that mind perception is involved
between people’s own beliefs on these issues and God’s in religious belief and experience, but the complexity of
beliefs was consistently stronger than it is with other religious and supernatural beliefs around the world also
human targets (including liked targets, disliked targets, makes it clear that mind perception is going to be only
and “average” others), and manipulating people’s beliefs one piece of a larger puzzle (Atran, 2002; Boyer, 2001;
altered their estimates of God’s beliefs in a similar fash- Wilson, 2002). The power of religious systems to serve
ion more so than for other human targets. A functional as social controls and enable organization (Norenzayan &
neuroimaging study in this same series also revealed that Shariff, 2008), the capacity for specific religious beliefs
the same regions were active when thinking about one’s to seem simultaneously intuitive and counterintuitive

CH14.indd 522 12/22/09 3:39:45 PM


Consequences of Mind Perception 523

(Boyer, 2003), the desire for ultimate meaning and fear of action and can therefore be held causally responsible for
death (Norenzayan & Hansen, 2006), and the dynamics their actions. Second, mindful agents are autonomous
of persuasion and influence certainly operate to create the agents that can have conscious goals and aspirations but
complex and persistent manifestations of religious systems can also suffer and experience emotions that render them
in the world today. How big a role mind perception plays moral agents worthy of respect, empathic concern, and the
in religious experience, religious beliefs, or the continued basic human right of autonomy. Third, mindful agents are
popularity of religious worldviews even in modern indus- capable of having thoughts and forming impressions that
trialized societies, compared with all of these other influ- render them agents of surveillance, who may be evaluating
ences, is unknown. us and whose impressions we may try to anticipate, moni-
tor, or influence. Mind matters for judgments of responsi-
bility, for moral agency and ethical action, and for social
Summary: Targets of Mind Perception
surveillance (for more detailed treatments of these topics,
The mechanisms that enable people to understand other see Kovera & Borgida, volume 2; Haidt & Kesebir, volume
minds appear to operate similarly across various other minds, 2; Leary, volume 2; Swann & Bosson, this volume).
including other people, past and future versions of the
self, and nonhuman agents ranging from other animals to Responsibility
supernatural agents. Social psychology has generally been
a person-centered discipline, equating “others” with other People explain behavior, at least in part, by attending to an
people. But social life is richer than that. The overwhelm- agent’s intentions. Determining whether an act was inten-
ing majority of people living today believe that a mind- tional or accidental is therefore an important component
ful God controls their future (Harris Poll, 2003). Rapid when determining the causes of a given action and when
advancements in technology means that many people in determining responsibility and blame (Heider, 1958; Malle,
the industrialized world now spend more of their day inter- 1999). Intentions can create what appear to be intentional
acting with technological agents than they do with other (or purposeful) actions when they are combined with the
people (Gallegos, 2007). The distinction between human ability to control the action, the desire to attain a particular
and nonhuman—between us and them—is often central outcome, a belief that an action will attain a particular out-
to conflicts over environmental policy, social and cultural come, and an awareness of the intention when performing
practices, ethical conduct of scientific research, and cul- the action (Malle & Knobe, 1997). All of these causal fac-
ture wars between religion and science about the origin tors reside squarely within the agent and place causal
of our species, among many others. Social psychologists responsibility for controlling the action squarely within the
interested in mind perception can contribute more to the agent (Alicke, 2000). The extent to which agents appear
world’s understanding of social life than simply the study capable of intentional action is therefore directly related
of interpersonal interaction. to their perceived responsibility for performing the action
(Fincham & Emery, 1988; Roberts & Golding, 1991) and
their willingness to punish agents for a negative or immoral
CONSEQUENCES OF MIND PERCEPTION action (Gray et al., 2007; Hogue & Pebbles, 1997; Kleinke,
Wallis, & Stadler, 1992). In times past and cultures more
Calling research “academic” outside of laboratories and distant, where people did not so naturally restrict inten-
universities is a kind way of demeaning it as irrelevant to tional capacities to humans, animals (e.g., rats) and objects
everyday life. Research on mind perception is indeed con- (e.g., “possessed” statues) were targets of criminal pros-
ducted by academics, but its implications and consequences ecution (Berman, 1994; see also Sunstein & Nussbaum,
are hardly “academic.” Explaining, understanding, and 2004). Beyond attributing responsibility to others, reducing
predicting both ourselves and other agents, from people to the extent to which people believe they can intentionally
gadgets to gods, is what much of everyday social thought control their own behavior appears to diminish their sense
is all about. Mind perception matters because it is often the of personal responsibility. Undermining people’s belief in
critical mediator between others’ real or imagine dactions their own “free will,” for instance, increases the likelihood
and our reactions or between our current predictions and our of behaving unethically by cheating on a test or harming
subsequent decisions. Although the consequences of mind another person (Baumeister, Masicampo, & DeWall, 2009;
perception range widely, we wish to focus here on three Vohs & Schooler, 2008).
of the most basic consequences that result from the per- Although intentionality—a guilty mind, or mens rea—
ceived presence or absence of mind in others. First, mind- has been necessary to find a defendant guilty of a crime
ful agents are capable of reasoned thought and intentional in most of the world’s criminal courts, it is not strictly

CH14.indd 523 12/22/09 3:39:45 PM


524 Mind Perception

necessary for assigning responsibility for most of the action, have the ability to consider perspectives other than
world’s ordinary perceivers but instead intensifies percep- their own, and experience everything from suffering and
tions of responsibility. We are angered when our neighbor regret to compassion and joy. Mental states do not merely
takes our morning paper accidentally, but we are enraged grant useful explanations or enable future predictions, they
when our neighbor does so intentionally (Alicke, 2000; also grant an agent moral worth and value. The most basic
Heider, 1958; Malle & Bennett, 2002; Weiner, 1995). This of all human rights is the principle of autonomy—that
“intensification effect” emerges not only in judgments of because all people have the same minimal capacity to suf-
praise and blame (Malle & Bennett, 2002) but also in inten- fer, deliberate, and choose, no person can compromise the
sity of consequences that result from intentional versus body, life, or freedom of another person. “When moral
unintentional actions. Unintentional sexism makes a per- worth is in question, it is not a matter of actions which one
son appear somewhat prejudiced, but intentional sexism sees but of their inner principles which one does not see”
makes the person appear blatantly prejudiced and dis- (Kant, 1785/2002, p. 23).
criminatory (Swim, Scott, Sechrist, Campbell, & Stangor, Ordinary perceivers listen to rules from philosophers
2003). Unintentional harm seems to hurt another per- like teenagers listen to rules from their parents, but this
son and is judged to be immoral, but intentional harm is basic principle of autonomy is followed at least somewhat
worse (Cushman, 2008; Darley & Shultz, 1990; Kohlberg, in both moral judgment and moral action. Attributing basic
1969). And being unintentionally shocked with electricity mental states of experience and agency to others confers
by another person hurts, but being intentionally shocked by the basic rights of “personhood” (even to distinctly non-
another person hurts even more (Gray & Wegner, 2008). human agents) that can engender empathic and altruistic
Such sensitivity to intentional versus unintentional responses, whereas denying those mental states in others
actions can appear perfectly logical, but these findings sug- appears to be the essence of dehumanization that can
gest that there may be some residual illogic as well because engender mistreatment and aggression (Haslam, 2006).
people still show a pervasive and consistent tendency to How mind perception influences moral value and ethi-
assign considerable personal responsibility to acciden- cal treatment, however, appears to depend on the mental
tal or unintentional actions (Walster, 1966). People may states being inferred. Perceiving mindful agency (inten-
unreasonably blame victims for their misfortunes (Lerner, tions, planning, and deliberate thought) is related to the
1977; Maes, 1994) or assume that an actor ’s intentions are judgments of causal responsibility and consequent praise
consistent with the consequences of an action—especially or blame for an agent’s actions. Perceiving the capacity for
a negative action—even when the consequence appears mindful experience (conscious awareness, secondary emo-
accidental (Alicke, Weigold, & Rogers, 1990; Knobe, tions, suffering, and pain) appears to guide empathy, com-
2003; Leslie, Knobe, & Cohen, 2006; Mazzocco, Alicke, & passion, and the willingness to harm or impinge on the basic
Davis, 2004). And anyone who has ever momentarily felt rights of another agent (Gray et al., 2007). In one study,
like thrashing the kitchen cabinet for getting in the way of for instance, people were interested in helping the victims
his or her head can appreciate that even mindless objects of a massive hurricane to the extent that they perceived
can seem to perform intentional harm, even if only for those victims to be experiencing secondary emotions such
a moment (Schultz, Imamizu, Kawato, & Frith, 2004). as anguish and remorse (Cuddy, Rock, & Norton, 2007).
These less logical findings appear to reflect the more gen- In another, the extent to which people were dispositionally
eral tendency described already for ordinary perceivers inclined to attribute humanlike mental states to nonhuman
to automatically assume a correspondence between agents’ agents also predicted the extent to which it appeared mor-
actions and their underlying intentions and dispositions ally wrong to harm a nonhuman agent, such as destroy-
and then discount those default inferences insufficiently ing IBM’s legendary computer Big Blue or even trampling
for unintentional causes that explain the action (Gilbert & over a bed of flowers (Waytz, Cacioppo, & Epley, 2009).
Malone, 1995; Rosset, 2008). This general tendency to And in general, the extent to which an agent has the capac-
infer consistent intentions from observable actions may ity for mindful experience predicts how unpleasant people
therefore lead to a greater number of angry neighbors, bro- report it would be to hurt that agent if they had to do so
ken cabinets, and guilty defendants than a purely logical (Gray et al., 2007).
analysis of responsibility would produce (Alicke, 2000). This connection between mind perception and moral
action does not simply influence moral reasoning; it influ-
ences moral action as well. Cultures that are especially
Moral Agency and Ethical Action
likely to perceive minds in their surrounding natural envi-
Fully mindful agents are perceived to have goals and ronment also exhibit the least harmful ecological prac-
intentions, be capable of reasoned thought and deliberate tices (Atran & Medin, 2008; Atran et al., 2002). Being

CH14.indd 524 12/22/09 3:39:46 PM


Consequences of Mind Perception 525

environmentally conscious is partly determined, it appears, position of power, increase the tendency to objectify others
by the extent to which a person perceives the environment as well (Gruenfeld et al., 2008).
as being conscious. Attending to the mental experience The consequences of mind perception for moral judg-
of another agent in pain by actively engaging in perspec- ment and behavior are not only relevant to everyday
tive taking also increases empathic concern for the agent interactions among individuals or groups but also feature
(including nonhuman agents; Schultz, 2000) and increases prominently in societal debates on moral issues including
the likelihood of behaving altruistically to help an agent in animal rights, euthanasia, the death penalty, and abortion.
need (for reviews, see Batson, 1994, 1998). Increasing the The importance of mind in these debates is clear:
likelihood of spontaneously considering another ’s mental
experience, such as by increasing the sense of similarity or Some think it’s obvious that a ten-week-old fetus has a mind,
desire to affiliate, also increases the extent to which people and others think it’s obvious that it does not. If it does not, then
experience empathic concern for another ’s pain or suf- the path is open to argue that it has no more interest than, say,
a gangrenous leg or an abscessed tooth—it can be destroyed
fering (Cialdini, Brown, Lewis, Luce, & Neuberg, 1997;
to save the life of (or just to suit the interests of ) the mind-
Pickett et al., 2004; Preston & de Waal, 2002). The essence
haver of which it is a part. If it does already have a mind, then,
of the autonomy principle is to treat other agents as ends whatever we decide, we obviously have to consider its inter-
in themselves rather than as means to an end, and actively ests along with the interests of its temporary host. (Dennett,
considering others’ mental experiences appears critical for 1996, p. 6)
producing such treatment.
If mindful experience is critical for treating others with Whether understanding the processes that increase
the basic human right of autonomy, then failing to perceive or decrease the perception of mind provides insight into
mindful experience in others should lead people to treat resolving these seemingly intractable disputes remains
others as mindless agents or objects. Indeed, the essence of to be seen, but research on mind perception is beginning to
dehumanization is denying mental states and experiences to actively inform at least some ethical debates and legal
others by representing them as either unthinking savages or decisions (Rogers & Kaplan, 2004). The Spanish govern-
unfeeling automata (Haslam, 2006; Loughnan & Haslam, ment, for instance, recently extended limited human rights
2007). Just as perceiving an agent’s capacity to suffer to captive chimpanzees based on research demonstrat-
makes harming that agent appear immoral and unethical, ing their humanlike cognitive capacities (Abend, 2008).
denying an agent the capacity to suffer makes aggression Determining how far such influence might extend will
and otherwise immoral harm seem permissible (Bandura, require rigorous programs of research that test scientific
2002). Dehumanizing outgroup members by denying them hypotheses rather than relying on current arguments from
mindful capacities increases the extent to which violence anecdotes or intuitions.
and aggression toward the outgroup appears acceptable and
justified (Castano & Giner-Sorolla, 2006; Goff et al.,
Social Surveillance
2008), increases actual aggression toward the outgroup
(Bandura et al., 1975), and increases negative attitudes Mindful agents have intentions and goals and are thus
toward the outgroup (Hodson & Costello, 2007). Some of responsible for their actions, they experience suffering
the most chilling images ever captured on film are of Nazi and pain and therefore deserve moral care and concern,
doctors sitting calmly beside Jewish prisoners amid hor- but they also have attitudes, hold beliefs, and form impres-
rific medical experiments, calmly monitoring humans who sions. Other minds are therefore sources of surveillance
had been thoroughly dehumanized to the point of being that may be forming attitudes, beliefs, or impressions
treated as mice in a laboratory (Spitz, 2005). about us. People care deeply about how they are viewed
Failing to consider an agent’s own intentions, goals, or by others (Leary & Kowalski, 1995), attempt to under-
mental experience can also lead to objectification whereby stand and anticipate others’ impressions as best they can
people represent others in terms of their instrumental (Kenny, 1994), and actively try to manage others’ impres-
qualities—how that agent can be used to achieve one’s own sions through self-presentational strategies (Jones &
goals—rather than as a mindful agent with goals, inten- Pittman, 1982; Leary, 1995). People are especially sen-
tions, and desires to be considered. Treating women as sex sitive to others’ negative impressions that might lead to
objects, for instance, involves attending to a woman’s social disconnection or ostracism, and for good reason.
physical attributes rather than to her mental attributes Being socially disconnected or ostracized by others is
(Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Some determinants already deeply painful (Williams, 2001), increases the incidence
discussed that reduce a person’s tendency to consider of depression and suicide ideation (Heinrich & Gullone,
another agent’s mental states, such as putting a person in a 2006), and decreases the quality of one’s health and

CH14.indd 525 12/22/09 3:39:46 PM


526 Mind Perception

the length of one’s life (Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Berntson, in the presence of the relatively nonevaluative dog (Allen,
2003; Hawkley, Masi, Berry, & Cacioppo, 2006; House, Blascovich, Tomaka, & Kelsey, 1991). In fact, the presence of
Landis, & Umberson, 1988). one’s nonevaluative pet before a stressful experience can
At the most basic level, surveillance by other mindful have a more positive influence on anxiety and psycho-
agents can have two effects on ordinary perceivers. First, physical responses to stress than the presence of one’s
monitoring others’ attitudes and impressions can be both highly evaluative spouse (Allen, Blascovich, & Mendes,
cognitively taxing (i.e., it consumes working memory 2002). Mindful agents require conscious monitoring, and
resources) and stressful (due to heightened concerns of that monitoring can alter attention, consume cognitive
negative evaluations), thereby influencing performance resources, and meaningfully affect behavior.
while under social scrutiny. Monitoring others’ impressions Second, surveillance from other mindful agents can
increases the attention people pay to themselves and their serve as a source of social control, leading people to
own behavior in social interaction to discern others’ evalu- behave in more socially desirable and prosocial ways than
ations (Buss, 1980; Duval & Wicklund, 1972; Fenigstein, they would when alone or when not attending to others’
1979), and in intergroup interactions monitoring increases evaluations. Imagine, for instance, the things you might do
the accessibility of stereotypes about how one’s own if you were given a cloak that would make you invisible
group is likely to be evaluated by other groups (Vorauer and you will get this point. People behave more desirably
et al., 2000). to the extent that they think about, and care about, the kinds
This increase in self-focused attention can lead people of impressions they convey to others (Leary, 1995). That
to exaggerate the extent to which they think others are mindful surveillance increases socially desirable behav-
attending to them (Gilovich, Medvec, & Savitsky, 2000), ior is clear, but it is especially interesting because such
the extent to which they think others’ behavior is explicitly effects emerge even when under the presumed surveil-
targeted at them (Fenigstein, 1984; Zuckerman, Kernis, lance of mindful agents. The essence of many large-scale
Guarnera, Murphy, & Rappoport, 1983), and the extent to world religions, for instance, is the constant presence of
which they believe their own inner thoughts and experi- at least one supernatural agent, an unseen source of social
ences are transparent to others (Cameron & Vorauer, 2008; surveillance that can control behavior and enable coopera-
Gilovich et al., 1998; Vorauer & Ross, 1999). At a more tive social organization. Omnipresent surveillance from a
extreme level, hyperattentiveness to others’ impressions religious agent solves the most basic problem facing any
can lead to persecutory delusions and intense paranoia cooperative organization—the difficulty of controlling
(Martin & Penn, 2001). Emerging research suggests that individuals when a leader or group organizer is absent—
an overactive theory of mind may be the core mechanism and may be the primary reason for the success of religious
underlying schizophrenia (Abu-Akel, 1999; Abu-Akel & belief systems across generations of cultural evolution
Bailey, 2000; Badcock, 2004; Crespi & Badcock, 2008; (Irons, 1991; Johnson & Bering, 2006; Johnson & Kruger,
Freeman & Garety, 2004). 2004; Norenzayan & Shariff, 2008; Sosis & Ruffle, 2004;
Actively attending to others’ impressions and evalua- Wilson, 2002). Priming people with religious concepts,
tions also consumes cognitive resources (e.g., working for instance, increases prosocial behavior in a competitive
memory) and thus diminishes performance on cognitively interaction compared with priming them with neutral con-
demanding tasks, such as solving math problems (Beilock & cepts (Shariff & Norenzayan, 2007). Cross-cultural evi-
Carr, 2005) or delivering a speech (Savitsky & Gilovich, dence suggests a positive association between the extent of
2003). This can disrupt otherwise automatic behavior by belief in an evaluative moral deity and the cultural group
increasing conscious attention to the behavior, such as size (Norenzayan & Shariff, 2008). Beyond surveillance
swinging a golf club among expert golfers (see Beilock & by presumed religious agents, people behave more pro-
Gray, 2007, for a review). These impairments are espe- socially while playing an economic game on a computer
cially likely in social interactions characterized by the most when it is displaying animated eyes than when there are
extreme evaluative concerns, such as in interracial interac- no eyes (Haley & Fessler, 2005). And participants in one
tions (Shelton & Richeson, 2006; Vorauer, 2006), and are experiment were less likely to cheat on a test in a labora-
diminished when others are unlikely to be evaluating the tory after being told by the experimenter that the ghost of
self (Bray & Sugarman, 1980; Feinberg & Aiello, 2006; a dead graduate student (“Paul”) had been seen repeatedly
Geen, 1981; Paulus & Murdoch, 1971). In one experiment, in the laboratory room (Bering, McLeod, & Shackelford,
participants performed a difficult counting task either in 2005). Mindful agents also appear to serve as a powerful
the presence of their friend or in the presence of a dog. source of social influence and control, increasing adherence
Participants had elevated stress responses and poorer per- to socially accepted norms of conduct whether those others
formance in the presence of their evaluative friend but not are actually present or merely presumed to be present.

CH14.indd 526 12/22/09 3:39:47 PM


Conclusion 527

Most parents have told their children to “mind your differs from the broader topics of person perception and
manners” in the presence of others, and this bit of folk wis- social cognition, focusing on inferences about momentary
dom captures the two most basic effects of social surveil- mental states of intentions, desires, motivations, and beliefs
lance on behavior—needing to mind or monitor others’ rather than on inferences about more invariant and endur-
impressions and then behaving in a way that is consistent ing dispositions or traits. This not only represents an
with accepted social norms and conventions. These effects increased focus on the initial moments of the causal attribu-
are first-order consequences that result from perceiving the tion process that enables people to explain others’ behavior
presence or absence of mindful surveillance. Once under and infer more enduring dispositional tendencies but also
mindful surveillance, more complicated second-order con- represents a considerable expansion of research interests
sequences arise that stem from the specific thoughts, beliefs, to include a more earnest focus on ongoing social inter-
or attitudes that those surveilling minds might possess. In actions, future predictions, and the relation between self
intergroup interactions, the exaggerated belief that one’s and other. The capacity to get inside the minds of others
own group is disliked by the other group increases anxiety enables not only an understanding of others’ actions in the
in social interaction, diminishes the likelihood of intergroup course of a social interaction but also the visceral expe-
contact, increases defensiveness once amid an interac- rience of others’ current mental states and foresight into
tion, and ultimately tends to confirm the mistaken belief their future actions or experiences. Mind perception is
about the other side’s negative evaluations (Cameron, therefore critical to the relational capacities of empathy,
Holmes, & Vorauer, 2009; Shelton & Richeson, 2006; compassion, trust, cooperation, and strategic interaction.
Vorauer, Main, & O’Connell, 1998; Vorauer & Sakamoto, The tendency to mentalize also goes far beyond perceiv-
2006). People also tend to overestimate the extent to which ing other humans, as people see minds in various biologi-
others are motivated simply by their own self-interest, a cyn- cal, technological, and supernatural agents. When Gordon
icism that can in turn operate as a social norm and increase Allport (1968) said that social psychology is “an attempt
people’s tendency to behave selfishly themselves (Miller, to understand how the thoughts, feelings, and behaviors
1999). Adopting another person’s perspective in these cases of individuals are influenced by the actual, imagined, or
can actually exaggerate the impact of these mistaken beliefs implied presence of others,” he did not constrain “others”
about others’ thoughts and evaluations, further diminishing to refer only to other people. Mind perception interprets
interest in intergroup interaction (Vorauer & Sasaki, 2009) “others” more generally and is therefore relevant to a rap-
or further increasing selfishness in a competitive interaction idly expanding set of topics, including religion, dehuman-
(Epley et al., 2006). Minding your manners may be a more ization, and anthropomorphism.
complicated business than your parents would have guessed. We have said little, however, about how inferences
about momentary mental states relate to inferences about
more stable and invariant features of another agent such
CONCLUSION as dispositions or abilities. That is not because the relation
is simple or uninteresting—indeed, more connecting work
Science is sometimes accused of diminishing people’s needs to be done—but rather because that is a topic for
sense of wonder and amazement by providing sensible another chapter (Gilbert, 1998; Macrae & Quadflieg, this
explanations for life’s deepest mysteries. Scientists illu- volume). Mind perception tells us about another agent’s
minating how people reason about other minds, in many state at a given moment (what it is currently thinking, feel-
ways, are doing precisely the opposite by taking a process ing, believing, or intending), and these inferences are then
that seems so easy that children can do it before tying their shuffled along to the causal calculus of attribution theory
shoes and revealing complexity that makes it seem some- and social cognition to tell us what others are like more
what amazing that anyone can do it at all. Philosophers generally (whether others are generally lazy or lively, bum-
have recognized this problem, calling into question the bling or brilliant, and callous or caring).
ability to conclude that any minds exist other than one’s We have also said much about how people reason about
own. Everyone else rolls their eyes at such skepticism other minds, focusing on the ability to simulate others’
because they have acquired the requisite machinery to experience by imagining oneself in another ’s shoes and
reason quite easily about other minds. We have sought to the ability to make inferences about mental states based
explain how people solve this other minds problem with on more general theoretical inferences. We have sug-
such apparent ease and why people’s intuitive solutions to gested, in line with more recent theoretical accounts, that
this problem matter so deeply for everyday social life. people likely use both of these mechanisms in varying
A chapter of this length has said much, but it has also left degrees depending on the context. We believe that think-
much unsaid. We have said much about how mind perception ing of mind perception as a basic process of induction will

CH14.indd 527 12/22/09 3:39:47 PM


528 Mind Perception

help to provide a more integrative account of mental state related capacities and whether mind perception is therefore
inferences, but this is a topic of heated debate and frenzied a small piece of a larger evolved capacity or whether it is a
research activity. The coming years of integration will be relatively distinct neural module. Not only can people tran-
exciting to those who care deeply about these basic mecha- scend their own skulls to think about other minds, but they
nisms and, we hope, clarifying for people who care about can also transcend the present to think about the future or
the consequences of these mechanisms for everyday social past and they can transcend their current location to imagine
interaction. themselves moving in another location. These seemingly
We have said little, however, about how accurately people distinct capacities appear to use the same neural circuitry
make inferences about other minds. At times, this ques- (Buckner & Carroll, 2007), suggesting that these capacities
tion is unanswerable. Nonhuman animals may or may not may be deeply related to one another, and their behavioral
actually have the mental capacities that people attribute to consequences might therefore be deeply related as well. Is
them. Whether other people actually have beliefs or free temporal discounting—underweighting future events com-
will has been debated for centuries with little meaningful pared with current events—related to biases in perspective
progress. And knowing the actual beliefs and intentions of taking and the tendency to consider others’ mental states?
religious agents is—well, complicated. At other times the Are extreme temporal discounters also inaccurate mind
question has too many answers. People in some situations readers? Are the clinical extremes of mind perception—
show an amazing ability to accurately intuit others’ mental possibly autism on the capacity’s low end and schizophrenia
states and at other times seem to have no ability (e.g., Ickes, on the high end—also related to variability in foresight
2003; Kenny, 1994). A speaker may be reasonably good, or spatial reasoning? Does increasing people’s future-
for instance, at determining whether a speech was gener- mindedness also increase their perspective-taking ability?
ally liked or disliked by the crowd, but may be no better Are religious experiences of transcendence—reasoning about
than chance at determining which audience members actu- one’s ultimate purpose, considering the image of God, or
ally liked the talk and which did not (Kenny & DePaulo, meditative experiences—part of this shared neural net-
1993). When predicting future emotional experiences or work? The brain is a cobbled piece of machinery shaped
even behavioral tendencies, there is often a strong corre- by specific demands of survival and reproduction. These
lation between predictions and reality, but there may be varied tasks and experiences need not be related to one
consistent biases that distort everyone’s predictions (e.g., another. They need not be completely unrelated, either.
Epley & Dunning, 2006; Gagné & Lydon, 2004; Gilovich We have said much about why mind perception matters
et al., 1998; Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, Gilbert, & Axsom, to social psychology, but interest in how people understand
2000). And at times people seem to overestimate how other minds is now among the dominant research topics
similar others’ beliefs and preferences will be to their own in other areas of psychology as well, with our developmen-
(Krueger & Clement, 1994), but at other times they tend tal, cognitive, and neuropsychological colleagues actively
to underestimate such similarities (Hoch, 1987). A better participating, along with philosophers, sociologists, and
understanding of the mechanisms that enable mind percep- biologists. At present, there is insufficient conversation
tion will begin providing better insight into when people among these groups, and no consistent language enables
are likely to be systematically accurate and when they are everyone to understand the rapidly expanding body of
not, as well as how to improve accuracy when it would findings and how they relate to one another. This is the
be desirable to do so. first chapter explicitly addressing mind perception in this
We have also said much about when people are likely handbook, but we doubt it will be the last. Whether this
to consider other minds and when they are not, suggest- topic will morph into another, adopt different language
ing that interdependence is a critical determinant. Factors to describe itself, or again be subsumed into the broader
that increase the need to explain, predict, or understand literature on person perception is unclear. It is, however,
another agent’s behavior are likely to increase the attention unlikely to disappear. There is much left to be said.
paid to other minds, as is the basic motivation to estab-
lish a social connection with another agent. The ability to
get beyond one’s own immediate perspective and imagine
what it is like inside the head of another person is surely REFERENCES
one of humans’ greatest capacities, and interdependence
facilitates people’s actual use of this capacity. Abend, L. (2008, July 18). In Spain, human rights for apes. Time.
Retrieved August 27, 2008, from http://www.time.com/time/world/
We have said little, however, about how this capacity article/0,8599,1824206,00.html?imw⫽Y.
to transcend one’s own egocentric experience to reason Abu-Akel, A. (1999). Impaired theory of mind in schizophrenia. Pragmatics
about other minds might be related to other seemingly and Cognition, 7, 247–282.

CH14.indd 528 12/22/09 3:39:48 PM


References 529

Abu-Akel, A., & Bailey, A. L. (2000). The possibility of different forms of science: Anthropological inquiry into boundaries, power, and knowl-
theory of mind impairment in psychiatric and developmental disorders. edge (pp. 239–258). New York: Routledge.
Psychological Medicine, 30, 735–738. Astington, J. W. (1993). The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge,
Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. MA: Harvard University Press.
Psychological Bulletin, 126, 556–574. Astington, J. W., & Gopnik, A. (1988). Knowing you’ve changed your
Alicke, M. D., Dunning, D., & Krueger, J. I. (2005). The self in social mind: Children’s understanding of representational change. In
judgment. New York: Psychology Press. J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories
Alicke, M. D., & Largo, E. (1995). The unique role of the self in the false of mind (pp. 193–206). New York: Cambridge University Press.
consensus effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, Atran, S. (2002). In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion.
28–47. New York: Oxford University Press.
Alicke, M. D., Weigold, M. F., & Rogers, S. L. (1990). Inferring intentions Atran, S., & Medin, D. L. (2008). The native mind and the cultural con-
and responsibility from motives and outcomes: Evidential and extra- struction of nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
evidential judgments. Social Cognition, 8, 286–305. Atran, S., Medin, D., Vapnarsky, V., Ucan Ek’, E., Coley, J. D., Timura, C.,
Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2002). Cardiovascular et al. (2002). Folkecology, cultural epidemiology, and the spirit of the
reactivity and the presence of pets, friends, and spouses: The truth commons: A garden experiment in the Maya lowlands, 1995–2000.
about cats and dogs. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 727–739. Current Anthropology, 43, 421–450.
Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., Tomaka, J., & Kelsey, R. M. (1991). Presence Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2004). Religion’s evolutionary landscape:
of human friends and pet dogs as moderators of autonomic responses Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral
to stress in women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, and Brain Sciences, 27, 713–770.
582–589. Austin, J. R. (2003). Transactive memory in organizational groups: The
Allport, F. H. (1924). Social psychology. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. effects of content, consensus, specialization, and accuracy on group
Allport, G. W. (1968). The person in psychology: Selected papers. Boston: performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 866–878.
Beacon. Babcock, L., & Loewenstein, G. (1997). Explaining bargaining impasse:
Ames, D. R. (2004a). Inside the mind reader ’s toolkit: Projection and The role of self-serving biases. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 11,
stereotyping in mental state inference. Journal of Personality and 109–126.
Social Psychology, 87, 340–353. Badcock, C. R. (2004). Mentalism and mechanism: The twin modes of
Ames, D. R. (2004b). Strategies for social inference: A similarity contin- human cognition. In C. Crawford & C. Salmon (Eds.), Human nature
gency model of projection and stereotyping in attribute prevalence esti- and social values: Implications of evolutionary psychology for public
mates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 573–585. policy (pp. 99–116). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ames, D. L., Jenkins, A. C., Banaji, M. R., & Mitchell, J. P. (2008). Taking Baker, C. L., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Saxe, R. (2008). Bayesian models
another ’s perspective increases self-referential neural processing. of human action understanding. Advances in Neural Information
Psychological Science, 19, 642–644. Processing Systems, 18, 1043–1050.
Amsterlaw, J. (2006). Children’s beliefs about everyday reasoning. Child Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint
Development, 77, 443–464. reference. Child Development, 62, 875–890.

Anggoro, F. K., Waxman, S. R., & Medin, D. L. (2008). Naming practices Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Early referential understanding: Infants’ abil-
and the acquisition of key biological concepts: Evidence from ity to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental
English and Indonesian. Psychological Science, 19, 314–319. Psychology, 29, 832–843.

Apperly, I. A. (2008). Beyond simulation–theory and theory–theory: Why Baldwin, D. A., & Moses, L. J. (1996). The ontogeny of social information
social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study gathering. Child Development, 67, 1915–1939.
“Theory of Mind.” Cognition, 107, 266–283. Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of
Apperly, I. A., Back, E., Samson, D., & France, L. (2008). The cost of moral agency. Journal of Moral Education, 31, 101–119.
thinking about false beliefs: Evidence from adult performance on a Bandura, A., Underwood, B., & Fromson, M. E. (1975). Disinhibition of
non-inferential theory of mind task. Cognition, 106, 1093–1108. aggression through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization
Apperly, I. A., Riggs, K. J., Simpson, A., Samson, D., & Chiavarino, C. of victims. Journal of Research in Personality, 9, 253–269.
(2006). Is belief reasoning automatic? Psychological Science, 17, Bargh, J. A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives
841–844. on Psychological Science, 3, 73–79.
Ariely, D. (1998). Combining experiences over time: The effects of dura- Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of
tion, intensity changes and on-line measurements on retrospective pain mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.
evaluations. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11, 19–45. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.
self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Barrett, J. L. (1998). Cognitive constraints on Hindu concepts of the divine.
Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 37, 608–619.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relation- Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends
ships as including other in the self. Journal of Personality and Social in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 29–34.
Psychology, 60, 241–253. Barrett, J. L., & Johnson, A. H. (2003). The role of control in attributing
Asch, S. (1952). Social psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. intentional agency to inanimate objects. Journal of Cognition and
Asquith, P. J. (1986). Anthropomorphism and the Japanese and Western Culture, 3, 208–217.
traditions in primatology. In J. G. Else & P. C. Lee (Eds.), Primate Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. C. (1996). Conceptualizing a non-natural entity:
ontogeny, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 61–71). Cambridge, Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31,
England: Cambridge University Press. 219–247.
Asquith, P. J. (1996). Japanese science and Western hegemonies: Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind.
Primatology and the limits set to questions. In L. Nader (Ed.), Naked Oxford: Oxford University Press.

CH14.indd 529 12/22/09 3:39:48 PM


530 Mind Perception

Bass, B., & Yammarino, F. (1991). Congruence of self and others’ leader- Breazeal, C. (2003). Emotion and sociable humanoid robots. International
ship ratings of naval officers for understanding successful performance. Journal of Human Computer Interaction, 59, 119–155.
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 40, 437–454. Breazeal, C., & Aryananda, L. (2002). Recognizing affective intent in robot
Batson, C. D. (1994). Prosocial motivation: Why do we help others? directed speech. Autonomous Robots, 12, 83–104.
In A. Tesser (Ed.), Advanced social psychology (pp. 333–381). Boston: Breazeal, C., & Scassellati, B. (2002). Robots that imitate humans. Trends
McGraw-Hill. in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 481–487.
Batson, C. D. (1998). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In D. T. Gilbert, Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The
S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology,
(Vol. 2, pp. 282–316). New York: McGraw-Hill. 18, 906–921.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire Brewer, M. B. (1979). Ingroup bias in the minimal intergroup situation:
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. A cognitive-motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 307–324.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Broadhurst, P. L. (1963). The science of animal behavior. Harmondsworth,
Baumeister, R. F., Masicampo, E. J., & DeWall, C. N. (2009). Prosocial England: Penguin.
benefits of feeling free: Disbelief in free will increases aggression and
Bruner, J. S., & Tagiuri, R. (1954). The perception of people. In G. Lindzey
reduces helpfulness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35,
(Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 634–654). Reading,
260–268.
MA: Addison-Wesley.
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling ver-
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V.,
sus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental
et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal
Psychology, 41, 328–337.
areas in a somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail: Neuroscience, 13, 400–404.
Working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., & Riggio, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system
Science, 16, 101–105. and action recognition. Brain and Language, 89, 370–376.
Beilock, S. L., & Gray, R. (2007). Why do athletes “choke” under pres- Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain.
sure? In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psy- Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 49–57.
chology (pp. 425–444). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Exploring the “planning fal-
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances lacy”: Why people underestimate their task completion times. Journal
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York: of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 366–381.
Academic Press.
Burris, C. T., Batson, C. D., Altstaedten, M., & Stephens, S. K. (1994).
Bem, D. J., & McConnell, H. K. (1970). Testing the self-perception expla- What a friend: Loneliness as a motivator of intrinsic religion. Journal
nation of dissonance phenomena: On the salience of premanipulational for the Scientific Study of Religion, 33, 326–334.
attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 23–31.
Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco:
Bennett, J. (1978). Some remarks about concepts. Behavioral and Brain W. H. Freeman.
Sciences, 1, 557–560.
Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press.
Benson, P. L., & Spilka, B. (1973). God image as a function of self-esteem
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Berntson, G. G. (2003). The anatomy of
and locus of control. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 12,
loneliness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 71–74.
297–310.
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary deter-
Bering, J. M. (2002). Intuitive conceptions of dead agents’ minds: The
minants of attitudes II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects
natural foundations of afterlife beliefs as phenomenological boundary.
on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.
Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2, 263–308.
Cain, B. S. (1988). Divorce among elderly women: A growing social
Bering, J. M., McLeod, K. A., & Shackelford, T. K. (2005). Reasoning about
phenomenon. Social Casework: Journal of Contemporary Social
dead agents reveals possible adaptive trends. Human Nature, 16, 60–81.
Work, 69, 563–568.
Berman, P. S. (1994). Rats, pigs, and statues on trial: The creation of cul-
Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Unwilling or
tural narratives in the prosecution of animals and inanimate objects.
unable: Chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action.
NYU Law Review, 69, 288–326.
Developmental Science, 7, 488–498.
Bernstein, D. M., Atance, C., Meltzoff, A. N., & Loftus, G. R. (2007).
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Distinguishing intentional from acciden-
Hindsight bias and developing theories of mind. Child Development,
tal actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan trog-
78, 1374–1394.
lodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative
Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., & Cutting, J. E. (1984). Infant sensitivity to Psychology, 112, 192–206.
invariant structure revealed through motion. Journal of Experimental
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). A nonverbal false belief task: The perfor-
Child Psychology, 37, 213–230.
mance of children and great apes. Child Development, 70, 381–395.
Birch, S., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge in reasoning about
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of
false beliefs. Psychological Science, 18, 382–386.
mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187–192.
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge,
Callaghan, T. C., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M. L., Odden, H., Itakura, S.,
MA: MIT Press.
Tapanya, S., & Singh, S. (2005). Synchrony in the onset of mental-state
Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby. New York: Basic Books. reasoning. Psychological Science, 16, 378–384.
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., & Weber, M. (1989). The curse of knowl-
thought. New York: Basic Books. edge in economic settings: An experimental analysis. Journal of
Boyer, P. (2003). Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain Political Economy, 97, 1232–1254.
function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 119–124. Cameron, J. J., Holmes, J. G., & Vorauer, J. D. (in press). Cascading meta-
Bray, R. M., & Sugarman, R. (1980). Social facilitation among interact- perceptions: Signal amplification bias as a consequence of reflected
ing groups: Evidence for the evaluation-apprehension hypothesis. self-esteem. Self and Identity. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 137–142. Manitoba, Winnipeg.

CH14.indd 530 12/22/09 3:39:49 PM


References 531

Cameron, J. J., & Vorauer, J. D. (2008). Feeling transparent: On metaper- Crespi, B., & Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis and autism as diametrical
ceptions and miscommunications. Personality and Social Psychology disorders of the social brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31,
Compass, 2, 1093–1108. 241–261.
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibi- Crichton, M. T., & Lange-Kuttner, C. (1999). Animacy and propulsion in
tory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, infancy: Tracking, waving and reaching to self-propelled and induced
1032–1053. moving objects. Developmental Science, 2, 318–324.
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Claxton, L. J. (2004). Individual differ- Cuddy, A. J. C., Rock, M., & Norton, M. I. (2007). Aid in the aftermath of
ences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of Hurricane Katrina: Inferences of secondary emotions and intergroup
inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Experimental Child helping. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 107–118.
Psychology, 87, 299–319. Cushman, F. A. (2008). The role of mental state attribution in moral judg-
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Hix, H. R. (1998). The role of inhibitory ment: A dissociation between help and harm. Unpublished manu-
processes in young children’s difficulties with deception and false script, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.
belief. Child Development, 69, 672–691. Darley, J. M., & Shultz, T. R. (1990). Moral rules: Their content and acqui-
Carnegie, D. (1981). How to win friends and influence people. London: sition. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 525–556.
Cedar. (Original work published 1936.) Darwin, C. (2002). The expression of emotions in man and animals. New
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., Tomasello, M. (1998). Sixteen-month-old York: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1872.)
infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy:
Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330. Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and
Case, T. I., & Williams, K. D. (2004). Ostracism: A metaphor for death. Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.
In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., & Luce, C. (1996). The effect of
of experimental existential psychology (pp. 336–351). New York: perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A merg-
Guilford Press. ing of self and other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Castano, E., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2006). Not quite human: Infra- 70, 713–726.
humanization as a response to collective responsibility for intergroup Davis, M. H., Soderlund, T., Cole, J., Gadol, E., Kute, M., Myers, M., &
killing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 804–818. Weihing, J. (2004). Cognitions associated with attempts to empathize:
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social How do we imagine the perspective of another? Personality and Social
psychology. New York: Guilford Press. Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1625–1635.
Chambers, J. R., Epley, N., Savitsky, K., & Windschitl, P. D. (2008). Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. Public
Knowing too much: Using private knowledge to predict how one is Opinion Quarterly, 47, 1–15.
viewed by others. Psychological Science, 19, 542–548. Dawes, R. M., & Mulford, M. (1996). The false consensus effect and
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception– overconfidence: Flaws in judgment, or flaws in how we study judg-
behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social ment? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65,
Psychology, 76, 893–910. 201–211.
Chartrand, T. L., Fitzsimons, G. M., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2008). Automatic de Villiers, P. A. (2005). The role of language in theory-of-mind devel-
effects of anthropomorphized objects on behavior. Social Cognition, opment: What deaf children tell us. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird
26, 198–209. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 266–297). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W., & Lakin, J. (2005). Beyond the percep-
tion–behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational modera- Decety, J., Grèzes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E.,
tors of nonconscious mimicry. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh Grassi, F., & Fazio, F. (1997). Brain activity during observation of
(Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 334–361). New York: Oxford actions: Influence of action content and subject’s strategy. Brain, 120,
University Press. 1763–1777.
Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Self-monitoring without aware- Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. P., Paladino, M. P., Rodriguez, R. T.,
ness: Using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. Journal of Rodriguez, A. P., & Dovidio, J. F. (2004). Dimensions of “uniquely” and
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1170–1179. “nonuniquely” human emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 71–96.
Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner ’s verbal behavior. Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain
Language, 35, 26–58. Sciences, 1, 568–570.
Churchland, P. M. (1984). Matter and consciousness: A contemporary Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT
introduction to the philosophy of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Press.
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L. Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of minds: Toward an understanding of con-
(1997). Reinterpreting the empathy–altruism relationship: When one sciousness. New York: Basic Books.
into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Descartes, R. (1998). Discourse on method (D. Cress, Trans.). Indianapolis,
73, 481–494. IN: Hacket. (Original work published 1637.)
Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative pro- Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T. (1982). Social interaction and the develop-
cess. Cognition, 22, 1–39. ment of definite descriptions. Cognition, 11, 159–184.
Cohen, D., & Gunz, A. (2002). As seen by the other . . . : Perspectives on Douglas, W. (1984). Initial interaction scripts: When knowing is behaving.
the self in the memories and emotional perceptions of Easterners and Human Communication Research, 11, 203–220.
Westerners. Psychological Science, 13, 55–59. Douglas, W. (1990). Uncertainty, information-seeking, and liking during ini-
Cohen, A. R., Stotland, E., & Wolfe, D. M. (1955). An experimental tial interaction. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 54, 66–81.
investigation of need for cognition. Journal of Abnormal and Social Dunbar, R. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language and society in evolu-
Psychology, 51, 291–294. tionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–181.
Converse, B. A., Lin, S., Keysar, B., & Epley, N. (2008). In the mood to get Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991).
over yourself: Mood affects theory-of-mind use. Emotion, 8, 725–730. Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs:

CH14.indd 531 12/22/09 3:39:49 PM


532 Mind Perception

Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, Feuerbach, L. (2004). The essence of religion. Amherst, NY: Prometheus
1352–1366. Books. (Original work published 1873.)
Duval, T. S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self- Fincham, F. D., & Emery, R. E. (1988). Limited mental capacities and per-
awareness. New York: Academic Press. ceived control in attribution of responsibility. British Journal of Social
Eddy, T. J., Gallup, G. G., Jr., & Povinelli, D. J. (1993). Attribution of cog- Psychology, 27, 193–207.
nitive states to animals: Anthropomorphism in comparative perspec- Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight =⁄ foresight: The effect of outcome
tive. Journal of Social Issues, 49, 87–101. knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental
Eich, E., Reeves, J. L., Jaeger, B., & Graff-Radford, S. B. (1985). Memory for Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288–299.
pain: Relation between past and present pain intensity. Pain, 23, 375–380. Fiske, S. T. (1992). Thinking is for doing: Portraits of social cognition
Elfenbein, H., Foo, M. D., White, Tan, H., & Aik, V. (2007). Reading your from daguerreotype to laserphoto. Journal of Personality and Social
counterpart: The benefit of emotion recognition accuracy for effective- Psychology, 63, 877–889.
ness in negotiation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31, 205–223. Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on ste-
Ellsberg, D. (1961). Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms. Quarterly reotyping. American Psychologist, 48, 621–628.
Journal of Economics, 75, 643–699. Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social beings: A core motives approach to social psy-
Epley, N. (2008). Solving the (real) other minds problem. Social and chology. New York: Wiley.
Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1455–1474. Flavell, J. H. (1986). The development of children’s knowledge about the
Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Creating social appearance–reality distinction. American Psychologist, 41, 418–425.
connection through inferential reproduction: Loneliness and perceived Flavell, J. H. (1988). The development of children’s knowledge about
agency in gadgets, gods, and greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19, the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. In
114–120. J. W. Astington, P. Harris, & D. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of
Epley, N., Caruso, E. M., & Bazerman, M. H. (2006). When perspective mind (pp. 244–267). New York: Cambridge University Press.
taking increases taking: Reactive egoism in social interaction. Journal Floridi, L., & Sanders, J. W. (2004). On the morality of artificial agents.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 872–889. Minds and Machine, 14, 349–379.
Epley, N., Converse, B., Delbosc, A., Monteleone, G., Cacioppo, J. T. Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolati, G.
(in press). Estimates of God’s beliefs are more egocentric than esti- (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention under-
mates of other people’s beliefs. Proceedings of the National Academy standing. Science, 308, 662–667.
of Sciences. Frantz, C. M., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (2000). Considering both sides: The
Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier than thou”: Are self- limits of perspective-taking. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22,
serving assessments produced by errors in self or social prediction? 32–41.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 861–875. Fredrickson, B. L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retro-
Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2006). The mixed blessings of self-knowledge spective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and
in behavioral prediction: Enhanced discrimination but exacerbated Social Psychology, 65, 45–55.
bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 641–655. Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory.
Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206.
taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality Freeman, D., & Garety, P. A. (2004). Paranoia: The psychology of perse-
and Social Psychology, 87, 327–339. cutory delusions. New York: Psychology Press.
Epley, N., Morewedge, C., & Keysar, B. (2004). Perspective taking in Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mental-
children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. izing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 760–768. 358, 459–473.
Epley, N., Savitsky, K., & Gilovich, T. (2002). Empathy neglect: Fussell, S. R., & Krauss, R. M. (1992). Coordination of knowledge in com-
Reconciling the spotlight effect and the correspondence bias. Journal munication: Effects of speakers’ assumptions about what others know.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 300–312. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 378–391.
Epley, N., Waytz, A., Akalis, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). When I need Gagné, F., & Lydon, J. (2004). Bias and accuracy in close relationships:
a human: Motivational determinants of anthropomorphism. Social An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8,
Cognition, 26, 143–155. 322–338.
Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three- Galinsky, A. D., Ku, G., & Wang, C. S. (2005). Perspective-taking:
factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114, Fostering social bonds and facilitating social coordination. Group
864–886. Processes and Intergroup Relations, 8, 109–125.
Erber, R., & Fiske, S. T. (1984). Outcome dependency and atten- Galinsky, A. D., Maddux, W. W., Gilin, D., & White, J. B. (2008). Why it
tion to inconsistent information. Journal of Personality and Social pays to get inside the head of your opponent: The differential effects of
Psychology, 47, 709–726. perspective-taking and empathy in strategic interactions. Psychological
Farah, M. J., & Heberlein, A. S. (2007). Personhood and neuroscience: Science, 19, 378–384.
Naturalizing or nihilating? American Journal of Bioethics. 7, 37–48. Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power
Feinberg, J. M., & Aiello, J. R. (2006). Social facilitation: A test of compet- and perspectives not taken. Psychological Science, 17, 1068–1074.
ing theories. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 1087–1109. Galinsky, A. D., Wang, C. S., & Ku, G. (2008). Perspective-takers behave
Fenigstein, A. (1979). Self-consciousness, self-attention, and social inter- more stereotypically. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 75–86. 95, 404–419.
Fenigstein, A. (1984). Self-consciousness and the overperception of self as Gallegos, D. (2007, January 24). Study: Americans spend more time with
a target. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 860–870. computer than spouse. Denver Post. Retrieved September 11, 2008,
Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., & from http://www.denverpost.com/search/ci_5075438.
Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recog-
Biology, 4, e302. nition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

CH14.indd 532 12/22/09 3:39:50 PM


References 533

Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation Gopnik, A. (1993). How we know our minds: The illusion of first-
theory of mindreading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501. person knowledge of intentionality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the 16, 1–15.
basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 396–403. Gopnik, A. (1996). Theories and modules: Creation myths, developmen-
Gaunt, R., Leyens, J. P., & Demoulin, S. (2002). Intergroup relations and tal realities and Neurath’s boat. In P. Carruthers & P. Smith (Eds.),
the attribution of emotions: Control over memory for secondary emo- Theories of theories of mind (pp. 169–183). Cambridge, England:
tions associated with ingroup versus outgroup. Journal of Experimental Cambridge University Press.
Social Psychology, 38, 508–514. Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children’s understanding of rep-
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, C. (2007). The anthro- resentational change and its relation to the understanding of false
pomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and belief and the appearance–reality distinction. Child Development, 59,
robotic actions. NeuroImage, 35, 1674–1684. 26–37.
Geen, R. G. (1981). Evaluation apprehension and social facilitation: A reply Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1994). Minds, bodies and persons:
to Sanders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 252–256. Young children’s understanding of the self and others as reflected in
imitation and “theory of mind” research. In S. Parker & R. Mitchell (Eds.),
Gernsbacher, M. A., & Frymiare, J. L. (2006). Does the autistic brain lack
Self-awareness in animals and humans. New York: Cambridge
core modules? Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 9,
University Press.
3–16.
Gopnik, A., Slaughter, V., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1994). Changing your views:
Gilbert, D. T. (1998). Ordinary personology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &
How understanding visual perception can lead to a new theory of the
G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 89–150).
mind. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Origins of a theory of mind
New York: McGraw-Hill.
(pp. 157–181). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gilbert, D. T., Brown, R. P., Pinel, E. C., & Wilson, T. D. (2000). The illu-
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. (1994). The “theory theory.” In L. Hirschfield &
sion of external agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in culture
79, 690–700.
and cognition (pp. 257–293). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Gilbert, D. T., & Gill, M. J. (2000). The momentary realist. Psychological
Gordon, R. (1986). Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language,
Science, 11, 394–398.
1, 158–170.
Gilbert, D. T., Gill, M. J., & Wilson, T. D. (2002). The future is now:
Gosling, S. D., Kwan, V. S. Y., & John, O. P. (2003). A dog’s got personal-
Temporal correction in affective forecasting. Organizational Behavior
ity: A cross-species comparative approach to personality judgments in
and Human Decision Processes, 88, 430–444.
dogs and humans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,
Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation 1161–1169.
and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and
Granqvist, P., & Hagekull, B. (2000). Religiosity, adult attachment, and
Social Psychology, 60, 509–517.
why “singles” are more religious. International Journal for the
Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraint: Psychology of Religion, 10, 111–123.
Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and
Grant, E., & Spivey, M. (2003). Eye movements and problem solving:
Social Psychology, 50, 269–280.
Guiding attention guides thought. Psychological Science, 14, 462–466.
Gilbert, D. T., Killingsworth, M. A., Eyre, R. N., & Wilson, T. D. (2009).
Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind per-
The surprising power of neighborly advice. Science, 323, 1617–1619.
ception. Science, 315, 619.
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias.
Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2008). The sting of intentional pain.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38.
Psychological Science, 19, 1260–1262.
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busy-
Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2009). Moral typecasting: Divergent percep-
ness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of
tions of moral agents and moral patients. Journal of Personality and
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733–740.
Social Psychology, 96, 505–520.
Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the
future. Science, 317, 1351–1354. Grice, H. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.),
Syntax and semantics: Speech acts. New York: Academic Press.
Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human
reason in everyday life. New York: Free Press. Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008).
Power and the objectification of social targets. Journal of Personality
Gilovich, T., Kruger, J., & Medvec, V. H. (2002). The spotlight effect revis- and Social Psychology, 95, 111–127.
ited: Overestimating the manifest variability in our actions and appear-
ance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 93–99. Guajardo, J. J., & Woodward, A. L. (2004). Is agency skin deep? Surface
attributes influence infants’ sensitivity to goal-directed action. Infancy,
Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky, K. (2000). The spotlight effect
6, 361–384.
in social judgment: An egocentric bias in estimates of the salience of
one’s own actions and appearance. Journal of Personality and Social Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Psychology, 78, 211–222. Haley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005). Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues
Gilovich, T., Savitsky, K., & Medvec, V. H. (1998). The illusion of trans- affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and
parency: Biased assessments of others’ ability to read our emotional Human Behavior, 26, 245–256.
states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 332–346. Hamlin, J. K., Hallinan, E. V., & Woodward, A. L. (2008). Do as I do:
Goethals, G. R., & Reckman, R. F. (1973). The perception of consistency 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. Developmental
in attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9, 491–501. Science, 11, 487–494.
Goff, P. A., Eberhardt, J. L., Williams, M., & Jackson, M. C. (2008). Not yet Hare, B. (2007). From nonhuman to human mind: What changed and why.
human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contempo- Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 60–64.
rary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what
292–306. conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61, 139–151.
Goldman, A. I. (2008). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees deceive a human
neuroscience of mindreading. New York: Oxford University Press. competitor by hiding. Cognition, 101, 495–514.

CH14.indd 533 12/22/09 3:39:50 PM


534 Mind Perception

Harman, G. (1978). Studying the chimpanzee’s theory of mind. Behavioral Higgins, E. T., & Rholes, W. J. (1978). “Saying is believing”: Effects of
and Brain Sciences, 1, 576–577. message modification on memory and liking for the person described.
Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 363–378.
low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological Hinde, R. A. (1999). Why gods persist: A scientific approach to religion.
Science, 17, 847–853. New York: Routledge.
Harris, P. L. (1991). The work of the imagination. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Hoch, S. J. (1987). Perceived consensus and predictive accuracy: The pros
Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of and cons of projection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
everyday mindreading (pp. 283–304). London: Blackwell. 53, 221–234.
Harris, P. L. (1992). From simulation to folk psychology: The case for Hodson, G., & Costello, K. (2007). Interpersonal disgust, ideological ori-
development. Mind & Language, 7, 120–144. entations, and dehumanization as predictors of intergroup attitudes.
Harris, P. L. (2002). What do children learn from testimony? In P. Carruthers, Psychological Science, 18, 691–698.
S. P. Stich, & M. Siegal (Eds.), The cognitive basis of science Hogue, T. E., & Pebbles, J. (1997). The influence of remorse, intent and
(pp. 316–334). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. attitudes toward sex offenders on judgments of a rapist. Psychology,
Harris Poll. (2003). The religious and other beliefs of Americans 2003. Crime, & Law, 3, 249–259.
Harris Poll No. 11. Retrieved September 11, 2008, from http://www House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships
.harrisinteractive.com/harris_poll/index.asp?pid ⫽359. and health. Science, 241, 540–545.
Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrated review. Personality Hsee, C. K., & Weber, E. U. (1997). A fundamental prediction error:
and Social Psychology Review, 10, 252–264. Self–other discrepancies in risk preference. Journal of Experimental
Haslam, N., & Bain, P. (2007). Humanizing the self: Moderators of the Psychology: General, 126, 45–53.
attribution of lesser humanness to others. Personality and Social Hume, D. (1957). The natural history of religion. Stanford, CA: Stanford
Psychology Bulletin, 33, 57–68. University Press. (Original work published 1757.)
Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M., & Bastian, B. (2005). More Hume, D. (1958). A treatise of human nature. Oxford: Oxford University
human than you: Attributing humanness to self and others. Journal of Press. (Original work published 1739.)
Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 937–950.
Humphrey, N. (1986). The inner eye. London: Faber and Faber.
Hass, R. G. (1984). Perspective-taking and self-awareness: Drawing an E
Iacoboni, M., Koski, L., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Woods, R. P.,
on your forehead. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,
Dubeau, M. C., et al. (2001). Re-afferent copies of imitated actions
788–798.
in the right superior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R.L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Academy of Sciences, 98, 13995–13999.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ickes, W. (2003). Everyday mind reading: Understanding what other
Hawkins, S. A., & Hastie, R. (1990). Hindsight: Biased judgments of past people think and feel. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.
events after the outcomes are known. Psychological Bulletin, 107,
Irons, W. (1991). How did morality evolve? Zygon: Journal of Religion
311–327.
and Science, 26, 49–89.
Hawkley, L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry, J. D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006).
Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differences in systolic Isaacs, E. A., & Clark, H. H. (1987). References in conversation between
blood pressure. Psychology and Aging, 21, 152–164. experts and novices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
116, 26–37.
Heal, J. (1986). Replication and functionalism. In J. Butterfield (Ed.),
Language, Mind and Logic (pp. 135–150). Cambridge, England: Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and neg-
Cambridge University Press. ative emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage, 34, 1744–1753.
Heath, C. (1999). On the social psychology of agency relationships: Lay James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Henry Holt.
theories of motivation overemphasize extrinsic rewards. Organizational Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factors and family
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 25–62. structure associated with theory of mind development in young chil-
Hebb, D. O. (1946). Emotion in man and animal: An analysis of the intui- dren. Developmental Psychology, 32, 70–78.
tive processes of recognition. Psychological Review, 53, 88–106. Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees are vengeful
Hegarty, M. (2004). Mechanical reasoning by mental simulation. Trends in but not spiteful. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
Cognitive Sciences, 8, 280–285. 104, 13046–13050.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Johnson, C. N. (1988). Theory of mind and the structure of conscious
Wiley. experience. In Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (Eds.),
Developing theories of mind (pp. 47–63). Cambridge, England:
Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent
Cambridge University Press.
behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–249.
Johnson, D. D. P., & Bering, J. M. (2006). Hand of God, mind of man:
Heinrich, L. A., & Gullone, E. (2006). The clinical significance of loneli-
Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary
ness: A literature review. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 695–718.
Psychology, 4, 219–233.
Heller, D. I. (1986). The children’s God. Chicago: University of Chicago
Johnson, D. D. P., & Kruger, O. (2004). The good of wrath: Supernatural
Press.
punishment and the evolution of cooperation. Political Theology, 5,
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. 159–176.
(2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition:
The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366. Johnson, S. C. (2003). Detecting agents. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society B, 358, 549–559.
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: accessibility, applicability, and
salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Johnson, S. C., Booth, A. E., & O’Hearn, K. (2001). Inferring the goals of a
Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York: Guilford Press. non-human agent. Cognitive Development, 16, 637–656.
Higgins, E. T., McCann, C. D., & Fondacaro, R. A. (1982). The “commu- Jones, E. E. (1990). Interpersonal perception. New York: W. H. Freeman.
nication game”: Goal-directed encoding and cognitive consequences. Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attri-
Social Cognition, 1, 21–37. bution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances

CH14.indd 534 12/22/09 3:39:51 PM


References 535

in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 371–388). New York: intuitive judgment (pp. 150–166). New York: Cambridge University
Academic Press. Press.
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1972). The actor and the observer: diver- Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking per-
gent perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. Kanouse, spective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehen-
H. H. Kelly, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: sion. Psychological Science, 11, 32–38.
Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 79–94). Morristown, NJ: Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2007). Integrating simulation and theory of
General Learning Press. mind: from self to social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11,
Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic 194–196.
self-presentation. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on Keysers, C., Kohler, E., Umilta, M. A., Nanetti, L., Fogassi, L., &
the self (Vol. 1, pp. 231–260). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gallese, V. (2003). Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition.
Kagan, J. (1972). Motives and development. Journal of Personality and Experimental Brain Research, 153, 628–636.
Social Psychology, 22, 51–66. Kihlstrom, J. F., Cantor, N., Albright, J. S., Chew, B. R., Klein, S. B., &
Kahn, M. (1970). Nonverbal communication and marital satisfaction. Niedenthal, P. M. (1988). Information processing and the study of self.
Family Process, 9, 449–456. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
Kahn, R. L. (1958). Human relations on the shop floor. In E. M. Hugh-Jones (Vol. 21, pp. 145–178). San Diego: Academic Press.
(Ed.), Human relations and modern management (pp. 43–74). Chicago: Kirkpatrick, L. (1999). Attachment and religious representations and
Quadrangle Books. behavior. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attach-
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: ment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 803–822). New
Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & York: Guilford Press.
D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intui- Kleinke, C. L., Wallis, R., & Stadler, K. (1992). Evaluation of a rap-
tive judgment (pp. 49–81). New York: Cambridge University Press. ist as a function of expressed intent and remorse. Journal of Social
Kahneman, D., & Lovallo, D. (1993). Timid choices and bold forecasts: Psychology, 132, 525–537.
A cognitive perspective on risk taking. Management Science, 39, 17–31. Knobe, J. (2003). Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language.
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J., & Thaler, R. (1990). Experimental tests of Analysis, 63, 190–193.
the endowment effect and the Coase theorem. Journal of Political Koda, T., & Maes, P. (1996). Agents with faces: The effect of personi-
Economy, 98, 1325–1348. fication. Proceedings of IEEE: Workshop on Robot and Human
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Intuitive prediction: Biases and Communication, 5, 189–194.
corrective procedures. TIMS Studies in Management Science, 12, Koehler, D. J., & Poon, C. S. K. (2006). Self-predictions overweight
313–327. strength of current intentions. Journal of Experimental Social
Kammer, D. (1982). Differences in trait ascriptions to self and friend: Psychology, 42, 517–524.
Unconfounding intensity from variability. Psychological Reports, 51, Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental
99–102. approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socializa-
Kant, I. (1953). Critique of pure reason. New York: Macmillan. (Original tion theory and research (pp. 151–235). New York: Academic Press.
work published 1781.) Kozak, M. N., Czipri, A., Felczak, J., & Correll, J. (2009). Friend or
Kant, I. (2002). Groundwork for the metaphysics of morals. New Haven, foe? Mind attribution as a function of perceived outgroup threat.
CT: Yale University Press. (Original work published 1785.) Unpublished manuscript, Pace University, New York.
Kozak, M. J., Marsh, A. A., & Wegner, D. M. (2006). What do I think
Karniol, R. (2003). Egocentrism vs. protocentrism: The status of self in
you’re doing? Action identification and mind attribution. Journal of
social prediction. Psychological Review, 110, 564–580.
Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 543–555.
Karremans, J. C., & Verwijmeren, T. (2008). Mimicking attractive opposite-
Krauss, R. M., & Fussell, S. R. (1991). Perspective-taking in communi-
sex others: The role of romantic relationship status. Personality and
cation: Representations of others’ knowledge in reference. Social
Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 939–950.
Cognition, 9, 2–24.
Katz, D., & Allport, F. (1931). Student attitudes: A report of the Syracuse
Krech, D., & Crutchfield, R. S. (1948). Theories and problems of social
University research study. Syracuse, NY: Craftsmen Press.
psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine
Krueger, J. (1998). On the perception of social consensus. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
(Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192–240).
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 163–240).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
San Diego: Academic Press.
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American
Krueger, J., & Acevedo, M. (2005). Social projection and the psychology
Psychologist, 28, 107–128.
of choice. In M. D. Alicke, D. Dunning, & J. Krueger (Eds.), The self in
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, social perception (pp. 17–41). New York: Psychology Press.
and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284.
Krueger, J., & Clement, R. W. (1994). The truly false consensus effect:
Keltner, D., & Robinson, R. J. (1997). Defending the status quo: Power An ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. Journal of
and bias in social conflict. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 596–610.
23, 1066–1077.
Kruger, J., Epley, N., Parker, J., & Ng, Z. (2005). Egocentrism over e-mail:
Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis. Can people communicate as well as they think? Journal of Personality
New York: Guilford Press. and Social Psychology, 89, 925–936.
Kenny, D. A., & DePaulo, B. M. (1993). Do people know how others view Kruger, J., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Actions, intentions, and trait assess-
them? An empirical and theoretical account. Psychological Bulletin, ment: The road to self-enhancement is paved with good intentions.
114, 145–161. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 328–339.
Keysar, B., & Barr, D. J. (2002). Self anchoring in conversation: Why lan- Kwan, V. S. Y., & Fiske, S. T. (2008). Missing links in social cognition:
guage users do not do what they “should.” In T. Gilovich,D. W. Griffin, The continuum from nonhuman agents to dehumanized humans. Social
& D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of Cognition, 26, 125–128.

CH14.indd 535 12/22/09 3:39:51 PM


536 Mind Perception

Kwan, V. S. Y., Gosling, S. D., & John, O. P. (2008). Anthropomorphism Locke, J. (1975). An essay concerning human understanding. In
as a special case of social perception: A cross-species compara- P. H. Nidditch (Ed.), Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Original work pub-
tive approach and a new empirical paradigm. Social Cognition, lished 1690.)
26, 129–142. Loewenstein, G. (2005). Hot-cold empathy gaps and medical decision-
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral making. Health Psychology, 24, S49–S56.
mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, Loewenstein, G., O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M. (2003). Projection bias
334–339. in predicting future utility. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118,
Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. (2008). I am too just like you: 1209–1248.
Nonconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social Loewenstein, G., & Adler, D. (1995). A bias in the prediction of tastes.
exclusion. Psychological Science, 19, 816–822. Economic Journal, 105, 929–937.
Landy, R. J. (2001). How we see God and why it matters. Springfield, IL: Loughnan, S., & Haslam, N. (2007). Animals and androids: Implicit asso-
Charles C. Thomas. ciations between social categories and nonhumans. Psychological
Lau, I. Y.-M., Chiu, C.-Y., & Hong, Y. (2001). I know what you know: Science, 18, 116–121.
Assumptions about others’ knowledge and their effects on message Luhrmann, T. M. (2004). Metakinesis: How God becomes intimate in con-
construction. Social Cognition, 19, 587–600. temporary U.S. Christianity. American Anthropologist, 106, 518–528.
Lautenschlager, G. J., & Flaherty, V. L. (1990). Computer administration MacDonald, T., & Ross, M. (1999). Assessing the accuracy of predictions
of questions: More desirable or more social desirability? Journal of about dating relationships: How and why do lovers’ predictions differ
Applied Psychology, 75, 310–314. from those made by observers? Personality and Social Psychological
Leary, M. R. (1995). Self-presentation: Impression management and Bulletin, 25, 1417–1429.
interpersonal behavior. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark. Macrae, C. N., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: Thinking
Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. (1995). Social anxiety. London: Guildford categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 93–120.
Press. Maes, J. (1994). Blaming the victim: Belief in control or belief in justice?
Lee, D. (2008). Game theory and neural basis of social decision making. Social Justice Research, 7, 69–90.
Nature Neuroscience, 11, 404–409. Malle, B. F. (1999). How people explain behavior: A new theoretical
Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early imita- framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 23–48.
tion. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423–433. Malle, B. F. (2005). Folk theory of mind: Conceptual foundations of human
Lemay, E. P., Jr., & Clark, M. S. (2008). How the head liberates the heart: social cognition. In R. Hassin, J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The
Projection of communal responsiveness guides relationship promotion. new unconscious (pp. 225–255). New York: Oxford University Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 647–671. Malle, B. F., & Bennett, R. E. (2002). People’s praise and blame for intentions
Lerner, M. J. (1977). The justice motive: Some hypotheses as to its origins and actions: Implications of the folk concept of intentionality. (Technical
and forms. Journal of Personality, 45, 1–52. Reports of the Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, No. 02-2).
Lesher, J. H. (1992). Xenophanes of Colophon: Fragments. Toronto: Eugene, OR: Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences.
University of Toronto Press. Malle, B. F., & Knobe, J. (1997). Which behaviors do people explain?
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of A basic actor-observer asymmetry. Journal of Personality and Social
mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412–426. Psychology, 72, 288–304.
Leslie, A., Knobe, J., & Cohen, A. (2006). Acting intentionally and Malle, B. F., Knobe, J., & Nelson, S. (2007). Actor-observer asymmetries
the side-effect effect: “Theory of mind” and moral judgment. in explanations of behavior: New answers to an old question. Journal
Psychological Science, 17, 421–427. of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 491–514.
Leung, A. K.-Y., & Cohen, D. (2007). The soft embodiment of culture: Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Robertson, T. E., Hofer, B., &
Camera angles and motion through time and space. Psychological Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Functional projection: How fundamental social
Science, 18, 824–830. motives can bias interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 88, 63–78.
Lewis, C., Freeman, N. H., Kyriakidou, C., Maridaki-Kassotaki, K., &
Berridge, D. (1996). Social influences on false belief access: Specific Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications
sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development, 67, for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98,
2930–2947. 224–253.
Lewis, C. S. (1952). Mere Christianity. New York: Macmillan. Markus, H. R., & Sentis, K. (1982). The self in social information process-
ing. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 1,
Lewis, K., Lange, D., & Gillis, L. (2005). Transactive memory systems,
pp. 41–70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
learning and learning transfer. Organization Science, 16, 581–598.
Martel, B. (2006, January 17). Storms payback from God, Nagin says.
Leyens, J. P., Cortes, B. P., Demoulin, S., Dovidio, J., Fiske, S. T., Gaunt,
Washington Post, p. A4.
R., Paladino, M. P., Rodriguez-Perez, A., Rodriguez-Torres, & R., Vaes,
J. (2003). Emotional prejudice, essentialism, and nationalism. European Martin, J. A., & Penn, D. L. (2001). Social cognition and sub-clinical para-
Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 703–717. noid ideation, British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 261–265.
Leyens, J. P., Paladino, P. M., Rodriguez, R. T., Vaes, J., Demoulin, S., Maurer, D. (1985). Infants’ perception of facedness. In T. Field & N. Fox
Rodriguez, A. P., & Gaunt, R. (2000). The emotional side of prejudice: (Eds.), Social perception in infancy (pp. 73–100). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
The role of secondary emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Mazzocco, P. J., Alicke, M. D., & Davis, T. L. (2004). On the robustness
Review, 4, 186–197. of outcome bias: No constraint by prior culpability. Basic and Applied
Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflexion Social Psychology, 26, 131–146.
and reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attribu- McAlister, A., & Peterson, C. (2007). A longitudinal study of child siblings
tional inference. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social and theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 22, 258–270.
psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 199–249). New York: Elsevier. McCabe, K. A., Smith, V. L., & LePore, M. (2000). Intentionality detection
Lillard, A. S. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theory of and “mindreading”: Why does game form matter? Proceedings of the
mind. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 3–30. National Academy of Sciences, 97, 4404–4409.

CH14.indd 536 12/22/09 3:39:52 PM


References 537

McFarland, C., & Miller, D. T. (1990). Judgments of self–other similar- Mitchell, R. W., Thompson, N. S., & Miles, H. L. (1997).
ity: Just like others only more so. Personality and Social Psychology Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals. Albany: State University
Bulletin, 16, 475–484. of New York Press.
McFarland, C., & Ross, M. (1987). The relation between current impres- Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind. London: Thames & Hudson.
sions and memories of self and dating partners. Personality and Social Moreale, E., & Watt, S. (2004). An agent-based approach to mailing list
Psychology Bulletin, 13, 228–238. knowledge management. Agent-Mediated Knowledge Management
McIntosh, D. N., Silver, R. C., & Wortman, C. B. (1993). Religion’s role Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence, 2926, 118–129.
in adjustment to a negative life event: Coping with the loss of a child. Moreland, R. L., & Argote, L. (2003). Transactive memory in dynamic
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 65, 812–821. organizations. In R. Peterson & E. Mannix (Eds.), Leading and man-
Mead, N. L., Caruso, E. M., Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2008). aging people in the dynamic organization (pp. 135–162). Mahwah,
There’s no “you” in money: Subtle reminders of money increase ego- NJ: Erlbaum.
centrism and decrease interpersonal likeability. Unpublished manu- Morewedge, C. K., Preston, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Timescale bias in
script, Tillburg University. the attribution of mind. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Medin, D. L., & Atran, S. (2004). The native mind: Biological categoriza- 93, 1–11.
tion, reasoning and decision making in development and across cul- Morris, M. W., Sheldon, O. J., Ames, D. R., & Young, M. J. (2007).
tures. Psychological Review, 111, 960–983. Metaphors and the market: Consequences and preconditions of agent
Melis, A. P., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees conceal and object metaphors in stock market commentary. Organizational
visual and auditory information from others. Journal of Comparative Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102, 174–192.
Psychology, 120, 154–162. Moses, L. J. (1993). Young children’s understanding of belief constraints
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re- on intention. Cognitive Development, 8, 1–25.
enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Mudge, E. L. (1923). The God experience. Cincinnati, OH: Caxton Press.
Psychology, 31, 838–850.
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., Bellavia, G., Griffin, D. W., & Dolderman, D.
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). “Like me”: A foundation for social cognition. (2002). Kindred spirits? The benefits of egocentrism in close relation-
Developmental Science, 10, 126–134. ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 563–581.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2001). “Like me” as a building block for
Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment:
understanding other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. In
Mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110, 472–489.
B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and inten-
tionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 171–191). Cambridge, Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2000). Imitation of intentional
MA: MIT Press. manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 114, 381–391.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual
gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78. Nass, C., Isbister, K., & Lee, E. (2000). Truth is beauty: Researching
embodied conversational agents. In J. Cassell, J. Sullivan, S. Prevost, &
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult
E. Churchill (Eds.), Embodied conversational agents (pp. 374–402).
facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the
Nass, C., Moon, Y., Fogg, B. J., Reeves, B., & Dryer, D. C. (1995). Can
representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17,
computer personalities be human personalities? International Journal
83–99.
of Human-Computer Studies, 43, 223–239.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: Neale, M. A., & Bazerman, M. H. (1983). The effects of perspective taking
A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–192. ability under alternate forms of arbitration on the negotiation process.
Mill, J. S. (1974). A system of logic ratiocinative and inductive. In Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 36, 378–388.
J. M. Robson (Ed.), Collected works of John Stuart Mill (Vol. 7). Neuberg, S. L., & Fiske, S. T. (1987). Motivational influences on impres-
Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (Original work published 1872.) sion formation: Outcome dependency, accuracy-driven attention, and
Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, individuating processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54, 1–8. 53, 431–444.
Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1987). Pluralistic ignorance: When similar- Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). Approach and avoidance: The influence
ity is interpreted as dissimilarity. Journal of Personality and Social of proprioceptive and exteroceptive cues on encoding of affective infor-
Psychology, 53, 298–305. mation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 39–48.
Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1991). When social comparison goes awry: Nichols, S., & Stich, S. (2003). Mindreading: An integrated account of
The case of pluralistic ignorance. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. New York:
comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 287–313). Oxford University Press.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know—and sometimes misjudge—what
Miller, D. T., & Nelson, L. (2002). Seeing approach motivation in the others know: Imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psychological
avoidance behavior of others: Implications for an understanding of Bulletin, 125, 737–759.
pluralistic ignorance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., &
83, 1066–1075. Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emo-
Mitchell, J. P. (2006). Mentalizing and Marr: An information processing tion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.
approach to the study of social cognition. Brain Research, 1079, 66–75. Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Innes-Ker, A. H. (2001).
Mitchell, J. P. (2008). Contributions of functional neuroimaging to the study When did her smile drop? Contrast effects in the influence of emotional
of social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, state on the detection of change in emotional expression. Cognition
142–146. and Emotion, 15, 853–864.
Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Dissociable medial Nisbett, R. E., Caputo, C., Legant, P., & Marecek, J. (1973). Behavior as
prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. seen by the actor and as seen by the observer. Journal of Personality
Neuron, 50, 655–663. and Social Psychology, 27, 154–164.

CH14.indd 537 12/22/09 3:39:52 PM


538 Mind Perception

Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996a). Chimpanzees: Joint visual attention.
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. Psychological Science, 7, 129–135.
Noller, P., & Ruzzene, M. (1991). Communication in marriage: The influ- Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996b). What young chimpanzees know
ence of affect and cognition. In G. J. O. Fletcher & F. D. Fincham about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
(Eds.), Cognition in close relationships (pp. 203–233). Hillsdale, NJ: Development, 61, 1–152.
Lawrence Erlbaum. Povinelli, D. J., & Vonk, J. (2003). Chimpanzee minds: Suspiciously
Norenzayan, A., & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural agents in the human. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 157–160.
face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 174–187. Preissler, M. A., & Bloom, P. (2008). Two-year-olds use artist intention to
Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of reli- understand drawings. Cognition, 106, 512–518.
gious prosociality. Science, 322, 58–62. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory
Oberman, L., Winkielman, P., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). Face to face: of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
Blocking facial mimicry can selectively impair recognition of emo- Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1993). Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol
tional expressions. Social Neuroscience, 2, 167–178. use on campus: Some consequences of misperceiving the social norm.
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants under- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 243–256.
stand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258. Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and prox-
Opotow, S. (1990). Moral exclusion and injustice: An introduction. Journal imate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–72.
of Social Issues, 46, 1–20. Pronin, E. (in press). The introspection illusion. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
Overbeck, J. R., & Park, B. (2006). Beyond “high” versus “low”: Advances in experimental social psychology.
Power effects on individuation as a function of the interaction con- Pronin, E., Berger, J., & Molouki, S. (2007). Alone in a crowd of sheep:
text. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99, Asymmetric perceptions of conformity and their roots in an introspection
227–243. illusion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 585–595.
Parish, S. (1991). The sacred mind: Newar cultural representations of Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the eye of
mental life and the production of moral consciousness. Ethos, 19, the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others.
313–351. Psychological Review, 111, 781–799.
Parish, S. (1994). Moral knowing in a sacred Hindu city (Vol. 19). New Pronin, E., Kruger, J., Savitsky, K., & Ross, L. (2001). You don’t know
York: Columbia University Press. me, but I know you: The illusion of asymmetric insight. Journal of
Paulus, P. B., & Murdoch, P. (1971). Anticipated evaluation and audi- Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 639–656.
ence presence in the enhancement of dominant responses. Journal of Pronin, E., & Kugler, M. B. (2007). Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior:
Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 280–291. The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot. Journal of
Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). On the lack of evidence that chim- Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 565–578.
panzees possess anything remotely resembling a “theory of mind.” Pronin, E., Olivola, C. Y., & Kennedy, K. A. (2008). Doing unto future
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 731–744. selves as you would do unto others: Psychological distance and decision
Pepperberg, I. M. (2002). The Alex studies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 224–236.
University Press. Pronin, E., & Ross, L. (2006). Temporal differences in trait self ascription:
Perloff, R. M. (1993). Third-person effect research 1983–1992: A review When the self is seen as an other. Journal of Personality and Social
and synthesis. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 5, Psychology, 90, 197–209.
167–184. Pronin, E., Wegner, D. M., McCarthy, K., & Rodriguez, S. (2006).
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, Everyday magical powers: The role of apparent mental causation in
MA: MIT Press. the overestimation of personal influence. Journal of Personality and
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year olds’ difficulty Social Psychology, 91, 218–231.
with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Ratner, R. K., Kahn, B. E., & Kahneman, D. (1999). Choosing less-
Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137. preferred experiences for the sake of variety. Journal of Consumer
Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S. R. (1994). Theory of mind is Research, 26, 1–15.
contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing:
1228–1238. 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422.
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that . . . ”: Read, D., & Loewenstein, G. (1995). The diversification bias: Explaining
Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. the discrepancy in variety seeking between combined and separated
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471. choices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1, 34–49.
Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. New York: Harcourt Redelmeier, D., & Kahneman, D. (1996). Patients’ memories of painful
Brace Jovanovich. medical treatments: Real-time and retrospective evaluations of two
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child. London: minimally invasive procedures. Pain, 66, 3–8.
Routledge and Kegan Paul. Reeves, B., & Nass, C. I. (1996). The media equation. Stanford, CA:
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. London: Cambridge University Press/CSLI.
Routledge and Kegan Paul. Ren, Y., Carley, K. M., & Argote, L. (2006). The contingency effects of
Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. (2004). Getting a cue: The transactive memory: When is it more beneficial to know what others
need to belong and enhanced sensitivity to social cues. Personality and know? Management Science, 52, 671–682.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1095–1107. Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D.
Plantinga, A. (1966). Induction and other minds. Review of Metaphysics, (2004). The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal
19, 441–461. interactions. NeuroImage, 22, 1694–1703.
Platek, S. M., Critton, S. R., Myers, T. E., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2003). Risucci, D. A., Tortolani, A. J., & Ward, R. J. (1989). Ratings of surgi-
Contagious yawning: The role of self-awareness and mental state attri- cal residents by self, supervisors and peers. Surgical Gynecology and
bution. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 223–227. Obstetrics, 169, 519–526.

CH14.indd 538 12/22/09 3:39:53 PM


References 539

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Schultz, J., Imamizu, H., Kawato, M., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Activation
Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. of the human superior temporal gyrus during observation of goal attri-
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor bution by intentional objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16,
cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 1695–1705.
3, 131–141. Schultz, P. W. (2000). Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mech- taking on concern for environmental issues. Journal of Social Issues,
anisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature 56, 391–406.
Reviews Neuroscience, 2: 661–670. Schwab, R., & Petersen, K. U. (1990). Religiousness: Its relation to loneli-
Roberts, C. F., & Golding, S. L. (1991). The social construction of criminal ness, neuroticism and subjective well-being. Journal for the Scientific
responsibility and insanity. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 349–376. Study of Religion, 29, 335–345.
Rock, I., & Nijhawan, R. (1989). Regression to egocentrically determined Semin, G. R., & Smith, E. R. (Eds.). (2008). Embodied grounding: Social,
description of form under conditions of inattention. Journal of Experimental cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. New York:
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 259–272. Cambridge University Press.
Rogers, L. J., & Kaplan, G. (Eds.). (2004). Comparative vertebrate cogni- Serpell, J. A. (2003). Anthropomorphism and anthropomorphic selection:
tion. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Beyond the “cute response.” Society & Animals, 11, 83–100.
Rokach, A., & Brock, H. (1998). Coping with loneliness. Journal of Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming
Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 192, 107–127. God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic
game. Psychological Science, 18, 803–809.
Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The false consensus effect: An
egocentric bias in social perception and attributional processes. Journal Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2006). Interracial interactions: A rela-
of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279–301. tional approach. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38,
121–181.
Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1996). Naïve realism: Implications for social conflict
and misunderstanding. In T. Brown, E. Reed, & E. Turiel (Eds.), Values Shultz, T. R. (1991). From agency to intention: A rule-based computational
and knowledge (pp. 103–135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. approach. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution,
development and simulation of everyday mindreading (pp. 79–95).
Ross, M., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1985). Attribution and social perception.
London: Blackwell.
In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology
(Vol. 2, pp. 73–122). New York: Random House. Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the person
vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture
Rosset, E. (2008). It’s no accident: Our bias for intentional explanations.
theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 158–199). New York:
Cognition, 108, 771–780.
Cambridge University Press.
Ruscher, J. B., & Fiske, S. T. (1990). Interpersonal competition can cause
Sinclair, S., Huntsinger, J., Skorinko, J., & Hardin, C. D. (2005). Social
individuating processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
tuning of the self: Consequences for the self-evaluations of stereotype
58, 832–843.
targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 160–175.
Ruskin, J. (1923). Modern painters (Vol. 3). New York: E. P. Dutton.
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, J. D., & Frith, C.
Russell, B. (1948). Human knowledge: Its scope and limits. London: (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory compo-
George Allen & Unwin. nent of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162.
Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley.
Sabini, J., Cosmas, K., Siepmann, M., & Stein, J. (1999). Underestimates Slote, M. A. (1966). Induction and other minds. Review of Metaphysics,
and truly false consensus effects in estimates of embarrassment and 20, 341–360.
other emotions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 223–241.
Smith, A. (1976). The theory of moral sentiments. Oxford: Clarendon.
Sacks, O. (2003, July 28). The mind’s eye: What the blind see. New Yorker, (Original work published 1759.)
79(20), 48–59.
Snyder, M. (1974). The self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of
Savitsky, K., Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Is it as bad as we fear? Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526–537.
Overestimating the extremity of others’ judgments. Journal of
Sonnenschein, S., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1984). Developing referential com-
Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 44–56.
munication: A hierarchy of skills. Child Development, 55, 1936–1945.
Savitsky, K., & Gilovich, T. (2003). The illusion of transparency and
Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. (2004). Ideology, religion, and the evolution of
the alleviation of speech anxiety. Journal of Experimental Social
cooperation: Field tests on Israeli kibbutzim. Research in Economic
Psychology, 39, 618–625.
Anthropology, 23, 89–117.
Saxe, R. (2005). Against simulation: The argument from error. Trends in
Southgate, V., & Hamilton, A. F. (2008). Unbroken mirrors: Challenging a
Cognitive Sciences, 9, 174–179.
theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 225–229.
Saxe, R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition. Current Opinion in
Spilka, B., Hood, R., & Gorsuch, R. (1985). The psychology of religion:
Neurobiology, 16, 235–239.
An empirical approach. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Saxe, R. (2008). The happiness of the fish: Evidence for a common theory
Spitz, V. (2005). Doctors from hell: The horrific account of Nazi experi-
of one’s own and others’ actions. In K. Markman, B. Klein, & J. Suhr
ments on humans. Boulder, CO: Sentient.
(Eds.), The handbook of imagination and mental simulation. New
York: Psychology Press. Staub, E. (1989). The roots of evil: The origins of genocide and other
group violence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Saxe, R., & Powell, L. (2006). It’s the thought that counts: Specific brain
regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Science, Strack, F., Martin, L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating con-
17, 692–699. ditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback
Schober, M. F. (1993). Spatial perspective-taking in conversation. hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.
Cognition, 47, 1–24. Sunstein, C. R., & Nussbaum, M. C. (2004). Animal rights: Current
Schreiber, C. A., & Kahneman, D. (2000). Determinants of the remem- debates and new directions. New York: Oxford University Press.
bered utility of aversive sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Swim, J. K., Scott, E. D., Sechrist, G. B., Campbell, B., & Stangor, C.
General, 129, 27–42. (2003). The role of intent and harm in judgments of prejudice and

CH14.indd 539 12/22/09 3:39:53 PM


540 Mind Perception

discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, Vorauer, J. D., & Ross, M. (1999). Self-awareness and feeling transparent:
944–959. Failing to suppress one’s self. Journal of Experimental Social
Tagiuri, R. (1969). Person perception. In G. Lindsey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Psychology, 35, 415–440.
The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 395–449). Reading, Vorauer, J. D., & Sakamoto, Y. (2006). I thought we could be friends,
MA: Addison-Wesley. but . . . systematic miscommunications and defensive distancing as
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge, obstacles to cross-group friendship formation. Psychological Science,
England: Cambridge University Press. 17, 326–331.
Tanenhaus, M. K., Magnuson, J. S., Dahan, D., & Chambers, C. (2000). Vorauer, J. D., & Sasaki, S. J. (2009). Helpful only in the abstract? Ironic
Eye movements and lexical access in spoken language comprehension: effects of empathy in intergroup interaction. Psychological Science,
Evaluating a linking hypothesis between fixations and linguistic pro- 20, 191–197.
cessing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 557–580. Walster, E. (1966). Assignment of responsibility for an accident. Journal of
Tanenhaus, M., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K., & Sedivy, J. (1996). Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 73–79.
Using eye-movements to study spoken language comprehension: Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants
Evidence for visually-mediated incremental interpretation. In T. Inui & and young chimpanzees. Science, 31, 1301–1303.
J. McClelland (Eds.), Attention & performance XVI: Integration
Waxman, S. R., & Medin, D. L. (2007). Experience and cultural models
in perception and communication (pp. 457–478). Cambridge, MA:
matter: Placing firm limits on anthropocentrism. Human Development,
MIT Press.
50, 23–30.
Taylor, M., Esbensen, B. M., & Bennett, R. T. (1994). Children’s under-
Waytz, A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Epley, N. (2009). Who sees human? The
standing of knowledge acquisition: The tendency for children to
stability and importance of individual differences in anthropomor-
report they have always known what they have just learned. Child
phism. Unpublished manuscript, University of Chicago.
Development, 65, 1581–1604.
Waytz, A., Morewedge, C. K., Epley, N., Monteleone, G., Gao, J.-H., &
Thompson, L., & Hastie, R. (1990). Social perception in negotiation.
Cacioppo, J. T. (2009). Making sense by making sentient: Unpredictability
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 47, 98–123.
increases anthropomorphism. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition
Wegner, D. M. (1986). Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis
in three young chimpanzees. Monographs of the Society for Research in
of the group mind. In B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of
Child Development, 70, 1–132.
group behavior (pp. 185–208). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Umilta, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., &
Rizzolatti, G. (2001). “I know what you are doing”: A neurophysiologi- Wegner, D. M. (1995). A computer network model of human transactive
cal study. Neuron, 32, 91–101. memory. Social Cognition, 13, 1–21.

Van Boven, L. (2000). Political correctness and pluralistic ignorance: The Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA:
case of affirmative action. Political Psychology, 21, 267–276. MIT Press.

Van Boven, L., Dunning, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2000). Egocentric empa- Wegner, D. M. (2005). Who is the controller of controlled processes?
thy gaps between owners and buyers: Misperceptions of the endow- In R. Hassin, J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious
ment effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 66–76. (pp. 19–36). New York: Oxford University Press.
Van Boven, L., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). Projection of transient drive Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in social
states. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1159–1168. life. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. L. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of
social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 446–496). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Van Boven, L., Loewenstein, G., & Dunning, D. (2005). The illusion
of courage in social predictions: Underestimating the impact of fear of Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility. New York: Guilford Press.
embarrassment on other people. Organizational Behavior and Human Wellman, H. M. (1993). Early understanding of mind: The normal case.
Decision Processes, 96, 130–141. In S. Baron-Cohen & H. Tager-Flusberg (Eds.), Perspectives from
Van Kleef, G. A., Oveis, C., Van der Löwe, I., LuoKogan, A., Goetz, J., & autism (pp. 10–39). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Keltner, D. (2009). Power, distress, and compassion: Turning a blind Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of head movement on per-
eye to the suffering of others. Psychological Science, 19, 1315–1322. suasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and
Viki, G. T., Winchester, L., Titshall, L., Chisango, T., Pina, A., & Russell, R. Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230.
(2006). Beyond secondary emotions: The infrahumanization of out- Welsh, J. (2006, March 10). Why cars got angry. Wall Street Journal,
groups using words. Social Cognition, 24, 753–775. pp. W1, W10.
Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological con- Wheatley, T., Milleville, S. C., & Martin, A. (2007). Understanding ani-
sequences of money. Science, 314, 1154–1156. mate agents: Distinct roles for the social network and mirror system.
Vohs, K. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). The value of believing in free will: Psychological Science, 18, 469–474.
Encouraging a belief in determinism increases cheating. Psychological White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence.
Science, 19, 49–54. Psychological Review, 66, 297–331.
Vorauer, J. D. (2006). An information search model of evaluative concerns Wilkes-Gibbs, D., & Clark, H. H. (1992). Coordinating beliefs in conversa-
in intergroup interaction. Psychological Review, 113, 862–886. tion. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 183–194.
Vorauer, J. D., Hunter, A. J., Main, K. J., & Roy, S. (2000). Meta- Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York:
stereotype activation: Evidence from indirect measures for specific Guilford Press.
evaluative concerns experienced by members of dominant groups in
intergroup interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Wilson, D. S. (2002). Darwin’s cathedral: Evolution, religion, and the
78, 690–707. nature of society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Vorauer, J. D., Main, K. J., & O’Connell, G. B. (1998). How do individuals Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. Advances in
expect to be viewed by members of lower status groups? Content and experimental social psychology, 35, 345–411.
implications of meta-stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what
Psychology, 75, 917–937. to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 131–134.

CH14.indd 540 12/22/09 3:39:54 PM


References 541

Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T. P., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. Zaitchik, D. (1991). Is only seeing really believing? Sources of true belief
(2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. in the false belief task. Cognitive Development, 6, 91–103.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821–836. Zajonc, R. B. (1960). The process of cognitive tuning of communication.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61, 159–167.
constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understand- Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (in press). You, me, and my brain: Self
ing of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128. and other representation in social cognitive neuroscience. In
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell. A. Todorov, S. T. Fiske, & D. Prentice (Eds.), Social neuroscience:
Toward understanding the underpinnings of the social mind.
Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an New York: Oxford University Press.
actor ’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.
Zhang, Z. X., Hempel, P. S., Han, Y. L., & Tjosvold, D. (2007). Transactive
Wu, S., & Keysar, B. (2007). Cultural effects on perspective taking. memory system links team characteristics and performance. Journal
Psychological Science, 18, 600–606. of Applied Psychology, 92, 1722–1730.
Wuthnow, R., Christiano, K., & Kozlowski, J. (1980). Religion and Zuckerman, M., Kernis, M. H., Guarnera, S. M., Murphy, J. F., &
bereavement: A conceptual framework. Journal for the Scientific Rappoport, L. (1983). The egocentric bias: Seeing oneself as cause and
Study of Religion, 19, 408–430. target of others’ behavior. Journal of Personality, 51, 621–630.

CH14.indd 541 12/22/09 3:39:55 PM


Chapter 15

Judgment and Decision Making


THOMAS D. GILOVICH AND DALE W. GRIFFIN

Making judgments and decisions are the most important We believe that the connections between the fields of
things people do. People must assess adversaries and social psychology and JDM run deep and that to exclude
decide to fight, flee, or raise the white flag. They must size judgment and decision making from the study of social
up the pool of potential mates and choose one to woo— psychology would rob social psychology of much of what
or decide to stay out of the game altogether. And in the is important in everyday experience. One way to make the
modern world, they must evaluate and choose careers, cell case for a natural connection between the two fields would
phone providers, plasma versus LCD televisions, even be to list all of the topics or findings in JDM with direct
what religion to raise the kids. Decision making, defined as and powerful implications for social life. It would be a long
the choice of one path among many based on an evaluation list. We have chosen to make the case differently. We aim
of the possible outcomes, necessarily involves judgment— to clarify the substantive overlap between the two fields
the evaluation process itself—but judgment also occurs by highlighting the tremendous influence they have had
in the absence of choice. on each other, thus showing how the two fields are inex-
Given the centrality of judgment and decision making (JDM) tricably linked, to the benefit of both. More specifically,
to nearly everything that is important in life, it stands to reason after a brief history of the development of JDM, we illus-
that scholars from many disciplines have been interested in trate the deep connections between social psychology and
understanding how people go about judging and deciding. This JDM in three ways. We first discuss three important ideas
means that JDM is something of an orphan field, lacking a ded- from JDM that have shaped how social psychologists think
icated and exclusive academic home. Notable contributions to about long-standing issues in their field. We then examine
the field have been made by cognitive psychologists (Kelly & three ideas from social psychology that have had an endur-
Jacoby, 1998; Markman & Medin, 1995; Shafir, Simonson, & ing impact on the study of JDM. To further show the strong
Tversky, 1993; Sloman, 1996; Slovic & Lichtenstein, 1968; and natural links between the two fields, we then discuss
Tversky & Kahneman, 1974, 1979), social psychologists three important ideas and areas of investigation that have
(Dawes, 1988; Miller & Taylor, 1995; Nisbett & Ross, arisen independently in each field, with mutually reinforc-
1980; Wilson & Gilbert, 2003), economists (Camerer, 1990; ing effect. We end with some thoughts about the application
Loewenstein, 1987, 1996; Thaler, 1980), marketing schol- of the ideas developed in social psychology and JDM to
ars (Johnson, Hershey, Meszaros, & Kunreuther, 1993; solving some of the most pressing problems confronting
Simonson, 1989), and academics in a host of other disci- the world today.
plines (Rachlinski, 1998; Redelmeier, Koehler, Liberman, &
Tversky, 1995; Simon, 1957; Ubel, Spranca, DeKay, Hershey,
& Asch, 1998). PROLOGUE: THREE FOUNDING STORIES
But having many homes can lead to uncertain member-
ship in any one family. And, in one sign of JDM’s quasi- Before discussing the rich interconnections between JDM
orphan status, questions have been repeatedly raised about and social psychology, we look back at the origins of the
whether some of the core topics in JDM should be included field of judgment and decision making. The founders of
in social psychology textbooks and taught in social psy- modern social psychology (Kurt Lewin, Solomon Asch,
chology survey courses. Updated and put in terms more Fritz Heider, and Leon Festinger among them) often
relevant to the present volume, Why is there—or should explored such questions as how people decide whether to
there even be—a chapter on judgment and decision mak- join a group, go along with others, or change their previous
ing in the Handbook of Social Psychology? opinions. Notably, Festinger wrote a book titled Conflict,

542
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Prologue: Three Founding Stories 543

Decision, and Dissonance (1964), and Lewin authored are added together (Simon, 1947). Such organizational
an influential paper titled “Group Decision and Social behavior was strikingly at variance with the dominant eco-
Change” (1952). The study of judgment was also central to nomic models of the time (and even today), which posited
such classic social psychological topics as attitude change that organizations and humans were rational (in the eco-
and prejudice toward outgroups. When Sherif and Hovland nomic sense of making ideal, profit-maximizing decisions,
(1961) developed social judgment theory, they drew on not in the Freudian sense of being in touch with reality).
perceptual psychology and the concepts of assimilation The heart of Simon’s critique was that full economic
and contrast to explain when persuasive messages are rationality was simply an unrealizable model for human
likely to be accepted or rejected. Our opening conundrum judgment and decision making (Simon, 1955). The “ratio-
thus arises again: If theories about decision making and nal man” at the center of his critique was a 20th-century
judgment have always been intrinsic to social psychology, invention built on advances in statistics and choice theory
why do we need to chart a separate path of influence on the offered by von Neumann and Morganstern (1944) and
part of the “field” of JDM? And why was this influence on Savage (1954) and developed by Nash (1950) into mod-
social psychology most pronounced during the 1970s? ern game theory. In essence, the rational models required
Surprisingly, given the long interest in decision making the decision-maker to consider every possible action, the
among scholars in a wide range of disciplines, the field of outcome of every possible action in every possible future
JDM as we know it today is a relatively modern invention, state of the world and the probability of that state, and to
with its birth in the 1950s. The modern field is defined calculate the choice that would lead to the best outcome
by the linkage of the study of actual behavioral tendencies (and in the case of game theory, to correctly forecast how
with the specification of formal mathematical models of others would respond to each action). Simon (1957) noted
judgment and decision-making developed in more prescrip- that these theories were computationally unrealistic as
tive fields such as statistics, economics, and the philosophy either guides to or descriptions of actual human decision
of logic. It is the tension between the careful analysis of how making because they required prodigious knowledge, an
judgment and decisions ought to be made and the careful immense calculation ability that surpassed the capabilities
observation of how decisions are actually made that defines of any computer at that time, and perfect prescience on the
the modern field. Formal models of judgment and decision part of the decision-makers regarding their own (future)
making are built from a set of fundamental axioms that preferences.
represent the most basic building blocks of logical analy- Simon did not build his theory on specific psychological
sis as applied to uncertainty, valuation, and choice among principles or processes: He explicitly noted that psycholog-
alternatives. It is astonishing to realize that these axiomatic ical theories of choice processes were not yet sufficiently
models that were built up in mathematics, philosophy, and developed to inform economics. Instead, he used general
economics are largely inventions of the 20th century psychological principles to outline some broad, realistic
and were not fully presented until the 1950s. This explains constraints on rational models as models of actual decision
why JDM—as the confluence of formal models and psy- making. These general psychological principles reflected
chological description—was not born until the 1950s and the zeitgeist of cognitive psychology at the time, which
consequently only started to exert its full influence on focused on the limits of memory and attention.
social psychology in the 1970s and 1980s. Simon’s realistic constraints set the stage for the field of
JDM as we know it today. Most generally, he asserted that
people cannot—and do not want to—carry out the complex
The First Behavioral Economist
and time-consuming calculations necessary to determine
The first tale of the founding of JDM focuses on Herbert the ideal choice out of all possible actions. Instead, they
Simon, a Nobel Prize winner in economics who, paradoxi- simplify the choice process by searching for a satisfactory
cally, was one of the fiercest critics of microeconomic the- outcome (Simon, 1957). This satisficing generally consists of
ory. Simon was trained in the field of public administration three elements: a strategy that examines local or easy
and was interested in modeling how bureaucracies worked (a options before looking further afield, a stopping rule that
goal more focused on “description” than on “prescription”). specifies an aspiration level that must be met and hence
The phenomena that Simon and colleagues observed could how far afield the search should continue, and a simpli-
be described as “muddling through”—large organizations fied assessment of future value that provides a rather
seemed to operate on simple rules of thumb in an environ- vague clue as to the actual value of the choice. Another,
ment in which no one person or department knows everything less well-known side to Simon’s critique emphasized that
but somehow everyone knows just enough to produce a sat- such simplified methods of choice can do surprisingly
isfactory overall outcome when the individual contributions well relative to optimizing methods and that “bounded
544 Judgment and Decision Making

rationality” could still be evolutionarily successful (Simon, at reconciling formal models and actual behavior. Notably,
1957, 1990). he studied how gamblers used probabilities in Las Vegas and
Simon offered the field of economics two other familiar how computer programs might bridge the gap between actual
psychological insights that were to echo repeatedly in the and ideal decision making (Edwards, Lindman & Savage,
development of JDM. First, the human mind (as well as 1963). He chronicled a long list of failures of the formal mod-
the aggregate mind of the organization) can only hold on els to match actual judgment and decision making, noting
to two or three alternatives at one time. Second, attention that people reacted differently to gains and losses and seemed
is a precious and costly commodity, a fact that must be to be most responsive to comparative values rather than
considered in any description of how judgment and choice absolute values, consistent with Lewin’s expectancy-value
processes operate (Simon, 1957). Thus, in the vocabulary model of aspiration level (Lewin, Dembo, Festinger, & Sears,
later introduced by Kahneman and Tversky, Simon had 1944). However, because of his deep interest in decision
both a negative agenda (explaining how ideal, rational analysis—which was the new subdiscipline of using formal
models were unrealistic and descriptively invalid) and a tools and models to provide useful guides to decision-makers—
positive agenda (providing guidelines as to how humans— Edwards was reluctant to discard what quickly became known
and animals—might make highly sensible, if simplified, as the “classical” formal models and preferred to use them as
choices). approximations to human judgment and decision making.
Simon was thus the first acknowledged “behavioral Thus, although his dissertation began with the clear statement:
economist” who strove to incorporate psychological real- “People in gambling situations do not make choices in such
ism into economic models and explanations. The second a way as to maximize their expected winnings or minimize
founding tale of JDM focuses on the individual most their expected losses” (Edwards, 1953 p. 349), Edwards
responsible for creating information flow in the opposite continued to use the formal models throughout his career
direction, importing economic and statistical models into as the core of his explanatory frameworks and maintained
psychology. an optimistic outlook as to the perfectibility of human judg-
ment through decision aids. Famously, he and his colleagues
concluded that human judgment could be characterized as
The First Behavioral Decision Theorist
“approximately Bayesian,” implying that people largely fol-
In 1954, Ward Edwards published a review paper that lowed the rules of subjective probability (or Bayesian prob-
introduced the formal theories of decision making to ability) as defined in SEU theory (Edwards, 1968).
the broader field of psychology. He introduced the now-
formalized subjective expected utility (SEU) model of The Third Path to the Psychology
decision making, with its distinctions between objective of Judgment and Decision Making
value (e.g., money) and subjective utility and between
objective probability (e.g., the proportion of sixes expected The heuristics and biases program of research instigated
when rolling a fair die) and subjective probability (a per- by Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky has come to
sonal belief about the likelihood that a six will turn up). define JDM in many social psychology textbooks and in
Although the notion of utility had been part of econom- the minds of many social psychologists, not least because
ics at least since Bentham (1789/1948), it received a sharp of the Nobel Prize awarded to Kahneman in 2002 for his
twist in the new formal models (Kahneman, Wakker, & joint work with Tversky. Although the program grew out
Sarin, 1997). Now, instead of referring to the pleasure or of the zeitgeist created by Simon, Edwards, and many oth-
pain a person received from an outcome—that is, experi- ers, it had a radically different agenda. The program began
enced utility, as originally used by Bentham—it referred to when Tversky, a mathematical psychologist who had worked
the predicted utility associated with a given choice. with Edwards and others on formal measurement models,
In a follow-up 1961 review, Edwards coined the term described the current state of the behavioral decision theory
“behavioral decision theory” and reviewed the now- paradigm circa 1968 to Kahneman, his colleague in the
burgeoning literature on empirical tests of the founda- Psychology Department at Hebrew University in Jerusalem.
tions of SEU and related models. Edwards also introduced Kahneman found the idea of tinkering with formal models
Bayesian statistical methods to psychologists in a review such as SEU to make them fit the accumulating empirical
coauthored with a leading statistician (Edwards, Lindman, & evidence to be an unpromising approach to understanding the
Savage, 1963). psychological processes involved in judgment and choice.
In addition to providing influential reviews of formal deci- Instead, he argued, based on his own research on visual
sion models and relevant empirical evidence, Edwards and his attention and processing, the principles of cognition under-
colleagues conducted programs of empirical research aimed lying judgment should follow the principles of perception.
Prologue: Three Founding Stories 545

Thus, instead of starting with formal models as the basis (Kahneman & Tversky, 1972, p. 450; italics in original).
of descriptive accounts of judgment and decision making, Unfortunately, or so it seems to us, this statement was taken
Kahneman and Tversky started with principles of percep- by some to imply that the heuristics and bias (hu)man was
tion and psychophysics and extended them to the kind of not simply un-Bayesian but was rather stupid.
processing necessary to evaluate probabilities and assess In a second phase of their collaborative research, Kahneman
subjective values. and Tversky took the perceptual framework they had used
This approach immediately suggested a guiding para- to study probability judgment and used it to illuminate deci-
digm for research on judgment and decision-making: the sion making, leading to their most complete and formal
study of visual illusions. The logic of studying percep- model, prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). Here,
tual illusions is that failures of a system are often more fundamental perceptual principles such as comparison lev-
diagnostic of the rules the system follows than are its els and adaptation (Helson, 1964), diminishing sensitivity,
successes. Consider, for example, the moon illusion: and the privileged status of pain were the primitives of a
The full moon looks enormous as it sits on the horizon model that again used specific biases and errors as tools of
but more modestly sized when high in the sky. Little can diagnosis.
be learned from the constancy of the perceived size of the It is illuminating to compare the evolutionary impli-
moon along the long arc of the overhead sky, but its illusory cations of Simon’s bounded rationality and the heuristics
magnification when it sits on the horizon provides insight and biases approach. For Simon, the guiding evolutionary
about the way that the visual system uses contextual detail principle was computational realism (i.e., simplified
to compute perceived distance and hence perceived size. approximation) that nonetheless was well adapted to fit the
The visual illusion paradigm, like the cognitive illusion information environment. For Kahneman and Tversky,
approach patterned on it, does not imply that judgments the guiding evolutionary principle was that existing pro-
of size are typically wrong—in fact, it provides a map to cesses in perceptual analysis were co-opted as tools for
those situations in which intuitive perceptions are likely higher-level cognitive processing. Although these tools
to be correct—but it highlights the processes by which per- might work well in many environments, they also lead
ception or judgment is constructed from imperfect cues. to signature biases that are endemic to human intuition.
Thus, the resulting guiding logic in the study of judg- In many cases, the biases that to Kahneman and Tversky
ment was in practice the opposite of the approach cham- were signals of underlying heuristics were already well
pioned by Simon, who had urged researchers to seek out known. For example, Meehl and Rosen (1955) had warned
and understand the environmental factors that maximized clinicians of the danger of neglecting base rates in psycho-
the success of simple processes (Simon, 1955, 1990). The logical diagnoses. In other cases, the biases were identified
cognitive illusion paradigm seeks out those environments by informal observation, whether of psychologists who
or problem descriptions in which the judgment and choice seemed to neglect power and underestimate sample sizes,
processes people rely on lead to clear errors. The purpose Israeli army officers who neglected regression effects in
was not to emphasize the predominance of bias over accu- determining the value of rewards versus punishment, or
racy but to find the clearest testing grounds for diagnosing army selection personnel who maintained their belief in
the underlying simple processes or judgmental heuristics the efficacy of interviews despite statistical evidence to the
that people habitually employ. contrary.
The heuristics that Kahneman and Tversky identified Without these three founding stories, the field of JDM
were also suggested by the principles of perceptual psy- would look different from its appearance today, if indeed
chology, especially the organizing principles of Gestalt it existed at all. Of course, many other influences shaped
psychology (e.g., Koffka, 1935). Gestalt psychology JDM and, through it, social psychology. For example, the
emphasized how the perceptual system effortlessly and study of logical reasoning in cognitive psychology became
without awareness creates whole forms even when the infor- an important strand of JDM as errors in reasoning became
mation reaching the receptors is incomplete and indetermi- a focus of study. As we shift the focus back to social psy-
nate. According to the heuristics and biases approach—and chology, we are in a position to answer the why and when
according to the pertinent evidence—these underlying questions. JDM had its impact on social psychology starting
heuristics are not a simplified version of an ideal statisti- in the late 1960s and early 1970s for three main reasons.
cal analysis but are instead something completely differ- First, formal theories of judgment and decision making blos-
ent. This constituted a key point of differentiation between somed in the 1950s and provided a vocabulary for talking
the heuristics and biases model and the other models about and studying the processes of judgment and decision
before it: “In his evaluation of evidence, man is apparently making. Second, the work of the three founding paradigms
not a conservative Bayesian: he is not Bayesian at all” just described provided a new and stimulating way to think
546 Judgment and Decision Making

about rationality and error in human thought and behav- If so, your actions would reflect a common tendency in
ior. Third, the methodology of the heuristics and biases inductive reasoning typically referred to as a confirmation
framework—simple pencil-and-paper demonstrations of bias (Beyth-Marom & Fischhoff, 1983; Crocker, 1982;
judgment and decision errors—was easily extended to the Klayman & Ha, 1987; Mynatt, Doherty, & Tweney, 1977;
investigation of issues central to social psychology, espe- Skov & Sherman, 1986). When evaluating a proposition
cially the cognitive social psychology of the 1970s. (hostas need a lot of water, happy people live longer, or
Japanese Americans are more self-critical than European
Americans), we more readily, reliably, and reflexively look
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY’S DEBT TO
for evidence that would support the proposition than for
JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING
evidence that would contradict it. The nomenclature is
important but potentially misleading. At times, we want
If the field of JDM never existed, social psychology would
a given proposition to be true, so we energetically, and
be different in several ways from what it is now. Social
not disinterestedly, sift through the pertinent evidence in
psychologists would have to reach for different terms to
an effort to uncover information that confirms its validity
characterize the phenomena they study if their field’s acces-
(Dawson, Gilovich, & Regan, 2002; Ditto & Lopez, 1992;
sible lexicon did not include the JDM terms base rates,
Ditto, Scepansky, Munro, Apanovich, & Lockhart, 1998;
counterfactuals, heuristics, illusory correlation, “a proper
Gilovich, 1983, 1991; Hsee, 1995, 1996; Kruglanski &
Bayesian,” and countless others. And the acknowledged
Webster, 1996; Kunda, 1990; Lord, Ross, & Lepper, 1979;
importance of such things as heuristics, counterfactuals, and
Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1987). But that is not the case
illusory correlations also means that social psychologists
in most investigations of the confirmation bias. The par-
must now confront journal editors and reviewers armed
ticipants have no stake in the outcome and are not trying to
with additional alternative interpretations of experimental
confirm the proposition they are testing. But they neverthe-
results. Social psychologists must now show that their find-
less tend to look for and examine information that would
ings are not “just” the reflection of a particular heuristic,
fit the proposition being tested more than information that
a possible counterfactual comparison, or a quasi-Bayesian
would contradict it. Partly to try to head off any confusion
analysis. Even the need to rule out regression artifacts—an
between these two strategies, some have suggested the
important caution in social psychology methods courses
term positive test strategy for the disinterested form of this
long before the field’s connection to JDM—would likely
tendency (Klayman & Ha, 1987).
not be as accessible, and hence raised as often, if the regres-
Interest in the confirmation bias among JDM research-
sion fallacy and the psychological mechanisms that give
ers was sparked by a pair of experimental paradigms pio-
rise to it were not such prominent topics in JDM.
neered by Peter Wason. In the first, participants were told
To illustrate how much JDM has influenced what the
that the experimenter had in mind a rule specifying accept-
field of social psychology looks like, how social psycholo-
able sets of three integers and that one acceptable set was
gists think, and how they conduct their research—to high-
2 4 6. The participants were to generate their own sets of
light the debt that social psychology owes to JDM—we
three integers and the experimenter would indicate whether
discuss three particularly important ideas developed in
each one satisfied the rule. Participants were allowed to
JDM that have found their way to social psychology.
record their sets and the experimenter ’s response, and they
In particular, we discuss how the notion of confirmation
were to tell the experimenter what they thought the rule
bias, the concept of heuristics, and the application of nor-
was once they were sure they had figured it out.
mative theories have largely originated in the field of JDM
Performance on the task was not impressive, with an
and come to influence theoretical development and empiri-
apparent confirmation bias getting in participants’ way
cal research in social psychology.
(Wason, 1960). That is, participants tended to generate
only positive instances of the rules they were entertaining,
Confirmation Bias
which, given the rule Wason had in mind (any ascending
Suppose a friend gives you a bunch of hostas from her gar- sequence), made it virtually impossible for them to be dis-
den for you to plant in yours. “I don’t really know what abused of their hypotheses. For example, someone with
I’m talking about,” she adds, “but I suspect they need a lot the hypothesis “equally spaced ascending integers” would
of water. You might want to test that out.” How would you tend to offer sets such as 10 15 20 or 25 50 75. This
conduct your test? If you are like most people, you would would result in consistent feedback that the set fit the rule,
plant them, give them a lot of water, and see how they fare. increased confidence on the part of participants that they had
What you would not do is give a lot of water to some, give figured it out, and then consternation when they learned that
little to the others, and compare the results. their assessment was incorrect. The tendency to construct
Social Psychology’s Debt to Judgment and Decision Making 547

positive tests of their hypotheses, in other words, made it problem: “If a person is drinking beer in a bar, then he or
difficult for them to discern that they were not on the right she is 21 or older.” Most people find this problem easy.
track when a negative test would readily have done so (the If shown the four statements “drinking beer,” “drinking
experimenter would have said, for example, that 10 11 15 soda,” “22 years old,” and “16 years old,” they quickly and
fit the rule, making it clear that “equally spaced ascending reliably select “drinking beer” and “16 years old”—here, in
integers” was not on the mark). other words, they quite naturally look for disconfirmatory
The second paradigm, one that has inspired hundreds of evidence (Griggs & Cox, 1982). Evolutionary psycholo-
replications, has come to be known as the Wason selection gists have interpreted this result as evidence of an innate
task. In the usual variant, participants are shown four cards module dedicated to reasoning about social contracts and
(often just pictures of four cards on a piece of paper or the detection of “cheaters” (Cosmides, 1989; Pinker, 1997;
computer screen), each said to have a number on one side Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). This claim has been criticized
and a letter on the other. The participant’s task is to specify on several grounds (Cheng & Holyoak, 1989; Evans &
which cards need to be turned over to determine whether Over, 1996; Fodor, 2000; Sperber, Cara, & Girotto, 1995;
a given rule is valid—say, “All cards with a vowel on one Sperber & Girotto, 2002), and without independent sub-
side have an even number on the other.” In this case, par- stantiating evidence, it rests on an invalid chain of logic.
ticipants might be shown cards with A, B, 2, and 3 face up That is, the claim that enhanced performance on social-
(Wason & Johnson-Laird, 1972). contract versions of the selection task stems from an
What Wason and many others have found is that a con- evolved, domain-specific module is essentially a claim that
siderable majority of participants state that either the A card the following syllogism is valid:
has to be turned over or the A card and 2 card (Evans, 2007;
Evans, Newstead, & Byrne, 1993; Wason & Johnson-Laird, Performance on most versions of the Wason selection
1972). The latter response appears to reflect the use of a task is poor.
positive test strategy. The rule says that all vowels have an Performance on social-contract versions of the task is
even number on the other side, so one looks at the vowel quite good.
and even-number cards to see whether evidence to support
the rule is obtained. Here, too, people do not tend to pur- Therefore, the ability to reason through social-contract
sue a disconfirmatory strategy that would allow them to versions is handled by a domain-specific module evolved
adequately assess the rule—they do not turn over the through natural selection.
potentially decisive 3 card (which, if it had a vowel on Exactly why deontic versions of the selection task are
the other side, would invalidate the rule). typically easier remains an issue of great interest and con-
troversy. Many argue for an evolutionary account (Fiddick,
Content and Domain Influences on
Spampinato, & Grafman, 2005; Stone, Cosmides, Tooby,
Hypothesis Testing
Kroll, & Knight, 2002), whereas others maintain that it
As one would expect, performance on the Wason selection stems from differential cuing (Sperber & Girotto, 2002) or
task is improved if the rule to be evaluated is more engag- the subtly different goals that are evoked by deontic and
ing than “every card with a vowel on one side has an even indicative versions of the selection task (Evans & Over,
number on the other” (Cheng & Holyoak, 1985; Cox & 1996; Manktelow & Over, 1991; Oaksford & Chater,
Griggs, 1982; Holyoak & Cheng, 1995; Johnson-Laird, 1994).
Legrenzi, & Legrenzi, 1972; Wason & Shapiro, 1971;
Yachanin & Tweney, 1982). But it is not a case of simply
Hypothesis Testing in Social Life
making the content of the task more like the real world.
Instead, there appear to be two broad classes of selection Whatever the cause of the pronounced content effects on
tasks, indicative and deontic, and thematic content tends to performance on selection tasks, social psychologists were
have a bigger facilitory effect on the latter (Evans, 2007; quick to see the relevance of the confirmation bias for every-
Manktelow & Over, 1991). Indicative rules refer simply day social life. In one often-cited study, Snyder and Swann
to empirical regularities—for example, all individuals with (1978) asked half of their participants to interview a target
red hair have freckles. Deontic rules involve permissions individual and ascertain whether that person was an extra-
or obligations—for example, everyone riding in a car must vert. The remaining participants were to ascertain whether the
wear a seat belt. target was an introvert. Participants selected their interview
People are more likely to select the equivalent of the questions from a list provided. Those charged with ascertain-
3 card for deontic rules rich in content. The best known ing whether the target was an extravert tended to ask ques-
and most widely cited example is the drinking age tions that focused on sociability (“In what situations are you
548 Judgment and Decision Making

most talkative?”), whereas those charged with ascertaining 100 meters. What Mussweiler and colleagues found was
whether the target was an introvert tended to ask questions that participants primed with moderate exemplars looked
that focused on social withdrawal (“In what situations do you for similarities between themselves and the targets, result-
wish you could be more outgoing?”). In an important wrin- ing in assimilation. Those primed with Lauda thought they
kle that shows the powerful implications of relying on such were stronger and faster than those primed with Clinton.
an information-search strategy, Snyder and Swann tape- Those primed with extreme exemplars, in contrast, looked
recorded the interview sessions, edited out the questions for dissimilarities between themselves and the targets,
so that only the responses remained, and then played the resulting in contrast. Those primed with Jordan thought
responses to another, uninformed set of participants. These they were weaker and slower than those primed with the
latter participants rated those who had been interviewed pope. Further studies have shown that these effects are
by someone testing whether they were extraverted as more the product of the enhanced accessibility of target-consis-
outgoing than those who had been interviewed by some- tent self-knowledge under conditions that foster similarity
one testing whether they were introverted. testing and target-inconsistent self-knowledge under con-
This work and much that followed showed how a ditions that foster dissimilarity testing (Dijksterhuis et al.,
positive test strategy can elicit behavior that confirms, 1998; Mussweiler & Bodenhausen, 2002; Mussweiler
erroneously, the very hypothesis being tested. Because et al., 2004).
people tend to be agreeable and hence somewhat acqui-
escent in most social interactions, they tend to respond in Heuristics
ways that accept the thrust of the questions they are asked
(Zuckerman, Knee, Hodgins, & Miyake, 1995). Extending If we were to identify the most important event that ties
this idea further, social psychologists were particularly together the fields of social psychology and JDM, it
interested in exploring how these processes can cement and would surely be Tversky and Kahneman’s short (seven-
exacerbate erroneous stereotypes. Entertaining the possi- page) paper in Science in 1974. In that paper, Tversky
bility that members of a particular group might conform to and Kahneman described three heuristics—anchoring,
a prevailing stereotype can lead people to behave toward availability, and representativeness—that influence judg-
them in ways that elicit stereotype-consistent behavior ment in a stunningly wide range of areas and that provide
(Hebl, Foster, Mannix, & Dovidio, 2002; Word, Zanna, & a unifying explanation of otherwise-isolated phenom-
Cooper, 1974). ena. In that paper, heuristics were described as “rules of
The idea of a confirmation bias has also been invoked thumb” that provide serviceable, but imperfect, answers to
to advance social psychologists’ understanding of a topic difficult judgment problems. They were contrasted with
of long-standing interest to the field: social comparison. algorithms, which are more labor intensive but yield pre-
Mussweiler (2003) argues that social comparison involves cise and perfectly accurate answers. For example, we can
a two-step process. First, a person makes a quick, holis- determine the number of people in a lecture hall by exhaus-
tic assessment of whether someone is similar to or dif- tively counting each person in attendance (the algorithmic
ferent from a comparison target. For example, a person solution), or we can simply estimate the average number
might make a snap implicit judgment that a target is differ- of people in a representative row and multiply by the esti-
ent from someone who is the opposite gender, a different mated number of rows (the heuristic solution). The heuris-
race, or thought of as an extreme exemplar (Albert Einstein, tic in this example is a narrow one that applies only to the
Angelina Jolie, LeBron James). Second, we tend to look special purpose of estimating attendance in an auditorium.
for similarities between self and similar targets but dissimi- One of the reasons that Tversky and Kahneman’s work had
larities between self and dissimilar targets—that is, to seek such impact is that they identified several general-purpose
out confirmatory information—with predictable effects on heuristics that apply to all sorts of judgments.
self-assessment. From the moment these three heuristics were intro-
In one notable study in support of this model, Mussweiler, duced, most readers tended to think of them as serving
Ruter, and Epstude (2004) had participants think about an effort-saving role, an interpretation that helped inspire
their own athletic ability while being subliminally primed and give shape to early “dual process” theories in social
with the names of individuals regarded as extremely high psychology (described later), such as the elaboration like-
or low in athleticism (basketball star Michael Jordan and lihood (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986) and heuristic–system-
Pope John Paul II) or moderately high or low in athleti- atic (Chaiken, 1980) models of persuasion. According
cism (racecar driver Niki Lauda and former President Bill to these models, a motivated person is likely to respond to
Clinton). Participants were then asked to estimate how a persuasive message by carefully processing all informa-
many pushups they could do and how fast they could run tion it contains and its implications and hence is likely to
Social Psychology’s Debt to Judgment and Decision Making 549

be influenced (or not) by the soundness of the arguments Kahneman and Tversky (1973) first demonstrated this
presented. A less motivated person is inclined to give the in a series of classic experiments. In one, participants were
message minimal attention and hence is likely to be influ- asked whether there are more words that begin with the
enced by such superficial features as the attractiveness of letter r or that have r as the third letter. Because it is easier
the communicator or the number (rather than the quality) to generate words that start with r (red, rabid, ratatouille,
of the arguments presented. etc.) than words that have an r in the third position (Huron,
Later refinements of dual-process accounts of cognition herald, unreasonable, etc.), most participants thought there
postulate that the two processes or systems of thought oper- were more of the former than the latter. In reality, there are
ate in less of an either–or fashion (Epstein, 1991; Evans, three times as many words with an r in the third position.
2007; Gilbert, 1999; Kahneman & Frederick, 2002; Sloman, Ross and Sicoly (1979) explored the implications of
1996; Stanovich, 1999; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). These the availability heuristic for everyday social life. They
later models stipulate that the two systems operate in paral- asked couples to specify their own percentage contribu-
lel, with the relative impact of each determined by the extent tion to various tasks and outcomes that come with living
to which the characteristics of the problem at hand activate together—keeping the house clean, maintaining the social
the different cognitive processes that constitute the two calendar, starting arguments, and so on. They predicted
systems. Heuristics, from this revised perspective, are auto- that each person’s contributions would be more salient
matic computations made by the reflexive, associative sys- than their partner ’s contributions and thus that both part-
tem (often referred to as system 1) that either powerfully ners would overestimate their own role. And that is just
influence the more reflective, rule-based analyses (known what participants did. When the estimates made by each
as system 2) or that are simply taken as acceptable answers member of a couple were summed, they tended to exceed
to the judgment problem at hand (Kahneman, 2003; the logical maximum of 100%. This was true, notably, for
Kahneman & Frederick, 2002). When reading a letter of negative actions (e.g., starting fights), as well as for posi-
recommendation, for example, one automatically assesses tive actions—evidence that the availability heuristic, not
the similarity between the candidate being described and the self-enhancing motivations, is responsible for this effect.
various prototypes of individuals who have previously Norbert Schwarz and colleagues have shown how the
occupied the position (“jack of all trades,” “brilliant but availability heuristic can influence people’s self-assess-
sloppy,” or “a grinder”). The similarity to the prototype ments. In so doing, they settled an important concep-
then either strongly influences a more reflective assessment tual issue that lies at the core of the availability heuristic
of the applicant’s likely success or entirely substitutes for (Schwarz & Vaughn, 2002; Schwarz et al., 1991; see also
such an assessment (“that type never works out here”). Gabrielcik & Fazio, 1984). Recall that people are assumed
Tversky and Kahneman initially identified the three to use the ease with which they can come up with instances
heuristics of availability, representativeness, and anchor- of a given category when making judgments about the
ing and adjustment. We therefore focus our review of heu- category. But note that if instances are easy to generate,
ristics on these three, and then briefly discuss additional people will probably come up with a lot of them. So how
heuristics that have been proposed since. can we be sure that people are influenced by the ease with
which they generate instances (a metacognitive feature)
Availability rather than the number of instances they generate (a cogni-
Given that there are a lot of Jewish comedians, you can tive feature)? Typically, we cannot be sure. What Schwarz
probably think of particular examples readily. There is and colleagues did was to disentangle these two, usually
merit, then, in turning this around and concluding that if intertwined features. In one representative experiment, they
you have an easy time thinking of Jewish comedians, there asked half their participants to think of times they had been
probably are a lot of them. The logic is generally sound, assertive and the other half to think of times they had
and it constitutes the essence of the availability heuristic, been unassertive. Some participants in each group were
or the tendency to use the ease with which one can generate asked to think of 6 examples, and the others were asked to
examples as a cue to category size or likelihood. But the think of 12 examples. The required number of instances, 6
“probably” in this inference is important. There can be other and 12, were carefully chosen so that thinking of 6 examples
reasons why examples of a given category are easy or hard would be easy but thinking of 12 would be a challenge.
to generate, so availability is not always a reliable guide to This manipulation separates ease of generation (pro-
actual frequency or probability (Folkes, 1988; Kahneman & cess) from number of examples generated (content). Those
Tversky, 1973; Macleod & Campbell, 1992; Oppenheimer, asked to think of 12 examples of their assertiveness (or
2004; Rothbart, Fulero, Jensen, Howard, & Birrell, 1978; unassertiveness) will think of more examples than those
Tversky & Kahneman, 1983). asked to think of 6, but they will have a harder time doing
550 Judgment and Decision Making

so. What Schwarz and colleagues found was that those thought of having an ulcer to “come to mind” when one
asked to think of 6 examples of their past assertiveness has just been presented the words “what are the chances
later rated themselves as more assertive than those asked that you will someday have an ulcer?” And what exactly
to think of 12 examples. The same pattern held for those are the “relevant instances” that easily (or not) come to
asked to think of past examples of unassertiveness. Thus, mind when one is asked to estimate the likelihood of hav-
it is the ease with which people can recall examples, not ing an ulcer?
the number of examples recalled, that dominates people’s The point here is that the very question about likelihood
judgments. The effect was so strong that those asked to puts the target event at the forefront of one’s mind regard-
come up with 12 examples of their own unassertiveness less of whether one had earlier imagined it. But thinking of
(and who thus had lots of examples of their failure to be the target event is nonetheless likely to have a different feel
assertive on the top of their heads) rated themselves as if one had, in fact, mentally tried it on earlier. It is likely to
more assertive than those asked to come up with 12 exam- feel more “fluent.” Fluency refers to the experience of ease
ples of assertiveness (and who thus had lots of examples of or difficulty associated with information processing. A clear
their past assertiveness on the top of their heads). Schwarz image is easy to process, and fluent. A phonemically irreg-
and colleagues have shown that the metacognitive experi- ular word is hard to process, and disfluent. People use the
ence of the ease with which people can access pertinent metacognitive experience of fluency as a cue when making
evidence affects their judgments of their own vulnerability inferences about all sorts of judgments (Jacoby & Dallas,
to sexual assault and heart disease (Grayson & Schwarz, 1981; Oppenheimer, 2008). People judge fluent names to
1999; Rothman & Schwarz, 1998), the quality of their be more famous (Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989), flu-
memory (Winkielman, Schwarz, & Belli, 1998), and the ent objects to be better category members (Whittlesea &
pleasantness of their childhood (Winkielman & Schwarz, Leboe, 2000), and adages that rhyme to be more valid
2001). Other investigators have shown that this instance- than those that do not (McGlone & Tofighbakhsh, 2000).
listing procedure influences people’s estimates of their past In addition to these direct effects on judgment, flu-
behavior (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 1999), their assessments ency appears to influence how people process relevant
of how much their hometowns and high-school friends information. In many respects, the feeling of fluency or
have changed (Eibach, Libby, & Gilovich, 2003), and their disfluency has the same effects as being in a good or bad
attitudes toward proposed policy changes (Brinol, Petty, & mood (see the mood effects on judgment described later).
Tormala, 2006). A feeling of disfluency while processing information
In a wry application of this paradigm, Craig Fox had appears to undermine people’s confidence in what they
students list either 2 or 10 ways a course could be improved are doing, leading to something of a “go slow, be care-
as part of the standard end-of-the-term course evaluation ful” approach to judgment and decision making. Thus,
process (Fox, 2006). Students asked to list 10 possible people are more likely to choose a default option when
improvements apparently had difficulty doing so because choosing among consumer products that are made dis-
they rated the course significantly more favorably (median fluent (Novemsky, Dhar, Schwarz, & Simonson, 2007).
of 5.5 on a 7.0-point scale) than students asked to list 2 The shift to more cautious information processing was
ways to improve (median of 5.0). shown even more directly in a study by Alter and col-
leagues (2007), who gave participants the cognitive
Availability’s Close Cousin: Fluency The mere act of reflection task (Frederick, 2005) in either a normal or a
imagining an outcome can make it seem more likely to degraded font. The cognitive reflection task requires sti-
occur. Imagining one candidate winning an election makes fling an immediate gut reaction to arrive at the correct
it seem more likely that that candidate will triumph (Carroll, answer to each question. For example, participants are
1978), and imagining what it would be like to have a disease given the question, “A bat and ball cost $1.10 in total.
makes it seem that one is more at risk of getting it (Sherman, The bat costs $1 more than the ball. How much does the
Cialdini, Schwartzman, & Reynolds, 1985). This effect was ball cost?” The participants must think beyond the imme-
originally interpreted as the result of availability. Imagining diate response of 10 cents to arrive at the correct response
the event made it more cognitively available; hence, it was of 5 cents. Alter and colleagues found that participants
judged more likely. answered more questions correctly when the questions
But is availability really the culprit? After all, the con- were presented in a degraded, and hence disfluent, font
cept of, say, getting an ulcer is made highly available— (Alter, Oppenheimer, Epley, & Eyre, 2007).
perhaps maximally available—by the very question that Fluency also appears to influence the level of abstrac-
elicits the judgment of likelihood: whether one had earlier tion at which information is encoded. As described later in
imagined having an ulcer or not. How hard can it be for the our discussion of temporal construal theory (Trope
Social Psychology’s Debt to Judgment and Decision Making 551

& Liberman, 2003), the same event (taking a test) can to show that the heuristic leads people to make judgments
be construed relatively concretely (answering questions) that violate clear normative standards. Judging whether a
or abstractly (assessing aptitude), and it has been shown sample is likely to have come from a particular generating
that physically distant objects tend to be construed more process by assessing the similarity between the two, for
abstractly (Fujita, Henderson, Eng, Trope, & Liberman, example, has been shown to give rise to a “law of small
2006) than close objects. Given that blurry (disfluent) numbers,” or a tendency to believe, contrary to probability
objects tend to appear to be farther away than distinct objects theory, that even small samples should be representative of
(Tversky & Kahneman, 1974), we might expect disflu- the populations from which they are drawn (which is true
ent entities more generally to appear relatively far away. of large samples and is captured in the law of large num-
Indeed, Alter and Oppenheimer (2008) found that cities are bers). The belief in a law of small numbers has been estab-
judged to be farther away when their names are presented lished by studies showing that people (including expert
in a difficult-to-read font. To link this finding to construal statisticians and psychologists) are excessively confident
level, they then examined archival records of an online about the replicability of research findings (Tversky &
Balderdash tournament, in which players make up bogus Kahneman, 1971), have difficulty recognizing or generat-
definitions for obscure words. They found that participants ing random sequences (Falk & Konold, 1997; Gilovich,
provided more abstract definitions of more phonemically Vallone, & Tversky, 1985; Wagenaar, 1972), and are
complex, or disfluent, words. overly influenced by the relative proportion of successes
and failures, and insufficiently influenced by sample size,
Representativeness in assessments of how confident they can be in a particular
Jenna Jones, nutrition program manager for Cornell hypothesis (Griffin & Tversky, 1993).
Cooperative Extension (a component of Cornell’s land- The work on representativeness that garnered the most
grant mission to disseminate information to the public), attention and sparked the greatest controversy, however,
informs readers of her column that a tomato “has four involved experiments demonstrating that the allure of rep-
chambers and is red” and that eating tomatoes is good for resentativeness can prevent people from using base rates
the heart; a walnut “looks like a little brain” and “we now or basic set-inclusion principles when making predictions.
know that walnuts help develop more than three dozen In one now-classic study (Kahneman & Tversky, 1973),
neuron-transmitters (sic) for brain function”; and kidney participants were given the following description of an
beans assist with the healthy functioning of their organ name- individual enrolled in graduate school:
sake (Jones, 2008). Jones’s advice to her readers appears to
be heavily influenced by a second heuristic identified by Tom W. is of high intelligence, although lacking in true cre-
Kahneman and Tversky: representativeness. ativity. He has a need for order and clarity, and for neat and
tidy systems in which every detail finds its appropriate place.
Making judgments on the basis of representative-
His writing is rather dull and mechanical, occasionally enliv-
ness reflects the mind’s tendency to automatically assess
ened by somewhat corny puns and by flashes of imagination
the similarity between two entities under consideration of the sci-fi type. He has a strong drive for competence. He
and to use that assessment as input to a judgment about seems to have little feel and little sympathy for other people
likelihood. Judgments about the likelihood of an object and does not enjoy interacting with others. Self-centered,
belonging to a category are powerfully influenced by how he nonetheless has a deep moral sense. (p. 238)
similar the object is to the category prototype (Kahneman &
Tversky, 1972, 1973; Tversky & Kahneman, 1983). One group of participants was asked to rank nine disci-
Judgments of the likelihood that an outcome stems from a plines in terms of how closely Tom resembled the typical
particular cause are powerfully influenced by the similar- student in that field. A second group ranked them in terms
ity between putative cause and observed effect (Gilovich & of the likelihood that Tom was actually enrolled in each. A
Savitsky, 2002; Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Judgments about third group simply estimated the percentage of all graduate
the likelihood of obtaining a given result are powerfully students in the United States who were enrolled in each dis-
influenced by the similarity between the features of the cipline. There were two critical findings. First, the rankings
imagined result and those of the processes thought to be of the likelihood that Tom actually studied each of the disci-
at work (Kahneman & Tversky, 1972, 1973; Tversky & plines were virtually identical to the rankings of how similar
Kahneman, 1971). he seemed to the typical student in each field. Participants’
The most compelling way to demonstrate that judg- assessments of likelihood, in other words, were powerfully
ments are “powerfully” influenced by a hypothesized pro- influence by representativeness. Second, the rankings of like-
cess is to show that they are excessively influenced. Much lihood did not correspond with what the participants knew
of the research on representativeness has therefore sought about the popularity of the different disciplines. Information
552 Judgment and Decision Making

about the base rate, or the a priori likelihood of Tom being a Linda, the feminist bank teller; we present the results for
student in each of the fields, was simply ignored. Bill to bring him out from Linda’s long shadow.)
Experiments like this sparked a long-running con- Because the conjunction fallacy violates one of the most
troversy about whether and when people are likely to basic rules of probability theory, Tversky and Kahneman
ignore or underutilize base rates (Cosmides & Tooby, (1983) anticipated controversy and provided a wide-rang-
1996; Gavanski & Hui, 1992; Gigerenzer, 1991; Griffin & ing discussion of alternative interpretations. They included
Buehler, 1999; Koehler, 1996). The controversy was pro- additional controls for the possibility that respondents
ductive, as it yielded such findings as people are more misunderstood the words “and” or “or”; they made sure
likely to use base rate information if it is presented after that the same effects occurred with frequencies, as well as
the information about the individual (Krosnick, Li, & probabilities, and that the effect applied when reasoning
Lehman, 1990), if the base rate is physically instantiated about heart attacks, not just when reasoning about person-
in a sampling paradigm (Gigerenzer, Hell, & Blank, 1988, ality descriptions; and they made sure that the same effects
but see Poulton, 1994, p. 153), and if the base rate is caus- were obtained with political forecasters as with students
ally related to the to-be-predicted event (Ajzen, 1977; from Stanford University and the University of British
Tversky & Kahneman, 1982). But in an important respect Columbia. Nonetheless, the anticipated controversy
the controversy was misguided because the essential idea ensued, centering on participants’ interpretation of the con-
being put forward was that people’s judgments are power- junction (e.g., Mellers, Hertwig, & Kahneman, 2001), the
fully influenced by representativeness, not that people can- effects of frequency versus probability response formats
not, or typically do not, use base rates. Instead, the Tom W. (Hertwig & Gigerenzer, 1999), and the limits of laboratory
studies and others like it were existence proofs. They research.
showed that, however often or intelligently people might
use base rates in their everyday and professional lives, the Anchoring
allure of representativeness is so strong that it can blind Suppose someone asks you how long it takes Venus to orbit
them to the relevance of information they would otherwise the sun. You reply that you do not know (few people do),
use quite readily. In fact, the more people typically use but your interrogator then asks for an estimate. How do
base rates, the stronger the demonstration: Showing that you respond? You might think to yourself that Venus is
representativeness leads people to ignore information they closer than the earth to the sun and therefore that it proba-
are too keen to ignore anyway is not impressive; show- bly takes fewer than the 365 days it takes the earth to make
ing that it leads them to ignore information they typically its orbit. You might then move down from that value of
embrace is. 365 days and estimate that a year on Venus consists of, say,
Every bit as controversial as the work on representa- 275 days. (The correct answer is 224.7.)
tiveness and base rates were Tversky and Kahneman’s To respond in this way is to use what Tversky and
(1983) demonstrations that the allure of representativeness Kahneman referred to as the anchoring and adjustment
could lead people to commit the “conjunction fallacy” and heuristic (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). One starts with a
end up judging that the conjunction of two events is more salient or convenient value and adjusts to an estimate that
likely than either of the two events alone. For example, seems right. The most notable feature of such adjustments
participants in one study were given the following descrip- is that they tend to be insufficient. In most investigations
tion of an individual: of such “anchoring effects,” the investigators take care to
ensure that the respondents know that the anchor value is
Bill is 34 years old. He is intelligent but unimaginative, com- entirely arbitrary and therefore carries no implication about
pulsive, and generally lifeless. In school, he was strong in math- what the right value might be. In the initial demonstration,
ematics but weak in social studies and humanities. (p. 297) Tversky and Kahneman (1974) spun a “wheel of fortune”
device and then asked participants whether the percent-
They were then asked to rank the likelihood of eight age of African countries in the United Nations is higher
possible life outcomes for Bill, including (1) Bill is an or lower than the number that came up. After participants
accountant, (2) Bill plays jazz for a hobby, and (3) Bill is indicated whether they thought it was higher or lower, they
an accountant who plays jazz for a hobby. Of the respon- were asked to estimate the actual percentage of African
dents, 92% assigned a higher rank to outcome 3 than countries in the United Nations. What they found was
to outcome 2, even though any state of the world that satis- that the transparently arbitrary anchor value significantly
fies outcome 3 automatically satisfies outcome 2 and thus influenced participants’ responses. Those who confronted
outcome 3 cannot be more likely than outcome 2. (The larger numbers from the wheel of fortune gave signifi-
results were the same for Bill’s better-known counterpart cantly higher estimates than those who confronted lower
Social Psychology’s Debt to Judgment and Decision Making 553

numbers. Anchoring effects using paradigms like this have people evaluate hypotheses by attempting to confirm them
been observed in people’s evaluation of gambles (Carlson, (Evans, 2007; Snyder & Swann, 1978; Skov & Sherman,
1990; Chapman & Johnson, 1999), estimates of risk and 1986), such a search generates evidence disproportionately
uncertainty (Plous, 1989; Wright & Anderson, 1989), per- consistent with the anchor. Mussweiler and Strack (2000)
ceptions of self-efficacy (Cervone & Peake, 1986), antici- provide support for their analysis by showing that informa-
pations of future performance (Switzer & Sniezek, 1991), tion consistent with the anchor value presented to partici-
and answers to general knowledge questions (Jacowitz & pants is indeed disproportionately accessible. For example,
Kahneman, 1995; Mussweiler & Strack, 1999, 2000; participants who were asked whether the price of an average
Strack & Mussweiler, 1997). German car is higher or lower than a high value were subse-
As the research on anchoring evolved, comparable quently quick to recognize words associated with expensive
effects using all sorts of other paradigms have been cars (Mercedes, BMW); those asked whether the price of an
observed and it appears that such effects are not always average German car is higher or lower than a modest value
the result of insufficient adjustment. Indeed, probably the were subsequently quick to recognize words associated with
fairest reading of the anchoring literature is that there is less expensive cars (Volkswagen, Golf ).
not one anchoring effect produced by insufficient adjust- Oppenheimer, LeBoeuf, and Brewer (2008) have
ment but, rather, a family of anchoring effects produced by recently shown that the semantic activation elicited by dif-
at least three distinct types of psychological processes ferent anchors can be quite general. They asked one group
(Epley, 2004). Epley and Gilovich (2001, 2004, 2005, of participants whether the Mississippi River was longer
2006) provided evidence that people do indeed adjust or shorter than 4,800 miles and another group whether it
insufficiently from at least some anchor values, par- was longer or shorter than 15 miles. They then asked their
ticularly those that people generate themselves (like the participants to draw a line equal to the length of a standard
question about Venus given earlier). They have found, toothpick. Those exposed to the high initial anchor drew
for example, that people articulate a process of adjusting longer toothpicks than those exposed to the low initial
from self-generated anchors and that manipulations that anchor. This suggests that exposure to the initial anchor
should influence adjustment, but not other potential causes activated the general concept of “long” or “short,” which
of anchoring, have a significant effect on people’s judg- influenced their representation (and production) of a stan-
ments. In particular, people who are incidentally nodding dard toothpick. To test this idea, Oppenheimer and col-
their heads while answering, are cognitively busy, or lack leagues had participants in a follow-up experiment perform
incentives for accurate responding tend to be more influ- a word completion task after being exposed to high or low
enced by self-generated anchor values than those who are anchor values. Participants exposed to the high anchors
incidentally shaking their heads, are not busy, or are given were more likely to form words connoting bigness (“big”
incentives for accuracy. for “b_g”, “long” for “_ong”) than were those exposed to
Manipulations such as these, however, have generally the low anchors.
been shown to have no effect on participants’ responses in Recent research suggests that there is likely a third source
the standard (experimenter-generated) anchoring paradigm of anchoring effects—pure numerical priming. That is, an
pioneered by Tversky and Kahneman (Chapman & Johnson, anchor activates its own numerical value and those close to
1999; Epley & Gilovich, 2001, 2005; Tversky & Kahneman, it, which are then highly accessible and influential when the
1974; Wilson, Houston, Etling, & Brekke, 1996). At first person tries to fashion a response. In one notable experiment,
glance, this is a bit of a puzzle because it raises the question participants were asked whether the runway at Hong Kong
of why, without insufficient adjustment, anchoring effects International Airport was longer or shorter than 7.3 kilome-
would occur. Why, if not because of insufficient adjustment, ters or 7,300 meters and were then asked to estimate the cost
do people’s estimates tend to assimilate toward anchor val- of an unrelated project. Those asked the question in terms of
ues presented to their attention? This question has been meters gave higher estimates on the second, unrelated task
addressed most extensively by Thomas Mussweiler and than those asked the question in terms of kilometers—
Fritz Strack (Mussweiler, 2002; Mussweiler & Strack, 1999, presumably because the latter primed smaller absolute num-
2000; Strack & Mussweiler, 1997). They maintain that most bers (Wong & Kwong, 2000). Although some have argued
anchoring effects are the result of the enhanced accessibility otherwise, this does not appear to be the result of the differ-
of anchor-consistent information. The attempt to answer the ential accessibility of semantic information consistent with
initial question put to them by the investigator—“Is the initial anchor because 7.3 kilometers and 7,300 meters
the Nile longer or shorter than 5,000 [or 800] miles?”— represent the same value, just in different units. More recent
leads the individual to first test whether the given value is research casts further doubt on the possibility that the differ-
correct—Is the Nile 5,000 (or 800) miles long? Because ential accessibility of anchor-consistent semantic information
554 Judgment and Decision Making

is responsible for such effects. Critcher and Gilovich (2008) adjustment thinking that those were the complete set of
asked participants what the percentage of the sales of a all general-purpose heuristics. Indeed, other heuristics
P-97 (or P-17) cell phone would be in the European mar- have been identified since the publication of their semi-
ket. Participants estimated a higher percentage of European nal work, although perhaps fewer than they might have
sales for the P-97 than the P-17. Note that the process that expected. We have already seen that a fluency heuristic
gives rise to anchor-consistent semantic accessibility is test- should be included. Kahneman and Tversky themselves
ing whether the anchor value might be the correct value. added the notion of a simulation heuristic, by which the
Here it seems far-fetched to maintain that participants asked likelihood and emotional impact of an event are influenced
themselves whether part of the model label (97 or 17) might by the ease with which alternatives to its occurrence can
be the European market share. be imagined in a simulated mental scenario (Kahneman &
Social psychologists have made great use of the idea of Tversky, 1982). Such mental simulations can yield a sense
anchoring, citing it as a core component of such diverse of causal propensity and related feelings of surprise, both of
phenomena as trait inference (Gilbert, 1989, 2002), self- which have also been argued to function much like heu-
enhancement (Kruger, 1999), self-efficacy (Cervone & ristics (Kahneman & Miller, 1986; Kahneman & Varey,
Peake, 1986), perspective taking (Ames, 2004; Epley, 1990). The heuristic that has probably captured the most
Keysar, Van Boven, & Gilovich, 2004), language produc- attention since Kahneman and Tversky’s Big Three is
tion and comprehension (Keysar & Barr, 2002), and the the affect heuristic, which involves using immediate
twin egocentric biases known as the “spotlight effect” good–bad affective reactions to stimuli as a cue to vari-
(Gilovich, Medvec, & Savitsky, 2000) and the “illusion ous judgments and decisions, such as valuation and, most
of transparency” (Gilovich, Savitsky, & Medvec, 1998; important, approach and avoidance (Slovic, Finucane,
Holder & Hawkins, 2007; Miller & McFarland, 1987; Peters, & MacGregor, 2002).
Savitsky & Gilovich, 2003; Van Boven, Gilovich, & A host of decision or choice heuristics has also been pro-
Medvec, 2003; Vorauer & Cameron, 2002; Vorauer & posed (see Frederick, 2002, for a discussion). Two notable
Ross, 1999). Some accounts of social psychological phe- programs of research that examine such heuristics are the
nomena that are based on anchoring simply draw on the adaptive decision-maker framework (Payne, Bettman, &
idea that people’s judgments are assimilated to prominent Johnson, 1990) and the fast and frugal heuristics framework
anchor values. Others draw more specifically on the notion (e.g., Gigerenzer, Todd, and the ABC Research Group,
that people adjust from an initial mental representation but 1999). Both derive from Simon’s (1990) call for delineat-
do so insufficiently. ing the satisficing rules people use that are well adapted
Probably the most influential of these accounts is to particular task or information environments. The adap-
Gilbert’s correction model of causal attribution (Gilbert, tive decision-maker model describes the role of effort–
1989). According to Gilbert, we first characterize people accuracy trade-offs in the selection of decision strategies
in terms of the behavior we see them perform. Someone and the use of more or less complex heuristics as a function
acting angrily is initially categorized as an angry person. of problem complexity and importance. The fast and fru-
But we later note the prevailing situational constraints act- gal program identifies heuristics that maximize simplicity
ing on the person in question and, if those constraints call and accuracy within a given task structure. Both programs
for it, we adjust our initial, unmitigated impression. The of research are highly compatible with the strong influ-
initial phase of characterizing individuals in line with their ence in social psychology of the cognitive miser analogy
behavior is thought to be automatic, unavoidable, and (e.g., Fiske & Taylor, 2007) and the related effort–accuracy
effortless. The later correction phase, however, is thought models of social cognition (e.g., Petty & Cacioppo, 1986),
to be effortful and therefore insufficient whenever ade- but despite—or because of—the overlap, the concept of
quate cognitive resources are scarce (Epley & Gilovich, choice heuristics has thus far had little impact on social
2006; Quattrone, Lawrence, Finkel, & Andrus, 1981). The psychology.
result is that observers end up falling prey to the “corre-
spondence bias” (Gilbert & Jones, 1986) or “fundamen- Normative Theories
tal attribution error” (Ross, 1977), believing that people’s
personal dispositions are a more important cause of their It is hard to imagine how someone could be interested
behavior than is justified. in judgment and decision making without being inter-
ested in how to make the best judgments and decisions.
Other Heuristics As a result, there has always been a normative bent to
Kahneman and Tversky did not offer their three heuris- JDM research, with the existence and character of the most
tics of availability, representativeness, and anchoring and prominent normative theories shaping the kind of research
Social Psychology’s Debt to Judgment and Decision Making 555

JDM scholars conduct. The existence of agreed-on norma- risky gamble even though the “expected” number of lives
tive theories leads naturally to investigations of how well saved would be equal. However, most respondents were
actual judgments and decisions measure up to the ideal. risk seeking when presented with a different version of
They also provide researchers with readily specified null the problem in which the same policies were described in
hypotheses, a useful attribute for a science that places terms of the number of lives lost rather than lives saved.
such great emphasis on significance testing. Indeed, the That is, only about 20% of the respondents preferred the
most prominent normative theories have often provided policy that would result in 400 people dying over the pol-
researchers the most convenient null hypotheses—straw icy with a one third chance of nobody dying and a two
men, really, that are easy to shoot down and allow the thirds chance of 600 dying. Participants’ responses are
investigators in question to stake a claim for the impor- clearly not always invariant across different descriptions
tance, and publishability, of a set of findings. For example, of the same problem.
the null hypothesis that people are entirely selfish and Tversky and Kahneman (1986) have used this discrep-
have no concern about fairness, although important to ancy between what people agree to in the abstract and what
investigate given the theoretical lay of the land at the time they do in concrete choice contexts to make the point that
(Kahneman, Knetsch, & Thaler, 1986), was not one that there can never be a single theory of decision making that is
had any real chance of standing. both normatively valid and descriptively adequate. The
JDM research is intricately connected to two crucial normative force of the axioms of rational choice is unas-
normative theories—SEU theory for choice and Bayes’ sailable and hence cannot be dispensed with. And yet it is
theorem for probability judgment (note that these theo- an empirical fact that people’s choices often fail to conform
ries are intertwined, as Bayesian probability is used as the to the axioms. SEU theory thus serves as an ideal to which
“expectation” component of SEU). According to SEU the- people’s decisions can be compared. And this comparison
ory, a decision-maker assigns a utility to each possible out- guides a great deal of research in JDM. It quite naturally
come for each course of action and weights each outcome raises the question of how decisions might be improved,
by its perceived probability of occurrence. The decision- but it also frames the effort to understand what it is that
maker then chooses the course of action with the highest people are doing when they are making choices if they are
expected utility. Few people find this idea objectionable or not doing what the axioms prescribe.
even noteworthy. More noteworthy, however, is the speci- Bayes’ theorem plays much the same role in the study
fication of several axioms and principles that one’s choices of judgment. It provides a mathematical specification of
must follow to ensure that one maximizes overall utility. how much one’s initial belief should change in response
Among these are the axioms of transitivity (if x is preferred to new information. Consider hypothesis A: People’s judg-
to y, and y is preferred to z, then x should be preferred to z), ments are predictably biased. Now consider event B: the
independence (if x is preferred to y, then x or a given chance cumulative research of Kahneman and Tversky. How
of z should be preferred to y or an equivalent chance of z), much should we change our belief in the probability of
and consistency (if x is preferred to y, then some probabil- hypothesis A (predictably biased judgment) given event B
ity mixture of receiving x or y should be preferred to y) (the evidence reported by Kahneman and Tversky)? The
and the principle of invariance (one’s preference between probability form of Bayes’ theorem is as follows:
x and y should not depend on the surface descriptions of x
and y or the method by which one’s preference is elicited). P(A|B)  [P(A)  P(B|A)]/P(B)
Few people find any of the axioms objectionable either,
but JDM researchers have had no trouble demonstrating Thus, to determine the subjective probability of systematic
that although people endorse these axioms in principle bias given the evidence, we take our prior belief in sys-
they often violate them in practice. tematic bias, P (A) (which can vary from person to person
For example, the principle of invariance is violated by and hence puts the “subjective” in subjective probability), and
people’s responses to one of the most famous decision adjust it by multiplying it by the extent to which the exper-
problems in the JDM literature, Kahneman and Tversky’s imental evidence is consistent with systematic bias, P (B | A).
“Asian disease” problem (1984). Faced with the outbreak The result is then further adjusted by the unconditional
of a disease that “is expected to kill 600 people,” nearly probability of finding the evidence whether or not people
three quarters of the respondents preferred a policy that are systematically biased, P (B).
would save 200 lives for sure over a policy with a one Early research comparing people’s probability judg-
third chance of saving 600 and a two thirds chance of ments with Bayes’ theorem indicated that people were
saving none. In other words, most respondents were risk too slow to revise their prior beliefs. They were “con-
averse in that they chose the guaranteed outcome over the servative Bayesians” (Edwards, 1968). A big part of the
556 Judgment and Decision Making

heuristics and biases program of research, however, was 1980; Davies, 1997; Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975;
the demonstration that on many occasions people are overly Sherman & Kim, 2002; Walster, Berscheid, Abrahams, &
influenced by new case-specific information (Tversky & Aronson, 1967).
Kahneman, 1971). A great deal of work in JDM is focused Nisbett and Ross’s explicit treatment of normative issues
on understanding the psychological processes that yield accentuated the consideration of normativity in several
judgmental conservatism versus judgmental rashness and on areas of social psychology that had always had something
specifying the features of the presented information and of a normative focus. Early treatments of attribution the-
of the problem context that give rise to each (Griffin & ory, for example, viewed the basic principles of attribu-
Tversky, 1993). tion—covariation and discounting or augmentation—as
The field of social psychology was always at least some- principles a person should follow in assigning causes to
what concerned with normative issues, of course, because it effects. Although it was generally thought that people typi-
is a field with considerable interest in applied issues and in cally did follow these principles, some attention was paid
improving the human condition—minimizing group con- to those instances in which the attributions that people
flict, extinguishing prejudice, overcoming excessive group made departed from the dictates of the theory (Kelley,
influence, and so on. But the effect of normative thinking 1973). The most notable departure is the tendency of peo-
on research in social psychology was amplified by Nisbett ple to make person-centered attributions when the behav-
and Ross’s (1980) treatise on human inference, a work ior in question can be entirely explained by the dictates of
that brought JDM front and center to social psychology. the prevailing situation (Gilbert & Jones, 1986; Gilbert &
Nisbett and Ross delineated many of the most important Malone, 1995). There is little question that research on
inferential tasks facing the social perceiver—covariation this correspondence bias (Gilbert & Jones, 1986) or fun-
detection, causal analysis, prediction, and belief revision— damental attribution error (Ross, 1977) took something of
and explicitly compared how the average person approaches a different shape as a result of the heightened concern with
those tasks with the procedures scientists use to approach normativity that was inherited from JDM. This is reflected
them. The direct comparison between formal and lay in how the phenomenon was cast, in how the research was
approaches to social judgment necessarily invokes a conducted (Gilbert & Jones, 1986; Quattrone, 1982), and
consideration of normative principles. Sometimes, as in in the nature of the theoretical controversies that were
the case of prediction and belief revision, the relevant nor- sparked (Funder, 1987; Hilton, 1990).
mative standard is the Bayesian analysis so commonly Another area of research in social psychology that was
invoked in JDM (cf. Ajzen & Fishbein, 1975). At other greatly affected by the enhanced emphasis on norma-
times, however, Nisbett and Ross took the general JDM tivity that came from JDM is the study of stereotyping.
focus on normative comparison and invoked, opportunis- The field had always been marked by the conviction that
tically, more circumscribed normative considerations that there was something “wrong” with stereotyping. Although
apply only to a particular inferential problem. For exam- it was granted that some stereotypes are valid (Volvo
ple, they considered what happens when two groups, on and Prius owners are more likely to listen to National
opposite sides of a policy debate, are exposed to a mixed Public Radio), the research was motivated and shaped by
body of evidence germane to the debate. It is hard to con- the concern that many stereotypes are erroneous and do
ceive of a normative analysis that would not dictate that the a great deal of harm, particularly to those belonging to
two sides’ opinions should converge to some degree. But the stereotyped group. It had long been recognized that
because each side readily accepts the evidence that supports stereotypes could sometimes arise not from base human
its position and finds fault with the evidence that sup- motives or intergroup rivalry but from faulty information
ports the other side, the two groups’ opinions can actually processing alone (Allport, 1954). But the rise of the cog-
diverge, not converge (Lord et al., 1979). They also consid- nitive revolution in psychology, and the emergence of the
ered what happens when the evidential basis of one’s opin- subfield of social cognition in particular, greatly accentu-
ion is invalidated. When the entire set of reasons one had ated this recognition. And the notion that information pro-
for holding a particular belief are unambiguously shown to cessing might be “faulty” necessitates a consideration—a
be in error, logic dictates that one should “start over” and consideration made easier because of the groundwork laid
have the same opinions as those who were never exposed by JDM—of what constitutes sound thinking. It is one
to the misleading information that gave rise to the belief. thing to say that the distinctiveness of negative behavior
But what has been found in numerous studies is that the on the part of minority group members is troublesome;
initial, unfounded belief hangs tough and survives to some it is quite another to specify how it is inferentially out
degree after its foundation is undercut (Anderson, 1995; of line (Fiedler, 1991; Hamilton & Gifford, 1976; Risen,
Anderson & Lindsay, 1998; Anderson, Ross, & Lepper, Gilovich, & Dunning, 2007). It is one thing to bemoan the
Judgment and Decision Making’s Debt to Social Psychology 557

fact that stereotypes often survive exposure to stereotype- or block desired behavior—an idea championed and elabo-
inconsistent information; it is something else to identify rated by Ross and Nisbett (1991). Subtle and often invis-
the normative principle it violates. ible elements of the surrounding situation, in other words,
create a channel that leads people down one path rather
than another. A study often used to illustrate the notion
JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING’S of channel factors is one that examined the effectiveness of
DEBT TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY different efforts to convince Yale University undergradu-
ates to get their free tetanus vaccines (Leventhal, Singer, &
Because humans are a social species, many of our most Jones, 1965). One attempt centered on frightening the stu-
important judgments and decisions are those that take place dents with all the ways one can get tetanus and what the
in a social context and concern other people. It would be late stages of lockjaw look like. The students were told
odd indeed, then, if JDM research did not deal extensively they could avoid such an awful fate by going to the health
with the social embeddedness of judgments and deci- center on campus at any time and getting a free inocula-
sions. And it would be odder still if JDM research were tion. This succeeded in impressing the students about
not heavily influenced by the one subdiscipline of psychol- the severity of the disease and the importance of getting
ogy specifically devoted to the inherently social nature of inoculated, but almost none of them actually did so. In
human life. an alternative approach, other participants were likewise
In fact, JDM researchers have had a long-standing inter- given the scary materials, but they were also handed a map
est in judgments about people and in social life, from the of the Yale campus with the health center circled and they
inherently social element of game theory (Axelrod, 1984; were asked to review their schedules and come up with
Nash, 1950), to the comparison of clinical versus statisti- a convenient time to visit the center and get their shots.
cal prediction (Dawes & Corrigan, 1974; Meehl, 1954), to These seemingly trivial details—pointing out the health
applications of Brunswik’s lens model (Hammond, 1996). center ’s location and encouraging thoughts about the best
And this interest has served to develop and strengthen the time to visit—increased the rate at which students actually
ties between JDM and social psychology. Just as social got their shots by a factor of nine. In other words, sim-
psychology would look different today if not for the influ- ply increasing students’ motivation to get inoculated had
ence of JDM, the latter field would look different if not almost no effect; creating a channel that made the desired
for the influence of social psychology. To showcase that behavior easier had a considerable effect.
influence, we discuss three particularly important ideas A similar effect of channel factors can be found in peo-
from social psychology that have had—and are having—a ple’s use of health services more generally. One of the most
substantial impact on JDM. In particular, we discuss how powerful predictors of whether people will use the services
social psychological theorizing about channel factors, available to them, more powerful than attitudes about
emotion, and norms and identity has influenced the field health and various demographic variables such as age,
of JDM. gender, and socioeconomic status, is the distance between
an individual’s residence and the closest facility (Van
Dort & Moos, 1976). When the University of Rochester
Channel Factors
moved its student health center from the campus to an off-
Kurt Lewin is generally credited with noticing that when campus site, usage of the facility declined 37% (Simon &
people try to change someone else’s behavior they typi- Smith, 1973).
cally try to increase the person’s motivation to behave dif- Another telling and illustrative study examined efforts
ferently. They try to increase the “push” toward the desired to get U.S. citizens to buy war bonds during World War II
behavior. Lewin recognized that often people are already (Cartwright, 1949). The war bond campaign, with memo-
motivated to perform the desired behavior but cannot get rable posters featuring vulnerable American children and
themselves to translate their good intentions into effective scary Axis warmongers, was generally considered effec-
action. A more effective strategy for changing behavior is tive and raised a considerable amount of money for the
to figure out what is preventing the desired behavior and war effort. Nevertheless, when the slogans depicted on
then to eliminate any sources of resistance. Rather than the posters were altered slightly (changing “Buy War
increase the push, it is often more effective to dampen the Bonds” to “Buy an Extra $100 Bond Today”) or the request
pushback. was made more specific (“Buy Them When the Solicitor at
Related to this analysis is Lewin’s concept of “channel Your Workplace Asks You to Sign Up”) to create a channel
factors” (Lewin, 1952), or the notion that seemingly minor that would smooth the path to donation, sales of war bonds
details of the situational context can powerfully facilitate doubled.
558 Judgment and Decision Making

It does not take much thought to recognize the relevance As Figure 15.1 so clearly shows, opt-out policies that
of the idea of channel factors to JDM. The structure of require people to take steps to avoid being organ donors
various problems, or the context surrounding those prob- “channel” donation and thus lead to rates of willingness to
lems, channel or facilitate certain responses (creating what donate that are near ceiling. In contrast, opt-in policies that
one might call “downhill” responses) and block or impede reverse the burden of action put up a formidable barrier to
others (what one might call “uphill” responses). This has donation, leading to donation rates that fall well short of
been powerfully demonstrated in research documenting the need for healthy organs for transplant.
the tremendous impact that defaults and status quo options Defaults appear to have such powerful effects on peo-
can have on people’s choices. One early and noteworthy ple’s choices for several reasons. For one thing, the default
investigation examined the automobile insurance purchase option—the deepest, clearest channel—profits from lazi-
decisions of consumers in New Jersey and Pennsylvania, ness, mindlessness, and decision paralysis (Iyengar &
two adjacent states that offered their residents essentially Lepper, 2000; Langer, 1989; Schwartz, 2004). When one
the same pair of options but with opposite defaults (Johnson cannot be bothered to figure out the right choice, forgets to
et al., 1993). One option was a “full-priced” policy that figure out the right choice, or cannot discern, despite con-
included the right to sue for any vehicle-related injury; siderable effort, what the right choice might be, the default
the other, cheaper option did not include the right to sue. becomes the choice. Second, that a particular option had
Pennsylvania offered the full-priced policy as the default, been selected as the default is often taken as informative
allowing residents to opt for the less expensive policy if (McKenzie, Liersch, & Finkelstein, 2006). It is often inter-
they chose to forgo the right to sue. In contrast, the less preted as a hint of what the best option might be—however
expensive policy was the default in New Jersey, but con- mindlessly the default may have been selected. Finally,
sumers could obtain the right to sue by paying more. In a the default is also often experienced as the status quo from
striking demonstration of the power of defaults, only about which people have been shown to be reluctant to deviate
20% of New Jersey residents thought it was in their best (Baron & Ritov, 1994; Samuelson & Zeckhauser, 1988;
interests to have the full-priced policy, compared with 75% Schweitzer, 1995).
of Pennsylvania residents. The existence of such a status quo bias makes it clear that
An even more striking demonstration of the influence there are other types of privileged options beyond defaults.
of defaults comes from a study of organ donation rates in In particular, although they are often conflated, the adher-
countries that have explicit consent (“opt-in”) or presumed ence to defaults, the status quo bias, and the omission bias
consent (“opt-out”) policies (Johnson & Goldstein, 2003). constitute separate influences on choice (Schweitzer, 1994).

Opt-In Countries Opt-Out Countries

99.98 98 99.91 99.97 99.5 99.64


100

90 85.9
Effective Consent Percentage

80

70

60

50

40

30 27.5

20 17.17
12
10 4.25
0
Netherlands

United
Kingdom

Germany

Austria

Belgium

France

Hungary

Poland

Sweden
Portugal
Denmark

Figure 15.1 Organ donation rates in countries with explicit consent (opt-in) and
presumed consent (opt-out) donation policies.
Judgment and Decision Making’s Debt to Social Psychology 559

The omission bias refers to the tendency to judge harmful institutions, such as check cashers and unregulated money
actions more harshly than harmful inactions and the con- lenders, that exact higher transaction costs. Many poor
comitant reluctance in many circumstances to go out on a households also fail to take advantage of various welfare
limb by taking action (Gleicher et al., 1990; Kahneman & services for which they are eligible. Bertrand and colleagues
Miller, 1986; Kahneman & Tversky, 1982; Spranca, have explored how several modest interventions—such as
Minsk, & Baron, 1991). All of these tendencies speak to the simplifying application forms and consolidating informa-
power of norms (which we discuss more extensively later), tional meetings and enrollment sessions into one encounter—
and the influence of each stems partly from the anticipated can dramatically increase the use of banking and welfare
regret that would come with a counternormative choice. services on the part of the poor.
To defy a default is to alter the way things are preset to be. To More broadly, channel factors play a prominent role in
defy the status quo is to depart from tradition. And to defy Thaler and Sunstein’s advocacy of “libertarian paternal-
the omission bias is to shoulder an extra burden of respon- ism” (Thaler & Sunstein, 2003, 2008; see also Camerer,
sibility. All of this makes one vulnerable to self-criticism Issacharoff, Loewenstein, O’Donoghue, & Rabin, 2003).
and the second-guessing of others. There are times, of The choice environment in which individuals confront
course, when the default or status quo is to take action, and many important decisions are often unintentionally struc-
when it is, people tend to experience more regret for fail- tured in ways that steer people away from acting in their best
ing to act rather than for acting—that is, the omission bias interests—for example, arbitrary and counterproductive
is turned on its head (Connolly & Zeelenberg, 2002; Davis, defaults, bewildering presentations of available options,
Lehman, Wortman, Silver, & Thompson, 1995; Gilovich & and salient displays of the most troublesome alternatives.
Medvec, 1995; Seta, McElroy, & Seta, 2001; Zeelenberg, Rather than mandating actions that are deemed to be in
Van den Bos, Van Dijk, & Pieters, 2002). the best interests of most citizens—that is, paternalism—
Although default and status quo effects are perfect illus- the choice environment can be structured in ways that foster
trations of the power of channel factors, they attracted the better decisions. No one is compelled to make a particular
interest of JDM researchers simply because they are such choice; they are simply nudged in the right direction—that
prominent elements of the landscape of decision making. is, libertarian paternalism. And much of the nudging they
Recently, however, JDM researchers have pursued research recommend involves creating the right channels in the
agendas inspired directly by the theoretical notion of chan- choice environment to encourage sound decision making
nel factors. This is particularly true of scholars associated and enhanced well-being.
with the field of behavioral economics. The aim of many
behavioral economists is to figure out why many people do
not spend, save, invest, or borrow as wisely as they might Affective Influences on Judgment and Decisions
and to design interventions that make it easier for people to
act in accordance with their economic interests. It is surely no accident that social psychology and JDM
Investigations of employee participation in company- became heavily intertwined in the 1970s, the heyday of
sponsored retirement plans, for example, have found that cognitive theorizing in psychology. The roots of JDM in
the channel leading people to productive savings tends to be mathematical decision theory, in reasoning, and in statistical
blocked by the provision of numerous investment options prediction guaranteed a pronounced, if not exclusive, focus
(Iyengar, Jiang, & Huberman, 2004) but is deepened when on cognitive mechanisms of judgment and choice. Such a
employers offer automatic (Madrian & Shea, 2001) or easy focus found a ready match in the predominant theoretical
enrollment (Choi, Laibson, & Madrian, 2009) options. perspectives and research interests of social psychologists at
Taking advantage of the fact that people find it easier to that time. But although social psychologists were obsessed
commit unpleasant or costly actions in the future than in with determining how virtually every social psychological
the present, Benartzi and Thaler (2004) found that sav- phenomenon might be explained solely with reference to
ings rates increased substantially when employees could cognitive processes, the field did not abandon its historical
sign up for increased (automatic) deductions from future interest in emotional influences on behavior (see Bruner,
raises—their “save more tomorrow” plan. 1992; Bruner & Goodman, 1947; Bruner & Klein, 1960;
Marianne Bertrand, Sendhil Mullainathan, and Eldar and Bruner & Postman, 1947, for earlier treatments of the
Shafir (2004, 2006) have applied the idea of channel factors effects of motivation on perception and judgment). And
to better understand, and to improve, the financial decisions when the “cognitive revolution” ebbed, there was a rapid
made by the poor. For instance, many poor households do and pronounced return to the study of emotion (Keltner &
not participate in the banking system, a decision that can cost Lerner, this volume). Given the new tight links between
them dearly as they are forced rely on alternative financial JDM and social psychology, the latter’s renewed interest
560 Judgment and Decision Making

in emotion was certain to have a pronounced influence 1978; Mayer, Gaschke, Braverman, & Evans, 1992; Mayer,
on JDM. Gayle, Meehan, & Haarman, 1990). A considerable body
Social psychologists had earlier studied the impact of of research has examined this explanation, but it remains
emotion on behavior, with a particular focus on the influ- controversial (Clore & Huntsinger, 2007).
ence of various affective states on compliance. Feeling According to an alternative account, the affect-as-
guilty, for example, has been shown to increase peo- information hypothesis, mood and emotional states are cues
ple’s willingness to comply with requests from strangers about how one feels about a given stimulus (Clore, 1992;
(Carlsmith & Gross, 1969; Cialdini, Darby, & Vincent, Clore & Huntsinger, 2007; Schwarz, 1990; Schwarz & Clore,
1973; Darlington & Macker, 1966; Regan, 1971; Regan, 2003; Sechrist, Swim, & Mark, 2003). Positive stimuli
Williams, & Sparling, 1972). On the other end of the emo- typically induce positive affect, so people implicitly rea-
tional spectrum, being in a good mood has also been shown son that if they feel good while confronting or contem-
to increase compliance (Carlson, Charlin, & Miller, 1988; plating a given stimulus they must think it has positive
Isen, Clark, & Schwartz, 1976; Isen & Levin, 1972). features. And the same applies for negative moods and
Inspired partly by findings such as these, Johnson and emotions. In the most widely cited study in support of
Tversky (1983) examined whether incidental affective this idea, students at a midwestern university were called
states influence people’s assessments of risk. They had on the telephone (by someone claiming to be from out of
participants first rate the journalistic quality of news stories town) and asked a few questions about their life satisfac-
depicting anxiety-provoking, depressing, or uplifting events tion (Schwarz & Clore, 1983). Some calls were made on
and then had them estimate the number of people who die sunny days and others on gloomy days, and as predicted,
each year as a result of such things as traffic accidents, respondents reported that they were significantly happier
leukemia, and homicide. Those who had read news stories and more satisfied with their lives overall if they were
that induced anxiety and depression provided significantly contacted on one of the sunny days. In a critical addi-
higher estimates than those who had read either neutral or tional condition of the study, half the respondents were
uplifting stories (see also Loewenstein, Weber, Hsee, & asked before the life-satisfaction questions, “By the way,
Welch, 2001; Slovic & Peters, 2006). Subsequent work how’s the weather down there?” Asking this question
established that hypnotically induced mood states have the erased the impact of the weather on respondents’ ratings
same effect: Those in a good mood estimated higher prob- of happiness and satisfaction with life. This manipula-
abilities of occurrence for such things as world peace and a tion almost certainly did not influence the respondents’
cure for cancer, and those in a bad mood estimated higher moods, but it did influence what they understood
probabilities for such things as nuclear power accidents their moods to signify about their lives. Without the
and automobile injuries (Wright & Bower, 1992). question about the weather, respondents interpreted their
More dramatic manifestations of this effect can be found feelings as indicative of their overall satisfaction with
in studies of stock market returns. An examination of stock life; with the question, they did not.
markets in 26 countries over a 15-year period found that As research on the affect-as-information hypothesis
the amount of sunshine on a given day was positively cor- progressed, it became clear that the informative value of
related with market performance. The investigators sug- moods was quite a bit more general than originally envi-
gested that this effect was due to investors attributing their sioned and had implications far beyond their influence
weather-induced good moods to positive economic circum- on evaluative judgments (Schwarz, 1990). This led to
stances rather than the true source, sunshine (Hirshleifer & the third account of the influence of mood and emotion
Shumway, 2003; Kamstra, Kramer, & Levi, 2003). A similar, on judgment, the processing style perspective. The idea
more recent study found that stock market returns decline here is that people implicitly understand that their moods
when a country’s soccer team is eliminated from a promi- reflect the state of their environment. A bad mood may
nent tournament such as the World Cup and that similar dips signify a troublesome situation, serving as something
occur in countries following losses in other sports (cricket, of a “trouble ahead, slow down” sign and leading to an
rugby, and basketball) popular in those countries (Edmans, information-processing style that is careful, systematic,
Garcia, & Norli, 2007). and deliberate. A happy mood, in contrast, may signify
Three explanations have been offered for the influence a benign situation and serve as something of a “smooth
of mood on judgment. According to the priming account, sailing” sign, leading people to process information more
positive and negative moods activate positive and negative heuristically and reflexively. A great deal of research
information, respectively, and the enhanced accessibility of supports this view, with people in positive moods being
one type over the other distorts how objects or propositions shown to engage more than those in negative moods
are evaluated (Bower, 1981; Isen, Shalker, Clark, & Karp, in top-down rather than bottom-up analysis, heuristic
Judgment and Decision Making’s Debt to Social Psychology 561

rather than systematic processing, and global or abstract found that participants led to feel angry or content (high
thought rather than specific or concrete thought (Bless, certainty) relied more on the source of a persuasive commu-
Mackie, & Schwarz, 1992; Bless, Schwarz, & Wieland, nication when evaluating its merits than did participants led
1996; Bodenhausen, Kramer, & Susser, 1994; Forgas, 1998; to feel worried or surprised (low certainty). The feeling of
Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005; Gasper & Clore, 2002; certainty associated with anger and contentment apparently
Park & Banaji, 2000). encouraged participants to think that they had a handle on
things and could rely on relatively superficial cues, such as
Beyond Positive and Negative who was pitching the message. This conclusion was rein-
Our emotional lives, of course, are more complex than feel- forced by the results of another study in which participants
ing bad and feeling good, so there is more to emotion than led to feel disgusted (high certainty) were more likely to
positive or negative feelings. Many emotion researchers use stereotypes in making predictions about another person
maintain that a key element of what distinguishes various than were participants led to feel anxious (low certainty).
emotional states is the pattern of cognitions—appraisals— In an investigation of the impact of other appraisals
associated with each (Lazarus, 1991; Smith & Ellsworth, associated with different emotions, Lerner, Small, and
1985). The good–bad dimension may be the primary Loewenstein (2004) examined how the incidental emo-
appraisal, but it is not the only one. Anger, for example, tional states of disgust and sadness might influence the
is not just negative; it is also associated with appraisals of endowment effect, or the tendency for owners of a good to
harm to oneself or someone one cares about, the source value it more than those considering owning it (Kahneman,
of the harm, and the attribution of the harm to an agent. Knetsch, & Thaler, 1991; Thaler, 1980). Disgust is associ-
And just as the positivity or negativity of an incidental ated with the appraisal of being too close to an entity or
emotional state might spill over and influence an unrelated idea, so the investigators predicted that it would lead to
judgment, so can these other appraisals. Most research that a desire to expel close objects and hence would under-
has examined this issue has focused on the appraisal of mine the endowment effect. It did. After having earlier
certainty. Emotions associated with certainty—anger, dis- seen a film that elicited feelings of disgust, those given a
gust, and happiness—are thought to be characterized by a small gift and asked how much they would sell it for did
sense of confidence about what is happening in the situa- not inflate their asking price—in marked contrast to sell-
tion, how to respond, and what will happen next, whereas ers in a control condition. The investigators also predicted
emotions associated with uncertainty—fear, sadness, and that sadness, associated with the implicit goal of changing
hope—are characterized by less confidence in these assess- one’s current circumstances, would lead to a reversal of
ments (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). the endowment effect. Those endowed with the gift would
In one study of the impact on judgment of emotions that want to change by getting rid of what they have and those
differ in certainty appraisals, Lerner and Keltner (2001) not given the gift would want to change by acquiring it.
found that participants who had been asked to think about And they did. Sellers stated a significantly lower price for
events from their lives that made them angry (high cer- the gift than neutral choosers did.
tainty) gave lower estimates of the likelihood of suffering
various maladies than did those who had been asked to
Noninferential Effects of Emotion
think about events that made them afraid (low certainty).
The feeling of certainty associated with anger made vari- All theoretical accounts of the impact of affective states
ous hazards seem less likely (see also Leith & Baumeister, on judgment discussed thus far posit an element of reason-
1996; Taylor, Lerner, Sage, Lehman, & Seeman, 2004; ing as a critical component of the reported effects. More
Tiedens & Linton, 2001). In a notable extension of this specifically, by these accounts, emotions are seen as cues.
finding, a national sample of Americans was contacted 2 According to the mood-as-information account, positive
months after the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks on feelings are taken as a cue that the stimulus being con-
the World Trade Center and the Pentagon. Some respon- fronted or the thought being entertained must be congenial.
dents were led to focus on fear-inducing elements of the According to the processing-style perspective, negative
attacks, others on anger-inducing elements. The angry feelings are taken as a cue that not all is right in the world
participants gave lower estimates of the likelihood of both and one must be careful and process all incoming infor-
future terrorist attacks and other, unrelated risks (Lerner, mation carefully. Such cognitive elements in theories of
Gonzalez, Small, & Fischhoff, 2003). emotional influence are to be expected, as people’s cogni-
Another examination of the impact of the certainty tive and affective systems are tightly interconnected and
appraisals associated with different emotional states went emotions typically have much of their effects through their
beyond perceptions of risk. Tiedens and Linton (2001) impact on cognitive processes.
562 Judgment and Decision Making

But emotions sometimes have simpler, more direct to be a critical region for uniting factual knowledge and
effects. We often think of emotion not as intertwined with somatic markers, or emotional reactions. If this region is
reason, but as highjacking reason. And we sometimes think not intact, individuals still recognize the long odds against
of emotions as at odds with reason and as exerting an influ- a given course of action but do not experience the emo-
ence on our behavior that is too strong to be overcome by tional warning signal—the somatic marker—that steers
our “better judgment.” Is there evidence of more direct, them away from it. Note that it is the emotion itself, not an
and less inferential, emotional influence on judgment and inference drawn from the emotion (even participants with
decisions? orbitofrontal damage have drawn the right inferences), that
One program of research that addresses the direct influ- directs behavior.
ence of emotion on judgment and behavior examines how At times, however, ignoring such emotional warning sig-
people physiologically encode the affective consequences of nals results in better decisions. In these cases, the absence
different courses of action and how these “somatic markers” of somatic markers improves decision making. Most peo-
influence action (Bechara, Damasio, & Damasio, 2000; ple, for example, will not accept 50–50 odds of winning
Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1997; Damasio, $200 or losing $150 despite the positive expected value of
1994; Damasio, Bechara, & Damasio, 2002; Hinson, such a bet because the fear of losing $150 looms too large.
Jameson, & Whitney, 2002; but see Dunn, Dalgleish, & Basketball coaches who find their teams trailing by two
Lawrence, 2006; Tomb, Hauser, Deldin, & Caramazza, points, but in possession of the ball, are reluctant to set up
2002). The researchers investigated the impact of somatic a game-settling three-point shot even though the chances of
markers by giving participants a stack of play money and making it and winning the game (~33%) are greater than the
having them play a card game in which the goal was to win chances of making a two-point shot and then winning
as much money as possible by turning over cards that spec- the game in overtime (~50% chance of making the shot 
ified wins and losses of varying amounts. Players selected ~50% chance of winning the overtime period  ~25%). In
cards from one of four decks, without knowing (at first) the situations like these, patients with damage to critical brain
composition of the decks or how long the game would last. regions have been found to make better decisions because
The composition of the decks is depicted in Table 15.1. they do not have the normal level of risk aversion (Shiv,
Note that the decks that often provide participants with a Loewenstein, Bechara, Damasio, & Damasio, 2005).
$50 payout have a positive expected value and should be
Emotional Influences on Probability Assessment
pursued; the decks that provide a $100 payout have a nega-
tive expected value and should be avoided. Another possible direct effect of emotion on judgment
Do participants learn to choose wisely from decks C and involves the tendency for emotionally laden events to dis-
D and to avoid decks A and B? All of them did—initially. tort the weights people assign to the events’ probability
But participants with damage to their orbitofrontal cortex of occurrence (Rottenstreich & Hsee, 2001). According
quickly resumed picking from the high-gain, high-loss to standard models of decision making, people indepen-
decks. It thus appears that although all participants knew dently assess the valence of an event and its likelihood of
these decks were excessively risky, those with damage to occurrence and then combine the two to decide on the best
the orbitofrontal cortex could not stop themselves from course of action. This can seem psychologically unreal-
going after the alluring $100 payout. Furthermore, patients istic, however, as the following thought experiment sug-
without brain damage, but not those suffering from damage gests. Suppose you were to roll a die and, if it lands on six,
to the orbitofrontal cortex, soon developed a pattern of skin you pay a fine of $10. How likely does it seem that a
conductance that preceded their choices from the exces- six will come up? Now suppose you play Russian roulette
sively risky decks. Thus, the orbitofrontal cortex appears with one bullet in a six-chambered revolver. How likely
does it seem that it will end in disaster? Most people report
that they recognize the odds of a bad outcome are the same
Table 15.1 Composition of the Card Decks Used by Bechara, in the two scenarios but that the odds “feel” less favorable in
Damasio, & Damasio (2000)
the latter.
Deck A Deck B Deck C Deck D The contamination of an event’s perceived likelihood
Reward $100 $100 $50 $50 by its valence appears to be partly responsible for people’s
Punishment $150–350 $1,250 $25–75 $250 reluctance to “tempt fate.” People know that a negative
P (punishment) .5 .1 .5 .1 outcome will be experienced as especially negative if
Expected value $25 $25 $25 $25 they will not be able to shake the thought that, accurate or
Punishment refers to how much the participant can lose; P(punishment) not, they did something to bring it about. Getting rained
refers to the probability of punishment on feels bad, but getting rained on after deciding not to
Judgment and Decision Making’s Debt to Social Psychology 563

carry an umbrella feels worse; the end of a winning streak Ariely and Loewenstein (2005) examined the effects of
feels bad, but having it end after calling attention to the sexual arousal on male participants’ self-reported will-
streak feels worse. And it appears to be the very negativity ingness to engage in problematic sexual behavior. All of
of imagined negative outcomes that follow from actions their participants were asked to answer several questions
that tempt fate that makes them especially accessible and on a laptop computer loaned to them for the experiment.
makes them seem especially likely to occur (Risen & Control participants answered the questions in their nor-
Gilovich, 2007, 2008). mal, presumably not intensely aroused, state. Experimental
If the emotions elicited by anticipating different uncer- participants did so in the course of following instructions
tain events—emotions like excitement and dread—influence to begin masturbating while viewing erotica. Aroused par-
the impact of their subjective probability of occurrence, we ticipants reported that they would be less likely to use a
might expect a “compression” of people’s reactions to condom during intercourse, that they would be more likely
varying probabilities of experiencing an emotionally rich to lie to obtain sex and persist in the effort to do so more
outcome. Rottenstreich and Hsee (2001) found just such vigorously after a woman said no, and that they found
an effect, showing that people’s willingness to pay to avoid practices such as S&M, anal sex, and sex with a wider age
an electric shock, an affect-rich outcome, was relatively range of partners more appealing. Note that the aroused
insensitive to the probability of receiving the shock. In participants’ physiological state did not influence their per-
contrast, their willingness to pay to avoid losing $20, a less ceptions of the dangers of some of these activities. They
affectively rich outcome, was sensitive to the probability were no less likely than control participants, for example,
of the loss. Thus, a very low probability of experiencing a to agree with the statement, “If you pull out before you
dreaded outcome can seem too likely, and a very low prob- ejaculate, a woman can still get pregnant.” But their aware-
ability of a delightful treat can be enough to maintain hope. ness of the dangers notwithstanding, the heat of the moment
Ditto, Pizarro, Epstein, Jacobson, and MacDonald (2006) had a pronounced effect on their inclinations to engage
obtained similar results in an experiment in which partici- in behavior that more dispassionate participants view
pants were given the opportunity to play a game in which as highly questionable (see also Loewenstein, Nagin, &
winning would result in their getting to eat chocolate chip Paternoster, 1997). As Loewenstein (1996) pointed out,
cookies but losing would result in their having to work on theories that fail to take account of the impact of visceral
a boring task for an extra half hour. Half of the participants states like sexual arousal, hunger, thirst, and tiredness
were simply told about the cookie reward; for the other fail to capture some of the most common and most power-
half, the cookies had been freshly baked and placed in ful determinants of the choices people make.
front of the participants as they decided whether to play
the game. In line with the results of Rottenstreich and Hsee Norms and Identity
(2001), the participants’ willingness to play the game was
sensitive to their odds of winning when the cookies were The study of norms has always been an important part of
described abstractly but insensitive to the probability of social psychology and doubtless will always remain an
winning when the aroma and sight of the cookies got their important area of investigation. At least two reasons exist
juices flowing. for this, one of them being the undeniable fact that norms
determine so much of human behavior. Consider what a
Visceral Influence on Behavior snapshot of rush hour at Grand Central Station would look
Strong visceral feelings can not only distort our sense of like in 1935 and, say, 50 years later. Two things would
the likelihood of an emotionally laden event, or the psy- immediately stand out. The overwhelming majority of
chological weight that we assign to its likelihood, but also the people in the 1935 photo would be men, but the 1985
can directly influence our actions, sometimes leading us photo would be more gender balanced. And nearly all men
to act in ways that are at variance with our better judg- in 1935 would be wearing hats. The only explanation for
ment (Ainslie, 1975). The average person has no difficulty this would be the change in norms governing fashion and
appreciating this idea, as nearly everyone is familiar with work outside the home. (A less attention-grabbing norm
such expressions as “I must have been crazy when I . . . ,” “I in the same scene, one present in both 1935 and 1985, is
just couldn’t control myself and . . . ,” and “My emotions just the one that prevents everyone from bumping into one
got the better of me.” George Loewenstein (1996) provided another—that of staying to the right as they walk.)
a formal account of visceral influences on behavior, with The second reason that norms are so important for
the aim of making the study of such influences a central social psychologists to study is that they are often invis-
component of the science of decision making. In one nota- ible. People follow them but often do not know and can-
ble experimental investigation inspired by this analysis, not articulate what they are following. For many of the
564 Judgment and Decision Making

most basic and powerful norms, people only notice their be claimed. Consider the impact that training in economics
existence on those rare occasions when someone violates has on contributions to the public good. Academic econo-
them, engaging in what Erving Goffman (1963) referred mists are less likely than their peers in other academic dis-
to as “negatively eventful actions.” Thus, norms are an ciplines to contribute to charity, and economics majors are
important focus of social psychological research because more likely than students majoring in other disciplines to
they hide in plain sight. defect in the prisoner ’s dilemma game (Frank, Gilovich, &
Norms influence human behavior in two ways. First, they Regan, 1993). The disciplinary training one receives in
impart meaning to the situations and stimuli a person encoun- economics doubtless influences the meaning one assigns to
ters and to the different courses of action a person must ambiguous situations such as the prisoner ’s dilemma game
choose between. This is a big component of what social psy- (“Is this about maximizing my own profit or about being
chologists have long referred to as “informational social cooperative?”). But in so doing, it also influences the iden-
influence” (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). Norms provide tities one ascribes to an individual who cooperates (a kind
answers to the implicit questions “What kind of situation is soul vs. a sap) or defects (a selfish opportunist vs. a savvy
this?” and “What would this response mean?” A series of player). The two components of the power of norms—the
studies by Kay, Wheeler, Bargh, and Ross (2004) provides assignment of meaning to situations and the assignment of
an informative illustration (see also Chen, Lee-Chai, & identities to individuals—cannot be cleanly separated.
Bargh, 2001; Fitzsimons, Chartrand, & Fitzsimons, 2008; Nearly all social behavior can be viewed through the
Gilovich, 1981). They exposed some participants to several lens of people’s understanding, often implicit, of prevailing
objects associated with business environments—a brief- norms and the importance they attach to them (Bicchieri,
case, a boardroom table, fountain pens—before having 2006; Cialdini, 2007; Cialdini, Kallgren, & Reno, 1991;
them participate in the ultimatum game. In this game, one McKirnan, 1980; Miller, 2006). The growing influence of
of two participants proposes how a sum of money given by this insight from social psychology on JDM is apparent
the experimenter should be split between them. The second in several areas, including several demonstrations of the
participant can either accept the proposed split or reject it, in influence of norms in commercial and economic contexts.
which case neither participant receives anything (Camerer & Consider two illustrative examples. Goldstein, Cialdini,
Thaler, 1995; Güth, 1995; Thaler, 1988). As Kay and col- and Griskevicius (2008) examined the efforts of hotel
leagues anticipated, exposure to the objects associated with managers to get their “guests” to reuse their towels as part
the business world activated the competitive norms associ- of an effort to conserve water and energy. They found that
ated with that environment, leading those participants to when the small card placed in the bathroom urging guests
make less generous offers compared with those made by to reuse their towels contained a statement that a majority
participants in a control condition. of past guests had chosen to reuse their towels, a signifi-
Norms also influence behavior through the identities cantly higher percentage of the current guests complied.
they establish. They influence both how one views one- Interestingly, the rate of compliance was increased even
self and how one is viewed and treated by others (opening further when the card stated that a majority of guests who
the door to the “normative social influence” long discussed “stayed in this room” reuse their towels. A parallel effect
by social psychologists). To follow a norm is to align one- was observed in an examination of efforts to improve
self with others and thereby signal respect for their take income tax compliance (Coleman, 1996). Tax payers in
on the world. To go against the norm, in contrast, is to Minnesota were sent one of several letters containing dif-
stand against others and can therefore be an implicit slap ferent types of information—for example, what services
in the face, an action not to be taken lightly. To go against their tax dollars provide, what would happen if they were
the norm is to be a renegade, for both good (if that is the found not to be in compliance, and how they might get
identify one seeks) or for ill (if one would prefer to fit in). help in filling out their returns. One letter informed tax-
Note that our review of research on norms in both JDM payers of the norm of compliance: “Many Minnesotans
and social psychology is especially selective, both because believe other people routinely cheat on their taxes. This is
much of the relevant literature is covered in the chapter on not true, however. Audits by the Internal Revenue Service
social influence (Hogg, volume 2) and because we exam- show that people who file tax returns report correctly and
ine how meaning is assigned to situations and to actions pay voluntarily 93 percent of the income taxes they owe.
in our discussion of construal that follows. Note also that Most taxpayers file their returns accurately and on time.”
it is not possible to completely separate the two ways that Notably, only the letter informing citizens of the typical
norms have their influence because the first, the meaning behavior of their peers significantly increased compliance.
one attaches to a stimulus or one’s response to it, deter- Certainly the biggest determinant of the prevailing
mines the second, the sorts of identities that fall out or can norms, and one’s understanding of those norms, is the
Concepts That Have Arisen Independently in Social Psychology and Judgment and Decision Making 565

culture in which one is embedded. A culture is a nexus of have attempted to generalize the notion of utility to include
norms. It stands to reason, then, that a great deal of recent the pleasure of seeing others appropriately rewarded.
work on the impact of norms on judgment and decision Rabin (1993) included the psychological concept of attri-
making would take a cross-cultural perspective. This has butions in the model of “fairness equilibriums,” arguing
been particularly true of research on cooperation and that the utility of a given action in experimental games is
resource allocation. This work takes its start from earlier determined not only by the behavior of the other but also
evidence that people do not act in the completely self- by interpretations of the other ’s intentions, leading to a
interested fashion postulated by traditional economics. In positive utility for seeing the unkind punished and the kind
the ultimatum game, for example, the traditional economic rewarded. Camerer and Thaler (1995, p. 216) went beyond
prediction is clear: If splitting, say, $10, the first person will the concept of utility and concluded that “the outcomes of
propose keeping $9.99 and giving $0.01 to the other. The ultimatum, dictatorship, and many other bargaining games
second person will accept, on the principle that a penny is have more to do with manners than altruism.” In other
more than nothing. This virtually never happens. In reality, words, even in economics, norms rule!
50–50 offers are common and those much below 50–50
are often rejected.
There thus appears to be a powerful norm of fairness
governing people’s behavior in the ultimatum game. CONCEPTS THAT HAVE ARISEN
Proposers’ awareness of the norm makes them disinclined INDEPENDENTLY IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
to make an offer much different from 50–50, and they AND JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING
recognize that the other person is aware of the norm and
hence unlikely to accept an imbalanced offer. Indeed, brain To understand social life, one must understand how people
imaging studies have shown that imbalanced offers tend make judgments about the social stimuli they confront and
to activate brain regions such as the anterior insular cortex the social dilemmas they face. And to understand judg-
that are associated with disgust (Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, ment, one must understand that many of our most impor-
Nystrom, & Cohen, 2002). But is this the case in all cul- tant judgments are made in a social context and are about
tures? One study compared the behavior in the ultimatum social phenomena. It thus stands to reason that, as we have
game of the Machiguenga, a largely hunter–gatherer peo- seen, social psychologists have drawn productively on
ple in the Peruvian Amazon, with that of various control ideas developed in the field of JDM and JDM research-
groups consisting of participants from Pittsburgh, Tokyo, ers have drawn productively on ideas developed in social
Jerusalem, and so on (Henrich, 2000). Machiguengan pro- psychology. And given this substantial overlap in the con-
posers were stingier than their counterparts in the other cerns of both fields, it would be odd indeed if some impor-
societies, offering the other person an average of only 26% tant ideas and perspectives had not arisen independently in
of the total. The Machiguengan respondents did not seem both social psychology and JDM.
to expect more than this, as nearly all offers were accepted, As it happens, there are several examples of similar
even the stingiest (less than 5% of the offers were refused). ideas being developed in the two fields, and we focus on
More extensive examination (Henrich et al., 2001) has three of them. We first discuss how both fields have strug-
linked the extent to which participants adhere to norms of gled to understand how people make some judgments and
fairness to the degree of market integration in their culture. decisions relatively quickly and intuitively and others with
Markets require mutually beneficial exchange and thus greater effort and deliberation. We then discuss the impor-
encourage norms of fairness and trust. These norms define tance both fields have attached to the way people construct
imbalanced offers as unfair, triggering emotions like anger or construe the stimuli they encounter. We end with a dis-
and disgust that lead to rejection of offers that, in the short cussion of the emphasis in both fields on what might be
term at least, would advance participants’ material inter- called the “givenness” of experience. Social psychologists
ests. Interestingly, Murnighan and Saxon (1998) found that have explored people’s predisposition toward naïve real-
American kindergarteners acted much like respondents in ism, or the tendency to treat one’s understandings of the
cultures with little market integration, being more willing world not as subjective constructions but as direct reflec-
to accept a minimal contribution in the ultimatum game tions of how the world really is. Research in JDM has
(one candy M&M or 1 cent) than were older children. This uncovered a parallel tendency for people to accept a given
implicates the role of learned norms in perceived fairness. frame or construction of a problem without seeing, or try-
The predominance of nonselfish behavior in experimen- ing to see, how it can be framed or constructed differently
tal games is a major challenge for economic theory rooted and what the implications of alternative frames and con-
in utility maximization. However, several recent models structions might be.
566 Judgment and Decision Making

Table 15.2 Attributes Generally Attributed to the Two Systems of People are likened to “cognitive misers” (Fiske & Taylor,
Human Thought 1991) who follow a “principle of least effort” in forming
System 1 System 2 impressions of others and thus can be expected to rely
Fast Slow heavily on relatively effortless top-down processes (Allport,
Automatic Deliberate 1954; Eagly & Chaiken, 1993; Tajfel, 1969).
Associative Rule based
This distinction between top-down and bottom-up pro-
cesses, with top-down processing viewed as less effortful,
Relatively effortless Effortful
is present in Fiske and Neuberg’s (1990) continuum model
Parallel operations Serial operations
of person perception. In this model, perceivers are thought
Concrete Can be abstract
to quickly (and often automatically) assign target individu-
Nonconscious Conscious als to a category and then apply the attributes of the cat-
egory to the individual in question. The target person is
Dual-Process and Two-Systems Theories understood in terms of these attributes. If the attributes do
not fit other information about the target, if they violate
As Neisser (1963) noted in an early review of dual
the perceiver ’s preferences, or if the perceiver is espe-
“modes” of cognition, “The psychology of thinking seems
cially motivated to form an accurate impression, this initial
to breed dichotomies.” Consistent with this, social psy-
category-based impression is effortfully refined or replaced
chologists and JDM researchers have both recognized
by individuating information about the target person. It
that people appear to approach various cognitive tasks in
is assumed that “perceivers tend toward category-based
two ways (Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Evans, 2004; Sloman,
processes as default processes” (Fiske, Lin, & Neuberg,
1996; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). One involves mental pro-
1999).
cesses that are fast, associationist, and often automatic
With varying levels of commitment, many of the most
and uncontrolled. The other involves processes that are
prominent dual-process models in social psychology also
slower, rule based, and more deliberate. Scholars in both
imply something of an either–or operation of the two sets
disciplines have devoted a lot of energy trying to specify
of mental operations. Perceivers are thought to most often
the nature of these two types of processes, or “systems”
go with an immediate, category-based judgment and to
of thought, and to delineate when each is operative and
sometimes supplant that judgment with a more careful,
how they interact when people make a judgment or choose
individuated analysis. The continuum model (as the name
a course of action. The two systems have been given many
suggests) specifies some feedback between the two pro-
names and, following Stanovich (1999), we refer to them
cesses, resulting in impressions that can be a blend of the
simply as system 1 and system 2 for ease of exposition.
two. Gilbert’s (1989, 2002) correction model also entails
The most agreed-on characteristics of the two systems are
considerable interaction between the two systems. An
depicted in Table 15.2.
initial, effortless impression of people in line with their
behavior is corrected by a more deliberate and effortful
Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology
recognition of prevailing situational constraints.
Dual-process or two-systems theories arose in social psy- But an either–or nature of the two processes is empha-
chology out of the effort to understand how people attach sized in other influential dual-process accounts, such as
meaning to the information they encounter. How does Petty and Cacioppo’s (1986) elaboration likelihood model
one know that a gesture is threatening, a person is kind, of persuasion. The elaboration likelihood model proposes
or a persuasive message is worth taking seriously? Doing two routes to persuasion—a central route in which persua-
so clearly takes some combination of top-down, theory- sive arguments are thought about in depth and detail, and a
driven, categorical processes on the one hand and bottom- peripheral route in which one attends (minimally) to more
up, data-driven, piecemeal processes on the other. Some superficial features of the persuasive communication, such
have argued that bottom-up processing is more effortful as how many arguments were offered and who offered them
than top-down processing. In their review of the history (Albarracı́n & Vargas, this volume). Note that the theory’s
of dual-process theories in social psychology, for example, very labels testify to the presumed either–or nature of the
Moskowitz, Skurnik, and Galinsky (1999) explicitly equate two processes: One cannot be in two places at one time;
top-down processes with effortless, mindless processes and hence, one is either on one “route” or the other. This either–or
bottom-up processes with effortful, mindful processes. It is assumption is also present in a close cousin of the elaboration
an important assumption in nearly all dual-process models likelihood model, Chaiken’s (1980) heuristic–systematic
in social psychology that people are motivated to minimize model of persuasion. This assumption of the model
the amount of effort they devote to information processing. is implicit in the statement that “recipients will employ
Concepts That Have Arisen Independently in Social Psychology and Judgment and Decision Making 567

a systematic strategy when reliability concerns outweigh Dual-Process Theories in Judgment


economic concerns, and a heuristic strategy when eco- and Decision Making
nomic concerns predominate” (Chaiken, 1980, p. 754). Note
Dual-process theories in JDM arose in response to questions
that it is one or the other.
different from those that motivated social psychologists, so
Another important building block in the development
it stands to reason that the accounts in the two fields differ
of dual-process theories in social psychology was the
somewhat in their details. JDM came to consider the ques-
awareness that social perception involves both implicit and
tion of whether there were two systems of thought by the
explicit processes and draws on both implicit and explicit
simple observation that when making various judgments
knowledge. This awareness was aided by Schneider and
and (especially) decisions, people often experience a con-
Shiffrin’s (1977) work on automatic and controlled infor-
flict between a “gut feeling” and a more considered analy-
mation processing, by Nisbett and Wilson’s (1977) influ-
sis (Denes-Raj & Epstein, 1994; Epstein, 1991; Hammond,
ential paper on people’s lack of awareness of many of their
1996; Sloman, 1996; Tversky & Kahneman, 1983). JDM
most consequential thought processes (see also Wilson,
thus inherited the long-standing concern of philosophers
2002), and from outside of psychology, by the availability
about the nature of “intuition” and “reason.”
of desktop computers. The latter made it easy for research-
To the extent that there is a consensus in JDM about the
ers to use subliminal priming and reaction time procedures
features of system 1 and system 2, it is a consensus that has
to reveal the frequent rift between implicit attitudes and
evolved slowly. Several strands of research were particu-
beliefs, on the one hand, and consciously accessible con-
larly influential. One was the study of reasoning, which
victions, on the other (Banaji & Heiphetz, this volume).
uncovered evidence of apparent conflict between two
One particularly influential paper used such procedures
to demonstrate that pejorative stereotypes can influence types of thinking on the part of participants asked to tackle
the inferences made by individuals who would (genuinely) various deductive and inductive problems. (To our knowl-
deny making any negative assumptions about members of edge, the term “dual processes” first appeared in Wason &
an outgroup (Devine, 1989). Devine subliminally presented Evans, 1975.) In particular, “belief bias” in syllogistic rea-
participants with either neutral words or words stereo- soning (i.e., the tendency to accept syllogistic conclusions
typically associated with African Americans (“welfare,” to the extent that they agree with prior beliefs) and “match-
“jazz,” and “busing”) and then had both groups read a ing bias” in the Wason selection task (i.e., selecting cards
vignette about a person who acted in an ambiguously hos- that match those mentioned in the rule to be tested) came to
tile manner. Participants who had just been primed with be seen as results of an intuitive system yielding tenta-
words associated with African Americans, even those tive responses based on relatively superficial features of
who showed no trace of bias on the modern racism scale the information presented—responses that are sometimes
(McConahay, Hardee, & Batts, 1981), rated the person as overridden by more reflective evaluations of an analytical
more hostile and more negative overall. system (see Evans, 2004, 2007, 2008, for reviews). Such
This and other conceptually (if not methodologically) a conclusion was strongly reinforced by evidence that
similar studies led to all manner of theorizing about the performance on tasks that tap mainly analytical processes
nature of implicit and explicit attitudes and to consider- tends to correlate with overall cognitive ability but per-
able controversy over whether and how readily implicit formance on tasks that largely tap intuitive processes do
attitudes can be consciously controlled (Fazio & Olson, not (Stanovich, 1999; Stanovich & West, 2008). This bol-
2003; Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006). It also inspired stered the idea that judgment is controlled by at least two
several empirical studies that probe the potentially destruc- types of cognitive processes with different constraints.
tive real-world consequences of implicit stereotyping and Perhaps the most dramatic evidence of two mental sys-
prejudice on the part of individuals who would deny being tems that guide judgment and behavior is Epstein’s work
in any way bigoted (Dovidio & Gaertner, volume 2). Most on the “ratio bias” phenomenon (Denes-Raj & Epstein,
noteworthy are studies showing that participants who deny 1994; Epstein, 1991). Epstein told participants that they
having any prejudice toward African Americans (nota- could win a prize by blindly selecting a jellybean of a given
bly, in some experiments, African American participants color from one of two urns. One urn had 1 winning jelly-
themselves) are more likely to decide that a target person bean and 9 of another, losing color. The second urn had,
is holding a handgun rather than an innocuous object if say, 9 winning jellybeans and 91 of the losing color. The
the target person is Black than if the target is White—and participants’ task was to select an urn from which to draw
to react accordingly (Correll, Park, Judd, & Wittenbrink, and then to try to pull out a winner. What Epstein found
2002; Judd, Blair, & Chapleau, 2004; Payne, 2001; Payne, was that many participants chose to select from the larger
Lambert, & Jacoby, 2002). urn that offered lower odds of winning because they could
568 Judgment and Decision Making

not resist the thought that the larger urn had more winning system 1 automatically computes an assessment with
beans. They did so even though the chances of winning with some connection to the task at hand—an emotional reac-
each of the urns were explicitly provided for them. When tion, a sense of fluency, the similarity between examples
the choice was between a 10% chance in the small urn or between an example and a category. Both the perceived
and a 9% chance in the large urn, 61% of the participants relevance of the assessment and its immediacy often give
chose the large urn. When it was a contest between 10% in rise to the sense that the task is done and that the assess-
the small urn and 5% in the large urn—odds only half as ment produced by system 1 is the answer being sought.
good in the latter—23% of the participants still chose the For example, one cause of death is judged to be more com-
large urn. mon than another because it is easier to think of examples
Epstein attributes this decidedly irrational result to an of the former (Slovic, Fischhoff, & Lichtenstein, 1982).
“experiential” system of reasoning that operates on con- One attribute (ease of retrieval) substitutes for another,
crete representations and hence finds the greater number desired attribute (likelihood).
of winning jellybeans in the large urn to be more promis- In many circumstances, however, and for various rea-
ing. This experiential or intuitive impulse, however, usu- sons, system 2 intervenes and deems the automatic assess-
ally conflicts with the rational realization that the actual ment inadequate for the task at hand. A more deliberate,
odds are better in the small urn. Some participants explic- rule-based response is given. For example, one might
itly stated that they knew they should pick the smaller urn, consciously realize, especially if one has received training
but they nonetheless were going with a gut feeling that in statistics and threats to validity, that a given cause of
they were more likely to pick a winner from the large one. death is highly available because it is often discussed in
This experience of being pulled in two different directions the media. Kahneman and Frederick’s attribute substitu-
suggests that there are two things—two mental systems— tion model has captured a great deal of attention because it
doing the pulling. This was emphasized by Sloman (1996), offered a unified account of a diverse set judgmental phe-
who described a possible cognitive architecture consist- nomena, such as the impact of heuristics, duration neglect,
ing of two relatively independent systems to explain the and the problems inherent in contingent valuation methods
diverse findings implicating dual processes in reasoning, used to assess people’s willingness to pay for such things
choice, and judgment. as environmental remediation.

Similarities, Differences, and Extensions The Two-Systems Frontier


Although developed to address different questions, the Although dual-process theories in social psychology
two-systems or dual-process models in JDM closely had somewhat different origins and therefore differed in
resemble those in social psychology. Because the system some details from those developed in JDM, the interac-
1 processes highlighted by JDM researchers are fast and tion between the two fields has been pronounced and their
relatively untaxing, they are consistent with the first wave perspectives have converged over time. Current theorizing
of dual-process theories in social psychology that empha- in both fields, furthermore, is being shaped by the same
sized cognitive economy. But as we saw earlier, many of forces. One of these is the field of neuroscience. Brain
those models specified relatively effortless and effortful imaging studies have, in fairly short order, uncovered evi-
processes that operate in a largely one-or-the-other fash- dence consistent with the broad outlines of a two-systems
ion (although some did allow for interaction and mutual view of judgment and decision making.
influence; see Gilbert, 1999). In contrast, the two-systems One notable study (Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom,
accounts in JDM, precisely because they were inspired Darley, & Cohen, 2001) found that people’s different reac-
by the conflict between rational and intuitive impulses, tions to the moral dilemmas posed in the “trolley” and
more pointedly emphasized the contemporaneous, side- “footbridge” dilemmas are matched by different patterns
by-side operation of the two systems. System 1 operates of brain activation. In the trolley dilemma, participants are
more quickly, so its output is often available sooner. But told that a trolley is heading down a track and will run over
according to these accounts, one does not “choose” to and kill an unseen group of five people in its path. The
respond reflexively because of laziness. System 2 is typi- five deaths can be avoided, however, if the participant is
cally engaged as well, unless the output of system 1 seems willing to flip a switch, which will move it to another track
particularly compelling. and lead to the death of a single individual. Most people
Kahneman and Frederick (2002) highlighted these endorse the idea of flipping the switch to reduce the car-
relations between system 1 and system 2 in their influ- nage from five to one. And, if asked to think about this
ential restatement of the heuristics and biases program dilemma while in a functional magnetic resonance imag-
of research. In their “attribute substitution” account, ing machine, they reveal activation in regions associated
Concepts That Have Arisen Independently in Social Psychology and Judgment and Decision Making 569

with working memory—consistent with the idea that they conceptions of the two systems of thought is Dijksterhuis’s
are rationally deliberating about the proper response. In work on the capacity of what he calls nonconscious
the footbridge dilemma, a trolley headed down a track is, thought to make accurate judgments and beneficial deci-
again, about to kill five people, but participants are asked to sions (Dijksterhuis, 2004; Dijksterhuis, Bos, Nordgren, &
imagine that they are standing next to a heavyset man on a von Baaren, 2006). Dijksterhuis argues that refraining
footbridge spanning the track. The man can be pushed off from consciously thinking about a decision and instead
the footbridge, killing him but sending the train off the track turning the task over to nonconscious thought typically
and sparing the five individuals in its path. (It is explained leads to higher-quality decisions if the decision is com-
that the participant is not heavy enough to derail the train— plex (Dijksterhuis, this volume). He bases this prediction
ruling out a self-sacrificial solution—and that the derailing on the relatively unlimited capacity of the associative, par-
of the train would not result in harm to anyone on board.) allel processes of nonconscious thought and the limited,
Most people say it is not appropriate to push the person off serial processes of consciousness. Dijksterhuis argues that
the footbridge even though it involves the very one-for-five if a great number of different cues need to be integrated to
exchange that they endorse in the trolley dilemma. And make the best decision, conscious thought will not be up
thinking about this dilemma reveals enhanced activation to the task. Better decisions will be made if the decision-
of brain regions associated with emotional responding— maker is distracted and the decision is made largely non-
consistent with the “gut feeling” that it would be wrong to consciously. The experimental demonstrations of this
sacrifice the man, even if it would save five others. “deliberation-without-attention” effect have been contro-
Building on findings such as this, Lieberman (2003; versial (Acker, 2008; Evans, 2008). The point we wish to
Lieberman, Gaunt, Gilbert, & Trope, 2002; Lieberman, make here is that if the effect were to prove reliable and
Jarcho, & Satpute, 2004) presented neuroscientific evi- robust (and if participants in nonconscious thinking con-
dence in support of an X system (for reflexive) and a C ditions really are not devoting any conscious thought to
system (for reflective). The reflective system involves the the task), it would constitute something of a challenge
prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate nucleus, and to current perspectives on the two systems of judgment.
the medial–temporal lobe. These regions have been impli- The results do not fit with the view that a set of quick
cated in executive control and explicit learning and thus assessments are made by system 1, which are then (some-
are responsible for most of the heavy lifting of system 2. times) elaborated and modified by system 2. In this case,
The reflexive system, in contrast, consists, in part, of the both systems do a lot of deliberating.
basal ganglia, the amygdala, and the lateral temporal cor- Theoretical anomalies like that of Dijksterhuis and fur-
tex. These regions have been linked to associative learning ther neuroscientific experiments are especially important
and hence the implicit processes of system 1. given how firmly dual-process theories have taken root in
Another influence on current views of the two systems both social psychology and JDM. At least some reference
of thought is the recognition that system 1 is almost cer- is made to reflexive and deliberate thought in virtually all
tainly not as unitary as system 2. System 2 may indeed be textbook and trade book treatments of human cognition
a single system involving deliberate, conscious, rule-based and decision making. And several scholars have confessed
cognition. System 1, in contrast, may be more accurately that they worry that the distinction may be too convenient
described as a set of processes with some similar properties. and that they are now hard pressed to think about any
Stanovich (2004) refers to them as a “set of autonomous higher-order cognitive output without reference to the two-
subsystems.” The processes assigned to system 1 include systems dichotomy. It is precisely in situations like this in
(1) some sort of general, associative learning mechanism; which theoretical perspectives benefit from being tested
(2) pragmatic inference processes that nonconsciously sup- and pushed to the limit.
ply system 2 with information for conscious processing;
(3) once-deliberate processes that have become automated The Primacy of Representation:
as a result of frequent use; and (4) something like a set There’s No “There” in There
of modules devoted to perception and language process-
ing. A key question that will doubtless attract attention in Both social psychology and JDM rely on insights about
the coming years is whether an increased understanding of the malleable, constructed nature of the represented world
these subsystems will undermine the whole two-systems to explain many of their central findings. This parallel
framework or whether it will still be conceptually useful to development is not surprising given the influence of per-
link them together as the drivers of human thought. ceptual psychology, especially Gestalt psychology, on the
Another development that has attracted attention and fields’ founders, including Kurt Lewin, Soloman Asch, and
might be considered something of a challenge to current Fritz Heider in social psychology and Egon Brunswik,
570 Judgment and Decision Making

Ken Hammond, and Daniel Kahneman in JDM. As to embrace such instability—not explain it away as
described earlier, Gestalt psychology focuses on how the measurement or response “errors”—and to account for
perceptual system constructs meaningful patterns out of changing and unstable behaviors and choices in a sys-
isolated cues. A few lines can suggest the face of an old tematic way. Both fields emphasize the importance of the
witch; but looking again, the viewer sees a smiling girl. “three Cs” in understanding the malleability of behavior.
Thus, the act of construction can cause the same cues to That is, reactions to a given stimulus differ across time and
give rise to different experiences, experiences that seem situations because of (1) the construal of the object of judg-
equally real. A person’s perceptions, moreover, although ment; (2) the construction of the perceiver ’s own attitudes,
constructed, feel immediate, raw, and impervious to delib- values, and preferences; and (3) the context-dependent
erate reconstruction. processes of evaluation. The choice between an apple and
Beyond this common foundation, there are two other a piece of chocolate cake depends not only on the construal
reasons that fated social psychology and JDM to embrace of the object (is the apple a symbol of self-deprivation or of
construction and construal as primary mechanisms. First, the fresh delights of Mother Nature, is the cake represented
each field faced a hegemonistic neighbor. For social psy- as a tempting treat or as a member of an abstract category
chology, the neighbor was behaviorism, which threatened of desserts) but also on the guiding attitudes and values
to engulf all of American psychology in the 1930s in that come to mind (perhaps a prior donation to a charity
stimulus–response learning theory. According to the behav- has “licensed” indulgence as the guiding consideration)
iorists, social behavior such as aggression, compliance, and and the decision processes and routines suggested by the
prejudice could be explained, predicted, and controlled in surrounding context (perhaps adding a salad to the menu
terms of the objective contingencies, the punishments of choices would highlight the distinctive properties of
and rewards, that followed behavior. This explanatory the cake).
rival pushed social psychology to be even more cogni- Situational construal (Griffin & Ross, 1991; Ross &
tivist in outlook and to emphasize the importance of the Nisbett, 1991) refers to the subjective representation
individual’s active search for meaning in social situations. of a stimulus or a person’s “definition of the situation”
In particular, it gave extra prominence to demonstrations (Thomas & Znaniecki, 1918). The notion that an individ-
that seemed to defy reinforcement or stimulus–response ual’s response to a situation can be predicted only from
explanations—for example, that less money could lead to knowledge of the meaning assigned to it has long been a
greater attitude change (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959), that central tenet of social psychology (Fazio, 1990), a perspec-
too much praise or unnecessary rewards could undermine tive powerfully shaped by Jerome Bruner ’s (1957a, 1957b)
intrinsic motivation (Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973), or account of how people “go beyond the information given”
that a person’s reaction to an event was a critical deter- to determine the meaning of a social stimulus and by Asch’s
minant of the degree of emotion it elicited (Schachter & (1940, 1952) discussion of “change of meaning” effects in
Singer, 1962). persuasion and conformity. Following Kant (1965/1781),
For JDM, the threatening neighbor has always been eco- Bruner pointed out that all perceptual and social stimuli
nomics, with its well-developed formal theories built on are inherently ambiguous (e.g., a secondhand account of a
the twin foundations of hyperrationality and self-interest, riot) and cannot be understood without the perceiver “fill-
which also assigned a privileged role to (objective) finan- ing in” details of context and content (e.g., the intentions
cial incentives. Again, this led to a concern with phenom- and expressions of the rioters). He also proposed that in
ena that were interestingly countereconomic—how more addition to chronic differences among people in their biases
choice can lead to more misery rather than more pleasure toward interpreting or categorizing behavior along certain
(Iyengar, Jiang, & Huberman, 2004; Iyengar & Lepper, dimensions—construal biases that might result from differ-
2000; Schwartz, 2004) or how 60 seconds of ownership ing motivations or ideologies (Hastorf & Cantril, 1954)—
can lead to a doubling of the perceived value of a mug or there is also systematic variability within people in the
pen (Kahneman et al., 1991). way they interpret or categorize the same stimulus at differ-
Both fields also confronted the empirical fact of wide- ent times. In particular, an individual may construe the same
spread instability and unpredictability of behavior. Why act, event, or object in different ways according to whatever
was it so difficult for social psychologists to predict on category label is most cognitively accessible at the moment
the basis of measured attitudes how people would behave or what aspects of the situation happen to be most imme-
in the polling booth or when told to administer electric diately salient or eye catching. This provided a conceptual
shock? Why did people change their consumer choices foundation for the remarkably rich body of research that has
when an additional but less-preferred alternative was added used priming manipulations to shape situational construal
to the choice set (Simonson, 1989)? Both fields needed by altering the cognitive accessibility of different labels or
Concepts That Have Arisen Independently in Social Psychology and Judgment and Decision Making 571

schemas (Gilovich, 1981; Higgins & Bargh, 1987; Wyer & primarily in terms of their feasibility (Liberman & Trope,
Srull, 1989), goals (Ferguson & Bargh, 2004), or identities 1998). Note that the basic principles of temporal “distance”
(Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martínez, 2000). apply to social and physical distance as well (Trope &
The key mediating role of representation or construal Liberman, 2003; Trope, Liberman, & Wakslak, 2007).
has remained in the forefront of social psychology and has These differences leave people open to manipulation
been used to explain such biases as the actor–observer because the definition of “near” is to some extent arbitrary
difference in attribution (Jones & Nisbett, 1972), the solo and can be set by context and instructions (e.g., Broemer
actor effect (Taylor & Fiske, 1978), the false consensus Grabowski, Gebauer, Ermel, & Diehl, 2008).
effect (Ross, Greene, & House, 1977), and the fundamen- Representation and construal also play decisive roles
tal attribution error (Ross, 1977; Ross & Nisbett, 1991). in judgment biases and attempts to ameliorate them.
Gilovich (1990) provided direct evidence that construal Consider base rate neglect and the role of causal versus
differences were associated with false consensus, the ten- statistical representation, described earlier. When the
dency to exaggerate the commonness of one’s own beliefs, base rate of accidents for one particular taxi company is
preferences, and actions. In one study, he found that those described purely statistically (85% of cabs in the city are
who preferred 1960s music to 1980s music brought to mind Blue cabs), this information is ignored in favor of the tes-
different exemplars of each era than did those who pre- timony of a witness (Ajzen, 1977; Tversky & Kahneman,
ferred 1980s music—and that the sixties’ exemplars from 1980). However, when the base rate is described in causal
the sixties’ lovers were indeed more consensually positive terms that imply a propensity for some types of cabs to
when rated by independent judges. In a social psycho- have accidents (85% of the accidents in the city involve
logical study with direct implications for JDM, Liberman, Blue cabs), the information is readily used, presumably be-
Samuels, and Ross (2002) found that merely renaming the cause the causal base rate or individual propensity is
prisoner ’s dilemma game as the “community game” led to compatible with such system 1 assessments as associa-
twice as much cooperation as renaming it the “Wall Street tive strength and causal simulation. A similar explanation
game.” speaks to the apparent paradox between the overuse of
Liberman and Trope’s (1998) temporal construal theory stereotypes (which imply a propensity for the members
spans social psychology and JDM, but its explicit focus of the category to act a particular way) and the underuse of
on the content of representation places it firmly in the statistical base rates.
social psychology tradition. According to the theory, and Many attempts at debiasing judgments focus on making
substantiated in many empirical tests, events or options in set-based representations more readily available; in essence,
the near future are represented in terms of low-level concrete using representations to engage system 2 reasoning rather
attributes embedded in a context; those in the distant future than system 1 operations. For example, many authors (e.g.,
are represented in terms of high-level abstractions removed Cosmides & Tooby, 1996; Gigerenzer & Hoffrage, 1995;
from any particular context (Liberman, Sagristano, & Kahneman & Tversky, 1983) have proposed that problems
Trope, 2002; Liberman & Trope, 2008; Trope & Liberman, presented in a frequency format (e.g., how many accidents
2003; Wakslak, Nussbaum, Liberman, & Trope, 2008). A out of 100 will involve a Blue cab?) should result in more
request to write a chapter that is due in 6 months leads to sound statistical reasoning than problems presented in a
a representation in terms of a rewarding scholarly accom- probability format (e.g., what is the probability that an acci-
plishment, whereas a chapter due next week is represented dent will involve a Blue cab?). However, different authors
in terms of the painful specific actions needed to complete call on different theories to account for the facilitation of
it within a well-specified context. More abstract, decon- statistical reasoning by frequency formats. Kahneman and
textualized (future) representations lead to more optimistic Tversky (1983) proposed that frequency formats trigger a
expectations because the low-level details that make them representation that makes the set inclusion relations more
difficult are absent from the representation. Numerous transparent and hence increase the apparent relevance of
demonstrations testify that confidence and optimism are set-based reasoning that underlies statistical logic. Others
higher for events in the far future and that such events explain the superior performance of frequency formats by
are represented in a simpler, more summary fashion appealing to evolved special-purpose reasoning modules
(Gilovich, Kerr, & Medvec, 1993; Nussbaum, Liberman, & that are compatible with natural frequencies but not prob-
Trope, 2006; Shepperd, Ouellete, & Fernandez, 1996; abilities (e.g., Cosmides & Tooby, 1996; Gigerenzer &
Taylor & Shepperd, 1998). Another central and well- Hoffrage, 1995). Overall, the evidence for the efficacy of
documented aspect of temporal construal theory is that frequency formats alone to improve Bayesian performance
distant-future events are evaluated primarily in terms of is mixed (Griffin & Buehler, 1999; Tversky & Kahneman,
their desirability, whereas near-future events are evaluated 1983; Barbey & Sloman, 2007) and most consistent with
572 Judgment and Decision Making

the view that frequency presentations and other format sushi is also listed on the menu (independence of irrele-
changes are effective when they engage a problem repre- vant alternatives). Violations of these principles not only
sentation that highlights and makes concrete the set inclu- have special significance in theoretical terms, but also cast
sion relations. The most effective way to do this appears to doubt on the most fundamental tool of applied economics,
be to draw out the probability logic in graphic formats such the idea that individuals have a unique “reference price”
as decision trees or Venn diagrams, rather than to rely on a or “stable preference” for each outcome (Knetsch & Tang,
frequency format per se (see Barbey & Sloman, 2007, for 2003; Loomes, 1999).
a review).
The idea of constructive preferences goes beyond a mere denial
Thinking about aggregate frequencies rather than indi-
that observed preferences result from reference to a master list in
vidual outcomes may not lead to Bayesian reasoning, but
memory. . . . It appears that decision-makers have a repertoire
it does change many important aspects of the problem rep- of methods for identifying their preferences and developing
resentation. As charity fund-raisers well know, individual their beliefs. (Payne, Bettman, & Johnson, 1992, p. 89)
bears, tigers, or babies evoke a more emotionally charged
representation than groups or aggregates—the “identifi- In a pioneering demonstration, Tversky (1969) showed
able victim effect” (Jenni & Loewenstein, 1997; Small & that people would make intransitive choices in even the
Loewenstein, 2003; Small, Loewenstein, & Slovic, 2007). most simple problems. Recall that transitivity is a key
For example, Redelmeier and Tversky (1990) found that normative axiom and one that virtually all people read-
practicing physicians gave different treatment recommen- ily endorse in the abstract. Moreover, intransitivity is
dations when they were presented with a problem faced not merely a theoretical nicety: Intransitivity cannot be
by a specific young woman than when told about the same rationalized because it can lead a consumer to become
problem faced by a set of young women. Those presented a “money pump”: Because pizza is preferred to falafel,
with the individual case were almost twice as likely to rec- the consumer will pay a premium to give up falafel and
ommend an additional blood test and were significantly receive pizza; then because sushi is preferred to pizza, the
more likely to suggest a follow-up appointment than those consumer will pay a premium to give up pizza and receive
who were presented with the aggregate case. sushi. However, an intransitive chooser could prefer falafel
The prominent role of normative theories in JDM in to sushi and pay a premium to get the falafel; thus, the
defining what is interesting and worth investigating has led cycle of preference would start again with the consumer
JDM researchers to focus more on value or preference con- paying a premium to receive pizza rather than the falafel
struction than on stimulus construal. Subjectivity enters the currently held, and so on.
SEU model in at least two ways. First, the objective stimu- Tversky’s demonstration required participants to choose
lus (whether money, kisses, or electric shocks) is translated which of a pair of university applicants should be admitted.
into subjective utility through a psychophysical function He carefully set up the attributes of the various applicants
that is characterized by decreasing marginal utility—that so that adjacent pairs differed only slightly in intelligence
is, $2 or two kisses are generally less than twice as reward- (the most important dimension), but more substantially on
ing as one (Bentham, 1789/1948; Bernoulli, 1738/1954). A other dimensions. Thus, Bob would be chosen over Ace
second psychophysical element arises in decisions across even though Ace was somewhat more intelligent, because
time, such that future events, whether rewards or punish- this slight difference was outweighed by Bob being nota-
ments, are discounted and worth less as they extend further bly more sociable and balanced that Ace. Cal would in turn
into the future (Frederick, Loewenstein, & O’Donoghue, be chosen over Bob, Deb would be chosen over Cal, and
2002; Read, 2004). However, once these psychophysi- Ed over Deb for the same reason. However, even though Ed
cal translations have occurred, rational models assume an stood at the top of the pairwise choice chain, he would be
underlying true and enduring preference relation among rejected when paired with Ace, at the bottom of the chain.
alternatives. An individual’s choice among alternatives is Ace would be chosen over Ed because the difference in the
assumed to be consistent, coherent, and determined only most important attribute, intelligence, was too big to ignore
by relevant, available alternatives. If one is willing to walk and overwhelmed even the sizable differences in the less
farther to eat sushi than to eat pizza, then one should also important attributes of balance and sociability. According
pay more to eat sushi than pizza (invariance across mea- to this account, people were using the stimulus array—the
surement, or consistency). If one is willing to pay more choice context—to decide what differences were worth
for sushi than pizza, and more for pizza than falafel, then attending to and hence showed predictable incoherence
one should be willing to pay more for sushi than falafel in their pattern of choices. Tversky suggested that many
(transitivity of choice, or coherence). One’s preference people would show the same preference incoherence when
between pizza and falafel should not depend on whether choosing optional equipment for a car: They would usually
Concepts That Have Arisen Independently in Social Psychology and Judgment and Decision Making 573

choose to add a single additional option because it added a speaker set. Why? Because the moderate price and qual-
negligible difference in price, but they would also tend to ity is a compromise on both dimensions and provides a
choose the basic model over the fully “loaded” car. defensible and convincing argument to buy it. This com-
The notion of a stable, underlying reference price or a promise effect (Simonson, 1989) illustrates the importance
true valuation was further undermined by the observation of of the context in resolving conflict. The context provided
systematic preference reversals between choice and valua- by the other choice options can be said to “construct” an
tion (Lichtenstein & Slovic, 1971, 1973; Tversky, Slovic, & observed preference (Shafir, Simonson, & Tversky, 1993).
Kahneman, 1990). People—including experienced gam- A similar analysis can be made of the attraction effect
blers in Las Vegas—regularly choose high-probability, low- (Simonson, 1989) originally known as the asymmetric
value bets (P bets) over low-probability, high-value bets dominance effect (Huber, Payne, & Puto, 1982). Here, a
($ bets) but place a higher cash equivalent (buying price) difficult choice between two or more options becomes
on $ bets. Thus, a typical participant prefers a P bet offering easier when a dominated option is added to the choice set.
a 35-in-36 chance of winning $4 (and a 1-in-36 chance to Choosing between a laptop computer that is fast but heavy
lose $1) to a $ bet offering an 11-in-36 chance of winning and one that is slower but light becomes easier when a
$16 (and a 25-in-36 chance of losing $1.50) but offers a moderately slow but extremely heavy laptop is added to
higher price to purchase the $ bet than the P bet. So which the choice set. That is, adding an additional option that is
bets do people “really” prefer? The answer, it seems, is “it inferior to one of the original choices (and only one) on all
depends on how they are asked.” This result was so shock- dimensions provides a knockout argument in favor of that
ing in its implications for the lability of preferences that a choice—the fast, heavy laptop dominates the moderately
pair of leading experimental economists (Grether & Plott, slow, extremely heavy laptop. This provides a clear reason
1979) conducted a series of follow-up studies, offering as to choose one of the original laptops over the other. Even
one hypothesis that the nonrationality of the subjects was though the dominated model is irrelevant and should be
due to the original studies being conducted by psycholo- disregarded in the choice process, its presence adds to the
gists! (Note that even when conducted by economists, that attraction of one of the options by providing a strong rea-
is, by Grether and Plott themselves, the preference reversals son to choose it. Again, an element of the choice context
proved robust to economic manipulations such as the mag- guides and even controls preferences.
nitude of incentives.) Slovic, Griffin, and Tversky (1990) The notion of constructed preferences or constructive
explained preference reversals and other related examples choice processes is often explained with reference to an
of violations of procedural invariance in terms of the scale old joke about three umpires discussing their philosophy
compatibility hypothesis—that is, that attributes that are of calling balls and strikes:
more compatible with the output task (e.g., choice or pric-
Umpire 1: I call them as I see them.
ing), in the sense of being easy to translate onto the output
Umpire 2: I call them as they are.
scale, receive more weight than less compatible attributes
(see also Fischer & Hawkins, 1993). In the preceding exam- Umpire 3: They ain’t nothing ’til I call them.
ple, the dollar value of the bet is more compatible with pric- These three epistemological perspectives could be
ing and thus is weighted more heavily in pricing than in called “social construal,” “naïve realism,” and “social con-
choice, leading to greater preference—as implied by higher struction” (Griffin & Ross, 1991). Naïve realism refers to
prices—for the $ bet. Tversky, Sattath, and Slovic (1988) the tendency of social observers to treat their social (and
proposed the related prominence effect, whereby choice physical) perceptions as veridical copies of the outside
(a qualitative procedure) is most influenced by a quali- world. Both social psychology and JDM recognize that
tative comparison of which option is higher on the most many phenomena result not simply because subjective rep-
important dimension and is less affected than quantitative resentation is important but because the act of representa-
procedures—such as pricing, matching, or rating—by the tion is transparent to the actor—that is, social observers
option’s actual values on the set of attributes. look through their lenses rather than at them (Bem, 1993;
A family of related phenomena testifies to people con- Goffman, 1963); hence, social perception is experienced
structing their preferences at least partly based on the stim- as “calling them as they are.”
ulus array itself—that is, the context of choice. Consider a
consumer who is indifferent between a moderately priced, The “Givenness” of Experience and the
average-quality pair of speakers and a high-priced, high- Transparency and Persistence of Representations
quality set. When a low-priced, low-quality pair of speak-
ers is added to the comparison set, the consumer becomes Both social psychologists and JDM researchers acknowledge
considerably more favorable toward the moderately priced that often it is not enough to explain certain phenomena by
574 Judgment and Decision Making

noting that the objective world is represented subjectively contaminates their ability to forecast others’ behavior. The
and that people act on their subjective representations. It implication of the curse of knowledge for overconfidence
is also necessary to understand that subjective represen- and human misunderstanding was illustrated by Newton
tations are experienced as objective copies of the world. (1990) in a notable tapping study (described in Griffin &
Because the represented or perceived world “in here” is Ross, 1991). When Stanford University undergraduates were
experienced phenomenologically as veridically mirroring given a list of songs to communicate to listener subjects by
what is “out there,” human judgment is susceptible to a tapping on a table, the “tappers” were relatively confident—
host of egocentric biases that promote misunderstanding providing estimates of at least a 50–50 chance—that they
and fuel conflict. For example, in a classic demonstration could communicate such tunes as “The Star-Spangled
of the hindsight bias, Fischhoff and Beyth (1975) found Banner.” But the listeners caught on less than 3% of the
that when individuals were provided with the outcome time. This pronounced mismatch between expectation and
of a historical battle, their knowledge contaminated their reality resulted from the tappers’ inability to undo the rich
judgments of the inevitability of that outcome, presumably representation of the song they had in their heads while
because their knowledge altered their construal of the cir- they tapped out their impoverished rhythms.
cumstances at the beginning of the conflict (Hawkins & Lee Ross and colleagues (e.g., Pronin, Gilovich, &
Hastie, 1990; Sanna & Schwartz, 2006). This effect of Ross, 2004; Ross & Nisbett, 1991; Ross & Ward, 1996)
outcome knowledge makes the world seem more predict- have linked naïve realism to the maintenance of ideologi-
able than it is and hence makes individuals who suffer cal enmity and the breakdown of negotiations. If people
misfortune seem more culpable than they are (Kamin & believe that their perceptions are deeply rooted in reality,
Rachlinski, 1995). In their analysis of the curse of knowl- it stands to reason that they would expect any reasonable
edge, Camerer, Loewenstein, and Weber (1989) generalize person to see things the way they do. In Ichheiser ’s classic
this finding to stock traders who have inside knowledge words, “We thus imply, of course, that things are in fact
of a company’s fate. This inside knowledge can be a as we see them, and that our ways are the normal ways.”
curse rather than a blessing because the insiders act as if (1949, p. 39). A summary of the diverse applications of
everyone else perceives the world the same way they do— naïve realism to bias, misperception, and misunderstand-
and they actually lose money because their knowledge ing is presented in Figure 15.2.

Naive Realism

Illusion of
personal objectivity
I “see” the
world “as it is” Unwarranted belief perseverance
Biased assimilation
of new evidence
“Attitude polarization” effects

Overconfidence about
“verdict” of third parties
Reasonable
others should Overconfidence in Assumed “transparency”
“see” it the social prediction of own beliefs
same way
False consensus effect

Perceived ease of persuading


others
Infer other is
If they don’t, uninformed
why? Hostile media/mediator effects
Infer other is biased
False polarization effect
Infer other is guided by
idiosyncratic traits
Fundamental attribution error

Figure 15.2 A guiding model of naïve realism and construal in social psychology.
Concepts That Have Arisen Independently in Social Psychology and Judgment and Decision Making 575

A second implication of naïve realism is that people Framing—and the role of the reference point in trigger-
hold but a single representation without any aware- ing risk seeking or risk aversion—can be seen in the fol-
ness of—or adjustment for—alternative representations lowing simple example (Tversky & Kahneman, 1986):
or what is not represented (Griffin, Dunning, & Ross,
1990). Fischhoff, Slovic, and Lichtenstein (1978, p. 333) A. Assume you are richer by $300 than you are today
described this as “what is out of sight is also out of and you have to choose between the following:
mind.” This simple principle underlies at least three a sure gain of $100 (72%)
major research areas in JDM: framing effects on choice, a 50% chance to gain $200 and a 50% chance to
description effects on probability judgment, and “inside” gain nothing (28%)
biases in prediction. In each case, the richness of per- OR
ceived experience crowds out any awareness of other B. Assume you are richer by $500 than you are today
perspectives or of the constructed nature of a representa- and you have to choose between the following:
tion. Like reversing optical illusions, people can see only a sure loss of $100 (36%)
one perspective at a time. a 50% chance to lose nothing and a 50% chance to
The various “framing effects” documented by JDM lose $200 (64%)
researchers involve instances in which choices are influ-
enced by different descriptions of the same objective Note first that both pairs of choices are objectively
information, as illustrated by the Asian disease problem identical (providing a final outcome of $400 for sure vs.
described earlier. In each case, whether it is gain–loss fram- 50% chance of $500 and $300) but are framed to evoke
ing, temporal framing, narrow framing, attribute framing, thoughts of gaining or losing money relative to an imag-
or goal framing, the respondent is manipulated by the ined reference point. The critical point is that people accept
given frame because people do not typically transform the frame as given and do not bother to (or have no way
the information given to them into some canonical or to) create a common overall representation. After accept-
“neutral” representation (Benartzi & Thaler, 1995; Read, ing the frame, most people choosing between relatively
Loewenstein, & Rabin, 1999; Tversky & Kahneman, balanced gains prefer the sure thing over a risky chance
1981). of a bigger prize—this risk aversion in gains is due to the
Framing is a key mechanism in Kahneman and decline in marginal utility between $100 and $200 (that is,
Tversky’s prospect theory (1979). Prospect theory follows the perceived difference between $0 and $100 is greater
the basic logic of the classical normative SEU theory—that than that between $100 and $200.) A gain of $100 is expe-
is, choices are made to optimize the combination of the rienced as more than half as valuable as a gain of $200,
subjective probability of gaining an outcome and its sub- so why gamble to get $200? However, because declining
jective value (or utility)—with four important alterations. marginal utility also operates on losses, people choosing
First, an editing phase is introduced to capture the sim- between relatively balanced losses prefer a risky chance
plifying heuristics that individuals use to reduce the com- to lose nothing or lose $200 to a sure loss of $100—and
plexity of many of the choices they confront. Second, a thus are risk seeking in losses. A loss of $100 is more than
reference point is introduced to capture that new options half as painful as a loss of $200, so why not gamble to
are evaluated in terms of the gain or loss relative to (possibly) avoid a loss altogether? The impact of gain–loss
some expectation, comparison, or aspiration level. This framing around a reference point has been found for many
reflects the perceptual principle that sensation is partic- kinds of outcomes beyond money. For example, medical
ularly sensitive to changes or differences (and adapts to recommendations made by experienced physicians are
steady states) and that declining marginal utility applies influenced by whether the outcomes are framed in terms
to both gains and losses. Third, losses relative to the of survival rates versus mortality rates (McNeil, Pauker,
reference point have disproportionate hedonic impact Sox, & Tversky, 1982), and negotiations fail more often
compared with the same magnitude of gains (loss aver- when outcomes are framed in terms of losses versus gains
sion), again reflecting a basic principle of sensation and (Bazerman, 1983).
perception—pain dominates pleasure (Baumeister, Loss aversion, the psychophysical principle that losses
Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001; Rozin & Royzman, have greater hedonic impact than comparable gains, inter-
2001). Fourth, probabilities are weighted nonlinearly, acts with framing to make some (objectively identical)
such that the change from impossibility to possibility and frames more acceptable than others. For example, credit
from high probability to certainty are noted and weighted card companies require that any consumer charges for
more than changes in the intermediate ranges of the prob- using credit cards be described in terms of regular prices
ability scale. (with the credit card charges built in) and cash discounts,
576 Judgment and Decision Making

rather than regular prices (without the credit card charges) period or a broader portfolio of stocks, the chance of expe-
and credit card surcharges. Using the card thus becomes a riencing the painful loss would be lowered and the equity
foregone gain rather than the source of a loss. Similarly, premium would be smaller. Narrow framing also explains
the tax break associated with having children can either the behavior of New York cab drivers who quit earlier
be framed as a deduction associated with each of the first during their most profitable days (Camerer, Babcock,
two children (that is, a reference point of zero children and Loewenstein, & Thaler, 1997). The cab drivers lease their
gains associated with having children) or framed as an cabs for a daily fixed fee and set a daily income target. This
additional tax for those with fewer than two children (that leads them to work longer hours during slow-traffic days
is, a reference point of two children and losses associated and shorter hours during high-traffic days. If, instead, they
with not having children; Schelling, 1981). Clearly, the framed their income targets more broadly (per week or per
loss frame is painful and hence politically unacceptable. month) they could allocate their time more efficiently by
As discussed earlier, the endowment effect refers to a driving longer when business was most profitable (and
manifestation of loss aversion whereby ownership—even shorter when it was unrewarding)—and thus provide better
randomly allocated ownership—immediately increases the service to customers as well.
selling price of an item partly because exchanging the item The family of framing effects also includes phenom-
for money means exchanging a loss for a gain (Kahneman, ena that are not directly linked to losses or gains. In
Knetsch, & Thaler, 1991; Thaler, 1980). Studies with mugs, attribute framing, the same quantitative information is
pens, and many other items of exchange show that the sell- expressed using either the positive or the negative end
ing prices specified by owners are generally about twice as of the scale as a reference point. For example, consum-
much as the buying prices of those who were not endowed ers evaluate beef described as 75% lean more positively
with the good. Interestingly, as predicted by prospect the- than beef described as 25% fat (Levin & Gaeth, 1988)
ory, items that are held specifically for trading purposes do and students evaluate a condom described as having a
not show the endowment effect (List, 2003). 90% success rate more positively than one with a 10%
Temporal framing (Loewenstein, 1987) also uses the failure rate (Linville, Fischer, & Fischhoff, 1993). Once
principle of loss aversion and framing but does so in again, the striking aspect of these results is that people
the context of time. Imagine that a desired gift, say a new fail to see through the frame they are given. Attribute
television, is to be shipped to you in 2 (4) weeks. How much framing is generally attributed to the salience of the posi-
money would have to be taken off (added to) the price to tive or negative information highlighted by the valence
delay (expedite) the shipping by 2 weeks? For the refer- of the frame. In goal framing (also known as mes-
ence point of 2 weeks, an additional 2-week delay is a loss, sage framing), the outcome of some preventive effort is
whereas for the reference point of 4 weeks, expediting by described either in terms of the positive effects of engag-
2 weeks is a gain; thus, the discount one would demand for ing in the action or the negative effects of failing to engage
a delay is greater than the fee one would be willing to pay in the action (e.g., Detweiler, Bedell, Salovey, Pronin, &
for expediting. Rothman, 1999). Messages that emphasize the latter typi-
Narrow framing refers to the tendency to treat choices cally have greater impact. (Note, however, that many stud-
one at a time and resist aggregating them (Benartzi & ies in this tradition do not provide identical information in
Thaler, 1995; Kahneman & Lovallo, 1993). This can lead the two “framing” conditions and thus would not fit our
to myopic loss aversion, as illustrated in the well-known technical definition of framing.)
Samuelson paradox: Even economists will turn down a bet Some framing effects are self-inflicted but are none-
offering a 50–50 chance to win $200 and lose $100 but are theless effective. For example, people allocate money
happy to play a set of 10 such bets. Each bet individually is to certain “mental accounts” and treat their funds in dif-
unattractive (when framed narrowly), but the set is attrac- ferent accounts differently (Epley, Mak, & Idson, 2006;
tive (because of its broader frame). People, whether gam- Thaler, 1980; Tversky & Kahneman, 1981). Money won
blers, managers, or stock market investors, often respond in a casino is treated differently than money inherited from
to each risky choice in its own separate frame, rather than a Puritan aunt. A lost $100 ticket to the opera prevents the
combining them across time or portfolios (Thaler, 1999). purchase of another ticket, but a loss of $100 cash on
Because individual investors assess their stockholdings the way to the opera does not. Mental accounting violates the
frequently—and are averse to losses—they find holding economic principle of the fungibility of wealth (that is,
stocks to be relatively painful. This leads to the “equity that all of one’s monetary assets should be interchange-
premium”—the discrepancy in payoffs between holding able, regardless of how they were acquired). But as Thaler
(safe) bonds and (risky) stocks. If individual investors were (1999) notes, mental accounts are just as binding for econ-
to broaden their framing and evaluate either a longer time omists as for anyone else.
Concepts That Have Arisen Independently in Social Psychology and Judgment and Decision Making 577

Probability judgments are also remarkably frame depen- Will I be happier if I move to Sydney? How much time will
dent. In one classic example, Fischhoff, Slovic, and a chapter in this handbook take? Clearly, predictions about
Lichtenstein (1978) asked automobile mechanics to judge the future involve irreducible uncertainty of many kinds,
the probability that a particular engine malady was caused including uncertainty about what will happen and how
by various mechanical failures. Several possible causes were we will respond when it does. However, the tendency for
specified, along with a residual category labeled “all other representations to be concrete, singular, and experienced
causes.” Remarkably, the probability allocated to the resid- as copies of the outside world also characterizes intuitive
ual category did not increase when important causes were prediction and forecasting. Kahneman and Tversky (1979,
removed or “pruned” from the list. That is, the mechanics 1982; see also Kahneman & Lovallo, 1993) contrasted the
seemed to consider only the specific causes they were pro- inside view of prediction with an outside view. The inside,
vided and divided the allotted 100% among them. singular, or case-based view focuses on the unique details
Support theory (Rottenstreich & Tversky, 1997; Tversky & of the problem at hand and involves a scenario representing
Koehler, 1994) was developed to explain the role of rep- the most (subjectively) important and available details. The
resentation in intuitive judgments. According to support outside, distributional, or class-based view focuses on
theory, subjective probability is attached not to events but to the set of comparable instances and gives little weight to
descriptions of events, a tenet that acknowledges that rep- what is unique about the current problem. The planning
resentations are crucially important in driving perceptions fallacy illustrates the intuitive appeal of the inside approach
of likelihood. For example, the probability of an earth- and the biases that can result.
quake killing 1,000 people this year may seem greater The planning fallacy is the juxtaposition of a general
than the probability of a natural disaster killing 1,000 peo- belief that some class of tasks generally take longer to
ple this year. And the probability of a homicide in Detroit complete than expected, with a specific belief that a par-
on a given day may be judged higher than the probabil- ticular current task will be completed in a shorter time
ity of homicide that day in Michigan as a whole. In both than usual. The cardinal example is the case of a group
cases, the more specific event is a subset of the second of academics predicting that their joint textbook would
more inclusive event and cannot be more likely—but the be completed within a couple of years, even when every
more specific and concrete representation provides a better one of them had firsthand knowledge that similar projects
search cue to bring to mind evidence and examples, hence had taken quite a bit longer and many were never finished
increasing the support for that hypothesis. (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). Because predictions are
Support theory provides a formal symbolic account of made from an inside perspective, the textbook writers came
the operation of judgment heuristics by proposing support up with detailed plans for completion, without considering
as an intervening psychological construct between repre- (and adjusting for) the many unspecified and unspecifiable
sented hypotheses and expressed probability. Subjective things that might go wrong. However, a consideration of
probability is constructed as a ratio of the support for vari- the relevant distribution of textbook projects as a whole
ous competing hypotheses, and support itself is constructed might have—and should have—triggered the necessary
from the balance of evidence for the hypotheses in question adjustment. There is considerable empirical support for
(Brenner, Koehler, & Rottenstreich, 2002). A key explana- the idea that the tendency to adopt an inside perspective
tory principle in support theory, akin to framing in prospect underlies the planning fallacy (Buehler, Griffin, & Ross,
theory, is unpacking, the process of breaking a superordinate 2002), including (1) thought protocols showing the pre-
or inclusive description into the sum of its parts. For exam- ponderance of scenario planning and the scarcity of either
ple, respondents judged the probability of death from natu- distributional thinking or representations of uncertainty,
ral causes to be 58% but also judged that the probability of (2) evidence that increasing the focus on details increases
death by heart disease was 22%, by cancer 18%, and by all the magnitude of the optimistic bias, (3) evidence that
other natural causes 33%. Thus, by unpacking the category of making the distribution of past outcomes more obviously
death by natural causes into three subsets, the total probabil- relevant to the present case reduces the optimistic bias, and
ity assigned to this category rose from 58% to 73% (22%  (4) evidence that predicting from a third-person perspec-
18%  33%). The unpacking effect works with counts of tive reduces the exclusive focus on case-based planning.
frequency as well as probability, so it is not simply a case The inside approach to prediction also leads to sys-
of an unfamiliar scale driving a contrived bias. tematic biases in forecasting one’s own future reactions.
Studies of affective forecasting (Buehler & McFarland,
Forecasting and Planning 2001; Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 2002;
Many of our most important judgments are forecasts and Gilbert & Wilson, 2000; Wilson & Gilbert, 2003) have
predictions. What will life be like after we are married? documented a general bias to overpredict the magnitude
578 Judgment and Decision Making

and duration of emotional responses to a given future further developed in the other, allows us, as we have seen,
event, whether it be a family holiday, a major professional to better explain a range of diverse phenomena. Because
setback, a loss by one’s favorite sports team, or a win by a of their distinct history and theoretical tools, the two fields
hated presidential candidate. The existence of this robust are complementary and able to reach different audiences.
bias is particularly noteworthy, given that we make such But note that the ideas advanced and investigated in the
forecasts and experience the resulting outcomes day in twin fields also hold considerable promise for helping
and day out. Two explanations have been offered. One is people solve some of the most pressing problems humans
focalism: People’s attempts to simulate their future emo- face and hence offer great promise for improving human
tional experience focus entirely on the target event itself lives. If we are to dampen global conflict, head off global
and neglect the many situational details and distractions warming, or remediate environmental degradation, we
that will reduce the impact of the event when it actually will have to thoroughly understand and wisely use channel
unfolds (Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, Gilbert, & Axsom, factors, cleverly frame the issues and potential outcomes,
2000; also called focusing bias by Schkade & Kahneman, manage emotional hot buttons, and mix appeals to reason
1998). Focalism also gives rise to the overweighting of with those to emotion and intuition. In short, solving the
intentions in predicting one’s future actions. For example, most important problems of today and tomorrow requires
stated intentions to give blood almost perfectly map onto the combined wisdom of the twin fields of social psychol-
behavioral predictions, but the observed relation between ogy and JDM.
intentions and actual donation is more moderate because
of the intervening effects of other situational variables,
such as competing priorities (Koehler & Poon, 2006; see REFERENCES
also Kruger & Gilovich, 2004). A second reason that peo-
ple overpredict their emotional reactions to future events Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). How often did I do it? Experienced
is their failure to anticipate the operation of adaptation and ease of retrieval and frequency estimates of past behavior. Acta
Psychologica, 103, 77–89.
rationalization, or what Gilbert, Wilson, and colleagues
Acker, F. (2008). New findings on unconscious versus conscious thought
call the psychological immune system (e.g., Gilbert, Pinel, in decision making: Additional empirical data and meta-analysis.
Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998). As with the plan- Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 292–303.
ning fallacy, people fail to learn sufficiently from the past Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness
and thus do not adjust their construals of the future to take and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463–469.
into account that the effects of even very good and very Ajzen, I. (1977). Intuitive theories of events and the effects of base-
rate information on prediction. Journal of Personality and Social
bad events regularly and predictably are less enduring than Psychology, 35, 303–314.
their mental simulations lead them to believe. Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1975). A Bayesian analysis of the attribution
In research on both the planning fallacy and affective process. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 267–277.
prediction, we see a complete overlap between social psy- Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, England:
chology and JDM, begging the question of how much it is Addison-Wesley.
Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Effects of fluency on psy-
possible to distinguish the two overlapping fields and their
chological distance and mental construal (or why New York is a large
mutual influence on current work. city, but New York is a civilized jungle). Psychological Science, 19,
161–167.
Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007).
SUMMARY Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic rea-
soning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 569–576.
Ames, D. R. (2004). Inside the mind-reader ’s toolkit: Projection and ste-
We began this chapter with the questions of why JDM can reotyping in mental state inference. Journal of Personality and Social
be considered a part of—or at least necessary for—social Psychology, 87, 340–353.
psychology and why there should be a chapter such as this Anderson, C. A. (1995). Implicit personality theories and empirical data:
one in the Handbook of Social Psychology. We confined Biased assimilation, belief perseverance and change, and covariation
detection sensitivity. Social Cognition, 13, 25–48.
our answer to historical and formal causes, focusing on
Anderson, C. A., & Lindsay, J. J. (1998). The development, perseverance,
the history of how the two fields mutually influenced each and change of naïve theories. Social Cognition, 16, 8–30.
other and on the inherent overlap in their intellectual con- Anderson, C. A., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1980). Perseverance of
tent and guiding concerns. social theories: The role of explanation in the persistence of discred-
We would now like to end by noting a teleological rea- ited information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39,
1037–1049.
son that the two fields are substantially conjoined. Aligning
Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2005). The heat of the moment: The effect
them serves a purpose. Drawing on the ideas that originated of sexual arousal on sexual decision making. Journal of Behavioral
in the two fields, or those that originated in one and were Decision Making, 18(1), 1–12.
References 579

Asch, S. E. (1940). Studies in the principles of judgments and attitudes: II. Broemer, P., Grabowski, A., Gebauer, J. E., Ermel, O., & Diehl, M. (2008).
Determination of judgments by group and by ego standards. Journal of How temporal distance from past selves influences self-perception.
Social Psychology, 12, 433–465. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 697–714.
Asch, S. E. (1952). Social psychology. New York: Prentice Hall. Bruner, J. (1957a). Going beyond the information given. In H. Gruber,
Axelrod, R. M. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic K. R. Hammond, & R. Jesser. (Eds.), Contemporary approaches in
Books. cognition (pp. 41–69). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Barbey, A. K., & Sloman, S. A. (2007). Base-rate respect: From ecologi- Bruner, J. (1957b). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64,
cal rationality to dual processes. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 123–152.
241–298. Bruner, J. S. (1992). Another look at New Look I. American Psychologist,
Baron, J., & Ritov, I. (1994). Reference points and omission bias. 47, 780–783.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 59, Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and need as organizing fac-
475–498. tors in perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 42,
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). 33–44.
Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. Bruner, J. S., & Klein, G. S. (1960). The functions of perceiving: New Look
Bazerman, M. H. (1983). Negotiator judgment: A critical look at the retrospect. In B. Kaplan & S. Wapner (Eds.), Perspectives in psycho-
rationality assumption. American Behavioral Scientist, 27, 618–634. logical theory (pp. 61–77). New York: International Universities Press.

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision Bruner, J. S., & Postman, L. (1947). Tension and tension-release as orga-
making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295–307. nizing factors in perception. Journal of Personality, 15, 300–308.

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding Buehler, R., Griffin, D. W., & Ross, M. (2002). Inside the planning fal-
advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, lacy: The causes and consequences of optimistic time predictions. In
275, 1293–1295. T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and
biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 250–270). New
Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender: Transforming the debate on York: Cambridge University Press.
sexual inequality. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Buehler, R., & McFarland, C. (2001). Intensity bias in affective forecast-
Benartzi, S., & Thaler, R. H. (1995). Myopic loss aversion and the equity ing: The role of temporal focus. Personality and Social Psychology
premium puzzle. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110, 73–92. Bulletin, 27, 1480–1493.
Benartzi, S., & Thaler, R. H. (2004). Save more tomorrow: Using behav- Camerer, C. (1990). Do markets correct biases in probability judgment?
ioral economics to increase employee saving. Journal of Political Evidence from market experiments. In L. Green & J. H. Kagel (Eds.),
Economy, 112, 164–187. Advances in behavioral economics (Vol. 2, pp. 125–172). Northwood,
Bentham, J. (1948) An introduction to the principle of morals and legisla- NJ: Ablex.
tions. Oxford: Blackwell. (Original work published 1789.) Camerer, C., Babcock, L., Loewenstein, G., & Thaler, R. (1997). Labor
Bernoulli, D. (1954). Exposition of a new theory on the measurement of supply of New York City cabdrivers: One day at a time. Quarterly
risk. Econometrica, 22, 23–36. (Original work published 1738.) Journal of Economics, 112, 407–441.
Bertrand, M., Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2004). A behavioral-eco- Camerer, C., Issacharoff, S., Loewenstein, G., O’Donoghue, T., & Rabin, M.
nomics view of poverty. American Economic Review Papers and (2003). Regulation for conservatives: Behavioral economics and the
Proceedings, 6, 419–423. case for “asymmetric paternalism.” University of Pennsylvania Law
Bertrand, M., Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2006). Behavioral economics Review, 1151, 1211–1254.
and marketing in aid of decision making among the poor. Journal of Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G., & Weber, M. (1989). The curse of knowl-
Public Policy and Marketing, 25, 8–23. edge in economic settings: An experimental analysis. Journal of
Beyth-Marom, R., & Fischhoff, B. (1983). Diagnosticity and pseudo- Political Economy, 97, 1232–1254.
diagnosticity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, Camerer, C., &. Thaler, R. H. (1995). Anomalies: Ultimatums, dictators
1185–1195. and manners. Journal of Economic Perspective, 9, 209–219.
Bicchieri, C. 2006. The grammar of society: The nature and dynamics of Carlsmith, J. M., & Gross, A. E. (1969). Some effects of guilt on compli-
social norms. New York: Cambridge University Press. ance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 232–239.
Bless, H., Mackie, D. M., & Schwarz, N. (1992). Mood effects on atti- Carlson, B. W. (1990). Anchoring and adjustment in judgments under
tude judgments: Independent effects of mood before and after mes- risk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
sage elaboration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, Cognition, 16, 665–676.
585–595. Carlson, M., Charlin, V., & Miller, N. (1988). Positive mood and helping
Bless, H., Schwarz, N., & Wieland, R. (1996). Mood and the impact of cat- behavior: A test of six hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social
egory membership and individuating information. European Journal Psychology, 55, 211–229.
of Social Psychology, 26, 935–959. Carroll, J. S. (1978). The effect of imagining an event on expectations
Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P., & Susser, K. (1994). Happiness and for the event: An interpretation in terms of the availability heuristic.
stereotype thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 88–96.
Social Psychology, 66, 621–632. Cartwright, D. (1949). Some principles of mass persuasion: Selected find-
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, ings of research on the sale of U.S. war bonds. Human Relations, 2,
129–148. 253–267.
Brenner, L. A., Koehler, D. J., & Rottenstreich, Y. (2002). Remarks on support Cervone, D., & Peake, P. (1986). Anchoring, efficacy, and action: The influ-
theory: Recent advances and future directions. In T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, ence of judgmental heuristics on self-efficacy judgments and behavior.
& D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 492–501.
judgment (pp. 489–509). New York: Cambridge University Press. Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing
Brinol, P., Petty, R. E., & Tormala, Z. L. (2006). The malleable meaning of and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of
subjective ease. Psychological Science, 17, 200–206. Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–766.
580 Judgment and Decision Making

Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social Davies, M. F. (1997). Belief persistence after evidential discrediting: The
psychology. New York: Guilford Press. impact of generated versus provided explanations on the likelihood of
Chapman, G. B., & Johnson, E. J. (1999). Anchoring, activation and the discredited outcomes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33,
construction of value. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision 561–578.
Processes, 79, 115–153. Davis, C. G., Lehman, D. R., Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., & Thompson, S. C.
Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). Relationship orientation (1995). The undoing of traumatic life events. Personality and Social
as a moderator of the effects of social power. Journal of Personality Psychology Bulletin, 21, 109–124.
and Social Psychology, 80, 173–187. Dawes, R. M. (1988). Rational choice in an uncertain world. New York:
Cheng, P. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391–416. Dawes, R. M., & Corrigan, B. (1974). Linear models in decision making.
Cheng, P. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (1989). On the natural selection of reason- Psychological Bulletin, 81, 95–106.
ing theories. Cognition, 33, 285–313. Dawson, E., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (2002). Motivated reasoning
Choi, J., Laibson, D., & Madrian, B. (2009). Reducing the complexity costs and performance on the Wason selection task. Personality and Social
of 401(k) participation through quick enrollment. In D. A. Wise (Ed.), Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1379–1387.
Research findings in the economics of aging (pp. 57–82). Chicago: Denes-Raj, V., & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational
University of Chicago Press. processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal
Cialdini, R. (2007). Descriptive social norms as underappreciated sources of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 819–829.
of social control. Psychometrika, 72, 263–268. Detweiler, J. B., Bedell, B. T., Salovey, P., Pronin, E., & Rothman, A. J.
Cialdini, R. B., Darby, B. L., & Vincent, J. E. (1973). Transgression (1999). Message framing and sunscreen use: Gain-framed messages
and altruism: A case for hedonism. Journal of Experimental Social motivate beach-goers. Health Psychology, 18, 189–196.
Psychology, 9, 502–516. Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informa-
Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of tional social influence upon individual judgment. Journal of Abnormal
normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the and Social Psychology, 51, 629–636.
role of norms in human behavior. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and con-
experimental social psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 202–234). San Diego: trolled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Academic Press. 56, 5–18.
Clore, G. L. (1992). Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the con-
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The merits of unconscious thought
struction of judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The
in preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality
construction of social judgments (pp. 133–163). Mahwah, NJ:
and Social Psychology, 87, 586–598.
Erlbaum.
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & von Baaren, R. B. (2006).
Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment
On making the right choice: The deliberation-without-attention effect.
and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 393–399.
Science, 311, 1005–1007.
Coleman, S. (1996). The Minnesota income tax compliance experiment
Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., Postmes, T., Stapel, D. A., Koomen, W.,
state tax results. Retrieved August 18, 2009, from http://www.taxes.
van Knippenberg, A., & Scheepers, D. (1998). Seeing one thing and
state.mn.us/legal_policy/research_reports/content/complnce.pdf.
doing another: Contrast effects in automatic behavior. Journal of
Connolly, T., & Zeelenberg, M. (2002). Regret and decision making. Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 862–871.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 212–216.
Ditto, P. H., & Lopez, D. F. (1992). Motivated skepticism: Use of dif-
Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police ferential decision criteria for preferred and nonpreferred conclusions.
officer ’s dilemma: Using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threat- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 568–584.
ening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
1314–1329. Ditto, P. H., Pizarro, D. A., Epstein, E. B., Jacobson, J. A., & MacDonald,
T. K. (2006). Visceral influences on risk-taking behavior. Journal of
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 99–113.
shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task.
Cognition, 31, 187–316. Ditto, P. H., Scepansky, J. A., Munro, G. D., Apanovich, A. M., & Lockhart,
L. K. (1998). Motivated sensitivity to preference-inconsistent informa-
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 53–69.
after all? Rethinking some conclusions of the literature on judgment
under uncertainty. Cognition, 58, 1–73. Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., & Lawrence, A. D. (2006). The somatic marker
hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Cox, J. R., & Griggs, R. A. (1982). The effects of experience on per-
Reviews, 30, 239–271.
formance in Wason’s selection task. Memory and Cognition, 10,
496–502. Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort
Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.
Critcher, C. R., & Gilovich, T. (2008). Incidental environmental anchors.
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21, 241–251. Edmans, A., Garcia, D., & Norli, O. (2007). Sports sentiment and stock
returns. Journal of Finance, 62, 1967–1998.
Crocker, J. (1982). Biased questions in judgment of covariation studies.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 214–220. Edwards, W. (1953). Probability-preferences in gambling. American
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human Journal of Psychology, 66, 349–364.
brain. New York: Putnam. Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychological
Damasio, H., Bechara, A., & Damasio, A. R. (2002). Reply to “Do Bulletin, 41, 380–417.
somatic markers mediate decisions on the gambling task?” Nature Edwards, W. (1961). Behavioral decision theory. Annual Review of
Neuroscience, 5, 1102–1104. Psychology, 12, 473–498.
Darlington, R. B., & Macker, C. E. (1966). Displacement of guilt-produced Edwards, W. (1968). Conservatism in human information processing.
altruistic behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Formal representation of human judgment
442–443. (pp. 17–52). New York: Wiley.
References 581

Edwards, W., Lindman, H., & Savage, L. J. (1963). Bayesian statistical Fiddick, L., Spampinato, M. V., & Grafman, J. (2005). Social contracts and
inference for psychological research. Psychological Review, 70, precautions activate different neurological systems: An fMRI investi-
193–242. gation of deontic reasoning. NeuroImage, 28, 778–786.
Eibach, R. P., Libby, L. K., & Gilovich, T. (2003). When change in the self Fiedler, K. (1991). The tricky nature of skewed frequency tables: An infor-
is mistaken for change in the world. Journal of Personality and Social mation loss account of distinctiveness-based illusory correlations.
Psychology, 84, 917–931. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 24–36.
Epley, N. (2004). A tale of tuned decks? Anchoring as accessibility and Fischer, G. W., & Hawkins, S. A. (1993). Scale compatibility, strategy
anchoring as adjustment. In D. J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), The compatibility and the prominence effect. Journal of Experimental
Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 240–256). Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 580–597.
Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. Fischhoff, B., & Beyth, R. (1975). I knew it would happen: Remembered
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2001). Putting adjustment back in the anchoring probabilities of once-future things. Organizational Behavior and
and adjustment heuristic: Divergent processing of self-generated and Human Performance, 13, 1–16.
experimenter-provided anchors. Psychological Science, 12, 391–396. Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1978). Fault trees: Sensitivity
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Are adjustments insufficient? Personality of estimated failure probabilities to problem representation. Journal of
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 447–460. Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4,
33–344.
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2005). When effortful thinking influences judg-
mental anchoring: Differential effects of forewarning and incentives on Fiske, S. T., Lin, M., & Neuberg, S. (1999). The continuum model: Ten
self-generated and externally-provided anchors. Journal of Behavioral years later. In Y. Trope & S. Chaiken (Eds.), Dual-process theories in
Decision Making, 18, 199–212. social psychology (pp. 231–254). New York: Guilford Press.
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring and adjustment heuris- Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum model of impression
tic: Why adjustments are insufficient. Psychological Science, 17, formation, from category-based to individuation processes: Influence
311–318. of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In
M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). New York: Academic Press.
taking as egocentric adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York:
Psychology, 87, 327–339.
McGraw-Hill.
Epley, N., Mak, D., & Idson, L. (2006). Bonus or rebate? The impact
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2007). Social cognition: From brains to cul-
of income framing on spending and saving. Journal of Behavioral
ture. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Decision Making, 19, 213–227.
Fitzsimons, G. M., Chartrand, T. L., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2008). Automatic
Epstein, S. (1991). Cognitive-experiential self-theory: An integrative theory
effects of brand exposure on motivated behavior: How Apple makes
of personality. In R. Curtis (Ed.), The self with others: Convergences
you “think different.” Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 21–35.
in psychoanalytic, social, and personality psychology (pp. 111–137).
New York: Guilford Press. Fodor, J. (2000). Why we are so good at catching cheaters. Cognition, 75,
29–32.
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2004). History of the dual process theory in reasoning.
Folkes, V. S. (1988). The availability heuristic and perceived risk. Journal
In K. I. Manktelow & M. C. Chung (Eds.), Psychology of reasoning:
of Consumer Research, 15, 13–23.
Theoretical and historical perspectives (pp. 241–266). Hove, England:
Psychology Press. Forgas, J. P. (1998). On being happy and mistaken: Mood effects on the
fundamental attribution error. Journal of Personality and Social
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2007). Hypothetical thinking: Dual processes in rea-
Psychology, 75, 318–331.
soning and judgment. New York: Psychology Press.
Fox, C. R. (2006). The availability heuristic in the classroom: How solic-
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment,
iting more criticism can boost your course ratings. Judgment and
and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.
Decision Making, 1, 86–90.
Evans, J. St. B. T., Newstead, S. E., & Byrne, R. M. J. (1993). Human rea-
Frank, R. H., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (1993). Does studying economics
soning: The psychology of deduction. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
inhibit cooperation? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 7, 159–171.
Evans, J., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove, England: Frederick, S. (2002). Automated choice heuristics. In T. Gilovich,
Psychology Press. D. W. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The
Falk, R., & Konold, C. (1997). Making sense of randomness: Implicit psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 548–558). Cambridge, England:
encoding as a basis for judgment. Psychological Review, 104, Cambridge University Press.
301–318. Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of
Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: Economic Perspectives, 19, 25–42.
The MODE model as an integrative framework. Advances in Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time dis-
Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109). New York: counting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic
Academic Press. Literature, 40, 350–401.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden
research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and
297–327. Emotion, 19, 313–332.
Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects Fujita, K., Henderson, M. D., Eng, J., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006).
of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and Spatial distance and mental construal of social events. Psychological
Social Psychology, 87, 557–572. Science, 17, 278–282.
Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Palo Alto, CA: Funder, D. C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of
Stanford University Press. social judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75–90.
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of Gabrielcik, A., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). Priming and frequency estima-
forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, tion: A strict test of the availability heuristic. Personality and Social
203–210. Psychology Bulletin, 10, 85–89.
582 Judgment and Decision Making

Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and one’s own actions and appearance. Journal of Personality and Social
global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Psychology, 78, 211–222.
Science, 13, 34–40. Gilovich, T., & Savitsky, K. (2002). Like goes with like: The role of repre-
Gavanski, I., & Hui, C. (1992). Natural sample spaces and uncertain belief. sentativeness in erroneous and pseudo-scientific beliefs. In T. Gilovich,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 585–595. D. W. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psy-
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and proposi- chology of intuitive judgment (pp. 617–624). New York: Cambridge
tional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and University Press.
explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. Gilovich, T., Savitsky, K., & Medvec, V. H. (1998). The illusion of trans-
Gigerenzer, G. (1991). How to make cognitive illusions disappear: Beyond parency: Biased assessments of others’ ability to read our emotional
heuristics and biases. European Review of Social Psychology, 2, states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 332–346.
83–115. Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball:
Gigerenzer, G., Hell, W., & Blank, H. (1988). Presentation and content: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17,
The use of base rates as a continuous variable. Journal of Experimental 295–314.
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 513–525. Gleicher, F., Kost, K. A., Baker, S. M., Strathman, A. J., Richman, S. A., &
Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reason- Sherman, S. J. (1990). The role of counterfactual thinking in judgments
ing without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychological Review, of affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 284–295.
102, 684–704. Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places. New York: Free Press.
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a
heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press. viewpoint: Using social norms to motivate environmental conservation
Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 472–482.
of the social inference process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Grayson, C. E., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Beliefs influence information pro-
Unintended thought. New York: Guilford Press. cessing strategies: Declarative and experiential information on risk
Gilbert, D. T. (1999). What the mind’s not. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope assessment. Social Cognition, 17, 1–18.
(Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 3–11). Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., &
New York: Guilford. Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement
Gilbert, D. T. (2002). Inferential correction. In T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, & in moral judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108.
D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of Grether, D. M., & Plott, C. R. (1979). Economic theory of choice and the
intuitive judgment (pp. 167–184). New York: Cambridge University preference reversal phenomenon. American Economic Review, 69,
Press. 623–638.
Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraint: Griffin, D. W., & Buehler, R. (1999). Frequency, probability, and predic-
Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and tion: Easy solutions to cognitive illusions? Cognitive Psychology, 38,
Social Psychology, 50, 269–280. 48–78.
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Griffin, D. W., Dunning, D., & Ross, L. (1990). The role of construal pro-
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38. cesses in overconfident predictions about the self and others. Journal
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1128–1139.
(1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective fore- Griffin, D. W., & Ross, L. (1991). Subjective construal, social inference,
casting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617–638. and human misunderstanding. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experi-
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. mental social psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 319–356). New York: Academic
(2002). Durability bias in affective forecasting. In T. Gilovich, Press.
D. W. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The Griffin, D. W., & Tversky, A. (1993). The weighing of evidence and the
psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 292–312). Cambridge, England: determinants of confidence. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 411–435.
Cambridge University Press. Griggs, R. A., & Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematic-materials effect in
Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2000). Miswanting: Some problems in the Wason’s selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 407–420.
forecasting of future affective states. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and Güth, W. (1995). On ultimatum bargaining experiments: A personal review.
thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 178–197). New Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27, 329–344.
York: Cambridge University Press.
Hamilton, D. L., & Gifford, R. K. (1976). Illusory correlation in interper-
Gilovich, T. (1981). Seeing the past in the present: The effect of asso- sonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments. Journal
ciations to familiar events on judgments and decisions. Journal of of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 392–407.
Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 797–808.
Hammond, K. R. (1996). Human judgment and social policy: Irreducible
Gilovich, T. (1983). Biased evaluation and persistence in gambling. Journal uncertainty, inevitable error, unavoidable injustice. New York:
of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1110–1126. Oxford.
Gilovich, T. (1990). Differential construal and the false consensus effect. Hastorf, A., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case study. Journal
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 623–634. of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 49, 129–134.
Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human Hawkins, S. A., & Hastie, R. (1990). Hindsight biased judgments of past
reason in everyday life. New York: Free Press. events after the outcomes are known. Psychological Bulletin, 107,
Gilovich, T., Kerr, M., & Medvec, V. H. (1993). The effect of temporal per- 311–327.
spective on subjective confidence. Journal of Personality and Social Hebl, M. R., Foster, J. B., Mannix, L. M., & Dovidio, J. F. (2002). Formal
Psychology, 64, 552–560. and interpersonal discrimination: A field study of bias toward homo-
Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: What, sexual applicants. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
when, and why. Psychological Review, 102, 379–395. 815–825.
Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky, K. (2000). The spotlight effect Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation level theory: An experimental and system-
in social judgment: An egocentric bias in estimates of the salience of atic approach to behavior. New York: Harper.
References 583

Henrich, J. (2000). Does culture matter in economic behavior? Ultimatum Jacowitz, K. E., & Kahneman, D. (1995). Measures of anchoring in
game bargaining among the Machiguenga of the Peruvian Amazon. estimation tasks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21,
American Economic Review, 90, 973–979. 1161–1167.
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., & Jenni, K. E., & Loewenstein, G. F. (1997). Explaining the “identifiable vic-
McElreah, R. (2001). In search of Homo economicus: Behavioral tim effect.” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 14, 235–257.
experiments in 15 small-scale societies. American Economic Review, Johnson, E. J., & Goldstein, D. (2003). Do defaults save lives? Science,
91, 73–78. 302, 1338–1339.
Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (1999). The “conjunction fallacy” revis- Johnson, E. J., Hershey, J., Meszaros, J., & Kunreuther, H. (1993). Framing,
ited: How intelligent inferences look like reasoning errors. Journal of probability distortions, and insurance decisions. Journal of Risk and
Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 275–306. Uncertainty, 7, 35–51.
Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. (1987). Social cognition and social perception.
Johnson, E. J., & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the per-
Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 369–425.
ception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,
Hilton, D. J. (1990). Conversational processes and causal explanation. 20–31.
Psychological Bulletin, 107, 65–81.
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Legrenzi, P., & Legrenzi, M. (1972). Reasoning and a
Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2002). Somatic markers, sense of reality. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 395–400.
working memory, and decision making. Cognitive, Affective, and
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1972). The actor and the observer: Divergent
Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 341–353.
perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse,
Hirshleifer, D., & Shumway, T. (2003). Good day sunshine: Stock returns H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution:
and the weather, Journal of Finance, 58, 1009–1032. Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 79–94). Morristown, NJ:
Holder, M. D., & Hawkins, C. (2007). The illusion of transparency: General Learning Press.
Assessment of sex differences in showing and hiding disgust. Basic Jones, J. (Feb 11, 2008). Eating right: Truly functional foods.
and Applied Social Psychology, 29, 235–243. Press-Republican. Retrieved August 18, 2009, from http://www.
Holyoak, K. J., & Cheng, P. W. (1995). Pragmatic reasoning with a point of pressrepublican.com/archivesearch/local_story_042224534.html.
view. Thinking and Reasoning, 1, 289–313. Judd, C. M., Blair, I. V., & Chapleau, K. M. (2004). Automatic stereo-
Hong, Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). types vs. automatic prejudice: Sorting out the possibilities in the Payne
Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and (2001) weapon paradigm. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709–720. 40, 75–81.
Hsee, C. K. (1995). Elastic justification: How tempting but task-irrelevant Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping out
factors influence decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720.
Decision Process, 62, 330–337. Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited:
Hsee, C. K. (1996). Elastic justification: How unjustifiable factors influ- Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In T. Gilovich, D. W.
ence judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology
Processes, 66, 122–129. of intuitive judgment (pp. 49–81). Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Huber, J., Payne, J. W., & Puto, C. (1982). Adding asymmetrically domi- University Press.
nated alternatives: Violations of regularity and the similarity hypoth- Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1986). Fairness as a
esis. Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 90–98. constraint on profit seeking: Entitlements and the market. American
Ichheiser, G. (1949). Misunderstandings in human relations: A study in Economic Review, 76, 728–741.
false social perceptions. American Journal of Sociology, 55, 1–72. Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1991). Anomalies: The
Isen, A. M., Clark, M., & Schwartz, M. F. (1976). Duration of effect of endowment effect, loss aversion, and status quo bias. Journal of
good mood on helping: Footprints on the sands of time. Journal Economic Perspectives, 5, 193–206.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 385–393. Kahneman, D., & Lovallo, D. (1993). Timid choices and bold forecasts:
Isen, A. M., & Levin, P. F. (1972). The effect of feeling good on helping: A cognitive perspective on risk taking. Management Science, 39(1),
Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17–31.
21, 384–388. Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to
Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibil- its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.
ity of material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1–14. of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430–454.
Iyengar, S., Jiang, W., & Huberman, G. (2004). How much choice is too Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction.
much: Determinants of individual contributions in 401K retirement Psychological Review, 80, 237–251.
plans. In O. Mitchell & S. Utkus (Eds.), Pension design and structure:
New lessons from behavioral finance (pp. 83–95). Oxford: Oxford Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of deci-
University Press. sion under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.

Iyengar, S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: Can Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychology of preferences.
one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Scientific American, 246, 160–173.
Social Psychology, 79, 995–1006. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames.
Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobio- American Psychologist, 39, 341–350.
graphical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Kahneman, D., & Varey, C. A. (1990). Propensities and counterfactu-
Psychology, 3, 306–340. als: The loser that almost won. Journal of Personality and Social
Jacoby, L. L., Woloshyn, V., & Kelley, C. (1989). Becoming famous with- Psychology, 59, 1101–1110.
out being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced Kahneman, D., Wakker, P. & Sarin, R. (1997). Back to Bentham?
by dividing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Explorations of experienced utility. The Quarterly Journal of
118, 115–125. Economics, 112, 375–406
584 Judgment and Decision Making

Kamin, K. A., & Rachlinski, J. J. (1995). Ex post  ex ante: Determining Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of
liability in hindsight. Law and Human Behavior, 19, 89–104. Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146–159.
Kamstra, M. J., Kramer, L. A., & Levi, M. D. (2003). Winter blues: Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and
Seasonal affective disorder (SAD) and stock market returns. American purse strings: Carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions.
Economic Review, 93, 324–343. Psychological Science, 15, 337–341.
Kant, I. (1965). The critique of pure reason (N. K. Smith, Trans.). Leventhal, H., Singer, R. P., & Jones, S. H. (1965). The effects of fear and
New York: St. Martin’s. (Original work published 1781.) specificity of recommendation upon attitudes and behavior. Journal of
Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004). Material prim- Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 20–29.
ing: The influence of mundane physical objects on situation construal Levin, I. P., & Gaeth, G. J. (1988). Framing of attribute information before
and competitive behavioral choice. Organizational Behavior and and after consuming the product. Journal of Consumer Research, 15,
Human Decision Processes, 95, 83–96. 374–378.
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Lewin, K. (1952). Group decision and social change. In G. E. Swanson,
Psychologist, 28, 107–128. T. M. Newcomb, & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology.
Kelly, C. M., & Jacoby, L. (1998). Subjective reports and process dissocia- New York: Henry Holt.
tion: Fluency, knowing, and feeling. Acta Psychologica, 98, 127–140. Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. (1944). Level of aspiration. In
Keysar, B., & Barr, D. J. (2002). Self-anchoring in conversation: Why lan- J. M. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (pp. 333–378).
guage users don’t do what they “should.” In T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, Oxford: Ronald Press.
& D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of Liberman, N., Sagristano, M. D., & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of tem-
intuitive judgment (pp. 150–166). Cambridge, England: Cambridge poral distance on level of mental construal. Journal of Experimental
University Press. Social Psychology, 38, 523–534.
Klayman, J., & Ha, Y. (1987). Confirmation, disconfirmation, and informa- Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability
tion in hypothesis testing. Psychological Review, 94, 211–228. considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal
Knetsch, J. L., & Tang, F. (2006). The context, or reference, dependence of construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
economics values: Further evidence and some predictable patterns. In 5–18.
M. Altman (Ed.), Handbook of contemporary behavioral economics: Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008). The psychology of transcending the here
A handbook (pp. 423–440). New York: Sharpe Publishers. and now. Science, 322, 1201–1205.
Koehler, D. J., & Poon, C. S. K. (2006). Self-predictions overweight Liberman, V., Samuels, S. M., & Ross, L. (2002). The name of the game:
strength of current intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Predictive power of reputations vs. situational labels in determining
Psychology, 42, 517–524. Prisoner ’s Dilemma game moves. Personality and Social Psychology
Koehler, J. (1996). The base-rate fallacy reconsidered: Descriptive, norma- Bulletin, 30, 1175–1185.
tive, and methodological challenges. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Lichtenstein, S., & Slovic, P. (1971). Reversals of preference between
19, 1–53. bids and choices in gambling decisions. Journal of Experimental
Koffka, K. (1935). The principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Psychology, 89, 46–55.
Harcourt. Lichtenstein, S., & Slovic, P. (1973). Response-induced reversals of prefer-
Krosnick, J. A., Li, F., & Lehman, D. R. (1990). Conversational conven- ence in gambling: An extended replication in Las Vegas. Journal of
tions, order of information acquisition, and the effect of base rates and Experimental Psychology, 101, 16–20.
individuating information on social judgments. Journal of Personality
Lieberman, M. D. (2003). Reflective and reflexive judgment processes: A
and Social Psychology, 59, 1140–1152.
social cognitive neuroscience approach. In J. Forgas, K. R. Williams, &
Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The “below-average effect” W. von Hippel (Eds.), Social judgments: Implicit and explicit pro-
and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of cesses (pp. 44–67). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 221–232.
Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflexion
Kruger, J., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Actions, intentions, and trait assess- and reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attribu-
ment: The road to self-enhancement is paved with good intentions. tional inference. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 328–339. psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 199–249). New York: Elsevier.
Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M., & Satpute, A. B. (2004). Evidence-
mind: “Seizing” and “freezing.” Psychological Review, 103, 263–283. based and intuition-based self-knowledge: An fMRI study. Journal of
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 421–435.
Bulletin, 108, 480–498. Linville, P. W., Fischer, G. W., & Fischhoff, B. (1993). AIDS risk per-
Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. New York: Perseus Books. ceptions and decision biases. In J. B. Pryor & G. D. Reeder (Eds.),
The social psychology of HIV infection (pp. 5–38). Mahwah, NJ:
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford
Erlbaum.
University Press.
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self- List, J.A. (2003). Does market experience eliminate market anomalies?
defeating behavior? Emotion, risk taking, and self-regulation. Journal Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 41–71.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1250–1267. Loewenstein, G. (1987). Anticipation and the valuation of delayed con-
Lepper, M., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s sumption. Economic Journal, 97, 666–684.
intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of “overjustifica- Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behav-
tion” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, ior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65,
129–137. 272–292.
Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Loewenstein, G., Nagin, D., & Paternoster, R. (1997). The effect of sexual
Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national arousal on predictions of sexual forcefulness. Journal of Research in
field experiment. Psychological Science, 14, 144–150. Crime and Delinquency, 34, 443–473.
References 585

Loewenstein, G., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as Muringhan, J. K., & Saxon, M. S. (1998). Ultimatum bargaining by child-
feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286. ren and adults. Journal of Economic Psychology, 19, 415–445.
Loomes, G. (1999). Some lessons from past experiments and some Mussweiler, T. (2002). The malleability of anchoring effects. Experimental
challenges for the future. Economic Journal, 109, 35–45. Psychology, 49, 67–72.
Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment:
attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently con- Mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110, 472–489.
sidered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, Mussweiler, T., & Bodenhausen, G. (2002). I know what you are but what
2098–2109. am I? Self-evaluative consequences of judging in-group and out-group
Macleod, C., & Campbell, L. (1992). Accessibility and probability members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 19–32.
judgments: An experimental evaluation of the availability heuristic. Mussweiler, T., Ruter, K., & Epstude, K. (2004). The ups and downs of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 890–902. social comparison: Mechanisms of assimilation and contrast. Journal
Madrian, B. C., & Shea, D. F. (2001). The power of suggestion: Inertia of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 832–844.
in 401(k) participation and savings behavior. Quarterly Journal of Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (1999). Hypothesis-consistent testing and
Economics, 116, 1149–1187. semantic priming in the anchoring paradigm: A selective accessibility
Manktelow, K. I., & Over, D. E. (1991). Social roles and utilities in reason- model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 136–164.
ing with deontic conditionals. Cognition, 39, 85–105. Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (2000). The use of category and exemplar
Markman, A. B., & Medin, D. L. (1995). Similarity and alignment in knowledge in the solution of anchoring tasks. Journal of Personality
choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63, and Social Psychology, 78, 1038–1052.
117–130. Mynatt, C. R., Doherty, M. E., & Tweney, R. D. (1977). Confirmation bias
Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). in a simulated research environment: An experimental study of scientific
Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of Personality inference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 85–95.
and Social Psychology, 63, 119–132. Nash, J. (1950). Equilibrium points in n-person games. Proceedings of the
Mayer, J. D., Gayle, M., Meehan, M. E., & Haarman, A. K. (1990). Toward National Academy of Sciences 36, 48–49.
better specification of the mood-congruency effect in recall. Journal of Neisser, U. ( 1963 ). The multiplicity of thought. British Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 465–480. Psychology, 54, 1–14.
McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined Newton., L. (1990). Overconfidence in the communication of intent:
in America? It depends upon who is asking and what is asked. Journal Heard and unheard melodies. Ph.D. dissertation., Stanford University,
of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563–579. Stanford, CA.
McGlone, M. S., & Tofighbakhsh, J. (2000). Birds of a feather flock con-
Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and short-
jointly(?): Rhyme as reason in aphorisms. Psychological Science, 11, comings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
424–428.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
McKenzie, G. R. M., Liersch, J. J., & Finkelstein, S. R. (2006).
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.
Recommendations implicit in defaults. Psychological Science, 17,
414–420. Novemsky, N., Dhar, R., Schwarz, N., & Simonson, I. (2007). Preference
fluency in choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 44, 347–356.
McKirnan, D. J. (1980). The identification of deviance: A conceptualiza-
tion and an initial test of a model of social norms. European Journal of Nussbaum, S., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2006). Predicting the near and distant
Social Psychology, 10, 75–93. future. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 152–161.
McNeil, B. J., Pauker, S. G., Sox, H. C., & Tversky, A. (1982). On the elici- Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (1994). A rational analysis of the selection task
tation of preferences for alternative therapies. New England Journal of as optimal data selection. Psychological Review, 101, 608–631.
Medicine, 306, 1259–1262. Oppenheimer, D. M. (2004). Spontaneous discounting of availability in
Meehl, P. E. (1954). Clinical versus statistical prediction. Minneapolis: frequency judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15, 100–105.
University of Minnesota Press. Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). The secret life of fluency. Trends in Cognitive
Meehl, P. E., & Rosen, A. (1955). Antecedent probability and the effi- Sciences, 12, 237–241.
ciency of psychometric signs, patterns, or cutting scores. Psychological Oppenheimer, D. M., LeBoeuf, R. A., & Brewer, N. T. (2008). Anchors aweigh:
Bulletin, 52, 194–216. A demonstration of cross-modality anchoring. Cognition, 206, 13–26.
Mellers, B., Hertwig, R., & Kahneman, D. (2001). Do frequency represen- Park, J., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Mood and heuristics: The influence of
tations eliminate conjunction effects? An exercise in adversarial col- happy and sad states on sensitivity and bias in stereotyping. Journal
laboration. Psychological Science, 12, 269–275. of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1005–1023.
Miller, D. T. (2006). Social psychology: An invitation. Belmont, CA: Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and
Belmont. controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality
Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1987). Pluralistic ignorance: When similar- and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192.
ity is interpreted as dissimilarity. Journal of Personality and Social Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1990). The adaptive decision
Psychology, 53, 298–305. maker: Effort and accuracy in choice. In R. M. Hogarth (Ed.), Insights
Miller, D. T., & Taylor, B. R. (1995). Counterfactual thought, regret, in decision making: A tribute to Hillel J. Einhorn (pp. 129–153).
and superstition: How to avoid kicking yourself. In N. J. Roese & Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
J. M. Olson (Eds.), What might have been: The social psychology Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1992). Behavioral deci-
of counterfactual thinking (pp. 305–332). Mahwah, NJ: sion research: A constructive processing perspective. Annual Review of
Erlbaum. Psychology, 43, 87–131.
Moskowitz, G. B., Skurnik, I., & Galinsky, A. D. (1999). The history of Payne, B. K., Lambert, A. J., & Jacoby, L. L. (2002). Best laid plans:
dual process notions in social psychology. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope Effect of goals on accessibility bias and cognitive control in race-
(Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 12–36). New based misperceptions of weapons. Journal of Experimental Social
York: Guilford Press. Psychology, 38, 384–396.
586 Judgment and Decision Making

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation:
of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social Perspectives of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). New York: Academic Press. Ross, M., & Sicoly, F. (1979). Egocentric biases in availability and attribu-
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton. tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 880–892.
Plous, S. (1989). Thinking the unthinkable: The effect of anchoring on like- Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1996). Naïve realism: Implications for social conflict
lihood estimates of nuclear war. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, and misunderstanding. In T. Brown, E. Reed, & E. Turiel (Eds.), Values
19, 67–91. and knowledge (pp. 103–135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Poulton, E. C. (1994). Behavioral decision theory: A new approach. Rothbart, M., Fulero, S., Jensen, C., Howard, J., & Birrell, P. (1978). From
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. individual to group impressions: Availability heuristics in stereotype
formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 237–255.
Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the eye of
the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others, Rothman, A. J., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Constructing perceptions of vul-
Psychological Review, 111, 781–799. nerability: Personal relevance and the use of experiential information
in health judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Toward an integration of cognitive
1053–1064.
and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypothesis-
testing model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social Rottenstreich, Y., & Hsee, C. K. (2001). Money, kisses, and electric
psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 297–340). New York: Academic Press. shocks: On the affective psychology of risk. Psychological Science,
12(3), 185–190.
Quattrone, G. A. (1982). Over attribution and unit formation: When behav-
ior engulfs the person. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Rottenstreich, Y., & Tversky, A. (1997). Unpacking, repacking, and
42, 593–607. anchoring: Advances in support theory. Psychological Review, 104,
406–415.
Quattrone, G. A., Lawrence, C. P., Finkel, S. E., & Andrus, D. C. (1981).
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity domi-
Explorations in anchoring: The effects of prior range, anchor extrem-
nance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5,
ity, and suggestive hints. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University,
296–321.
Stanford, CA.
Samuelson, W., & Zeckhauser, R. (1988). Status quo bias in decision-
Rabin, M. (1993). Incorporating fairness into game theory and economics.
making. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 1, 7–59.
American Economic Review, 83, 1281–1302.
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D.
Rachlinski, J. (1998). A positive psychological theory of judging in hind-
(2002). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultima-
sight. University of Chicago Law Review, 65, 571–625.
tum game. Science, 300, 1755–1758.
Read, D. (2004). Intertemporal choice. In D. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), Sanna, L. J., & Schwarz, N. (2006). Metacognitive experiences and
Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 424–443). human judgment: The case of hindsight bias and its debiasing. Current
Oxford: Blackwell. Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 172–176.
Read, D., Loewenstein, G., & Rabin, M. (1999). Choice bracketing. Savage, L. J. (1954). The foundation of statistics. New York: Wiley.
Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 171–197.
Savitsky, K., & Gilovich, T. (2003). The illusion of transparency and
Redelmeier, D., Koehler, D. J., Liberman, V., & Tversky, A. (1995). the alleviation of speech anxiety. Journal of Experimental Social
Probability judgment in medicine: Discounting unspecified alterna- Psychology, 39, 618–625.
tives. Medical Decision Making, 15, 227–230.
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological
Redelmeier, D. A., & Tversky, A. (1990). Discrepancy between medical determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.
decisions for individual patients and for groups. New England Journal
Schelling, T. C. (1981). Economic reasoning and the ethics of policy.
of Medicine, 322, 1162–1164.
Public Interest, 63, 37–61.
Regan, D. T., Williams, M., & Sparling, S. (1972). Voluntary expia- Schkade, D., & Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California make
tion of guilt: A field experiment. Journal of Personality and Social people happy? A focusing illusion in judgments of life satisfaction.
Psychology, 24, 42–45. Psychological Science, 9, 340–346.
Regan, J. W. (1971). Guilt, perceived injustice, and altruistic behavior. Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 124–132. information processing: Detection, search, and attention. Psychological
Risen, J. L., & Gilovich, T. (2007). Another look at why people are reluc- Review, 84, 1–66.
tant to exchange lottery tickets. Journal of Personality and Social Schwartz, B. (2004). The paradox of choice. New York: HarperCollins.
Psychology, 93, 12–22.
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motiva-
Risen, J. L., & Gilovich, T. (2008). Why people are reluctant to tempt fate. tional functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 293–307. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp. 527–561). New
Risen, J. L., Gilovich, T., & Dunning, D. (2007). One-shot illusory cor- York: Guilford Press.
relations and stereotype formation. Personality and Social Psychology Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-Schatk, A. H., &
Bulletin, 33, 1492–1502. Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings. In availability heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology 61, 195–202.
(pp. 173–220). New York: Academic Press. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments
Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states.
egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.
of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279–301. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20 years later.
Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self per- Psychological Inquiry, 14, 296–303.
ception and social perception: Biased attributional processes in the Schwarz, N., & Vaughn, L. A. (2002). The availability heuristic revisited:
debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Ease of recall and content of recall as distinct sources of informa-
32, 880–892. tion. In T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics
References 587

and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 103–119). Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1968). Relative importance of probabili-
New York: Cambridge University Press. ties and payoffs in risk-taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology
Schweitzer, M. (1994). Disentangling status quo and omission effects: Monograph, 78, 1–18.
Experimental analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Slovic, P., & Peters, E. (2006). Risk perception and affect. Current
Processes, 58(3), 457–476. Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 323–325.
Schweitzer, M. (1995). Multiple reference points, framing, and the status Small, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). Helping a victim or helping the victim:
quo bias in health care financing decisions. Organizational Behavior Altruism and identifiability. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 26, 5–16.
and Human Decision Processes, 63, 69–72. Small, D., Loewenstein, G., & Slovic, P. (2007). Sympathy and callous-
Sechrist, G. B., Swim, J. K., & Mark, M. M. (2003). Mood as informa- ness: The impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable
tion in making attributions to discrimination. Personality and Social and statistical victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Psychology Bulletin, 29, 524–531. Processes, 102, 143–153.
Seta, J. J., McElroy, T., & Seta, C. E. (2001). To do or not to do: Desirability Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in
and consistency mediate judgments of regret. Journal of Personality and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838.
Social Psychology, 80, 861–870. Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. (1978). Hypothesis-testing in social interac-
Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Reason-based choice. tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1202–1212.
Cognition, 49, 11–36. Sperber, D., Cara, F., & Girotto, V. (1995). Relevance and theory explains
Shepperd, J. A., Ouellette, J. A., & Fernandez, J. K. (1996). Abandoning the selection task. Cognition, 57, 31–95.
unrealistic optimism: Performance estimates and the temporal prox- Sperber, D., & Girotto, V. (2002). Use or misuse of the selection task?
imity of self-relevant feedback Journal of Personality and Social Cognition, 85, 277–290.
Psychology, 70, 844–855. Spranca, M., Minsk, E., & Baron, J. (1991). Omission and commission in
Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I. (1961). Social judgment: Assimilation and judgment and choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27,
contrast effects in communication and attitude change. New Haven, 76–105.
CT: Yale University Press. Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences
Sherman, D. K., & Kim, H. S. (2002). Affective perseverance: The resis- in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
tance of affect to cognitive invalidation. Personality and Social Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 224–237. age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Sherman, S. J., Cialdini, R. B., Schwartzman, D. F., & Reynolds, K. D. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the relative independence of
(1985). Imagining can heighten or lower the perceived likelihood thinking biases and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social
of contracting a disease: The mediating effect of ease of imagery. Psychology, 4, 672–695.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 118–127. Stone, V. E., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Kroll, N., & Knight, R. T. (2002).
Shiv, B., Loewenstein, G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. Selective impairment of reasoning about social exchange in a patient
(2005). Investment behavior and the negative side of emotion. with bilateral limbic system damage. Proceedings of the National
Psychological Science, 16, 435–439. Academy of Sciences, 99, 11531–11536.
Simon, H. A. (1947/1997). Administrative Behavior. New York: Free Press. Strack, F. & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of
Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247.
Journal of Economics, 69, 99–118. Strack, F., & Mussweiler, T. (1997). Explaining the enigmatic anchoring
Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man: Social and rational. New York: Wiley. effect: Mechanisms of selective accessibility. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 73, 437–446.
Simon H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of
Psychology, 41,1–19. Switzer, F., & Sniezek, J. A. (1991). Judgment processes in motiva-
tion: Anchoring and adjustment effects on judgment and behavior.
Simon, J. L., & Smith, D. B. (1973). Change in location of a student health
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 49, 208–229.
service: A quasi-experimental evaluation of the effects of distance on
utilization. Medical Care, 11, 59–67. Tajfel, H. (1969). Cognitive aspects of prejudice. Journal of Social Issues,
25, 79–97.
Simonson, I. (1989). Choice based on reasons: The case of attraction and
compromise effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 16, 158–174. Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1978). Salience, attention and attribution: Top
of the head phenomena. In L.Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
Skov, R. B., & Sherman, S. J. (1986). Information-gathering processes: social psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 249–288). New York: Academic Press.
Diagnosticity, hypothesis-confirmatory strategies, and perceived
hypothesis confirmation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Taylor, K. M., & Shepperd, J. A. (1998). Bracing for the worst: Severity,
22, 93–121. testing and feedback as moderators of the optimistic bias. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 915–926.
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning.
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sage, R. M., Lehman, B. J., & Seeman, T. E.
Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.
(2004). Early environment, emotions, response to stress, and health.
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The Journal of Personality, 72, 1365–1393.
affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, & D. Kahneman
Thaler, R. H. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal
(Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment
of Economic Behavior and Organization, 1, 39–60.
(pp. 397–420). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Thaler, R. H. (1988). Anomalies: The ultimatum game. Journal of
Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Facts versus fears:
Economic Perspectives, 2, 195–206.
Understanding perceived risk. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky
(Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 463–489). Thaler, R. H. (1999). Mental accounting matters. Journal of Behavioral
New York: Cambridge University Press. Decision Making, 12(3), 183–206.
Slovic, P., Griffin, D., & Tversky, A. (1990) Compatability effects in Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2003). Libertarian paternalism. American
judgment and choice. In R. Hogarth (Ed.), Insights in decision- Economic Review, 93, 175–179.
making: Theory and applications (pp. 5–27). New York: Cambridge Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about
University Press. health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
588 Judgment and Decision Making

Thomas, W. I., & Znaniecki, F. (1918). The Polish peasant in Europe and Wakslak, C., J., Nussbaum, S., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008).
America. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Representations of the self in the near and distant future. Journal of
Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 757–773.
and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information pro- Walster, E., Berscheid, E., Abrahams, D., & Aronson, V. (1967).
cessing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 973–988. Effectiveness of debriefing following deception experiments. Journal
Tomb, I., Hauser, M., Deldin, P., & Caramazza, A. (2002). Do somatic of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 371–380.
markers mediate decisions on the gambling task? Nature Neuroscience, Wason, P. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual
5, 1102–1103. task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 129–140.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of cul- Wason, P. C., & Evans, J. St. B. T. (1975). Dual processes in reasoning?
ture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted Cognition, 3, 141–154.
mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp.
Wason, P., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning:
19–136). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Structure and content. London: Batsford.
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological
Wason, P. C., & Shapiro, D. (1971). Natural and contrived experience in a
Review, 110, 403–421.
reasoning problem. Quarterly Journal or Experimental Psychology,
Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Wakslak, C. (2007). Construal levels and psy- 23, 63–71.
chological distance: Effects on representation, prediction, evaluation,
Whittlesea, B. W., & Leboe, J. P. (2000). The heuristic basis of remember-
and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17, 83–95.
ing and classification: Fluency, generation, and resemblance. Journal
Tversky, A. (1969). Intransitivity of preferences. Psychological Review, of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 84–106.
76, 31–48.
Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University
Psychological Bulletin, 76, 105–110. Press.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. Zanna
Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 345–
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D., (1980). Causal schemas in judgments 411). New York: Elsevier.
under uncertainty. In: Fishbein, M., (Ed.), Progress in social psychol- Wilson, T. D., Houston, C., Etling, K. M., & Brekke, N. (1996). A new look
ogy, (pp. 49–72). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. at anchoring effects: Basic anchoring and its antecedents. Journal of
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the Experimental Psychology: General, 4, 387–402.
psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458. Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000).
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1982). Evidential impact of base rates. In Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal
D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncer- of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821–836.
tainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 153–160). New York: Cambridge
Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (2001). How pleasant was your childhood?
University Press.
Beliefs about memory shape inferences from experienced difficulty of
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reason- recall. Psychological Science, 12, 176–179.
ing: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological
Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N., & Belli, R. F. (1998). The role of ease of
Review, 90, 293–315.
retrieval and attribution in memory judgments: Judging your memory as
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1986). Rational choice and the framing of worse despite recalling more events. Psychological Science, 9, 124–126.
decisions. Journal of Business, 59, 5251–5278.
Wong, K. F. E., & Kwong, J. Y. Y. (2000). Is 7300 m equal to 7.3 km? Same
Tversky, A., & Koehler, D. J. (1994). Support theory: A nonextensional repre- semantics but different anchoring effects. Organizational Behavior
sentation of subjective probability. Psychological Review, 101, 547–567. and Human Decision Processes, 82, 314–333.
Tversky, A., Sattath, S., & Slovic, P. (1988). Contingent weighting in judg- Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal media-
ment and choice. Psychological Review, 95, 371–384. tion of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of
Tversky, A., Slovic, P., & Kahneman, D. (1990). The causes of preference Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 109–120.
reversal. American Economic Review, 80, 204–217. Wright, W. F., & Anderson, U. (1989). Effects of situation familiarity and
Ubel, P. A., Spranca, M., DeKay, M., Hershey, J., & Asch, D. A. (1998). Public financial incentives on use of the anchoring and adjustment heuris-
preferences for prevention versus cure: What if an ounce of prevention is tic in probability assessment. Organizational Behavior and Human
worth only an ounce of cure? Medical Decision Making, 18, 141–148. Decision Processes, 44, 68–82.
Van Boven, L., Medvec, V., & Gilovich, T. (2003). The illusion of transpar- Wright, W. F., & Bower, G. H. (1992). Mood effects on subjective prob-
ency in negotiations. Negotiation Journal, 19, 117–131. ability assessment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Van Dort, B. E., & Moos, R. H. (1976). Distance and the utilization of Processes, 52, 276–291.
a student health center. Journal of the American College Health Wyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in their social
Association, 24, 159–162. context. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and eco- Yachanin, S. A., & Tweney, R. D. (1982). The effect of thematic content
nomic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. on cognitive strategies in the four-card selection task. Bulletin of the
Vorauer, J. D., & Cameron, J. J. (2002). So close, and yet so far: Does col- Psychonomic Society, 19, 87–90.
lectivism foster transparency overestimation? Journal of Personality Zeelenberg, M., Van den Bos, K., Van Dijk, E., & Pieters, R. (2002). The
and Social Psychology, 83, 1344–1352. inaction effect in the psychology of regret. Journal of Personality and
Vorauer, J. D., & Ross, M. (1999). Self-awareness and transparency Social Psychology, 82, 314–327.
overestimation: Failing to suppress one’s self. Journal of Experimental Zuckerman, M., Knee, C. R., Hodgins, H. S., & Miyake, K. (1995).
Social Psychology, 35, 415–440. Hypothesis confirmation: The joint effect of positive test strategy
Wagenaar, W. A. (1972). Generation of random sequences by human sub- and acquiescence response set. Journal of Personality and Social
ject: A critical survey of literature. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 65–72. Psychology, 68, 52–60.
Chapter 16

Self and Identity


WILLIAM B. SWANN JR AND JENNIFER K. BOSSON

From the beginning, psychology’s relationship with the cultural settings (e.g., Banaji & Prentice, 1994). And
“self ”has been a tempestuous one. When, for example, modern researchers have complemented their long-stand-
William James (1890/1950) marched the self to psychology’s ing interest in personal self-views or identities (we use
center stage in his classic text, the field promptly ushered these terms interchangeably) with investigations of social
it to the wings. There it languished for more than half a identities. It was this growing interest in social identity
century, ignored by a psychological mainstream whose that prompted us to cover this work and title the chapter
embrace of positivism made it squeamish about constructs “Self and Identity” instead of simply “The Self,” the title
that seemed to lack clear empirical referents (e.g., Allport, of Baumeister’s (1998) earlier contribution to this volume.
1943). And when the self finally did gain admission into Before turning to the specific substantive issues that
the social psychological mainstream in the 1960s, it had we cover here, we place our analysis in historical context.
been stripped of some crucial features of the construct In particular, we briefly describe the chain of events that led
that James introduced. Whereas James saw the self as a to the legitimization of a multifaceted, enduring conception
source of continuity that gave the individual a sense of of the self.
“connectedness” and “unbrokenness” (p. 335), the 1960s
were dominated by an ephemeral, shape-shifting self that
routinely reinvented itself in the service of winning social EMERGENCE OF THE “NEO-JAMESIAN” SELF
approval (e.g., Scheibe, 1985).
Happily, over the last few decades, conceptualiza- Psychology’s failure to follow up on James’s (1890/1950)
tions of the self have reclaimed much of the richness and initial investigation of the self left a void that scholars
integrity with which James (1890/1950) first imbued the from other fields quickly stepped in to fill. Two of the most
construct. Moreover, contemporary social-personality psy- prominent such scholars, the sociologists Charles Horton
chologists have warmly embraced these emerging, “neo- Cooley and George Herbert Mead, rallied behind the banner
Jamesian” visions of the self: Between 1972 and 2002, of a theoretical perspective known as symbolic interaction-
the percentage of self-related studies published in the ism (Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934). This theory was designed
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology increased to illuminate the nature and origins of self-knowledge,
fivefold (Swann & Seyle, 2005). The rejuvenated image especially the reactions of others and the roles people play.
of the self is multidimensional. Most researchers now We know ourselves, the theory assumed, by observing
assume that the self has a rich history, some of which is how we fit into the fabric of social relationships and how
conscious and accessible through self-reports and some others react to us. In its emphasis on the social construc-
of which is presumably nonconscious and accessible pri- tion of the self, symbolic interactionism zeroed in on the
marily through indirect measures. Although a strong belief aspect of self that James dubbed the “social self” and about
still exists in the prepotency of a desire to win approval which he famously noted that “a man has as many social
from others, most theorists acknowledge the significance selves as there are individuals who recognize him and
of rival motivational forces, particularly in non-Western carry an image of him in their mind” (1890/1950, p. 294).

We are grateful to Jennifer Beer, Matt Brooks, Serena Chen, Susan Fiske, Dan Gilbert, Josh Hart, Steven Heine, Stan Klein, Tracy
Kwang, Andrea Lindzey, Thomas Pyszczynski, and Morgan Ward, for their comments on earlier versions of this chapter.

589

CH16.indd 589 10/22/09 2:50:02 PM


590 Self and Identity

Conspicuously absent from these accounts were the other, the world was, as Daniel Webster put it, “governed more
more enduring aspects of the self that figured prominently in by appearances than by reality” (D. Webster, F. Webster,
James’s account, notably the “empirical self,” which includes Sanborn, 1857, p. 146).
the physical self, and the “spiritual self,” which consists of Even when researchers became interested in motives that
beliefs about one’s qualities. As symbolic interaction- seemed superficially incompatible with approval seeking
ism assumed center stage in the scientific community’s or “self-enhancement,” these motives were not informed
emerging understanding of the nature of the self, James’s by an enduring sense of self. For example, when research-
relatively enduring forms of self-knowledge faded into ers began to examine “self-consistency,” they typically left
obscurity. the enduring self out of the equation. Dissonance research-
Several decades later, the dominance of the social self ers, for instance, would subtly persuade participants to
was augmented by one of symbolic interactionism’s most behave in ways that made them look more or less deficient
prominent intellectual progeny, the dramaturgical move- and then observe their subsequent efforts to save face (e.g.,
ment. Spearheaded by Goffman (1959), this movement Aronson, 1968). Again, social actors were presumed to be
assumed that people are like actors in a play who perform interchangeable. Consequently, researchers had no need
for different audiences. As people take on various identi- to consider how an enduring sense of self might influence
ties, the self is merely a consequence, rather than a cause, people’s reactions to the situations in which they found
of the performance, a “product of the scene that comes off” themselves.
(p. 252). Once people lay claim to an identity, they are obli- It was not until the 1970s that the paradigm began to
gated to remain “in character” until they move to the next shift and the enduring sense of self began to gain currency
scene, at which point the former self is discarded in favor within mainstream social psychology. Snyder (1974)
of a self that fits the new context. For Goffman, there was developed a personality measure (the “self-monitoring”
no enduring sense of self; instead, Goffman envisioned the scale) that distinguished people who were thought to be
self as an ahistorical construction that emerged and van- perpetually engaged in Goffman-esque impression man-
ished at the whim of the situational cues that regulated its agement activities from those whose actions were guided
form and structure. by a deep-seated, enduring sense of self that valued cross-
When mainstream social psychologists developed an situational consistency. In a somewhat parallel effort that
interest in the systematic study of the self in the 1960s, drew on developments in cognitive psychology, Markus
they looked to sociology for a promising paradigm. They (1977) introduced the idea that some people possessed
were smitten with Goffman’s (1959) newly minted vision enduring “self-schemas” that systematically guided infor-
of self and identity. Goffman’s influence is most obvious mation processing about the self. Shortly afterwards,
in accounts of impression management (Jones, 1964), Kuiper and Rogers (1979) provided evidence that people
accounts that were later embellished by Edward Jones’s store representations of the self in memory and that these
students (e.g., Roy Baumeister and Fred Rhodewalt), as mental representations facilitate the retrieval of self-
well as others (Schlenker, 1980; Tedeschi, 1981). These relevant information.
theorists proved to be extremely influential in shaping By 1980, the stage had been set for a wide-ranging
early social psychological views of the self. But Goffman’s examination of the nature and consequences of a multi-
vision of the self had broader impacts as well. First, if any- faceted self that featured enduring, as well as relatively
one could assume any identity that the situation demanded, fleeting, components (Markus & Wurf, 1987; Swann,
then people were essentially interchangeable. This senti- 1983). No longer were social psychologists’ conceptualiza-
ment helped legitimize a situationist approach to the self tions of the self hitched to the wagon of pretense stubbornly
and identity. Second, the theatre metaphor that Goffman intent on self-enhancement. Increasingly, researchers were
used to exemplify social interaction led researchers to focus abandoning the stage-acting metaphor of the self and the
narrowly on a single goal: gaining the approval of “the audi- superficial relationships it illuminated and instead turning
ence” (i.e., other people). From this vantage point, people their attention to the relatively stable identities that people
were presumably in the business of constructing whichever negotiated in their ongoing social relationships. This is not
identities they believed would help them win the favor of to say that all prominent social psychologists followed this
their interaction partners, with the only proviso being that trend. But even the few who continued to emphasize the
they should strive to prevent observers from viewing them ephemeral self over the enduring self updated and refined
as inconsistent or dishonest (e.g., Schlenker, 1980, 1985). their analyses considerably (e.g., Gergen, 1991). And when
Nowhere in this scheme was there an intrinsic need to rec- more mainstream self theorists began to acknowledge
oncile the presented self with an enduring, underlying, or people’s stable identities, they quickly came to embrace the
authentic sense of self. For social psychologists of the day, richness and complexity of the multifaceted, neo-Jamesian

CH16.indd 590 10/22/09 2:50:03 PM


Self as a Mental Representation 591

conception of self. In the following section, we begin to In contrast, stored self-knowledge includes information about
examine the fruits of these efforts by turning to work that the self that is held in memory but is not being attended to.
conceptualizes the self as a mental representation. Thus, whereas most self-knowledge is at least theoretically
accessible to conscious awareness, only information in the
working self-concept is available for immediate reflection.
SELF AS A MENTAL REPRESENTATION The working self-concept is highly responsive to con-
text, such that people are particularly likely to bring to mind
Although we believe that psychology’s love affair with aspects of the self that stand out or differentiate them from
logical positivism explains most of its historical ambiva- others (e.g., McGuire et al., 1978). One consequence of
lence toward the self, an additional problem has been this malleability is that self-knowledge can shift somewhat
that the term “self ” has been used in multiple, sometimes easily to fit the demands of the current situation, without
contradictory ways (Leary, 2004). In its most common eliciting troubling feelings of inconsistency or inauthentic-
usage, the self refers to a representation or set of represen- ity (e.g., Swann, Bosson, & Pelham, 2002). We have more
tations about oneself, parallel to the representations people to say about this later.
have of other individuals. This is the most straightforward
Semantic Versus Episodic Self-Knowledge
and common usage of the term and the one on which we
focus most of our attention in this chapter. It is the “me,” or Based on Tulving’s (1983) distinction between two types of
self-as-object, about which James (1890/1950) wrote—the declarative memory, Klein and Loftus (1993) distinguished
entire set of beliefs, evaluations, perceptions, and thoughts semantic and episodic representations of the self. Semantic
that people have about themselves. memory is relatively abstract, context-free knowledge such
Nevertheless, the term “self ” has also been substituted as “Elephants are heavy” and “George H. W. Bush was
for “behavior,” as in “self-regulation.” Our review does not considered unpopular until his son brought new meaning
focus on work exemplifying the latter usage, partly because to the word.” Although semantic memory is not necessarily
this work was covered comprehensively in Baumeister ’s linked to the self, it can consist of propositions about the
(1998) chapter. In addition, however, we are concerned that self (e.g., “I have brown hair”). More relevant here,
if the boundary conditions of the subarea “self” are relaxed the semantic memory system may contain a subsystem
to encompass all research that involves behavior, then vir- in which information about one’s qualities, traits, and
tually any activity can be incorporated within the domain social roles is stored (e.g., “I can be assertive if pushed”).
of self-psychology simply by prefixing it with “self-.” For Such a system would be useful to those who are asked to
these reasons, our review focuses on work that directly or describe themselves quickly and succinctly. For example,
indirectly involves the represented self. first dates, job interviews, and other first-time encounters
often compel people to generate global self-characterizations
with little time to consult the evidence on which such char-
Types of Self-Representations
acterizations are based.
In what follows, we identify and define several important As the name implies, episodic memories encapsulate
distinctions that underlie people’s mental representations specific episodes or events that occurred in a person’s life.
of self. Although not exhaustive, this list is intended to When accessed, the retrieved events are experienced in
capture most major forms that self-views (self-concepts conjunction with a conscious awareness that they actually
and self-esteem) assume. occurred in the person’s life (e.g., Suddendorf & Corballis,
1997). Most people can recall hundreds if not thousands
Active Versus Stored Self-Knowledge of episodic memories, including events in the distant
The amount of self-knowledge—beliefs, thoughts, mem- past (e.g., their first kiss) or only moments ago (e.g., the
ories, and feelings about the self—that people possess is sentence they just finished reading).
theoretically unlimited in quantity and scope. As such, it Although it is obvious that episodic self-knowledge is
cannot all be brought to attention at once. Beginning in the based on specific events in people’s lives, it is less obvious
late 1960s, researchers began to acknowledge this fact by where semantic self-knowledge comes from. At first blush,
differentiating between active and stored self-knowledge. it might seem that self-knowledge is organized inductively,
Active self-knowledge includes information about oneself with specific episodes of episodic self-knowledge giving
that is held in consciousness. It has been referred to as the rise to and supporting semantic knowledge about the self.
phenomenal self (Jones & Gerard, 1967), the spontaneous Although this surely occurs in some instances, the research
self-concept (McGuire, McGuire, Child, & Fujioka, 1978), literature shows clearly that this is not always so. Instead,
and the working self-concept (Markus & Kunda, 1986). at least some semantic beliefs about the self seem to be

CH16.indd 591 10/22/09 2:50:03 PM


592 Self and Identity

formed and stored quite independently of specific episodic and consequences of the “implicit self” has grown at a
memories. Early support for this generalization came from remarkable rate. At the time of this writing, a PsycINFO
studies of normal college students who completed priming search for publications with keywords including “implicit”
tasks. The results showed that priming a trait stored in and either “self ” or “identity” yielded 292 publications
semantic memory (e.g., “Does ’stubborn’ describe you?”) between 2000 and 2009, as compared to only 50 such pub-
does not facilitate the recall of corresponding episodic lications during the preceding decade.
memories, namely, behavioral incidents that exemplify the Research on the implicit self explores several themes.
trait (e.g., Klein & Loftus, 1993; Klein, Loftus, Trafton, & Some work focuses on documenting a positivity bias on
Fuhrman, 1992). Moreover, semantic and episodic self- implicit measures of self-knowledge that parallels the
representations seem to be stored in different regions of positivity bias observed with explicit measures of self-
the brain. For example, participants who were asked to knowledge (Taylor & Brown, 1988). At the trait level, people
judge trait adjectives for self-descriptiveness showed acti- generally display highly favorable self-views and high self-
vation of cortical areas associated with semantic memory esteem when these variables are measured implicitly (e.g.,
retrieval (left frontal regions) but not of areas associated Kitayama & Karasawa, 1997; Yamaguchi et al., 2007). At
with episodic memory (right frontal regions; e.g., Kelley the group level, however, members of minority and low-
et al., 2002). status groups display relatively weak implicit liking for
Converging evidence for the independence of seman- their own social group relative to comparison majority or
tic and episodic representations of self comes from case high-status groups (Jost, Pelham, & Carvallo, 2002; Nosek,
studies of people with various cognitive impairments (e.g., Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002). These findings are consis-
amnesia, autism, and Alzheimer ’s dementia). For example, tent with work suggesting that members of disadvantaged
patients with brain injuries that make them unable to access groups can preserve their personal feelings of self-worth
and recall episodic memories are nevertheless able to make while still recognizing that their social groups are devalued
accurate judgments about their own traits. In addition, by the wider culture (Crocker & Major, 1989).
people with impaired episodic memories are capable of Another research theme explores the manner in which
updating their semantic memories to accommodate newly implicitly measured self-knowledge shapes people’s thoughts,
acquired self-knowledge (e.g., Klein, Loftus, & Kihlstrom, feelings, or behaviors. For example, in their work on implicit
1996; for a review, see Klein, 2004). egotism—an automatic preference for things that resemble
the self—Pelham, Carvallo, and Jones (2005) argue that
Implicit Versus Explicit Self-Knowledge people’s implicit feelings about the self guide many of their
Like other types of knowledge stored in memory, self- most important life decisions, including choice of occupa-
knowledge varies in how explicit it is. Whereas explicit tion, romantic partner, and residence. Because most people
self-knowledge is relatively controllable and deliberate, feel quite favorably toward the self, they tend to seek out
implicit self-knowledge is relatively uncontrollable and people, places, and things that remind them of the self.
automatic (Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986). A third theme in research on the implicit self focuses on
Moreover, explicit self-knowledge is readily reported but the emotional and behavioral implications of discrepancies
implicit self-knowledge is often gleaned indirectly by between people’s implicitly and explicitly measured self-
observing its effects on people’s feelings and automatic knowledge. For instance, some work reveals that people
behaviors. Indeed, Greenwald and Banaji (1995, p. 11) who display favorable self-views on explicit measures, but
defined implicit self-esteem as “the introspectively uniden- relatively unfavorable self-views on implicit measures, are
tified (or inaccurately identified) effect of the self-attitude characterized by heightened levels of self-aggrandizement
on evaluation of self-associated and self-dissociated (e.g., Bosson, Brown, Zeigler-Hill, & Swann, 2003; Jordan,
objects.” Spencer, Zanna, Hoshino-Browne, & Correll, 2003), ver-
Although indirect measures might ordinarily seem less bal defensiveness (Kernis, Lakey, & Heppner, 2008), and
desirable than measures that assess the target construct belief conviction (McGregor & Marigold, 2003). Thus, it
directly, some suggest that implicit self-esteem measures appears that discrepancies between implicitly and explic-
circumvent self-presentational processes to lay bare the itly measured self-knowledge may predict a defensive
unvarnished self (Farnham, Greenwald, & Banaji, 1999). tendency to present the self in an overly zealous manner.
Others propose that implicit self-esteem measures circum- Despite the attention that implicit self-knowledge—and
vent deliberative thought processes and thus reveal the implicit self-esteem in particular—has commanded in
“intuitive” self (Jordan, Whitfield, & Zeigler-Hill, 2007). recent years, troubling questions have been raised regarding
So enticing is the prospect of bypassing respondents’ several fundamental issues, including what the construct
deliberative self-views that research on the nature, origins, is. There are currently (at least) two competing schools of

CH16.indd 592 10/22/09 2:50:03 PM


Self as a Mental Representation 593

thought on the distinction between explicit and implicit that people have many nonconscious associations with
self-knowledge. One perspective, exemplified in Epstein’s various aspects of themselves. This might explain why dif-
(1994) cognitive–experiential self theory, assumes that ferent measures of implicit self-esteem are often uncorre-
explicit self-knowledge and implicit self-knowledge rep- lated with one another (Bosson et al., 2000).
resent fundamentally distinct constructs that derive from A final concern is specific to measures of implicit
different types of learning experiences; have independent self-esteem that are based on minimizing the ability of
effects on thoughts, feelings, and behaviors; and may even participants to reflect before responding (Farnham et al.,
be processed via separate systems in the brain. Some advo- 1999). This approach, which is used in some of the most
cates of this perspective assume further that implicit self- popular measures such as the Implicit Association Test,
knowledge is nonconscious. That is, not only do people presumably reduces the capacity of respondents to engage
lack conscious awareness of the effects of implicit self- in self-presentation. This is not necessarily true, as self-
knowledge on their behavior—as Greenwald and Banaji presentational activity can be automatized (Paulhus, 1993).
(1995) initially asserted about implicit self-esteem—but In addition, depriving respondents of the opportunity to
people may also lack conscious awareness of the contents reflect may have the additional effect of preventing them
of their implicit self-knowledge (e.g., Devos & Banaji, from accessing autobiographical knowledge, an activity that
2003). As such, the same self-views measured via explicit requires cognitive work. Therefore, when they are deprived
and implicit methods should not necessarily be expected to of cognitive resources, people with negative and positive
correlate; in fact, they often do not (e.g., Bosson, Swann, & self-views tend to respond similarly to self-relevant feedback
Pennebaker, 2000). (Hixon & Swann, 1993; Swann, Hixon, Stein-Seroussi, &
The other perspective, exemplified in Fazio’s motivation Gilbert, 1990). Such findings raise the possibility that
and opportunity as determinants model (Fazio & Towles- measures that diminish the capacity to reflect may unin-
Schwen, 1999), holds that explicit and implicit measures tentionally throw out the self-knowledge baby with the
of the same self-view often do access the same underlying self-presentational bathwater. This possibility is supported
attitude. According to this perspective, factors such as by evidence that responses to the Implicit Association
people’s opportunity and motivation to control their Test predict theoretically relevant outcomes more strongly
behavioral responses determine the degree of correspon- when they are contaminated by recently activated explicit
dence between a self-view that is measured by self-report beliefs about the self (e.g., Bosson et al., 2000).
(an explicit method) and the same self-view measured by an Together, these considerations raise vexing questions
implicit method such as response latency. Advocates of about whether implicit measures of self-esteem do in
this perspective assume that both types of measures tap the fact provide clearer insight into people’s self-views than
same self-view but that explicit measures afford respon- do explicit measures. An alternative view is that implicit and
dents more opportunities to influence the manner in which explicit measures both reveal valid information about people’s
they present the self than do implicit measures (Olson & self-knowledge but that the image of self that emerges from
Fazio, 2008; Olson, Fazio, & Hermann, 2007). Thus, explicit such measures may differ as a result of various underlying
and implicit measures of the same self-view predict dif- processes and situational features. Recent research and the-
ferent outcomes, but this need not imply that these mea- ory offers insight into the processes that underlie responses
sures access different underlying constructs. At present, to implicit and explicit measures of attitudes in general (e.g.,
the debate between these two perspectives continues. Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006; Nosek, 2005). Time will
Concerns have also been raised about whether implicit tell whether these insights from the general attitudes litera-
measures can, in principle, deliver on their promise. Initial ture will generalize to attitudes toward the self.
enthusiasm for measures of implicit self-esteem was based
Actual Versus Possible Self-Views
on the hope that they would tap an unvarnished or “true”
form of self-esteem and would therefore outpredict measures Whereas we have restricted our discussion thus far to self-
of explicit self-esteem in at least some domains. This theory knowledge that people hold about themselves in the pres-
has received some support (e.g., Spalding & Hardin, 1999) ent, several influential theories focus instead on potential
but likely not as much as hoped. One reason for this may be or possible self-knowledge. For example, E. Tory Higgins’s
that, like explicit self-esteem, implicit self-esteem is a (1987; Higgins, Klein, & Strauman, 1985) self-discrepancy
broad-based construct that has a wide bandwidth (Marsh & theory proposes that people store self-knowledge not only
Craven, 2006; Swann, Chang-Schneider, & McClarty, 2007). in the form of actual beliefs about the self but also in the
If so, it may not be feasible to assess implicit self-esteem form of ideal and ought beliefs about the self. The ideal self
by way of a simple association between one or more spe- contains people’s beliefs about their personal aspirations,
cific characteristics and the self. Instead, it seems likely as well as their beliefs about important others’ hopes for

CH16.indd 593 10/22/09 2:50:04 PM


594 Self and Identity

them; the ought self contains people’s beliefs about their 2006; Shavelson, Hubner, & Stanton, 1976; Swann et al.,
personal obligations and duties, as well as their beliefs 2007). The latter approach assumes that self-knowledge is
about important others’ expectations for them. According to structured hierarchically in memory, with global self-esteem
self-discrepancy theory, discrepancies between actual and at the top of the hierarchy. Beneath global self-esteem lie
ideal selves are associated with heightened levels of sad- more specific self-concepts nested within domains such as
ness and dejection, while actual–ought discrepancies are academic, physical, and social. Empirical investigations
associated with fear and anxiety. Thus, the ideal and ought support this hierarchical model. For example, evidence
selves serve as guides that motivate behaviors aimed at indicates that individual self-concepts, measured sepa-
minimizing existing discrepancies. Initial support for these rately, combine statistically to form a superordinate global
predictions (e.g., Higgins, Bond, Klein, & Strauman, 1986; self-esteem factor (Marsh & Hattie, 1996).
Strauman & Higgins, 1988) was followed by the publica- Conceptualizing self-esteem as a global representa-
tion of some inconsistent findings (Tangney, Niedenthal, tion of the self can shed light on an ongoing debate in
Covert, & Barlow, 1998). In an effort to reconcile these the self-esteem literature. Whereas some suggest that
inconsistencies, researchers subsequently identified modera- global self-esteem lacks predictive ability when it comes
tors of the effects such as the magnitude and importance of to important life outcomes (e.g., Baumeister et al., 2003),
the self-discrepancy, the accessibility of the self-discrepancy, others find that global self-esteem does predict important
and the applicability and relevance of the self-discrepancy in outcomes, as long as those outcomes are measured at a
a current context. Eventually, Higgins (1998) developed his global level, such as several outcomes bundled together
ideas into a new theory of regulatory focus. (e.g., Donnellan, Trzesniewski, Robins, Moffitt, & Caspi,
Similar to Higgins’s (1987) self-guides, Markus and 2005; Trzesniewski et al., 2006). Thus, recognizing that
Nurius (1986) proposed the construct of possible selves, self-knowledge assumes both global and specific forms may
which are people’s projections about what they might bear practical fruit by increasing researchers’ ability to pre-
become, would like to become, and are afraid to become dict criterion variables of interest (e.g., Swann, et al., 2007).
in the future. Possible selves motivate behaviors intended Some theorists seek a middle ground between concep-
to achieve desired possible selves and to avoid feared ones tualizing self-esteem as a single global entity and seeing it
(e.g., Oyserman, Bybee, Terry, & Hart-Johnson, 2004). as numerous specific self-views. Based on the assumption
Nevertheless, possible selves alone may not be sufficient that agency and communion represent universal dimen-
to motivate effective behaviors unless they are accompa- sions that underlie much of human behavior and thought
nied by plausible strategies for achieving desired goals (e.g., Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002; Wiggins, 1979),
(Oyserman, Bybee, & Terry, 2006). this “middle ground” approach identifies two components
of global self-esteem that correspond to agency and com-
Global Versus Specific Self-Knowledge munion (e.g., Franks & Marolla, 1976; Gecas, 1971).
Self-views vary in their breadth or specificity, which corre- Tafarodi and Swann (2001) labeled these components self-
sponds directly to the amount of information they convey competence, an evaluation of one’s ability to bring about
(Hampson, John, & Goldberg, 1987). At the broadest level, desired outcomes, and self-liking, an evaluation of one’s
global self-views are generalized beliefs that encompass a goodness, worth, and lovability. Supporting this distinc-
range of personal qualities (e.g., “I am worthwhile” and “I tion, research indicates that self-competence and self-liking
like myself ”). At the narrowest level, specific self-views or predict unique outcomes (e.g., Bosson & Swann, 1999;
self-concepts pertain to relatively specific qualities (e.g., Tafarodi & Vu, 1997).
“I am a world-class guitarist”). Between these extremes
Personal Versus Social Self-Knowledge
lie midlevel self-views that convey a moderate amount
of information about the self (e.g., “I am cooperative” and Within social psychology, social identity theorists were among
“I lack common sense”). the first to distinguish personal from social self-knowledge
The distinction between global and specific self-views (Tajfel & Turner, 1979, 1986). Whereas personal self-views
offers an alternative means of conceptualizing self-esteem. refer to individual, trait-like attributes (e.g., submissive, intel-
Instead of conceptualizing self-esteem as primarily affec- ligent), social self-views consist of people’s knowledge of the
tive (i.e., how people feel about the self) and self-concepts social groups to which they belong, along with their feelings
as primarily cognitive (i.e., what people believe about about those groups. One important consequence of this dis-
the self), as have some theorists (Baumeister, Campbell, tinction is the recognition that people can derive feelings of
Krueger, & Vohs, 2003), it is possible to think of self- value and worth not only from their personal qualities but also
esteem as a global belief about the self and self-concepts as from their associations with valued groups (e.g., Luhtanen &
relatively specific beliefs about the self (Marsh & Craven, Crocker, 1992; Tajfel & Turner, 1986).

CH16.indd 594 10/22/09 2:50:04 PM


Self as a Mental Representation 595

Further refining the personal–social distinction, some bottom-up perspective, global self-esteem derives from
theorists propose the existence of several levels at which the overall valence of individual self-views in the self-
self-knowledge is represented (e.g., Brewer & Gardner, concept (e.g., Marsh, 1990). As such, a woman who thinks
1996). According to these perspectives, self-knowledge of herself as intelligent, sociable, and attractive has higher
pertaining to people’s distinct traits and qualities, or per- global self-esteem than a woman who thinks of herself
sonal self-views, is stored at the individual level. At the as unintelligent, socially awkward, and unattractive. The
interpersonal level reside relational self-views, which competing, top-down perspective, holds that feelings of
describe qualities that are relevant to people’s social roles global self-esteem are the driving force behind the valence
and relationships (e.g., protective older sister). Finally, of people’s relatively specific self-views (e.g., Brown,
two types of self-views associated with group member- Dutton, & Cook, 2001). According to this perspective,
ships can be distinguished, collective self-views and group people develop global feelings of self-esteem early in life,
identities. Collective self-views refer to personal qualities and their global regard for the self determines whether they
that are associated with people’s group memberships (e.g., subsequently develop positive or negative beliefs about the
open-minded Democrat; Chen, Chen, & Shaw, 2004), and self within specific domains. Thus, a man who has high
group identities refer to characteristics of a group that may global self-esteem thinks of himself as more intelligent,
or may not describe an individual member of that group sociable, and attractive than a man who has low global
(Lemay & Ashmore, 2004). For example, people may self-esteem. At present, evidence exists for both of these
hold convictions about the groups to which they belong perspectives, pointing to an interactive effect wherein bidi-
(“Spaniards are impulsive”) that conflict with their per- rectional, direct and indirect links are found between the
sonal self-views (“I am cautious”). valence of self-knowledge and the valence of global self-
Although all people presumably store self-knowledge at esteem (Showers & Zeigler-Hill, 2006).
all three levels (personal, relational, and group), there exist
Importance of Self-Knowledge
stable individual differences in the extent to which people
focus on, value, and derive self-esteem from each form James (1890/1950) first observed that self-views can vary in
of self-view. For example, people from collectivistic cul- importance and that such variation can have important impli-
tures tend to focus more on their relational and collective cations for the self. In fact, this observation led to his classic
self-knowledge, whereas those from individualistic cul- formula in which self-esteem equals success (actual achieve-
tures tend to focus more on their personal self-knowledge ments) divided by pretensions (desired achievements). Thus,
(Cousins, 1989; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Moreover, competent performance in important domains fosters self-
whereas women emphasize their relational self-views esteem, but incompetence does not threaten self-esteem if it
more than men, men emphasize their collective self- occurs in devalued domains. As James put it, “I, who for the
views more than women (Gabriel & Gardner, 1999). These time have staked my all on being a psychologist, am mor-
differences in chronic focus suggest that people’s cultural tified if others know much more psychology than I. But I
background and gender play important roles in the types am contented to wallow in the grossest ignorance of Greek”
of self-relevant information they are most likely to notice, (1890/1950, p. 310). Initial attempts to test James’s formula
recall, and be influenced by. were unsupportive (Hoge & McCarthy, 1984; Marsh, 1986).
Later work, however, revealed that the importance of self-
views is related to self-esteem primarily among people who
Metacognitive Aspects of Self-Knowledge
have relatively negative self-views overall but are highly cer-
Metacognitive aspects of self-knowledge refer to charac- tain of their positive self-views (Pelham, 1995; Pelham &
teristics—such as importance, certainty, and stability—that Swann, 1989).
differentiate some self-views from others. Here, we cover The importance that people place on their specific self-
several metacognitive aspects of self-knowledge that have views predicts other self-relevant phenomena. For example,
attracted substantial empirical scrutiny. when people deem a self-view high in importance they
are more likely to behave in accordance with it (Pelham,
Valence of Self-Knowledge 1991), and they demonstrate higher levels of cross-
Not surprisingly, robust associations exist between the situational consistency in their self-descriptions of it
valence of people’s specific self-views and their global feel- (English & Chen, 2007). Indeed, people behave so as to
ings of self-esteem, such that people higher in self-esteem protect and maintain their highly important self-views. For
tend to have more positive self-views and fewer negative instance, people exhibit stronger resistance to challenges
ones (Brown, 1998; Pelham & Swann, 1989). Theorists to highly important self-views than to self-views that are
have explained this relation in two ways. According to the less important (Markus, 1977), and they work especially

CH16.indd 595 10/22/09 2:50:04 PM


596 Self and Identity

hard to surround themselves with people and feedback (Campbell et al., 1996). Like certainty, the clarity of people’s
that verify the self-views they deem most important (Chen self-views is associated with higher global self-esteem
et al., 2004; Swann & Pelham, 2002). Moreover, people (Campbell, 1990). Moreover, heightened self-concept clarity
avoid painful social comparisons when the domain of com- is associated with decreased neuroticism (Campbell et al.,
parison is linked to their most important self-views (Wood, 1996), more adaptive coping skills (Smith, Wethington, &
1989), and they may even distance themselves from Zhan, 1996), and increased psychological adjustment
close friends who outperform them in such domains (Campbell, Assanand, & Di Paula, 2003).
(Tesser, 1988).
Stability of Self-Knowledge
Just as specific self-views can vary in importance, so can
collective self-views and group identities. Whereas some Despite an overall tendency toward stability across long
people place great importance on their memberships in periods (see the section on Identity Negotiation and
various social groups, others attribute little significance to Change), some self-views fluctuate a great deal across
“being male” or “being Native American” (e.g., Luhtanen & shorter time frames. Much of the research on short-term
Crocker, 1992; Turner & Brown, 2007). Placing a lot of fluctuations in self-knowledge focuses on individual dif-
stock in collective self-views is linked to both positive and ferences in self-esteem stability. Whereas some people
negative outcomes. On the one hand, for members of nega- provide similar ratings of their global self-esteem from
tively stereotyped social groups, placing importance on the one measurement to the next, others experience relatively
collective self can serve as a buffer against the hurtful effects frequent, transient fluctuations in state self-esteem (e.g.,
of discrimination on self-esteem and well-being (Crocker & Kernis, Cornell, Sun, Berry, & Harlow, 1993). Moreover,
Major, 1989; Wong, Eccles, & Sameroff, 2003). On the other although there is a modest, positive association between
hand, those who value strongly their group memberships the level and stability of self-esteem (e.g., Kernis, Paradise,
are particularly vulnerable to the harmful effects of stereo- Whitaker, Wheatman, & Goldman, 2000), high levels of
types about their group, and they display heightened levels instability may occur at any level of global self-esteem.
of conformity to maladaptive group norms. We have more In general, higher levels of self-esteem stability are asso-
to say about these effects of group identification in our ciated with superior psychological well-being. For example,
discussions of social identity (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) and independent of their self-esteem level, people with more
stereotype threat (Steele, 1997) theories. stable self-esteem are more likely to pursue everyday
goals for intrinsic reasons (e.g., interest and enjoyment)
Certainty and Clarity of Self-Knowledge rather than extrinsic reasons (e.g., feeling forced), and they
The certainty with which people hold self-views has impor- feel less anxiety associated with the pursuit of such goals
tant implications for the self. Increases in the certainty (Kernis et al., 2000). People higher in self-esteem stability
of people’s self-views, for example, are associated with also report fewer depressive symptoms in the face of daily
increases in global self-esteem (Baumgardner, 1990; Story, stressors (Kernis, Grannemann, & Barclay, 1989).
2004). Conversely, low levels of self-view certainty are To date, much of the research on self-esteem stability
associated with increased tendencies toward maladaptive focuses on the experiences of people with stable versus
psychological conditions, such as social phobia (Wilson & unstable high self-esteem. Compared with their stable high
Rapee, 2006). self-esteem peers, individuals with unstable high self-esteem
As with important self-views, people work espe- appear hypervigilant for social feedback, and they react to
cially hard to maintain their highly certain self-views. negative performance feedback with heightened anger, hos-
For instance, people who are more certain of their self- tility, and defensiveness (e.g., Kernis et al., 1989). Because
views tend to behave more consistently across situations of its high reactivity to events that challenge the self, Kernis
(Baumgardner, 1990). Similarly, people are more likely to (2003) calls unstable high self-esteem a form of “fragile”
seek (Pelham, 1991) and receive (Pelham & Swann, 1994) high self-esteem.
interpersonal feedback that is consistent with self-views of Note that actual stability of self-knowledge and perceived
which they are highly certain. When confronted with feed- stability of self-knowledge are independent. For instance,
back that challenges highly certain self-views, people dis- those who assume that their belief structures tend to
play resistance (Swann & Ely, 1984), and such resistance remain stable across time may perceive greater consis-
efforts may further buttress the certainty of their self-views tency between their past and their present attitudes than
(Swann, Pelham, & Chidester, 1988). is actually the case (Ross, 1989). Conversely, those whose
Closely related to self-view certainty is self-concept implicit theories lead them to expect that training programs
clarity, which is defined as the extent to which self-views will improve their skills (e.g., Conway & Ross, 1984), or
are clear, confident, consistent, and stable across time that personal and social adjustment generally increase with

CH16.indd 596 10/22/09 2:50:05 PM


Self as a Mental Representation 597

age (Woodruff & Birren, 1972), may perceive less stability base their self-esteem (Crocker & Wolfe, 2001). According
across time in these self aspects than they actually display. to this perspective, college students differ reliably in
For these reasons and related ones, people’s beliefs about the extent to which they base their self-esteem on their
the stability of their self-knowledge may fail to track the accomplishments within seven broad domains: academics,
actual stability of such knowledge. appearance, approval from others, competition, family sup-
port, God’s love, and virtue (Crocker, Luhtanen, Cooper, &
Organization of Self-Knowledge Bouvrette, 2003). Moreover, consistent with James’s
Research suggests that people differ in terms of how they (1890/1950) self-esteem formula, people’s feelings of
organize self-knowledge in memory. Much of this research global self-esteem tend to fluctuate as a function of their
considers four features of the structure of self-knowledge. successes and failures primarily within domains in which
First is the number of different self-aspects—superordinate they are psychologically invested (Crocker, Karpinski,
traits or roles (e.g., wife and social self)—that house all Quinn, & Chase, 2003; Crocker, Sommers, & Luhtanen,
lower-order pieces of self-knowledge in the self-concept 2002). Although some work suggests that contingencies
(e.g., Linville, 1987). Next is the valence of self-knowledge, of self-worth can interfere with adaptive functioning (e.g.,
often measured as a function of the ratio of positive to neg- Crocker & Luhtanen, 2003), some domains of contingency
ative self-views in the self-concept (Showers, 1992). Third may be healthier than others. For instance, basing self-
is the level of compartmentalization versus integration that esteem on internal contingencies, such as virtue or God’s
characterizes the self-aspects. Compartmentalization refers love, is associated with fewer signs of psychological dis-
to the tendency to store positive and negative self-views tress than basing self-esteem on external contingencies,
within separate self-aspects, whereas integration refers to such as appearance or others’ approval.
the tendency to store both positive and negative self-views The other approach to contingent self-esteem assumes
within the same self-aspects. Finally, some researchers individual differences exist in the overall extent to which
consider the importance that people place on their different people possess contingent versus “true” (noncontingent)
self-aspects, with the assumption that more important self- self-esteem (Deci & Ryan, 1995). According to this perspec-
aspects—and their accompanying contents—are likely to tive, contingent and true self-esteem represent two types of
be activated most frequently (Showers, 1992). self-esteem that derive from different learning experiences.
Consideration of these features of the self-concept has Contingent self-esteem develops when individuals learn
led to several important insights into the links between that their worth and lovability depend on their attainment
self-concept and mental health. For example, work done of specific outcomes. Having internalized this belief, indi-
by Showers and colleagues (Showers, 1992; Showers & viduals with contingent self-esteem tend to pursue goals for
Kling, 1996) reveals that compartmentalization is gener- extrinsic reasons (e.g., others’ approval) rather than intrinsic
ally associated with higher self-esteem and reduced depres- reasons (e.g., interest), and they show heightened levels of
sion among people who place importance on their positive conformity to external forces (Gagné, Ryan, & Bargmann,
self-aspects. For such individuals, compartmentalization 2003; Ryan, Sheldon, Kasser, & Deci, 1996). Conversely,
limits their cognitive access to painful or threatening self-rel- true high self-esteem develops when individuals learn that
evant information. Conversely, integrative self-structures are they are valued for who they are and receive high levels of
associated with higher self-esteem and lower depression for care and autonomy in the pursuit of their goals. Not surpris-
people who place importance on their negative self-aspects, ingly, true self-esteem is associated with a reduced focus
because experiences that activate negative self-aspects call on extrinsic reinforcers and higher levels of psychological
to mind both negative and positive pieces of self-knowledge. adjustment (Kasser, Ryan, Zax, & Sameroff, 1995).
Integration also promotes resilience in the face of extreme Although these approaches focus on different aspects
stress or adversity (Showers & Zeigler-Hill, 2007) or intense of contingent self-worth, they need not be viewed as
negative mood states (Showers & Kling, 1996). antagonistic. Indeed, while people do differ in the specific
domains on which they base their self-esteem (Crocker,
Contingency of Self-Esteem Luhtanen, et al., 2003), meaningful overall differences
The contingency of self-esteem refers to the extent to which also occur in the degree to which people exhibit contingent
people base their feelings of self-worth on their ability to versus true self-esteem (Kernis et al., 2008; Neighbors,
achieve specific outcomes or match specific standards. As Larimer, Markman Geisner, & Knee, 2004).
noted by Kernis (2003), at least two different approaches
Narcissism: A Special Case of Fragile Self-Esteem
exist to the study of contingent self-esteem. One approach
assumes that most people have contingent self-esteem but Whereas most metacognitive features of self-knowledge
that they differ in the particular domains on which they discussed thus far have been unidimensional, narcissism

CH16.indd 597 10/22/09 2:50:05 PM


598 Self and Identity

is a multidimensional construct. Some qualities associated shame-proneness, and low self-esteem (Cooper &
with narcissism were discussed earlier, including highly Ronningstam, 1992; Gramzow & Tangney, 1992). Moreover,
positive self-views (in agentic domains; Campbell, Bosson, vulnerable narcissists tend to hide their feelings of grandi-
Goheen, Lakey, & Kernis, 2007; Campbell, Rudich, & osity behind a façade of modesty. Thus, whereas grandiose
Sedikides, 2002), low levels of certainty and clarity, and narcissists demand admiration and respect from others,
contingent self-esteem within competitive, but not affili- vulnerable narcissists crave approval but are too inhibited
ative, domains (Crocker, Luhtanen,et al., 2003; Zeigler- to demand it.
Hill, Clark, & Pickard, 2008). We include narcissism in
our list of metacognitive features because narcissism has
broad effects on how people value, select, organize, store, ORIGINS OF SELF-REPRESENTATIONS
and activate self-knowledge (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001;
Rhodewalt, 2005). The forms and features of self-knowledge described in the
Freud (1914/1957) first introduced the idea of narcis- preceding sections do not arise in a vacuum. Self-knowledge
sism to the psychoanalytical literature, viewing it as a dis- is shaped by numerous interacting forces, both biological
order that arises when individuals attach too much libido, and social. Here, we outline some of the raw materials of
or psychic energy, to the self and not enough to their inter- self-knowledge, as well as the mechanisms through which
nalized representations of relationship partners. As a result, people develop mental representations of the self. We also
the individual develops excessive levels of self-regard but consider questions and findings concerning the accuracy of
does not feel sufficient love for others. Later psychoana- people’s representations of the self.
lytical theorists (e.g., Kernberg, 1986; Kohut, 1966, 1971)
differed from Freud in their understanding of the origins
of narcissism but still conceptualized it as an outgrowth of Biological Origins of the Self and Identity
troubled relationships and unmet needs early in life (see
Brain
Bosson et al., 2008).
Although narcissism is typically viewed as a person- Where, in the brain, is the “self ” represented? Although this
ality disorder among clinical psychologists (American question defies a simple answer, researchers have begun
Psychiatric Association, 2000; Westen, 1990), social and to specify the neurological correlates of various aspects of
personality psychologists often treat it as an individual dif- self-knowledge. In general, much of this work converges on
ference variable that can be assessed meaningfully within the conclusion that self-referential tasks—such as thinking
any population. This approach gave rise to the Narcissistic about one’s traits or feelings or evaluating the self—trigger
Personality Inventory (Raskin & Hall, 1981), a scale heightened activity in the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC;
designed to measure narcissistic tendencies within normal, e.g., Johnson et al., 2002; Kelley et al., 2002; Mitchell,
nonpathological populations. When treated as a unidimen- Banaji, & Macrae, 2005; Ochsner et al., 2004; Saxe,
sional scale, the Narcissistic Personality Inventory assesses Moran, Scholz, & Gabrieli, 2006). Notably, heightened
grandiose narcissism, which is characterized by high self- MPFC activity is also observed when people think about
esteem, vanity, entitlement, a willingness to manipulate the traits and mental states of close others (Ochsner et al.,
and exploit others for personal gain, and high levels of 2005), suggesting that the MPFC may be part of a network
defensiveness in response to self-threats (e.g., Paulhus, of brain systems that mediates social knowledge in general.
Robins, Trzesniewski, & Tracy, 2004; Raskin, Novacek, & Moreover, different areas of the brain become active when
Hogan, 1991). Like individuals with unstable high self- people report on the self-descriptiveness of trait terms
esteem and contingent self-esteem, those high in grandi- associated with domains with which they have high versus
ose narcissism appear to have “fragile” high self-esteem low levels of personal experience (Lieberman, Jarcho, &
because their self-esteem is easily threatened and requires Satpute, 2004). This latter work is interpreted as evidence
constant validation (Kernis, 2003). that different brain systems process evidence-based (high
Recently, theorists have given increasing attention to a personal experience) and intuition-based (low personal
second narcissistic subtype referred to as vulnerable nar- experience) self-knowledge. The larger point here is that
cissism (see Dickinson & Pincus, 2003). Like grandiose no single brain system or area of the brain appears to
narcissists, vulnerable narcissists entertain self-aggran- be, of itself, responsible for our sense of self. Instead, mul-
dizing fantasies about themselves, and they demonstrate tiple systems work together to create the sense of a unitary
a heightened sense of entitlement and a willingness to self, and some of the same systems that mediate self-
exploit others. In contrast to grandiose narcissists, how- knowledge are involved in mediating knowledge about
ever, vulnerable narcissists report feelings of inferiority, others’ traits and states.

CH16.indd 598 10/22/09 2:50:05 PM


Origins of Self-Representations 599

Genes and Heredity and worth early in life, before they have self-awareness.
Working models reflect the consistency and responsiveness
Much of what is known about the genetic bases of the
of treatment that infants receive from primary caregiv-
self comes from the personality literature, which typically
ers. Specifically, caregiving that is both consistent and
assesses personality by having people report their self-
adequately responsive to infants’ needs should convince
views (e.g., Vazire, 2006). Based largely on twin studies,
them that they are worthy of love and capable of effica-
this research indicates a substantial genetic basis to people’s
cious action. This, in turn, should instill in children the
self-views within the broad, “Big Five,” personality
foundations of favorable self-concepts and high global
factors of extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, con-
self-esteem (Bretherton, 1988; Verschueren, Marcoen, &
scientiousness, and openness. Specifically, approximately
Schoefs, 1996). Conversely, caregiving that is inconsis-
40% to 60% of the population variance in self-reports of
tent, unresponsive, neglectful, or abusive teaches children
the Big Five factors is accounted for by genes (for a review,
that they are not valuable, that others are not trustworthy
see Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001). The remaining variance
and dependable, or both. In such cases, relatively negative
is typically attributed to environmental influences, gene–
self-concepts and low esteem will likely result.
environment interactions, and chance factors.
This is not to suggest, however, that young children with
Recently, there has been a surge of interest in under-
negative models of self will describe themselves in unfa-
standing the genetic bases of global self-esteem. Thus far,
vorable terms. In fact, young children appear to display
researchers in this area have demonstrated that self-esteem
what Swann and Schroeder (1995, p. 1310) refer to as a
is heritable (McGuire et al., 1999) and that genes explain
“positive tropism”—a cognitively simplistic, automatic,
approximately 30% to 50% of the population variance in self-
and adaptive propensity to seek positive evaluations and
esteem (Kamakura, Ando, & Ono, 2007; Kendler, Gardner, &
avoid negative ones. Indeed, research reveals that young
Prescott, 1998). Heredity also appears to explain a substantial
children generally describe their qualities and skills in an
amount of the variance in self-esteem stability and change
extremely positive manner (Harter, 1999), and the ten-
across time (Neiss, Sedikides, & Stevenson, 2006).
dency to endorse positive self-descriptions emerges before
Despite the vigor with which some theorists have inte-
the tendency to endorse negative ones (e.g., Benenson &
grated genetic influences into broad, biosocial models of
Dweck, 1986; Stipek & Tannatt, 1984). This may occur
the self (e.g., Tesser, 2002), the biology of the self remains
because, before middle childhood, children lack the cogni-
an area of inquiry in which research lags behind theory.
tive capacity to differentiate between their actual and ideal
One interesting challenge for future researchers will be to
selves, and they answer questions about the self primar-
specify the biological bases of distinctions that are basic
ily in terms of their ideals (Harter, 2006; Turner, 1968).
to the area, such as enduring versus weakly held identities.
Alternatively, it may be that children in the West are social-
One possibility is suggested by a general theory of learning
ized to embrace positive evaluations spontaneously and
known as Hebb’s (1949) law. The law states that if one neu-
without reflection (e.g., Heine & Hamamura, 2007). In any
ron (A) is repeatedly involved in causing another neuron
event, around middle childhood, children begin to display
(B) to fire, metabolic changes occur in one or both cells
a more nuanced understanding of the self, and stable indi-
that enhance the ability of A to cause B to fire. Simply put,
vidual differences in self-concepts and self-esteem emerge.
cells that “fire together, wire together.” Insofar as this prin-
Specifically, children at this age begin to internalize
ciple applies to the clusters of neurons or pathways that are
the appraisals of others (see the next section). Thus, it
associated with self-knowledge, then it may be that endur-
may be that the working models developed during infancy
ing self-representations are simply ones that have been
provide a lens through which children interpret others’
activated repeatedly in the past (which makes them more
reactions to them. Indeed, some research suggests that
readily activated in the future). Although this particular
the working models that are set in place during infancy
account is speculative, it is clear that further elaboration of
continue to influence people’s interpretations of social
the neural bases of self-knowledge could be extremely ben-
feedback into adulthood (e.g., Hazan & Shaver, 1987). It
eficial to the development of theory within the subarea.
is important to note, however, that a substantial minority
(30–45%) of people change their attachment style—their
Interpersonal Origins of the Self and Identity characteristic pattern of relating to others—across time
(e.g., Cozzarelli, Karafa, Collins, & Tagler, 2003).
Attachment Relationships
Appraisals
According to attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969; Hazan &
Shaver, 1994), infants begin to formulate rudimentary Whereas working models presumably filter people’s
schemas—or working models—about their lovability interpretations of self-relevant experiences and feedback,

CH16.indd 599 10/22/09 2:50:05 PM


600 Self and Identity

appraisals are part of the raw materials from which people people undoubtedly base their self-knowledge to some
derive specific beliefs about the self. As noted earlier, degree on the feedback they receive from others, they have
Cooley (1902) and Mead (1934) relied on the theory of other influential sources of self-knowledge.
symbolic interactionism to explain how people translate
Social Comparisons
others’ reactions into self-knowledge. According to this
theory, people come to know their own abilities, traits, and According to Festinger ’s (1954) social comparison theory,
qualities by seeing themselves through other people’s eyes. people develop self-knowledge by comparing their own
More specifically, the symbolic interactionists described a traits, abilities, opinions, and emotions with those of similar
sequence in which we (a) observe others’ reactions to our others (for reviews, see Suls & Wills, 1991; Taylor & Lobel,
behaviors; (b) use others’ reactions to construct reflected 1989). Moreover, the direction of comparison that people
appraisals, or inferences about how others perceive us; make—upward versus downward—is assumed to influence
(c) internalize these reflected appraisals as elements of the their resulting self-views and feelings of self-esteem. For
self-concept; and (d) use the self-concept to guide subse- example, while comparing oneself with someone who is
quent behaviors. Thus, the self is created socially and is better than the self on a particular dimension of evaluation
subsequently sustained in a cyclical, self-perpetuating (an upward comparison) can diminish a person’s feeling
manner. of self-esteem, comparing oneself with someone who is
Because children younger than 8 years lack the worse off than the self (a downward comparison) tends
perspective-taking skills to evaluate themselves through to boost self-esteem (e.g., Helgeson & Mickelson, 1995;
the eyes of others (e.g., Selman, 1980), they do not typi- Marsh & Parker, 1984).
cally demonstrate an awareness of others’ appraisals until Besides increasing people’s self-knowledge, social
middle childhood. It is most likely for this reason that clear comparisons serve an important motivational purpose.
individual differences in self-reported self-esteem do not For instance, sufferers of stressful events and painful life
emerge until middle childhood (Harter, 1999). At this point, experiences can facilitate their own coping and improve
people who perceive that they are respected, admired, and their affective state by comparing themselves with others
loved accordingly internalize these appraisals as positive who are worse off than them (Wood, Taylor, & Lichtman,
self-views, whereas those who perceive that they are eval- 1985). Indeed, a large body of research suggests that people
uated unfavorably develop negative self-views. In turn, tend to make downward social comparisons when under
people’s self-views shape their subsequent interpretations conditions of threat (Taylor & Lobel, 1989). When moti-
of others’ reactions to them: Whereas people high in self- vated to improve the self, however, people may make
esteem believe that others perceive them quite favorably, upward comparisons with those who embody excellence
those low in self-esteem tend to underestimate how favor- along particular dimensions (Blanton, Buunk, Gibbons, &
ably they are appraised by others (Bohrnstedt & Felson, Kuyper, 1999).
1983; Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 2000). As with reflected appraisals, research suggests that
Despite the intuitive appeal of symbolic interactionism people do not develop the cognitive ability to compare the
in its original form, research has necessitated significant self explicitly with others until middle childhood (Harter,
refinements of some of its tenets (see Tice & Wallace, 1999; Ruble, Boggiano, Feldman, & Loebl, 1980). Once this
2003, for a review). In particular, while it is clear that ability emerges, social comparisons tend to occur spontane-
people’s reflected appraisals correlate strongly with their ously, effortlessly, and even unintentionally. For example,
self-views—that is, people see themselves the way they some work demonstrates that people change their self-
believe that others see them—it is not clear that people’s views automatically on comparison with both appropriate
reflected appraisals correspond to others’ actual evalua- and inappropriate comparison partners. Given adequate
tions of them (Shrauger & Schoeneman, 1979). Moreover, mental resources, however, people “mentally undo” modi-
little evidence indicates that people observe specific oth- fications to the self-concept that are based on inappropriate
ers’ reactions to them and then base their self-views on that comparisons (Gilbert, Giesler, & Morris, 1995).
feedback. Instead, people’s own beliefs about the self seem
Incorporating Others’ Qualities
to shape their assumptions about how others view them
(Kenny & DePaulo, 1993). Finally, people have a more As we saw in the previous three sections, people can
accurate understanding of how they are perceived by others acquire novel self-knowledge through their interactions
in general than of how they are perceived uniquely by with significant relationship partners. Self-expansion the-
specific others (Kenny & Albright, 1987), a finding that ory (Aron & Aron, 1996), however, highlights yet another
further challenges the notion that people internalize the route through which people’s interactions with close others
feedback they receive from specific others. Thus, although can lead to changes in the self. The theory predicts that

CH16.indd 600 10/22/09 2:50:06 PM


Origins of Self-Representations 601

as closeness between two people grows, they gradually (Alicke & Govorun, 2005; Taylor & Brown, 1988). As a
come to experience a cognitive “overlapping” of their self- result, people who are raised in individualistic cultures
concepts. As a consequence, relationship partners begin report substantially higher global self-esteem than do peo-
to act as if the resources, perspectives, and characteristics ple raised in collectivistic cultures (Heine & Hamamura,
of the close other are at least partially their own (Aron, 2007). In analyses that treat culture as the unit of analysis,
Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991). Thus, in close relation- there is a strong positive correlation between a culture’s
ships, people acquire novel self-knowledge in the form of individualism and the global self-esteem of its inhabit-
features of the close other that have been subsumed into ants (Oyserman et al., 2002).
the self-concept. Support for these ideas can be found in Using a two-component definition of self-esteem,
research demonstrating that people cognitively confuse however, may lead to a more nuanced understanding of
the self more with close others than they do with non- the effects of culture on the positivity of the self-concept.
close, but equally familiar, others (e.g., Mashek, Aron, & Consider research showing that people raised in collectivistic
Boncimino, 2003). Similarly, people’s self-concepts cultures demonstrate relatively high levels of self-liking,
contain more self-descriptive information in the weeks whereas those raised in individualistic cultures demonstrate
immediately after, versus before, they fall in love (Aron, relatively high levels of self-competence (Tafarodi, Lang, &
Paris, & Aron, 1995), suggesting that features of the new Smith, 1999; Tafarodi & Swann, 1996). As noted earlier,
loved one are incorporated into the self. self-liking reflects people’s evaluations of themselves in
terms of qualities that link them to others; conversely, self-
Influences of Culture and Gender competence reflects people’s evaluations of themselves in
To a large degree, people’s self-knowledge reflects the cul- terms of qualities that distinguish the self from others. If
ture in which they are socialized. Researchers interested collectivistic cultures value the group over the individual,
in the effects of culture on the self have long assumed that then it makes sense that people with interdependent selves
the broad dimensions of collectivism and individualism derive self-esteem primarily from those qualities that make
differentiate not only the normative rules and structures of them good group members, such as loyalty (Sedikides,
societies but the self-structures of individuals as well (for Gaertner, & Vevea, 2005; but see Heine, Kitayama, &
a review, see Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). Hamamura, 2007, who raise serious questions regarding the
Individualism refers to a set of beliefs and values that has, at evidentiary basis of this assertion). It is also no surprise
its core, the assumption that individuals are ascendant over that people with independent selves tend to derive self-
the groups to which they belong. Conversely, collectiv- esteem primarily from the qualities that make them stand
ism holds that individuals are mutually interdependent and out from others. Within their cultural contexts, each way of
that groups take priority over individuals (Hofstede, 1980; constructing self-esteem makes adaptive sense.
Triandis, 1995). Given these different emphases on the Mirroring these cultural differences are gender differ-
individual versus the group, it is not surprising that people ences in the extent to which people exhibit interdependent
who are socialized in different cultures often display versus independent selves. Whereas girls are often social-
self-concepts with remarkably different structures, proper- ized to prioritize the qualities that align them to others,
ties, and contents. boys are taught to prioritize the qualities that distinguish
In their review of cross-cultural differences in the self, and differentiate them from others (e.g., Spence, Deaux, &
Markus and Kitayama (1991) proposed that collectivistic Helmreich, 1985). Accordingly, women tend to develop
cultures generally give rise to people with interdependent more interdependent selves, and men tend to develop more
selves, whereas individualistic cultures engender people independent selves (Cross, Bacon, & Morris, 2000; Cross &
with independent selves. In the interdependent self, the Madson, 1997). Moreover, women tend to link their self-
individual is connected to significant others, relatively esteem to their relational qualities, whereas men link
undifferentiated, and fluid across contexts and time; in their self-esteem to their independent qualities (Josephs,
the independent self, the individual is distinct from oth- Markus, & Tafarodi, 1992).
ers, autonomous, and stable across contexts and time.
Consistent with the different values that underlie collec- Intrapsychic Origins of the Self and Identity
tivism versus individualism, people with interdependent
Self-Perception
versus independent selves tend to exhibit divergent motiva-
tions regarding the self. For instance, whereas people with Like outside observers, people sometimes learn about them-
interdependent selves appear to value modesty and self- selves by observing their own behavior, and the situation in
criticism (Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama, 1999), those which it occurs, and then inferring their underlying attitudes
with independent selves prefer being better than others and dispositions (Bem, 1972). According to self-perception

CH16.indd 601 10/22/09 2:50:06 PM


602 Self and Identity

theory, people are most likely to acquire self-knowledge When people do engage in introspection, the fruits
through observation of their own behavior when their self- of their efforts are sometimes rather bitter. For example,
knowledge is weak, ambiguous, or difficult to interpret. introspection about the reasons behind one’s attitudes,
Moreover, many attributional principles that guide people’s behaviors, and feelings is likely to produce inaccurate self-
perceptions of others also operate when they infer their own knowledge (Wilson, Laser, & Stone, 1982). As a conse-
dispositions via self-perception. Thus, for example, the self- quence, when people introspect about the reasons behind
knowledge that people acquire through self-perception of their feelings, they sometimes change their feelings to
their behavior is less certain to the extent that multiple pos- match the reasons they generate (Wilson & Kraft, 1993),
sible causes exist for that behavior (Kelley, 1971). As such, which can lead them to make decisions that they later
an overjustification effect occurs when people lose intrinsic regret (Wilson et al., 1993).
motivation to perform a certain activity because extrinsic Introspection can also lead people to compare their cur-
rewards convince them that they are performing the behav- rent achievements and behaviors with their beliefs about
ior merely to obtain the rewards (for reviews, see Deci, how they should or ought to be, which can create dis-
Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Lepper, Henderlong, & Gingras, comfort if there is a disparity (Duval & Wicklund, 1972;
1999). Higgins, 1987). Falling short of internal standards can pro-
As noted earlier, research on the inaccuracy of reflected duce painful feelings of shame for those who are prone to
appraisals casts doubt on the notion that others truly serve attribute their shortcomings to their whole self (Tangney &
as “looking glasses” in which people see the self reflected Dearing, 2002). Some propose that this state of self-aware-
(Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934). On this point, self-perception ness can be so troubling that people go to great lengths to
theory may provide an answer. Instead of accurately per- “escape the self ” through activities such as drinking, drug
ceiving themselves through the eyes of others, people may use, binge eating, and even suicide (Baumeister, 1991).
engage in self-perception of their own behaviors and then Introspection can also have desirable effects, however.
attribute these perceptions of the self to others (Kenny & Introspecting about who one is, for example, can produce
DePaulo, 1993). If so, then self-perceptions of behavior accurate self-knowledge if people have sufficient cogni-
may play an important role in two related, but distinct, tive resources (Hixon & Swann, 1993). Similarly, when
self-knowledge processes: First, self-perceptions are used self-reflection reveals that one meets or exceeds one’s
as a basis for inferring one’s own internal qualities, traits, standards, positive feelings result (Greenberg & Musham,
attitudes, and the like, and second, self-perceptions are 1981; Silvia & Abele, 2002). Moreover, people are more
used as a basis for inferring how others view the self. likely to behave in line with their personal values when
In an interesting twist on self-perception theory, Goldstein in a state of self-awareness, suggesting that introspection
and Cialdini (2007) proposed that people can learn about can promote adaptive (or at least self-consistent) self-
their own internal states—at least their temporary ones—by regulation (e.g., Beaman, Klentz, Diener, & Svanum, 1979;
observing the behavior of others with whom their identities Gibbons, 1978).
are merged. The logic is that, when viewing a close other
Experiencing the Subjective Self
perform a behavior, people vicariously infer novel informa-
tion about the self, resulting in a change in the self-concept. Yet another source of self-knowledge is the continual,
Although the notion of “vicarious self-perception” is rela- ever-changing “stream of consciousness” about which
tively new, it fits well with the general idea that people mod- James (1890/1950) wrote—the spontaneous thoughts,
ify the self to achieve greater congruence with the presumed feelings, and reactions that constitute the self-as-subject
beliefs, self-views, and attitudes of close others (Baldwin, (or “I”). Experiencing the subjective self differs from both
Carrell, & Lopez, 1990; Hinkley & Andersen, 1996). self-perception and introspection in fundamental ways. For
instance, whereas both self-perception and introspection
Introspection
involve reflection on the self, experiencing the subjective
Unlike self-perception, which involves observing one’s self can involve an outward focus, a full engagement in
overt behaviors and using them to infer one’s internal the moment that draws attention away from the self (e.g.,
qualities, introspection involves deliberate attempts to Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). Moreover, whereas behaviors
achieve self-knowledge by directing attention “inward.” provide the raw material for self-perception processes,
While introspecting about the self seems like a fairly private thoughts and feelings provide the raw material for
obvious route to self-knowledge, research suggests that subjective self processes.
people spend surprisingly little time (about 8% of total Research suggests that people rely more on their sub-
thoughts) reflecting on themselves (Csikszentmihalyi & jective experiences than on their overt behaviors when con-
Figurski, 1982). structing self-knowledge (Andersen, 1984; Andersen & Ross,

CH16.indd 602 10/22/09 2:50:06 PM


Motivational Properties of the Self 603

1984), and when people encounter others who share their such as “worthwhile,” questions of accuracy are impossible
subjective reactions to the world, they feel especially to answer definitively because choosing one or more crite-
attracted to them (Pinel, Long, Landau, Alexander, & ria is inherently subjective. More specific self-views such
Pyszczynski, 2006). Apparently, when people sense that as “extroverted” or “fastidious,” however, have relatively
they and others perceive the world through the same psy- clear empirical referents. Empirical assessments of accu-
chological “lens,” their confidence in the validity of their racy have therefore focused on lower-level self-views.
own visions of reality is reinforced. Such “I-sharing” may Some of the most telling studies of accuracy of self-
constitute a powerful antidote to the problem of existential knowledge involve comparing the capacity of people’s
isolation. self-ratings and the ratings of peers to predict some objec-
tive outcome, such as the ratings of observers, or some
naturally occurring outcome, such as success in military
Accuracy of Self-Knowledge
training. Different methodologies lead to different conclu-
Humans routinely assert that they know themselves. Most sions, but the studies using the strongest methodologies
people assert that they know themselves better than oth- generally conclude that well-acquainted observers are
ers do (Pronin, Kruger, Savitsky, & Ross, 2001). And it at least as accurate as are people themselves. The results
is not just laypeople who harbor this belief in the fidelity of one recent study (Vazire & Mehl, 2008) suggest that
of self-knowledge: Behavioral scientists harbor this same the specific content of criterion behaviors may be criti-
conviction. For example, in 2003, 70% of the studies pub- cal. These researchers compared the ability of individuals
lished in a leading personality psychology journal (Journal and acquainted others to predict naturally occurring behav-
of Research in Personality) relied on self-reports as the iors over a 4-day period. Findings revealed that individuals
index of personality (Vazire, 2006). Such confidence in were more accurate in predicting some of their behaviors
the veracity of self-knowledge is challenged by a spate of (e.g., deliberate behaviors such as arguing) but acquain-
demonstrations, mostly conducted in the laboratory, indicat- tances were more accurate in predicting other behaviors
ing that some aspects of self-knowledge are simply wrong (e.g., spontaneous behaviors such as talking one on one).
(e.g., Epley & Dunning, 2006; Wilson & Gilbert, 2003; Although it is too early to draw definitive conclusions
for non-laboratory studies, see Gosling, John, Craik, & from this work, it is probably safe to say that people
Robins, 1998; Vazire & Mehl, 2008). At least two potential are accurate about the self within some arenas but that
sources of such errors exist. In the tradition of Freud, many others—particularly close others—may be better able to
have argued that people unconsciously or consciously sup- predict people’s reactions within other arenas, especially
press unwanted thoughts and feelings. Although intrigu- when people’s own wishes and desires compromise their
ing, these processes have been notoriously difficult to objectivity. Therefore, some analysts have concluded that
demonstrate empirically (for reviews, see Erdelyi, 1974, accurate self-knowledge is best obtained not from intro-
1993). The other source of errors in self-knowledge is that specting but instead from consulting with friends and
people simply lack access to many processes that give rise acquaintances or observing one’s own behavior (Wilson &
to self-relevant behaviors, which throws a wrench into Dunn, 2004).
the process of introspection. As a result, people err when
asked about the causes of their actions (Nisbett & Wilson,
1979), their attitudes (e.g., Galdi, Arcuri, & Gawronski, MOTIVATIONAL PROPERTIES OF THE SELF
2008), or their future emotional reactions (e.g., Wilson &
Gilbert, 2003; for a review, see Wilson & Dunn, 2004). In My thinking is first and last and always for the sake of my
the end, such errors could undermine the veracity of peo- doing.
ple’s representations of themselves. —William James (1890/1950, p. 333)
Such sharp clashes between people’s intuitions and the Although James believed that the major function of self-
results of systematic research invariably led theorists and knowledge was to guide action, this proposition has proven
researchers to ask which source—individuals or research surprisingly controversial in certain quarters. Indeed, some
findings—really is more accurate. Such questions regard- prominent thinkers have gone as far as to take the oppo-
ing the validity of self-knowledge are particularly vexing site position, suggesting instead that self-knowledge is
due to the criterion problem: It is easy to say whether or an epiphenomenal product of social relations that has no
not someone has brown eyes or even a pleasant smile, but causal status. In self-perception theory, for example, Daryl
the problem of assessing the validity of self-knowledge is Bem (1972) proposed that the flow of influence between
knotty indeed (e.g., Kruglanski, 1989; Swann, 1984). For behavior and the self is unidirectional: We infer who we
example, when it comes to high-level, global self-views by observing our own behavior and the conditions under

CH16.indd 603 10/22/09 2:50:06 PM


604 Self and Identity

which it unfolds, but self-knowledge has no impact on their standing relative to others. Kruger and Dunning
subsequent action. Theorists from different theoretical tra- (1999), for example, had participants estimate their perfor-
ditions have echoed Bem’s assertions. Group researcher mance on dimensions such as humor, grammar ability, and
John Turner, for example, contended that personal self- logical reasoning. Regardless of their actual performance,
views are “there to be explained, not in themselves expla- participants estimated that they scored in the 60th to 70th
nations” (Turner, Reynolds, Haslam, & Veenstra, 2006, percentile. The result was that low scorers overestimated
p. 25). Such pockets of skepticism notwithstanding, it their performance but high scorers underestimated their
is fair to say that today the self ’s motivational proper- performance. Similarly, other findings reinforce the notion
ties are widely accepted among most students of the self that people’s performance estimates should not be taken at
(e.g., Higgins & Pittman, 2008; Pittman & Zeigler, 2007). face value. Indeed, in estimating performance relative to
Testimony to contemporary enthusiasm for motivational “average performance,” people seem to rely on a heuris-
processes is offered by the burgeoning literature on self- tic that leads them to rate everyone—including unknown
enhancement processes. strangers—slightly above average (Klar & Giladi, 1997).
To be sure, some researchers (Alicke, Klotz,
Breitenbecher, Yurak, & Vredenburg, 1995) have shown
Self-Enhancement Motive
that people display self-enhancing performance estimates
The self-enhancement motive has been defined in many even when asked to compare themselves against specific
ways, but it is most commonly conceptualized as a desire others. Nevertheless, evidence that such biases are stronger
to maximize the positivity of one’s self-views (e.g., when people have positive self-views (Brown, 1986) raises
Leary, 2007). The notion that people prefer and seek self- a further issue regarding the mechanism that gives rise to
enhancement is enormously popular, with one landmark these effects. That is, almost all studies that are taken as evi-
statement of the viewpoint—Taylor and Brown’s (1988) dence of self-enhancement suffer from a serious potential
literature review—garnering more than 2,200 citations. confound. Researchers have been aware of this confound
The popularity of the self-enhancement motive is easy to for some time but have failed to appreciate its full implica-
understand. After all, the notion that people want positive tions. For example, in their review of the positive illusions
evaluations seems like a relatively simple and readily test- literature, Taylor and Brown (1988) noted the following:
able argument. In addition, and perhaps more importantly,
the notion that self-enhancement is a basic human motive One caveat, however, deserves mention. A considerable amount
lends scientific credibility to the cultural assumptions and of the research cited demonstrates that people solicit and receive
self-confirming feedback, not necessarily positive feedback. For
normative behaviors of the researchers conducting the
example, a woman who thinks of herself as shy may seek and
research, most of whom happen to be Westerners.
receive feedback that she is (see Swann, 1983). At first, these
Little wonder, then, that self-enhancement assumptions results may seem contradictory with the position that social feed-
have found their way into an extraordinarily wide range back fosters positive self-conceptions, but in fact, they are not.
of contemporary theories (e.g., Hoyle, Kernis, Leary, & Because most people think well of themselves on most attributes,
Baldwin, 1999; Sedikides & Gregg, 2003). Purported confirming feedback is typically positive feedback. (p. 202)
evidence for the theory abounds. One popular phenom-
enon is the “better-than-average effect,” wherein most Taylor and Brown (1988) were alluding to an ambiguity
people assert that they are above average, a mathematical imposed by almost all research on self-enhancement having
impossibility. For example, college students overwhelm- been conducted on unselected participants, roughly 70% of
ingly report that they are above-average drivers (Svenson, whom have positive self-views (Diener & Diener, 1995).
1981). Ironically, when told of the existence of such “posi- Therefore, evidence of self-enhancement may reflect, to an
tive illusions,” people claim that they are less susceptible unknown degree, a desire for confirmation of chronic self-
to them than most others are (Pronin, Gilovich, & Ross, views (e.g., Kwan, John, Kenny, Bond, & Robins, 2004;
2004). At this juncture, dozens of such self-enhancing Kwan, John, Robins, & Kuang, 2008).
illusions have been reported in the research literature (for To illustrate the import of Taylor and Brown’s (1988)
reviews, see Helweg-Larsen & Shepperd, 2001; Sedikides & caveat, consider one of the most robust findings in the self-
Gregg, 2008). enhancement literature: the tendency for people to make
Yet, in recent years, researchers have begun to voice self-serving attributions (e.g., Mezulis, Abramson, Hyde, &
reservations about social psychology’s motivational cen- Hankin, 2004; Miller & Ross, 1975). When researchers
terpiece. Some findings suggest that some specific effects conducted a parallel study in which they measured the self-
reflect the failure of participants to comprehend fully what views of participants, they discovered that those with posi-
they are being asked when they are encouraged to estimate tive self-views displayed the self-serving pattern but those

CH16.indd 604 10/22/09 2:50:07 PM


Motivational Properties of the Self 605

with negative self-views displayed precisely the opposite Some counter such contentions by arguing that the East–
pattern, perceiving negative evaluators to be more accurate West difference in behaviors related to self-enhancement
than positive evaluators (e.g., Swann, Predmore, Griffin, & reflects a difference not in the strength of the self-enhance-
Gaines, 1987). This pattern of data clearly indicates that a ment motive itself but only in how people pursue this
desire to confirm, rather than enhance, self-views under- motive (e.g., Kurman, 2003; Yik, Bond, & Paulhus, 1998).
lies participants’ responses. Japanese are modest, the argument goes, as a means of
Another finding that has widely been attributed to attaining social acceptance, which is considered self-
self-enhancement strivings is the tendency for people enhancing in Japanese culture (e.g., Sedikides, Gaertner, &
to selectively recall positive feedback about themselves Toguchi, 2003). Although this tactic may appear to rescue
(Sanitioso & Wlodarski, 2004). Again, when researchers self-enhancement theory from disconfirmation, it does so at
measured the self-views of participants in such studies, the cost of redefining self-enhancement from a theory about
they discovered that their responses seemed to be driven by a preference for positive evaluations to a theory about a
a desire for self-confirmation. That is, only those with posi- desire for social acceptance or communion. As we argue later,
tive self-views preferentially recalled positive feedback; the two motives are quite different in form, structure, and
people with negative self-views displayed the opposite ten- consequence. Moreover, to the best of our knowledge,
dency, recalling more negative than positive feedback (e.g., no one ever contended that the Japanese eschewed social
Story, 1998; Swann & Read, 1981). Similarly, people’s ten- acceptance or communion. To the contrary, it would seem
dency to define virtues as qualities they possess and vices as that, if anything, Japanese individuals are especially inter-
qualities they lack (Dunning, Perie, & Story, 1991) appears ested in social acceptance and communion (Kitayama,
to be due primarily to people who have relatively positive Markus, Matsumoto, & Norasakkunkit, 1997).
views of themselves (Beauregard & Dunning, 2001). A final concern grows out of mounting evidence that
The upshot of such findings is simple: Although “self- self-enhancement strivings can degrade the quality of
enhancement strivings” seem to be pervasive, the motive that is people’s relationships and even their well-being (Colvin,
driving such strivings may be self-confirmation rather than Block, & Funder, 1995; Paulhus, 1998; Robins & Beer,
self-enhancement. And even if it turns out that such puta- 2001; for a review, see Crocker & Park, 2004). Such evi-
tive self-enhancement strivings among people with positive dence leads one to wonder why the pursuit of a suppos-
self-views are indeed compelled by a self-enhancement edly basic human motivation should be associated with
motive, self-enhancement theory still cannot account for dysfunctional outcomes.
the responses of those with negative self-views. This is There are, then, reasons to ask whether the superficial
problematic for a propensity that is alleged to represent a charms of self-enhancement theory and research are out-
basic human motivation. If the self-enhancement motive weighed by some fundamental difficulties with the theory
is so basic, why has this news escaped the attention of the and the data that ostensibly support it. We think that
roughly 30% of the population who possess negative self- the answer to this question is yes, and we accordingly pro-
views? The nonenhancing responses of people with negative pose an alternative approach to self-related motivation in
self-views are particularly perplexing when we consider that the next section. Our goal is not to banish self-enhancement
when most human needs are frustrated people redouble their theory. Rather, we seek to partition it into two motives that
efforts to gratify those needs. Instead, people with nega- we perceive as more viable, both logically and empiri-
tive self-views actually embrace negative evaluations (for a cally. We then add an additional motive to the mix. The
review, see Swann, Chang-Schneider, & Angulo, 2007). result is three broad self-motives that serve to guide most
Even if evidence of self-enhancement from participants identity-relevant functioning.
in the West could be assumed to reflect a desire for self- Before turning to our three-motive scheme, let us add
enhancement, growing evidence indicates that such striv- three caveats. Our goal is to identify three broad self motives.
ings do not generalize across cultures. Japanese people, One can surely make fine-grained distinctions among vari-
for example, posses a relatively strong desire to be viewed ants of each of our motives, and it is no doubt useful to do so
as modest, and this desire causes them to eschew positive in certain contexts. In addition, our scheme is limited to iden-
evaluations at times. In addition, a relatively strong interest tity-related motives and hence excludes biological motives
in self-improvement among the Japanese seems to make (e.g., sex), as well as other motives that have appeared in
them more receptive to negative feedback than Americans formulations that are broader in scope (e.g., Fiske, 2004).
are (e.g., Heine, Kitayama, & Lehman, 2001). Japanese Finally, although we believe that this scheme offers a useful
participants are also less unrealistically optimistic about lens for viewing the self literature, we see its role as lim-
their futures when compared with American participants ited to just that; we make no claim to having discovered the
(Chang, Asakawa, & Sanna, 2001). motives that underlie all human social behavior.

CH16.indd 605 10/22/09 2:50:07 PM


606 Self and Identity

Tripartite Motivational Approach In addition to the desires for communion and agency,
we propose a third motive: the desire for coherence. In
There is no doubt that people prefer and enjoy positive over our usage, coherence encompasses feelings of regularity,
negative evaluations when they perceive that such evalua- predictability, meaning, and control. Coherence is distinct
tions are appropriate and deserved (e.g., Swann, Krull, & from consistency, which emerges whenever any two psy-
Pelham, 1989). Nevertheless, we suggest that obtain- chological elements follow logically from each other (e.g.,
ing positive evaluations cannot be an end in itself (e.g., Festinger, 1957). Thus, to maintain consistency between
Leary, 2007). Like paper currency, positive evaluations two elements, such as a behavior and a related identity,
are valuable more for what they symbolize than for their one can change either element. In contrast, coherence is
intrinsic properties. In particular, positive evaluations are a special case of consistency that refers specifically to the
valued because they are markers of one’s social worth (and degree of correspondence between one’s enduring self-
thus satisfy a desire for communion) and competence concept and the other elements in one’s psychological uni-
(and thus satisfy a desire for agency). From this vantage verse (English, Chen, & Swann, 2008).
point, motives for communion (belonging and interpersonal In some respects, the coherence motive may be even more
connectedness) and agency (autonomy and competence) fundamental than the desires for communion and agency
may be responsible for the phenomena that researchers (Guidano & Liotti, 1983; Popper, 1963). After all, those who
have attributed to self-enhancement strivings. lack the conviction that their knowledge system offers coher-
The desires for communion and agency have a promi- ent and trustworthy insights into the world around them are
nent history in the psychological literature. The needs for unable to evaluate evidence of social worth or competence
communion and agency are assumed to underlie many because they are not confident that they know such evi-
aspects of personality and social behavior (Baumeister & dence when they see it. If the self-views serve as the lenses
Leary, 1995; Wiggins & Broughton, 1991), and theories of through which people perceive reality, incoherence degrades
optimal functioning emphasize the importance of meeting the vision of reality that these lenses offer. Deprived of a
both needs (e.g., Ryff, 1989). In the domain of attitudes, clear vision of reality, people have little means of knowing
researchers suggest that constructs similar to communion whether what they “see” faithfully reflects reality. Indeed, if
and agency (i.e., warmth and competence) represent the people completely lose faith in the veracity of their knowl-
two basic dimensions of attitudes (e.g., Abele & Wojciszke, edge system, their sense of self begins to unravel and they fall
2007; Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007; Fiske et al., 2002; into a state of disintegration anxiety (Kohut, 1971). Deprived
Judd, James-Hawkins, Yzerbyt, & Kashima, 2005). Of of stable self-knowledge, people feel that they have no basis
even more relevance here, communion and agency corre- for knowing how to act, and guiding action is the primary
spond with the dual forms of self-esteem discussed ear- objective of thinking in the first place (James, 1890/1950).
lier in this chapter (e.g., Franks & Marolla, 1976; Gecas, Consider evidence that people who experienced events
1971). Using Tafarodi and Swann’s (2001) terms, the need that bolstered their feelings of communion or agency, or
for communion motivates a desire for self-liking and the both, also suffered deficits in mental and physical health
need for agency motivates a desire for self-competence. if those events challenged their need for coherence (e.g.,
Replacing the self-enhancement motive with the com- Swann et al., 2007). This research was based on the
munion and agency motives evades the reservations raised assumption that experiences that challenge one’s enduring
earlier regarding self-enhancement. For example, whereas self-views are stressful enough that, over time, they may
the self-enhancement formulation requires that people actually be physically debilitating. The first two studies
with both positive and negative self-views prefer positive (Brown & McGill, 1989) examined the impact of posi-
evaluations over negative ones, our formulation does not. tive life events on the health outcomes of people with low
Therefore, evidence that people with negative perceptions and high self-esteem. Positive life events (e.g., improve-
of their social worth and competence fail to embrace posi- ment in living conditions or getting a high grade) pre-
tive evaluations of themselves on these dimensions (e.g., dicted increases in health among high self-esteem people
Bosson & Swann, 1999) does not challenge the assumption but decreases in health among people low in self-esteem.
that such individuals want to enjoy feelings of communion A more recent study (Shimizu & Pelham, 2004) extended
and agency. Rather, communion and agency motives theo- these results by demonstrating that the effects replicated
retically encourage people to achieve actual communion even while controlling for negative affectivity (thus under-
and agency rather than merely seeking positive evaluations mining the rival hypothesis that negative affect influenced
for their own sake. Finally, although evidence indicates both self-reported health and reports of symptoms).
that people from Southeast Asia are less inclined to self- But if the desire for coherence may sometimes override
enhance than Westerners (e.g., Heine et al., 1999), the needs the desires for communion and agency, we do not mean
for communion and agency appear to be pancultural. to imply this is always the case. Whereas some degree

CH16.indd 606 10/22/09 2:50:07 PM


Motivational Properties of the Self 607

of coherence may be necessary for people to effectively assumed that people are profoundly invested in estimating
pursue their communion and agency needs, the opposite the extent to which they are valued by interaction partners,
may also be true. group members, and relationship partners. Signs of rejec-
At first blush, it might seem that the three-motive con- tion trigger an alarm reaction that is punctuated by a loss
ceptualization overlooks several motives that other theo- of self-esteem. Thus, self-esteem is a psychological “fuel
rists have deemed important. For example, whereas some gauge” that is sensitive to variations in perceived inclusion.
propose motives that are somewhat overlapping with ours Support for sociometer theory comes from evidence that
(acceptance, status, and meaning; Hogan & Shelton, 1998), manipulations that convey rejection, disapproval, or disin-
others differentiate between motives that we instead classify terest on the part of others tend to lower participants’ state
together (autonomy and competence; Deci & Ryan, 1995) self-esteem (e.g., Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995;
or introduce other motives into the mix (self-assessment; Nezlek, Kowalski, Leary, & Holgate, 1997). Similarly,
Sedikides & Strube, 1997). Still others propose a six-motive field studies demonstrate that self-esteem dips when
scheme (i.e., self-esteem, continuity, distinctiveness, belong- people experience rejection (Murray, Griffin, Rose, &
ing, efficacy, and meaning; Vignoles, Regalia, Manzi, Bellavia, 2003) and ostracism (e.g., Williams, 2001).
Golledge, & Scabini, 2006) that appears broader than our Moreover, longitudinal research shows that perceived rela-
approach. Nevertheless, our approach incorporates most of tional value is linked to changes in self-esteem over time
these other motives if one allows that several of the motives (Srivastava & Beer, 2005).
distinguished by previous researchers in fact reflect the same Sociometer theory has performed the useful service of
core motive with different criteria used to gauge its gratifica- focusing attention on the utility of people’s efforts to forge
tion. In what follows, we consider several examples of these connections with others. From an evolutionary perspec-
phenomena. tive, positive evaluations and the feelings of self-esteem
that they foster are useful not because of their intrinsic
Communion value but because they are markers of acceptance within a
The communion motive is designed to maximize feelings larger social group whose protection and shared resources
of acceptance, belongingness, and social worth. Humans were vital to humans’ survival. Conceivably, the argu-
evolved in the context of small, close-knit groups, and the ment could be taken even further, such that all self-views
need for communion remains a constant theme in the con- serve as indices of the manner in which we are perceived
struction and maintenance of the self and identity (Bowlby, by others. Accepting this broader interpretation, however,
1969; McAdams, 1989). On a biological level, evidence sug- could lead one to question the novel contribution of soci-
gests that people require a minimum number of close, posi- ometer theory, because the notion that self-views provide
tive, interpersonal connections to thrive. Those who lack such a window into the perceptions of others has been around
connections exhibit relatively poor physical health, weak- for more than a century (e.g., Cooley, 1902). These and
ened immune functioning, and even higher mortality rates other considerations led some theorists to raise troubling
(House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988; Uchino, Cacioppo, & indictments of the theory (e.g., Pyszczynski, Greenberg,
Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996). On a psychological level, people Solomon, Arndt, & Schimel, 2004). A further limitation
who lack positive affiliations with others experience trou- of the model, however, is that although people are aware of
bling feelings of loneliness (Archibald, Bartholomew, & how others perceive them in general, they seem fairly inept
Marx, 1995; Newcomb & Bentler, 1986), while those with at discriminating the appraisals of specific other individu-
rich social networks report higher levels of happiness and als (e.g., Kenny & DePaulo, 1993). Therefore, the “self-
life satisfaction (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). So esteem fuel gauge” sometimes offers faulty information.
central is communion to humans’ existence that Baumeister In addition, it is clear that self-esteem tracks more than
and Leary (1995) deemed the need to belong a “fundamen- simply social acceptance. For example, self-esteem seems
tal” human motive. Although researchers generally agree acutely sensitive to indicators of agency.
that the criterion for this motive should be the appraisals
Agency
of others, the precise nature of these appraisals has varied.
Whereas advocates of the self-enhancement motive argue The agency motive is theoretically designed to maximize
for the importance of positive evaluations (e.g., Murray, feelings of autonomy (e.g., self-determination) and compe-
Holmes, & Griffin, 1996; Sedikides & Strube, 1997), others tence. The need for agency begins to guide behavior early
emphasize feelings of connectedness or belonging (e.g., in life, such as when infants strain to escape their caregivers
Baumeister & Leary, 1995). so that they can explore and manipulate the world around
In recent years, the desire for communion assumed them (Bowlby, 1969). Later in life, people’s sense of effi-
center stage in one prominent approach: Leary’s socio- cacy forms a core component of personality (Bandura, 1991)
meter theory (Leary & Downs, 1995). Leary and colleagues and contributes to psychological well-being (Ryff, 1989).

CH16.indd 607 10/22/09 2:50:07 PM


608 Self and Identity

In the most general case, the agency motive encourages and China, suggesting that it is not limited to a specific
people to strive for successful performance of valued activ- cultural milieu (e.g., Heine et al., 2006).
ities. A special case of agency strivings emerges when peo- Moreover, two self theories feature a desire for coherence
ple seek to improve themselves (e.g., Heine, Kitayama, & as the primary motivational mechanism: self-assessment
Lehman, 1999; Taylor, Neter, & Wayment, 1995). Self- (e.g., Trope, 1983) and self-verification (Swann, 1983).
improvement strivings are presumably initiated when peo- Each theory focuses on different criteria for assessing the
ple focus on negative aspects of the self that they need to coherence of self-related information. Self-assessment
change to meet personal or social standards. theorists have suggested that when people are uncertain
Some theorists argue that the prevalence of the self- of their self-views they seek relatively objective, diagnos-
improvement motive is strongly influenced by cultural tic information about themselves. A series of laboratory
factors. In particular, researchers suggest that East Asian studies provides support for this general approach (e.g.,
cultures place an emphasis on self-improvement at the Brown, 1990; Strube, 1990). While the issue of the relative
expense of self-enhancement (e.g., Heine et al., 1999; potency of self-assessment strivings remains to be deter-
Kitayama & Markus, 1999). The rationale for this conten- mined, its range of application is limited to self-views of
tion rests largely on the relative levels of individualism ver- which people are uncertain. This is an important limita-
sus interdependence in East Asian versus Western cultures. tion, for people tend to be highly certain of the qualities
Individualistic cultures (e.g., those in United States and that they care about. Such highly certain self-views have
Australia) place a premium on independence and therefore been the province of various self-confirmation theories
emphasize individual needs, goals, and rights. In contrast, (e.g., Lecky, 1945; Secord & Backman, 1965), the most
collectivistic cultures (e.g., those in East Asia and Latin recent of which is self-verification theory (Swann, 1983).
America) emphasize ingroup goals, needs, and obligations Self-verification theory assumes that, out of a desire for
and thus strongly value interdependence. In such cultures, social worlds that are coherent and predictable, people
it is particularly crucial to attend to others’ perspectives want others to see them as they see themselves. This desire
so as to meet the expectations of ingroup members and can be understood on both epistemic and pragmatic levels.
maintain interpersonal harmony (Heine et al., 1999). This Epistemically, receiving self-verifying evaluations reas-
greater sensitivity to social standards presumably explains sures people that their self-views accurately reflect social
the relative eagerness of East Asians to improve them- reality and that they can count on their self-views to guide
selves to meet others’ expectations. their behavior. Pragmatically, self-verifying appraisals
signal to people that others hold appropriate expectations
Coherence
of them and that their interactions will therefore proceed
Widespread support exists for the notion that people have a smoothly. Among people with positive self-views, the
deep-seated need for psychological coherence (Guidano & desire for self-verification works with the desires for com-
Liotti, 1983; Heine, Proulx, & Vohs, 2006). As Popper munion or agency, as all of these motives encourage people
(1963) contended, infants are born with a predisposition to who view themselves positively to embrace positive evalu-
identify patterns and regularities. Without this predisposi- ations. Among people with negative self-views, however,
tion, they would be incapable of learning: self-verification theory predicts that they will seek nega-
tive evaluations (e.g., Swann et al., 1989). Self-verification
The expectation of finding a regularity . . . connected with an theory thus makes divergent predictions for people with
inborn propensity to look for regularities, or with a need to
enduring positive versus negative self-views.
find regularities. . . . This “instinctive” expectation of find-
One focus of research has been on the variables that
ing regularities . . . is logically a priori to all observational
experience, for it is prior to any recognition of similarities . . .
determine when people will prioritize self-verification
and all observation involves the recognition of similarities (or over the competing desire for positive feedback. The
dissimilarities). (pp. 47–48) desire for self-verification prevails (e.g., people with nega-
tive self-views prefer and seek negative evaluations) when
The coherence construct has gone by several labels, the self-view is firmly held (i.e., certain and important;
including security in Maslow’s (1954) motivational hierar- Pelham & Swann, 1994; Swann & Pelham, 2002) or extreme
chy, need for closure in Kruglanski’s (1990) formulation, (Giesler, Josephs, & Swann, 1996), when the relationship
need for structure in Neuberg and Newsom’s (1993) model, is relatively enduring (Campbell, Lackenbauer, & Muise,
and meaning according to Hogan and Shelton (1998). 2006; Swann, De La Ronde, & Hixon, 1994), and when
Evidence of the coherence need has surfaced in many cul- people have the cognitive resources needed to compare
tures in addition to North America, including countries in the feedback against a relevant mental self-representation
Eastern and Western Europe, as well as in Australia, Korea, (Hixon & Swann, 1993; Swann et al., 1990). In addition,

CH16.indd 608 10/22/09 2:50:08 PM


Motivational Properties of the Self 609

challenges to a self-view will trigger compensatory activity Interestingly, people strive to affirm their positive self-
that shores up that self-view or some other component of views in ways that may have little to do with the nature of
the self-system (Swann & Hill, 1982; Swann, Wenzlaff, & the self-threat (Aronson, Cohen, & Nail, 1999; Tesser &
Tafarodi, 1992). Interestingly, such compensatory reac- Cornell, 1991). In some early research, the researchers
tions are symmetrical with respect to self-esteem; just as used a cognitive dissonance paradigm to show that the
high self-esteem people strive to reaffirm their positive self-threat that arises from counterattitudinal behavior can
self-views in the wake of negative feedback, those with be alleviated by having participants first affirm an impor-
low self-esteem strive to reaffirm their negative self-views tant, self-relevant value in a domain unrelated to that of the
in the wake of positive feedback (Swann & Read, 1981; dissonant behavior (Steele & Liu, 1983). In a similar vein,
Swann, Wenzlaff, & Tafarodi, 1992). Finally, whereas the later studies indicated that people were more willing to
foregoing research focused on personal self-views, other examine useful but potentially threatening feedback about
research extended the findings to collective self-views themselves if they first enjoyed success on an unrelated
(Chen et al., 2004; Lemay & Ashmore, 2004) and group task (Trope & Pomerantz, 1998).
identities (Gómez, Seyle, Huici, & Swann, in press). One especially fruitful line of research was designed to
Researchers have identified several distinct strategies explore the health implications of self-affirmation theory.
of self-verification. For example, people gravitate toward The results of one study indicated that people were more
self-verifying environments, such as interaction partners willing to examine potentially threatening information
who see them congruently and who are apt to provide them related to AIDS prevention after an important but unre-
with self-verification (e.g., Robinson & Smith-Lovin, lated value was affirmed (e.g., Sherman, Nelson, & Steele,
1992; Swann, Wenzlaff, Krull, & Pelham, 1992). Once 2000). In this and related studies, researchers appear to
in a given setting, people display “identity cues” (i.e., overt have uncovered an effective strategy for neutralizing the
signs of who they are, such as clothing or office décor) defensive reactions that have long impeded efforts to enlist
that communicate their identities to others (Gosling, Ko, the compliance of people who engage in risky behav-
Mannarelli, & Morris, 2002). In addition, people can elicit iors (e.g., Jemmott, Ditto, & Croyle, 1986; Liberman &
self-verifying reactions by behaving in ways that evoke Chaiken, 1992).
such responses (Swann & Hill, 1982; Swann & Read, Within our scheme, another hybrid approach is terror
1981). And if these strategies fail to evoke self-verifying management theory (Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski,
evaluations, people can distort nonverifying feedback 2004). At its core, this theory is concerned with people’s
through preferential attention and recall (Swann & Read, attempts to evade the existential anxiety that arises from
1981), construe the feedback in ways that make it fit with awareness of their own mortality. To quell the fear of
their enduring self-views (Swann et al., 1987), or even death, people work to convince themselves that they are
leave the relationship (Swann & Pelham, 2002). worthwhile actors who are playing an important role in
a world that has meaning and purpose. More specifically,
people rely on their self-concepts (beliefs about the self
Hybrid Theories
relative to culturally valued standards), their cultural
Elements of the three motives described above can be worldviews (sets of socially shared beliefs and values),
found in two major social psychological theories of and their close relationships to help them manage the fear
the self, self-affirmation theory and terror management of death. When people encounter challenges to any compo-
theory. Self-affirmation theory (Steele, 1988) combines nents of this belief system, death awareness increases and
elements of the agency and coherence motives (see also existential anxiety ensues (Schimel, Hayes, Williams, &
self-esteem maintenance theory, Tesser, 1988). This Jahrig, 2007). Note that self-views, worldviews, and rela-
theory focuses on how people react when they encoun- tionships provide much (if not all) of the raw material
ter challenges to their positive self-views—specifically, through which people meet their needs for agency, coher-
challenges to the sense of being a moral, adaptive, and ence, and communion. Moreover, clear parallels can be
capable person. The theory assumes that the self-system drawn between the needs for self-esteem, meaning, and
is composed of many interrelated parts that interact with relationships, as discussed in terror management theory,
one another. As a result, shoring up one component of and the three motives that we emphasize here. Thus,
the system can buttress other components against threats. while sharing our recognition of the importance of the tri-
Thus, whereas people ordinarily respond defensively partite self-motives, terror management theory uniquely
when they receive information that challenges a positive proposes that people pursue these three self motives as
self-view, these defensive reactions can be attenuated by a means of buffering themselves against a primitive and
inoculating them with positive feedback. basic fear of death.

CH16.indd 609 10/22/09 2:50:08 PM


610 Self and Identity

Not surprisingly, people react strongly when their mortal- and communion are really performed in the service of
ity is made salient (for a review, see Solomon et al., 2004). In fending off anxiety associated with death. Yet some have
dozens of inventive and provocative studies, researchers have raised serious challenges to this assumption. For example,
shown that those who are reminded of their own mortality are Hart and colleagues independently threatened each of these
more concerned with having high self-esteem (Greenberg, three self-motives and found that, at least among some indi-
Solomon, et al., 1992) and are more inclined to behave in viduals (those who had insecure attachment styles), each
ways that defend and maximize self-esteem (Taubman Ben- type of threat produced defensive reactions that were similar
Ari, Florian, & Mikulincer, 1999). Mortality salience manip- in character to those produced by mortality salience manip-
ulations also have a bearing on group relations. For example, ulations (Hart, Shaver, & Goldenberg, 2005). From this
those high in mortality salience are especially motivated to vantage point, reminders of death may gain their potency
defend their cultural worldviews by derogating people who because they represent a triple whammy: they simultane-
challenge these beliefs (e.g., Florian & Mikulincer, 1997; ously undermine the assumptions that we have a future
Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon, self to which we can aspire (challenging coherence needs),
1989). Notably, the tendency for mortality salience to trig- we have enduring relationships (challenging communion
ger such activities is diminished among people with elevated needs), and we will accomplish things in the future (chal-
levels of self-esteem (e.g., Greenberg, Solomon, et al., 1992) lenging agency needs; for a similar view, see McGregor,
and among those in whom hope for an afterlife has been Gailliot, Vasquez, & Nash, 2007). This reasoning raises a
primed (Dechesne et al., 2003). critically important question: Is fear of death the ultimate
Terror management theory has also helped illuminate motivator of behavior that terror management theory would
recent political trends in the United States. For many have us believe it is, or are other high-level motives (such
Americans, the attack on the World Trade Center on as our tripartite motives) of themselves responsible, with
September 11, 2001, was a naturally occurring manipulation the potency of mortality salience manipulations residing in
of mortality salience. Terror management theory suggests their capacity to activate all three motives simultaneously?
that under such conditions people reach out for a strong- At this juncture, this question remains unanswered.
willed and decisive leader who promises to defend and
protect them. U.S. President George W. Bush represented
just such a leader to many, and as the theory would predict, SELF IN RELATIONSHIP TO OTHERS
his popularity soared after the attacks on the twin towers.
More impressive evidence for the theory was provided by The idea that the self is socially constructed was first elabo-
a series of experiments indicating links among the attacks, rated by the symbolic interactionists (Cooley, 1902; Mead,
mortality salience, and endorsement of Bush. For exam- 1934). Since then, numerous theorists have emphasized the
ple, subliminal exposure to stimuli related to September fundamentally interpersonal nature of the self (for a review,
11 increased participants’ death-related thoughts, as well see Markus & Cross, 1990). So crucial are social interac-
as their support for Bush. Furthermore, mortality salience tions to the construction and maintenance of the self-concept
made participants more inclined to vote for Bush in the that people surely would not possess self-views were it not
upcoming presidential election and less inclined to vote for for their interactions with others. Consider Gallup’s (1977)
his opponent, Senator John Kerry (Landau et al., 2004). seminal work on self-awareness in chimpanzees, which
More generally, death anxiety appears to be a robust pre- compared the self-recognition abilities of chimps raised
dictor of rightwing, conservative thinking (Jost, Glaser, in isolation with those of chimps raised with conspecifics.
Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003), and salient encounters Whereas the chimps with prior social experience readily
with mortality-threatening events can compel people to recognized their own reflections in a mirror, those raised
adopt more politically conservative values and beliefs in isolation showed no signs of self-recognition. Although
(Bonanno & Jost, 2006). It is important to note, however, they undoubtedly saw themselves reflected in the surface
that the link between mortality salience and political con- before them, the isolated chimps possessed no basis for
servatism is not always so direct. Among both strong adher- understanding exactly who or what they were looking at.
ents of political liberalism and those in whom the value Our genetic similarity to chimps suggests that a similar
of tolerance has been primed, mortality salience actually fate might befall humans raised in isolation.
increases acceptance of differing worldviews (Greenberg, To make sense of the vast theoretical and empirical
Simon, Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Chatel, 1992). literatures on the interpersonal self, Markus and Cross
In short, terror management theory raises the interesting (1990) identified three ways in which others shape the self.
possibility that many everyday behaviors that have tradition- First, individuals come to know who they are, within a
ally been chalked up to motives such as agency, coherence, larger social structure, through their interactions with others.

CH16.indd 610 10/22/09 2:50:08 PM


Self in Relationship to Others 611

This type of interpersonal influence occurs when individuals that they attach to these memberships (Tajfel, 1981;
internalize the values, attitudes, thoughts, feelings, and Tajfel & Turner, 1979). These identities presumably
social roles to which they are exposed. It refers to those emerge throughout the life span, beginning when children
aspects of the self that are acquired via ongoing interac- learn, through interactions and communications with care-
tions with significant others and that become internalized givers and others, the normative behaviors, feelings, and
so thoroughly by the individual that “they seem the natural values associated with the various social groups to which
and inevitable consequences of his or her own thoughts” they belong. Once formed, social identities seem to exert a
(Markus & Cross, 1990, p. 582). Next, people rely on feed- powerful influence on social thought and behavior. Indeed,
back and information from others to form the basis of their some argue that because social identities are the building
self-knowledge, as well as to evaluate, maintain, and regu- blocks of personal identities, social identities are more apt
late the self. This type of interpersonal influence is exem- to influence behavior than are personal identities (Turner
plified by work on symbolic interactionism (Cooley, 1902; et al., 2006).
Mead, 1934), social comparisons (Festinger, 1954), self- One version of social identity theory assumes that people
presentation (Tedeschi, 1981), and self-verification (Swann, enter groups that they perceive as both positive and distinc-
1983), among other theories. What these approaches share tive as a means of self-enhancement (e.g., Abrams & Hogg,
is an emphasis on the ways in which the self is influenced 1988). Consistent with this idea, evidence indicates that
by others’ real, perceived, and imagined reactions. Finally, people display a strong ingroup bias, or tendency to favor
people’s interpersonal relationships themselves become their own group relative to outgroups (e.g., Brewer & Kramer,
part of the self, as when individuals store mental representa- 1985; Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971). Combined
tions of close others alongside information about the self in with the outgroup homogeneity effect—the perception of
memory. Work on self-expansion (Aron & Aron, 1996) and greater similarity among the members of outgroups as com-
individual differences in individualism versus collectivism pared with ingroups (Linville & Jones, 1980)—this bias
(Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1995) illustrates this facilitates people’s ability to dehumanize members of out-
type of interpersonal influence on the self. An assumption groups by perceiving them as lacking in human qualities.
that underlies this work is that close relationship partners Dehumanization, in turn, plays a role in the justification and
do not just exist outside of us—in a real sense, close part- maintenance of intergroup prejudice and conflict (Cortes,
ners “get inside our heads.” Demoulin, Rodriguez, Rodriguez, & Leyens, 2005; Vaes,
In what follows, we consider theories of self and iden- Paladino, Castelli, Leyens, & Giovanazzi, 2003).
tity that illustrate each of Markus and Cross’s (1990) three In recent years, social identity approaches have shifted
types of interpersonal influence. Note that much of the away from an emphasis on self-enhancement as the opera-
material discussed elsewhere in this chapter (e.g., symbolic tive motive. Self-categorization theory avoids the issue
interactionism, attachment dynamics, and self-motives) of motivation altogether, stressing instead that the per-
could fit just as easily in this section. To avoid redundancy, ceptual processes that prompt humans to parse the world
however, we confine this section to material that we have into “us” and “them” are hardwired and basic to human
not discussed at length elsewhere. Moreover, the placement existence (Turner, 1985; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, &
of theoretical approaches into one of the three categories of Wetherell, 1987). Other approaches argue that social
influence is, admittedly, rough at times. Certainly, the identities reduce uncertainty (e.g., Hogg, 2000; Hogg &
processes by which people construct, maintain, and store Mullin, 1999), make the world more sensible and coher-
self-knowledge will, at times, reflect multiple forms of ent (e.g., Ellemers & Van Knippenberg, 1997), or pro-
interpersonal influence. Nonetheless, we impose order by tect people from the existential terror of death (Castano,
discussing social identity, self-categorization, and stereo- Yzerbyt, Paladino, & Sacchi, 2002). Whatever the nature
type approaches under the Constructing the Self heading; of the motive that causes people to identify with groups, it
desires for self-consistent versus overly positive partner is ironic that although group memberships are essential for
appraisals under the Evaluating and Maintaining the Self survival, they also place people in grave danger, such as
heading; and broad cognitive models of the interpersonal when social identities motivate people to confront or even
self under the Including Others in the Self heading. kill one another.
Of course, considerable individual differences exist in
Constructing the Self how central of a role social identities play in people’s lives.
Most people perceive gender and ethnicity to be impor-
Social Identity Approach tant social identities, but variation occurs in the strength
Social identities refer to people’s knowledge of their mem- of people’s identification with these groups (Luhtanen &
berships in social groups and the emotional significance Crocker, 1992). Moreover, the centrality of people’s social

CH16.indd 611 10/22/09 2:50:08 PM


612 Self and Identity

identities varies not only as a function of the desirability of antagonism posits that as the salience of group identities
the group (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) but also as a function increases, the salience of personal identities decreases.
of the group’s structure, such as its size and distinctiveness Similarly, the theory argues that when people enter groups,
relative to outgroups (Brewer, 2003). they undergo a “depersonalization” process wherein they
Placing importance on one’s social identities can yield come to see themselves as categorically interchangeable
both positive and negative consequences. On the positive with other group members. Recently, some theorists have
side, for members of low-status groups, higher levels of suggested that these principles may not always apply
group identification can provide a psychological buffer (e.g., Postmes & Jetten, 2006; Simon, 2004). A case in
against the negative effects of discrimination on self- point is offered by a recent study of compensatory self-
esteem, well-being, and achievement (e.g., Wong et al., verification among “fused” people, that is, people whose
2003). Some propose that strong identification with stig- personal and social self-views have fused. Compensatory
matized ingroups at least partially explains the relatively self-verification refers to the tendency for people to react
high self-esteem and favorable self-views of members of to self-discrepant (i.e., overly positive or negative) evalu-
many low-status groups (Branscombe, Schmitt, & Harvey, ations by intensifying their efforts to elicit self-verifying
1999; see also Crocker & Major, 1989). On the negative evaluations (e.g., Swann & Read, 1981). Because the per-
side, those who identify more strongly with their social sonal and social identities of fused people are functionally
groups are more likely to display the perceptual and moti- equivalent, challenging either type of identity should fuel
vational processes that contribute to intergroup conflict behavioral efforts to reaffirm the other type of identity.
and prejudice (e.g., Branscombe & Wann, 1994; Feather, Consistent with this reasoning, when researchers presented
1994; Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1996). Strong identifi- participants with overly positive feedback that challenged
cation with social groups can also encourage rigid com- the validity of their personal self-views, fused participants
pliance with the group’s behavioral norms, even when (but not nonfused participants) compensated by affirming
noncompliance would be beneficial. For example, some their social self-views. Specifically, they expressed greater
research reveals that members of ethnic minority groups willingness to fight and die for their country (Swann,
avoid beneficial health behaviors—such as exercise and Gómez, Seyle, Morales, & Huici, 2009). Such findings
eating healthy foods—to the extent that they perceive those indicate that personal and social self-views may sometimes
behaviors as violating their ingroup’s norms (Oyserman, combine synergistically rather than at cross-purposes (see
Fryberg, & Yoder, 2007). Thus, stronger identification also Jetten, Branscombe, & Spears, 2002).
with ingroups increases people’s motivation to defend and In a related vein, optimal distinctiveness theory sug-
uphold group norms even when these norms are harmful. gests that just as people have an inherent drive to identify
Since its inception, social identity theory has offered a with groups, they also have an opposing drive for individu-
powerful and generative framework for understanding how ation (Brewer, 1991). To cope with these conflicting agen-
individuals connect themselves to the larger social structure das, people strike a balance by finding a point of “optimal
and rely on groups to provide them with self-knowledge, distinctiveness,” an identity that simultaneously addresses
meaning, and purpose. Although the seeds of the theory their needs for affiliation and individuation. This approach
were sewn during post–World War II Europe (e.g., Tajfel, shares with self-verification theory the assumption that
Jaspars, & Fraser, 1984), social identity theory’s impact group members remain interested in being individuated
now extends far beyond the continent of its birth and it and attaining verification of their personal identities when
is considered a major social psychological theory on an they enter groups. One could go even further to suggest that
international level. As evidence of the theory’s genera- people may affiliate (at least in part) as a means of obtain-
tivity, a PsycINFO search of articles and chapters with ing verification for their personal identities. An example of
keywords of “social identity,” “ingroup,” or “outgroup” this would be a woman who joins a chess club to verify her
produced more than 3,000 publications at the time of this personal identity as highly intelligent.
writing. Moreover, the theory has been revitalized by Researchers operating outside the social identity tradi-
new approaches (e.g., Oyserman et al., 2007; Vaes et al., tion have independently investigated the interplay between
2003), as well as applications to diverse subareas within personal and social self-views. One line of research
the behavioral sciences. focused on what happens when the social stereotypes of
some individuals (“perceivers”) channeled their behavior
Interplay of Personal and Social Selves toward other individuals (“targets”). This work revealed
Self-categorization theory (Turner et al., 1987) proposes that perceivers elicited behaviors from targets that con-
that the relationship between personal and social self- firmed their stereotypes (Snyder, Tanke, & Berscheid,
views is hydraulic. For example, the principle of functional 1977). Subsequent investigations examined the conditions

CH16.indd 612 10/22/09 2:50:09 PM


Self in Relationship to Others 613

under which targets who confirmed the expectations of that, in performance settings, anxiety due to stereotypes
perceivers internalized these expectations into correspond- can distract the individual and cause poor performance
ing self-views (for a review, see Snyder & Klein, 2005). and failure. Such failure may, in turn, cause the marginal-
In more recent years, researchers have suggested that, ized group member to disengage from the activity. If the
even if stereotypes do not cause perceivers to behaviorally activity happens to involve education, such disengagement
constrain the response options of targets, the mere exis- may undercut the future socioeconomic viability of the
tence of a stereotype may shape the behavior of targets in marginalized group (see Aronson & Steele, 2005). Such
undesirable ways. In part, this research was a reaction to effects appear to be strongest when the targets of stereo-
indictments of Black American culture that can be traced types value strongly their group identities. That is, targets
to the anthropologist John Ogbu. On the basis of anecdotal who value their group memberships are more likely to
evidence, Ogbu suggested that in the United States, the perceive discrimination against their group (Eccleston &
Black minority culture gradually developed an “opposi- Major, 2006; Major, Quinton, & Schmader, 2003), and
tional” orientation that encouraged them to disengage from their performance is more likely to suffer when they
the educational system, which was perceived as a “White” are reminded of negative stereotypes about their group
domain (e.g., Fordham & Ogbu, 1986). This argument (Kiefer & Sekaquaptewa, 2007; Schmader, 2002).
gained widespread acceptance in the popular media, which
used it to explain the Black–White achievement gap. Evaluating and Maintaining the Self
Although the validity of Ogbu’s assertions was never
established, the psychological literature offers some evi- In Orson Welles’s (1941) Citizen Kane, Charles Foster
dence that people who identify themselves as having low Kane holds grandiose illusions about his wife’s talents
status sometimes embrace these negative identities (e.g., as an opera singer. So smitten is Kane with Susan’s sing-
Spears, Jetten, & Scheepers, 2002) or even the political ing voice that he uses his wealth and power to secure her
systems that perpetuate their low status (Jost, Banaji, & headlining roles in world-class venues. Sadly, however,
Nosek, 2004). Moreover, the more people face discrimi- Susan knows that she is not the musical virtuoso that Kane
nation, the more they emphasize the devalued identity believes her to be. Painfully aware of her vocal limitations,
(Branscombe, Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje, 1999; Jetten, Susan grows increasingly traumatized by the humiliation
Branscombe, Schmitt, & Spears, 2001). of having to parade her mediocrity in front of an audience.
More direct evidence that cultural groups differ in the Eventually, the pressure of Kane’s misguided illusions
extent to which they promote academic achievement has grows too much for Susan to bear, and she tries to escape
come from researchers who asked why Asian Americans by taking her own life. Although Susan survives, she never
(specifically Chinese and Korean immigrants) outperform forgives Kane for refusing to see her for who she really is.
both Blacks and Whites on most indices of academic per- The relationship experiences of Kane—one of the most
formance. Adopting an interactionist framework, some unique and memorable movie characters of all time—are
argue that the relatively high academic performance of by no means typical. Nonetheless, we believe that his con-
Asian Americans is multiply determined (Portes & Zhou, flict with Susan illustrates a fairly common relationship
1993). For example, factors such as immigration selectivity, problem. Specifically, when couples disagree about “who
above-average levels of pre- and postmigration socioeco- is who” within the relationship, unhappiness ensues.
nomic status, and ethnic social structures are thought to As noted in our discussion of the coherence motive,
interact with immigrant optimism and the belief in edu- people desire appraisals from their relationship partners that
cation to override blocked mobility (Zhou & Kim, 2006). verify their highly certain and important self-views, even if
From this perspective, it is overly simplistic to blame the these self-views are negative. Moreover, the need for self-
underperformance of some groups on constructs such as confirming appraisals runs particularly strong in the context
“oppositional culture,” since social structural variables of relationships characterized by high levels of interdepen-
must surely play a role as well. At a minimum, a culture of dence. For example, among both college roommates and
achievement requires economic resources to support it. married couples, people with positive self-concepts prefer
Others have developed formulations that attempt to partners who view them favorably, whereas those with
explain the underachievement of minorities and other negative self-concepts prefer partners who view them nega-
negatively stereotyped groups without referring to cultural tively (Swann & Pelham, 2002; Swann et al., 1994). And
variables. For example, Steele (1997) contended that for when people’s spouses view them in a manner that is dis-
the marginalized, stereotypes represent a “threat in the crepant with their stable self-views, their relationships are
air” that can trigger anxiety—even when the stereotypes characterized by high levels of marital distress (Schafer,
are recognized as fallacious. Research supports the notion Wickrama, & Keith, 1996). Indeed, as illustrated by the

CH16.indd 613 10/22/09 2:50:09 PM


614 Self and Identity

case of Susan Kane, psychological health and well-being An approach that confronts this issue directly assumes
may suffer when people’s close relationship partners dis- that it is a covariate of self-view globality—relationship
agree with them about who they are (Swann et al., 2007). relevance—that is critical. Whereas Swann and colleagues
And yet, despite the results of research demonstrating (1994) tend to measure people’s self-views within domains
that people desire consistent appraisals from their close that vary in their relevance to relationship satisfaction and
relationship partners, some research suggests instead that functioning (e.g., intelligence, social skills, artistic abili-
people prefer overly positive evaluations from their part- ties, and physical attractiveness), Murray and colleagues
ners. For example, Murray and colleagues find that even limit their focus to domains that are high in relationship
people with negative self-views feel most satisfied in their relevance, such as warmth, kindness, and dependability
relationships, and closest to their partners, when partners (e.g., Murray et al., 1996). Stated differently, whereas
view them more favorably than they view themselves (e.g., Swann and colleagues consider self-views that fall along
Murray et al., 1996). Furthermore, Murray suggests that the both agentic and communal dimensions, Murray and col-
tendency to idealize romantic partners facilitates relation- leagues focus primarily on communal self-views. Given
ship success by assuaging people’s doubts and giving them the critical importance of communal qualities in the context
the confidence to trust each other (Murray et al., 2000). of relationships, it is perhaps not surprising that Murray’s
From this perspective, it is overly positive appraisals— research participants were particularly happy with part-
not self-confirming ones—that members of intimate rela- ners who held idealized images of their communal traits.
tionships crave. Consistent with this possibility, Swann et al. (2002) found
What might account for the apparent discrepancy that romantic partners preferred appraisals from their part-
between these two bodies of work? One possibility con- ners that matched their self-views in most domains but
cerns the level of abstraction (e.g., global versus specific) desired overly positive appraisals in domains that they
at which these two sets of researchers typically mea- considered highly important for relationship satisfaction,
sure partners’ self-views and perceptions of each other. such as physical attractiveness.
Whereas most desires for self-verifying appraisals occur at
the level of specific self-views (“My partner think I am Including Others in the Self
at the 55th percentile in cooking ability”), most desires
for illusory appraisals occur at the level of global self- Several theorists have examined the ways in which close
views (“My partner thinks I am loving and kind”). Thus, relationships alter and influence the cognitive contents of
it may be that members of happy couples maintain ador- the self. For instance, as noted earlier, research on self-
ing appraisals of their partners at a global level while also expansion reveals that people incorporate the perspectives,
appraising their partners accurately at a more specific level resources, and characteristics of close others into their own
(e.g., Neff & Karney, 2002). Indeed, the results of longitu- self-concepts (Aron et al., 1991). Thus, closeness with
dinal investigations suggest that marriages are most likely others inevitably leads to an expansion of the self, as the
to endure over the long haul when partners’ global love for self-concept grows to incorporate new features (e.g., Aron
each other is based on an accurate understanding of each et al., 1995). One consequence of this cognitive overlap
other ’s specific traits and qualities (Neff & Karney, 2005). between self and other is that people sometimes display
As such, overly positive appraisals of partners may promote “self–other confusions” (Mashek et al., 2003), suggest-
relationship satisfaction, provided that they are grounded ing that information about close others and the self is pro-
in reality. cessed similarly.
One shortcoming of the global-enhancement, specific- Whereas self-expansion research focuses on the ten-
verification argument is that there is no theoretical reason dency to assume the features of close relationship part-
people should suspend their self-verification attempts once ners, other approaches look at how cognitive processes
self-views exceed some threshold of globality. After all, are shaped more broadly by relationships. For example,
if the primary function of self-views is to enable people Baldwin (1992) proposes that people store information
to understand the world and guide behavior, then people about relationship partners in the form of relational sche-
should be motivated to verify their global self-views in mas, or mental models consisting of scripts that describe
non-relational contexts. In fact, research on depression typical interaction patterns, as well as representations of
(e.g., Giesler et al., 1996; Swann, Wenzlaff, Krull, & the self and other that capture how the self typically feels
Pelham, 1992; Swann, Wenzlaff, & Tafarodi, 1992 ) and and behaves when interacting with the other. As such, cues
self-esteem (Wiesenfeld, Swann, Brockner & Bartel, 2007) that bring a particular relationship partner to mind also
has shown that people are indeed motivated to verify their activate people’s mental representations of self with other
global negative self-views. (Ogilvie & Ashmore, 1991) and call to mind those aspects

CH16.indd 614 10/22/09 2:50:09 PM


Identity Negotiation and Change 615

of self-knowledge that are contained within that relational encourage people to negotiate identities that are compatible
schema (Hinkley & Andersen, 1996). with their chronic self-views. At times, however, target
Similar assumptions form the foundation of Andersen individuals may encounter partners who are unable or
and Chen’s (2002) relational self theory. In their theory, unwilling to honor their chronic identities. To the extent
Andersen and Chen suggest that people’s stored represen- that targets are invested in the identity (e.g., it is high in
tations of self and others play important roles in shaping certainty and importance), they may intensify their efforts
personality and the self by guiding the cognitive, affec- to elicit self-verifying evaluations. If investment is low and
tive, motivational, and behavioral patterns that become resistance from the perceiver is high, however, targets may
activated in particular contexts. Because representations behaviorally confirm the expectations of perceivers (e.g.,
of the self are linked in memory with representations of Snyder & Klein, 2005). Eventually, they may internalize
significant others, any chronic or transient reminders the new behaviors into their self-concept, resulting in iden-
of significant others activate particular relational selves tity change.
and their accompanying styles of thinking, feeling, and
acting. Thus, personality and the self are interpersonal Identity Negotiation in the East and West
patterns that reflect the various selves an individual has
constructed in the context of relationships with signifi- Like most psychological structures, identities survive only
cant others. Although they acknowledge that the self most insofar as they receive periodic nourishment from the
likely contains some aspects that are not directly related to social environment. Therefore, characteristics of the social
representations of significant others, Andersen and Chen milieu are a key determinant of how much identity change
propose that the bulk of self-knowledge is acquired in the any given individual experiences. Consider Western versus
context of relationships and that significant others are thus Eastern cultures. Western cultures encourage identity sta-
“basic to self-experience” (p. 638). bility by placing a premium on consistency in the identities
people negotiate both over time and across settings. Given
this, it is not surprising that identity stability seems to be
IDENTITY NEGOTIATION AND CHANGE relatively high in samples of Western participants. For
instance, people’s self-descriptions on the Big Five factors
People can take on numerous identities. The same man, for of extraversion, neuroticism, conscientiousness, agreeable-
example, may be warm with his children, guarded with his ness, and openness remain stable (correlations between
co-workers, and a blend of both with his neighbors. This .30 and .50) over periods of up to 20 years (e.g., Conley,
fact of social life can prove challenging for those who are 1985). Stability estimates are even higher (correlations in
trying to predict what their partners are going to do next. the .55–.85 range) if the measurement period is reduced to
We propose that people meet this challenge through the several years (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1988) or if the self-
process of identity negotiation, which allows relationship views are high in importance or certainty (English & Chen,
partners to establish “who is who” via ongoing, mutual, 2007; Pelham, 1991).
give-and-take interactions with each other. Once people Relative to Western culture, the culture of East Asians
establish a “working consensus” that is agreeable to both places more emphasis on relationships, connectedness, and
parties (e.g., Goffman, 1959; Swann & Bosson, 2008), belonging. This makes East Asians particularly inclined to
their mutually agreed on expectations transform discon- conform their identities to the expectations and preferences
nected individuals into collaborators who have common of their current interaction partner. As a result, relative to
obligations, goals, and often, a modicum of commitment North Americans, East Asians show lower cross-situational
to each other. In this way, identity negotiation processes stability in their self-descriptions (Kanagawa, Cross, &
provide the “interpersonal glue” that allies people with Markus, 2001; Suh, 2002). Furthermore, when describing
one another. More generally, just as identities define themselves, East Asians are especially inclined to endorse
people and make them viable as humans, identity negotia- semantically opposite self-views (Choi & Choi, 2002) and
tion processes define relationships and make them viable contradictory statements about themselves (Cousins, 1989;
as a foundation for organized social activity. Spencer-Rodgers, Peng, Wang, & Hou, 2004).
Identity negotiation theory (Swann & Bosson, 2008; On the surface, evidence that the identities of East
Swann, Johnson, & Bosson, in press) elaborates on the Asians appear to be relatively situation specific may seem
interpersonal principles that guide identity negotiations. to undermine the notion that there exists a universal desire
People follow these processes, albeit largely unintention- for coherence. But perhaps not. For those who define
ally, during each of several successive stages of social themselves in relational terms, coherence may hinge on
interaction. Typically, the principles of identity negotiation the propensity to honor identities that are negotiated with

CH16.indd 615 10/22/09 2:50:10 PM


616 Self and Identity

specific others. Consistent with this reasoning, among the members of these groups. The Women’s Liberation movement,
Japanese, cross-situational consistency is less valued and for instance, led to the erosion of cultural stereotypes that char-
more weakly related to the sense of having a “true” self acterized women as weak and dependent (e.g., Spence et al.,
than it is among Westerners (Kashima et al., 2004). 1985). As these stereotypes lost force and more egalitarian
From this vantage point, there appear to be some intriguing attitudes took hold, girls and women adopted corresponding
differences in identity negotiation processes in the East changes in their identities and associated behaviors.
versus the West. It is tempting, for example, to conclude Changes in people’s immediate social environment can
that the tendency for behavioral confirmation to trump self- also foster identity change. When, for example, people
verification is more prevalent in East Asian than in Western enter college or move they may encounter relatively unique
cultures. Although this characterization seems technically expectations and behavioral norms among the locals (e.g.,
accurate, it is probably somewhat misleading because self- Iyer, Jetten, & Tsivrikos, 2008). By altering the way people
verification may simply take a different form in Eastern relate to others, new settings may encourage people to alter
cultures. More specifically, highly relational cultures may their self-views (Hormuth, 1990). In addition, new envi-
prioritize the tendency for relationship partners to remain ronments may foster identity change because they lack
true to the identities that they have negotiated within the the opportunity structures (McCall & Simmons, 1966)
relationship while being relatively unconcerned with that once nurtured and sustained the original identity. For
the partner ’s behavior outside the confines of the relation- people to sustain their identities, on entering a new set-
ship (see the discussion of circumscribed versus global accu- ting they must remoor their identities within the new social
racy in Gill & Swann, 2004). More generally, evidence that structure (Ethier & Deaux, 1994). Failure to do so results
East Asians display less cross-situational consistency in identity change.
than Westerners does not necessarily imply that Asians
Developmental Growth and Role Changes
routinely experience true changes in their identities. In
the section that follows, we suggest that for true identity When the community recognizes a significant change in an
change to occur a relatively dramatic shift in the social individual, it may set in motion a sequence of events that
environment must occur that supports the change. produces identity change. Examples of such community-
initiated changes include changes in age (e.g., when
adolescents become adults), status (e.g., when graduate
Antecedents of Identity Change
students become professors), or social role (e.g., when sin-
Our emphasis on the importance of coherence thus far gles get married). When such transformations occur, com-
would suggest that changes in identity are typically unwel- munities may abruptly alter the way they treat the person.
come phenomena that can confuse or even derail the process Even if targets of such differential treatment resist change
of identity negotiation (as well as cause stress and under- at first, eventually they recognize the inevitable, become
mine health at the individual level). Yet as wrenching less invested in maintaining the initial identity, and bring
as identity changes may sometimes be, they are a natu- their identities into agreement with the treatment they
ral and critically important part of life (Robins, Noftle, receive. Studies of adolescence support this reasoning.
Trzesniewski, & Roberts, 2005). Although many identity Theory and research alike suggest that late adolescence
changes are triggered by events over which the person has marks a developmental period during which changing
no control, at times people disregard their coherence striv- treatment and expectations trigger dramatic identity change
ings and deliberately attempt to change their identities. (Arnett, 2000; Erikson, 1959; Pals, 1999).
Note, for example, that communion or agency motives
Acquisition and Loss of Abilities
may sometimes override the desire for coherence, and the
former motives may sometimes require identity change if The process of maturation is marked by the acquisition
they are to be gratified. In what follows, we consider four of new competencies and the loss of established ones.
sets of conditions that foster identity change. Whether one gains or loses an ability, the associated iden-
tity needs to be updated. This may explain why people’s
Sociocultural and Environmental Changes identities are especially turbulent early and late in life.
Over the past half century, the United States saw sweeping Although both gains and losses are sprinkled throughout
changes in cultural expectations regarding groups that were the life span, gains tend to occur often during the early
historically saddled with minority status. For example, the years (e.g., acquiring the ability to scale mountains and
Civil Rights and Women’s Liberation movements altered cul- drive automobiles) and losses tend to occur during the later
tural expectations for Blacks and women, respectively. These years (e.g., losing the ability to scale mountains and drive
shifting expectations gradually influenced the identities of automobiles). Indeed, one of the most troubling aspects of

CH16.indd 616 10/22/09 2:50:10 PM


References 617

the aging process is not age itself but the loss of capacities James (1890/1950) more than a century ago. The result of
that have become essential to the person’s feelings of this neo-Jamesian approach to the self has been an unprec-
agency. From this vantage point, it is easy to understand edented explosion of conceptual and methodological inno-
how the physical changes that accompany aging can take a vations that have breathed new life into the subarea.
toll on people’s identities (Whitbourne, 1996). But if this sudden burst of creativity has had clear bene-
fits, it has had costs as well. To us, the most worrisome risk
Self-Initiated Changes is that the subarea will become so broad that it will begin
When people recognize that an identity is undermining to lose focus. Eventually, newcomers may begin to wonder
their capacity to achieve a valued goal, they may negotiate whether there is any “there, there.” To avert this unhappy
a different identity within a circumscribed set of circum- outcome, we suggest setting a boundary condition for the
stances. If the fruits of such negotiations remain in effect subarea: Work on the self should involve some consider-
for an extended period, the changes may generalize to other ation of the self as a mental representation.
settings and eventually lead to permanent identity change. A secondary concern is the lack of integration both
Consider, for example, a woman who suspects that her low within and across topic areas. This is understandable
self-perceived attractiveness will block her efforts to win given the complexity of the subject matter and the result-
the heart of a would-be lover. Recognizing the dilemma, she ing challenge of constructing meaningful integrations.
may strive to be exceptionally attractive in the presence of Nevertheless, if the field is ever to develop a unified theory
her love interest (Swann et al., 2002). If she succeeds and of the self, it is critical that theorists continue to forge con-
wins her beloved’s affections, she may internalize his nections among different themes in the literature.
appreciation of her beauty and upgrade her self-perceived As future researchers rise to the challenge of forging such
attractiveness (Jones, Gergen, & Davis, 1962). connections, they will build on the fundamental truths that
The foregoing scenario is just one example of a larger are gradually emerging in the literature. One truth that has
class of instances in which people initiate an identity change already been distilled is that the self is, as the symbolic inter-
either because they want to repair an unsatisfying life situ- actionists have long emphasized (e.g., Stryker, 2000), a social
ation or because they aspire to self-improvement. Some phenomenon. From our first inklings of self-awareness to our
evidence indicates that such intentional identity change final reflections on the meaning of life, our social interactions
requires a self-focused state of mental preparedness or sub- define, nurture, and alter our sense of self. This basic truth
jective readiness to change (Anthis & LaVoie, 2006). Even has given rise to a range of investigations that are continuing
for those who feel prepared for change, the tendency to to provide crucial insights into the nature of the self.
ensconce oneself in self-verifying social environments (e.g., Many more such truths are within the grasp of contem-
Swann et al., 2007) may complicate the business of identity porary self psychologists. Indeed, if our review of the lit-
change. For the effects of self-initiated identity change to erature has left us with a single impression, it is that as
be permanent, people must change not only their own self- the topic area has increasingly attracted attention, the rate
views and narratives but also the social environments that of scientific advances has accelerated proportionally. We
typically support those self-views and narratives. are left with a strong feeling of optimism and a conviction
that, in the future, the answers will come even faster.

SUMMARY

What does it mean to have a sense of “self ”? Is there a REFERENCES


single self, or does the self have multiple, independent
components? Are there aspects of the self that cannot be Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the
accessed consciously, and if so, can they be measured? perspective of self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social
How do people derive and maintain a sense of self, and Psychology, 93, 751–763.

once they do, can it be altered? Do people from different Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (1988). Comments on the motivational
status of self-esteem in social identity and intergroup discrimination.
cultures experience the self in the same way? And what European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 317–334.
are the personal and social consequences of our represen- Alicke, M. D., & Govorun, O. (2005). The better-than-average effect. In
tations of self? M. D. Alicke, D. A. Dunning, & J. I. Krueger (Eds.), The self in social
This chapter was designed to address these and related judgment (pp. 85–106). New York: Psychology Press.

questions. We began with a brief history of the self, noting Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D. L., Yurak, T. J., &
Vredenburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, and the
that in recent decades social psychology has at long last better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Social
embraced some seminal conceptions of the self offered by Psychology, 68, 804–825.

CH16.indd 617 10/22/09 2:50:10 PM


618 Self and Identity

Allport, G. W. (1943). The ego in contemporary psychology. Psychological Baumgardner, A. H. (1990). To know oneself is to like oneself: Self-
Review, 50, 451–478. certainty and self-affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man- 58, 1062–1072.
ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Beaman, A. L., Klentz, B., Diener, E., & Svanum, S. (1979). Self-awareness
Andersen, S. M. (1984). Self-knowledge and social inference: II. The and transgression in children: Two field studies. Journal of Personality and
diagnosticity of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 1835–1846.
Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 294–307. Beauregard, K. S., & Dunning, D. (2001). Defining self-worth: Trait self-
Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. (2002). The relational self: An interpersonal esteem moderates the use of self-serving trait definitions in social judg-
social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619–645. ment. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 135–161.
Andersen, S. M., & Ross, L. (1984). Self-knowledge and social inference: Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
I. The impact of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. Journal of in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York:
Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 280–293. Academic Press.
Anthis, K., & LaVoie, J. C. (2006). Readiness to change: A longitudinal Benenson, J., & Dweck, C. (1986). The development of trait explana-
study of changes in adult identity. Journal of Research in Personality, tions and self-evaluations in the academic and social domains. Child
40, 209–219. Development, 57, 1179–1187.
Archibald, F. S., Bartholomew, K., & Marx, R. (1995). Loneliness in early Blanton, H., Buunk, B. P., Gibbons, F. X., & Kuyper, H. (1999). When
adolescence: A test of the cognitive discrepancy model of loneliness. better-than-others compare upward: Choice of comparison and
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 296–301. comparative evaluation as independent predictors of academic
performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from
420–430.
the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55,
469–480. Bohrnstedt, G. W., & Felson, R. B. (1983). Explaining the relations among
children’s actual and perceived performances and self-esteem: A com-
Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1996). Love and expansion of the self: The state
parison of several causal models. Journal of Personality and Social
of the model. Personal Relationships, 3, 45–58.
Psychology, 45, 43–56.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relation-
Bonanno, G. A., & Jost, J. T. (2006). Conservative shift among high-
ships as including other in the self. Journal of Personality and Social
exposure survivors of the September 11th terrorist attacks. Basic and
Psychology, 60, 241–253.
Applied Social Psychology, 28, 311–323.
Aron, A., Paris, M., & Aron, E. N. (1995). Falling in love: Prospective
Bosson, J. K., Brown, R. P., Zeigler-Hill, V., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003).
studies of self-concept change. Journal of Personality and Social
Self-enhancement tendencies among people with high explicit self-
Psychology, 69, 1102–1112.
esteem: The moderating role of implicit self-esteem. Self and Identity,
Aronson, E. (1968). A theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. 2, 169–187.
In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
Bosson, J. K., Lakey, C. E., Campbell, W. K., Zeigler-Hill, V., Jordan, C. H., &
(Vol. 4, pp. 1–34). New York: Academic Press.
Kernis, M. H. (April, 2008). Untangling the links between narcissism
Aronson, J., Cohen, G., & Nail, P. R. (1999). Self-affirmation theory: An and self-esteem: A theoretical and empirical review. Personality and
update and appraisal. In E. Harmon-Jones & J. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive Social Psychology Compass, 2(3), 1415–1439.
dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology
Bosson, J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1999). Self-liking, self-competence, and
(pp. 127–147). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
the quest for self-verification. Personality and Social Psychology
Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Stereotypes and the fragility of Bulletin, 25, 1230–1241.
academic competence, motivation, and self-concept. In A. J. Elliot &
Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., Jr., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking
C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation
the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the
(pp. 436–456). New York: Guilford.
elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of informa- 631–643.
tion. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 461–484.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and person-
Baldwin, M. W., Carrell, S. E., & Lopez, D. F. (1990). Priming relationship- ality. Behavior Genetics, 31, 243–273.
schemas: My advisor and the pope are watching me from the back of
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment (Vol. 1). New York:
my mind. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 435–454.
Basic Books.
Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). The self in social contexts. Annual
Branscombe, N. R., Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (1999). The con-
Review of Psychology, 45, 297–332.
text and content of social identity threats. In N. Ellemers, R. Spears, &
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational B. Doosje (Eds.), Social identity: Context, commitment, content (pp.
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 248–287. 35–58). Oxford: Blackwell.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Escaping the self: Alcoholism, spirituality, Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Harvey, R. D. (1999). Perceiving
masochism, and other flights from the burden of selfhood. New York: pervasive discrimination among African-Americans: Implications for
Basic Books. group identification and wellbeing. Journal of Personality and Social
Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & Psychology, 77, 135–149.
G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 680–740). Branscombe, N. R., & Wann, D. L. (1994). Collective self-esteem con-
New York: McGraw-Hill. sequences of outgroup derogation when a valued social identity is on
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). trial. European Journal of Social Psychology, 6, 641–657.
Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, Bretherton, I. (1988). Open communication and internal working models:
happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Their role in the development of attachment relationships. Nebraska
Interest, 4, 1–44. Symposium on Motivation, 36, 57–113.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and different
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17,
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. 475–482.

CH16.indd 618 10/22/09 2:50:10 PM


References 619

Brewer, M. B. (2003). Optimal distinctiveness, social identity, and the self. Cooper, A. M., & Ronningstam, E. (1992). Narcissistic personality disorder.
In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity Review of Psychiatry, 11, 80–97.
(pp. 480–491). New York: Guilford. Cortes, B. P., Demoulin, S., Rodriguez, R. T., Rodriguez, A. P., & Leyens, J.-P.
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of col- (2005). Infrahumanization or familiarity? Attribution of uniquely
lective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and human emotions to the self, the ingroup, and the outgroup. Personality
Social Psychology, 71, 83–93. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 243–253.
Brewer, M. B., & Kramer, R. M. (1985). The psychology of intergroup Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adulthood: A six-
attitudes and behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 219–243. year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO
Brown, J. D. (1986). Evaluations of self and others: Self-enhancement Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
biases in social judgments. Social Cognition, 4, 353–376. 54, 853–863.
Brown, J. D. (1990). Evaluating one’s abilities: Shortcuts and stumbling Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United
blocks on the road to self-knowledge. Journal of Experimental Social States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 124–131.
Psychology, 26, 149–167. Cozzarelli, C., Karafa, J. A., Collins, N. L., & Tagler, M. J. (2003). Stability
Brown, J. D. (1998). The self. New York: McGraw-Hill. and change in adult attachment styles: Associations with personal
vulnerabilities, life events, and global constructs of self and others.
Brown, J. D., Dutton, K. A., & Cook, K. E. (2001). From the top down: Self-
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 315–346.
esteem and self-evaluation. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 615–631.
Crocker, J., Karpinski, A., Quinn, D. M., & Chase, S. K. (2003). When
Brown, J. D., & McGill, K. J. (1989). The cost of good fortune: When
grades determine self-worth: Consequences of contingent self-worth
positive life events produce negative health consequences. Journal of
for male and female engineering and psychology majors. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 1103–1110.
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 507–516.
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal
Crocker, J., & Luhtanen, R. K. (2003). Level of self-esteem and contin-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 538–549.
gencies of self-worth: Unique effects on academic, social, and finan-
Campbell, J. D., Assanand, S., & Di Paula, A. (2003). The structure of the cial problems in college students. Personality and Social Psychology
self-concept and its relation to psychological adjustment. Journal of Bulletin, 29, 701–712.
Personality, 71, 115–140.
Crocker, J., Luhtanen, R. K., Cooper, M. L., & Bouvrette, A. (2003).
Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L. F., & Contingencies of self-worth in college students: Theory and measure-
Lehman, D. R. (1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 894–908.
correlates, and cultural boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-
Psychology, 70, 1114.
protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608–630.
Campbell, L., Lackenbauer, S. D., & Muise, A. (2006). When is being
Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem.
known or adored by romantic partners most beneficial? Self-
Psychological Bulletin, 130, 392–414.
perceptions, relationship length, and responses to partner ’s verifying
and enhancing appraisals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Crocker, J., Sommers, S. R., & Luhtanen, R. K. (2002). Hopes dashed
32, 1283–1294. and dreams fulfilled: Contingencies of self-worth and admissions to
graduate school. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
Campbell, W. K., Bosson, J. K., Goheen, T. W., Lakey, C. E., & Kernis, M. H.
1275–1286.
(2007). Do narcissists dislike themselves “deep down inside”?
Psychological Science, 18, 227–229. Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth.
Campbell, W. K., Rudich, E. A., & Sedikides, C. (2002). Narcissism, self- Psychological Review, 108, 593–623.
esteem, and the positivity of self-views: Two portraits of self-love. Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational interde-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 358–368. pendent self-construal and relationships. Journal of Personality and
Castano, E., Yzerbyt, V., Paladino, M.-P., & Sacchi, S. (2002). I belong, Social Psychology, 78, 791–808.
therefore, I exist: Ingroup identification ingroup entitativity, and ingroup Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and
bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 135–143. gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5–37.
Chang, E. C., Asakawa, K., & Sanna, L. J. (2001). Cultural variations in Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience.
optimistic and pessimistic bias: Do Easterners really expect the worst New York: HarperCollins.
and Westerners really expect the best when predicting future life Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Figurski, T. J. (1982). Self-awareness and aver-
events? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 476–491. sive experience in everyday life. Journal of Personality, 50, 15–28.
Chen, S., Chen, K., & Shaw, L. (2004). Self-verification motives at the Dechesne, M., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Ransom, S., Sheldon, K., van
collective level of self-definition. Journal of Personality and Social Knippenberg, A., & Janssen, J. (2003). Literal and symbolic immortal-
Psychology, 86, 77–94. ity: The effect of evidence of literal immortality on self-esteem striving
Choi, I., & Choi, Y. (2002). Culture and self-concept flexibility. Personality in response to mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1508–1517. Psychology, 84, 722–737.
Colvin, C. R., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive self- Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review
evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental health. of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1152–1162. motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.
Conley, J. J. (1985). Longitudinal stability of personality traits: A Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis for true
multitrait–multimethod–multioccasion analysis. Journal of Personality self-esteem. In M. H. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem
and Social Psychology, 49, 1266–1282. (pp. 31–49). New York: Plenum Press.
Conway, M., & Ross, M. (1984). Getting what you want by revising Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Implicit self and identity. In M. R. Leary &
what you had. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 153–175).
738–748. New York: Guilford.
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Dickinson, K. A., & Pincus, A. L. (2003). Interpersonal analysis of grandiose
Scribner ’s. and vulnerable narcissism. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 188–207.

CH16.indd 619 10/22/09 2:50:11 PM


620 Self and Identity

Diener, E., & Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions
and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive
653–663. Science, 11, 77–83.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often
well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow
276–302. from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and
Donnellan, B., Trzesniewski, K., Robins, R., Moffitt, T., & Caspi, A. Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
(2005). Low self-esteem is related to aggression, antisocial behavior, Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1997). Fear of death and the judgment of
and delinquency. Psychological Science, 16, 328–335. social transgressions: A multidimensional test of terror management
Dunning, D., Perie, M., & Story, A. L. (1991). Self-serving prototypes as theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 369–380.
social categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. U. (1986). Black students’ school success: Coping
957–968. with the “burden of acting white.” Urban Review, 18, 176–206.
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. Franks, D. D., & Marolla, J. (1976). Efficacious action and social approval
Oxford: Academic Press. as interacting dimensions of self-esteem: A tentative formulation
Eccleston, C. P., & Major, B. N. (2006). Attributions to discrimination and through construct validation. Sociometry, 39, 324–341.
self-esteem: The role of group identification and appraisals. Group Freud, S. (1957). On narcissism: An introduction. In J. Strachey (Ed. and
Processes and Intergroup Relations, 9, 147–162. Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works
Ellemers, N., & Van Knippenberg, A. (1997). Stereotyping in social con- of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 67–104). London: Hogarth Press.
text. In R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), (Original work published 1914).
The social psychology of stereotyping and group life (pp. 208–235). Gabriel, S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Are there “his” and “her” types of
Malden, MA: Blackwell. interdependence? The implications of gender differences in collective
English, T., & Chen, S. (2007). Culture and self-concept stability: versus relational interdependence for affect, behavior, and cognition.
Consistency across and within contexts among Asian Americans and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 642–655.
European Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Gagné, M., Ryan, R. M., & Bargmann, K. (2003). Autonomy support
93, 478–490. and need satisfaction in the motivation and well-being of gymnasts.
English, T., Chen, S., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2008). A cross-cultural analysis Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15, 372–390.
of self-verification motives. In R. Sorrentino & S. Yamaguchi (Eds.), Galdi, S., Arcuri, L., & Gawronski, B. (2008). Automatic mental associa-
Handbook of motivation and cognition across cultures (pp. 119–142). tions predict future choices of undecided decision-makers, Science,
San Diego: Elsevier. 321, 1100.
Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2006). The mixed blessings of self-knowledge Gallup, G. G. (1977). Self-recognition in primates: A comparative
in behavioral prediction: Enhanced discrimination but exacerbated approach to the bidirectional properties of consciousness. American
bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 641–655. Psychologist, 32, 329–338.
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and proposi-
unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724. tional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and
Erdelyi, M. H. (1974). A new look at the new look: Perceptual defense and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731.
vigilance. Psychological Review, 81, 1–25. Gecas, V. (1971). Parental behavior and dimensions of adolescent self-
Erdelyi, M. H. (1993). Repression: The mechanism and the defense. In evaluation. Sociometry, 34, 466–482.
D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control Gergen, K. (1991). The saturated self: Dilemmas of identity in contempo-
(pp. 126–148). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. rary life. New York: Basic Books.
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. New York: International Gibbons, F. X. (1978). Sexual standards and reactions to pornography:
University Press. Enhancing behavioral consistency through self-focused attention.
Ethier, K. A., & Deaux, K. (1994). Negotiating social identity when con- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 976–987.
texts change: Maintaining identification and responding to threat. Giesler, R. B., Josephs, R. A., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1996). Self-verification
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 243–251. in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 358–368.
Farnham, S. D., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1999). Implicit self- Gilbert, D. T., Giesler, R. B., & Morris, K. A. (1995). When comparisons
esteem. In D. Abrams & M. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity and social arise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 227–236.
cognition (pp. 230–248). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell. Gill, M. J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2004). On what it means to know someone:
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). A matter of pragmatics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and 86, 405–418.
Social Psychology, 50, 229–238. Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City,
Fazio, R. H., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE model of attitude NY: Doubleday–Anchor.
processes. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in Goldstein, N. J., & Cialdini, R. B. (2007). The spyglass self: A model
social psychology (pp. 97–116). New York: Guilford. of vicarious self-perception. Journal of Personality and Social
Feather, N. T. (1994). Values, national identification and favouritism Psychology, 92, 402–417.
towards the in-group. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, Gómez, A., Seyle, D. C., Huici, C., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (in press). Can
467–476. self-verification strivings fully transcend the self-other barrier? Seeking
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human verification of ingroup identities. Manuscript submitted for publica-
Relations, 7, 117–140. tion. Journal of Personality and Psychology.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Oxford: Row, Gosling, S. D., John, O. P., Craik, K. H., & Robins, R. W. (1998). Do people
Peterson. know how they behave? Self-reported act frequencies compared with
Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social beings: A core motives approach to social psy- on-line codings by observers. Journal of Personality and Social
chology. New York: Wiley. Psychology, 74, 1337–1349.

CH16.indd 620 10/22/09 2:50:11 PM


References 621

Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A room Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a
with a cue: Personality judgments based on offices and bedrooms. motivational principle. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379–398. social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 1–46). New York: Academic Press.
Gramzow, R., & Tangney, J. P. (1992). Proneness to shame and the Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1986). Self-
narcissistic personality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, discrepancies and emotional vulnerability: How magnitude, accessibility,
18, 369–376. and type of discrepancy influence affect. Journal of Personality and
Greenberg, J., & Musham, C. (1981). Avoiding and seeking self-focused Social Psychology, 51, 5–15.
attention. Journal of Research in Personality, 15, 191–200. Higgins, E. T., Klein, R. L., & Strauman, T. J. (1985). Self-concept discrep-
Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Chatel, D. ancy theory: A psychological model for distinguishing among different
(1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does mortality salience aspects of depression and anxiety. Social Cognition, 3, 51–76.
always intensify negative reactions to others who threaten one’s world- Higgins, E. T., & Pittman, T. S. (2008). Motives of the human animal:
view? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 212–220. Comprehending, managing, and sharing inner states. Annual Review of
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Rosenblatt, A., Burling, J., Psychology, 59, 361–385.
Lyon, D., Simon, L., & Pinel, E. (1992). Why do people need self- Hinkley, K., & Andersen, S. M. (1996). The working self-concept in
esteem? Converging evidence that self-esteem serves an anxiety- transference: Significant-other activation and self-change. Journal of
buffering function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1279–1295.
63, 913–922. Hixon, J. G., & Swann, W. B. (1993). When does introspection bear fruit?
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Self-reflection, self-insight, and interpersonal choices. Journal of
Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 35–43.
4–27. Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Guidano, V. F., & Liotti, G. (1983). Cognitive processes and emotional Hogan, R., & Shelton, D. (1998). A socioanalytic perspective on job
disorders: A structural approach to psychotherapy. New York: performance. Human Performance, 11, 129–144.
Guilford Press.
Hoge, D. R., & McCarthy, J. D. (1984). Influence of individual and
Hampson, S. E., John, O. P., & Goldberg, L. R. (1987). Category-breadth group identity salience in the global self-esteem of youth. Journal of
and social-desirability values for 573 personality terms. European Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 403–414.
Journal of Personality, 1, 241–258.
Hogg, M. A. (2000). Subjective uncertainty reduction through self-catego-
Hart, J., Shaver, P. R., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2005). Attachment, self-esteem, rization: A motivational theory of social identity processes. European
worldviews, and terror management: Evidence for a tripartite security Review of Social Psychology, 11, 223–255.
system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 999–1013.
Hogg, M. A., & Mullin, B. A. (1999). Joining groups to reduce uncer-
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self. New York: Guilford Press. tainty: Subjective uncertainty reduction and group identification. In
Harter, S. (2006). The development of self-esteem. In M. Kernis (Ed.), D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity and social cognition
Self-esteem issues and answers: A sourcebook of current perspectives (pp. 249–279). Oxford: Blackwell.
(pp. 144–150). New York: Psychology Press. Hormuth, S. E. (1990). The ecology of the self: Relocation and self-con-
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an cept change. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships
511–524. and health. Science, 241, 540–545.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational frame- Hoyle, R. H., Kernis, M. H., Leary, M. R., & Baldwin, M. W. (1999).
work for research on close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5, Selfhood: Identity, esteem, regulation. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
1–22.
Iyer, A., Jetten, J., & Tsivrikos, D. (2008). Torn between identities:
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley. Predictors of adjustment to identity change. In F. Sani (Ed.), Self
Heine, S. J., & Hamamura, T. (2007). In search of East Asian self- continuity: Individual and collective perspectives (pp. 187–197). NY:
enhancement. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 1–24. Psychology Press.
Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., & Hamamura, T. (2007). The inclusion of James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover.
additional studies yields different conclusions: A reply to Sedikides, (Original work published 1890).
Gaertner, & Vevea (2005), JPSP. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, Jemmott, J. B., Ditto, P. H., & Croyle, R. T. (1986). Judging health
10, 49–58. status: Effects of perceived prevalence and personal relevance. Journal
Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., & Lehman, D. R. (2001). Cultural differences in of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 899–905.
self-evaluation: Japanese readily accept negative self-relevant informa- Jetten, J., Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Spears, R. (2001).
tion. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 434–443. Rebels with a cause: Group identification as a response to perceived
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there discrimination from the mainstream. Personality and Social
a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1204–1213.
766–794. Jetten, J., Branscombe, N. R., & Spears, R. (2002). On being peripheral:
Heine, S. J., Proulx, T., & Vohs, K. D. (2006). The meaning maintenance Effects of identity insecurity on personal and collective self-esteem.
model: On the coherence of social motivations. Personality and Social European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 105–123.
Psychology Review, 10, 88–110. Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1996). Intergroup norms and
Helgeson, V. S., & Mickelson, K. D. (1995). Motives for social compari- intergroup discrimination: Distinctive self-categorization and social
son. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1200–1209. identity effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
Helweg-Larsen, M., & Shepperd, J. A. (2001). Do moderators of the opti- 1222–1233.
mistic bias affect personal or target risk estimates? A review of the Johnson, S. C., Baxter, L. C., Wilder, L. S., Pipe, J. G., Heiserman, J. E., &
literature. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 74–95. Prigatano, G. P. (2002). Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain:
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. A Journal of Neurology, 125, 1808–1814.
Psychological Review, 94, 319–340. Jones, E. E. (1964). Ingratiation. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

CH16.indd 621 10/22/09 2:50:11 PM


622 Self and Identity

Jones, E. E., & Gerard, H. (1967). Foundations of social psychology. importance of stability of self-esteem. Journal of Personality and
Oxford: Wiley. Social Psychology, 65, 1190–1204.
Jones, E. E., Gergen, K. J., & Davis, K. E. (1962). Some determinants of Kernis, M. H., Grannemann, B. D., & Barclay, L. C. (1989). Stability and
reactions to being approved or disapproved as a person. Psychological level of self-esteem as predictors of anger arousal and hostility. Journal
Monographs, 76, 17. of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 1013–1022.
Jordan, C. H., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Hoshino-Browne, E., & Kernis, M. H., Lakey, C. E., & Heppner, W. L. (2008). Secure versus frag-
Correll, J. (2003). Secure and defensive high self-esteem. Journal of ile high self-esteem as a predictor of verbal defensiveness: Converging
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 969–978. findings across three different markers. Journal of Personality, 76,
Jordan, C. H., Whitfield, M., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2007). Intuition and the 477–512.
correspondence between implicit and explicit self-esteem. Journal of Kernis, M. H., Paradise, A. W., Whitaker, D. J., Wheatman, S. R., &
Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1067–1079. Goldman, B. N. (2000). Master of one’s psychological domain?
Josephs, R. A., Markus, H. R., & Tafarodi, R. W. (1992). Gender and self- Not likely if one’s self-esteem is unstable. Personality and Social
esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 391–402. Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1297–1305.
Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system Kiefer, A. K., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007). Implicit stereotypes, gender
justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and uncon- identification, and math-related outcomes: A prospective study of
scious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25, 881–919. female college students. Psychological Science, 18, 13–18.
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. (2003). Political Kitayama, S., & Karasawa, M. (1997). Implicit self-esteem in Japan: Name
conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, letters and birthday numbers. Personality and Social Psychology
129, 339–375. Bulletin, 23, 736–742.
Jost, J. T., Pelham, B. W., & Carvallo, M. R. (2002). Non-conscious Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (1999). Yin and yang of the Japanese self:
forms of system justification: Implicit and behavioral preferences for The cultural psychology of personality coherence. In D. Cervone &
higher status groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality: Social-cognitive
586–602. bases of consistency, variability, and organization (pp. 242–302).
Judd, C. M., James-Hawkins, L., Yzerbyt, V., & Kashima, Y. (2005). New York: Guilford Press.
Fundamental dimensions of social judgment: Understanding the rela- Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997).
tions between judgments of competence and warmth. Journal of Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-
Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 899–913. enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal
Kamakura, T., Ando, J., & Ono, Y. (2007). Genetic and environmental of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245–1267.
effects of stability and change in self-esteem during adolescence. Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. E. (1997). No one in my group can be below the
Personality and individual Differences, 42, 181–190. group’s average: A robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers.
Kanagawa, C., Cross, S., & Markus, H. (2001). “Who am I? ” The cultural Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 885–901.
psychology of the conceptual self. Personality and Social Psychology Klein, S. B. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of knowing one’s self.
Bulletin, 27, 90–103. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (3rd ed.,
Kashima, Y., Kashima, E., Farsides, T., Kim, U., Strack, F., Wert, L., & pp. 1077–1089). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Yuki, M. (2004). Culture and context-specific self: The amount and Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1993). The mental representation of trait and
meaning of context-sensitivity of phenomenal self differ across autobiographical knowledge about the self. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer
cultures. Self and Identity, 3, 125–141. (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 5, pp. 1–49). Hillsdale, NJ:
Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., Zax, M., & Sameroff, A. J. (1995). The relations of Erlbaum.
material and social environments to late adolescents’ materialistic and Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Self-knowledge of an
prosocial values. Developmental Psychology, 31, 907–914. amnesic patient: Toward a neuropsychology of personality and social
Kelley, H. H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. In E. E. Jones, psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125,
D. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), 250–260.
Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 1–26). Morristown, Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., Trafton, J. G., & Fuhrman, R. W. (1992). Use of
NJ: General Learning Press. exemplars and abstractions in trait judgments: A model of trait knowl-
Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., & edge about the self and others. Journal of Personality and Social
Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI Psychology, 63, 739–753.
study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 785–794. Kohut, H. (1966). Forms and transformations of narcissism. Journal of the
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (1998). A population- American Psychoanalytic Association, 14, 243–272.
based twin study of self-esteem and gender. Psychological Medicine, Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. Madison, WI: International
28, 1403–1409. Universities Press.
Kenny, D. A., & Albright, L. (1987). Accuracy in interpersonal perception: Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How
A social relations analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 390–402. difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated
Kenny, D. A., & DePaulo, B. W. (1993). Do people know others view self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
them? An empirical and theoretical account. Psychological Bulletin, 1121–1134.
114, 145–161. Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). The psychology of being “right”: The problem
Kernberg, O. F. (1986). Factors in the psychoanalytic treatment of narcissistic of accuracy in social perception and cognition. Psychological Bulletin,
personalities. In A. P. Morrison (Ed.), Essential papers on narcissism 106, 395–409.
(pp. 213–244). New York and London: New York University Press. Kruglanski, A. W. (1990). Lay epistemic theory in social-cognitive psy-
Kernis, M. H. (2003). Toward a conceptualization of optimal self-esteem. chology. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 181–197.
Psychological Inquiry, 14, 1–26. Kuiper, N. A., & Rogers, T. B. (1979). Encoding of personal information:
Kernis, M. H., Cornell, D. P., Sun, C. R., Berry, A. J., & Harlow, T. (1993). Self–other differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
There’s more to self-esteem than whether it is high or low: The 37, 499–514.

CH16.indd 622 10/22/09 2:50:12 PM


References 623

Kurman, J. (2003). Why is self-enhancement low in certain collectivist Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and malleability of the
cultures? An investigation of two competing explanations. Journal of self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 496–510. 858–866.
Kwan, V. S. Y., John, O. P., Kenny, D. A., Bond, M. H., & Robins, R. W. Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist,
(2004). Reconceptualizing individual differences in self-enhancement 41, 954–969.
bias: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Review, 111, 94–111. Markus, H., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A social
Kwan, V. S. Y., John, O. P., Robins, R. W., & Kuang, L. (2008). psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38,
Conceptualizing and assessing self-enhancement bias: A componen- 299–337.
tial approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, Marsh, H. W. (1986). Global self-esteem: Its relation to specific facets of
1062–1077. self-concept and their importance. Journal of Personality and Social
Landau, M. J., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., Pyszczynski, T., Psychology, 51, 1224–1236.
Arndt, J., Miller, C. H., Ogilvie, D. M., & Cook, A. (2004). Deliver Marsh, H. W. (1990). A multidimensional, hierarchical model of self-
us from evil: The effects of mortality salience and reminders of 9/11 concept: Theoretical and empirical justification. Educational
on support for President George W. Bush. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 77–172.
Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1136–1150.
Marsh, H. W., & Craven, R. G. (2006). Reciprocal effects of self-
Leary, M. R. (2004). Editorial: What is the self? A plea for clarity. Self and concept and performance from a multidimensional perspective: Beyond
Identity, 3, 1–3. seductive pleasure and unidimensional perspectives. Perspectives on
Leary, M. R. (2007). Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annual Psychological Science, 1, 133–163.
Review of Psychology, 58, 317–344. Marsh, H. W., & Hattie, J. (1996). Theoretical perspectives on the struc-
Leary, M. R., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Interpersonal functions of the ture of self-concept. In B. Bracken (Ed.), Handbook of self-concept
self-esteem motive: The self-esteem system as a sociometer. In (pp. 38–90). New York: Wiley.
M. H. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem (pp. 123–144). Marsh, H. W., & Parker, J. W. (1984). Determinants of student self-
New York: Plenum. concept: Is it better to be a relatively large fish in a small pond even
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self- if you don’t learn to swim as well? Journal of Personality and Social
esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal Psychology, 47, 213–231.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518–530. Mashek, D. J., Aron, A., & Boncimino, M. (2003). Confusions of self
Lecky, P. (1945). Self-consistency: A theory of personality. New York: with close others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29,
Island Press. 382–392.
Lemay, E. P., Jr., & Ashmore, R. D. (2004). Reactions to perceived cate- Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. Oxford: Harper.
gorization by others during the transition to college: Internalization McAdams, D. P. (1989). Intimacy: The need to be close. New York:
and self-verification processes. Group Processes and Intergroup Doubleday.
Relations, 7, 173–187. McCall, G. J., & Simmons, J. L. (1966). Identities and interactions.
Lepper, M. R., Henderlong, J., & Gingras, I. (1999). Understanding the New York: Free Press.
effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation—Uses and abuses McGregor, I., Gailliot, M. T., Vasquez, N. A., & Nash, K. A. (2007).
of meta-analysis: Comment on Deci, Koestner and Ryan (1999). Ideological and personal zeal reactions to threat among people with
Psychological Bulletin, 125, 669–676. high self-esteem: Motivated promotion focus. Personality and Social
Liberman, A., & Chaiken, S. (1992). Defensive processing of personally Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1587–1599.
relevant health messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, McGregor, I., & Marigold, D. C. (2003). Defensive zeal and the uncer-
18, 669–679. tain self: What makes you so sure? Journal of Personality and Social
Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M., & Satpute, A. B. (2004). Evidence- Psychology, 85, 838–852.
based and intuition-based self-knowledge: An fMRI study. Journal of McGuire, S., Manke, B., Saudino, K. J., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., &
Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 421–435. Plomin, R. (1999). Perceived competence and self-worth during ado-
Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress- lescence: A longitudinal behavioral genetic study. Child Development,
related illness and depression. Journal of Personality and Social 70, 1283–1296.
Psychology, 52, 663–676. McGuire, W. J., McGuire, C. V., Child, P., & Fujioka, T. (1978). Salience
Linville, P. W., & Jones, E. E. (1980). Polarized appraisals of out- of ethnicity in the spontaneous self-concept as a function of one’s
group members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, ethnic distinctiveness in the social environment. Journal of Personality
689–703. and Social Psychology, 36, 511–520.
Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self- Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago: University of
evaluation of one’s social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Chicago Press.
Bulletin, 18, 302–318. Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., & Hankin, B. L. (2004). Is
Major, B., Quinton, W. J., & Schmader, T. (2003). Attributions to discrimi- there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review
nation and self-esteem: Impact of group identification and situational of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-
ambiguity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 220–231. serving attributional bias. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 711–747.
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemas and processing information about the Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in attribution of cau-
self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63–78. sality: Fact or fiction? Psychological Bulletin, 82, 213–225.
Markus, H., & Cross, S. (1990). The interpersonal self. In L. A. Pervin Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). The link between
(Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 576–608). social cognition and self-referential thought in the medial prefrontal
New York: Guilford. cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1306–1315.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of nar-
for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, cissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological
224–253. Inquiry, 12, 177–196.

CH16.indd 623 10/22/09 2:50:12 PM


624 Self and Identity

Murray, S. L., Griffin, D. W., Rose, P., & Bellavia, G. (2003). Calibrating Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking
the sociometer: The relational contingencies of self-esteem. Journal of individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 63–84. and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.
Murray, S. L. Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The benefits of Oyserman, D., Fryberg, S. A., & Yoder, N. (2007). Identity-based
positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in motivation and health. Journal of personality and Social Psychology,
close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 93, 1011–1027.
79–98. Pals, J. L. (1999). Identity consolidation in early adulthood: Relations
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (2000). Self-esteem and the with ego-resiliency, the context of marriage, and personality change.
quest for felt security: How perceived regard regulates attachment pro- Journal of Personality, 67, 295–329.
cesses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 478–498. Paulhus, D. L. (1993). Bypassing the will: The automatization of affirma-
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2002). Judgments of a relationship partner: tions. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of men-
Specific accuracy but global enhancement. Journal of Personality, 70, tal control (pp. 573–587). Hillsdale, NJ: Psychology.
1079–1112. Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2005). To know you is to love you: The impli- trait self-enhancement: A mixed blessing? Journal of Personality and
cations of global adoration and specific accuracy for marital relation- Social Psychology, 74, 1197–1208.
ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 480–497. Paulhus, D. L., Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Tracy, J. L.
Neighbors, C., Larimer, M. E., Markman Geisner, I., & Knee, C. R. (2004). (2004). Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research.
Feeling controlled and drinking motives among college students: Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 303–329.
Contingent self-esteem as a mediator. Self and Identity, 3, 207–224. Pelham, B. W. (1991). On confidence and consequence: The certainty
Neiss, M. B., Sedikides, C., & Stevenson, J. (2006). Genetic influences on and importance of self-knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social
level and stability of self-esteem. Self and Identity, 5, 247–266. Psychology, 60, 518–530.
Neuberg, S. L., & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure: Pelham, B. W. (1995). Self-investment and self-esteem: Evidence for a
Individual differences in the desire for simpler structure. Journal of Jamesian model of self-worth. Journal of Personality and Social
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 113–131. Psychology, 69, 1141–1150.
Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1986). Loneliness and social support: Pelham, B. W., Carvallo, M., & Jones, J. T. (2005). Implicit egotism.
A confirmatory hierarchical analysis. Personality and Social Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 106–110.
Psychology Bulletin, 12, 520–535. Pelham, B. W., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1989). From self-conceptions to self-
Nezlek, J. B., Kowalski, R. M., Leary, M. R., & Holgate, S. (1997). worth: The sources and structure of self-esteem. Journal of Personality
Personality moderators of reactions to interpersonal rejection: and Social Psychology, 57, 672–680.
Depression and trait self-esteem. Personality and Social Psychology Pelham, B. W., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1994). The juncture of intrapersonal
Bulletin, 23, 1235–1244. and interpersonal knowledge: Self-certainty and interpersonal congru-
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal ence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 349–357.
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. Pinel, E. C., Long, A. E., Landau, M. J., Alexander, K., & Pyszczynski, T.
Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and (2006). Seeing I to I: A pathway to interpersonal connectedness.
explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 243–257.
134, 565–584. Pittman, T. S., & Zeigler, K. (2007). Basic human needs. In A. Kruglanski &
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles
implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration website. (2nd ed., pp. 473–489). New York: Guilford.
Group Dynamics, 6, 101–115. Popper, K. R. (1963). Conjectures and refutations. London: Routledge.
Ochsner, K. N., Beer, J. S., Robertson, E. R., Cooper, J. C., Gabrieli, Portes, A., & Zhou, M. (1993). The new second generation: Segmented
J. D. E., Kihlstrom, J. F., & D’Esposito, M. (2005). The neural correlates assimilation and its variants. Annals of the American Academy, 530,
of direct and reflected self-knowledge. NeuroImage, 28, 797–814. 74–96.
Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D., Hanelin, J., Ramachandran, T., &
Postmes, T., & Jetten, J. (2006). Individuality and the group: Advances in
Mackey, S. (2004). Reflecting upon feelings: An fMRI study of neural
social identity. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
systems supporting the attribution of emotion to self and other. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1748–1772. Pronin, E., Gilovich, T., & Ross, L. (2004). Objectivity in the eye of
the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others.
Ogilvie, D. M., & Ashmore, R. D. (1991). Self-with-other representation
Psychological Review, 111, 781–799.
as a unit of analysis in self-concept research. In R. C. Curtis (Ed.),
The relational self: Theoretical convergences in psychoanalysis and Pronin, E., Kruger, J., Savitsky, K., & Ross, L. (2001). You don’t know
social psychology (pp. 282–314). New York: Guilford. me, but I know you: The illusion of asymmetric insight. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 639–656.
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2008). Implicit and explicit measures of atti-
tudes: The perspective of the MODE model. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J., & Schimel, J.
P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (2004). Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical
(pp. 19–64). New York: Psychology Press. review. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 435–468.
Olson, M. A., Fazio, R. H., & Hermann, A. D., Sr. (2007). Reporting ten- Raskin, R. N., & Hall, C. S. (1981). The Narcissistic Personality Inventory:
dencies underlie discrepancies between implicit and explicit measures Alternative form reliability and further evidence of construct validity.
of self-esteem. Psychological Science, 18, 67–291. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 159–162.
Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K. (2006). Possible selves and aca- Raskin, R. N., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991). Narcissism, self-esteem,
demic outcomes: How and when possible selves impel action. Journal and defensive self-enhancement. Journal of Personality, 59, 19–38.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 188–204. Rhodewalt, F. (2005). Social motivation and object relations: Narcissism and
Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., Terry, K., & Hart-Johnson, T. (2004). Possible interpersonal self-esteem regulation. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, &
selves as roadmaps. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, S. Laham (Eds.), Social motivation: Conscious and unconscious pro-
130–149. cesses (pp. 332–350). New York: Psychology Press.

CH16.indd 624 10/22/09 2:50:13 PM


References 625

Robins, R. W., & Beer, J. S. (2001). Positive illusions about the self: Sedikides, C., & Gregg, A. P. (2008). Self-enhancement: Food for thought.
Short-term benefits and long-term costs. Journal of Personality and Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 102–116.
Social Psychology, 80, 340–352. Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self evaluation: To thine own self be
Robins, R. W., Noftle, E. E., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Roberts, B. W. (2005). good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine
Do people know how their personality has changed? Correlates of per- own self be better. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
ceived and actual personality change in young adulthood. Journal of social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 209–269). San Diego: Academic.
Personality, 73, 489–521. Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of personal understanding. New York:
Robinson, D. T., & Smith-Lovin, L. (1992). Selective interaction as a Academic Press.
strategy for identity maintenance: An affect control model. Social Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Stanton, G. C. (1976). Validation of con-
Psychology Quarterly, 55, 12–28. struct interpretations. Review of Educational Research, 46, 407–441.
Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. Sherman, D. A. K., Nelson, L. D., & Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages
(1989). Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mor- about health risks threaten the self? Increasing the acceptance of threat-
tality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural ening health messages via self-affirmation. Personality and Social
values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681–690. Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1046–1058.
Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of Shimizu, M., & Pelham, B. W. (2004). The unconscious cost of good
personal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341–357. fortune: Implicit and explicit self-esteem, positive life events, and
Ruble, D. N., Boggiano, A. K., Feldman, N. S., & Loebl, J. H. (1980). health. Health Psychology, 23, 101–105.
Developmental analysis of the role of social comparison in self- Showers, C. J. (1992). Compartmentalization of positive and negative
evaluation. Developmental Psychology, 16, 105–115. self-knowledge: Keeping bad apples out of the bunch. Journal of
Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. (1996). All goals Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1036–1049.
are not created equal: An organismic perspective on the nature of Showers, C. J., & Kling, K. C. (1996). Organization of self-knowledge:
goals and their regulation. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The Implications for recovery from sad mood. Journal of Personality and
psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior Social Psychology, 70, 578–590.
(pp. 7–26). New York: Guilford. Showers, C. J., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2006). Pathways among self-knowledge
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the and self-esteem: Are they direct or indirect? In M. Kernis (Ed.), Self-
meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and esteem issues and answers: A sourcebook of current perspectives
Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081. (pp. 216–223). New York: Psychology Press.
Sanitioso, R. B., & Wlodarski, R. (2004). In search of information that con- Showers, C. J., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2007). Compartmentalization and inte-
firms a desired self perception: Motivated processing of social feedback gration: The evaluative organization of contextualized selves. Journal
and choice of social interactions. Personality and Social Psychology of Personality, 75, 1181–1204.
Bulletin, 30, 412–422. Shrauger, J. S., & Schoeneman, T. J. (1979). Symbolic interactionist view
Saxe, R., Moran, J. M., Scholz, J., & Gabrieli, J. (2006). Overlapping and non- of self-concept: Through the looking glass darkly. Psychological
overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individ- Bulletin, 86, 549–573.
ual subjects. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 229–234. Silvia, P. J., & Abele, A. E. (2002). Can positive affect induce self-focused
Schafer, R. B., Wickrama, K. A. S., & Keith, P. M. (1996). Self-concept attention? Methodological and measurement issues. Cognition and
disconfirmation, psychological distress, and marital happiness. Journal Emotion, 16, 845–853.
of Marriage and the Family, 58, 167–177. Simon, B. (2004). Identity in modern society: A social psychological
Scheibe, K. E. (1985). Historical perspectives on the presented self. In perspective. Oxford: Blackwell.
B. R. Schlenker (Ed.), The self and social life (pp. 33–64). New York: Smith, M., Wethington, E., & Zhan, G. (1996). Self-concept clarity and
McGraw-Hill. preferred coping styles. Journal of Personality, 64, 407–434.
Schimel, J., Hayes, J., Williams, T., & Jahrig, J. (2007). Is death really Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of
the worm at the core? Converging evidence that worldview threat Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526–537.
increases death-thought accessibility. Journal of Personality and
Snyder, M., & Klein, O. (2005). Construing and constructing others: On
Social Psychology, 92, 789–803.
the reality and the generality of the behavioral confirmation scenario.
Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management. Monterey, CA: Interaction Studies, 6, 53–67.
Brooks/Cole.
Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and
Schlenker, B. R. (1985). Identity and self-identification. In B. R. Schlenker interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereo-
(Ed.), The self and social life (pp. 65–99). New York: McGraw-Hill. types. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656–666.
Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2004). The cultural ani-
effects on women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social mal: Twenty years of terror management theory and research. In
Psychology, 38, 194–201. J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of
Secord, P. E., & Backman, C. W. (1965). An interpersonal approach to experimental existential psychology (pp. 13–34). New York: Guilford.
personality. In B. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality Spalding, L. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1999). Unconscious unease and self-
research (Vol. 2, pp. 91–125). New York: Academic Press. handicapping: Behavioral consequences of individual differences
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self- in implicit and explicit self-esteem. Psychological Science, 10,
enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 535–539.
60–79. Spears, R., Jetten, J., & Scheepers, D. (2002). Distinctiveness and the defi-
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Vevea, J. L. (2005). Pancultural self- nition of collective self: A tripartite model. In A. Tesser, D. A. Stapel, &
enhancement reloaded: A meta-analytic reply to Heine (2005). Journal J. V. Wood (Eds.), Self and motivation: Emerging psychological per-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 539–551. spectives (pp. 147–171). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Sedikides, C., & Gregg, A. P. (2003). Portraits of the self. In M. A. Hogg & Association.
J. Cooper (Eds.), Sage handbook of social psychology (pp. 110–138). Spence, J. T., Deaux, K., & Helmreich, R. L. (1985). Sex roles in con-
London: Sage. temporary American society. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.),

CH16.indd 625 10/22/09 2:50:13 PM


626 Self and Identity

The handbook of social psychology (3rd ed., pp. 149–178). New York: Swann, W. B., Jr., & Ely, R. J. (1984). A battle of wills: Self-verification
Random House. versus behavioral confirmation. Journal of Personality and Social
Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K., Wang, L., & Hou, Y. (2004). Dialectical Psychology, 46, 1287–1302.
self-esteem and East–West differences in psychological well-being. Swann, W. B., Jr., Gómez, A., Seyle, C., Morales, F., & Huici, C. (2009).
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1416–1432. Identity fusion: The interplay of personal and social identities in extreme
Srivastava, S., & Beer, J. S. (2005). How self-evaluations relate to being group behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96,
liked by others: Integrating sociometer and attachment perspectives. 995–1011.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 966–977. Swann, W. B., & Hill, C. A. (1982). When our identities are mistaken:
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the Reaffirming self-conceptions through social interaction. Journal of
integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 59–66.
social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). New York: Academic Press. Swann, W. B., Hixon, J. G., Stein-Seroussi, A., & Gilbert, D. T. (1990). The
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape the intellec- fleeting gleam of praise: Cognitive processes underlying behavioral
tual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629. reactions to self-relevant feedback. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 59, 17–26.
Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self-affirmation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 5–19. Swann, W. B., Jr., Johnson, R. E., & Bosson, J. K. (in press). Identity nego-
tiation in the workplace. In B. Staw & A. Brief (Eds.), Research in
Stipek, D. J., & Tannatt, L. (1984). Children’s judgments of their own and organizational behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
their peers’ academic competence. Journal of Educational Psychology,
76, 75–84. Swann, W. B., Krull, D. S., & Pelham, B. W. (1989). Agreeable fancy or
disagreeable truth? Reconciling self-enhancement and self-verification.
Story, A. L. (1998). Self-esteem and memory for favorable and unfavorable Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 782–791.
personality feedback. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
51–64. Swann, W. B., Jr., & Pelham, B. W. (2002). Who wants out when the going
gets good? Psychological investment and preference for self-verifying
Story, A. L. (2004). Self-esteem and self-certainty: A meditational analysis. college roommates. Self and Identity, 1, 219–233.
European Journal of Personality, 18, 115–125.
Swann, W. B., Jr., Pelham, B. W., & Chidester, T. (1988). Change through
Strauman, T. J., & Higgins, E. T. (1988). Self-discrepancies as predictors paradox: Using self-verification to alter beliefs. Journal of Personality
of vulnerability to distinct syndromes of chronic emotional distress. and Social Psychology, 54, 268–273.
Journal of Personality, 56, 685–707.
Swann, W. B., Predmore, S. C., Griffin, J. G., & Gaines, B. (1987). The
Strube, M. J. (1990). In search of self: Balancing the good and the true. cognitive–affective crossfire: When self-consistency confronts self-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 699–704. enhancement. Journal Personality and Social Psychology, 52,
Stryker, S. (2000). Symbolic interactionism: A social structural version. Menlo 881–889.
Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings, and Caldwell, NJ: Blackburn Press. Swann, W. B., Jr., & Read, S. J. (1981). Self-verification processes: How
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evo- we sustain our self-conceptions. Journal of Experimental Social
lution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Psychology, 17, 351–372.
Monographs, 123, 133–167. Swann, W. B., Jr., & Schroeder, D. G. (1995). The search for beauty
Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being. and truth: A framework for understanding reactions to evaluations.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1378–1391. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1307–1318.
Suls, J., & Wills, T. A. (1991). Social comparison: Contemporary theory Swann, W. B., Jr., & Seyle, C. D. (2005). Personality psychology’s come-
and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. back and its emerging symbiosis with social psychology. Personality
Svenson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 155–165.
drivers? Acta Psycholocia, 47, 143–148. Swann, W. B., Jr., Wenzlaff, R. M., Krull, D. S., & Pelham, B. W. (1992).
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into har- The allure of negative feedback: Self-verification strivings among
mony with the self. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological depressed persons. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 293–306.
perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 33–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Swann, W. B., Wenzlaff, R. M., & Tafarodi, R. W. (1992). Depression and
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1984). The quest for accuracy in person perception: the search for negative evaluations: More evidence of the role of self-
A matter of pragmatics. Psychological Review, 91, 457–477. verification strivings. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 314–317.
Swann, W. B., Jr., & Bosson, J. K. (2008). Identity negotiation: A theory of Tafarodi, R. W., Lang, J. M., & Smith, A. J. (1999). Self-esteem and the
self and social interaction. In O. John, R. Robins, & L. Pervin (Eds.), cultural trade-off: Evidence for the role of individualism–collectivism.
Handbook of personality psychology: Theory and research (3rd ed., Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 620–640.
pp. 448–471). New York: Guilford. Tafarodi, R. W., & Swann, W. B. (1996). Individualism–collectivism and
Swann, W. B., Jr., Bosson, J. K., & Pelham, B. W. (2002). Different part- global self-esteem: Evidence for a cultural trade-off. Journal of Cross-
ners, different selves: The verification of circumscribed identities. Cultural Psychology, 27, 651–672.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1215–1228. Tafarodi, R. W., & Swann, W. B. (2001). Two-dimensional self-esteem:
Swann, W. B., Jr., Chang-Schneider, C., & Angulo, S. (2007). Self-verification Theory and measurement. Personality and Individual Differences, 31,
in relationships as an adaptive process. In J. Wood, A. Tesser, & 653–673.
J. Holmes (Eds.), Self and relationships (pp. 49–72). New York: Tafarodi, R. W., & Vu, C. (1997). Two-dimensional self-esteem and
Psychology Press. reactions to success and failure. Personality and Social Psychology
Swann, W. B., Jr., Chang-Schneider, C., & McClarty, K. (2007). Do our Bulletin, 23, 626–635.
self-views matter? Self-concept and self-esteem in everyday life. Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social
American Psychologist, 62, 84–94. psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Swann, W. B., De La Ronde, C., & Hixon, J. G. (1994). Authenticity and Tajfel, H., Billig, M., Bundy, R., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categoriza-
positivity strivings in marriage and courtship. Journal of Personality tion and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology,
and Social Psychology, 66, 857–869. 1, 149–178.

CH16.indd 626 10/22/09 2:50:13 PM


References 627

Tajfel, H., Jaspars, J. M. F., & Fraser, C. (1984). The social dimension Turner, J., Reynolds, K. J., Haslam, A., & Veenstra, K. E. (2006).
in European social psychology. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), The social dimen- Reconceptualizing personality: Producing individuality by defining the
sion: European developments in social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 1–5). personal self. In T. Postmes & J. Jetten (Eds.), Individuality and the group:
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Advances in social identity (pp. 11–36). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup rela- Turner, K. L., & Brown, C. S. (2007). The centrality of gender and
tions. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of ethnic identities across individuals and contexts. Social Development,
intergroup relations (pp. 33–48). Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole. 16, 700–719.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup Turner, R. (1968). The self-conception in social interaction. In C. Gordon &
behaviour. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of inter- K. Gergen (Eds.), The self in social interaction (pp. 93–106).
group relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson Hall. New York: Wiley.
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The rela-
Guilford. tionship between social support and physiological processes: A review
Tangney, J. P., Niedenthal, P. M., Covert, M. V., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health.
Are shame and guilt related to distinct self-discrepancies? A test Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488–531.
of Higgins’ (1987) hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Vaes, J., Paladino, M. P., Castelli, L., Leyens, J.-P., & Giovanazzi, A.
Psychology, 75, 256–268. (2003). On the behavioral consequences of infrahumanization: The
Taubman Ben-Ari, O., Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1999). The impact implicit role of uniquely human emotions in intergroup relations.
of mortality salience on reckless driving: A test of terror manage- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1016–1034.
ment mechanisms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, Vazire, S. (2006). Informant reports: A cheap, fast, and easy method for per-
35–45. sonality assessment. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 472–481.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psy- Vazire, S., & Mehl, M. M. (2008). Knowing me, knowing you: The accu-
chological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, racy and unique predictive validity of self-ratings and other-ratings
193–210. of daily behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
Taylor, S. E., & Lobel, M. (1989). Social comparison activity under threat: 1202–1216.
Downward evaluations and upward contacts. Psychological Review, Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., & Schoefs, V. (1996). The internal working
96, 569–575. model of the self, attachment, and competence in five-year-olds. Child
Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation pro- Development, 67, 2493–2511.
cesses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278–1287. Vignoles, V. L., Regalia, C., Manzi, C., Golledge, J., & Scabini, E. (2006).
Tedeschi, J. (1981). Impression management theory and social psycho- Beyond self-esteem: Influence of multiple motives on identity construc-
logical research. New York: Academic Press. tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 308–333.
Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social Webster, D., Webster, F., Sanborn, E. D. (Eds). (1857). The private corre-
behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental psy- spondence of Daniel Webster (Vol. 2). Boston: Little Brown.
chology: Social psychological studies of the self: Perspectives and Welles, O. (Producer and Director). (1941). Citizen Kane [Motion picture].
programs (Vol. 21, pp. 181–227). San Diego: Academic. USA: RKO Radio Pictures.
Tesser, A. (2002). Constructing a niche for the self: A bio-social, PDP Westen, D. (1990). The relations among narcissism, egocentrism, self-
approach to understanding lives. Self and Identity, 1, 185–190. concept, and self-esteem: Experimental, clinical, and theoretical consid-
Tesser, A., & Cornell, D. P. (1991). On the confluence of self processes. erations. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought, 13, 183–239.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 501–526. Whitbourne, S. K. (1996). Psychosocial perspectives on emotions: The role
Tice, D. M., & Wallace, H. M. (2003). The reflected self: Creating yourself of identity in the aging process. In C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.),
as (you think) others see you. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging (pp. 83–98). San
Handbook of self and identity (pp. 91–105). New York: Guilford. Diego: Academic Press.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Wiesenfeld, B. M., Swann, W.B., Jr, Brockner, J. & Bartel, C. (2007). Is
Westview Press. More Fairness Always Preferred? Self-Esteem Moderates Reactions to
Procedural Justice. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 1235–1253.
Trope, Y. (1983). Self-assessment in achievement behavior. In J. M. Suls &
A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait-descriptive
(Vol. 2, pp. 93–121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. terms: The interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 37, 395–412.
Trope, Y., & Pomerantz, E. M. (1998). Resolving conflicts among self-
evaluative motives: Positive experiences as a resource for overcoming Wiggins, J. S., & Broughton, R. (1991). A geometric taxonomy of person-
defensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 22, 53–72. ality scales. European Journal of Personality, 5, 343–365.
Trzesniewski, K., Donnellan, B., Moffitt, T., Robins, R., Poulton, R., & Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York:
Caspi, A. (2006). Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor Guilford Press.
health, criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects during adult- Wilson, J. K., & Rapee, R. M. (2006). Self-concept certainty in social phobia.
hood. Developmental Psychology, 42, 381–390. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 113–136.
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford Wilson, T. D., & Dunn, E. (2004). Self-knowledge: Its limits, value,
University Press. and potential for improvement. Annual Review of Psychology, 55,
Turner, J. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cogni- 493–518.
tive theory of group behavior. In E. J. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In
processes: Theory and research (Vol. 2, pp. 77–122). Greenwich, CT: M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
JAI Press. 35, pp. 345–411). San Diego: Academic Press.
Turner, J., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. Wilson, T. D., & Kraft, D. (1993). Why do I love thee? Effects of repeated
(1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. introspections about a dating relationship on attitudes toward the rela-
Oxford: Blackwell. tionship. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 759.

CH16.indd 627 10/22/09 2:50:14 PM


628 Self and Identity

Wilson, T. D., Laser, P. S., & Stone, J. I. (1982). Judging the predictors of Woodruff, D. S., & Birren, J. E. (1972). Age changes and cohort
one’s own mood: Accuracy and the use of shared theories. Journal of differences in personality. Developmental Psychology, 6, 252–259.
Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 537–556. Yamaguchi, S., Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Murakami, F., Chen, D.,
Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D. J., Schooler, J. W., Hodges, S. D., Klaaren, & Shiomura, K., Kobayashi, C., Cai, H., & Krendl, A. (2007). Apparent
La Fleur (1993). Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice universality of positive implicit self-esteem. Psychological Science,
satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 18, 498–500.
331–339. Yik, M. S. M., Bond, M. H., & Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Do Chinese self-
Wong, C. A., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. (2003). The influence of ethnic enhance or self-efface? It’s a matter of domain. Personality and Social
discrimination and ethnic identification on African American adoles- Psychology Bulletin, 24, 399–406.
cents’ school and socioemotional adjustment. Journal of Personality, Zeigler-Hill, V., Clark, C. B., & Pickard, J. D. (2008). Narcissistic subtypes
71, 1197–1232. and contingent self-esteem: Do all narcissists base their self-esteem on
Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of the same domains? Journal of Personality, 76, 753–774.
personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231–248. Zhou, M., & Kim, S. S. (2006). Community forces, social capital, and
Wood, J. V., Taylor, S. E., & Lichtman, R. R. (1985). Social comparison educational achievement: The case of supplementary education in the
in adjustment to breast cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Chinese and Korean immigrant communities. Harvard Educational
Psychology, 49, 1169–1183. Review, 76, 1–29.

CH16.indd 628 10/22/09 2:50:14 PM


Chapter 17

Gender
WENDY WOOD AND ALICE H. EAGLY

Why is this chapter titled Gender? Why not Sex? The Just as psychologists seemed to be reaching agreement
terms “sex” and “gender” are closely intertwined in that gender is to sociocultural causes as sex is to biological
science. Sex often connotes sexuality, and gender has come ones, empirical findings muddied any such neat distinction.
to refer to the cross-disciplinary field of study devoted to By the 1990s, it was clear that the biology and psychology
understanding the origins and consequences of being of masculinity and femininity could not be boxed into sepa-
male or female. This chapter reviews this field of study. rate theories. Biological sex and the social environment are
Specifically, it considers the joint influence of biological now understood to work together in influencing the attri-
and sociocultural factors on the behavior of women and butes and behavior of women and men. If most differences
men. It provides a broad biosocial framework to organize between males and females are joint products of biology
the state of the evidence on the social thoughts, feelings, and society, then psychologists face a conundrum: Should
and behaviors of and about women and men. the modifier of differences and similarities between women
Within psychology, the terms gender and sex are often and men be “gender” or “sex”? What about the modifiers
understood to represent distinct sets of causes for the behavior for roles, stereotypes, and identities—“sex” or “gender”?
of women and men. Feminist researchers separated sex from The underlying questions about causes cannot be answered
gender during the 1970s and 1980s to distinguish between through mere labeling. Establishing the contributions of
female and male biology and the meanings that societies and biological and sociocultural causation is the end product
individuals ascribe to male and female categories (e.g., Unger, of research, not its starting point.
1979). In so doing, they were casting off the assumption This dilemma can be resolved by abandoning the now-
that observable differences between the sexes arise because of traditional biology versus culture meaning of sex versus
inborn, biological factors that are impervious to social input. gender in favor of definitions that recognize the intertwin-
In an ironic twist, gender as a scientific term was apparently ing of nature and nurture. “Sex” is defined in this chapter
used first in the medical literature to describe the psychology by the common-language meaning of male and female
accompanying certain biological conditions (Haig, 2004). as categories (e.g., “into which humans and most other
Gender referred to the masculine or feminine self-concepts living things are divided on the basis of their reproduc-
of individuals whose physical anomalies did not fit usual tive functions”; Oxford English Dictionary, 2009). These
genital designation as male or female (Money, Hampson, & two groups are based on a biological reality of differing
Hampson, 1955). Nonetheless, gender became the standard chromosomes and associated hormonal and reproductive
term for cultural distinctions between men and women and differences. Yet this classification in daily life is a social
sex the standard term for biological distinctions (American act based on personal assessments or observers’ judgments
Psychological Association, 2001). Whereas in the 1960s of maleness or femaleness (Kessler & McKenna, 1978).
social science publications rarely mentioned gender, by the Therefore, visible cues of sex can sometimes contradict
turn of this century they used it more than twice as often as chromosomes. And the classification has to be expanded
sex (Haig, 2004). beyond two categories to include individuals who are

This chapter was prepared while Wendy Wood was a fellow at the Radcliffe Institute for Advanced Study. The authors would like to thank
Amanda Diekman, Paul Eastwick, Elizabeth Garrett, Peter Glick, Melissa Hines, Robert A. Josephs, Anne Koenig, N. Pontus Leander,
Laurie Rudman, Janet Spence, and Carmen Tanner for their thoughtful comments on an earlier draft of the chapter.

629
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
630 Gender

intersexed and thus possess both male and female biologi- explanations that they hope will allow them to master the
cal characteristics or are transsexual and have undertaken complexities of sex and gender. Some such accounts focus
surgery or hormone therapy to change their biological exclusively on hormones and other biological processes,
sex (Fausto-Sterling, 1993). Nonetheless, the term “sex” and they fail to acknowledge how these processes depend
designates female and male categories that fit the great on sociocultural factors. Others focus exclusively on socio-
majority of humans, and, in our usage, the terms sex differ- cultural factors such as the social construction of gender
ences and sex similarities do not imply particular types of and do not recognize how these are grounded in biology.
underlying causes. However, contemporary science has revealed the value of
The term “gender” refers to the meanings that individuals a different approach—one that recognizes the inherent depen-
and societies ascribe to males and females—meanings dence between biological and sociocultural causes in produc-
that rest on a biology in which most humans possess the ing and erasing differences between women and men.
standard XX or XY chromosomes. As this chapter explains, In striking evidence of this dependence, researchers
this biological difference emerges in human societies as a who pursue causal threads associated with the biological
division of labor between men and women, which in turn factors of sex-related genes and hormones sometimes have
drives the meanings that cultures impute to male and female found themselves revealing the influences of socialization
and the meanings that individuals impute to themselves. and culture. This interdependence reflects that the effects
Terms such as gender role and gender identity refer to cultural of male and female biology are moderated by aspects of the
meanings in this sense. Sex and gender thus are separated social environment. Genes function not like encapsulated
into convenient categories that reflect the natural language units of heredity but instead like response systems that
definition of these words and simultaneously acknowledge are highly contingent on environmental input (Lickliter &
the intertwining of nature and nurture. With this distinction, Honeycutt, 2003). Illustrating this contingency, the onset
research findings and not terminologies do the heavy lifting of menarche in girls is now understood to depend on
of identifying the causes of sex and gender. the psychosocial environment (Ellis, 2004). Girls begin
In brief, the present chapter reviews the research evi- menstruating as early as around 12 years in some urban
dence on the biological and sociocultural causes of the postindustrial societies and as late as 18 years or more
thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of and about men and in rural highland Papua New Guinea or high altitude
women. Our scope is necessarily broad and considers Nepali groups. Age of onset is regulated biologically by
causes at multiple levels, including individual-level causes the maturation of the adrenal glands and the hypothalamic–
of hormones and personal identities, societal-level causes in pituitary–gonadal axis. The rate of this maturation is
stereotypical beliefs about the sexes, and evolutionary increased by psychosocial stressors such as father absence,
pressures that influence men’s and women’s behavior. emotionally distant mother–daughter relationships, depres-
Building on these various causes, the chapter considers the sion, and exposure to family conflicts. As Ellis (2004,
evidence for sex differences in aggression and prosocial p. 948) indicated, until recently, “the notion that social
behavior. It concludes with a discussion of how biosocial experiences influence something as biological and presum-
processes account for change and stability in men’s and ably genetic as pubertal timing was not taken seriously” by
women’s roles in society. The next sections explain our the research community.
approach, first by outlining why it is necessary to consider Is the reverse also true—that the influence of sociocul-
both biological and social influences and then by organiz- tural factors depends on biology? If so, in the Gordian knot
ing these various causes into a coherent theory. of causal influences, research that pulls a thread associ-
ated with the social determinants of sex differences would
reveal the effects of genes and hormones. We are not refer-
BIOSOCIAL CAUSES OF MEN’S ring to the obvious idea that men and women are living
AND WOMEN’S BEHAVIOR beings whose survival and reproduction is undergirded by
biological processes. Instead, in its more profound mani-
The biological and social causes of sex differences are festation, the sociocultural meaning of gender is grounded
closely interlinked in their effects. This presents a Gordian partly in biological differences between the sexes. As an
knot of complexity, with the threads of the knot representing illustration, boys’ preferences for masculine, wheeled toys
the intertwined biological and sociocultural influences. that afford motion can be traced to more than the socializa-
To disentangle these causes, researchers do not have the tion of toy preferences by parents, peers, and the media.
mythical power of Alexander the Great who with a single Masculine toy preferences also appear to have a hormonal
cut from his sword sliced the Gordian knot in two. Yet, basis in prenatal androgen exposure. Girls with congenital
some psychologists do continue to apply simple, unitary adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), a disorder involving exposure
Gender Roles 631

to high levels of prenatal androgen, show these mascu- women bear and nurse children and men possess greater
line toy preferences along with other boyish attributes— size, speed, and strength, especially in the upper body,
elevated activity levels and greater rough-and-tumble play certain activities in a society are more efficiently accom-
(Hines, 2009). Also suggesting that toy preferences have plished by one sex than the other. In short, the division of
a hormonal basis, male juvenile vervet and rhesus mon- labor arises because it can be easier for one sex to perform
keys, much like young boys, showed a stronger prefer- certain tasks of daily life.
ence for wheeled toys than did their female counterparts By looking downward in the nomological net toward
(Alexander & Hines, 2002; Hassett, Siebert, & Wallen, the more proximal causes of individuals’ behavior, we can
2008). The socialization of children elaborates this ini- identify the immediate determinants of differences between
tial hormonal effect by fostering different toy choices and male and female behavior, as well as differences within each
play activities in girls and boys within societies (Lytton & sex. Social and personality psychologists typically ask
Romney, 1991). questions about proximal causes, such as why women gen-
The interwoven influences of biology and culture in erally are more nurturant than men toward close others and
enhancing and reducing sex differences might seem to over- why individual women vary in their propensity to nurture.
whelm personality and social psychological approaches As this chapter explains, the proximal causes of sex differ-
by adding a laundry list of genetic and hormonal variables ences in individual behavior are framed by gender roles, or
to investigations of female and male behavior. Worse yet, the shared beliefs that apply to individuals on the basis of
studying these intertwined influences might seem to place their socially identified sex (Eagly, 1987; Eagly, Wood, &
social and personality psychologists at risk for relocating Johannesen-Schmidt, 2004).
their labs in the biology building on campus. Fortunately, Gender roles, as well as specific social roles such as
theorizing about the various causes has kept pace with the daughter, boss, and friend, influence the behavior of indi-
emerging complexity of empirical findings. In particular, vidual women and men through more immediate causes.
biosocial theories, which consider the interface between bio- We consider a trio of proximal causes of sex differences and
logical and sociocultural influences, articulate a clear role similarities that reflect the biosocial interaction close-up:
for social and personality psychology variables in scientific Roles influence behavior through chemical signals of hor-
models about sex differences in behavior, as well as in peo- monal changes in interaction with personal gender identity
ple’s beliefs about men and women. and others’ stereotypical expectations. The second and
third of these causes, which are the sociocultural aspects
of this biosocial model, are reminiscent of Deaux and
DISTAL AND PROXIMAL CAUSES OF SEX Major ’s (1987) argument that gender is enacted in dyadic
DIFFERENCES AND SIMILARITIES interactions as a function of gendered beliefs about the self,
others’ expectations, and contextual influences that make
From the biosocial perspective of this chapter, sex and gender more or less salient (see also Deaux & LaFrance,
gender constructs in psychology are part of a nomological 1998). The next sections of the chapter first outline the
net, or series of connected theoretical concepts and nature and functioning of gender roles and then address
observable properties, within which the constructs have a the immediate biosocial mechanisms by which these
particular meaning (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). By looking beliefs about women and men influence behavior.
upward in the net toward the distal, fundamental causes
of male and female behavior, we can answer the kind of
big picture questions about women and men that a Martian GENDER ROLES
might puzzle over when landing on earth for the first time
and observing any human society. In all known societies, Gender role beliefs arise from the specific social roles
men and boys to some extent specialize in activities differ- occupied by women and men—that is, from the division
ent from those favored by women and girls. An extrater- of labor in society. Most social behavior is embedded in
restrial visitor thus is likely to wonder why the sexes fill the performance of specific roles, and gender roles serve
different social roles and thereby engage in a division of as a backdrop that pervades the performance of such spe-
labor. Answering this question at a societal level of analy- cific roles. Because in all cultures women and men tend
sis illuminates the biosocial interaction writ large. That is, to specialize in different behaviors, people have different
the division of labor arises from the ways in which cultural beliefs about what each sex can and should do. These beliefs
and ecological forces in a society interact with humans’ constitute socially shared stereotypes within a society. In
biology in terms of female and male physical attributes and essence, gender roles are reflected in a society’s stereo-
reproductive activities (Wood & Eagly, 2002). Because types about men and women. Thus, women may be viewed
632 Gender

as kind and compassionate and men as bold and fearless. often labeled agency, or self-assertion, and communion, or
Gender stereotypes might also include specific skills, such connection with others (Bakan, 1966). These basic dimen-
as women having the ability to weave baskets and men to sions, in various forms, underlie people’s beliefs about
tend crops, or vice versa (Murdock & Provost, 1973). different social groups (Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu,
Our definition of gender roles derives from the broader 2002; Judd, James-Hawkins, Yzerbyt, & Kashima, 2005).
concept of social role, which refers to the shared beliefs Men, more than women, are thought to be agentic—that
that apply to people who occupy a certain social position is, masterful, assertive, competitive, and dominant (e.g.,
or are members of a particular social category (e.g., Biddle, Newport, 2001; Spence & Buckner, 2000). Women, more than
1979). For gender roles, these social categories are male men, are thought to be communal—that is, friendly, unself-
and female. In the mind of individuals, roles are schemas, ish, concerned with others, and emotionally expressive. The
or abstract knowledge structures, pertaining to a group of expressiveness accorded to women extends to a range of emo-
people. When role schemas are shared among members tions, including sadness, embarrassment, fear, distress, sym-
of a society, they constitute structures at the societal level, as pathy, love, and happiness, but not to anger and pride, which
well as the individual level. Roles are thus symbolic are ascribed more to men than to women (Alexander &
aspects of social structure, which consists of persisting and Wood, 2000; Plant, Hyde, Keltner, & Devine, 2000).
bounded patterns of behavior and social interaction (e.g., Agency and communion were the predominant themes
House, 1995). that emerged in the foundational studies of gender stereo-
Gender roles, like roles based on group memberships types. When respondents in such research listed the ways
such as age, social class, and race, apply to many aspects of in which men and women differ, a high proportion of the
daily life. In their Handbook of Social Psychology chapter most consensual attributes were either agentic or communal
on gender, Deaux and LaFrance (1998) likened this aspect (Broverman, Vogel, Broverman, Clarkson, & Rosenkrantz,
of gender to air—a pervasive, ever-present part of people’s 1972). When respondents in another study free associated
experience. In contrast, more specific roles based on fac- to the terms masculine and feminine, the great majority of
tors such as family relationships (e.g., father or daughter) these associations were agentic or communal traits (Deaux &
and occupations (e.g., teacher or police officer) are rele- Lewis, 1983). These stereotypes emerge with implicit
vant mainly to behavior in a particular social context. For measures, such as the Implicit Association Test, as well as
example, occupational roles are pertinent mostly at work. with traditional explicit measures based on rating scales
Given their general applicability across settings, gender (Rudman, Greenwald, & McGhee, 2001). Male agency
roles influence behavior simultaneously with specific roles and female communion also appear to be pancultural,
and roles based on other group memberships (e.g., racial albeit with some variation across cultures (Best & Thomas,
groups). These intersections of gender roles with other 2004; Williams & Best, 1990).
roles lend complexity to female and male behavior. Agency and communion are not the whole story on gen-
Gender roles specify what men and women usually do der stereotypes. People also take into account contrasting
and what they should do—that is, roles are descriptive and features of male and female bodies. They regard men as
prescriptive (or injunctive; Cialdini & Trost, 1998; Prislin & muscular, strong, and tall and women as pretty, sexy, and
Wood, 2005). The descriptive aspect of gender roles indi- petite (Cejka & Eagly, 1999; Deaux & Lewis, 1984). With
cates what is typical for each sex. People rely on this respect to the mind, intellectual ability is regarded as having
descriptive information when they are concerned about subtly different contours in women and men, with women
what is normal for their sex. Especially if a situation is being more creative and verbally skilled and men more
ambiguous or confusing, people tend to enact sex-typical analytical and quantitatively skilled (Cejka & Eagly, 1999;
behaviors. The prescriptive aspect of gender roles describes Swim, 1994). Sheer intelligence is ascribed somewhat
what is desirable and admirable for each sex. People rely more to women than men in contemporary U.S. repre-
on this prescriptive information when they are motivated sentative surveys (Newport, 2001; Pew Research Center,
to gain social approval or to bolster their own esteem. 2008), as opposed to the earlier tendency to ascribe greater
intelligence to men (e.g., Fernberger, 1948).
Content of Gender Roles People readily take other group memberships and social
roles into account along with gender roles. The meaning
What are the gender role beliefs that people commonly of male and female social categories differs depend-
hold about women and men? Research on gender stereo- ing on group membership such as nationality (Eagly &
types reveals this content (see review by Kite, Deaux, & Kite, 1987), age (Kite, Deaux, & Miele, 1991), race, and
Haines, 2007). Most people’s beliefs about men and women ethnicity (Timberlake & Estes, 2007). To give one illustra-
can be summarized in two dimensions, which are most tion, sexual orientation makes a difference: Homosexual
Gender Roles 633

individuals are regarded as similar to opposite-sex of inferring traits from observed behavior is known as
heterosexuals—that is, gay men as more feminine and les- correspondent inference or correspondence bias (Gilbert,
bian women as more masculine—than typical members of 1998). For example, on observing an act of kindness, per-
their sex group (Kite & Deaux, 1987). Specific social roles ceivers automatically identify the behavior in trait terms
also moderate gender roles. For example, acknowledging and characterize the actor by the trait that is implied—as
women’s domestic role, perceivers accord more agency a nice, caring person. By making this inference, people
to women than men in some domestic contexts (Mendoza- commit the fundamental attribution error by assuming
Denton, Park, & O’Connor, 2008). In addition, portraying that people are what they do. This process is widespread
women and men in the same specific social role (e.g., occu- (Gawronski, 2003; Ross, Amabile, & Steinmetz, 1977) and
pation) appears to reduce the impact of gender stereotypes largely spontaneous (Uleman, Saribay, & Gonzalez, 2008).
(Eagly & Steffen, 1984; Eagly & Wood, 1982). From observations of individuals, perceivers then gen-
Also reflecting that gender roles coexist with other eralize to the traits of entire groups of people—that is, to
social roles, the general categories of men and women group stereotypes. Illustrating how thinking proceeds from
are composed of subtypes. Vonk and Ashmore (2003) individuals to groups, Ridgeway and Erickson (2000)
identified more than 200 masculine and feminine sub- created differential status between two individuals by
types that varied in their young versus old and traditional having one person (an experimental confederate) act in a
versus modern meaning. Gender subtypes represent men deferential or a superior way to the other person (the par-
and women in, for example, occupations such as busi- ticipant). These two people had been arbitrarily assigned
nessman and career woman and interpersonal roles such to different nominal groups, A or B. The status difference
as family man and lover (Carpenter & Trentham, 1998; established between these two individuals then was gen-
Eckes, 2002). New subtypes continually emerge, such eralized, by the research participants and by observers, to
as Joe six-pack and hockey mom to represent the intersec- other members of A and B groups, as if it were a character-
tions of gender with political interest groups in the 2008 istic of the groups (see also Ridgeway, 2006b). Perceivers
U.S. presidential election campaign. appear to be especially adept at jumping from observations
Children gradually gain sophistication in their thinking of even a single man or woman to generalizations about
about the sexes. Young children do not stereotype at the qualities typical of each sex (Prentice & Miller, 2006).
abstract level of the agentic and communal personality If gender stereotypes are largely data driven, what
traits that dominate adults’ gender stereotypes (see Miller, are the observations that feed them? Because the divi-
Trautner, & Ruble, 2006, for a review). Instead, children sion between female domestic labor and male wage labor
as young as 3 years associate concrete objects and activi- remains partially intact (Bianchi, Robinson, & Milkie,
ties with each sex—for example, dolls, hairbrushes, and 2006), people have many opportunities to observe women
sewing with females and baseball bats, cars, and playing and men engaging in different behaviors. They tend to
basketball with males. Also, young children link qualities see women engaged in supportive, nurturing behaviors in
such as pink and softness with females and blue and rough- their domestic role and in occupations (e.g., teacher and
ness with males. In middle childhood, the core agency and nurse) that emphasize communal characteristics (Cejka &
communion stereotypes start to emerge. With maturity, Eagly, 1999; England, Budig, & Folbre, 2002). Also, peo-
perceivers acquire a rich set of social psychological con- ple tend to see men in family roles of provider and head
cepts for thinking about men and women. of household, as well as in certain occupations that foster
assertive, task-oriented behaviors (Cejka & Eagly, 1999).
Additionally important are indirect observations provided
Roots of Gender Roles in the Division of Labor
by media portrayals and cultural lore. Given repeated
The gender role beliefs, or stereotypes shared within a observations of men and women engaging in different
society, are not arbitrary or random. Instead, they are types of behaviors, gender roles effortlessly emerge.
firmly rooted in a society’s division of labor whereby peo- Do these observations of male and female roles boil
ple observe men and women engaged in different types of down to sex differences in observed social status, as
activities (Eagly, Wood, & Diekman, 2000). From these some social psychologists have argued (e.g., Conway,
observations, people develop beliefs about women’s and Pizzamiglio, & Mount, 1996; Ridgeway & Bourg, 2004)?
men’s attributes, especially their personality traits. How do People often observe men in higher-status roles and women
perceivers make their way from observations of concrete in lower-status ones—for example, male executives inter-
behaviors to abstract ideas about traits? acting with female secretaries and clerks. Accordingly,
As a first step, people’s behaviors are assumed to perceivers infer that men have the correspondent attri-
reflect their intrinsic characteristics. This cognitive process butes of competitiveness and agency and women have the
634 Gender

attributes of compliance and supportiveness (Wood & people believe that women smile more than men, and
Karten, 1986). However, status is more strongly related to research has found this to be a sex difference (LaFrance,
agency than to communion (e.g., Conway et al., 1996) and Hecht, & Paluck, 2003). Also suggesting accuracy, gender
thus is more suited to explaining stereotypes of men. stereotypical beliefs were correlated with men’s and
The perception that women are communal can be women’s experiences of the emotions of anger, fear, love,
traced largely to their cooperative interdependence with joy, and sadness (Grossman & Wood, 1993). In addition,
other groups (e.g., men, children, and the elderly; Fiske people can successfully estimate the social attitudes held
et al., 2002). These helpful and supportive social relation- by men and women on various topics (Diekman, Eagly, &
ships underlie the “women are wonderful” effect, whereby Kulesa, 2002). Furthermore, Hall and Carter (1999a) found
the female stereotype is often more favorable than the that people with more accurate gender stereotypes were
male stereotype (Eagly & Mladinic, 1994; Rudman & also more interpersonally sensitive and believed more in the
Goodwin, 2004). accuracy of their social perceptions.
Women’s greater cooperative interdependence and men’s Despite this evidence of a substantial kernel of truth,
greater status also have a dark side (Spence, Helmreich, & cognitive processes can exaggerate people’s judgments of
Holohan, 1979). Social perceivers regard women as more male–female differences. Categorizing people by sex is
vulnerable than men to a communal focus on others that one such process. When individuals are categorized into
results in neglect of oneself (e.g., inability to say no and groups, they might seem more similar to other group mem-
excessive concern with others’ problems). They also bers than they actually are, as well as more different from
regard men as more vulnerable than women to an agentic members of other groups (e.g., Tajfel, 1981; Wilder, 1984).
self-focus that results in neglect of others (e.g., arrogant, Sex provides the strongest basis of categorizing people,
greedy, and cynical; Helgeson & Fritz, 1999). Thus, gen- even when compared with race, age, and occupation
der ’s troubled waters consist of agency not blended with (Fiske, Haslam, & Fiske, 1991; Stangor, Lynch, Duan, &
some communion and communion not blended with some Glass, 1992; but see Quinn & Macrae, 2005).
agency. Stereotypes also slant the way in which people encode
instances of behavior. By assimilating women and men to
gender stereotypes, perceivers may not judge them accu-
Accuracy of Gender Stereotypes
rately (von Hippel, Sekaquaptewa, & Vargas, 1995). In
Are gender stereotypes accurate? Yes and no. To the extent illustration, research participants informed about another
that stereotypes are grounded in reality, they inevitably person’s agentic acts of leadership (e.g., “when speaking,
possess a kernel of truth. People’s frequent observations of motivates employees”) were more likely to activate in
male and female behavior provide myriad opportunities to memory the implied agentic traits (e.g., “charismatic”) when
correct biased beliefs (Fiedler & Walther, 2004). Moreover, the other person was male (Scott & Brown, 2006). Thus,
categorizing people as female and male would not be use- given the same act of agency, observers are more likely to
ful unless the meanings associated with the categories were accord men than women the correspondent trait because
at least broadly accurate. But saying “yes, stereotypes are agency is regarded typical of men. As a result of such biased
accurate on average,” could mean “no” for any specific processes, an assertive, decisive woman might not be con-
instance. An intelligent answer separates the accuracy of sidered for leadership positions (Eagly & Carli, 2007).
beliefs about a category from those about individual cat- As another source of inaccuracy, differences are over-
egory members (Ryan, 2002). Beliefs about groups may be estimated when men and women are viewed as opposites
quite accurate based on group averages (e.g., men like to along a masculine–feminine bipolar dimension (Green,
shop for tools) but inaccurate when applied to individuals Ashmore, & Manzi, 2005; Krueger, Hasman, Acevedo, &
within the groups (e.g., Steve is a man, so he will enjoy Villano, 2003) or as having opposing group interests
tool shopping). For people not typical of their sex, stereo- (Diekman et al., 2002). Nevertheless, empirical support for
typical judgments are necessarily inaccurate. the exaggeration of group-level sex differences is mixed,
Providing fairly good evidence of accuracy, research has with some studies showing overestimation (Allen, 1995;
related gender stereotypes to the sex differences and simi- Martin, 1987) and others showing underestimation (e.g.,
larities established in psychological research. Participants’ Cejka & Eagly, 1999; Swim, 1994; see discussion in sec-
beliefs about the direction and magnitude of sex differences tion, Sex Differences in Psychological Research).
are moderately correlated with the findings of meta-analyses Popular media also reduce stereotype accuracy by exag-
of studies that compared the sexes on a range of person- gerating differences between the sexes, despite occasional
ality traits, abilities, and social behaviors (Briton & Hall, counterstereotypical portrayals (e.g., a female U.S. president
1995; Hall & Carter, 1999a; Swim, 1994). For example, in the television series Commander in Chief). One such
Gender Roles 635

bias is that men are depicted more often than women in Controlling Detrimental Effects of Gender Roles
prime-time television commercials and more often in pri- on Judgments of Individuals
mary roles (Furnham & Mak, 1999; Ganahl, Prinsen, &
Netzley, 2003). Media also mirror the division of labor, The accuracy of gender stereotypes at the group level
with prime-time television programming of situation com- does not make stereotypes benign influences in daily life.
edy, drama, and reality offering 23% more men than women Instead, stereotypes constrain behavior and foster sanc-
in employment-related roles and 33% more women than tions for deviation (see the later section Influence of Social
men in interpersonal roles involving family, romance, and Expectations). Stereotypes also help to retain traditional
friends (Lauzen, Dozier, & Horan, 2008). These paycheck- patterns of behavior: Gender stereotypes derive from
earning men and relationship-focused women fuel gender observing women and men enacting a division of labor and
stereotypes in a media-intensive society. Suggesting the in turn hold people into the very roles that make up the
influence of media portrayals, frequent television viewing division of labor.
is associated with more stereotypical beliefs about women Controversy surrounds the question of whether people
and men (Morgan & Shanahan, 1997). can inhibit stereotypes about social groups. Consistent
Other stereotyping processes compromise accuracy by with the inevitability of stereotyping are numerous experi-
minimizing people’s reports of sex differences. This effect ments suggesting that stereotype activation does not
occurs when perceivers use shifting standards to judge require intentional control or conscious awareness. For
men and women. If people compare women with other example, priming of gender stereotypical words (e.g.,
women and men with other men, then the sexes are judged jobs such as “nurse” or “doctor”) versus nonstereotypical
by different standards (Biernat, 2003, 2005). To illustrate, words led participants to classify quickly gender-matched
consider athleticism, a set of skills in which men predomi- pronouns (e.g., “he” or “she”) into male and female
nate so much that athletic competitions are typically segre- categories (e.g., Banaji & Hardin, 1996). Similarly, implicit
gated by sex. When people describe a woman and a man as priming induced by having participants unscramble
“athletic,” they usually mean that the woman is athletic for sentences with male or female stereotypical (vs. nonste-
a woman, just as the man is athletic for a man. Given typi- reotypical) content produced more stereotypical ratings of
cally lesser athletic prowess in women, observers would a target person of the sex implied by the priming (Banaji,
judge a woman athlete by a different and lower standard Hardin, & Rothman, 1993). Such phenomena suggest
than a man. The result could be that a man and woman are that perceivers have acquired a network of associations
judged as equally “athletic,” for example, on a subjective about men and women that are ordinarily activated by rel-
rating scale ranging from “very athletic” to “not at all ath- evant concepts (Bargh, 1999; Fiske, 1998).
letic.” Yet, if observers judged this man and woman by the Questioning the inevitability of stereotyping are experi-
same, or common, standard (e.g., “how far can he or she ments demonstrating that mere exposure to sex-related
throw a ball?”), the (accurate) stereotype of greater male ath- cues does not necessarily bring gender stereotypes to
leticism would ordinarily dominate judgments (Biernat & mind, at least not in their generic form. For example,
Vescio, 2002). after imagining a “strong woman,” such as a business-
Stereotypes also are minimized when perceivers con- woman in charge of others or a female athlete, perceivers
trast individual men’s and women’s behavior with gender indicated less extreme gender stereotypes on the Implicit
stereotypes. For example, when confronted with unam- Association Test (Blair, Ma, & Lenton, 2001). A host of
biguously counterstereotypical behavior, perceivers may other factors can modulate the activation of gender ste-
try to explain the unusual behavior and end up conclud- reotypes, including time pressure and other cognitive
ing that the individuals possess especially strong attributes constraints (Blair & Banaji, 1996), perceivers’ processing
corresponding to the behavior. Thus, perceivers observing goals (Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, Thorne, & Castelli,
exceptional service from a female financial adviser or a 1997; Moskowitz, Gollwitzer, Wasel, & Schaal, 1999), and
male wedding planner inferred that they must possess spe- a social environment with women in leadership positions
cial competence at financial or wedding services (Matta & (Dasgupta & Asgari, 2004). Such phenomena suggest that
Folkes, 2005). gender stereotypes are fluid, contextually sensitive, and
In summary, gender stereotypes are subject to conflict- potentially controllable.
ing pressures: People’s many opportunities to observe Even when stereotypes have been activated, they are not
males and females ensure overall group-level accuracy always used in judging others (Devine, 1989). Information
despite the various sources of bias that can exaggerate or about individuals’ other qualities can restrain the applica-
minimize genuine differences between the sexes. tion of stereotypes to some extent (Fiske, 1998). Thus, in
newly formed discussion groups, members perceived men
636 Gender

as higher in agency than women except when given direct nurturing. In general, the more attributes actually differ
information about the others’ relative aptitudes (Wood & between women and men, the more desirable they seem
Karten, 1986). In addition, the application of stereotypes to be for one sex as opposed to the other (Hall & Carter,
depends on a complex of cognitive and motivational vari- 1999b). This prescriptiveness makes sense, given that gen-
ables (Blair, 2002; Kunda & Spencer, 2003). For example, der roles capture the qualities that facilitate sex-typical
perceivers can inhibit stereotyping when they have suffi- activities in society.
cient cognitive resources and motivation (e.g., Dasgupta & As an additional contribution to the power of ste-
Rivera, 2006; see Fazio & Olson, 2003). Their motiva- reotypes about women and men, sex strikes people as a
tion to restrain prejudicial reactions can reflect internal, natural-kind category, associated with a deep, inherent
personal desires to avoid sexism and external pressures quality that makes the attributes linked with it seem rela-
deriving from others’ reactions (Klonis, Plant, & Devine, tively unalterable. For many social perceivers, women
2005). Nonetheless, the motivation to correct for gender possess an essential nature that is different from that of
stereotypes is not guaranteed. People feel less compunc- men. Of 40 social categories, male–female categories were
tion when they are confronted with their own sexism, judged as the most natural, necessary, immutable, dis-
compared with their other types of biases (e.g., Czopp & crete, and stable (Haslam, Rothschild, & Ernst, 2000). As
Monteith, 2003). a consequence, people often regard feminine or masculine
In summary, perceptions of individuals are not always attributes and behaviors as inherent, stable properties of
at the mercy of gender stereotypes. Although social catego- the sexes (Prentice & Miller, 2006).
ries such as gender may be automatically activated outside In the next sections of this chapter, we consider
of awareness and without conscious intent, such activa- various mechanisms through which gender roles and spe-
tion does not always occur. Even when gender stereotypes cific social roles are enacted to produce the behavior of
have been activated, perceivers can control their potential men and women. For social and personality psychologists,
effects on judgments, given sufficient motivation and cog- this is where the rubber meets the road—where theories
nitive resources. However, in the hurly-burly of daily life, about psychological and biological processes are tested as
people often lack the motivation or resources to exert this predictors of behavior. However, within the nomological
control (see Macrae & Quadflieg, this volume). network of gender constructs, equally important are the
distal biosocial causes of gender roles that we consider
toward the end of the chapter.
Potency of Gender Roles

Gender roles do not always influence judgments and


behavior, but they wield considerable power in various ROLES GUIDE BEHAVIOR
circumstances. The strength of gender roles arises from
several sources: The shared beliefs that comprise these Proximal Influences
roles seem consensual, they have an injunctive or prescrip-
tive quality, and they appear to describe qualities that are In daily life, people carry out gender roles as they enact
deeply embedded in human nature. specific social roles such as athlete, employee, and par-
Stereotypical beliefs that are supported by social con- ent. Performing social roles is enabled by biological and
sensus seem valid—after all, others endorse them, and this psychological mechanisms that interact in various ways.
consensus establishes pressures to comply (Crandall & Biological processes include hormones acting as chemi-
Stangor, 2005). In research manipulating the consen- cal signals and psychological mechanisms include gender
sus behind racial stereotypes, college students with the identities and others’ expectations. These factors work in
impression that their anti-Black beliefs were shared by tandem to promote role performance.
other students sat further from an African American To understand how hormonal and psychological factors
(Sechrist & Stangor, 2001). Similarly, the widespread con- work together to guide behavior, consider the aggressive,
sensus about female communion and male agency fuels the competitive behaviors required to win athletic contests.
effects of these stereotypes on judgments and behavior. Performance of these behaviors is promoted by increases
Gender roles acquire additional power from their pre- in the steroid hormone, testosterone, which is an andro-
scriptiveness. Gender stereotypical ways of behaving seem gen secreted in men by the testes and in women to a lesser
generally desirable for that sex (Prentice & Carranza, 2002; extent by the adrenal gland and ovaries. Athletic competi-
Spence & Helmreich, 1978). For example, if women, far tion also is promoted by a masculine, agentic identity, as
more than men, care for infants and young children, it athletes regulate their behavior in line with this gendered
is reasonable that people prefer that women be kind and self-view. Athletic performance also is likely to be highly
Roles Guide Behavior 637

responsive to the agentic expectations of fans, other players, recruited for performance of roles involving competitive
and coaches. In short, fluctuations in testosterone guide dominance. The model also postulates a reciprocal rela-
responses when people are engaged in roles involving tion by which increased T promotes dominant, aggres-
dominance and competition, as defined by their identities sive responding. That is, circulating T orients people to
and by others’ expectations. Thus, the biosocial interaction assume roles or engage in behaviors involving competitive
reflects the selective recruitment of hormones and other dominance.
neurochemical processes for particular roles, given men’s Providing evidence that T is recruited in performance
and women’s gender identities and others’ expectations for of social roles or in reaction to situational provocations,
role performance. T levels among men rise in anticipation of athletic and
The biological and psychological influences we identify other competition and in response to insults, presumably to
in this chapter are honed through socialization to support energize and direct their physical and cognitive performance
men’s and women’s role performances. Through socializa- (e.g., Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle, & Schwarz, 1996; Suay et al.,
tion for particular roles, boys and girls learn to channel 1999). This relation was secured in Archer ’s (2006) meta-
and respond to neurochemical signals, gender identities, and analysis in which T rose in men anticipating and playing
expectations of others in ways that enable them to carry sports and other competitions (e.g., video games), espe-
out their everyday social roles. cially among the contest winners. In addition, higher
T levels among male financial traders predicted their daily
profitability (Coates & Herbert, 2008). This finding sug-
Hormonal Influences
gests that T facilitates competition even against the gen-
Gender roles and specific social roles guide behavior eralized competitors who make up financial markets.
partly through the activation of hormonal changes. The C levels also may be implicated, given that these have
hormones most relevant to understanding sex differences been found to rise in male and female athletes before and
are testosterone (T), oxytocin (OT), and to some extent during a match (Bateup, Booth, Shirtcliff, & Granger,
cortisol (C). These hormones act as chemical transmitters 2002; Edwards, Wetzel, & Wyner, 2006).
in the brain that promote performance of certain social T does not increase in the absence of social roles or
behaviors. With culturally masculine roles, higher levels situational provocations that call for aggressive, dominant
of T are associated with dominance, or behaviors that gain behavior. Thus, men’s T did not reliably increase in con-
or maintain status. In humans, such behaviors often entail trived laboratory competitions (Archer, 2006). Also, in
competition, risk taking, and aggression that may harm or women’s competitions, T did not rise in players of a video
injure others (Booth, Granger, Mazur, & Kivlighan, 2006). game before a contest (Mazur, Susman, & Edelbrock,
In contrast, with culturally feminine roles, higher levels 1997), but it rose in female rugby and soccer players before
of OT (as well as reduced C and T) are associated with and during a match (Bateup et al., 2002; Edwards et al.,
behaviors that produce parental bonding, nurturance, and 2006). Hence, despite women’s production of only one fifth
intimacy (Campbell, 2008). to one tenth of men’s T levels, the hormone is activated in
According to the biosocial interaction, T is relevant both sexes by interactions that are interpreted as dominance
when, due to personal identities and social expectancies, contests. Because men and women may not always agree in
people experience social interactions as dominance con- their interpretations of dominance interactions, some differ-
tests. OT is relevant when, due to personal identities and ences may occur in the details of these effects. Nonetheless,
social expectancies, people define social interactions as consistent with the classic model, role performances involv-
involving bonding and affiliation with close others. T and ing competitive dominance activate T.
OT promote role performance in conjunction with other How about the reciprocal relation whereby T activates
hormones, including C, a hormone associated with the competitive, dominant behavior, as also stipulated by the
experience of stress that is secreted by the adrenal gland. classic model? A meta-analysis of 11 studies that experi-
Lowered C may facilitate bonding with others, and height- mentally injected men with T or related synthetic andro-
ened C may facilitate aggression and dominance. And gens found no systematic rise in anger, aggression, or
other neurochemical processes, especially those associated hostility (Archer, 2006). It may be that the experimental
with rewards and learning of particular behaviors, supple- contexts did not reliably activate masculine identities and
ment or even supplant influences of T and OT. social expectations. In the absence of competition, circu-
lating T appears largely unrelated to behavior. Instead, it
Hormones and Masculine Social Roles shows effects primarily when dominance battles are immi-
In Mazur and Booth’s (1998) classic model, performance nent (e.g., Josephs, Sellers, Newman, & Mehta, 2006;
of dominant, aggressive behaviors activates T. That is, T is Mehta, Jones, & Josephs, 2008).
638 Gender

In humans, dominance contests and displays are highly in mothers viewing pictures of their infants and in male
varied, with most occurring without physical aggression and female lovers viewing pictures of their romantic part-
(Mazur & Booth, 1998). Thus, it is not surprising that only ners (Bartels & Zeki, 2004).
a small, positive association emerged in a meta-analytical In humans and other large-brained primates, parental
review of the relation between circulating T and various bonds and affiliation are promoted not only by OT but
behavioral and self-report measures of aggression (meta- also by hormones involved in reward learning, including
analytical r  .08, k  42; Archer, Graham-Kevan, & opioids and dopamine (Depue & Morrone-Strupinsky,
Davies, 2005). High levels of circulating T might activate 2005; Taylor, 2002). Reward mechanisms enable bonds to
behavior in a range of dominance-provoking situations, not develop even without the hormones of pregnancy, parturi-
just those associated with aggression. In support, high T tion, and lactation. For example, close bonds to infants can
in men is associated with criminal acts, especially violent develop in adoptive relationships. Fathers and other rela-
crime, and some antisocial behaviors (Booth et al., 2006; tives and caretakers also bond with infants. Such bonding
Dabbs & Dabbs, 2000). In addition, higher T characterizes is promoted by experiences of reward that can be triggered
occupations such as professional football, in which suc- by infants’ vulnerability and need, by physical sensations
cess involves self-aggrandizing displays of dominance in of tactile contact and smell, and by the high value societies
face-to-face confrontations, compared with occupations place on children. Underlying this positive affect are neuro-
such as minister, which generally involve selflessness and chemical reward systems that promote nurturing separately
concern for others (Dabbs & Dabbs, 2000). Also, experi- from the hormones of pregnancy. According to Kendrick
mental manipulations of T in college women appeared to (2004), these learned rewards can account for much of the
heighten their tendencies to dress and groom their hair positive affect arising from human maternal behavior. In
attractively—behaviors that may yield popularity and evidence, manipulations of neurochemicals associated with
social power in everyday competition for young women reward influence bonding even in nonhuman primates (e.g.,
(Dabbs et al., 2003). Additionally suggesting a role for Kalin, Shelton, & Lynn, 1995). Thus, humans and other
T in displays of dominance, men with high circulating T large-brained primates establish and maintain social bonds
smiled less in facial photographs (Dabbs, 1997). In these through both hormonal activation and reward learning.
ways, hormonal processes facilitate various behaviors as This dual basis of relational bonding reflects “evolution-
people interact with others and carry out social roles. ary progression away from hormonal-centric determinants
of maternal behaviour to emotional, reward-fulfilling
Hormones and Feminine Social Roles activation” (Broad, Curley, & Keverne, 2006, p. 2204).
OT and other neurochemicals that promote the expression Other hormones also promote intimacy and tending of
of intimacy and caring for others are relevant to the per- others. The stress hormone C and T both are implicated in
formance of roles involving nurturance and affiliation. performance of spousal and caretaking roles. A drop in C ordi-
OT also influences the experience of stress and may have narily accompanies initiation of the parental role, especially
a dual function in promoting affiliative behavior to cope among mothers, evidently to support nurturing (Corter &
with stress and in reflecting the level of stress experienced Fleming, 1995; Fleming, Ruble, Krieger, & Wong, 1997).
in relationships (Taylor et al., 2000, 2006). OT is especially In addition, fathers’ anticipation and vicarious experience of
relevant to women because of the regulation of OT recep- childbirth produce a fall in T, as well as hormonal changes in
tors by estrogen and OT’s stimulation of uterine contrac- estradiol, C, and prolactin that mimic the changes that occur
tions during labor and milk expression during lactation. in mothers (Berg & Wynne-Edwards, 2001, 2002; Storey,
The enhancement of maternal bonding by OT is most Walsh, Quinton, & Wynne-Edwards, 2000). Also, men’s T
evident in rodents and sheep, which have been subjected levels decline with marriage, an effect that may favor enact-
to experimental manipulations of hormones. In humans, ment of the caring spousal and parental roles (Booth et al.,
women’s OT levels increase in contexts involving intimacy 2006). Consistent with this idea, lower levels of circulating
and caring for others. For example, OT rises in women dur- T are associated with married men’s close involvement with
ing childbirth (Takagi et al., 1985) and with massage and their spouse and emotional responsiveness to their infants’
sexual contact (Insel, 2000; Pedersen, 2004). Moreover, cries (e.g., Fleming, Corter, Stallings, & Steiner, 2002; Gray,
women with higher OT in early pregnancy and postpar- Kahlenberg, Barrett, Lipson, & Ellison, 2002).
tum engaged in more maternal bonding behaviors such as In general, performance of roles that involve dominance
gazing at the infant’s face, affectionate touch, and speak- and competition is associated with increased T levels.
ing in the high-pitched, expressive tones of motherese Performance of roles involving nurturance and intimacy
(Feldman, Weller, Zagoory-Sharon, & Levine, 2007). Also, coincides with increased OT levels and reduced C and T. It
brain regions associated with OT receptors were activated might seem plausible that this hormonal regulation is more
Roles Guide Behavior 639

reliable in the sex that has chronically higher levels of a found in several brain systems early in life, each of which
hormone, or at least that the regulatory processes func- is active at different developmental time points, androgen
tion somewhat differently for the sexes, given differences exposure might affect various behaviors through somewhat
in biology (e.g., T in men is generated through the tes- separate brain mechanisms (Hines, 2009).
tes). Nonetheless, women’s competitiveness and aggres- Extensive research has examined the effects of prenatal
siveness are associated with increments of T (Archer androgen exposure on nonhuman primates. For example,
et al., 2005). When T was calculated as a percentage in rhesus monkeys, artificially increasing the exposure of
increase in each sex above basal levels, T increased 24% female fetuses to prenatal androgens enhanced masculine
in female rugby players anticipating a game and 49% in behaviors of juvenile rough-and-tumble play and foot-clasp
those playing it, as well as 36% in male tennis players mounting (a male sexual posture), whereas increasing the
anticipating a match and 6% in those playing it, along exposure of male fetuses to androgen agonists decreased
with a 12% rise in male wrestlers during a match (Bateup these behaviors (Wallen, 1996, 2005). Prenatal androgens
et al., 2002). Also, under conditions of stress, artificially had less consistent effects on other sex-typical behaviors,
increasing OT in men appears to promote their bonding and including aggression, threat, and submission, which dif-
affiliation (Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirshbaum, & Ehlert, fered between normal males and normal females only in
2003). Additionally enabling flexibility in male and female some rearing environments and social contexts.
role performance, the neurochemical systems that under- In human studies, naturally occurring variation in pre-
pin reward learning work, in addition to OT, to regulate natal hormones has sometimes, although not consistently,
attach ments in both sexes. Research has yet to fully clarify related to the differing behavior patterns of girls and boys
the extent to which these neurochemical systems that regu- (Auyeung et al., 2009). In contrast, masculinized and
late culturally masculine and feminine behavior—that is, defeminized behaviors occur in girls with CAH disorder,
dominant, competitive behavior and nurturing, supportive which involves levels of prenatal androgen exposure that
behavior—function differently in men and women (Bateup are comparable to those of normal males (Cohen-Bendahan
et al., 2002; Broad et al., 2006). et al., 2005). CAH girls, more than normal girls, play with
construction and transportation toys, choose boys as play-
Organizing Influences of Hormones mates, and engage in physically aggressive, highly active,
The responsiveness of OT, T, and C to performance of rough-and-tumble play (Pasterski et al., 2007). In addition,
social roles explained in the prior section can be termed although CAH girls were found in one meta-analytical
activational effects. Also, gonadal hormones, especially review to perform better than normal girls on tests of men-
androgens, have organizational effects during early devel- tal rotation, an aspect of spatial ability (Puts, McDaniel,
opment and adolescence that produce enduring changes in Jordan, & Breedlove, 2008), a subsequent meta-analysis
the nervous system and brain structure (Cohen-Bendahan, of the same data revealed no significant difference (Hines,
van de Beek, & Berenbaum, 2005; Sisk & Zehr, 2005). 2009). Also, as adults, women with CAH are less likely to be
Exposure to androgens during these periods affects the heterosexual than are other women (e.g., Hines, Brook, &
developing brain in ways that masculinize or defeminize Conway, 2004). Such effects may be partially due to envi-
certain behaviors and establish differential receptivity to ronmental causes because the masculinization of CAH
particular socializing influences. Thus, organizational girls’ external genitalia plausibly alerts them and their par-
effects might modulate the interactions between neuro- ents to their atypical status. Nonetheless, parents of CAH
chemical and psychological processes described in the girls have been observed to encourage sex-typical toy play
prior section, with the result that men might be disposed in their CAH daughters, as well as in their other daughters
toward certain activities and women toward others. (Pasterski et al., 2005).
The prenatal development of males and females As we noted in the introduction to this chapter, exposure
diverges primarily because the testes of the developing to prenatal androgens influences children’s play preferences
XY fetus secrete T during gestation. The ovaries of the XX and activity levels and may thereby affect their receptivity
fetus produce minimal hormones, but the placenta exposes to some socializing efforts of parents and peers (Beaulieu &
both XY and XX fetuses to high levels of estrogen. Without Bugental, 2006). Fathers and male peers may respond
some exposure to T, development follows a female- to boys’ play in ways that promote learning of particular
typical course. Despite some limited evidence of direct, social skills (Pellis & Pellis, 2007), especially those associ-
nonhormonal genetic effects on male and female develop- ated with the agentic, dominant performance of masculine
ment (Arnold, 2004), the organizing effects that have been roles in adulthood (Pellegrini, 1995). These themes may
clearly established involve prenatal (and early postnatal) be elaborated further in fantasy play, which emerges by
exposure to androgens. Because androgen receptors are the age of 3. Compared with girls’ play about domestic
640 Gender

situations and close relationships, boys’ play more often as a standard against which to regulate their behavior
involves action adventures of pursuit and conquest (Leaper & (Wood, Christensen, Hebl, & Rothgerber, 1997). People
Friedman, 2006). With increasing maturity, girls continue who have a masculine self-concept involving traits of
to favor relational themes and boys to favor aggression and dominance and assertiveness might regulate their behavior
adventure themes, often in the context of video games by, for example, seeking opportunities for leadership. Self-
and sports. Through these experiences, boys and girls regulation proceeds in stages, beginning with testing the
develop distinctive expectations, preferences, and abilities extent to which current behavior matches self-standards
(Bussey & Bandura, 1999). (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 2008). Closer matches produce
In general, prenatal exposure to androgens biases the positive emotions and increased self-esteem, whereas mis-
biosocial processes that produce sex differences in some matches produce negative emotions and decreased esteem.
behaviors by increasing boys’ activity levels, preferences When signaled by negative feelings, people operate on
for active toys, and rough-and-tumble play. These factors their behavior to bring it more in line with the desired
may orient boys toward particular socialization experiences, standard. In this way, esteem and emotions constitute feed-
especially ones that involve vigorous pursuit and physical back about whether adjustments are necessary to meet
dominance contests. As a result of this socialization, standards.
men may develop personal identities that are relatively Illustrating such self-regulation, Wood and colleagues
agentic, and others may expect them to act in agentic ways. (1997) first assessed the strength of participants’ gender
Consequently, men may be especially suited to perform identities on items that evaluated, for example, the impor-
roles that require agency combined with brief bursts of tance of being similar to the ideal man or woman in society.
energy, strength, and speed. Then participants imagined acting in masculine (dominant
and assertive) or feminine (warm and communal) ways.
People who were strongly identified with their sex showed
INFLUENCE OF GENDER IDENTITIES a self-evaluation boost when their vicarious experience was
congruent with that identity—that is, dominant behavior
Gender roles influence people’s self-concepts and thereby for men and communal behavior for women. Additional
become gender identities—individuals’ sense of them- research extended these findings to everyday behavior by
selves as female or male. Gender identities arise because having participants keep diaries of their social interactions
most people accept, or internalize, at least some aspects for a week (Witt & Wood, in press). When men acted in
of cultural meanings associated with their sex—meanings masculine ways or women in feminine ways, those with
that in turn arise from the differing social roles of men and a stronger gender identity reported higher self-esteem and
women (Wood & Eagly, 2009). Gender identities thereby more positive feelings.
put the culture inside the person. According to gender schema theory (Bem, 1981), self-
People differ in the extent to which they incorporate regulation works not only through motivational signals of
gender roles into their self-concepts. These individual affect and self-esteem but also through enhanced attention,
differences have varying origins, including socialization processing, and recall of information relevant to gender
experiences, role occupancies (e.g., paid occupations), and standards. For example, those who are highly identified
early hormonal influences. Also, not everyone does mas- with culturally feminine warmth and concern for others
culine and feminine in the same way—people differ in the may especially attend to, process, and recall information
aspects of gender roles that they adopt. For example, men relevant to these qualities in themselves and others.
who regard themselves as masculine could be invested Following the logic of self-regulation, role congruity
in culturally masculine traits such as aggressiveness and theory (Diekman & Eagly, 2008) anticipates that men and
dominance or in masculine interests such as football women select into certain social roles because those roles
and hunting. Historically, most research on gender iden- afford pursuit of valued goals and thereby promote positive
tity has emphasized the agentic and communal person- outcomes and well-being (Evans & Diekman, 2009). For
ality traits established as the core of gender role beliefs example, because women on average place more impor-
(Bem, 1974; Spence & Helmreich, 1978). This approach tance than men on caregiving goals, including in fam-
illustrates personality psychologists’ search for traits that ily relationships and marriage (Cinamon & Rich, 2002),
directly represent male and female gender and that thereby they pursue communally demanding occupations such as
can account for individual differences in masculine and nurse and teacher (Evans & Diekman, 2009) and are more
feminine behaviors. involved in family roles (Abele, 2003).
Gender identities motivate responding through self- Also in line with self-regulation, the greater importance
regulatory processes. That is, people use their gender identity of close relationships for women renders their well-being
Influence of Gender Identities 641

especially sensitive to relationship quality. Thus, being traditional view of women as homemakers. Supporting
married, although beneficial for both sexes’ well-being, is this distinction, women with stronger collective identity
associated for women with greater emotional highs, as well expressed more sexist attitudes if this identity referred to
as greater lows (Wood, Rhodes, & Whelan, 1989). Physical traditional rather than progressive relations between the
health outcomes yield the same pattern: Both sexes benefit sexes (Becker & Wagner, 2009).
from marriage, but women show especially negative out- A collective identity as a man or women reflects
comes from marital distress (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, the classification of people into two categories, male
2001). Also, men have been found to value employment or female. Alternative collective gender identities also
roles more than women (Cinamon & Rich, 2002), espe- exist—for example, intersex, intergender, pangender, and
cially positions affording social status (Evans & Diekman, genderqueer—all of which refer to individuals whose gen-
2009), and extent of satisfaction with paid employment der identity is a combination of male and female or invokes
tends to be more important to men’s well-being (Aldous & a third sex. And transgender rejects the biological inevi-
Ganey, 1999). In these ways, the life roles that men and tability of being one sex or the other. Additional variants
women value influence their role pursuit and also influ- depart from normative heterosexuality, including gay, bear,
ence how much role-related outcomes impact happiness fag, lesbian, butch, femme, and bisexual. In some societ-
and life satisfaction. ies, certain of these alternative identities are not rare, such
With development, children learn to regulate their as the kathoey or ladyboy identity adopted by feminine
behavior according to their sense of themselves as female boys and men in Thailand, comprising about 10% of the
or male. For instance, in a study of children ranging from male population. Suggesting increasing acceptance, boys
2 to 4 years, only the older children anticipated feeling with this identity have been assigned transgender toilet
better about themselves after playing with toys typical of facilities in some secondary schools (Head, 2008).
their own sex (Bussey & Bandura, 1992). Furthermore, The most popular measures of gender identity assess
these older children’s anticipatory affective reactions pre- not collective identification with a gender group but rather
dicted their subsequent toy choices. beliefs about self attributes, in particular the agentic and
communal personality traits that reflect the main compo-
nents of gender stereotypes. Specifically, personality traits
Types of Gender Identity
more stereotypical of one sex than the other and more
The most basic type of gender identity, ordinarily found favorably evaluated in that sex constitute the items of
in children as young as 2 years, is experiencing oneself the Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI; Bem, 1974) and
as male or female (Kohlberg, 1966; Ruble, Martin, & Spence and Helmreich’s (1978) closely related Personal
Berenbaum, 2006). With this realization, young children Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ). These measures yield
also tend to prefer their own sex. Because collective one scale of self-reported feminine, communal traits (e.g.,
identities link people to their various roles and groups, warm and gentle) and another of self-reported mascu-
maturation brings individuals multiple identities, based on, line, agentic traits (e.g., aggressive and self-confident).
for example, family status, occupation, religion, race and With these two dimensions, it is possible to represent a
ethnicity, and sexual orientation (Stewart & McDermott, masculine identity of high masculinity and low femininity,
2004). Gender is not necessarily the most important of a feminine identity of high femininity and low masculinity,
these identities (Smith, 2007). Among schoolchildren, and identities with similar levels of masculinity and
for example, gender identity is more important than eth- femininity (Bem, 1974).
nic identity for members of the majority ethnicity, but the Agentic and communal identities develop relatively
two identities are equally important for minority ethnicities slowly in children. Preschool children self-attribute
(Turner & Brown, 2007). primarily favorable characteristics, not gender stereotypi-
Adults experience themselves as male or female when cal traits (e.g., Aubry, Ruble, & Silverman, 1999; Cowan &
they align themselves psychologically with their own Hoffman, 1986). By middle childhood, most children
sex, as in, “I identify with women/men.” These collective ascribe gender-stereotypical personality traits to them-
social identities can reflect what is normative for gender selves, and this tendency grows stronger in adolescence
groups in either a descriptive sense (“I am a typical guy”; (Ruble et al., 2006).
Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992) or a prescriptive sense (“I am Despite psychologists’ reliance on the BSRI and PAQ,
an ideal guy”; Wood et al., 1997). Also, given women’s people ascribe gender-stereotypical attributes to themselves
changing roles in postindustrial societies, their identities on dimensions other than agency and communion. For
may encompass a progressive view of women as having example, people also possess a gender identity based on sex-
careers and sharing domestic work with men or with a more differentiated vocations and interests (Lippa, 2001, 2005).
642 Gender

Additional facets of gender identity reflect investment interests predict the differing occupational preferences
in an individual versus social sense of self. Cross and of women and men (Lippa, 2005). Feminine relational
Madson (1997) built on the cultural dimensions of individ- measures assessing one’s feelings of interdependence with
ualism and collectivism to define masculine identity as an close others predict attention to and valuing of close rela-
independent sense of self highlighting individuals’ unique tionships (e.g., Gabriel & Gardner, 1999; Gore, Cross, &
attributes and feminine identity as an interdependent sense Morris, 2006). The logic of compatibility follows from
of self highlighting relationships and group memberships. self-regulation: People with a strong gender identity in
However, other researchers parsed the interdependence one domain regulate their behavior in that domain. Thus,
dimension into a masculine focus on oneself within teams people who define themselves as highly agentic act in dom-
and organizations and a feminine focus on oneself within inant, assertive ways, whereas those who define themselves
relationships with close others such as friends and family as highly interdependent bond with significant others.
(Baumeister & Sommer, 1997; Gardner & Gabriel, 2004). In general, individuals regulate their behavior in line
These two aspects of interdependence link identities to with their gender identities, whether these are based on
the differing role occupancies of men and women, collective male and female groups, gender-stereotypical
whereby more men than women strive for status within traits, or relational closeness to others. Through self-
organizations and collectives and more women than men regulatory mechanisms, people enact these personally
invest in close relationships through their caring activities defining gender roles as they carry out such everyday roles
in families and other relationships. as parent and employee. Regulation of behavior by gen-
Group identities can emerge and fade, depending on der identities is one facet of our biosocial model of sex
the context (Sinclair, Hardin, & Lowery, 2006), although and gender. This mechanism works in conjunction with
psychologists typically have treated these identities as hormonal processes and neurochemical mechanisms asso-
chronic self-attributes. Identities shift with the specifics of ciated with reward to enable successful role performance.
the local contexts in which people interact (Burke, 2004; In line with this analysis, women high in masculinity on the
Tajfel, 1978). For example, gender identity can become BSRI, who perceived themselves as self-directed, action
salient through being a solo representative of one’s sex oriented, and resourceful, were likely to have higher circu-
in a mixed-sex group (e.g., Sekaquaptewa & Thompson, lating T (Baucom, Besch, & Callahan, 1985). Given that T
2002). Also, gender identity varies in strength depending is recruited in the service of role performance, this pattern
on features of the situation such as the sex of an interaction suggests that agentic women are sensitive to dominance
partner (e.g., Leszczynski & Strough, 2008). issues in daily life and recruit T as they assert domi-
nance. Other neurochemicals associated with reward also
might be implicated in the performance of gender-typical
Predicting Behavior From Gender Identity
behavior, including dopamine reactivity in the brain that
Measures of gender identity are useful for research to the underlies the learning of preferences (Schultz, 2006).
extent that they predict relevant behaviors. Often they do not
predict behavior—and they should not be expected to do so.
Gender identity measures, like personality trait and attitude INFLUENCE OF SOCIAL EXPECTATIONS
measures, predict behavior successfully when the content of
the behavioral measure is compatible with the content of the Gender roles permeate social expectations, as well as indi-
predictive measure (Ajzen, 2005; Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). viduals’ sense of themselves as male or female. Simply put,
Consistent with this compatibility principle, the strength we expect men to act in masculine ways and fill male-
of one’s collective identification with men or women pre- typical roles (e.g., primary family provider) and women to
dicts not all culturally masculine and feminine behaviors act in feminine ways and to fill female-typical roles (e.g.,
but instead group-related phenomena such as a preference primary caretaker of children). Gender role expectations
for one’s own sex and self-stereotyping on gender stereo- influence behavior through their social consequences.
typical qualities (Wood & Eagly, 2009). Measures based on Conformity to gender expectations usually garners
agency and communion self-ratings, the BSRI and PAQ, are social rewards, and nonconformity usually garners fewer
not general-purpose predictors of all sex-related behaviors rewards and even social rejection. Social expectations
(e.g., interest in fashion). Instead, these scales predict spe- shaped by gender thereby influence social interaction.
cifically whether people will act in agentic or communal Social constructionists refer to this process as doing
ways (Spence & Buckner, 2000; Taylor & Hall, 1982). gender, as people recurrently produce behaviors stereo-
Other identity measures also predict compatible behaviors. typical of their sex as they interact with others (West &
Gender identities involving vocational and leisure-time Zimmerman, 1987).
Influence of Social Expectations 643

Consensual gender role expectations exert influence not Keltner, & Kring, 2001), and modest and unassuming men
only through the beliefs and actions of specific interaction are viewed as insufficiently competent for leadership roles
partners but also through individuals’ expectations about (Rudman, 1998; Rudman & Glick, 2001). Nonetheless,
the beliefs and probable reactions of others who are not sometimes the benefits of gender nonconformity outweigh
present (Allport, 1954). Such expectations are not neces- its social costs, motivating people to act in ways that coun-
sarily explicit but often implicit and influential largely ter gender stereotypes, such as when women anticipate gen-
outside of awareness. Also, interaction partners often are der prejudice (e.g., Kaiser & Miller, 2001).
unable or unwilling to acknowledge their gender-based People often attempt to deflect negative responses to
expectations and instead communicate them in subtle ways their nonconforming behaviors by reclaiming a conven-
(e.g., falling silent or standing farther away). tional gender identity. For example, men and women who
believed that they had performed well on a task typical
of the other sex attempted to hide their success from oth-
Social Expectations Promote
ers, falsely claimed success on a task typical of their own
Behavioral Confirmation
sex, and expressed greater interest in same-sex activities
People generally approve of others who conform to gender (Rudman & Fairchild, 2004). These mitigating strategies
roles and penalize others who counter them. This approval were stronger among those who reported expecting repri-
is directed even toward young children, whose parents sals from others for their gender nonconformity. Also, men
tend to encourage activities and toys that are typical for experiencing discomfort from performing the feminine
children’s sex (Lytton & Romney, 1991; Pasterski et al., task of braiding hair successfully reduced this discomfort
2005). This is not to say that people favor hyperfeminized by publicly claiming a conventional sexual orientation
women and hypermacho men. But conformity to gender (“I am heterosexual,” e.g., Bosson, Prewitt-Freilino, &
roles garners rewards because it validates shared beliefs Taylor, 2005). Some evidence also suggests that men’s
about women and men and promotes social interaction that gender identity (i.e., their collective identity or “man-
is easy to follow and understand. hood”) requires continual social proof and thus is more
Positive and negative sanctions for gender conformity easily threatened than women’s identity (or “womanhood”;
and deviation are evident in the prevalence of approving, Vandello, Bosson, Cohen, Burnaford, & Weaver, 2008).
benevolent beliefs about women who conform to tradi- Instilling conformity to gender norms does not require
tional gender roles and of disapproving, hostile beliefs overt rewards and punishments. Instead, the explicit commu-
about those who violate them (Glick & Fiske, 2001). On nication of stereotypical expectations by influential others
individual difference measures, hostile and benevolent can be sufficient. In a classic demonstration, female stu-
beliefs were correlated—that is, they were two sides of the dents shaped their self-presentations to fit the preferences
same coin. Thus, the people who endorsed negative beliefs of a highly eligible male interaction partner (Zanna & Pack,
about nontraditional women (labeled hostile sexists by 1975). When this man reported preferring women who were
Glick and Fiske, 2001) tended to be the same people who traditional (vs. nontraditional), these young women pre-
endorsed positive beliefs about traditional women (labeled sented themselves as conforming to his preferences and fur-
benevolent sexists). thermore performed worse on a test of intellectual aptitude
People commonly express hostility to various coun- that was to be shared with this male partner. This stereotype
terstereotypical behaviors. Children disapprove of peers’ confirmation is not surprising given that these female stu-
violations of gender norms concerning clothing, hair- dents anticipated meeting a desirable man who had explic-
styles, and styles of play (e.g., Blakemore, 2003). Adults itly stated his preferences (see the conceptual replication
react similarly in more mature domains. For example, in with male participants in Morier & Seroy, 1994).
small-group interaction, women who behave in a dominant In the standard account, such behavioral confirmations
or extremely competent manner tend to lose likability and emerge through social perceivers forming expectancies
influence (Carli, 2001; Shackelford, Wood, & Worchel, about an individual target based on gender stereotypes and
1996). Women in supervisory roles may be penalized then behaving toward that individual as if the stereotypical
for failing to attend to others’ emotions or for expressing beliefs were true (Olson, Roese, & Zanna, 1996). The target
angry emotions (Brescoll & Uhlmann, 2008; Byron, 2007), person responds so as to confirm the gender stereotype, and
as well as for performing at outstanding levels in stereo- the perceiver interprets the target’s behavior in line with the
typically masculine roles (Heilman, Wallen, Fuchs, & expectancy. The perceiver then encodes yet another
Tamkins, 2004). In contrast, men, more than women, tend instance of stereotype-consistent behavior and thereby
to lose social status for behaving passively, unassert- strengthens gender role expectations. Although the link
ively, anxiously, and negatively (e.g., Anderson, John, between expectancies and behavior is contingent on various
644 Gender

conditions (Olson et al., 1996), the behavioral confirmation involving, for example, mechanics, math, and leadership
of gender-based expectations through this sequence serves and women to excel at feminine tasks involving social
to maintain, propagate, and justify people’s stereotypes sensitivity, sewing, and emotional intelligence. When
about women and men. one of these abilities is evaluated, activating expectations
Even without the explicit statement of gender norms, about the inferior competence of one sex can impair their
people often conform to interaction partners’ presumed performance. This phenomenon is called stereotype threat
gender stereotypical expectancies (see review by Geis, (Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995).
1993). For example, in an experiment by Skrypnek and Negative performance stereotypes can be activated in
Snyder (1982), task partners negotiated a more traditional various ways immediately before a test or competition.
division of labor when they believed that their (unseen) For example, performance decrements occurred when par-
partner was of the other sex, regardless of their partner ’s ticipants in a laboratory experiment were told that one sex
actual sex. In addition, lack of awareness of certain stages excelled at the task in the past (e.g., Johns, Schmader, &
in the confirmation process enhances such effects. If per- Martens, 2005), were presented as the solo member of their
ceivers were aware that they caused another ’s gender ste- sex in a competition (e.g., Ben-Zeev, Fein, & Inzlicht, 2005),
reotypical behavior, they would not attribute the behavior or were exposed to stereotypical media portrayals of their
to that person’s disposition or conclude support for gender sex (e.g., Davies, Spencer, Quinn, & Gerhardstein, 2002).
stereotypes. Also, if targets were aware of a perceiver ’s Activated gender stereotypes impair performance when
stereotypical expectations, they might act to counter individuals become anxious about confirming the negative
instead of fulfill them (Miller & Turnbull, 1986). stereotype in others’ eyes or in their own. This anxiety can
Consistent with current understanding of behavioral result in impairments in working memory (Schmader & Johns,
confirmation, these effects do not require and may even be 2003), mental intrusions (Dardenne, Dumont, & Bollier,
impaired by the explicit communication of expectations. In 2007), physiological stress responses (Murphy, Steele, &
the new generation of confirmation research, this insight is Gross, 2007), and perhaps depletion of self-control strength
shrinking the importance of explicit expectations in produc- (Inzlicht, McKay, & Aronson, 2006). People feel anxious
ing behavioral confirmation. In one study that eliminated because their self-integrity is threatened by the simultaneous
the necessity for perceivers’ conscious expectations, prim- activation of three conflicting beliefs (Schmader, Johns, &
ing men to think of women as sexual objects made them Forbes, 2008): (a) the group stereotype of inferior ability (e.g.,
more likely to treat an individual woman applicant in a sex- women cannot read maps), (b) personal identification with
ual way during a mock job interview (Rudman & Borgida, the group (e.g., I am a woman), and (c) knowledge of one’s
1995). If women respond in stereotypical ways, then the own ability (e.g., I am good at map reading). Because these
perceivers’ initial impressions are supported (see Chen & beliefs are imbalanced only when people initially believe in
Bargh, 1997). Eliminating the necessity for perceiver expec- their own ability, stereotype threat is experienced most often
tations altogether, subtle cues such as nonverbal behavioral by people who are highly identified in a counterstereotypic
mimicry from an interaction partner could yield conformity domain, including, for example, women who view them-
to gender stereotypes (Leander, Chartrand, & Wood, 2009). selves as mathematically talented.
Specifically, following mimicry by an interaction partner, Stereotype threat often produces a decrement in test
participants apparently increased their desire to affiliate and performance in the unfavorably stereotyped sex. For
therefore enhanced their conformity to gender stereotypes example, women’s math test performance is sensitive to
that presumably were shared with their partner. Thus, mim- negative ability stereotypes (e.g., Spencer, Steele, & Quinn,
icked women performed worse on a math test. These find- 1999; Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002), and women’s will-
ings suggest that people’s own imaginings about interaction ingness to lead appears to be lessened by stereotype threat
partners’ and others’ expectations are sufficient to promote (Davies, Spencer, & Steele, 2005). In mirror fashion, when
stereotype-consistent behavior. gender stereotypes are salient, men’s performance falters
at tasks involving social sensitivity (Koenig & Eagly,
2005), emotional intelligence (Keller & Bless, 2005), and
Stereotype Threat
affective information processing (Leyens, Désert, Croizet, &
Gender expectations also influence behavior when they Darcis, 2000). In addition, the sex that is stereotyped as
are simply “in the air” and not held by any specific inter- superior in an ability can experience improved perfor-
action partner or social audience. Because gender stereo- mance, or stereotype lift, from downward comparison with
types specify task abilities, they can establish performance the less able group. Yet, this lift typically is weaker than the
expectations in culturally masculine or feminine domains. decline in performance experienced by the unfavorably
Men are expected to have advantage at masculine tasks stereotyped sex (Walton & Cohen, 2003).
Empirical Evidence for Sex Differences and Similarities 645

Performance does not always falter when the lesser ability variability of sex differences in many aspects of social
of one’s group is made salient. It remains unclear whether the behavior. Aggregating across all available meta-analyses
mild forms of stereotype threat inherent in asking students that had compared female and male social behavior,
to note their sex on high-stakes tests such as the SAT rou- Richard, Bond, and Stokes-Zoota (2003) concluded that
tinely affect performance (Cullen, Waters, & Sackett, 2006; sex differences were somewhat smaller (r  .13) than
Stricker & Ward, 2004). Also, a counterreaction of enhanced effects averaged across the entire field of social psychology
performance can emerge among threatened individuals (r  .22). Yet, the average magnitude of the sex effects was
who are especially confident in their own ability (Hoyt & comparable to the effects in several foundational research
Blascovich, 2007). And when individuals have more than one areas in social psychology, including attribution (r  .14)
identity relevant to a domain, as with Asian women and math and social influence (r  .13). Similarly, Hyde (2005,
aptitude, performance depends on which identity is salient— 2007) aggregated 128 meta-analytical effects representing
Asian identity enhanced math performance, but female iden- sex differences and similarities in personality, social, and
tity reduced it (Shih, Pittinsky, & Ambady, 1999). cognitive psychology. While noting the importance of con-
In general, people conform to gender role expectations textual variation, Hyde highlighted the evidence for simi-
that are explicitly communicated, just implied or expected, or larities between women and men. Specifically, she termed
merely floating in the air. Resistance is possible but unlikely almost half of the meta-analytical results “small” effects,
when these expectations hide out below the level of conscious notwithstanding the larger sex differences that emerged
awareness. Others’ expectations work to promote role perfor- with motor performance, sexuality, and aggression.
mance in conjunction with self identities and with the hor- Are sex differences small? Even if they are, small does
monal processes and neurochemical mechanisms associated not mean unimportant. Small effects can have substantial
with reward. Thus, in our three-way biosocial model empha- impact when they reliably characterize behavior across
sizing others’ expectations, gender identity, and hormonal time and experiences (e.g., Abelson, 1985). But questions
processes, a woman holding an infant might be responding about effect size are not answered effectively by aggre-
to others’ expectations and to her own identity as a nurturer. gating results across many meta-analyses. Such superag-
Such close contact can activate OT and neurochemicals of gregations of sex comparisons are not theory driven and
reward that further promote attachment (Taylor, 2002). thus do not distinguish between the behavioral domains
that theoretically should versus should not yield sex differ-
ences. Moreover, each individual meta-analysis typically
EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE FOR SEX aggregates findings across a broad behavioral category,
DIFFERENCES AND SIMILARITIES often collapsing across contexts and behaviors for which
theories hold that sex differences are more or less likely.
At the beginning of this chapter, we claimed that a Martian The standard result in individual quantitative synthe-
landing anywhere on earth would puzzle over why men and ses comparing women and men is that some studies yield
boys engage in different activities than women and girls. large sex differences, most yield smaller sex differences,
That is, sex differences in everyday activities are large and a few yield reversals of the overall tendency, just as for
enough to be readily apparent without the aid of statisti- other phenomena in social and personality psychology. The
cal analyses or controlled research designs. Any observer principle that virtually all psychological phenomena vary
of humans’ daily lives would be struck also by variability across settings, methods, and participant attributes has been
in sex differences across contexts. These patterns of vari- labeled contextualism by McGuire (1983) and other meth-
ability depend partly on how male and female reproductive odologists, who argue that this is the very patterning theo-
and physical attributes facilitate or impair performance at rists of social behavior are obliged to address. Researchers
specific life tasks that emerge within a given society. Thus, can focus on the rich tapestry of difference and similar-
in a society that engages in warfare, men might display ity and build their theories to address this complexity. The
marked physical aggression on the battlefield but much alternative is for researchers to bury within aggregates
less aggression in friendships and family relationships. the striking sex differences that people recognize in daily life
as characteristic of male or female behavior. To illustrate these
issues, we consider the bad and the good of social behavior—
Sex Differences in Psychological Research
that is, aggressive behavior and prosocial behavior.
Many studies that have been conducted include compari-
Aggressive Behavior
sons of female and male behavior; thus, sex differences
were an early and continuing target of meta-analytical In view of the association of agency with men, it is not
integrations. Quantitative syntheses estimate the size and surprising that people ordinarily ascribe aggressiveness
646 Gender

more to men than to women (e.g., Williams & Best, 1990). somewhat more aggressive than men, presumably because
Aggression, as behavior intended to harm others, might such behaviors violated social norms about behaving
seem less related to communion. Nevertheless, the ascrip- politely toward women (Bettencourt & Miller, 1996).
tion to women of passive–aggressive traits such as whiny, Consistent with men’s greater physical strength, the more
complaining, and nagging suggests forms of female aggres- that female (vs. male) judges perceived acts likely to cause
sion (Spence et al., 1979; Williams & Best, 1990), espe- harm to others and danger to themselves, the larger the sex
cially relational acts that can wreak psychological harm. difference in the male direction in the original research
As expected given the biosocial constraints of men’s (see also Bettencourt & Miller, 1996). Women are thus not
greater size and strength, physical aggression produced expected to confront others physically in clearly dangerous
the largest sex differences favoring men, with meta- situations.
analytical effects as large as r  .41 (Knight, Fabes, & In summary, although aggression is often in the male
Higgins, 1996; see summary in Hyde, 2005). Research with domain, the equation between men and aggressiveness is
children and adolescents yielded a similar effect favoring most valid for physical aggression. It is illustrated dramati-
boys (r  .34; Card, Stucky, Sawalani, & Little, 2008). cally in extreme forms of damaging violence. However,
Much larger sex differences are found with extreme forms women and girls can sometimes be more indirectly aggres-
of real world aggression, as reflected in crime statistics that sive than men and boys. Also, women’s aggressiveness is
more men than women commit murders (r  .93), all kinds likely to equal or exceed men’s when others violate social
of violent crime (r  .81), and property crime (r  .56; norms by being mean to women or when gender norms are
U.S. Federal Bureau of Investigation, 2008). nullified by anonymity.
In contrast to findings involving physical aggression,
Prosocial Behavior
direct verbal aggression generally produces smaller sex
differences in the male direction (e.g., Eagly & Steffen, Consistent with gender role beliefs, sex differences in
1986). Also, indirect forms of aggression that involve, prosocial behavior, ordinarily defined by psychologists
for example, gossiping and spreading damaging rumors as acts intended to help others, depend on whether such
produce small meta-analytical differences in the female acts invoke communion or agency. The female gender
direction (Archer & Coyne, 2005; Card et al., 2008). In role’s demand for communal behavior fosters acts of caring
addition, in interactions between heterosexual intimate for others and tending to their individual needs, primarily in
partners, meta-analytical data show that physically damag- close relationships. The male gender role’s demand for agen-
ing aggression is male dominated but everyday, minor acts tic behavior can foster some forms of prosocial behavior,
of physical aggression (e.g., slapping) are slightly female especially physically challenging acts of rescuing and the
dominated (Archer, 2000). chivalrous protection of dependent others (see Eagly &
The logic behind this variability lies in culturally shared Koenig, 2006, for review; Eagly, in press).
gender role beliefs. At the descriptive level, these beliefs This variability across types of prosocial behaviors is
accurately track the variability in findings across studies. mirrored in gender role beliefs. In a meta-analysis inte-
Specifically, in Eagly and Steffen’s (1986) meta-analysis, grating studies of varied helping behaviors (Eagly &
judges contemplated the aggressive acts examined in Crowley, 1986), judges’ estimates of the likelihood that
each of the studies in the review. As expected, the more women versus men would engage in the behaviors thus
likely these judges thought it would be for typical men accurately tracked the sex differences obtained in the
(vs. women) to engage in a behavior, the larger was the studies.
behavioral sex difference in the meta-analyzed studies. Culturally feminine prosocial behavior includes com-
Thus, beliefs about men and women predicted the size of munally caring for and supporting others. In the United
the actual sex differences in aggressive acts. States, for example, women comprise approximately 75%
Because gender role beliefs are prescriptive, as well as of caregivers for older family members and friends and
descriptive, people are attentive to others’ expectations about approximately 63% of grandparents living with and caring
their aggressive behavior. For example, the usual tendency for grandchildren (U.S. Health Resources and Services
for men to aggress more than women in a game situation Administration, 2005). Consistent with these findings, the
disappeared when participants were deindividuated— moral reasoning of women (vs. men) is based somewhat
that is, made anonymous to one another, thus nullifying the more on caring and responsibility to others (r  .14; Jaffee &
effects of others’ expectations (Lightdale & Prentice, 1994). Hyde, 2000). Emotional support of others is facilitated by
Also, in meta-analytical data, provocation of research partici- the greater emotional expressiveness of women than men,
pants reduced the sex difference in aggression. When con- especially their more frequent and intense expressions of
fronted by insults or negative evaluations, women became joy, love, fear, and happiness (Grossman & Wood, 1993).
Empirical Evidence for Sex Differences and Similarities 647

Also, in a meta-analysis of personality research, women’s Crowley’s (1986) review, men were more helpful in the
(vs. men’s) self-ratings indicated more tender-minded and original research to the extent that women perceived help-
nurturant personalities (r  .35; Feingold, 1994). ing as more dangerous than did men or that masculine skills
Research has documented similar findings with chil- were required (e.g., changing a tire). Consistent with these
dren. In Eisenberg and Fabes’s (1998) meta-analysis of pro- findings, the predominance of men among helpers is espe-
social behavior, girls were slightly more helpful than boys cially large in the extremely dangerous forms of help-
overall (r  .09) but more so when helping expressed ing that yield Carnegie Hero Medals (r  .82; Becker &
kindness and consideration (r  .21). In close relation- Eagly, 2004). These awards recognize public acts of
ships among adults, women generally provide more sensi- extremely risky prosocial behavior, such as saving people
tive emotional support (see review by Burleson & Kunkel, from fires, drownings, attacks by animals, and assaults by
2006). This pattern extends to same-sex and other-sex criminals. However, in a different type of extremely danger-
friendships (e.g., Rose & Rudolph, 2006) and to marital ous situation—the rescuing of Jews during the holocaust—
relationships (e.g., Cutrona, 1996), especially in women’s women helped as often as men. In addition, women were
provision of emotional support to their spouse when it is represented somewhat more often than men as donators of
most needed (Neff & Karney, 2005). living kidneys, volunteers for the Peace Corps, and medical
Women’s caring and emotional support should be volunteers in dangerous settings. These prosocial actions
enhanced by their tendencies to manifest empathy and that were not male dominated, especially holocaust rescuing,
sympathy and to be sensitive to subtle cues conveying oth- entailed risk but rarely required highly strength-intensive
ers’ emotional states. Meta-analyses of empathy and sym- actions that can result in Carnegie Medals (Becker & Eagly,
pathy have favored girls and women, with developmental 2004). In addition, most of these actions likely involved a
trends showing an increase in this sex difference with age mix of agentic and communal behaviors.
(Eisenberg, Fabes, & Spinrad, 2006). Meta-analyses exam-
Conclusions About Sex Differences and Similarities
ining various forms of nonverbal sensitivity also have gener-
ally favored women and girls (Hall, 2006). Both aggression and prosocial behavior are highly varied
Studies of helping behavior in social psychology have domains that can yield a range of sex difference findings.
not usually addressed caring behavior in close relationships. As evident from the preceding brief review, researchers’
Instead, most studies examined brief encounters between claims about difference or similarity depend on the level at
strangers, often in field experiments conducted in natural which they choose to aggregate their data. Simple aggre-
settings (see meta-analysis by Eagly & Crowley, 1986). gation of either aggressive or prosocial behavior overall
Given that most of these studies involved bystander inter- or many of its manifestations can suggest only relatively
ventions or polite, chivalrous behaviors, it is not surpris- small differences and no consistent pattern. However,
ing that men helped somewhat more than women in these framing expectations for differences in terms of gender
studies (r  .17). The power of social norms to induce this roles highlights the conditions under which similarity or
type of masculine behavior is revealed in the substantial difference is more likely. With this simple understanding,
sex difference in the male direction when onlookers were psychology researchers can be as accurate as everyday per-
present (r  .37), compared with its absence when the ceivers, whose descriptive knowledge of gender roles accu-
potential helpers were alone (r  .01). rately tracks the direction and magnitude of sex differences
Helping in these social psychological studies did not both across different categories of behaviors (e.g., Hall &
always require assertive intervention (Eagly & Crowley, Carter, 1999a) and across specific instances of behaviors
1986). To identify behaviors requiring an active, agentic within such categories (Eagly & Crowley, 1986; Eagly &
approach, this meta-analysis separated the studies accord- Steffen, 1986).
ing to whether a need merely presented itself to partici- Reaching beyond simple observations of difference
pants (e.g., observing that someone is ill or endangered) or and similarity, social role theory adds the proposition that
an explicit request to help was directed to them (e.g., ask- the size and direction of sex differences in aggressive and
ing for a charity contribution). Consistent with the agentic prosocial behaviors depend partly on whether the behaviors
theme of the male gender role, men were especially more require agentic attributes associated with masculinity or
helpful than women when the need was merely presented communal attributes associated with femininity. Prosocial
and the helper therefore had to take the initiative to offer behavior seems to be the more variable domain in terms of
aid (r  .28), compared with when a request was made clearly encompassing both communal and agentic behav-
explicitly (r  .04). iors. Whether the differences implied by gender roles are
Parallel to findings on aggressive behavior, when manifested in behavior also depends on features of the sit-
independent judges evaluated the studies in Eagly and uation (e.g., the presence of onlookers) and of individuals
648 Gender

(e.g., their gender identities, Cohn & Zeichner, 2006). In such conflicts because effective soldiering is believed to
addition, male physical prowess yields male advantage require possession of masculine and rejection of feminine
in both aggression and prosocial behavior when actions attributes (Biernat, Crandall, Young, Kobrynowicz, &
require physical strength or threaten physical retaliation. Halpin, 1998; Boldry, Wood, & Kashy, 2001).
Thus, men tend to be heroic helpers in emergencies and Inconsistencies between gender roles and workplace
with violent criminals’ and women are inclined to be care- roles can produce prejudice and discrimination, which
takers of children and elderly relatives and sensitive sup- has been studied most extensively in relation to women in
porters of spouses and friends. leader and manager roles (Eagly & Carli, 2007; Heilman,
Although the pattern of meta-analytical findings is gen- 2001). People commonly believe that managers and other
erally consistent with a gender role account, most of the leaders are endowed with masculine agentic qualities of
research has not directly identified the proximal processes ambition, confidence, self-sufficiency, and dominance
that produce sex differences in aggression or prosocial and less endowed with feminine communal qualities (e.g.,
behavior. From our perspective, the direct precursors of Powell, Butterfield, & Parent, 2002). This “think manager–
these sex differences are likely to be gendered self-concepts, think male” effect (Schein, 2001) is robust, despite some
others’ expectations, and hormonal processes, with T being recent weakening (e.g., Duehr & Bono, 2006). The incon-
especially relevant to aggressive behavior and assertive pro- gruity between beliefs about what it means to be a good
social interventions and OT pertinent to nurturing and caring leader and what it means to be female (e.g., Eagly &
forms of prosocial behavior. Karau, 2002) can generate the perception that women do
not have what it takes to lead. The incompatible beliefs
place female leaders in a dilemma—a double bind (Eagly &
Sex Differences in Organizational Settings
Carli, 2007): Communal female leaders may be criti-
In typical organizational settings, people contend with cized for not being agentic enough and not properly tak-
their job roles along with their gender roles. Illustrating the ing charge, and agentic female leaders may be criticized
influence of these roles is a study that sampled Canadian for lacking communion and not being nice enough (e.g.,
employees’ agentic and communal behaviors in their Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2004; Rudman & Glick, 2001).
workplaces (Moskowitz, Suh, & Desaulniers, 1994). Illustrating the double bind, a meta-analysis of experi-
Demonstrating the influence of job roles, employees of ments that varied the sex of leaders while holding constant
both sexes behaved most agentically when interacting with their other attributes showed stronger prejudice against
a subordinate and least agentically when interacting with a women leaders when they managed others in stereotypi-
superior. Also, demonstrating the influence of gender cally masculine ways (Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992).
roles, women, regardless of their workplace status, deliv- Thus, a male manager who acts in a forceful or assertive
ered more communal behaviors, such as friendly, unself- manner is perceived as behaving appropriately, whereas a
ish, and expressive acts, especially when interacting with female leader who behaves in exactly the same way may
other women. Similarly, meta-analyses of research on be considered unacceptably pushy. To cope with the dou-
physicians’ interactions with their patients yielded effects ble bind, women managers might offer a blend of mascu-
reflecting both gender roles and job roles. Specifically, line and feminine behaviors (Eagly & Carli, 2007). This
female physicians, although the same as male physicians style has proven effective at enhancing women’s influence
in providing medical information, displayed more com- in small, mixed-sex discussion groups (Shackelford et al.,
munal behaviors than the men, including more positive 1996). In general, women entering engineering and other
talk, psychosocial counseling, emotion-focused talk, and traditionally male fields cope through various mecha-
nodding and smiling (Roter, Hall, & Aoki, 2002). It thus nisms, such as garnering social support from family and
appears that agentically demanding supervisory and physi- friends outside of work settings (Richman, van Dellen, &
cian roles are sufficiently flexible to allow women to enact Wood, in press).
them while displaying communal behavior consistent with
gender role norms.
Despite the apparent flexibility of many occupa- MALE AND FEMALE SOCIAL ROLES ARE
tional roles, conflicts between demands of gender and ROOTED IN A BIOSOCIAL REALITY
workplace roles can pose challenges. Such conflicts are not
marked for female nurses, male truck drivers, and others Gender roles are not arbitrary or random. Instead, they are
employed in sex-typical occupations. However, conflicts are firmly rooted in a society’s division of labor and the social
more common for people in job roles dominated by the other roles filled by men and women. Up to this point, the chapter
sex. For example, in military settings, women experience considered how this division shapes the proximal, immediate
Male and Female Social Roles Are Rooted in a Biosocial Reality 649

causes of sex differences and similarities. In brief, from tasks, especially those that require speed, uninterrupted
observation of women’s and men’s activities, people infer periods of activity and training, or long-distance travel away
the attributes of each sex. These gender role beliefs in turn from home. Yet, reproductive activities have less impact on
influence behavior through the trio of proximal causes that women’s roles in societies with low birthrates, much less
we considered, including hormonal changes, self-regulation reliance on lactation for feeding infants and young children,
of identities, and social expectations. But what are the and more nonmaternal care of young children. These condi-
origins of the human division of labor? In the nomological tions hold in postindustrial societies.
network of gender constructs, this question addresses the Men’s larger size and greater upper-body strength and
distal, evolutionary causes of male and female behavior. speed also shape the division of labor. In addition, social-
The origins of the division of labor can be traced partly ization channels boys’ masculinized play preferences and
to humans’ evolved capacities to invent new solutions to high activity levels to hone males’ skills in physically
adaptive problems and to share these innovations through intensive activities. Because of these intrinsic differences
complex forms of social learning that include teaching, in size, strength, speed, and activity level, the average
imitation, and conformity. With these capacities, cultural man is more likely than the average woman to perform
knowledge, including beliefs about female and male roles, efficiently tasks that demand brief bursts of force and upper-
cumulates with modifications across generations and cul- body strength. In foraging, horticultural, and agricultural
tures. Humans’ extended juvenile period further favors the societies, these tasks include hunting large animals, plow-
transmission of cultural knowledge. Children engage in ing, and conducting warfare (Murdock & Provost, 1973).
exploratory play and practice female and male roles, and Nonetheless, some tasks usually performed by women
societies socialize boys and girls by encouraging skills require considerable strength, including fetching water,
and preferences suited to the prevailing division of labor. carrying children, and doing laundry (Mukhopadhyay &
Variability in the roles of women and men occurs within Higgins, 1988). Whatever the advantages are of men’s
the framework of the sexes’ physical and reproductive ability to execute highly strength-intensive tasks, these
attributes (Wood & Eagly, 2002). Specifically, women attributes have less effect on role performance in postin-
bear and nurse infants, and men have greater size, upper- dustrial and other societies in which few occupational roles
body strength, and speed. These attributes organize behav- demand these attributes.
ioral and psychological sex differences and similarities Were men’s size and strength sculpted by sexual selec-
across societies. The specific pattern of female and male tion pressures? Perhaps ancestral males who were larger,
behavior in a society emerges from the biosocial interac- stronger, and more aggressive had better fitness outcomes
tion between socioeconomic and cultural factors and the because they were able to compete with other males for
sexes’ physical and reproductive attributes. This interaction access to many mates. Some researchers have argued that
influences behavior because some activities are more effi- sexual selection pressures organized human psychology
ciently accomplished by one sex than the other, depending and physical attributes in these ways (Kenrick, Maner, &
on societal conditions. Although these differences in Li, 2005; see Neuberg, Kenrick, & Schaller, volume 2).
physical characteristics and reproductive activities do not However, comparative research with primates suggests
apply to all men or women (e.g., some women are taller or that differences between men and women require a more
stronger than some men), social norms emerge that support complex explanation. Evaluated in relation to other anthro-
the performance of tasks by the more efficient sex and dis- poid primate species, humans proved to have relatively
courage their performance by the other sex. “low” male–female dimorphism in both body weight and
canines (i.e., the size and shape of teeth; Plavcan & van
Schaik, 1997, p. 351). Even though across all primate
Women’s Reproductive Activities and Men’s Size,
species greater bodily dimorphism was associated with
Strength, and Speed
polygynous mating and male–male competition, dimor-
Women’s reproductive activities are especially important in phism at the low levels existing in humans “can be found
shaping female and male social roles. Because women are among species with various mating systems and competi-
responsible for gestating, nursing, and caring for infants, tion levels” (Plavcan, 2000, p. 338). It follows that the rela-
they perform childcare roles across societies (Barry & tively small amount of bodily dimorphism in humans does
Paxson, 1971). In societies without effective birth control not imply sexual selection for particular psychological or
technology, fertile women on average have a child every physical characteristics. Also undermining sexual selection
3.7 years and nurse each child for 2.8 years, with frequent accounts is evidence that both size and canine dimorphism
suckling being the norm (Huber, 2007; Sellen, 2007). These were likely influenced by selection of females, as well as
activities limit women’s ability to perform certain other males (Plavcan & van Schaik, 2005; Wood & Eagly, 2002).
650 Gender

Selection pressures on females are especially plausible Laland, & Feldman, 2003). Niche construction plausibly
given that the decreasing size dimorphism as homi- accelerated the pace of humans’ environmental change
nids evolved from the earlier Australopithecus to Homo by promoting the development of new technologies,
was due to an increase in the size of females relative to resources, and social organizations that supported the eco-
males (McHenry & Coffing, 2000). logical and geographical expansion of humans across the
In general, human bodily dimorphism follows from a globe (Sterelny, 2003).
complex set of factors and cannot be adequately explained By developing mechanisms for innovation and intensive
merely by sexual selection pressures involving male com- social learning, ancestral humans developed the capacity
petition and female choice. Also, this dimorphism does not for a cumulative form of culture that accrues modifications
imply that any particular psychological sex differences are over time. As illustrated by clothing manufacture, one per-
characteristic of humans as a species or caused by sexual son or group initially cut and draped hides or skins, and
selection pressures on males (e.g., competition among males others subsequently modified this practice by sewing
for sexual access to females). Instead, the distal, evolution- and perhaps adding woven materials. Such simple con-
ary causes of male and female psychology lie in the ways struction was then adopted by others, possibly for genera-
that men’s physical attributes and women’s reproductive tions and only eventually modified further. This process
activities interact with sociocultural conditions. As we have depends on several evolved capacities, including initial
argued, the resulting division of labor and associated gender innovation of an artifact or practice, faithful cultural trans-
role beliefs in turn frame the interactions among hormonal mission that works as a sort of ratchet to retain that knowl-
processes, self-regulatory mechanisms, and social expecta- edge (Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll, 2005),
tions that produce sex differences in behavior. and cumulative modifications of the original. The human
flexibility inherent in this cultural progression does not
imply that the mind is a blank slate. Instead, humans pos-
Evolutionary Origins of
sess a suite of cognitive and social abilities that enable the
Human Cultural Variation
production and sharing of novel solutions to the challenges
The specific roles that men and women perform in the divi- of reproduction and survival.
sion of labor vary across societies partly because humans The idea that social learning evolved as an adaptive
developed abilities to innovate and to engage in complex response to changing environments is buttressed by the
forms of social learning and knowledge sharing. These evidence that even in nonhuman species the relations
capacities also have evolutionary origins: They developed between males and females are responsive to some extent
because they solved problems of reproduction and survival. to social learning, despite their more constraining adapta-
Specifically, humans and their ancestors became increas- tions to specific ecologies and climates. Through social
ingly adept at responding with behavioral flexibility and and sexual interaction, individuals in many species learn
generating cultural solutions to variability in evolutionary basic capacities such as discriminating between males and
environments (Potts, 1998; Richerson & Boyd, 2005). females, a precondition for mating with a particular sex
The extraordinary variability in ancestral environ- (Woodson, 2002), and learn about the range of attributes
ments was due partly to environmental changeability in of potential mates (Dukas, 2008). Socially learned infor-
the late Pleistocene climate. The increasing climate varia- mation complements genetically coded preferences and
tion over the last 3 million years represented major shifts behavior patterns, allowing animals to fine-tune behavior
in vegetation, water, and other resources that sometimes to local circumstances.
emerged abruptly between periods of relative stability
(e.g., Ditlevsen, Ditlevsen, & Andersen, 2002). Diversity Socialization in Humans
in adaptive conditions also arose from the piecemeal devel-
opment of human attributes, each constellation of which Opportunities for social learning are extensive in humans
yielded unique selection pressures on human ancestors because their relatively long juvenile period allows child-
(Foley, 2007). Because the suite of uniquely human attri- hood play and socialization to prepare children for the
butes developed in fits and starts, human evolution was adult roles of their society. As already noted at various
marked by a sequence of significant changes (e.g., develop- points in this chapter, socialization enlists various cogni-
ment of stone tools followed by growth in human societies) tive and social learning processes; it emanates from par-
and thus of adaptive problems to be solved. Additionally ents, the extended family, peers and other community
contributing to diversity in adaptive conditions, humans members, media, religion, and other social institutions.
engage in extensive niche construction through which they Play and socialization enable children to acquire skills and
alter the environments in which they live (Odling-Smee, preferences compatible with sex-typical social roles. For
Male and Female Social Roles Are Rooted in a Biosocial Reality 651

example, as shown by Barry, Bacon, and Child’s (1957) men’s physical attributes and women’s reproductive activi-
classic study of child rearing in 110 cultures, in most of the ties. We illustrate this biosocial interaction by analyzing the
societies, girls were encouraged more than boys to be nur- conditions under which men and women express particular
turant. Also, the structuring of childhood activities to give mate preferences and societies develop patriarchal social
girls greater practice in nurturing was a clear-cut finding structures.
in the Six Cultures Project, which involved the collection
Mate Preferences
of extensive data on children’s lives in diverse cultures
(Whiting & Edwards, 1988; Whiting & Whiting, 1975). Mate preferences are inherently closely linked to evolu-
Amplifying these classic cross-cultural investigations, tionary processes because differential reproduction, along
Lytton and Romney’s (1991) meta-analysis of studies of with survival selection, drives evolutionary outcomes. In
parents’ differential treatment of girls and boys yielded the our analysis, these preferences vary as women and men
clearest evidence for gender-stereotypical play, games, and attempt to maximize their outcomes given the prevailing
chores. Such play activity generally models and provides division of labor and gender ideology. In demonstration
tutelage in adult roles; for example, doll play models car- of how this works, Eagly and Wood (1999) reanalyzed the
ing for children, and play with action figures models more data from Buss’s (1989) study of the mate preferences of
violent and warlike activity. Also, the common tendency of young adults from 37 diverse, primarily urbanized, cash-
parents to assign household chores such as lawn mowing economy cultures. In societies with a strong division of
and kitchen work on the basis of their children’s sex pro- labor between male providers, and female homemakers,
vides apprenticeship in sex-typical adult roles. women were more likely to prefer a mate with resources
Whether parents encourage the development of sex- who could be a good provider, and men were more likely
typical personality attributes such as warmth and aggres- to prefer a mate who was a skilled homemaker and child
siveness has remained more ambiguous (Lytton & Romney, caretaker (see also Lippa, 2007). This marital system of a
1991). Some have argued that parents may not convey good provider paired with a domestic worker also gener-
such qualities explicitly but instead do so subtle ways— ated a spousal age difference, given that older men were
for example, by noting and contrasting female and male more likely to have acquired resources and younger women
categories (e.g., Gelman, Taylor, & Nguyen, 2004). Obser- without resources were more likely to value marriage and
vational learning is an ongoing feature of family life as older partners with resources.
well; parents’ and other family members’ own behavior The importance of the marital division of labor to these
and activities convey adult roles and sex-differentiated mate preferences is consistent with experiments in which
behavior patterns to children. envisioning oneself as a future homemaker caused partici-
Through these various socialization experiences, girls pants of both sexes to increase their preference for a mate
and boys develop self-efficacy beliefs that they can engage with good provider qualities and older age, compared with
in behavior typical of their sex and develop gender iden- envisioning oneself as a future family provider (Eagly,
tities incorporating sex-typical attributes. Such beliefs Eastwick, & Johannesen-Schmidt, 2009). Analogously,
enable children to administer self-praise or self-criticism within each of nine nations, more traditional gender ideol-
when they conform to their personal standards for gender- ogy, as manifested in individuals’ sexist attitudes toward
appropriate behavior (Bussey & Bandura, 1999). Also, women or men, was associated with conventional sex-typing
distinctive boy and girl cultures emerge, enhanced by con- of mate preferences—that is, men’s preferences for mates
siderable voluntary sex segregation in childhood (Maccoby, with homemaking skills and younger age and women’s for
1998). As a result, boys and girls tend to develop the skills mates with provider skills and older age (Eastwick et al.,
and preferences that equip them to enact their society’s 2006). And sex-typical courtship roles may directly influence
division of labor. mate preferences. The agentic act of physically approaching
a potential romantic partner, a behavior that is normatively
more expected of men than women, increased students’
Evolutionary Accounts of Sex Differences in
attraction to potential partners and reduced their selectivity
Human Behavior
in a speed-dating study (Finkel & Eastwick, 2009).
A biosocial evolutionary theory provides an organizing Additional evidence that mate preferences emerge flex-
framework to understand sex differences and similarities ibly from the division of labor comes from Sweeney’s
in behavior (Wood & Eagly, 2002, 2007). From this per- (2002) investigation of cross-temporal changes within the
spective, humans’ capacity for innovation and social learn- U.S. population in the relation between economic pros-
ing enabled flexibility in the behavior and social roles of pects and marriage formation. The traditional tendency
men and women, within the constraints that followed from for higher earnings to increase the likelihood of marriage
652 Gender

for men but not women has changed over time as earnings women’s reproductive activities, such that both women and
have become more important for women’s marital pros- men more efficiently performed certain roles given the con-
pects. As a result, the relation between earning and mar- ditions of their society. Therefore, mate preferences varied
riage is now similar for men and women. Also, the age gap with the division of labor within a society and with indi-
in first marriages in the United States has declined from viduals’ endorsement of that division. Patriarchy emerged
husbands being 2.8 years older than wives in 1940 to 1.8 with the development of social roles, assumed largely by
years in 2005 (U.S. Census Bureau, 2006). The findings men, that provided privileged access to power and author-
of variability in mate preference suggest flexibility in ity. Thus, the evolutionary origins of men’s and women’s
response to current conditions. role performance took the form of a biosocial interaction
between the differing physical attributes and reproduc-
Patriarchy tive activities of the sexes and the local socioeconomic,
Societies also vary in whether they have a social hierarchy cultural, and ecological conditions.
in which men have more status and power than women, with
patriarchy becoming more widespread as societies devel-
oped greater complexity (Wood & Eagly, 2002). In contrast, TEMPORAL AND CULTURAL CHANGE
some evolutionary scientists have argued that early humans IN SEX DIFFERENCES
evolved in the context of patriarchy fueled by males’ desire
for paternity certainty and the importance of male hunting Variation over time and across cultures in sex differences
and provisioning (e.g., Kaplan & Robson, 2002; Sidanius & in psychological dispositions and behaviors should reflect
Pratto, 1999). Contrary to this view of human evolution, changes in women’s and men’s social roles. Simply put,
recent evidence supports the survival value of female coali- as the division of labor changes, the demands on women
tions in early human societies, with mothers garnering help and men change. Gender role beliefs mirror the changing
from grandmothers and female kin, as well as from pair- content of each sex’s roles and in turn influence gender
bonded men (e.g., Opie & Power, 2008). identities and stereotypical social expectations.
As societies advanced and human activities became Given that role change is linked to psychological change,
more specialized, patriarchal relations emerged from men’s the apron-wearing homemakers enshrined in U.S. situa-
greater upper-body strength and speed giving them facility tion comedies of the mid-20th century were psychologi-
to perform physically demanding activities (e.g., warfare cally different from the uniformed or professionally attired
and plow technology) that can confer decision-making working women of the 21st century. To understand these
power, authority, and access to resources. Patriarchy also differences, we first outline the U.S. sociodemographic
emerged when women’s reproductive activities interfered shifts that have placed many women in formerly male-
with performing the activities that yielded the most status dominated roles yet largely retained women’s participation
and power in a society. Although women combined their in childcare and other forms of caring for others. Then we
reproductive responsibilities with gathering in foraging evaluate cross-cultural variation in men’s and women’s
societies, gestation and lactation limited their participation roles and in the attributes of each sex. This discussion sets
in the newly emerging roles (e.g., blacksmith and warrior) the stage to analyze the equality of men and women in the
that required intensive specialized training, acquisition Summary section of the chapter.
of complex skills, and extended, uninterrupted periods of
task performance (Huber, 2007). With little participation Variation in the United States Over Time
in such activities, women lacked influence outside of the
household and acquired few resources valuable for trade in Traditionally, men’s labor force participation was much
the broader economy. Because simple economies in which higher than women’s. In the United States since the mid-
people subsist by nomadic foraging lacked the special- 20th century, women greatly increased and men slightly
ized roles that gave some subgroups power over others, decreased their employment, with women’s labor force par-
especially men over women, the contrasting physical and ticipation nearly doubling in the last half of the century (e.g.,
reproductive attributes of the sexes had weaker influence Eagly & Carli, 2007). By 2009, the labor force participation
on power and status in such societies, which were gener- of Americans 20 years and older was 61% for women and
ally more egalitarian (e.g., Salzman, 1999). 75% for men (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2009). In
In summary, ancestral humans evolved a suite of social terms of hours on the job per week between 1965 and 2003,
and cognitive skills that, along with a long juvenile period, employed men’s work declined by more than 6 hours,
promoted flexible performance of male and female roles. This whereas employed women’s increased by more than 3 hours
flexibility was structured by men’s physical attributes and (Aguiar & Hurst, 2007). Yet, the traditional sex difference
Temporal and Cultural Change in Sex Differences 653

remains in weakened form. From 2003 to 2006, even when personality traits). Additional complexities enter when
men and women filled the same roles of being married, par- researchers compare the variabilities of male and female
ents, and employed full time, men devoted 1.25 hours to test scores, in addition to mean differences. Although
their jobs for every hour devoted by women and enjoyed the classic hypothesis of greater male variability enjoys
1.30 hours of leisure and sport for every hour enjoyed by some support (e.g., Archer & Mehdikhani, 2003; Johnson,
women (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2008b). Carothers, & Deary, 2008), causation continues to be
Sex segregation in the workplace also declined over debated among theories emphasizing sexual selection,
this period (Tomaskovic-Devey et al., 2006), with women sampling artifacts, and differential opportunities for devel-
increasing substantially in professional occupations and oping abilities and traits.
especially in managerial occupations (Wootton, 1997). Also linking women’s personal attributes to their employ-
Women now constitute 51% of individuals in manage- ment, research has shown that both full-time employment
ment, professional, and related occupations (U.S. Bureau and employment in higher-prestige occupations predicted
of Labor Statistics, 2008a). U.S. mothers’ self-reported agency (Kasen, Chen, Sneed,
Other sociodemographic shifts include the increas- Crawford, & Cohen, 2006). Among German university
ing education of women, who earned 58% of bachelor ’s graduates of both sexes, self-reported agency predicted
degrees in 2006 versus 43% in 1970 (U.S. National Center career success, which in turn enhanced agency (Abele,
for Education Statistics, 2007a, 2007b). Strikingly, women 2003). Women’s increasing agency includes ambition for
earned 46% of all doctoral degrees in the United States careers outside of the home. In multiple surveys of college
in 2006, an increase from 25% in 1977 (Welch, 2008). In freshmen conducted between 1966 and 2006, the career
addition, patterns of course taking have changed, with goals of men and women converged, primarily because
high school girls as likely as boys to take calculus and of women’s increased aspiration for traditionally male-
women earning 48% of undergraduate degrees in math- dominated careers (Pryor, Hurtado, Saenz, Santos, & Korn,
ematics (Hyde, Lindberg, Linn, Ellis, & Williams, 2008). 2006). In addition, among high school seniors in 2004,
Women and girls now also participate more in sports, slightly more women (93%) than men (90%) rated “being
with women occupying 45% of Division 1 collegiate successful at work” as an important life value, indicating
athletic positions in 2006 compared with 31% in 1992 a reversal from the greater male emphasis on this value in
(National Collegiate Athletic Association, 2008). With the 1970s (U.S. National Center for Education Statistics,
these female inroads into the formerly male-dominated 2007a, 2007b). In a meta-analysis of the attributes valued
domains of management and professions, higher education, in jobs, many sex differences weakened over time among
mathematics, and sport, more women are entering roles adults in similar occupations, including the traditionally
that require agentic behavior and quantitative competence. greater male preferences for leadership, promotions, and
Some research indicates that women are increasing autonomy (Konrad, Ritchie, Lieb, & Corrigall, 2000).
in agency, consistent with their changing social roles. The changes in the roles of men and women since the mid-
A meta-analysis of self-reported agentic traits from 1973 20th century are not symmetrical. Although women have
to 1993 found that the sex difference decreased over time; been moving into many traditionally male-dominated occu-
specifically, agency rose for both sexes but especially pational roles, traditionally female-dominated roles involving
among women (Twenge, 1997). Similarly, a meta-analysis caretaking of others continue to be female dominated.
that focused more narrowly on the personality traits of Women still are the majority in occupations such as elemen-
assertiveness and dominance found little change in men, tary school teacher, social worker, and nurse that emphasize
but women’s scores rose from 1931 to 1945, dropped from caring for others or communal characteristics more gener-
1946 to 1967, and rose again from 1968 to 1993 (Twenge, ally (Cejka & Eagly, 1999; England et al., 2002; U.S. Bureau
2001), apparently mirroring 20th-century fluctuations in of Labor Statistics, 2008a). Women also continue to take
women’s employment roles. However, other research has primary responsibility for childcare and other household
found that, despite the increase in both sexes’ agency, men service work, despite some increase in men’s childcare and
are still higher (e.g., Feingold, 1994; Lueptow, Garovich- housework and a decrease in women’s housework (Aguiar &
Szabo, & Lueptow, 2001). In general, the direction and Hurst, 2007; Bianchi et al., 2006). From 2003 to 2006, even
magnitude of the sex difference may depend on what spe- for men and women who fill the same role—married par-
cific aspect of self-reported agency is being evaluated. ents who were employed full time, women devoted 1.5 hours
Traditional sex differences favoring men may still be pres- to childcare for every hour devoted by men, as well as 1.5
ent in some aspects of agency, whereas other aspects favor hours to other household work for every hour devoted by
women (see Costa, Terracciano, & McCrae, 2001, for men (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2008b). Less equality
male–female comparisons on specific facets of Big Five prevails when mothers are employed part time or not at all.
654 Gender

Consistent with this continuity in women’s caretaking concept of cultural lag (Brinkman & Brinkman, 1997;
of others, the sex difference in communal orientation has Ogburn, 1922/1964). Traditional representations of women
remained relatively unchanged over time. In a meta-analysis also continue, despite the addition of nontraditional rep-
of self-reported personality characteristics, communal resentations. Not only does cultural lore still feature fairy-
characteristics were fairly stable from 1973 to 1993 tale princesses and ecstatic brides, but people’s personal
(Twenge, 1997). The continuation of higher levels of self- encounters with occupants of roles such as primary care-
reported communion in women than men also was present taker and teacher of children also are overwhelmingly with
in a meta-analysis of self-reports of caring (or “tender- women. Traditional assumptions about gender are perpetu-
minded”) personality traits (Feingold, 1994), as well as ated in various other ways, such as being embedded in lan-
in a single investigation spanning 1974 to 1997 (with evi- guage use and grammatical forms (e.g., Stahlberg, Braun,
dence of increasing female communion; Lueptow et al., Irmen, & Sczesny, 2007).
2001). Life goals concerning family also have retained tra-
ditional sex differences—for example, among high school
Variation Across Cultures
seniors in 2004, more women (53%) than men (45%) rated
“having children” as an important life value (U.S. National Just as the psychology of women has changed across time
Center for Education Statistics, 2007a, 2007b). in the United States, with changes mainly in women’s roles,
Sex differences also were stable across time in communal- the values and attributes of men and women should differ
related values and attitudes. Women, more than men, across cultures depending on the distribution of women
valued the welfare of others in the 1970s, and this effect and men into social roles. That is, the attributes and beliefs
persisted in the 1990s (Beutel & Marini, 1995). From of women and men should take a traditional form in societ-
1973 to 1998, women’s (vs. men’s) stronger endorse- ies with greater male–female inequality. In such societies,
ment of socially compassionate social policies (e.g., sup- women generally have limited participation in the paid
port for disadvantaged groups) showed no change (Eagly, labor force and are otherwise restricted by, for example,
Diekman, Johannesen-Schmidt, & Koenig, 2004). Also having less access to education than men and by the segre-
stable over time was women’s greater endorsement of tradi- gation of women and men into different life roles.
tional morality (e.g., disapproval of divorce or extramarital Across societies, a patriarchal division of labor is reflected
relations), which upholds communal-oriented institutions, in gender role beliefs (Inglehart & Norris, 2003). For exam-
such as marriage, the family, and organized religion. ple, across 19 world societies, women’s greater access to
In general, although women have entered the workplace resources and power was associated with lesser sexism in
in large numbers, they continue to be underrepresented in the form of both benevolent beliefs about traditional women
the more lucrative positions and in positions that confer high and hostile beliefs about nontraditional women (Glick &
levels of authority (e.g., Helfat, Harris, & Wolfson, 2006). Fiske, 2001). Moreover, these hostile and benevolent beliefs
Thus, despite women’s increasing agency, sex differences tended to coincide in nations (Glick et al., 2000). Gender role
remain in some agentic attributes and beliefs. Less evi- beliefs about men similarly depend on the division of labor,
dence exists of change in sex-differentiated attributes and with people believing more in men’s inherent dominance in
beliefs related to communal qualities, perhaps due to the patriarchal societies (Glick et al., 2004). Providing causal
continuing female predominance in caretaking roles, both evidence that women’s labor force participation influences
in the family and in the labor force. These trends closely gender role beliefs, Seguino (2007) evaluated, for a sample of
match people’s everyday understanding of changes over world societies, the effects of increases in women’s share
time in sex differences. Research on the stereotypical traits of economic activity. Across societies, the social experience of
ascribed to women and men of the past, present, and future women moving into paid employment increased beliefs in
showed that social perceivers view the sex difference in equality between the sexes.
communal qualities as remaining relatively constant over Sex differences in self-ratings on personality attributes
time, even though they view the sex difference in agency as and abilities across cultures are difficult to interpret because
eroding as women gain more of these qualities (Diekman & they can be influenced by various features of women’s and
Eagly, 2000). men’s roles. In particular, the extent of segregation of men
Despite the role changes of recent years, overall stereo- and women into social roles likely influences the com-
types about women and men have apparently not under- parison standard that people use to evaluate themselves
gone marked shifts (Lueptow et al., 2001). Although and others (see the discussion of shifting standards in the
conclusions about change of gender stereotypes require earlier section titled Accuracy of Gender Stereotypes). In
additional research evidence, the idea that these beliefs traditional cultures in which occupational and other roles
change more slowly than roles is consistent with the tend to be segregated by sex, men and women would judge
Summary 655

their own and others’ psychological attributes through a in the more egalitarian societies, not an enhancement of
comparison with salient others, who are mainly of the presumably sexually selected personality traits of risk tak-
same sex. Thus, a man might rate himself as only moder- ing and dominance. However, such data remain clouded by
ately assertive because he is comparing himself with other possible shifting judgment standards across cultures.
men, who are generally somewhat assertive in his society. In summary, variation over time and across cultures in
In contrast, in more egalitarian societies with less sex- the roles of men and women corresponds to variation
segregated roles, a man might compare himself with indi- in sex-typical psychological traits. As women have entered
viduals of both sexes and conclude that he is relatively the workforce in increasingly large numbers in postindus-
assertive. The result of this shifting comparison standard trial societies, they have acquired agentic traits so that they
is that sex differences should appear to be smaller in less have become more comparable to men in these qualities.
egalitarian, more hierarchical societies in which individu- In more traditional societies with greater sex segregation,
als compare themselves with their own sex (Guimond et al., fewer sex differences are evident in self-rated traits and
2007; see also Lippa, in press). abilities, presumably because these men and women tend
In line with this shifting standards prediction, several to judge themselves in relation to salient others who are
studies have found stronger sex differences in self-reports of the same sex. The logic of the shifting sex differences
and other reports in more egalitarian societies. For example, in psychological data over time and cultures thus derives
across world societies, men place greater value on power, from changes in men’s and women’s roles in society.
social status, and prestige, whereas women place greater
value on benevolence and concern for the welfare of close
others. These sex differences were larger in more egalitar- SUMMARY
ian societies (Schwartz & Rubel, 2005). Self-reported emo-
tions showed a similar pattern across cultures, with larger Psychologists’ debates about sex differences and simi-
sex differences in less traditional societies (Fischer & larities and their causes have important implications for
Manstead, 2000). In addition, in comparisons of personality people’s lives, especially for women’s potential to attain
traits across societies, women are generally higher in neu- equality in power and status in industrialized societies.
roticism, agreeableness, warmth, and openness to feelings, This equality issue lurks in the background of many debates
whereas men are higher in assertiveness and openness to about gender, especially given that the second-wave feminist
ideas. These personality differences were more pronounced movement fueled the upsurge of gender research that began
in more egalitarian societies (Costa et al., 2001). A simi- in the 1970s. Relevant to equality are some researchers’
lar pattern was found for observers’ ratings of men’s and claims that women and men are similar on most conse-
women’s personality traits (McCrae et al., 2005). quential psychological attributes (Hyde, 2005). If similarity
This variation in self-reported personality traits across is present, few intrinsic psychological barriers to equality
cultures is less congenial with an alternative explanation in exist. The implication is that both sexes should have access
which, “in the ancestral past, as hunter gatherers, men and to all societal roles and that behavioral differences held in
women naturally developed sexually selected differences place by societal expectations, gender identities, and hor-
in personality traits such that men were more risk taking monal influences are likely to erode over time.
and dominance seeking and women were more nurturing” One prominent example of sex similarities is the equiv-
(Schmitt, Realo, Voracek, & Allik, 2008, pp. 178–179). alent performance of girls and boys on standardized tests
Supposedly, more prosperous, egalitarian societies are sup- of math achievement in grade school through high school
portive of the expression of these innate sex differences (Hyde et al., 2008). Additionally provocative are contem-
because they are similar in crucial aspects to the egalitar- porary findings of female advantage in domains that tradi-
ian, hunter–gatherer societies in which humans evolved. tionally advantaged men—for example, in leadership styles
However, the pattern of change in the sex differences is associated with effective management (Eagly, Johannesen-
not consistent with this claim. Specifically, in the data from Schmidt, & van Engen, 2003) and in performance in certain
Schmitt et al., the larger sex differences in egalitarian soci- task-oriented groups (Wood, 1987). Reports of null sex dif-
eties were primarily due to men scoring lower on neuroti- ferences and of female advantage have been enthusiastically
cism, agreeableness, and conscientiousness, as well as on received by those who are committed to furthering gender
extroversion, which reflects men’s view of themselves as equality. The reaction is understandable, given that evi-
less depressed and anxious, cooperative and outgoing, dis- dence of male superiority in traditionally male-advantage
ciplined and dutiful, and friendly and assertive in egalitarian domains tends to disqualify women in relation to attractive
societies than patriarchal societies. Thus, this pattern of sex roles and opportunities and even to justify unequal treat-
differences suggests that a “male slacker” effect is emerging ment under the law (Barnett & Rivers, 2004; Hyde, 2005).
656 Gender

In contrast to these concerns about impediments to in personality in recent decades have been asymmetrical,
women’s occupancy of masculine roles, equality can be with evidence that women are adopting some of men’s
compromised by women’s privileged access to feminine masculine, agentic characteristics and that men are not
roles. Evidence of female advantage on communal attri- adopting women’s feminine, communal characteristics. We
butes has such implications, given that nurturance and ascribed this shift in women’s personal attributes mainly to
concern for other people are compatible with women’s tra- their increasing labor force participation, including inroads
ditional caretaking roles in the family and in communally into many jobs once dominated by men.
demanding occupations. Examples include Gilligan’s From our social role perspective, men’s psychological
(1982) claims that women take a caring approach to moral attributes will shift to the extent that they perform more
reasoning and Taylor and colleagues’ (2000; Taylor, 2002) family-caring activities and enter into more communally
proposal that women react to stress not by fight or flight demanding occupations. But only modest changes can be
but by tending children and befriending allies. Other seen in men’s activities thus far, in either domestic work
reports have highlighted women’s greater social sensitiv- (Bianchi et al., 2006) or paid employment (Queneau, 2006).
ity (Hall, 2006) and emotional intelligence (e.g., Brackett, Men have not undergone major transitions in their daily
Rivers, Shiffman, Lerner, & Salovey, 2006). activities in the United States or other industrialized nations.
Psychologists have offered various opinions about Is it possible for men to adopt more communal roles
whether sex differences that remain in agentic and commu- and develop more communal-oriented personalities?
nal traits are due to socialization and situational influences The answer requires knowledge of the biosocial roots of
or to intrinsic, hardwired causes deriving from genetic dif- the role structure and the limits it may impose on role flex-
ferences between the sexes. Advocating for environmental ibility. As we have explained, these roots lie mainly in the
influences are many developmental psychologists (e.g., ways in which male size and strength and female repro-
Bussey & Bandura, 2004), plus social constructionists in ductive activities interact with socioeconomic complexity.
many social science fields (e.g., Marecek, Crawford, & Through human history, as societies shifted from simple
Popp, 2004). Advocating for intrinsic causes are evolu- foraging through agricultural and eventually to industrial
tionary psychologists who trace sex differences in modern economies, patriarchy became the dominant form of rela-
human psychology to sexual selection pressures on human tions between the sexes. This transition emerged with
ancestors (e.g., Browne, 2002; Kenrick, Trost, & Sundie, several societal developments, including technologies such
2004; Schmitt et al., 2008; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). as the plow, provisioning by hunting large animals, and
Our biosocial theory falls into neither of these two societal structures such as organized warfare. The biosocial
camps. Our review of the empirical evidence identified restraints of male size and strength and female reproduc-
sex differences in several important personal attributes and tive activity increasingly gave men better access to the new
behaviors, especially in natural settings. These differences roles (e.g., farmer, hunter, and warrior) that yielded wealth
take various forms, depending on men’s and women’s and prestige, thereby reducing women’s share of economic
roles in society. The differences between male and female contribution while retaining their domestic obligations.
behavior that emerge in daily life reflect the immediate, Patriarchy has eroded in most industrialized societies,
proximal causes of hormonal regulation, self-regulation by especially in the second half of the 20th century, as women
gender identities, and social regulation by others’ sanctions have gained power and status. This shift reflects the loosen-
and rewards. Women’s nurturing thus arises from the inclu- ing of biosocial restraints on women’s roles through sharp
sion of caring qualities in their personal identities, as well reductions in birth rates and length of lactation, combined
as social expectations that they manifest such behavior, with shifts in the occupational structure. The occupations
both of which recruit supportive neurochemical processes that now garner status, power, and resources reward brains,
(e.g., increased OT and mechanisms of reward). In essence, not brawn. This shift toward intellectual demands dimin-
female and male psychology is not fixed but emerges from ishes the male advantage once inherent in their physical
interactions across multiple biological and sociocultural prowess, which in turn derives partly from the organizing
factors. The varying forms of this interaction depend on effects of male hormones. In addition, leadership roles are
the division of labor within a society and the ways in which increasingly defined as requiring an androgynous mix of
boys and girls are socialized into sex-typical roles. culturally masculine and feminine abilities and personal-
The psychological attributes of men and women vary ity traits (Eagly & Carli, 2007). Nonetheless, these several
across cultures and time periods depending on the demands changes have so far produced only semiequality between
of their social roles: Women more than men have under- the sexes. Men continue to dominate leadership roles
gone role transitions on a major scale in the United States at highest levels (e.g., Helfat et al., 2006), and women
and many other industrialized nations. Therefore, changes continue to take responsibility for the majority of childcare
References 657

and housework (e.g., Bianchi et al., 2006). Furthermore, mechanisms of reward learning that can undergird
on many attitudinal and behavioral indicators in the United nurturing of infants and young children (Broad et al., 2006;
States, changes toward gender equality appear to be slow- Depue & Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005). Fathering activities
ing down, and sociologists debate why this is happening also are supported by changing norms and attitudes in the
(Blau, Brinton, & Grusky 2006). United States, especially among younger adults, who have
The continuing wage and authority gaps in the work- become considerably more accepting of men’s equal par-
force can be traced partly to women continuing to fill care- ticipation in childcare (e.g., Milkie, Bianchi, Mattingly, &
taking roles, especially childcare. Childcare roles that take Robinson, 2002).
women out of the labor force or reduce their employment to Change in social roles is slowed by societal ideolo-
part time lessen their training and experience. Even many gies and status beliefs that legitimize social inequalities
privileged women who have high educational credentials on the basis of sex and other attributes (Ridgeway, 2006a;
and outstanding career potential reduce their employment Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). To some extent, even women and
to accommodate family obligations (Hewlett, 2007). This members of other subordinated groups accept the system-
reduced participation in employment is compounded by job justifying ideologies of the dominant group (Jost, Pelham,
discrimination against mothers (e.g., Correll, Benard, & Sheldon, & Sullivan, 2003) and endorse paternalistic,
Paik, 2007) and tax laws that encourage women to be pri- benevolently sexist ideas (Glick & Fiske, 2001). However,
mary family caretakers (McCaffery, 1999). These effects women’s attitudes and ideologies are more progressive
lessen women’s opportunities to attain jobs that offer high than men’s (e.g., Eagly & Diekman, 2006; Eagly et al.,
wages and substantial workplace authority (Polachek, 2004; Seguino, 2007), and their political commitments
2006). Thus, the historical origins of patriarchy continue and actions can speed social change (e.g., Dodson, 2006).
to play out in modern times through women’s childcare For those committed to gender equality, the major chal-
activities that reduce their access to roles that confer status, lenge for the future is to encourage both men and women
high wages, and prestige. to occupy a wider range of social roles.
What explains men’s continued low levels of childcare
and limited interest in communally demanding occupa-
tions? Barriers to men taking on such roles include lower
monetary compensation of communally demanding occu- REFERENCES
pations (England, 2006), social expectations that men are
less well endowed with the necessary communal skills Abele, A. E. (2003). The dynamics of masculine-agentic and feminine-
(Cejka & Eagly, 1999), and stigma associated with nontra- communal traits: Findings from a prospective study. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 768–776.
ditional male communal roles such as stay-at-home dads
Abelson, R. P. (1985). A variance explanation paradox: When a little is a
(Brescoll & Uhlmann, 2005).
lot. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 129–133.
Women specialize in childcare partly because of the
Aguiar, M., & Hurst, E. (2007). Measuring trends in leisure: The allocation of
continuing efficiency for women of performing these roles. time over five decades. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 969–1006.
The energetic demands of bearing children and the health Ajzen, I. (2005). Attitudes, personality, and behavior (2nd ed.). Milton
benefits of some months of breast-feeding can orient moth- Keynes, England: Open University Press.
ers away from continued paid employment and toward Aldous, J., & Ganey, R. F. (1999). Family life and the pursuit of happiness:
The influence of gender and race. Journal of Family Issues, 20, 155–180.
infant care. This arrangement is fostered by socialization
Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to
of women and societal beliefs that promote sex-typical
children’s toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops
role performance. Hormonal processes also may encourage sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479.
mothers’ childcare, as the cascading hormones of pregnancy Alexander, M. G., & Wood, W. (2000). Women, men, and positive emo-
and lactation support women’s tending (Campbell, 2008; tions: A social role interpretation. In A. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and
emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 189–210). New York:
Taylor, 2002). Research is still discovering these nurturing
Cambridge University Press.
effects, and one possibility is that OT’s activating effects on
Allen, B. P. (1995). Gender stereotypes are not accurate: A replication of
human behavior function primarily in the service of ongo- Martin (1987) using diagnostic vs. self-report and behavioral criteria.
ing social roles, as does T. If so, then OT largely accom- Sex Roles, 32, 583–600.
modates and supports the expression of self and social Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus
expectations for maternal behavior within a society. Within Books.
American Psychological Association. (2001). Publication manual of the
families, paternal behavior also is supported by hormonal
American Psychological Association (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
processes, as fathers show parallel hormonal accommoda-
Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who attains
tion to parenthood (Berg & Wynne-Edwards, 2001, 2002). social status? Effects of personality and physical attractiveness in social
In both sexes, caretaking is facilitated by neurochemical groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 116–132.
658 Gender

Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual Becker, S. W., & Eagly, A. H. (2004). The heroism of women and men.
partners: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, American Psychologist, 59, 163–178.
651–680. Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal
Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: An evaluation of of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.
the challenge hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex
30, 319–345. typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.
Archer, J., & Coyne, S. M. (2005). An integrated review of indirect, rela- Ben-Zeev, T., Fein, S., & Inzlicht, M. (2005). Arousal and stereotype threat.
tional, and social aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 174–181.
Review, 9, 212–230.
Berg, S. J., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2001). Changes in testosterone,
Archer, J., Graham-Kevan, N., & Davies, M. (2005). Testosterone and cortisol, and estradiol levels in men becoming fathers. Mayo Clinic
aggression: A re-analysis of Book, Starzyk, and Quinsey’s (2001) Proceedings, 76, 582–592.
study. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10, 241–261.
Berg, S. J., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2002). Salivary hormone concen-
Archer, J., & Mehdikhani, M. (2003). Variability among males in sexually trations in mothers and fathers becoming parents are not correlated.
selected attributes. Review of General Psychology, 7, 219–236. Hormones and Behavior, 42, 424–436.
Arnold, A. P. (2004). Sex chromosomes and brain gender. Nature Reviews Best, D. L., & Thomas, J. J. (2004). Cultural diversity and cross-
Neuroscience, 5, 701–708. cultural perspectives. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg
Aubry, S., Ruble, D. N., & Silverman, L. B. (1999). The role of gender (Eds.), The psychology of gender (2nd ed., pp. 296–327). New York:
knowledge in children’s gender-typed preferences. In L. Balter & Guilford Press.
C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of con- Bettencourt, B. A., & Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in aggression
temporary issues (pp. 363–390). New York: Psychology Press. as a function of provocation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., 119, 422–447.
Hackett, G., et al. (2009). Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differen- Beutel, A. M., & Marini, M. M. (1995). Gender and values. American
tiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. Psychological Science, Sociological Review, 60, 436–448.
20, 144–148.
Bianchi, S. M., Robinson, J. P., & Milkie, M. A. (2006). Changing rhythms
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: Isolation and commu- of American family life. New York: Russell Sage.
nion in Western man. Boston: Beacon.
Biddle, B. J. (1979). Role theory: Expectancies, identities, and behaviors.
Banaji, M., & Hardin, C. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psychological New York: Academic.
Science, 7, 136–141.
Biernat, M. (2003). Toward a broader view of social stereotyping. American
Banaji, M., Hardin, C., & Rothman, A. (1993). Implicit stereotyping in person Psychologist, 58, 1019–1027.
judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 272–281.
Biernat, M. (2005). Standards and expectancies: Contrast and assimila-
Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster: The case against the controlla- tion in judgments of self and others. New York: Psychology Press.
bility of automatic stereotype effects. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.),
Biernat, M., Crandall, C. S., Young, L. V., Kobrynowicz, D., &
Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 361–382). New York:
Halpin, S. M. (1998). All that you can be: Stereotyping of self and
Guilford Press.
others in a military context. Journal of Personality and Social
Barnett, R., & Rivers, C. (2004). Same difference: How gender myths Psychology, 75, 301–317.
are hurting our relationships, our children, and our jobs. New York: Biernat, M., & Vescio, T. K. (2002). She swings, she hits, she’s great,
Basic Books. she’s benched: Implications of gender-based shifting standards for
Barry, H., III, & Paxson, L. M. (1971). Infancy and early childhood: Cross- judgment and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
cultural codes 2. Ethnology, 10, 466–508. 28, 66–77.
Barry, H., III, Bacon, M. K., & Child, I. L. (1957). A cross-cultural sur- Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice.
vey of some sex differences in socialization. Journal of Abnormal and Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 242–261.
Social Psychology, 55, 327–332. Blair, I. V., & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basic of romantic love. in stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
NeuroReport, 11, 3829–3834. 70, 1142–1163.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and Blair, I. V., Ma, J. E., & Lenton, A. P. (2001). Imagining stereotypes
romantic love. NeuroImage, 21, 1155–1166. away: The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery.
Bateup, H. S., Booth, A., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Granger, D. A. (2002). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 828–841.
Testosterone, cortisol, and women’s competition. Evolution and Blakemore, J. E. O. (2003). Children’s beliefs about violating gender
Human Behavior, 23, 181–192. norms: Boys shouldn’t look like girls, and girls shouldn’t act like boys.
Baucom, D. H., Besch, P. K., & Callahan, S. (1985). Relation between tes- Sex Roles, 48, 411–419.
tosterone concentration, sex role identity, and personality among females. Blau, F. D., Brinton, M. C., & Grusky, D. B. (Eds.). (2006). The declining
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1218–1226. significance of gender? New York: Russell Sage.
Baumeister, R. F., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). What do men want? Gender Boldry, J., Wood, W., & Kashy, D. (2001). Gender stereotypes and the
differences and two spheres of belongingness: Comment on Cross and evaluation of men and women in military settings. Journal of Social
Madson (1997). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 38–44. Issues, 57, 689–703.
Beaulieu, D. A., & Bugental, D. B. (2006). An evolutionary approach to Booth, A., Granger, D. A., Mazur, A., & Kivlighan, K. T. (2006).
socialization. In J. Grusec & P. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of social- Testosterone and social behavior. Social Forces, 85, 167–191.
ization (pp. 71–95). New York: Guilford Press. Bosson, J. K., Prewitt-Freilino, J. L., & Taylor, J. N. (2005). Role rigidity:
Becker, J. C., & Wagner, U. (2009). Doing gender differently: The interplay A problem of identity misclassification? Journal of Personality and
of strength of gender identification and content of gender identity in Social Psychology, 89, 552–565.
predicting women’s endorsement of sexist beliefs. European Journal Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., & Salovey, P.
of Social Psychology, 39, 487–508. (2006). Relating emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison
References 659

of self-report and performance measures of emotional intelligence. Cinamon, R. G., & Rich, Y. (2002). Gender differences in the importance
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 780–795. of work and family roles: Implications for work–family conflict. Sex
Brescoll, V. L., & Uhlmann, E. L. (2005). Attitudes toward traditional and Roles, 47, 531–541.
nontraditional parents. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 436–445. Coates, J. M., & Herbert, J. (2008). Endogenous steroids and financial
Brescoll, V. L., & Uhlmann, E. L. (2008). Can an angry woman get ahead? risk taking on a London trading floor. Proceedings of the National
Status conferral, gender, and expression of emotion in the workplace. Academy of the Sciences, 105, 6167–6172.
Psychological Science, 19, 268–275. Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B. F., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult,
Briton, N. J., & Hall, J. A. (1995). Beliefs about female and male nonverbal aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An “experimental ethnog-
communication. Sex Roles, 32, 79–90. raphy.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 945–960.
Broad, K. D., Curley, J. P., & Keverne, E. B. (2006). Mother–infant bond- Cohen-Bendahan, C. C. C., van de Beek, C., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2005).
ing and the evolution of mammalian social relationships. Philosophical Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior:
Transactions of the Royal Society, 361, 2199–2214. Methods and findings. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29,
353–384.
Broverman, I. K., Vogel, S. R., Broverman, D. M., Clarkson, F. E., &
Rosenkrantz, P. S. (1972). Sex-roles stereotypes: A current appraisal. Cohn, A., & Zeichner, A. (2006). Effects of masculine identity and gender
Journal of Social Issues, 28, 59–78. role stress on aggression in men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity,
7, 179–190.
Browne, K. R. (2002). Biology at work: Rethinking sexual equality.
New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Conway, M., Pizzamiglio, M. T., & Mount, L. (1996). Status, communality,
and agency: Implications for stereotypes of gender and other groups.
Burke, P. J. (2004). Identities and social structure: The 2003 Cooley-Mead
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 25–38.
Award address. Social Psychology Quarterly, 67, 5–15.
Correll, S. J., Benard, S., & Paik, I. (2007). Getting a job: Is there a mother-
Burleson, B. R., & Kunkel, A. W. (2006). Revisiting the different cultures
hood penalty? American Journal of Sociology, 112, 1297–1338.
thesis: An assessment of sex differences and similarities in supportive
communication. In K. Dindia & D. J. Canary (Eds.), Sex differences Corter, C. M., & Fleming, A. S. (1995). Psychobiology of maternal behavior
and similarities in communication (2nd ed., pp. 137–159). Mahwah, in human beings. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. (Vol. 2, pp. 87–116). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Costa, P. T., Jr., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differ-
Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain ences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising find-
Sciences, 12, 1–49. ings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 322–331.
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1992). Self-regulatory mechanisms governing Cowan, G., & Hoffman, C. D. (1986). Gender stereotyping in young
gender development. Child Development, 63, 1236–1250. children: Evidence to support a concept-learning approach. Sex Roles,
14, 211–224.
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender devel-
opment and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713. Crandall, C. S., & Stangor, C. (2005). Conformity and prejudice. In
J. Dovidio, P. S. Glick, & L. A. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of preju-
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (2004). Social cognitive theory of gender develop-
dice: Fifty years after Allport (pp. 295–309). Oxford: Blackwell.
ment and functioning. In A. H. Eagly, A. E., Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.),
The psychology of gender (pp. 92–119). New York: Guilford Press. Cronbach, L., & Meehl, P. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests.
Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281–302.
Byron, K. (2007). Male and female managers’ ability to “read” emotions:
Relationships with supervisor ’s performance ratings and subordinates’ Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and
satisfaction ratings. Journal of Occupational and Organizational gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5–37.
Psychology, 80, 713–733. Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2004). When professionals
Campbell, A. (2008). Attachment, aggression and affiliation: The role of become mothers, warmth doesn’t cut the ice. Journal of Social Issues,
oxytocin in female social behavior. Biological Psychology, 77, 1–10. 60, 701–718.
Card, N. A., Stucky, B. D., Sawalani, G. M., & Little, T. D. (2008). Direct Cullen, M. J., Waters, S. D., & Sackett, P. R. (2006). Testing stereotype
and indirect aggression during childhood and adolescence: A meta- threat theory predictions for math-identified and non-math-identified
analytic review of gender differences, intercorrelations, and relations to students by gender. Human Performance, 19, 421–440.
maladjustment. Child Development, 79, 1185–1229. Cutrona, C. E. (1996). Social support in couples: Marriage as a resource
Carli, L. L. (2001). Gender and social influence. Journal of Social Issues, in times of stress. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
57, 725–741. Czopp, A. M., & Monteith, M. J. (2003). Confronting prejudice (literally):
Carpenter, S., & Trentham, S. (1998). Subtypes of women and men: A new Reactions to confrontations of racial and gender bias. Personality and
taxonomy and an exploratory categorical analysis. Journal of Social Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 523–544.
Behavior and Personality, 13, 679–696. Dabbs, J. M., Jr. (1997). Testosterone, smiling, and facial appearance.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2008). Self-regulatory systems: Action and Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 45–55.
affect. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Cate, K., Brower, A., Emery, C., Leander, P., & Zachary, M.
science (pp. 308–324). New York: Guilford Press. (2003). Testosterone treatment, affect, and appearance: Slight effects in
Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender-stereotypic images of occupa- normal subjects. Social Behavior and Personality, 31, 387–394.
tions correspond to the sex segregation of employment. Personality Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Dabbs, M. G. (2000). Heroes, rogues, and lovers:
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 413–423. Testosterone and behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confirmation Dardenne, B., Dumont, M., & Bollier, T. (2007). Insidious dangers of
processes: The self-fulfilling consequences of automatic stereotype benevolent sexism: Consequences for women’s performance. Journal
activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 541–560. of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 764–779.
Cialdini, R. B., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, Dasgupta, N., & Asgari, S. (2004). Seeing is believing: Exposure to
conformity, and compliance. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on the malleability
(Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 151–192). of automatic gender stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social
Boston: McGraw-Hill. Psychology, 40, 642–658.
660 Gender

Dasgupta, N., & Rivera, L. M. (2006). From automatic antigay prejudice Eagly, A. H., Eastwick, P., & Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C. (2009). Possible
to behavior: The moderating role of conscious beliefs about gender and selves in marital roles: The impact of the anticipated division of labor
behavioral control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, on the mate preferences of women and men. Personality and Social
268–280. Psychology Bulletin, 35, 403–414.
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D. M., & Gerhardstein, R. (2002). Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & van Engen, M. L. (2003).
Consuming images: How television commercials that elicit stereotype Transformational, transactional, and laissez-faire leadership styles:
threat can restrain women academically and professionally. Personality A meta-analysis comparing women and men. Psychological Bulletin,
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1615–1628. 129, 569–591.
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Clearing the air: Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice
Identity safety moderates the effects of stereotype threat on women’s toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.
leadership aspirations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Eagly, A. H., & Kite, M. E. (1987). Are stereotypes of nationalities
88, 276–287. applied to both women and men? Journal of Personality and Social
Deaux, K., & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & Psychology, 53, 451–462.
G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, Eagly, A. H., & Koenig, A. M. (2006). Social role theory of sex differences
pp. 788–827). New York: McGraw-Hill. and similarities: Implication for prosocial behavior. In K. Dindia &
Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1983). Components of gender stereotypes. D. J. Canary (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in communication
Psychological Documents, 13, 25. (Ms. No. 2583). (2nd ed., pp. 179–194). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interac- Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the
tive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3–22.
369–389. Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1994). Are people prejudiced against women?
Depue, R. A., & Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005). A neurobehavioral Some answers from research on attitudes, gender stereotypes, and judg-
model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human ments of competence. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European
trait of affiliation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 313–395. review of social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 1–35). New York: Wiley.
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and con- Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the dis-
trolled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, tribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality
5–18. and Social Psychology, 46, 735–754.
Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior:
Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature.
Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 309–330.
Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2008). Of women, men, and motivation: Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1982). Inferred sex differences in status as a
A role congruity account. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), determinant of gender stereotypes about social influence. Journal of
Handbook of motivational science (pp. 434–447). New York: Guilford Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 915–928.
Press. Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in
Diekman, A. B., Eagly, A. H., & Kulesa, P. (2002). Accuracy and bias in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American
stereotypes about the social and political attitudes of women and men. Psychologist, 54, 408–423.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 268–282. Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. (2000). Social role theory of
Ditlevsen, P. D., Ditlevsen, S., & Andersen, K. K. (2002). The fast climate sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes &
fluctuations during the stadial and interstadial climate states. Annals of H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender
Glaciology, 35, 457–462. (pp. 123–174). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dodson, D. L. (2006). The impact of women in Congress. New York: Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C. (2004). Social role
Oxford University Press. theory of sex differences and similarities: Implications for the partner
Duehr, E. E., & Bono, J. E. (2006). Men, women, and managers: Are ste- preferences of women and men. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J.
reotypes finally changing? Personnel Psychology, 59, 815–846. Sternberg (Eds.), Psychology of gender (pp. 269–295). New York:
Guilford Press.
Dukas, R. (2008). Evolutionary biology of insect learning. Annual Review
of Entomology, 53, 145–160. Eastwick, P. W., Eagly, A. H., Glick, P., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., Fiske,
S. T., Blum, A. M. B., et al. (2006). Is traditional gender ideology
Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role associated with sex-typed mate preferences? A test in nine nations.
interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sex Roles, 54, 603–614.
Eagly, A. H. (in press). The his and hers of prosocial behavior: An exami- Eckes, T. (2002). Paternalistic and envious gender stereotypes: Testing pre-
nation of the social psychology of gender. American Psychologist. dictions from the stereotype content model. Sex Roles, 47, 99–114.
Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (2007). Through the labyrinth: The truth about Edwards, D. A., Wetzel, K., & Wyner, D. R. (2006). Intercollegiate soccer:
how women become leaders. Boston: Harvard Business School Press. Saliva cortisol and testosterone are elevated during competition, and
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort testosterone is related to status and social connectedness with team-
Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. mates. Physiology & Behavior, 87, 135–143.
Eagly, A. H., & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: A meta- Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon
analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 701–778). New York:
Bulletin, 100, 283–308. Wiley.
Eagly, A. H., & Diekman, A. B. (2006). Examining gender gaps in socio- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial develop-
political attitudes: It’s not Mars and Venus. Feminism & Psychology, ment. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook
16, 26–34. of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development
Eagly, A. H., Diekman, A. B., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & Koenig, A. M. (6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 646–718). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
(2004). Gender gaps in sociopolitical attitudes: A social psychological Ellis, B. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life
analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 796–816. history approach. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 920–958.
References 661

England, P. (2006). Toward gender equality: Progress and bottlenecks. Gawronski, B. (2003). On difficult questions and evident answers:
In F. D. Blau, M. C. Brinton, & D. B. Grusky (Eds.), The declining Dispositional inference from role-constrained behavior. Personality
significance of gender? (pp. 245–264). New York: Russell Sage. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1459–1475.
England, P., Budig, M., & Folbre, N. (2002). Wages of virtue: The relative Geis, F. (1993). Self-fulfilling prophecies: A social psychological view of
pay of care work. Social Problems, 49, 455–473. gender. In A. Beall & R. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender
Evans, C. D., & Diekman, A. B. (2009). On motivated role selection: (pp. 9–54). New York: Guilford Press.
Gender beliefs, distant goals, and career interest. Psychology of Women Gelman, S. A., Taylor, M. G., & Nguyen, S. P. (2004). Mother–child
Quarterly, 33, 235–249. conversations about gender. Monographs of the Society for Research
Fausto-Sterling, A. (1993). The five sexes: Why male and female are not in Child Development, 69, vii–127.
enough. Sciences, March–April, 20–24. Gilbert, D. T. (1998). Ordinary personology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &
Fazio, R., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2,
Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. pp. 89–150). Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and
Psychological Bulletin, 116, 429–456. women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Feldman, R., Weller, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Levine, A. (2007). Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benev-
Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: olent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality.
Plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.
predict mother–infant bonding. Psychological Science, 18, 965–970.
Glick, P., Fiske, S. T., Mladinic, A., Saiz, J. L., Abrams, D., Masser, B.,
Fernberger, S. W. (1948). Persistence of stereotypes concerning sex differ- et al. (2000). Beyond prejudice as simple antipathy: Hostile and
ences. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 43, 97–101. benevolent sexism across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social
Fiedler, K., & Walther, E. (2004). Stereotyping as inductive hypothesis Psychology, 79, 763–775.
testing. New York: Psychology Press. Glick, P., Lameiras, M., Fiske, S. T., Eckes, T., Masser, B., Volpato, C.,
Finkel, E. J., & Eastwick, P. W. (2009). Arbitrary social norms influence et al. (2004). Bad but bold: Ambivalent attitudes toward men predict
sex differences in romantic selectivity. Psychological Science. gender inequality in 16 nations. Journal of Personality and Social
Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2000). The relations between gen- Psychology, 86, 713–728.
der and emotion in different cultures. In A. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and Gore, J. S., Cross, S. E., & Morris, M. L. (2006). Let’s be friends:
emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 71–94). London: Relational self-construal and the development of intimacy. Personal
Cambridge University Press. Relationships, 13, 83–102.
Fiske, A. P., Haslam, N., & Fiske, S. T. (1991). Confusing one person with Gray, P. B., Kahlenberg, S. M., Barrett, E. S., Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T.
another: What errors reveal about the elementary forms of social rela- (2002). Marriage and fatherhood are associated with lower testosterone
tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 656–674. in males. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 193–201.
Fiske, S. T. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In Green, R. J., Ashmore, R. D., & Manzi, R., Jr. (2005). The structure of
D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social gender type perception: Testing the elaboration, encapsulation, and
psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 357–411). New York: McGraw-Hill. evaluation framework. Social Cognition, 23, 429–464.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often
Grossman, M., & Wood, W. (1993). Sex differences in intensity of emo-
mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively
tional experience: A social role interpretation. Journal of Personality
follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 65, 1010–1022.
and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Guimond, S., Branscombe, N. R., Brunot, S., Buunk, A. P., Chatard, A.,
Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Stallings, J., & Steiner, M. (2002). Testosterone
Désert, M., et al. (2007). Culture, gender, and the self: Variations
and prolactin are associated with emotional responses to infant cries in
and impact of social comparison processes. Journal of Personality and
new fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 42, 399–413.
Social Psychology, 92, 1118–1134.
Fleming, A. S., Ruble, D., Krieger, H., & Wong, P. Y. (1997). Hormonal and
Haig, D. (2004). The inexorable rise of gender and the decline of sex:
experiential correlates of maternal responsiveness during pregnancy and the
Social change in academic titles, 1945–2001. Archives of Sexual
puerperium in human mothers. Hormones and Behavior, 31, 145–158.
Behavior, 33, 87–96.
Foley, R. A. (2007). The emergence of culture in the context of hominin
evolutionary patterns. In S. C. Levinson & P. Jaisson (Eds.), Evolution Hall, J. A. (2006). How big are nonverbal sex differences? The case of smil-
and culture: A Fyssen Foundation symposium (pp. 53–77). Cambridge, ing and nonverbal sensitivity. In K. Dindia & D. J. Canary (Eds.), Sex
MA: MIT Press. differences and similarities in communication (2nd ed., pp. 59–81).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Furnham, A., & Mak, T. (1999). Sex-role stereotyping in television com-
mercials: A review and comparison of fourteen studies done on five Hall, J. A., & Carter, J. D. (1999a). Gender-stereotype accuracy as an indi-
continents over 25 years. Sex Roles, 41, 413–437. vidual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
350–359.
Gabriel, S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Are there “his” and “hers” types of
interdependence? The implications of gender differences in collective Hall, J. A., & Carter, J. D. (1999b). [Unpublished data]. Northeastern
versus relational interdependence for affect, behavior, and cognition. University, Boston, MA.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 642–655. Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs
Ganahl, D. J., Prinsen, T. J., & Netzley, S. B. (2003). A content analysis of about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39,
prime time commercials: A contextual framework of gender represen- 113–127.
tation. Sex Roles, 49, 545–551. Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in
Gardner, W. L., & Gabriel, S. (2004). Gender differences in relational rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children. Hormones
and collective interdependence: Implications for self-views, social and Behavior, 54, 359–364.
behavior, and subjective well-being. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & Head, J. (2008, July 29). Thai school offers transsexual toilet. BBC News
R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender (2nd ed., Online. Retrieved October 10, 2008, from http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/
pp. 169–191) New York: Guilford Press. asia-pacific/7529227.stm
662 Gender

Heilman, M. E. (2001). Description and prescription: How gender stereo- system: Evidence of enhanced system justification among the disadvan-
types prevent women’s ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal of taged. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 13–36.
Social Issues, 57, 657–674. Kaiser, C. R., & Miller, C. T. (2001). Reacting to impending discrimina-
Heilman, M. E., Wallen, A. S., Fuchs, D., & Tamkins, M. M. (2004). tion: Compensation for prejudice and attributions to discrimination.
Penalties for success: Reactions to women who succeed at male gender- Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1357–1367.
typed tasks. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 416–427. Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., & Lynn, D. E. (1995). Opiate systems in
Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirshbaum, C., & Ehlert, U. (2003). Social mother and infant primates coordinate intimate contact during reunion.
support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses Psychoneuroendocrinology, 20, 735–742.
to psychosocial stress. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 1389–1398. Kaplan, H. S., & Robson, A. J. (2002). The emergence of humans: The
Helfat, C. E., Harris, D., & Wolfson, P. J. (2006). The pipeline to the top: coevolution of intelligence and longevity with intergenerational
Women and men in the top executive ranks of U.S. corporations. transfers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99,
Academy of Management Perspectives, 20, 42–64. 10221–10226.
Helgeson, V. S., & Fritz, H. L. (1999). Unmitigated agency and unmiti- Kasen, S., Chen, H., Sneed, J., Crawford, T., & Cohen, P. (2006). Social
gated communion: Distinctions from agency and communion. Journal role and birth cohort influences on gender-linked personality traits in
of Research in Personality, 33, 131–158. women: A 20-year longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and
Hewlett, S. A. (2007). Off-ramps and on-ramps: Keeping talented women Social Psychology, 91, 944–958.
on the road to success. Boston: Harvard Business School Press. Keller, J., & Bless, H. (2005). When negative expectancies turn into nega-
Hines, M. (2009). Gonadal hormones and sexual differentiation of tive performance: The role of ease of retrieval. Journal of Experimental
human brain and behavior. In D. W. Pfaff, A. P. Arnold, A. M. Etgen, Social Psychology, 41, 535–541.
S. E. Fahrbach, & R. T. Rubin (Eds.), Hormones, brain, and behaviour Kendrick, K. M. (2004). The neurobiology of social bonds. Journal of
(2nd ed.). Oxford: Academic/Elsevier. Neuroendocrinology, 16, 1007–1008.
Hines, M., Brook, C., & Conway, G. S. (2004). Androgen and psychosex- Kenrick, D. T., Maner, J. K., & Li, N. P. (2005). Evolutionary social
ual development: Core gender identity, sexual orientation, and recalled psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary
childhood gender role behavior in women and men with congenital psychology (pp. 803–827) Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Journal of Sex Research, 41, 75–81.
Kenrick, D. T., Trost, M. R., & Sundie, J. M. (2004). Sex roles as adap-
House, J. S. (1995). Social structure, relationships, and the individual. In tations: An evolutionary perspective on gender differences and
K. S. Cook, G. A. Fine, & J. S. House (Eds.), Sociological perspectives similarities. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The
on social psychology (pp. 387–395). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. psychology of gender (2nd ed., pp. 65–91). New York: Guilford Press.
Hoyt, C. L., & Blascovich, J. (2007). Leadership efficacy and women Kessler, S. J., & McKenna, W. (1978). Gender: An ethnomethodological
leaders’ responses to stereotype activation. Group Processes and approach. New York: Wiley.
Intergroup Relations, 10, 595–616.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His
Huber, J. (2007). On the origins of gender inequality. Boulder, CO: and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 472–503.
Paradigm.
Kite, M. E., & Deaux, K. (1987). Gender belief systems: Homosexuality
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, and the implicit inversion theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11,
60, 581–592. 83–96.
Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, A. B., & Williams, C. C. Kite, M. E., Deaux, K., & Haines, E. L. (2007). Gender stereotypes. In
(2008). Diversity: Gender similarities characterize math performance. F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook
Science, 321, 494–495. of issues and theories (2nd ed., pp. 205–236). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Inglehart, R., & Norris, P. (2003). Rising tide: Gender equality and cultural Kite, M. E., Deaux, K., & Miele, M. (1991). Stereotypes of young and old:
change around the world. New York: Cambridge University Press. Does age outweigh gender? Psychology and Aging, 6, 19–27.
Insel, T. R. (2000). Toward a neurobiology of attachment. Review of Klonis, S. C., Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2005). Internal and exter-
General Psychology, 4, 176–185. nal motivation to respond without sexism. Personality and Social
Inzlicht, M., McKay, L., & Aronson, J. (2006). Stigma as ego depletion: Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1237–1249.
How being the target of prejudice affects self-control. Psychological Knight, G. P., Fabes, R. A., & Higgins, D. A. (1996). Concerns about drawing
Science, 17, 262–269. causal inferences from meta-analyses: An example in the study of gen-
Jackson, R. M. (2006). Opposing forces: How, why, and when will gender der differences in aggression. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 410–421.
inequality disappear? In F. D. Blau, M. C. Brinton, & D. B. Grusky Koenig, A. M., & Eagly, A. H. (2005). Stereotype threat in men on a test of
(Eds.), The declining significance of gender? (pp. 215–244). New social sensitivity. Sex Roles, 52, 489–496.
York: Russell Sage.
Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-
Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orientation: role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development
A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 703–726. of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Stanford, CA: Stanford University
Johns, M., Schmader, T., & Martens, A. (2005). Knowing is half the battle: Press.
Teaching stereotype threat as a means of improving women’s math per- Konrad, A. M., Ritchie, J. E., Jr., Lieb, P., & Corrigall, E. (2000). Sex
formance. Psychological Science, 16, 175–179. differences and similarities in job attribute preferences: A meta-
Johnson, W., Carothers, A., & Deary, I. J. (2008). Sex differences in vari- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 593–641.
ability in general intelligence. Perspectives in Psychological Science, Krueger, J. I., Hasman, J. F., Acevedo, M., & Villano, P. (2003). Perceptions
3, 518–531. of trait typicality in gender stereotypes: Examining the role of attribu-
Josephs, R. A., Sellers, J. G., Newman, M. L., & Mehta, P. H. (2006). The tion and categorization processes. Personality and Social Psychology
mismatch effect: When testosterone and status are at odds. Journal of Bulletin, 29, 108–116.
Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 999–1013. Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. J. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind and
Jost, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Sheldon, O., & Sullivan, B. N. (2003). Social when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for ste-
inequality and the reduction of ideological dissonance on behalf of the reotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 522–544.
References 663

LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Paluck, E. L. (2003). The contingent smile: Mazur, A., Susman, E. J., & Edelbrock, S. (1997). Sex differences in tes-
A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological Bulletin, tosterone response to a video game contest. Evolution and Human
129, 305–334. Behavior, 18, 317–326.
Lauzen, M. M., Dozier, D. M., & Horan, N. (2008). Constructing gender McCaffery, E. J. (1999). Taxing women. Chicago: University of Chicago
stereotypes through social roles in prime-time television. Journal of Press.
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 52, 200–214. McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 79 Members of the Personality Profiles
Leander, N. P., Chartrand, T. L., & Wood, W. (2009). Mind your manner- of Cultures Project. (2005). Personality profiles of cultures: Aggregate
isms: Eliciting stereotype conformity through behavioral mimicry. personality traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89,
Unpublished manuscript, Duke University, Durham, NC. 407–425.
Leaper, C., & Friedman, C. K. (2006). The socialization of gender. In McGuire, W. J. (1983). A contextualist theory of knowledge: Its impli-
J. Grusec & P. Hastings (Eds.), The handbook of socialization: Theory cations for innovation and reform in psychological research. In
and research (pp. 561–587). New York: Guilford Press. L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
Leszczynski, J. P., & Strough, J. (2008). The contextual specificity of mas- (pp. 1–47). New York: Academic.
culinity and femininity in early adolescence. Social Development, 17, McHenry, H. M., & Coffing, K. (2000). Australopithecus to Homo:
719–736. Transformations in body and mind. Annual Review of Anthropology,
Leyens, J., Désert, M., Croizet, J., & Darcis, C. (2000). Stereotype 29, 125–146.
threat: Are lower status and history of stigmatization preconditions of Mehta, P. H., Jones, A. C., & Josephs, R. A. (2008). The social endocri-
stereotype threat? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, nology of dominance: Basal testosterone predicts cortisol changes and
1189–1199. behavior following victory and defeat. Journal of Personality
Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2003). Developmental dynamics: Toward and Social Psychology, 94, 1078–1093.
a biologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psychological Mendoza-Denton, R., Park, S. H., & O’Connor, A. (2008). Gender stereo-
Bulletin, 129, 819–835. types as situation–behavior profiles. Journal of Experimental Social
Lightdale, J. R., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). Rethinking sex differences Psychology, 44, 971–982.
in aggression: Aggressive behavior in the absence of social roles. Milkie, M. A., Bianchi, S. M., Mattingly, M. J., & Robinson, J. P. (2002).
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 34–44. Gendered division of childrearing: Ideals, realities, and the relationship
Lippa, R. A. (2001). Gender-related traits in transsexuals and nontrans- to parental well-being. Sex Roles, 47, 21–38.
sexuals. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 30, 603–614. Miller, C. F., Trautner, H. M., & Ruble, D. N. (2006). The role of gen-
Lippa, R. A. (2005). Gender, nature, and nurture. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence der stereotypes in children’s preferences and behavior. In L. Balter &
Erlbaum. C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of con-
temporary issues (2nd ed., pp. 293–323). New York: Psychology Press.
Lippa, R. A. (2007). The preferred traits of mates in a cross-national study
of heterosexual and homosexual men and women: An examination of Miller, D. T., & Turnbull, W. (1986). Expectancies and interpersonal pro-
biological and cultural influences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, cesses. Annual Review of Psychology, 37, 233–256.
193–208. Money, J., Hampson, J. G., & Hampson, J. L. (1955). Hermaphroditism:
Lippa, R. A. (in press). Sex differences in personality traits and gender- Recommendations concerning assignment of sex, change of sex and
related occupational preferences across 53 nations: Testing evolutionary psychologic management. Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin, 97,
and social-environmental theories. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 284–300.
Lueptow, L. B., Garovich-Szabo, L., & Lueptow, M. B. (2001). Social Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (1997). Two decades of cultivation research: An
change and the persistence of sex-typing: 1974–1997. Social Forces, appraisal and meta-analysis. In B. R. Burleson (Ed.), Communication
80, 1–36. yearbook 20 (pp. 1–45). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self- Morier, D., & Seroy, C. (1994). The effect of interpersonal expectancies on
evaluation of one’s social identity. Personality and Social Psychology men’s self-presentation of gender role attitudes to women. Sex Roles,
Bulletin, 18, 302–318. 31, 493–504.
Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socializa- Moskowitz, D. S., Suh, E. J., & Desaulniers, J. (1994). Situational influ-
tion of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, ences on gender differences in agency and communion. Journal of
267–296. Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 753–761.
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Moskowitz, G. B., Gollwitzer, P. M., Wasel, W., & Schaal, B. (1999).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Preconscious control of stereotype activation through chronic egali-
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., Thorne, T. M. J., & tarian goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
Castelli, L. (1997). On the activation of social stereotypes: The mod- 167–184.
erating role of processing objectives. Journal of Experimental Social Mukhopadhyay, C. C., & Higgins, P. J. (1988). Anthropological studies of
Psychology, 33, 471–489. women’s status revisited: 1977–1987. Annual Review of Anthropology,
Marecek, J., Crawford, M., & Popp, D. (2004). On the construction of gender, 17, 461–495.
sex, and sexualities. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg Murdock, G. P., & Provost, C. (1973). Factors in the division of labor by
(Eds.), The psychology of gender (2nd ed., pp. 192–216). New York: sex: A cross-cultural analysis. Ethnology, 13, 203–225.
Guilford Press.
Murphy, M. C., Steele, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat: How
Martin, C. L. (1987). A ratio measure of sex stereotyping. Journal of situational cues affect women in math, science, and engineering set-
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 489–499. tings. Psychological Science, 18, 879–885.
Matta, S., & Folkes, V. (2005). Inferences about the brand from counter- National Collegiate Athletic Association. (2008). 2005–06 NCAA gen-
stereotypical service providers. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, der equity report. Retrieved November 10, 2008, from http://www.
196–206. ncaa.org/wps/wcm/connect/0462e7804e0d4e469171f11ad6fc8b25/
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. GenderEquityRept-Final.pdf?MOD=AJPERES&CACHEID=0462e78
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353–397. 04e0d4e469171f11ad6fc8b25
664 Gender

Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2005). Gender differences in social support: Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women should be, shouldn’t
A question of skill or responsiveness? Journal of Personality and be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive
Social Psychology, 88, 79–90. gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281.
Newport, F. (2001, February 21). Americans see women as emotional and Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2006). Essentializing differences between
affectionate, men as more aggressive: Gender specific stereotypes per- women and men. Psychological Science, 17, 129–135.
sist in recent Gallup poll. Gallup Brain. Retrieved September 13, 2008, Prislin, R., & Wood, W. (2005). Social influence: The role of social consen-
from http://brain.gallup.com sus in attitudes and attitude change. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, &
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche con- M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Handbook of attitudes and attitude change
struction: The neglected process in evolution (Monographs in popula- (pp. 671–706). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
tion biology). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Pryor, J., Hurtado, S., Saenz, V., Santos, K., & Korn, W. (2006). The
Ogburn, W. F. (1964). Social change with respect to culture and original American freshman: Forty year trends. Los Angeles: Higher Education
nature. Gloucester, MA: Peter Smith. (Original work published 1922.) Research Institute, University of California at Los Angeles.
Olson, J. M., Roese, N. J., & Zanna, M. P. (1996). Expectancies. In Puts, D. A., McDaniel, M. A., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (2008).
E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook Spatial and prenatal androgens: Meta-analyses of congenital adre-
of basic principles (pp. 211–238). New York: Guilford Press. nal hyperplasia and digit ratio (2D:4D) studies. Archives of Sexual
Opie, K., & Power, C. (2008). Grandmothering and female coalitions: Behavior, 37, 100–111.
A basis for matrilineal priority? In N. J. Allen, H. Callan, R. Dunbar, & Queneau, H. (2006). Is the long-term reduction in occupational sex seg-
W. James (Eds.), Early human kinship: From sex to reproduction regation still continuing in the United States? Social Science Journal,
(pp. 168–186). Malden, MA: Blackwell. 43, 681–688.
Oxford English Dictionary. (2009). Sex. Oxford English Dictionary Quinn, K. A., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). Categorizing others: The dynamics
Online. Retrieved September 30, 2009, from http://www.askoxford. of person construal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,
com/concise_oed/sexx?view=uk 467–479.
Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., Jr., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred
Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by years of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General
parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congeni- Psychology, 7, 331–363.
tal adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76, 264–276. Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture
Pasterski, V., Hindmarsh, P., Geffner, M., Brook, C., Brain, C., & Hines, M. transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
(2007). Increased aggression and activity level in 3- to 11-year-old girls Richman, L. S., van Dellen, M., & Wood, W. (in press). How women cope:
with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Hormones and Behavior, Being a numerical minority in a male-dominated profession. Journal
52, 368–374. of Social Issues.
Pedersen, C. A. (2004). Biological aspects of social bonding and the roots Ridgeway, C. L. (2006a). Gender as an organizing force in social relations:
of human violence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Implications for the future of inequality. In F. D. Blau, M. C. Brinton,
1036, 106–127. & D. B. Grusky (Eds.), The declining significance of gender? (pp.
Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). A longitudinal study of boys’ rough-and-tumble 245–264). New York: Russell Sage.
play and dominance during early adolescence. Journal of Applied Ridgeway, C. L. (2006b). Status construction theory. In P. J. Burke (Ed.),
Developmental Psychology, 16, 77–93. Contemporary social psychological theories (pp. 301–323). Stanford,
Pellis, S. M., & Pellis, V. C. (2007). Rough-and-tumble play and the CA: Stanford University Press.
development of the social brain. Current Directions in Psychological Ridgeway, C. L., & Bourg, C. (2004). Gender as status: An expectation
Science, 16, 95–98. states theory approach. In A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg
Pew Research Center. (2008). Men or women: Who’s the better (Eds.), The psychology of gender (2nd ed., pp. 217–241). New York:
leader? A paradox in public attitudes. Pew Research Center Publ- Guilford Press.
ications. Retrieved September 30, 2008, from http://pewresearch. Ridgeway, C. L., & Erickson, K. G. (2000). Creating and spreading status
org/pubs/932/men-or-women-whos-the-better-leader beliefs. American Journal of Sociology, 106, 579–615.
Plant, E. A., Hyde, J. S., Keltner, D., & Devine, P. G. (2000). The gen- Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer
der stereotyping of emotions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and
81–92. behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin,
Plavcan, J. M. (2000). Inferring social behavior from sexual dimorphism in 132, 98–131.
the fossil record. Journal of Human Evolution, 39, 327–344. Ross, L. D., Amabile, T. M., & Steinmetz, J. L. (1977). Social roles,
Plavcan, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (1997). Interpreting hominid behavior social control, and biases in social-perception processes. Journal of
on the basis of sexual dimorphism. Journal of Human Evolution, 32, Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 485–494.
345–374. Roter, D. L., Hall, J. A., & Aoki, Y. (2002). Physician gender effects in
Plavcan, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2005). Canine dimorphism. Evolutionary medical communication: A meta-analytic review. Journal of the
Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 2, 208–214. American Medical Association, 288, 756–764.
Polachek, S. W. (2006). How the life-cycle human-capital model explains Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender
why the gender wage gap narrowed. In F. D. Blau, M. C. Brinton, & development. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.),
D. B. Grusky (Eds.), The declining significance of gender? Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality
(pp. 102–124). New York: Russell Sage. development (Vol. 3, 6th ed., pp. 858–932). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Potts, R. (1998). Environmental hypotheses of hominin evolution. Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 27, 93–136. costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management.
Powell, G. N., Butterfield, D. A., & Parent, J. D. (2002). Gender and mana- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 629–645.
gerial stereotypes: Have the times changed? Journal of Management, Rudman, L. A., & Borgida. G. (1995). The afterglow of construct acces-
28, 177–193. sibility: The behavioral consequences of priming men to view women
References 665

as sexual objects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, Sisk, C. L., & Zehr, J. L. (2005). Pubertal hormones organize the ado-
493–517. lescent brain and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26,
Rudman, L. A., & Fairchild, K. (2004). Reactions to counterstereotypic 163–174.
behavior: The role of backlash in cultural stereotype maintenance. Skrypnek, B. J., & Snyder, M. (1982). On the self-perpetuating nature of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 157–176. stereotypes about women and men. Journal of Experimental Social
Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and back- Psychology, 18, 277–291.
lash toward agentic women. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 743–762. Smith, T. W. (2007). Social identity and socio-demographic structure.
Rudman, L. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender differences in automatic International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 19, 380–390.
in-group bias: Why do women like women more than men like men? Spence, J. T., & Buckner, C. E. (2000). Instrumental and expressive traits,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 494–509. trait stereotypes, and sexist attitudes. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., & McGhee, D. E. (2001). Implicit self- 24, 44–62.
concept and evaluative implicit gender stereotypes: Self and ingroup Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity:
share desirable traits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Austin:
27, 1164–1178. University of Texas Press.
Ryan, C. S. (2002). Stereotype accuracy. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holohan, C. K. (1979). Negative and
(Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 75–109). positive components of psychological masculinity and femininity
Hove, England: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis. and their relationships to self-reports of neurotic and acting out behav-
Salzman, P. C. (1999). Is inequality universal? Current Anthropology, 40, iors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1673–1682.
31–61. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat
Schein, V. E. (2001). A global look at psychological barriers to women’s and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social
progress in management. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 675–688. Psychology, 35, 4–28.
Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype Stahlberg, D., Braun, F., Irmen, L., & Sczesny, S. (2007). Representation of
threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and the sexes in language. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Social communication (pp.
Social Psychology, 85, 440–452. 163–187). New York: Psychology Press.
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model Stangor, C., Lynch, L., Duan, C., & Glass, B. (1992). Categorization of indi-
of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, viduals on the basis of multiple social features. Journal of Personality
115, 336–356. and Social Psychology, 62, 207–218.
Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man be Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual
more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 168–182. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual
Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and
Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 87–115. Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.
Schwartz, S. H., & Rubel, T. (2005). Sex differences in value priorities: Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with
Cross-cultural and multimethod studies. Journal of Personality and group image: The psychology of stereotype and identity threat. In
Social Psychology, 89, 1010–1028. M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34,
Scott, K. A., & Brown, D. J. (2006). Female first, leader second? Gender pp. 379–400). San Diego: Academic Press.
bias in the encoding of leadership behavior. Organizational Behavior Sterelny, K. (2003): Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human
and Human Decision Processes, 101, 230–242. cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.
Sechrist, G. B., & Stangor, C. (2001). Perceived consensus influences inter- Stewart, A. J., & McDermott, C. (2004). Gender in psychology. Annual
group behavior and stereotype accessibility. Journal of Personality Review of Psychology, 55, 519–544.
and Social Psychology, 80, 645–654. Storey, A. E., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2000).
Seguino, S. (2007). Plus ça change? Evidence on global trends in gender Hormonal correlates of paternal responsiveness in new and expectant
norms and stereotypes. Feminist Economics, 13, 1–28. fathers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 79–95.
Sekaquaptewa, D., & Thompson, M. (2002). The differential effects of solo Stricker, L. J., & Ward, W. C. (2004). Stereotype threat, inquiring about
status on members of low- and high-status groups. Personality and test takers’ ethnicity and gender, and standardized test performance.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 694–707. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 665–693.
Sellen, D. W. (2007). Evolution of infant and young child feeding: Suay, F., Salvador, A., Gonzalez-Bono, E., Sanchís, C., Martínez, M.,
Implications for contemporary public health. Annual Review of Martínez-Sanchís, S., et al. (1999). Effects of competition and its out-
Nutrition, 27, 123–148. come on serum testosterone, cortisol, and prolactin. Psychoneuroendo-
Shackelford, S., Wood, W., & Worchel, S. (1996). Behavioral styles and crinology, 24, 551–566.
the influence of women in mixed-sex groups. Social Psychology Sweeney, M. M. (2002). Two decades of family change: The shifting eco-
Quarterly, 59, 284–293. nomic foundations of marriage. American Sociological Review, 67,
Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: 132–147.
Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An assessment
Science, 10, 80–83. of the accuracy of gender stereotypes. Journal of Personality and
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory Social Psychology, 66, 21–36.
of social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cambridge University Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in
Press. the social psychology of intergroup relations. London: Academic
Sinclair, S., Hardin, C. D., & Lowery, B. S. (2006). Self-stereotyping in the Press.
context of multiple social identities. Journal of Personality and Social Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social
Psychology, 90, 529–542. psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
666 Gender

Takagi, T., Tanizawa, O., Otsuki, Y., Sugita, N., Haruta, M., & Yamaji, U.S. National Center for Education Statistics. (2007a). Digest of
K. (1985). Oxytocin in the cerebrospinal fluid and plasma of pregnant education statistics: 2007. Table 258. Degrees conferred by
and non-pregnant subjects. Hormone and Metabolism Research, 17, degree-granting institutions, by level of degree and sex of student:
308–310. Selected years, 1869–70 through 2016–17. Retrieved November 10,
Taylor, M. C., & Hall, J. A. (1982). Psychological androgyny: Theories, 2008, from http://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d07/tables/dt07_258.
methods, and conclusions. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 347–366. asp?referrer=list
Taylor, S. E. (2002). The tending instinct: How nurturing is essential to U.S. National Center for Education Statistics. (2007b). Digest of education
who we are and how we live. New York: Holt. statistics: 2007. Table 383. Percentage of 1972 high school seniors,
1992 high school seniors, and 2004 high school seniors who felt that
Taylor, S. E., Gonzaga, G. C., Klein, L. C., Hu, P., Greendale, G. A., &
certain life values were “very important,” by sex: Selected years,
Seeman, T. E. (2006). Relation of oxytocin to psychological stress
1972 through 2004. Retrieved November 10, 2008, from http://nces.
responses and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis activity in
ed.gov/programs/digest/d07/tables/dt07_383.asp?referrer=list
older women. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 238–245.
Vandello, J. A., Bosson, J. K., Cohen, D., Burnaford, R. M., & Weaver, J. R.
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A. R., &
(2008). Precarious manhood. Journal of Personality and Social
Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-
Psychology, 95, 1325–1339.
and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107, 411–429.
von Hippel, W., Sekaquaptewa, D., & Vargas, P. (1995). On the role of
Timberlake, J. M., & Estes, S. B. (2007). Do racial and ethnic stereotypes
encoding processes in stereotype maintenance. In M. Zanna (Ed.),
depend on the sex of target group members? Evidence from a survey-
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 27, pp. 177–254).
based experiment. Sociological Quarterly, 48, 399–433.
San Diego: Academic Press.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005).
Vonk, R., & Ashmore, R. D. (2003). Thinking about gender types:
Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cogni-
Cognitive organization of female and male types. British Journal of
tion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.
Social Psychology, 42, 257–280.
Tomaskovic-Devey, D., Zimmer, C., Stainback, K., Robinson, C., Taylor, T., &
Wallen, K. (1996). Nature needs nurture: The interaction of hormonal and
McTague, T. (2006). Documenting desegregation: Segregation in
social influences on the development of behavioral sex differences in
American workplaces by race, ethnicity, and sex, 1966–2003. American
rhesus monkeys. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 364–378.
Sociological Review, 71, 565–588.
Wallen, K. (2005). Hormonal influences on sexually differentiated
Turner, K., & Brown, C. (2007). The centrality of gender and ethnic
behavior in nonhuman primates. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology,
identities across individuals and contexts. Social Development, 16,
26, 7–26.
700–719.
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2003). Stereotype lift. Journal of
Twenge, J. M. (1997). Changes in masculine and feminine traits over time:
Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 456–467.
A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 36, 305–325.
Welch, V., Jr. (2008). Doctorate recipients from United States universities:
Twenge, J. M. (2001). Changes in women’s assertiveness in response to
Selected tables 2007. National Opinion Research Center. Retrieved
status and roles: A cross-temporal meta-analysis, 1931–1993. Journal
September 30, 2009, from http://www.norc.uchicago.edu/NR/
of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 133–145.
rdonlyres/2D5FD7C8-4AE0-4932-B777-0BC8EA7965EF/0/2007_
Uleman, J. S., Saribay, S. A., & Gonzalez, C. M. (2008). Spontaneous selectedtabs.pdf
inferences, implicit impressions, and implicit theories. Annual Review
West, C., & Zimmerman, D. H. (1987). Doing gender. Gender & Society,
of Psychology, 59, 329–360.
1, 125–151.
Unger, R. K. (1979). Toward a redefinition of sex and gender. American
Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds: The
Psychologist, 34, 1085–1094.
formation of social behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2008a). Current population survey:
Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. (1975). Children of six cultures: A psycho-
Household data annual averages. Table 10. Employed persons by
cultural analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
detailed occupation, race, Hispanic or Latino ethnicity, and sex.
Retrieved February 15, 2009, from http://www.bls.gov/cps/cpsaat10.pdf Wilder, D. A. (1984). Predictions of belief homogeneity and similarity
following social categorization. British Journal of Social Psychology,
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2008b). News: Married parents’ use of
23, 323–333.
time, 2003–06. Retrieved December 10, 2008, from http://www.bls.
gov/news.release/pdf/atus2.pdf Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1990). Measuring sex stereotypes: A multi-
nation study. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2009). Current population survey:
Household data seasonally adjusted. Table A-3: Employment status Witt, M. G., & Wood, W. (in press). Self-regulation of gender in daily life.
of the civilian noninstitutional population by sex and age, seasonally Sex Roles.
adjusted. Retrieved February 15, 2009, from http://www.bls.gov/web/ Wood, W. (1987). Meta-analytic review of sex differences in group perfor-
cpseea3.pdf mance. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 53–71.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2006). Table MS-2. Estimated median age at first Wood, W., Christensen, P. N., Hebl, M. R., & Rothgerber, H. (1997).
marriage, by sex: 1890 to the present. Internet release. Retrieved Conformity to sex-typed norms, affect, and the self-concept. Journal
September 29, 2007, from http://www.census.gov/population/socdemo/ of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 523–535.
hh-fam/ms2.pdf Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior
U.S. Federal Bureau of Investigation. (2008). 2008 Crime in the United of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences.
States: Arrests. Retrieved September 30, 2009, from http://www.fbi. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 699–727.
gov/ucr/cius2008/arrests/index.html Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2007). Social structural origins of sex
U.S. Health Resources and Services Administration. (2005). Women’s differences in human mating. In S. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.),
health USA 2005. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies
Human Services. (pp. 383–390). New York: Guilford Press.
References 667

Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2009). Gender identity. In M. Leary & R. Hoyle (Eds.), Woodson, J. C. (2002). Including “learned sexuality” in the organization
Handbook of individual differences (pp. 109–128). New York: Guilford Press. of sexual behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26,
Wood, W., & Karten, S. J. (1986). Sex differences in interaction style 69–80.
as a product of perceived sex differences in competence. Journal of Wootton, B. H. (1997). Gender differences in occupational employment,
Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 341–347. Monthly Labor Review, 120(4), 15–24.
Wood, W., Rhodes, N., & Whelan, M. (1989). Sex differences in Zanna, M. P., & Pack, S. J. (1975). On the self-fulfilling nature of appar-
positive well-being: A consideration of emotional style and marital ent sex differences in behavior. Journal of Experimental Social
status. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 249–264. Psychology, 11, 583–591.
Chapter 18

Personality in Social Psychology


DAVID C. FUNDER AND LISA A. FAST

Social psychology and personality psychology have Roger Brown described one memorably awkward encounter
the same job: to seek to understand the meaningful, con- between the two traditions:
sequential, and mostly social behaviors of daily life.
Cognitive psychology examines component processes such As a psychologist, in all the years . . . I had thought individual
as memory, perception, and cognition. Biological psychol- differences in personality were exaggerated. . . . I had once
ogy seeks to understand the physical underpinnings of presumed to say to Henry A. Murray, Harvard’s distinguished
behavior in the anatomy, physiology, functional organiza- personologist: “I think people are all very much the same.”
tion, genetic basis, and evolutionary history of the nervous Murray’s response had been; “Oh you do, do you? Well, you
system. Developmental psychology explores the roots of don’t know what the hell you’re talking about!” And I hadn’t.
behavior in genetics and early childhood experience and (Brown, 1996, p. 169)
changes across the life course. All of these fields could be
viewed as foundational for the common concern of social This little exchange illustrates the odd historical fact
and personality psychology, which is to understand what that although social and personality psychology were born
people do every day. In this light, it is unsurprising that about the same time, of the same or closely related par-
courses in social and personality psychology are among ents, the relationship between these sibling sciences often
the most popular offerings on most college campuses; their has been uneasy, bordering at times on outright estrange-
subject matter is not only important but also personally rel- ment. This is unfortunate given that the two fields not only
evant and intrinsically interesting. share a common goal but also offer complementary—not
Social and personality psychology began to come into conflicting—methodological approaches.
their own about the same time—the 1920s and 1930s— At their core, social and personality psychology focus
through the work of many of the same people, such as the on two orthogonal main effects. On the one hand, the clas-
Allport brothers, Floyd and Gordon (F. H. Allport, 1924; sic method of social psychology uses experimental designs
G. W. Allport, 1931, 1937; F. H. Allport & G. W. Allport, that manipulate elements of situations to show how those
1921). What is surprising, in retrospect, is how the two fields elements affect what people do. On the other hand, the
diverged over the subsequent decades. Social psychology classic method of personality psychology uses correla-
came to specialize in the study of what people have in com- tional methods to assess how psychological properties of
mon, in particular how aspects of situations can change people—personality traits—covary with individual differ-
what people, on average, will do. Personality psychology ences in behavior. Arguments about whether the situational
came to specialize in the study of how people differ from effects uncovered by social psychological research are or
one another psychologically and ways to characterize and are not stronger than the dispositional effects uncovered
measure these differences. This division of labor makes a by personality research dominated an important subset of
certain amount of sense, but problems arose as the fields the psychological literature for decades. The best avail-
gradually became so specialized that many practitioners of able evidence indicates that at a hugely aggregate level the
each field became unaware of the basic principles, find- effect sizes in both fields average out to be about the same
ings, and methods of the other. The difficulties grew worse (Richard, Bond, & Stokes-Zoota, 2003). But that is get-
when social psychologists began to suspect that personal- ting ahead of our story. Ordinary observation of the social
ity psychology’s emphasis on individual differences was world is enough to verify that (1) people do different things
misguided. In his memoirs, the eminent social psychologist in different situations and (2) even in the same situation,

668
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Basic Approaches to Studying Personality 669

different people often do different things. And those two to look at every possible aspect of personality at the same
conclusions are enough to verify that a complete under- time, personality research proceeds along different theo-
standing of why people behave the way they do naturally retical avenues. Some researchers examine the biological
requires social and personality psychology to be informed underpinnings of personality, others look at developmental
by each other. trajectories, others examine how the environment affects
The goal of the present chapter is to help to rebuild the personality, others study how people differ in how they
bridge between social and personality psychology. The chap- perceive and process information, and still others—and all
ter is organized into six parts. The first three parts provide a of them, in some sense—seek to discover and assess the
basic outline of personality psychology and an overview of basic psychological dimensions along which individuals
some current research. The first part defines the field, and differ. All of these areas of research are similar in that they
the second part describes the basic conceptual and theoreti- focus on individual differences and patterns of behavior
cal approaches to studying personality. It is proposed that, to but are guided by different paradigmatic frameworks that
the degree that each basic approach to personality represents specify which phenomena are the focus of attention (e.g.,
empirical science, they all depend on the assessment of indi- particular traits and behaviors) and which mechanisms are
vidual differences through behavior. This dependency puts used for explanation (e.g., genes vs. the environment vs.
the trait approach at the center of personality psychology. The cognition). The basic approaches to studying personality
third part discusses current research and outlines some ways are biological, psychoanalytic, humanistic, learning based,
in which behavior has been used to assess personality. These cognitive, and trait based (Funder, 2007).
not only include the prominent method of self-report but also Although the different approaches sometimes com-
include peers’ judgments and other, wider-ranging and cre- pete with one another for the ultimate status of explaining
ative techniques for observing and measuring behavior. everything there is to know about personality, the reality is
The last three parts of this chapter deal with the com- that different research questions are better addressed through
petition that has characterized the relationship between different paradigmatic perspectives. For example, the
personality and social psychology for the past 40 years or principles of behaviorism can be used to explain how gam-
so. The fourth part describes the intersection of personal- bling behavior is maintained but say nothing about why
ity and social psychology. It focuses on research in person those who have gambling addictions are often unable
perception and accurate personality judgment and the con- to admit that they have a problem. In contrast, psycho-
trast between these two traditions. The fifth part outlines analysis has much to say about denial and other defense
the basis and unfortunate evolution of the estrangement mechanisms but offers little toward understanding how the
between personality and social psychology, which appears intermittent reinforcement schedule associated with gam-
to be slowly ending. The final part offers suggestions for bling can make this maladaptive behavior so persistent.
reintegrating these fields toward a relationship that can Therefore, it makes more sense to view each approach
become more cooperative and less competitive. as useful for addressing its own key concerns rather than
viewing them as mutually exclusive alternatives.

PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY
BASIC APPROACHES TO STUDYING
Personality can be defined as an individual’s characteristic PERSONALITY
patterns of thought, emotion, and behavior, together with
the psychological mechanisms—hidden or not—behind Biological Approach
those patterns (Funder, 2007). The goal of personality psy-
chology is to explain every individual from the inside out. The biological approach to studying personality searches
The mission includes describing, measuring, and explain- for the organic roots of individual differences using anat-
ing how people differ from one another; uncovering omy, physiology, genetics, and evolutionary theory.
the conscious and unconscious thoughts and feelings that
Anatomy
drive behavior; and predicting what people will do in the
future. But this mission has one problem: It is impossible. Research focusing on anatomy attempts to identify brain
The complete study of the individual encompasses too many structures that play a role in various personality traits. For
considerations at once to be feasibly pursued by investiga- example, research using functional magnetic resonance
tors with human limitations of time and intelligence. imaging (fMRI) has shown that shy people, compared with
One way to make personality research more manageable people described as “bold,” respond to pictures of unfamil-
is to divide it into organized chunks. Rather than trying iar people with bilateral activation of the amygdala and to
670 Personality in Social Psychology

pictures of familiar people with activation on just the left Physiology


side of this organ (Beaton et al., 2008). Bolder individu- Biological research on personality also addresses physi-
als respond to pictures of familiar and unfamiliar people ology, examining biochemicals (neurotransmitters and
with stronger activation in their nucleus accumbens com- hormones) that might be associated with individual differ-
pared with activation in shy people. Research by Barrett ences in behavior. Dopamine and serotonin are widely stud-
(2006) also shows that the amygdala plays an important ied neurotransmitters. Research suggests that dopamine is
role in positive emotions such as sexual responsiveness. involved in the experience of reward and the reinforce-
Another intriguing finding is that activity in the left fron- ment of behavior (Blum, Cull, Braverman, & Comings,
tal lobe appears to be associated with pleasant emotion 1996) while serotonin plays a role in emotional regulation
and motivation to approach attractive people and objects and feelings of well-being (Knutson et al., 1998). The hor-
while activity in the right frontal lobe seems to be associ- mone testosterone has received considerable attention; it
ated with unpleasant emotion and motivation to withdraw appears to play important roles in sexual behavior and in
(Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis, & Frisesen, 1990; aggression (Zuckerman, 1991, and Dabbs & Morris, 1990,
Hewig, Hagemann, Seifert, Naumann, & Bartussek, 2004). respectively). Cortisol, the well-known “fight or flight”
Areas of the brain traditionally associated with emotional hormone associated with anxiety, fear, and aggressive
responsiveness (e.g., the posterior cingulate and the insula) response, appears to be surprisingly low in shy individuals
appear to be particularly active in response to images rele- (Beaton et al., 2006). But it also is low in people high on
vant to rejection in individuals who suffer from a syndrome the trait of sensation seeking, so the situation, as always,
known as rejection sensitivity (Kross, Egner, Ochsner, is complicated (Zuckerman, 1998). Like the fMRI work
Hirsch, & Downey, 2007). surveyed earlier, these studies are tantalizingly sugges-
Findings like these continue to accumulate rapidly in tive of the possible chemical bases of aggression, sexual
the research literature and are yielding the beginning of a response, and motivation, and of personality traits such
map of locations in the brain that might be the basis of as aggressiveness, hypersexuality, depression, emotional
specific personality traits—the amygdala for emotionally resiliency, and shyness. Their findings can have direct
relevant traits, hemispheric dominance for overall positive implications for therapeutic interventions; for example,
and negative affectivity, the posterior cingulate for rejec- drugs to increase levels of the neurotransmitter serotonin
tion sensitivity, and so on. The findings are complex, how- (selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors such as Prozac,
ever, and the intricate experimental controls that this kind generically fluoxetine) and doses of the hormone testoster-
of research requires and the typical focus, in a single study, one have been used in the treatment of depression.
on just one or a few brain regions makes interpretation and Although psychophysiology has provided insights about
firm conclusions difficult. Questions have also been raised the biological basis of behavior and individual differences
about the way effect sizes in this literature are calculated. in personality traits, researchers must be careful about
Sometimes quite dramatic correlations in the range of .70 inferring causal relationships. For example, Bernhardt,
to .90 between levels of brain activation and personality Dabbs, Fielden, and Lutter (1998) found that, after watch-
have been reported, but these numbers are probably exag- ing a World Cup playoff game, fans of the winning soccer
gerated (Vul, Harris, Winkielman, & Pashler, 2009). team had higher testosterone levels than fans of the los-
Moreover, the larger implications for personality theory ers. And psychotherapy can change measurable aspects of
have yet to become clear. To put the matter bluntly: If shy- brain activity (Isom & Heller, 1999). Findings like these
ness is indeed associated with specific processes in the suggest that biology is not just a cause of individual dif-
amygdala, for example, what difference does that make? ferences in behavior and psychological experience; it is
In what way does this finding lead us to think differently also an effect. Neuroanatomy, physiology, and patterns of
about shyness? Indeed, some researchers have worried behavior and experience are complicated phenomena, and
that fMRI and other imaging technologies yield a “new the relationship between personality and biology is surely
phrenology” that produces brain maps in lieu of psycho- even more complex, with causal arrows pointing in both
logical insight (Uttal, 2001). The challenges for the next directions.
generation of research are to use these intriguing findings
to illuminate aspects of personality that were not previ-
Behavioral Genetics
ously apparent and to outline psychological processes and
interactions among them that are not detectable from overt Behavioral genetics and evolutionary psychology both
behavioral data alone. Modern imaging technology offers focus on the inheritance of individual differences in
a theoretical promissory note that will someday be paid behavior. For the good and the bad, we are more similar to
but, to date, remains to be cashed. people with whom we share more genes (e.g., our parents)
Basic Approaches to Studying Personality 671

than fewer genes (e.g., our cousins). We look like our par- their mate being emotionally unfaithful than sexually
ents, we are more likely to have high blood pressure if our unfaithful, whereas males are more distressed by imagin-
parents do, and we even have an IQ level similar to that ing sexual infidelity than emotional infidelity. The expla-
of our parents. Behavioral genetics extends this knowl- nation for this gender difference is that attending to cues of
edge and studies the following question: Are those who sexual infidelity (becoming distressed) resulted in greater
are more genetically similar (e.g., monozygotic twins) reproductive fitness for males in evolutionary history
more similar in personality compared with those who are because males face paternal uncertainty. It was more costly
less genetically similar (e.g., dizygotic twins)? Decades for a male to mate with a female who might be mating
of research has established that most and perhaps all per- with other males and possibly invest in offspring who were
sonality traits are heritable to some degree. Indeed, one not his own than to mate with a female who might form
authoritative researcher seriously suggested that “the first an emotional attachment with another male. Attending to
law of behavioral genetics” should be everything is heri- cues of emotional infidelity, however, resulted in greater
table (Turkheimer, 1998, p. 789). Genes matter, to at least reproductive fitness for females because females do not
some degree, to any psychological outcome and certainly face parental uncertainty. It was more costly for a female
any personality trait. to mate with a partner who might form an emotional bond
Having established this fact, current research is directed with another female and fail to provide resources for her
toward more fine-tuned questions, such as how genes affect offspring than to mate with a male who might have other
personality and how genes and the environment interact to sexual partners. Although the male might have other off-
influence personality outcomes. For example, Caspi and spring, his emotional attachment ensures that he provides
colleagues (2002) found that boys whose genes caused a resources to the female’s offspring and thus promotes her
low level of expression of an enzyme called monoamine genetic fitness.
oxidase (MAO) were more likely to be antisocial if they Are the conclusions of evolutionary psychology rel-
were maltreated as children. If, however, their genes caused evant to personality? If one assumes that men and women
a high level of expression of MAO, they were protected have different “personalities”—and they certainly are indi-
to some degree from such adverse effects. As the field of viduals who, as a group, differ from each other—then the
behavioral genetics continues to develop, the goal will be answer would seem to be yes. Evolutionary theorizing pro-
to generate increasingly fine-grained accounts, such as the vides an explanation of one area of behavior in which the
one just emerging concerning MAO, of how genes inter- big two groups of humans appear to be characteristically
act with the environment to create brain structures and different. But mostly personality is defined at a level more
aspects of physiology that lead to individual differences in specific than “typical male” or “typical female,” and it is
behavior. less clear how to apply evolutionary theorizing to explana-
tions of personality traits. Indeed, some researchers have
Evolutionary Psychology argued that evolutionary theory almost implies that indi-
Evolutionary psychology studies behavioral patterns pro- vidual differences do not matter, because any traits that
posed to have been adaptive during the development of the were disadvantageous for survival and reproduction should
human species. It assumes that behaviors that are common have been selected out of the gene pool long ago (Tooby &
to humans (1) have a genetic basis and (2) increased the Cosmides, 1990).
likelihood of survival, reproduction, or both during evo- However, a trait that is disadvantageous in one context
lutionary history. The more a behavior helps an individual may be helpful in another. A tendency to be agreeable
survive and reproduce, the more likely the behavior is to might make one more susceptible to being swindled but
be genetically transmitted and, therefore, appear in sub- also would allow one to make more friends. As a result,
sequent generations. Evolutionary psychology has par- through the generations, people high and low in agreeable-
ticularly focused on variation in sexual behavior between ness both tend to succeed, in different settings, and across
males and females. It is commonly hypothesized that gen- individuals differences between people high and low on
der differences in behavior that are still present today exist this trait continue to be found.
because, in the history of evolution, the behaviors that Another evolutionary explanation for the existence of
increased the likelihood of reproduction for males were individual differences is that some traits may evolve as
different from the behaviors that increased the likelihood responses to particular environmental contingencies and
of reproduction for females. are designed to come “online” only under certain circum-
Sexual jealousy has been a hot topic in evolutionary stances, just as one develops calluses on one’s hands only
research. Buss, Larsen, Westen, and Semmelroth (1992) if they are used in manual work (Buss & Greiling, 1999).
observed that females are more distressed by imagining For example, many of us might have a latent tendency to
672 Personality in Social Psychology

be aggressive, but only a few people have experienced decisions relevant to survival and reproduction would not
environments that have brought that trait into behavioral have allowed its body to pass genes to succeeding genera-
reality—presumably, environments similar to those that in tions. This line of reasoning suggests that the basic mecha-
evolutionary history made aggressiveness advantageous. nisms of cognitive inference that have survived millennia
Similarly, potential traits might or might not develop of harsh environments and reproductive competition are
depending on whether they will promote outcomes for more likely to be adaptive than fundamentally flawed
particular individuals. The aggressive style works bet- (Gigerenzer, Todd, & the ABC Research Group, 2000;
ter for boys who are big and strong rather than for boys Haselton & Funder, 2006). In this and other ways, evo-
who are small and weak; this might be why the former lutionary considerations offer new ways to look at estab-
are more likely to become juvenile delinquents (Glueck & lished theories in social and personality psychology and
Glueck, 1956). Still other traits might have evolved to be new grounds on which to evaluate them.
frequency dependent, meaning that they appear depend-
ing on how prevalent the trait already is in the population. Psychoanalytic Approach
One theory of primary psychopathy claims that this style
of conscienceless and exploitative behavior is biologically While biological research seeks to identify the specific
controlled to appear only in a small number of individu- physical foundations of behavior and personality, the psy-
als, because if it grew to be more widespread the behavior choanalytic approach often operates on a level of almost
would become self-defeating (Mealy, 1995). metaphysical abstraction—one that, nonetheless, leads
Whatever one thinks about its accounts for specific to unique insights and, occasionally, testable hypotheses.
traits and behaviors, evolutionary theorizing does offer a Psychoanalysis seeks to understand personality at the deep-
distinctive route for explaining why people do what they est psychological level and takes on the unique challenge
do. Considering the question of why a behavior might have of explaining what is going on in the hidden and some-
been or may still be evolutionarily adaptive—especially times dark recesses of the human mind. From a psychoana-
when the answer is not immediately obvious—can be illu- lytic perspective, personality is shaped by early childhood
minating. And the more complex questions concerning experiences and behavior is determined by the outcomes
when certain behaviors might be or have been adaptive can of unconscious processes and conflict. The psychoanalytic
be even more interesting. approach focuses on constructs such as the unconscious
Beyond the explanatory stories it offers, evolutionary mind, defense mechanisms, attachment, and ego strength.
psychology can serve one other purpose: as a constraint Psychoanalysis has long been criticized for being unsci-
on theorizing. Psychological scientists are used to evaluat- entific because it was historically based on subjective
ing theories on the basis of their internal consistency and interpretations of patients by clinical practitioners—most
parsimony. Evolutionary theory offers an additional crite- notably Sigmund Freud—using the case study method.
rion for evaluating theories: their evolutionary plausibility. However, aspects of psychoanalytic theory sometimes have
For example, versions of psychoanalytic theory that posit received empirical tests, and some of those tests have been
a drive toward seeking one’s own death and destruction successful (for reviews, see Baumeister, Dale, & Sommer,
would seem to fail this elementary test. 1998; Westen, 1998).
For another example, a tradition in cognitive psychology Freud died in 1939, but his theory lives on in several
and some parts of social psychology examines the inferen- forms. In one form, Freud himself is still the issue. A small
tial capabilities of the human mind and concludes that in psychoanalytic community continues to take Freud’s writ-
some respects these capabilities are fundamentally flawed ings literally as infallible sources of truth; a countercom-
(e.g., Gilovich, 1993; Ross, 1977). Evolutionary consider- munity continues to attack everything from his research
ations suggest that any such suggestion must be examined methods to his personal life (Crews, 1996; Masson, 1984).
closely. On the one hand, evolutionary reasoning does not Both of these effectively allied groups miss the point
suggest that the mind should have evolved to be perfect, because psychoanalytic theory continues to evolve and
any more than it suggests that our biceps should have has become detached from and largely independent of its
evolved to lift unlimited amounts of weight. Some objects long-deceased creator. This development was seen in the
are too heavy for us to lift; presumably we have evolved neo-Freudians (who are themselves no longer “neo” or
to be strong enough to lift what our ancestors had to deal even, mostly, alive), such as Alfred Adler, Carl Jung, Erik
with. Current-day adult humans cannot lift an automo- Erikson, and Karen Horney, and in the growth of psycho-
bile, but almost everyone can lift a baby. In a similar vein, analytically inspired approaches such as object relations
our minds make many mistakes of memory and inference theory (Greenberg & Mitchell, 1983) and attachment the-
but a mind that was not smart enough to make essential ory (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007).
Basic Approaches to Studying Personality 673

The most interesting modern manifestation of psycho- from other sciences and more difficult than is usually
analytic theory has arisen among researchers who are cur- acknowledged. The humanistic approach proposes that
rently using rigorous methodologies to test psychoanalytic understanding behavior requires appreciating each indi-
ideas, with or without acknowledging their Freudian roots. vidual from his or her own unique perspective. Humanism
According to Westen (1998, pp. 334–335), five key postu- is also different from the other approaches in that it focuses
lates of psychoanalytic theory are often studied and gener- on human strength, growth, and well-being, rather than
ally supported: human weakness. Although early humanism was most
influential within clinical and developmental psychology,
1. Much of mental life is unconscious, which means that it has inspired modern empirical personality research that,
people may do or think things that they do not them- unlike its humanistic precursors, uses rigorous empirical
selves understand. methods. Core ideas from the humanistic perspective can
2. Different mental processes can operate at the same be found in current research in “positive psychology” and
time, and this parallel processing can produce conflict- cross-cultural studies.
ing thoughts and behavioral impulses. The newly emerging area of positive psychology iden-
3. The roots of adult personality can be found in child- tifies human strengths and studies how they can be used
hood, and early experience has especially important to increase well-being and happiness. Positive psychology
implications for how individuals form later social has provided useful insights about how cognitive processes
relationships. affect individual differences in happiness. For example,
4. Social interactions are shaped by psychological repre- studies suggest that people who engage in unproductive
sentations of the self, others, and relationships. rumination and do not take the time to appreciate the good
5. Personality development involves learning to regulate things in life are less likely to be happy (Lyubomirsky,
sexuality and aggression as an individual moves from 2001; Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade, 2005). A more
immaturity and dependence on others to maturity and basic question concerns the definition of happiness or
independence. well-being. Some researchers have defined happiness in
terms of high positive affect, low negative affect, and high
Among the research that supports one or more of these life satisfaction (hedonic well-being), while others have
tenets are studies that show that a part of the mind (i.e., defined happiness in terms of striving toward meaning and
the unconscious) perceives things that the conscious self-actualization (eudaimonic well-being). These concep-
mind does not (Bornstein, 1999; Erdelyi, 1974; see also tualizations are theoretically distinct and lead to different
Dijksterhuis, this volume), behavior and consciousness predictions about how happy people feel and behave. For
are a result of numerous independently operating mental example, those who are higher in eudaimonic well-being
subsystems (Rumelhart, McClelland, & the PDP Research may not necessarily be low in negative affect because striv-
Group, 1986), the unconscious mind can prevent the con- ing for meaning in life can involve enduring struggle and
scious mind from perceiving anxiety-provoking stimuli adversity. Although the different conceptualizations of
(Erdelyi, 1985), and childhood attachment with one’s par- happiness seem to have different implications, self-reports
ents may translate into styles of adult romantic attachment, of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being have been observed
with important consequences for emotional life (Hazan & to have highly similar external correlates; therefore, they
Shaver, 1990; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Overall, psy- might overlap empirically more than they do theoretically
choanalysis is the most widely and heavily criticized of all (Nave, Sherman, & Funder, 2008; Ryan & Deci, 2001).
approaches to studying personality; however, it continues Humanistic psychology’s core concern with how people
to provoke interest and raises questions that the rest of psy- construct their views of reality is echoed in cross-cultural
chology sometimes ignores. research, which involves understanding the ways in which
people from different cultures may have fundamentally
different views of the world. Triandis (1994, 1997) pro-
Humanistic Approach
poses that one of the ways in which members of different
The humanistic approach was originally based on an even cultures might experience reality differently concerns the
less scientific tradition than psychoanalysis. Early pioneers, degree to which the cultures are individualistic versus col-
such as Carl Rogers (1951) and Abraham Maslow (1987), lectivistic. Individualistic cultures (including most Western
believed that personality is a special entity that cannot be cultures) have a sharp boundary between the self and
studied dispassionately from a distance. They argued that others and value independence over interdependence. In
unlike rocks and trees, people can perceive, think, and feel, collectivistic cultures (including most Eastern cultures),
which makes the study of people fundamentally different the boundary between self and others is more blurry and the
674 Personality in Social Psychology

well-being of the group is seen as more important than that of reinforcements, but his theory also emphasizes expecta-
any individual. In support, research suggests that people in tions about the self. Bandura’s version explains how beliefs
collectivist cultures are more likely to report experiencing about one’s own capabilities (e.g., self-efficacy) influence
other-focused emotions (e.g., sympathy) while people in what one attempts to do and how watching the behavior of
individualist cultures are more likely to experience self- others (e.g., observational learning) influences one’s own
focused emotions (e.g., anger; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). actions.
More recent research branches out in various directions, Walter Mischel’s “cognitive affective personality sys-
including the study of cultural groups that go beyond the tem” (CAPS) is a social learning theory specifically
traditional East–West dichotomy (e.g., Nisbett & Cohen, intended to explain personality (Mischel, 1999; Mischel &
1996; Tsai & Chentsova-Dutton, 2003), a closer examina- Shoda, 1995). CAPS proposes that personality is a system
tion of multicultural individuals (e.g., Benet-Martínez & composed of several person variables that interact with each
Haritatos, 2005), and a renewed focus on the elements of situation in which people find themselves. Person variables
human nature that are similar, as well as different, across include mental abilities and behavioral skills, ways of con-
cultural contexts (e.g., Matsumoto, 2007; Oishi, Diener, struing reality and efficacy expectations, procedures for
Napa Scollon, & Biswas-Diener, 2004). controlling behavior, and affects or emotions. According
to the CAPS theory, each individual can be characterized
by a unique set of “if–then” statements that describe what
Learning-Based Approaches
a person will do in different situations. For example, “if ” a
Classic behaviorists ignore concepts like happiness and conflict arises, one person might “then” become confronta-
construals of reality because their approach strictly dictates tional and escalate the hostilities, whereas “if ” in conflict,
that they study only that which can be directly observed. another individual might “then” seek to withdraw from the
Behaviorism is a learning-based approach to studying per- situation. Thus, the situation is the “if ” and the behavior
sonality, and it places heavy emphasis on overt behavior is the “then,” and every individual is characterized by a
and the rewards and punishments in the environment that pattern of reactions to particular situational stimuli. This
condition individuals to behave in certain ways. From this description of personality is, in some ways, reminiscent of
perspective, personality is simply the behaviors that an the stimulus–response personality theory of John Watson
individual performs as a result of environmentally imposed (1930). The approach is also reminiscent of classic behav-
reinforcement contingencies. Although some research- iorism in its reluctance to acknowledge the importance
ers still conduct classic behavioral research (e.g., applied of unobservable mental structures. As Mischel recently
behavioral analysis), most psychologists now recognize that wrote, “whatever way one chooses to define ‘personality’
pure behaviorism leaves out important psychological ingre- it is certainly not a de-contextualized ‘entity within the
dients. For example, one’s beliefs about reinforcements, mind’” (Mischel, 2009, p. 289).
not just the reinforcements themselves, play an important
role in determining behavior. In particular, the evaluative
Cognitive Approach
properties of rewards can be as important as the rewards
themselves and can, depending on the circumstances, The cognitive approach, which evolved from and over-
undermine or enhance their effects (Harackiewicz & laps with the social learning theories, focuses on percep-
Sansone, 2000). People also learn how to act by watching tual processes, thoughts and beliefs, and motivational
the behaviors and consequences of the behaviors of others. processes that form the basis of personality and behavior.
Considerations such as these led to the development of the One way of conceptualizing personality traits is to think of
social learning theories. them as dimensions along which people think and perceive
Social learning theories stay true to behaviorism in information differently. For example, one person might
acknowledging the importance of environmental influ- have the disposition to have positive thoughts more readily
ences on behavior, but they add unobservable elements accessible, whereas another person might have the dispo-
that make their theories more attractive, and perhaps sition to have negative thoughts more accessible. Gordon
more plausible. Julian Rotter ’s (1954, 1982) social learn- Allport pointed out this possibility many years ago, when
ing theory discusses the importance of expectations for he wrote
behavior and proposes that behavioral decisions are based
on one’s beliefs about the attractiveness of reinforcements For some the world is a hostile place where men are evil and
and the perceived likelihood of attaining reinforcements. dangerous; for others it is a stage for fun and frolic. It may
Like Rotter, Albert Bandura’s (1971, 1977a, b) social learn- appear as a place to do one’s duty grimly; or a pasture for cul-
ing theory recognizes the importance of expectations of tivating friendship and love. (Allport, 1961, p. 266)
Basic Approaches to Studying Personality 675

Individuals who perceive the world differently might Big Five personality traits—extraversion, neuroticism,
be expected to behave accordingly, and research has con- conscientiousness, agreeableness, and openness or intel-
firmed this expectation. Downey and Feldman (1996) pro- lect or culture—which can be measured by any of several
posed that individuals who are higher in the trait of rejection different, widely used questionnaires (McCrae & Costa,
sensitivity, for whom thoughts of rejection are readily 1987; Saucier & Goldberg, 2003). Research suggests that
available, are more likely to interpret any ambiguous sig- individuals’ standing on the Big Five begins to stabilize in
nal as confirmation that their partner is about to abandon their thirties (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005) but contin-
them. The slightest expression of irritation from a partner ues to change to a smaller degree thereafter. Basic traits
invokes panic, and because the person has a negative reac- (including traits in addition to the Big Five) that continue
tion toward the partner every time a threat is perceived, the to rise between ages 30 and 70 include social dominance,
person indeed becomes more likely to be rejected. Such agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability.
individual differences in “chronic accessibility” may also Openness to experience appears to remain about stable,
be involved in aggression. Dodge and Frame (1982) found on average, and social vitality goes down over this period
that aggressive boys were quick to perceive hostility in the (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006). Traits like these
characters of a short story, whereas nonaggressive boys predict a range of important behaviors and life outcomes,
generally reached a more benign interpretation. such as happiness, social adjustment, marital satisfaction,
career choice, job performance, and civic engagement
(Ozer & Benet-Martínez, 2006; Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner,
Trait-Based Approach
Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007).
The trait-based approach to studying personality places One common critique of traits is that while they describe
individual difference constructs (i.e., personality traits) patterns of behavior, they do not explain where those pat-
front and center. According to Gordon Allport (1931), traits terns come from (Pervin, 1994). For example, a person
are psychological mechanisms that determine people’s who constantly worries and has an intense reaction to stress
responses to stimuli. He believed that traits motivate and could be described as high in neuroticism. But where does
organize an individual’s behavior and knowing an indi- neuroticism come from? What are the psychological mech-
vidual’s traits requires observing that person’s behavior anisms that produce and maintain neuroticism? Although
repeatedly. Such observation reveals the consistent behav- traits are insufficient to fully explain behavior, they (1)
ioral patterns from which the underlying psychological provide an efficient means for describing how individu-
mechanisms can be inferred. als are different from one another (e.g., not all individuals
From this perspective, traits are like gravity. Gravity are neurotic, an interesting observation), (2) offer a basis
is a concept that describes the force of attraction between from which to predict future behavior (e.g., you can expect
objects and explains a range of astronomical observations people high in neuroticism to be upset regardless of what
(e.g., how planets orbit the sun). Similarly, traits are psy- is going on in their life), and (3) provide a useful explana-
chological concepts that are used to describe individual tory resting point (Funder, 1994). That is, once traits have
differences in behavior and give coherence to a range of been identified and validly measured, research can be
psychological observations. For example, “narcissism” is directed toward deeper explanation. The other approaches
a construct that ties together a complex pattern of attitudes to studying personality, discussed earlier, are partly an
and behaviors of people who believe they are exceptional attempt to push the explanatory regress one or more steps
individuals entitled to exploit others for their own advan- deeper, but they all depend on this first step, of identify-
tage (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001; Vazire & Funder, 2006). ing and measuring consistent individual differences in
Nobody has ever seen either gravity or narcissism directly, behavior.
but important aspects of their nature can be discerned from The basic approaches to personality are committed to a
their effects. particular way of explaining these differences: the biologi-
The main aims of trait research include identifying and cal approach in anatomy, physiology, genetics, and evolu-
conceptualizing important personality traits, accurately tion; the psychoanalytic approach in unconscious processes
measuring individual differences in traits, and using traits and early experience; the humanistic approach in individual
to understand behavior. Trait research also examines the construals of reality; the learning approaches in responses
developmental antecedents of personality and seeks to pre- to reward and punishment; and the cognitive approach in
dict short-term behaviors (e.g., delay of gratification and processes of perception and thought. The exception is the
cooperation with others in a group task) and long-term out- trait approach, which is primarily methodological (as seen
comes (e.g., job performance, health, and divorce). Perhaps in its emphasis on psychometric technology) and stands
the most well-known area of trait research concerns the apart from a commitment to any particular explanation of
676 Personality in Social Psychology

the individual differences it identifies and measures. This self-esteem, to conscientiousness, to gender identity. The
atheoretical stance might be viewed as a weakness, but it scientific study of personality rests on the following simple
puts the trait approach at the center of personality psychol- formula: P → B. A researcher might theoretically view P as
ogy because it provides an outlook and technology that is causing B, or view P as a summary of B, but the method of
critical to all the approaches, and indeed to any researcher study remains the same.
who would seek to understand how individuals are psy- A range of techniques can be used to examine the behav-
chologically different from one another. iors associated with personality, but in practice the most
For example, a positive psychologist who studies hap- common method is self-report. Self-report has at least three
piness must create a valid method of measuring it and advantages for personality assessment (along with some
observe how individual differences in happiness are asso- important disadvantages that are considered later). First,
ciated with differences in behavior. Similarly, a behavioral a person lives through many different situations and is the
geneticist who is interested in the inheritance of psycho- only one who has had a chance to observe his or her own
pathology must find an appropriate way to measure indi- behavior in all of them. The self is also the only observer
vidual differences in various aspects of maladjustment. with direct access to the inner mental life, which is largely
In short, if one is interested in psychological dimensions invisible from the outside. In short, the self has more infor-
along which people differ, then there is no escaping the mation than anyone else and has unique access to some
basic issues of psychometrics, whether one chooses to information.
label one’s construct as a “trait,” a “person variable,” or Second, self-views tend to have a causal force. Research
some other near synonym. on self-verification (Swann, Chang-Schneider, & Angulo,
Moreover, descriptions of person variables or other 2007) suggests that people actively seek to behave in ways
individual difference constructs labeled with terms other that confirm what they believe to be true about themselves.
than “trait” often amount to restatements. For example, the Studies of self-efficacy (e.g., Bandura, 1977a) show how
cognitive measures such as self-descriptive reaction what people attempt to do depends on they believe about
time associated with “self-schemas” in the research by their capabilities.
Markus (1977) are similarly associated with scores from Finally, and perhaps most importantly in practice, self-
conventional self-report scales such as the California reports are the simplest and easiest type of data to obtain.
Psychological Inventory (Fuhrman & Funder, 1995). One Gathering observational behavioral data, assessing life out-
can also observe that the “if–then” statements that char- comes, or recruiting friends or family to provide personal-
acterize the CAPS theory largely amount to operational ity judgments of target individuals is expensive and time
definitions of personality traits. Whereas in trait terms one consuming. Self-report allows researchers to quickly col-
might say that a person who is more extraverted is more lect information about many people at relatively little cost.
likely to be talkative in social situations, the CAPS theory Self-reports yield behavioral data in two senses. First,
would more specifically claim that “if ” a particular per- many questions on self-report inventories are questions
son perceives a situation as social, “then” that individual about behavior, ranging from whether the person goes
will talk. There is not much difference between these state- to many parties to how often the individual gets angry. To
ments, and one might even suggest that the trait descrip- the extent that the answers to these questions are accurate,
tion is more economical. Therefore, the remainder of this self-report offers an efficient method to gather wide-ranging
chapter is oriented toward trait psychology. It focuses on information about what people do in daily life. Second,
the conceptualization and measurement of individual dif- self-report is itself a behavior (Hogan & Nicholson, 1988).
ferences, which is a core issue in personality research The act of claiming that one is friendly or hostile is a
regardless of deeper theoretical preference. self-presentation that may be interesting in its own right.
Hundreds and perhaps thousands of self-report person-
ality questionnaires are available and are widely used in
BEHAVIORAL ASSESSMENT research, business, and clinical settings. Some self-report
OF PERSONALITY inventories measure one specific personality trait, others
measure numerous traits, and others measure a few essen-
The foundation of empirical personality psychology is the tial traits.
observation of behavior—the only way to examine a per-
sonality construct is to propose a behavioral manifestation Single Traits
and then observe it. This is true regardless of the nature of
the construct, which, as we have seen, might be anything When a researcher develops a personality test that mea-
from stimulus generalization, to rejection sensitivity, to sures a single trait, the trait is usually one that that seems
Behavioral Assessment of Personality 677

particularly important and a huge effort may be made to people to gauge how well they are doing socially. It tends
explore all possible implications. The traits that receive to be lower when one has disappointed a social group, and
this kind of attention vary over the years, for reasons that the negative feeling that accompanies low self-esteem
may be less than clear. Some traits appear to become well should motivate a person to restore the previous reputation.
known and widely researched because they address a social Therefore, a person with unrealistically high self-esteem
issue that seems particularly important at the time. For might fail to detect when others are unhappy with him or
example, the post–World War II years saw an explosion of her and fail to respond in an appropriate manner. Overall,
research on authoritarianism (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, it seems that adaptive self-esteem is based on legitimate
Levinson, & Sanford, 1950), a trait hypothesized to accomplishment, rather than having an extremely high or
underlie the combination of racism and reverence for tra- low level independent of reality.
ditional authority that allowed the Nazi atrocities. Other Self-monitoring is another personality trait that has been
traits might come into prominence because of the ingenu- widely studied, its prominence pushed along by the highly
ity of their researchers, such as the creative investigations productive original investigator Mark Snyder and various
Richard Christie (Christie & Geis, 1970) conducted while creative studies by him and others. The self-monitoring
illuminating the trait of effective manipulativeness that he scale measures individual differences in the degree to
called Machiavellianism. Or perhaps they just fit the cul- which a person is concerned with the impression made on
tural zeitgeist for some ineffable reason, such as may have others and adjusts behavior to each social situation to bring
been manifested by the surge of interest in locus of control about the desired impression (Snyder, 1974). According to
during the 1960s and 1970s (Rotter, 1954, 1982). theory, high self-monitors are sensitive to situational cues
Three traits that are of current interest, probably for a and monitor their behavior to behave in socially desirable
combination of all these reasons, are self-esteem, self-mon- ways. In contrast, low self-monitors are less concerned
itoring, and attributional complexity. The concept of self- with the social climate and act more consistently, regard-
esteem (see Swann & Bosson, this volume) originates in less of the situation. Not surprisingly, high self-monitors
the humanistic tradition. Rogers (1951) laid the theoretical are more likely to be described as popular, expressive, and
groundwork when he introduced the concept of uncondi- socially poised, whereas low self-monitors are more likely
tional positive regard. According to Rogers, people who to be described as introspective and independent (Funder &
accept themselves as they are and regard themselves in a Harris, 1986). Individuals higher in self-monitoring also
positive manner unconditionally will enjoy better psycho- perform better in job interviews (Osborn, Field, & Veres,
logical health. Years later, Rosenberg (1965) developed a 1998), use more strategies to influence their co-workers
self-report questionnaire that measures individual differ- (Caldwell & Burger, 1997), and are more willing to lie to
ences in self-esteem such that those who score higher are get a date (Rowatt, Cunningham, & Druen, 1998). Finally,
hypothesized to have a positive self-orientation and believe research suggests that high self-monitors are more likely
they have value and worth. Indeed, research suggests that to look to the social environment to gauge how they are
low self-esteem is related to various negative outcomes feeling, whereas low self-monitors are more likely to look
such as depression, hopelessness, dissatisfaction with life within (Graziano & Bryant, 1998).
(Crocker & Wolfe, 2001), and loneliness (Cutrona, 1982). Attributional complexity is an individual difference
Self-esteem was regarded as so important that the California construct that may have arisen to exceptional prominence
legislature set up a task force to increase self-esteem as part because of the innovative way it attempts to bridge tra-
of a phenomenon known as the self-esteem movement. ditional concerns of personality and social psychology.
More recently, research has suggested that self- The attributional complexity scale (Fletcher, Danilovics,
esteem cuts both ways. While low self-esteem is unhealthy, Fernandez, Peterson, & Reeder, 1986) was developed to
extremely high self-esteem may lead to abusive, even crim- reconcile two opposing views concerning how lay social
inal behavior. Baumeister, Bushman, and Campbell (2000) perceivers determine whether the causes of another per-
argue that extremely high self-esteem, or inflated beliefs son’s behavior are internal, external, or a combination
about one’s superiority over others, underlies aggression, of the two. One view proposes that people are cognitive
and a literature review provided evidence that extreme misers who rely on simple heuristics when attributing the
favorable self-regard is related to various violent behav- causes of other ’s behavior (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974),
iors such as murder, rape, and spousal abuse (Baumeister, while the other view holds that the attributional process
Smart, & Boden, 1996). One reason unrealistically high is complex (Ross & Fletcher, 1985). Rather than viewing
self-esteem could be bad is that low self-esteem may be all social perceivers as either simple or complex, the attri-
an adaptive danger signal. According to sociometer theory butional complexity scale measures individual differences
(Leary, 1999), self-esteem is a mechanism that allows in the motivation and preference for complex attributions.
678 Personality in Social Psychology

Those higher in attributional complexity are more likely planful are more likely to delay gratification; however,
to consider both dispositional and situational factors when girls who delay are also more intelligent, competent, and
trying to understand others’ behavior, while those lower in resourceful, whereas boys who delay are more shy, compli-
attributional complexity are less likely to think about the ant, and anxious. The explanation offered for this finding
causes of behavior or to consider multiple causes. Research was that girls are taught by society to be controlled, whereas
has shown that, compared with those lower in attributional boys are taught to be more rambunctious. Therefore, boys
complexity, individuals higher in this trait are relatively who delay gratification may be slightly less adjusted in
less likely to fall prey to various errors of social judg- terms of the social lessons that they absorb.
ment (Follett & Hess, 2002; Schaller, Boyd, Yohannes, & The CAQ has also been used to examine the develop-
O’Brien, 1995) and, under some circumstances, are better mental antecedents of political orientation. Block and
at “mind reading” the thoughts of others in social interac- Block (2006) reported that children who were described
tion (Thomas & Fletcher, 2000). Research also suggests by their teachers (using a version of the Q-set adapted for
that individuals who are higher in attributional complexity use with children) as anxious, unable to handle stress, and
are more likely to be described as thoughtful, empathic, tending to feel guilty were more likely to describe them-
open to experience, and generally likable (Fast, Reimer, & selves as politically conservative 20 years later. On the
Funder, 2008). other hand, children who grew up to see themselves as lib-
eral were described years earlier as self-reliant, confident,
Multiple Traits and independent. These findings suggest that psychological
attributes already apparent in childhood can anticipate adult
Other widely used personality tests measure numerous political orientation, and they are consistent with indepen-
traits at once. The “many trait” inventories are typically dent findings that political conservatism in adults is asso-
used to examine the many possible psychological charac- ciated with traits including death anxiety, intolerance of
teristics related to an important behavior or life outcome. ambiguity, (low) openness to experience, and fear of threat
For example, the California Adult Q-set1 (CAQ; Bem & and loss (Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003).2
Funder, 1978; Block, 1978, 2007) consists of 100 descrip-
tions of specific psychological attributes (e.g., “is critical, Essential Traits
skeptical, not easily impressed” and “is a genuinely depend-
able and responsible person”). Raters use the CAQ by sort- Finally, some personality research is motivated by the fol-
ing the items into nine categories that range from “highly lowing question: Which personality traits are the most
characteristic” (category 9), to “neither characteristic nor important? Currently, the most widely accepted answer to
uncharacteristic” (category 5), to “highly uncharacteristic” this question is the Big Five. The Big Five represent the
(category 1). The resulting set of ratings is quasinormally culmination of more than 100 years of research aimed at
distributed because the technique prescribes that a pre- reducing the many possible personality traits to an essential
determined number of items be placed in each category. few (Allport & Odbert, 1936; Cattell, 1943; Digman, 1990;
The largest number of items must be placed in category 5, Fiske, 1949; Galton, 1884; Goldberg, 1990; McCrae &
and only a few items can be placed in the extreme categories Costa, 1987; Norman, 1967; Thurstone, 1934; Tupes &
(1 and 9). The advantage of the Q-sort rating method is Christal, 1961). Research on the Big Five traits is based,
that it forces raters to make fine-grained distinctions about in part, on the lexical hypothesis (e.g., Goldberg, 1981),
the person being rated and reduces social desirability and which proposes that anything truly important to human
various response sets (Block, 1978, 2007). life will be labeled with words. This idea suggests that the
Raters completing the CAQ may include acquaintances most important personality traits are encoded in language.
of the individual, therapists, researchers, or the individuals Therefore, researchers extracted trait-adjective words from
themselves. The CAQ has been used to study the psycho- the Oxford English Dictionary and factor analyzed ratings
logical correlates of many behaviors. Funder, Block, and of them, many times over, along with scores on a multitude
Block (1983) used the CAQ to examine sex differences
in delay of gratification. They found that boys and girls 2 There is clearly more to this story, however, because conserva-
described by teachers and researchers as reflective and
tives also apparently are happier than liberals (Taylor, Funk, &
Craighill, 2006). Maybe this is because they are good at rational-
izing inequality (Napier & Jost, 2008), but it should be observed
1 A setof items that are sorted into a predetermined, forced distri- that characterizing a particular, widely held political belief sys-
bution is called a “Q-set”; the act of rating them in this format is tem as essentially pathological entails philosophical and ethical
“Q-sorting,” and a completed set of ratings is a “Q-sort.” issues (Haidt, 2008).
Behavioral Assessment of Personality 679

of personality inventories. Eventually, a consensus emerged contributed most of what has been learned about personality,
that much of the variance in trait words and personality rat- they entail two major disadvantages. First, people may
ings is captured by the Big Five. Ozer and Benet-Martínez sometimes be unwilling to reveal undesirable aspects of
(2006) conducted a massive research synthesis and summa- their personalities. Second, people are not always aware
rized the most robust correlates of these traits, mostly but of every aspect of themselves. Several studies suggest
not exclusively measured via self-report: that people rate themselves higher on socially desirable
characteristics (e.g., trustworthiness) and rate themselves
• The first factor, extraversion, refers to the degree to lower on undesirable characteristics (e.g., laziness). For
which an individual is outgoing, is energetic, and ex- example, Funder and Dobroth (1987) found that people’s
periences positive emotion.3 Those individuals who are self-ratings of extraversion (a relatively neutral trait)
higher in extraversion are more likely to attend parties, tended to agree better with their friends’ ratings of their
are higher in subjective well-being and happiness, are extraversion than self-ratings of neuroticism and friends’
more likely to attain positions of leadership, are more ratings of neuroticism (a relatively undesirable trait).
popular, and live longer. Perhaps more telling, people’s reports of their own behav-
• Neuroticism involves the degree to which an individual ior do not always agree with direct observations (Gosling,
worries, is reactive to stress, and experiences negative John, Craik, & Robins, 1998; Vazire & Mehl, 2008). For
emotion. Those who are higher in neuroticism are more these reasons, personality psychology increasingly empha-
likely to become unhappy, depressed, and anxious and sizes two methods that go beyond self-report: acquain-
are more likely to have family problems, to be dis- tances’ judgments of personality and direct behavioral
satisfied with their jobs, and to experience conflict in observation.
romantic relationships. The people who share one’s social space are in a posi-
• Conscientiousness involves the degree to which an indi- tion to observe many behaviors under realistic, meaning-
vidual is dependable, organized, and punctual. People ful, and consequential circumstances. In various studies,
who are higher in conscientiousness are less likely to these observers have included friends, acquaintances,
engage in risky behavior, which is possibly one reason teachers, interviewers, and therapists. As a result of their
they live longer, and they perform better and have more observations, considerable evidence shows, their judg-
success in the workplace. ments of personality tend to be accurate. Peers’ judgments
• Agreeableness refers to the degree to which an indi- of personality largely agree with self-judgments, with
vidual is cooperative, warm, and gets along well with some exceptions (Funder & Colvin, 1997), and are predic-
others. Not surprisingly, individuals higher in this trait tive of directly observed behavior in the laboratory (e.g.,
enjoy better peer acceptance. They also are more satis- Funder & Colvin, 1991) and in daily life (e.g., Vazire &
fied with their dating partners, more likely to volunteer, Mehl, 2008). Teachers’ ratings of children’s personality
and less likely to suffer heart disease. can predict personality, behavior, and important life out-
• Finally, openness to experience is the most controversial comes such as physical health and even longevity years
trait of the Big Five in that researchers have disagreed later (e.g., Friedman et al., 1993; Hampson, Goldberg,
about which characteristics should be subsumed by this Vogt, & Dubanoski, 2006).
factor and what it should be called (e.g., openness vs. Turning to direct behavioral observation, a study by
intellect vs. culture). However, in general, this trait Borkenau, Mauer, Riemann, Spinath, and Angleitner (2004)
involves the degree to which an individual is creative, suggests that personality information can be revealed even in
open minded, and aesthetic. Individuals who are higher seemingly trivial scenarios that observers view only briefly.
in openness are more likely to pursue investigative, artistic, Participants were videotaped performing 15 tasks that var-
or both types of careers and are more likely to have left- ied from 1 minute to 12 minutes in length. Tasks included
leaning, liberal values (Ozer & Benet-Martínez, 2006). telling a joke to a confederate, introducing a stranger to the
experimenter, inventing a definition for a neologism, read-
Beyond Self-Report ing newspaper headlines, and singing a song. These vid-
eotapes were later viewed by judges who had never met
Although self-report questionnaires have traditionally dom- the participants. Each judge viewed only one videotaped
inated the literature and probably deserve credit for having task of each participant. Judges then rated the participants
along the Big Five traits and intelligence. Results indicated
3Different researchers emphasize slightly different aspects of the that judges’ personality ratings were positively correlated
Big Five traits, but in this chapter we emphasize the aspects of with participant’s self-ratings, as well as ratings provided by
each trait that are common across researchers. the participants’ close acquaintances.
680 Personality in Social Psychology

The authors also examined the possibility that some of because shyness evokes a moderately negative evaluation
the 15 tasks might be especially diagnostic of personality in Western cultures, individuals might downplay their
traits. Results indicated that judges’ ratings of extraversion, shyness on explicit self-report questionnaires and use
neuroticism, conscientiousness, and agreeableness agreed impression management strategies to conceal their shy-
with self and acquaintance ratings equally well across all ness. Explicit shyness was measured by asking individuals
tasks. However, judges’ ratings of openness agreed most to rate the degree to which shyness adjectives described
strongly with self and acquaintance ratings when judges them. Implicit shyness was measured via reaction time,
viewed the task in which participants described multiple such that participants who were quicker to associate self-
uses of a brick using pantomime. Also, judges’ ratings relevant words (such as “self ” ) than other-relevant words
of intelligence were more strongly correlated with par- (such as “them”) to the word “shy” were considered higher
ticipant’s objective intelligence scores in the tasks where in implicit shyness. Shy behavior was coded from a video-
participants read newspaper headlines and invented a defi- taped situation in which participants were told to get to know
nition for a neologism. This suggests that social aspects an interaction partner for 5 minutes. The authors hypoth-
of personality generally leak out regardless of what one esized that duration of speech within the interaction was
is doing but that judging an individual’s intelligence and a relatively controllable shy behavior, whereas self-
openness requires specific observations of ability-demand- stimulation and body tension were less controllable shy
ing behavior. behaviors. Results indicated that explicit ratings of shy-
Gosling, Ko, Mannarelli, and Morris (2002) examined ness were more highly correlated with observations of con-
the possibility that the environments that people construct trolled shy behavior than were implicit ratings and implicit
for themselves contain information about their personali- ratings of shyness were more highly correlated with obser-
ties. They argued that people craft their environments to be vations of uncontrollable shy behavior than were explicit
consistent with and reinforce their self-views and to display ratings. This study broadly suggests that people may be
their identity to others. Judges viewed the offices or bed- unable or unwilling to accurately assess certain aspects
rooms of participants and then rated the participants along of their own personalities, which might still leak out via
the Big Five traits. Judges’ personality ratings were found behaviors that they are unable to consciously control.
to positively correlate with participant’s own personality Finally, Fast and Funder (2008) examined the possibil-
ratings, as well as ratings provided by close acquaintances. ity that personality is manifested in the words that people
Judges’ ratings of openness were most strongly correlated use. This study was based on the idea that words are one
with self and peer ratings, followed by conscientiousness, of the most explicit means through which people express
emotional stability, extraversion, and agreeableness. their thoughts and emotions; therefore, the frequency with
Furthermore, many cues from the offices and bedrooms which people use different kinds of words might reveal
that judges used to form their personality judgments were aspects of their personalities. Word use was measured by
found to be valid. For example, judges reported that their counting the number of words that each participant used in
ratings of openness were based on cues such as number of 66 categories (e.g., positive emotion words, negative emo-
and variety of books and magazines, and indeed, individu- tion words, and achievement words). The frequency with
als higher in openness were found to have a higher number which individuals used words in categories was correlated
of and wider variety of books and magazines. Also, judges’ with self-reports of personality, personality judgments by
personality ratings of extraversion were validly based on close acquaintances, and direct observations of behav-
cues such as clutter and colorfulness and personality rat- ior. For example, those who used more sexuality words
ings of conscientiousness were validly based on cues such (e.g., “horny” and “nude”) were described by themselves
as organization and cleanliness. This study suggests that the and acquaintances and were directly and independently
environments that people inhabit contain a wealth of infor- observed to be high in the need for attention. Those who
mation about their personalities and that casual observers used more certainty words (e.g., “absolutely” and “clearly”)
of these environments are sometimes able to detect and use were rated as and directly and independently observed
this information. to be generally more smart, thoughtful, and likable.
Using an even more subtle possible behavioral indicator of This study suggests that word choice is a subtle manife-
personality, Asendorpf, Banse, and Mücke (2002) employed station of personality that relates to how people view them-
the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald, McGhee, & selves, how they are described by their acquaintances, and
Schwartz, 1998; Banaji & Heiphetz, this volume) to how they are observed to behave.
examine the possibility that explicit and implicit measures In summary, several methods can be used to study per-
of shyness might be differentially related to more and less sonality, and each type of behavioral information provides a
controllable aspects of shy behavior. They reasoned that, different perspective. Self-report is by far the most common
Person Perception and Accuracy 681

type of data gathered in personality research; however, They tended to conclude that pro-Castro authors held
researchers are increasingly using reports by friends, relatively pro-Castro opinions, compared with anti-Castro
acquaintances, teachers, and other observers and putting authors, despite having been told the authors had no choice
serious effort into the development of direct and subtle in what to write and therefore were deemed by the experi-
behavioral indicators of personality. menters to have committed an attribution error that Gilbert
and Jones (1986) later called the “correspondence bias.” In
other writings the bias was dubbed the “fundamental attri-
PERSON PERCEPTION AND ACCURACY bution error” (FAE), and the original study was described as
establishing “people’s overwillingness to ascribe behavior
The way that an individual is perceived by others is highly to enduring dispositions” (Nisbett & Ross, 1980, p. 131).
consequential. Reputation determines the opportunities that We consider the fundamental status of the error later, but for
others will make available to the individual and the expecta- now the methodological point concerns the way this study
tions they will hold. A person with a good reputation will be exemplifies a research design in which hypothetical stimuli
trusted and will find that other people like him or her; a per- are used to test putatively optimal models of information
son with a bad reputation probably will not get a desired job, processing, which participants generally fail to follow. This
will not attract his or her love interest, and will be generally design is typical of much research in person perception.
disliked. Moreover, people tend to live up or down to their An essential characteristic of this design is that it pro-
good or bad reputations because they tend to behave in ways vides little or no information about the variables that might
that confirm the expectations of others (Rosenthal & Rubin, influence accuracy outside the laboratory. For example,
1978; Snyder & Swann, 1978). The study of how people per- in the study by Jones and Harris, participants were clearly
ceive one another is a major research topic in social and per- wrong to ascribe different attitudes to the pro- and anti-
sonality psychology (Macrae & Quadflieg, this volume). It is Castro essay writers, because in similar experimental con-
called “person perception research” within social psychology texts nearly everybody agrees to write the prescribed essay
and “accuracy research” within personality psychology. The regardless of actual opinion. In real life, however, people
difference in labels is more than superficial. The two research perhaps more often say (and write) what they believe.4 Even
paradigms are based on fundamentally different philosophi- though a strategy of inferring corresponding beliefs from
cal perspectives, focus on different aspects of social percep- written statements leads to error in Jones and Harris’s exper-
tion, and are studied using different methodologies. iment, the same strategy might produce correct judgments in
realistic contexts. An analogous situation is found in dem-
Person Perception Research onstrations of visual effects such as the Ponzo or “railroad
lines” illusion (Funder, 1987). Looking at Figure 18.1, a
Person perception research in social psychology is based
on social constructivism, in the sense that it treats the
views people have of one another as mental constructions,
a property of the social perceiver rather than of the person
who is described. Therefore, the focus of person percep-
tion research is on the cognitive processes that underlie the
construction of impressions. One common methodology
used to examine such processes involves creating an artifi-
cial social stimulus (such as description of a hypothetical
person), proposing an optimal model for how the stimu-
lus ought to be processed, and observing whether or not
participants process the stimulus in the proposed optimal
manner. To the extent that participants fail to process the
stimulus correctly in this sense, they are presumed to be in
error, and the cognitive processes that led to the error are
Figure 18.1 The Ponzo illusion.
inferred by the researcher (Krueger & Funder, 2004). Source: Tony Phillips. Downloaded November 26, 2008 from
In a classic study, Jones and Harris (1967) asked partici- http://science.nasa.gov/headlines/y2002/24jun_moonillusion.htm.
pants to read essays for and against favor of Fidel Castro
that, participants were told, were written by individuals
who had been instructed in which position to take. The par- 4 Under some circumstances, they also may come to believe what
ticipants then estimated the essay writers’ actual opinions. they have said or written (Bem, 1972).
682 Personality in Social Psychology

subject who believes the upper line to be longer than the lower of person A’s behavior (e.g., Does person A talk more than
line is simply wrong. In real-life settings with a similar others?), and with relevant life outcomes (e.g., Does person
appearance, however, such as two objects sitting on a set A successfully engage in an occupation that requires being
of actual railroad tracks, the upper object really would be outgoing and energetic?). If sources of data converge with
larger. one another (e.g., person A describes himself or herself as
In general, a bias to ascribe behavior to dispositional extraverted, talks a lot, and is a successful salesperson), we
causes promotes accuracy in real life to the extent that can be reasonably confident that person A’s acquaintance
behavior does tend to be dispositionally determined, just has provided an accurate personality judgment. Accuracy
as visual effects such as the Ponzo illusion reveal biases research differs from person perception research in that
that cause two-dimensional pictures to be misperceived the typical study uses real people as the target of judgment
but allow correct judgments in three-dimensional contexts. and gathers personality judgments from people who know
Person perception research is silent on this critical point. them in real life, along with other indicators such as self-
The typical research design can demonstrate the direction judgments, behavioral observations, and life outcomes.
of a bias but does not provide evidence concerning the
degree to which the bias undermines or promotes accuracy History of Accuracy Research
under realistic circumstances. For just this reason, the pio- Accuracy research in this sense has a history that goes back
neering person perception researcher E. E. Jones once com- more than 70 years (e.g., see Chapter 18 in Allport, 1937).
mented that the common experimental paradigm “solved Early studies focused on agreement between self and oth-
the accuracy problem by bypassing it” (1985, p. 77). ers’ judgments of personality in search of correlates of the
He did not intend this comment as a complaint, or confes- “good judge” (Estes, 1938; Taft, 1955). Research activity
sion. Rather, he was expressing appreciation for the way in this area almost came to a halt in the 1950s, however,
person perception research had managed to develop exper- for at least three reasons (Funder, 1995, pp. 653–654).
imental methods that could address interesting questions First, few consistent findings concerning the correlates of
while bypassing the difficult issues entailed in the study judgmental ability emerged from a multitude of studies.
of judgmental accuracy. Ironically, however, subsequent Second, severe critiques of the methods used by almost
work within this paradigm often was interpreted as demon- all the studies of the time concluded that the numbers used
strating shortcomings of human judgment. to index self–other agreement—the typical standard for
accuracy—were possibly confounded by actual and
assumed similarity between judge and target and response
Accuracy Research
sets such as positivity and acquiescence (Cronbach, 1955;
Accuracy research within personality psychology follows a Gage & Cronbach, 1955). While the problems the critiques
fundamentally different strategy. It is based on critical real- raised were not insurmountable, the difficulty that appeared
ism (Rorer, 1990), a philosophical approach asserting that to be entailed in overcoming them discouraged many
the best way to assess a judgment is in terms of multiple researchers from further studies in the area. The third reason
fallible external criteria that probabilistically indicate the for the falloff in accuracy research was that it began to be
degree to which it is a true reflection of reality.5 Therefore, supplanted by experimental research in person perception
accuracy research focuses on establishing criteria by which (e.g., Tagiuri & Petrullo, 1958)—an approach that, as Jones
to evaluate accuracy and identifying the factors that make noted, solved the problem of accuracy by bypassing it.
accurate personality judgment more or less likely. Accuracy research began to revive in the early 1980s,
however, as investigators turned their attention to the
Methods of Accuracy Research issues Cronbach had raised and developed new meth-
The primary methodology involves comparing multiple ods for addressing accuracy issues. Kenny (e.g., 1994)
sources of information about a person to evaluate the developed statistical tools (and associated computer pro-
degree to which a personality judgment is accurate. For grams) for decomposing interpersonal judgments into
example, if person A is judged to be high in extraversion their components, along with a theoretical model of how
by an acquaintance, we can evaluate this judgment by information is combined to yield personality judgments.
comparing it with person A’s self-judgment of the degree A robust, general finding of this research has been that
to which he or she is extraverted, with direct observations people generally judge one another accurately on impor-
tant traits such as aggressiveness (e.g., Kenny et al., 2007).
Other researchers showed how even brief observations
5The distinction between constructivism and critical realism was of behavior, called “thin slices,” can yield surprisingly
discussed as “logical” versus “objective” by Gilbert (1998). valid judgments of personality and important interpersonal
Person Perception and Accuracy 683

outcomes (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992, 1993; Rule, Ambady, or where they are may be better adjusted than individuals
Adams, & Macrae, 2008). who are more difficult to judge. In the end, the “what you
see is what you get” factor appears to be an important
Moderators of Accuracy property of the target individual that affects the likelihood
Research has identified four basic moderators that affect of accurate personality judgment.
the likelihood of making an accurate personality judgment: Personality traits also vary in the degree to which they
(1) properties of the judge, (2) properties of the target indi- are easy to judge. For example, imagine someone you know
vidual who is being judged, (3) properties of the trait that and rate the degree to which that person is talkative on a
is being judged, and (4) properties of the information on scale of 1 to 5. It seems easy enough. Now try to rate the
which the personality judgment is based. degree to which that person is deceptive. At first glance this
The oldest and perhaps most studied moderator is prop- task might seem easy, but further consideration reveals that
erties of the judge. Early research suggested that judges it is not straightforward. By definition, deception is a trait
who are highly intelligent and conscientious render the that describes people who purposefully lead others to form
most accurate judgments of personality; however, this was false impressions of them. Therefore, if you know a person
the research that was harshly criticized for using inadequate who is skilled in deception, then you really do not know
methods (e.g., Colvin & Bundick, 2001; Cronbach, 1955). that person and your personality judgment will be inaccu-
More recent research suggests that judges who are higher rate. It is no surprise, then, that those personality traits that
in “communion,” invested in developing and maintain- are more evaluatively neutral (neither socially desirable nor
ing interpersonal relationships, are more accurate judges socially undesirable) and traits associated with observable
of personality (Vogt & Colvin, 2003). Another study sug- behaviors are more likely to be judged accurately (Funder &
gests that people who generally describe others in favor- Dobroth, 1987; John & Robins, 1993). Talkativeness
able terms (e.g., friendly and helpful) are more likely to be can be judged with a high degree of accuracy because it
accurate because most people are generally friendly and is a trait that is relatively neutral and it can be directly
helpful (Letzring & Funder, 2006). observed. Deception, on the other hand, usually cannot be
Overall, the good judge seems to be someone who is judged with a high degree of accuracy because, although
socially engaged and optimistic about people (Letzring, it is possible to imagine overt behaviors that would allow
2008). However, there may be subtle differences in the per- deception to be identified (e.g., a behavior that contradicts
sonality characteristics that are associated with accuracy a false impression), these behaviors are purposefully con-
between male and female judges. Kolar (1996) found that cealed and are therefore not easily observed.
the most accurate male judges of personality have a confi- Those who wonder just which traits are visible and
dent and outgoing interpersonal style, while the most accu- which are not probably already know the answer. Funder
rate female judges are more likely to be open to and have a and Dobroth (1987) asked undergraduates to rate the
high interest in others. This finding hints that being a good degree to which each of the 100 personality attributes in
judge has important consequences for the judge, consistent the CAQ was easy or difficult to judge. The aggregate of
with a recent report that people who are good at identifying their ratings correlated r ⫽ .42 (p < .001) with the degree to
facial expressions associated with fear tend to act in proso- which these attributes manifested good interjudge agree-
cial ways such as donating money or helping someone in ment (between self and others’ ratings and between the
need (Marsch, Kozak, & Ambady, 2007). ratings of different raters of the same target) in judg-
Another moderator of accuracy concerns the target, or ments of real target people. Consistent findings have since
the person who is judged. Some people seem like an open been reported many times, including studies by Bernieri,
book, while other people are more elusive and difficult to Zuckerman, Koestner, and Rosenthal (1994); Borkenau
know. Colvin (1993) found that individuals who behave and Liebler (1995); Kenny, Albright, Malloy, and Kashy
in a more consistent manner, regardless of the situation or (1994); McCrae (1982); Park and Judd (1989); and Watson
the people with whom they are interacting, are easier to (1989). The most visible traits tend to be relevant to attri-
accurately judge than people who seem to have a different butes such as social skill and extraversion, and the least
personality for every situation and person they encounter. visible traits are associated with relatively difficult-to-see
Interestingly, Jourard (1971) hypothesized that individuals attributes such as “motivation to work” (Gifford, Ng, &
who put on a psychological façade, and for whom there Wilkinson, 1985).
is a large discrepancy between who they are on the inside This finding might seem obvious, and it probably is,
and what they display on the outside, are more likely to at least in retrospect. It amounts to the profound conclu-
be psychologically maladjusted. This suggests that individ- sion that more visible traits are easier to see. However,
uals who behave the same regardless of who they are with the research has two important implications. One stems
684 Personality in Social Psychology

from the finding that people in general can rate trait vis- a person is relatively free to behave as he or she wishes at a
ibility accurately. When set against research implying party, and observing behavior in that situation would likely
people are poor interpersonal judges, it suggests at least be more revealing. Letzring, Wells, and Funder (2006)
one area in which people know when their judgments observed that people who met in an unstructured situa-
are more likely and less likely to be accurate. A second tion, where they could talk about whatever they wanted,
implication concerns the suggestion, sometimes raised, made more accurate personality judgments of one another
that personality judgments by acquaintances are based on compared with those who met under more structured cir-
overheard reputation or superficial stereotypes rather than cumstances, in which they were given specific goals and
behavioral observation (e.g., McClelland, 1972). But arti- instructions on what to do. Situations relevant to the per-
ficially constructed reputations or superficial stereotypes sonality trait being judged are also more likely to provide
could concern nonvisible traits, as well as visible ones. better-quality information. For example, if you have never
More interjudge agreement is found for visible traits, sug- observed someone in a situation that affords the opportu-
gesting that one basis of this agreement is valid behavioral nity to demonstrate courage, then it would be difficult to
observation. make an accurate judgment of that person’s courageous-
Finally, the information that personality judgments are ness. Similarly, to judge a person’s sociability, observa-
based on plays an important role in the degree to which they tions of that person at a party would be more informative
are accurate. Two properties of information affect accuracy: than observations while the person is studying alone at the
quantity and quality. In terms of quantity, it is generally library (e.g., Freeberg, 1969).
found that more information is better. Personality judg-
Realistic Accuracy Model
ments by close acquaintances have been observed to agree
better with self-judgments of personality than judgments The moderators that affect accuracy can be explained in
by strangers (Funder & Colvin, 1988), and longer peri- terms of the realistic accuracy model, which proposes
ods of observation lead to greater accuracy (Blackman & that the achievement of accurate personality judgment
Funder, 1998). However, there seems to be at least one cir- has four necessary stages: relevance, availability, detec-
cumstance under which judgments by strangers can be as tion, and utilization (Funder, 1995, 1999). First, the per-
accurate as judgments by acquaintances. Colvin and Funder son being judged must do something relevant. That is, the
(1991) found that the advantage of acquaintances over target of judgment must do something informative about
strangers vanishes when their personality judgments are the trait being judged. If we want to judge the degree to
used to predict the behavior of a target person in a situation which someone is narcissistic, that person must display
that is similar to one in which the stranger has observed the a behavior that pertains to narcissism (e.g., excessively
target but the acquaintance has not. For example, your par- brag). Second, the relevant information must be avail-
ents have known you throughout your entire life but might able to observe. If the target of judgment excessively
not ever have seen you deliver an academic lecture. If your brags only in contexts that the judge does not share, then
mother was asked to predict how you would act during a the judge will not have access to this information. Third, the
lecture and one of your students was asked the same ques- judge of personality must detect the relevant and available
tion, Colvin and Funder ’s findings suggest that your mom information. If the judge is distracted or does not notice
and the student would make approximately equally accu- the narcissist’s bragging, then the information will not
rate predictions. However, if asked how you would behave figure into the judgment. Finally, the judge must cor-
in any other situation, the prediction by your mom would rectly use the relevant and available information that was
likely be more accurate than the prediction by the student. detected. If the judged noticed the narcissist bragging but
The quality of the information also affects accuracy. interpreted this behavior as charming confidence, then
Information that is gathered in relatively weak situations accurate judgment has failed at the final stage.
may be better for purposes of personality judgment than The realistic accuracy model in Figure 18.2 has several
information that is gathered in strong situations (Snyder & implications. The first is that accurate personality judgment
Ickes, 1985). Strong situations have social norms that is difficult. Four hurdles need to be overcome to achieve
restrict how people behave in them, so they dilute indi- accuracy, and a failure at any step in the process leads to
vidual differences and personality relevant information, an inaccurate judgment. Second, the model provides a way
whereas weak situations are less socially scripted and allow to anticipate and explain the four moderators previously
for wider variation in behavior. For example, a person’s discussed. For example, a good judge of personality is
behavior during Sunday Mass is largely a function of the someone who spends enough time around people to detect
rules of the situation, and observing behavior would not available and relevant information and is able to use this
likely yield much information about personality; however, information correctly. A good target is someone who emits
Competition 685

Relevance Availability Detection Utilization

Personality Judgment
(Achievement) Figure 18.2 The realistic accuracy model.

genuine cues that are personality relevant in a range of It is not difficult to imagine an interdisciplinary effort that
settings, enhancing the availability of these cues. A good combined social cognition and accuracy research into one
trait is one for which many relevant cues are available for big theory that explained how people perceive one another.
detection, and finally, good information involves expo- However, deep philosophical and methodological differ-
sure to relevant cues. The final implication of the realistic ences between social and personality psychology have
accuracy model is that it suggests four ways to improve stood in the way of this integration.
the accuracy of personality judgment. Much person per- The differences in research methodology are also asso-
ception research in social psychology focuses on cogni- ciated with differences in data analysis. Social psychology
tive errors and biases that occur at the utilization stage of research almost always analyzes data using analysis of
the realistic accuracy model; however, accurate personal- variance, which prominently displays p levels and obscures
ity judgment is more than correct thinking, and judgment effect sizes.6 In contrast, personality psychology almost
could be improved at the three other stages. always uses correlational and regression analyses, and
One way to be a better judge of personality is simply these analyses are fundamentally based on and reported
to pay more attention to people (i.e., detection). If relevant in terms of effect size. Effect sizes and p levels have an
and available cues are flying around but a judge is not pay- algebraic equivalence that allows one to be converted into
ing attention, then accurate judgment fails. Another way to the other (the only additional piece of information needed
become a better judge is to behave in ways that create situ- is N, the sample size), but this simple fact and its impli-
ations in which people express their true selves. Letzring cations have not been universally appreciated. The blind
(2008) found that observers were able to make more accu- spot concerning effect sizes and the mutual failure of each
rate judgments of people observed in situations that included field to fully appreciate the methodological and philo-
individuals who were good judges themselves. People might sophical approach of the other left the fields of personality
be more inclined to “be themselves” in social situations and social psychology vulnerable to two key influences:
where they feel comfortable. When people are self-conscious Mischel’s (1968) critique of personality traits and Lee
or worried about the impression they are making, they are Ross’s (1977) descriptions of the shortcomings of every-
probably less likely to emit personality relevant information. day social perceivers.
This might be why socially skilled and agreeable people tend
to make more accurate judgments of personality. Finally,
Two Converging Critiques
another way to be a better judge is to spend a lot of time with
the target one wishes to judge. Observing people in numer- Mischel (1968) reviewed various empirical personality
ous and various situations makes it more likely that relevant studies to evaluate a core assumption of trait theory that,
cues will become available. For example, when trying to get in his opinion, had not been adequately addressed previ-
to know a potential love interest on a first date, it is probably ously. He examined evidence concerning the degree to
not the best idea to go to a movie. This kind of situation offers which behavior is consistent across different situations and
little chance to observe much about personality, whereas tak- concluded that observations of similar behaviors rarely
ing someone on a hike or to the zoo (situations that allow for
interpersonal interaction) would likely yield better informa- 6 Experimental research focuses on p levels rather than effect
tion and a more accurate first impression.
sizes because laboratory studies aim to demonstrate the exis-
tence of causal effects rather than their size, which in a labora-
tory is largely determined by specific aspects of the experimental
COMPETITION procedure (e.g., how prominently a stimulus is displayed). The
real-world importance of experimentally discovered effects must
Although the accuracy and person perception paradigms be determined in field studies, often with correlational design
differ in many ways, they are potentially complementary. (Mook, 1983).
686 Personality in Social Psychology

correlate with each other greater than r ⫽ .30. Mischel In principle, there is no necessary connection between
cautioned readers against being misled by a “significant” the situationist critique of personality and the promulga-
correlation of this size. He argued that a .30 correlation tion of the FAE within social psychology. A particular trait
could be significant if a sample size is large enough and might have a powerful influence on a particular behavior,
that squaring a correlation (to yield the percentage of vari- and people still might overestimate its effect by believing
ance explained) is more informative than focusing on it to be even stronger than it is.8 However, such a connec-
p levels. This led to the conclusion that a correlation of tion was established. Prominent writers drew direct analo-
.30 among behaviors is small because it means that traits gies between the shortcomings of personality psychology
account for less than 10% of the variance. By subtraction, identified by the Mischelian critique and the shortcomings
it was implicitly assumed that the remaining 90% of vari- of “lay personality theory” exemplified by the FAE (e.g.,
ance in behavior must be accounted for by details of the Ross & Nisbett, 1991, pp. 120–139). This convergence
situation.7 In Mischel’s (1968, p. 37) own words, “It is evi- led to the establishment of the FAE as a foundation of the
dent that the behaviors which are often construed as stable way that social psychology came to view personality psy-
personality trait indicators actually are highly specific and chology. The main message of social psychology became
depend on the details of the evoking situations.” This mes- advertised as “the power of the social situation is much
sage—sometimes called “situationism” (Bowers, 1973)— greater than most people believe.” One major introductory
was accepted by many readers and created an adverse textbook describes this claim as “perhaps the single most
climate for personality research. important lesson from social psychology” (both quotes
Meanwhile, research on person perception was blos- from Gazzaniga & Heatherton, 2006, p. 634). A fairly
soming in social psychology. Although some researchers direct implication of this lesson would seem to be that per-
focused on demonstrating that social perceivers follow sonality matters less than most people, including personal-
logical or rational models when making judgments (Kelley, ity psychologists, believe.
1967; McArthur, 1972), others concentrated on demon- A classic example that is often cited as strong evi-
strating biases, errors, and imperfections. Jones’s (1985) dence of situational power is Milgram’s obedience study.
comment notwithstanding, their findings were generally Milgram (1963, 1974/2004) demonstrated that a surprising
interpreted as showing how and when people were wrong. number of people will administer lethal shocks to an inno-
People were observed to erroneously attribute the causes of cent victim if a perceived authority figure commands it.
their failure to external sources and the causes of their suc- Furthermore, a panel of psychiatrists erroneously predicted
cess to internal sources (Davis & Davis, 1972), erroneously that fully 98% of Milgram’s participants would disobey
use self-referent information as an anchor from which to such an order. In textbook after textbook, this study has
infer information about others (the false consensus effect; been described as demonstrating the power of situations
Ross, Greene, & House, 1977), and as previously men- to elicit bad behavior and the vulnerability of even expert
tioned, erroneously assume a correspondence between an psychological judges to the FAE.
author ’s written opinions and private opinions, even when
armed with the knowledge that the author had no choice Evaluating the Power of Personality
in the point of view expressed (Jones & Harris, 1967).
Ross (1977) summarized this literature and proposed After 40 years of research, a variety of evidence indicates
that many errors in social judgment could be explained that personality research was given a bad rap. Starting with
by an underlying illusion common to everyday social per- Mischel (1968), the critique of traits was based on a lim-
ceivers and personality psychologists alike. The FAE, he ited perspective on trait theory and an incomplete analysis
proposed, is the tendency for people to overestimate the of the implications of effect sizes. The basic critique relies
influence of dispositional factors on behavior and to under- on the assumption that the viability of trait psychology
estimate the influence of situational factors. Ross (1977) requires that a single trait predict a single behavior. For
cited Mischel’s (1968) critique of trait theory as further example, Mischel reported a study in which the correlation
evidence of this illusion and proposed that an important between positive attention-seeking behavior (e.g., seeking
direction for social psychology was to continue document- praise) and negative attention-seeking behavior (e.g., dis-
ing the ways in which everyday social perceivers and trait ruption) in preschool children was r ⫽ .23 (Sears, 1963).
theorists fail to appreciate the power of situations. This correlation was deemed low because both behaviors are
thought to be indicators of the broader trait of dependency.

7For a discussion of the common but questionable practice of


squaring r’s to yield “variance explained” (see Ozer, 1985). 8I thank the editors of this book for raising this important point.
Competition 687

To the eyes of a trait theorist, however, .23 is a surprisingly Table 18.1 The BESD for a Study in Which a Drug Had No
Effect
high correlation for the two behaviors because, although
they might both be manifestations of dependency, it is easy Alive Dead Total

to imagine other traits that would independently influ- Treatment 50 50 100


ence each of them. A highly agreeable child who is high Control 50 50 100
in dependency would probably seek positive attention but Total 100 100 200
not negative attention, and a highly aggressive child who BESD, binomial effect size display.
is high in dependency would probably seek negative atten-
tion but not positive attention. In short, trait theory does
Table 18.2 The BESD for a Drug Study with an Effect Size of
not assume that one trait can perfectly predict one behav- r ⴝ .30
ior; rather, it expects that any number of traits can influ- Alive Dead Total
ence a single behavior.
Treatment 65 35 100
Ahadi and Diener (1989) used Monte Carlo simulations
Control 35 65 100
to determine the maximum possible correlation that a per-
Total 100 100 200
sonality researcher could hope to find given that multiple
BESD, binomial effect size display.
traits influence a given behavior. They found that when
three traits influence two behaviors and the two behaviors
are influenced by one common trait, the upper bound corre- Table 18.3 The BESD for a Study of Job Performance with an
lation between the two behaviors is approximately r ⫽ .30. Effect Size of r ⴝ .30

When they adjusted the model so that four traits influenced High Performance Low Performance Total
two behaviors and the two behaviors were influenced by High 65 35 100
conscientious
one common trait, the upper bound correlation between the
two behaviors was approximately r ⫽ .25. These estimates Low 35 65 100
conscientious
(.30 and .25) are comparable to the magnitude of correla-
Total 100 100 200
tion previously mentioned regarding dependency behav-
iors, and more importantly, the estimates are comparable BESD, binomial effect size display.

to the correlations typically found in personality research.


Moreover, Ahadi and Diener ’s (1989) data analytical model
assumed that only five personality traits exist and behav- participants in the treatment group were significantly more
ior is completely determined by those five traits, so their likely to be alive at the end of the study than participants
model overestimates the correlations among behaviors that in the control group and that the magnitude of effect for the
we would expect to find in the real world where situations, treatment was r ⫽ .30. To display the effect size in terms of
moods, and various other factors influence behavior. Their the likely outcome in the four cells of Table 18.2, the fol-
findings suggest that the typical correlation among behav- lowing equation is used to determine the number of people
iors in different situations, r ⫽ .30, is impressive rather who achieve the predicted outcome: 50 ⫹ (r/2 ⫻ 100.)
than trivial. In this case, the calculation is: 50 ⫹ (.30/2 ⫻ 100) ⫽ 65,
A second problem with the critique involves the idea where 65 represents the number of people that would be
that r ⫽ .30 is a small effect because squaring it reveals expected to live out of 100 people in the treatment group
how little behavioral variance (e.g., 9%) is explained by (the upper left-hand cell of the table). Because this table
traits. Rather than squaring correlations, another, perhaps has one degree of freedom, the numbers in the remaining
more informative way to evaluate an effect size is to calcu- cells can be computed by simple subtraction.
late the binomial effect size display (BESD; Rosenthal & It should be apparent from Table 18.2 that a treatment
Rubin, 1982). The BESD is an intuitively accessible way to with an effect size of .30 increases the survival rate from
display the practical importance of an effect size in terms 35% to 65% and that this is indeed a notable difference.
of dichotomous outcomes. For example, imagine that Would you want the treatment? In terms of personality
a researcher has conducted a study examining the effect coefficients, suppose that a researcher found that ratings of
of a medication in which 100 participants received the conscientiousness (e.g., high ratings vs. low ratings) cor-
medication and 100 participants were in the control group. related with work performance (e.g., high performance vs.
If the medication had zero effect (e.g., r ⫽ 0), then people low performance) at r ⫽ .30. As can be seen in Table 18.3,
in the treatment and control groups would be equally likely this means that a recruiter could greatly increase the
to live or die. This outcome is displayed in Table 18.1. chances of correctly hiring a high-performing employee by
However, suppose that the experimenter observed that selecting applicants who score high on conscientiousness
688 Personality in Social Psychology

(e.g., 65% of those who are high in conscientiousness will are being watched—instead of (more correctly) to the
perform well, and 65% of those who are low in conscien- diligence of the workers themselves.
tiousness will perform poorly). The BESD illustrates how Overattribution to the situation occurs in the domains
an effect size of .30 is large enough to be important under of emotion and happiness, as well as in the world of work.
many circumstances. People overestimate the long-term effects of positive and
Finally, Funder and Ozer (1983) examined three promi- negative events on their emotional well-being; one team of
nent studies in social psychology that are universally rec- researchers surmised on the basis of their results that even
ognized as impressive demonstrations of the influence of the effects of seemingly strong situations as winning the
situational factors on behavior. These studies included Nobel Prize or having one’s academic department abol-
Festinger and Carlsmith’s (1959) study of attitude report ished might have weaker long-term effects than we tend
as a function of incentive for counterattitudinal advo- to assume (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley,
cacy, Darley and Latané’s (1968) and Darley and Batson’s 1998, p. 617). Their research on “emotional forecasting”
(1973) studies of bystander intervention as a function of shows that people overestimate the affective impact of life
the number of other people who are present and the degree events. More generally, research from several laboratories
to which one is in a hurry, and Milgram’s (1974/2004) has consistently shown that people tend to overestimate the
study on obedience to instructions to harm another person influence of the situation on happiness and underestimate
as a function of the proximity of the authority figure and the influence of dispositional factors (Diener & Lucas,
proximity of the victim. For each of these studies, analy- 1999; Lykken & Tellegen, 1996; Lyubomirsky et al.,
sis of variance procedures or t-tests were originally used 2005). As one investigator summarized the evidence,
to demonstrate situational effects; however, Funder and
Ozer (1983) used the available published data to calculate Research shows that external life circumstances have a sur-
the corresponding effect sizes and found that they ranged prisingly small effect on happiness and well-being. In con-
from .36 to .42, a magnitude often observed in personality trast, personality traits and other stable factors appear to play
research. Of course, these effect sizes were calculated from a more important role in happiness. Happiness is heritable,
some of the most prominent studies in the social psycho- stable over time, and moderately to strongly associated with
logical literature. A more thorough review found that the personality characteristics. (Lucas, 2007, p. 168, emphasis
average effect size of social psychological experiments is added; see also Lucas, 2008)
equivalent to an r ⫽ .21 (sd ⫽ .15; Richard et al., 2003).
Personality traits and situations cannot be distinguished The study that most directly showed the FAE going
from one another on the basis of effect size, at least not to opposite to the traditionally expected direction was an
the advantage of situations. experiment by Quattrone (1982). In a close replication of
the pioneering study by Jones and Harris (1967), Quattrone
Fundamental Attribution Error Reconsidered asked subjects to estimate the situational forces that deter-
mined the essay-writing behavior by authors who, subjects
This conclusion suggests that the everyday social percep- were clearly informed, had free choice of what to write.
tion that people behave in a relatively consistent manner is By making situational forces instead of attitudes salient,
more than mere illusion. Therefore, social perceivers’ ten- and asking participants the opposite question from the one
dency to infer dispositional causes as the source of others, employed in the classic study, Quattrone reversed the clas-
behavior may not be an error, much less a fundamental one. sic correspondence bias—in this case, subjects overattrib-
We do not argue that the FAE really runs in the opposite uted to the situation.
direction (that personality is more powerful than is usually Everyday experience suggests that such reversals occur
acknowledged), but a surprising number of studies, as well outside the laboratory as well. For example, recall the last
as everyday observations, suggest this might sometimes be time that you discussed politics with someone holding
the case. contrary beliefs. Regardless of how elegant, rational, and
convincing your arguments might have been, did this con-
Empirical Considerations
versational situation change the other person’s mind about
An intriguing early study by Strickland (1958) found that anything? To frame this question in terms of the direction
supervisors trust employees less the more they supervise of attributional error, did your lucid arguments affect the
them. In a context where workers actually perform the other person’s beliefs more, or less, than you expected?
same whether they are supervised or not, supervisors over- Other illuminating examples arise when a parent tries to
attribute the work they directly supervise to the situation— affect the behavior of a child or spouses try to change each
they believe the workers are performing only because they other ’s behavior. Countless books offer advice on how to
Competition 689

parent children and how to change behavior in relationships; The principal conflict of the experiment . . . is between
but ask any parent or couple about their ability to effec- the experimenter’s demands that he continue to administer the
tively influence what the other person does—regardless electric shock and the learner ’s demands, which become
of the degree of control one has of that individual’s situa- increasingly insistent, that the experiment be stopped.
(1974/2004, p. 26)
tion (which in the case of parents and relationship partners
is considerable)—and most likely you will hear frustrated
reports about a disappointing—and, somehow, always Indeed, Milgram found that as each force increased it
surprising—failure to bring about the desired change. affected behavior in a predictable manner: The closer the
A final example is provided by the many expensive experimenter was to the subject, the more obedience was
governmental and other programs intended to curb drug obtained; the closer the victim was to the subject, the more
abuse, risky sexual activity, drunk driving, and other unde- disobedience was obtained.
sirable behaviors. It would not be correct to say that these In addition, the experiment evokes two dispositional
programs are never effective, but it does seem safe to forces toward compassion and compliance, which also
observe that they generally have less success than is hoped compete within every subject. As Milgram noted,
for and expected in their optimistic early days. In all of
these examples, the moral is clear: People often are just There were both obedient and defiant outcomes, frequently
going to do what they are going to do. Attempts to derail accompanied by extreme tension. A conflict develops between
these tendencies through manipulations of the situational the deeply ingrained disposition not to harm others and the
context are difficult at best and, often, are more difficult equally compelling tendency to obey others who are in author-
than would-be behavior changers expect. ity. (1974/2004, pp. 42–43)

Conceptual Considerations
Thus, the surprising degree of obedience obtained by
A deeper, conceptual complication with the “situations are Milgram can be explained in one of four ways:
more powerful than people think” mantra is that explana-
tions for behavior that exclusively attribute causality to 1. The situational forces toward obedience (e.g., the orders
either the person or the situation may not be as coherent as of the authority figure) were stronger than the situational
they seem. Consider two possible ways in which a mother forces toward compassion (e.g., the victim’s cries).
might explain the cause of her child’s behavior: “Joey didn’t
2. The subjects’ dispositions toward obedience were stron-
finish his homework because it was too hard” versus “Joey
ger than the subjects’ dispositions toward compassion.
didn’t finish his homework because he gives up too easily.”
Although the first explanation seems to attribute causality 3. The situational forces toward obedience were stronger
to the situation (the homework) and the second explanation than the dispositional forces toward compassion.
seems to attribute causality to the person (Joey), an inter- 4. The dispositional forces toward obedience were stron-
nal versus external dichotomy oversimplifies what these ger than the situational forces toward compassion.
explanations imply. The seemingly situational explanation
implies something about personality; it implies that Joey Explanation 3 is the standard one found in many text-
has a disposition toward giving up when things are tough. books. Explanation 4 reverses the FAE, and might be
Moreover, the seemingly dispositional explanation implies a considered heretical. However, explanations 3 and 4 are
situational influence; it implies that something about Joey’s actually equivalent (they mean the same thing). Worse,
homework (i.e., it must be difficult because it requires per- both are incoherent because they rely on a simple inter-
sistence) influenced his behavior. Almost any explanation nal versus external dichotomy that pits the person against
for the cause of a behavior implies something about the per- the situation. The reality is that at any given point, person
son and the situation. Especially smart psychologists (e.g., variables and situational variables both affect behavior and
Gilbert, 1998; Ross, 1977) have long understood that the they both take part in the net result. One of them does not
distinction between personal and situational causation is “win” because each is necessary for the other. The situa-
fuzzier and more complex than it might seem at first. tional force toward obedience in the Milgram study would
Consider, again, the Milgram obedience study. Looked have no effect on someone lacking an inclination to obey,
at broadly, the experimental situation contains at least two and an inclination to obey produces no harmful effect in
situational forces and two dispositional ones. The experi- the absence of orders.
menter’s orders constitute an obvious situational force in the In contrast, explanations 1 and 2 are coherent and cor-
direction of obedience; the victim’s cries are a situational rect, but they are also equivalent to each other. The situ-
force in the direction of disobedience. As Milgram noted, ational forces toward obedience were stronger than the
690 Personality in Social Psychology

situational forces toward compassion, and the disposi- standard offered by Ross (1977) and Gilbert (1998), the
tion to obey was generally stronger than the disposition psychiatrists committed the inverse of the FAE.
to resist. Notice that these two statements are equivalent As we promised, the purpose of this discussion is not
because, again, situational forces work in inextricable tan- to argue that personality factors are generally more power-
dem with the dispositions to respond to them.9 ful than situational factors or to assert that the consistent
Is there any way to separate situational from dispo- direction of the FAE is opposite to the one traditionally
sitional causation of behavior? Ross (1977) provided an claimed. Rather, we hope to offer a reductio ad absurdum
interesting solution to this conundrum. He suggested in support of the point that an adversarial contrast between
that situational causes can be distinguished from disposi- situations and people has for too long defined much of the
tional causes by examining the degree to which a behav- relationship between personality and social psychology. A
ior is unique or uncommon. When all or almost all people more productive direction will involve turning attention to
behave the same way in a situation, it seems fair to con- how situational and personality variables together deter-
clude that the behavior was situationally influenced. But mine behavior—in another word, interactionism.
when behavior varies within a situation (at the extreme,
50% of people behave in one way and 50% in the other),
then behavior would seem more influenced by individual TOWARD A COOPERATIVE SOCIAL–
dispositions. PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY
This method of distinguishing situational from disposi-
tional causation is illuminating and according to one writer, The most useful way to consider situational and personal
is “a logical standard [that] does not seem . . . to have any variables is as interactional partners. This view was oper-
serious competition” (Gilbert, 1998, p. 135). However, it ationalized in Lewin’s (1936) well-known formula B ⫽ f
has some surprising implications. For example, it means (P,S), which explicitly defines behavior as a function of the
that the widely studied “false consensus bias” (Ross et al., person and the situation. This equation implies that if we
1977), in which people see their own behavior as more knew all relevant psychological properties of a person and
common than it really is, leads people to over estimate the all relevant properties of that individual’s situation, we could
influence of the situation on themselves rather than, as is predict with high precision what the person would do.
more commonly argued, underestimating the effect of the Lewin’s equation can be further arranged to illumi-
situation on other people.10 nate other associations among behavior, personality,
The application of this standard to the FAE in Milgram’s and situations (Bandura, 1978). P ⫽ f (B,S) implies that
study is even more surprising, because it sets the standard people can be conceptualized in terms of their behav-
interpretation on its head. When Milgram’s panel of psy- iors in every situation of their lives. This arrangement
chiatrists predicted that 98% of participants would behave of the equation exemplifies classical Watsonian behav-
the same way (by disobeying), the psychiatrists underes- iorism (Watson, 1930) and characterizes Mischel and
timated the amount of behavioral variance and, therefore, Shoda’s (1995) “if–then” statements. Another way to
over estimated the power of the situation. They expected a arrange the equation, S ⫽ f (B,P), suggests that a situa-
98–2 split; the real split was much closer to even.11 By the tion can be understood in terms of the behaviors that
different people might perform in it. This idea formed the
basis for the template-matching technique introduced by
Bem and Funder (1978). For example, it is possible to
9 There is a good chance that Milgram would have agreed. He describe a college campus in terms of the kind of people
once wrote that the “social psychology of the reactive individual, who commonly attend it and do well there. A college cam-
the recipient of forces and pressures emanating from outside one- pus in which students who are introverted, philosophical,
self . . . represents, of course, only one side of the coin of social and intellectual do well is a very different situation from
life” (Milgram, 1977, quoted in Blass, 2004, p. 290). a campus where better outcomes are attained by students
10Thus, studies of the false consensus bias could be added to the who are extraverted, athletic, and rambunctious.
earlier list of reversals of the FAE. The psychological triad represented by these three
11 formulae suggests that people, situations, and behav-
In the two most famous conditions, in which the experimenter
was present and the victim could be heard but not seen, the rates of iors should be studied in unison (Funder, 2006). A seri-
obedience were 63% (at Yale University) and 48% (at the fictitious ous obstacle to achieving this goal concerns the uneven
“Research Associates of Bridgeport”); the overall average rate of development of conceptualizations and measurement
obedience across 18 experimental conditions was 37.5% (Milgram, technologies. Although many methods are available for
1974/2004, Tables 2–5; see Krueger & Funder, 2004, p. 327). assessing personality, relatively fewer methods have been
Toward a Cooperative Social–Personality Psychology 691

established for studying behavior and almost no methods constructs and social situations. It has been used to illus-
exist for describing situations. trate the independence of behavioral change and consis-
The conceptualization and measurement of personality tency (Funder & Colvin, 1991), the behavioral correlates
traits are well developed and range from the assessment of various personality traits (e.g., Fast, Reimer, & Funder,
of a small number of essential global traits (e.g., the Big 2008), and for many other purposes.12
Five) to large, comprehensive sets of midlevel character- Numerous researchers have complained about the lack of
istics that describe many ways in which individuals dif- methods for describing features of situations (e.g., Funder,
fer (e.g., the CAQ). Moreover, numerous trait measures 2000; Hogan & Roberts, 2000; Reis, 2008; Swann &
come packaged with a theory that explains the behaviors Seyle, 2005). In social psychological experiments, a situa-
and outcomes to which the trait is purportedly related and tional manipulation is typically chosen to test a specific the-
an adequate validity literature that addresses psychometric oretical prediction, not because it is necessarily viewed as
properties and observed external correlates. an important dimension of situations in general. Social psy-
In contrast, true behavioral measurement (i.e., direct chology could be said to contain a huge amount of informa-
observations of behavior by independent observers who tion about how narrowly defined situations affect behavior,
describe a behavior that they have seen someone do) is rarer but this knowledge is fragmented because there exists no
than one might think (Baumeister, Vohs, & Funder, 2007). method for organizing findings into a coherent summary.
Systematic attempts to develop a taxonomy of behavior What is needed is a way to conceptualize and measure the
within a theoretical framework are even rarer. Behavior, active ingredients of situations. This goal requires identify-
when it is actually observed, is almost always chosen to ing attributes that can be used to describe all situations, a
illustrate a particular theoretical prediction, and the typi- daunting task. Gilbert and Malone (1995, p. 25) observed
cal study uses one behavior, which might be something that “when one tries to point to a situation, one often stabs
as detached from real-life action as a button press on a empty air. Indeed, the constructs to which the word situa-
computer keyboard or a questionnaire response. Although tion refers often have little or no physical manifestation.”
classic social psychology studies in the 1960s and 1970s Thus far, researchers have suggested that situations can
sometimes directly observed single behaviors that were be described along three conceptual levels (Block & Block,
important and consequential (e.g., bystander intervention 1981; Saucier, Bel-Bahar, & Fernandez, 2007). Level 1,
and obedience), focusing on a single behavior provides a the broadest level, involves objective aspects of situations
narrow view of the many different things that people might that are relatively resistant to differences in perception.
be doing at the same time. For example, when people obey According to Saucier et al. (2007), this includes factors
commands by an authority figure to shock a victim, do they such as temperature and the number of people present.
plead with the authority figure that shocking the victim Level 2 involves describing situations in terms of an over-
seems wrong? Do they ask the authority figure how dan- arching characterization that most people in the situation
gerous the shocks are? Do they try to communicate with would agree on—a research seminar, a funeral, a party, and
the victim? (Milgram, 1974/2004, informally reported that so on. Level 3 is comparatively subjective and involves
his subjects did all of these things but did not provide any properties of situations that are psychologically provoking;
direct measurements.) In short, broader conceptualization for each individual, the specific provoking properties may
and measurement of behavior is sorely needed. be different.
The Riverside Behavioral Q-set (RBQ; Funder, Furr, & The problem with the level 1 description is that it is
Colvin, 2000; RBQ 3.0) is one possible, partial solution unlikely to capture the psychologically active features of
to this problem. The RBQ is a comprehensive set of 67 situations. Gilbert and Malone (1995) suggest that more
items that describe a range of socially relevant behaviors. subtle aspects, such as another person smiling or making
RBQ items describe behavior at a midlevel of generality so eye contact with a person, are likely to provoke psycho-
that the behaviors that are coded are not too microscopic logical reactions. The problem with level 3 is that it is too
(e.g., eye blinks) or too macroscopic (e.g., socially suc- solipsistic and renders the study of people in situations
cessful). The items were originally derived from the items impossible (Reis, 2008). Indeed, it absorbs the analysis
of the CAQ (Bem & Funder, 1978; Block, 1978, 2007) and of situations into the analysis of people, because it would
were reworded to emphasize behavioral display rather than require, for example, that a “noisy party” be described as
trait inference. For example, an item in the CAQ reads “is
critical, skeptical, not easily impressed” and the associated
RBQ item reads “expresses criticism.” The RBQ is a valu- 12For the current version of the RBQ and other Q-sets, and a free
able tool for a researcher who is interested in measuring computer program that simplifies the process of Q-sorting, see
various behaviors that are relevant to a range of personality http://www.rap.ucr.edu/qsorter.
692 Personality in Social Psychology

attractive for an extravert and aversive for an introvert. The next step, mapping the interactions between
Redescribed in that way, the noisy party disappears. In personality and situational variables, will also be necessary
contrast, level 2 is at a midlevel of analysis that has the but difficult. Interactions only get the variance left after the
potential to reveal psychologically active features of situa- main effects of people and situations have had their way.
tions in an empirical, reasonably consensual manner. So as any active researcher knows, they tend to be frag-
The Riverside Situational Q-set (RSQ: Funder & ile things, difficult to find and more difficult to replicate
Wagerman, 2008; Wagerman & Funder, 2009) has been (Chaplin, 1991). Perhaps it is enough to ask, for now, for a
offered as a new method to describe situations at level 2. renewed research focus on these critically important main
Similar to the RBQ previously discussed, the items of the effects, of people and situations on behavior, about which
RSQ derive from the items of the CAQ (Bem & Funder, we still know far too little. This enterprise may offer the
1978; Block, 1978, 2007). The 100 CAQ items were exam- best hope of at last reuniting the long-estranged siblings
ined for situational relevance and reworded to describe of personality and social psychology in a way that would
characteristics of situations that afford the opportunity to have made their parents proud.
express each of the corresponding personality characteris-
tics. For example, the CAQ item, “is critical, skeptical, not
easily impressed” has an associated RSQ item that reads,
“Someone is trying to impress someone or convince some- REFERENCES
one of something.” The idea is that the degree to which
an individual is critical or skeptical might be revealed in Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D., & Sanford, N. (1950).
The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper.
a situation in which another person is trying to be impres-
Ahadi, S., & Diener, E. (1989). Multiple determinants and effect size.
sive or convincing. The advantage of basing the RSQ on Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 398–406.
the CAQ is that items are specifically intended to describe Allport, F. H. (1924). Social psychology. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
aspects of situations that are personologically relevant. Allport, F. H., & Allport, G. W. (1921). Personality traits: Their classifica-
Likely, the particular items of the RBQ and the RSQ fail tion and measurement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
to address all essential attributes of behavior and of situa- 16, 6–40.
tions, and certainly their representation of these domains is Allport, G. W. (1931). What is a trait of personality? Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 25, 368–372.
incomplete. The development of theoretical conceptions of
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New
behaviors and situations, and of the assessment tools to York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
make these conceptions addressable through empirical Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt,
research, is a long-term project that will take long years Rinehart, & Winston.
of work by many investigators before it comes to fruition. Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names: A psycho-lexical
The intention of this section of this chapter is simply to study. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 47, 171–220.
(1, While No. 211).
suggest that it is time to begin.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behav-
An important agenda for future research is to return our ior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis.
attention to important behaviors in meaningful situations, Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.
where situational and personality variables are assessed Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a minute: Predicting teacher
alongside each other and treated in an equivalent manner. evaluations from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical
attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(3),
A catalogue of the main effects of situational variables 431–441.
on behavior, to place alongside the (too thin, but slowly Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissocia-
expanding) catalogue of main effects of personality vari- tion between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case
ables on behavior, would offer a useful contribution to psy- of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
380–393.
chological understanding.
Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
It is worth pausing for a moment to realize how impor- Prentice Hall.
tant an accomplishment it would be to have a map of how a Bandura, A. (1977a). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral
range of personality variables affect behavior, alongside change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
a map of how a range of situational variables (not just a Bandura, A. (1977b). Social learning theory. New York: General Learning
few) affect behavior. This endeavor might come to be the Press.
Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American
psychological equivalent of the human genome project.
Psychologist, 33, 344–358.
This project would not itself be theoretical (neither is the
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on
human genome project) but rather would be a wide-ranging Psychological Science, 1, 28–58.
descriptive enterprise to gather descriptions of what people Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2000). Self-esteem,
actually do in the diverse situations of their lives. narcissism, and aggression: Does violence result from low self-esteem or
References 693

from threatened egotism? Current Directions in Psychological Science, Bowers, K. S. (1973). Situationism in psychology: An analysis and cri-
9(1), 26–29. tique. Psychological Review, 80, 307–336.
Baumeister, R. F., Dale, K., & Sommer, K. L. (1998). Freudian defense Brown, R. (1996). Against my better judgment: An intimate memoir of an
mechanisms and empirical findings in modern social psychology: eminent gay psychologist. New York: Harrington Park.
Reaction formation, projection, displacement, undoing, isolation, sub- Buss, D. M., & Greiling, H. (1999). Adaptive individual differences.
limation, and denial. Journal of Personality, 66, 1081–1124. Journal of Personality, 67, 209–243.
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex
egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology and psychology.
Psychological Review, 103, 5–33. Psychological Science, 3, 251–255.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as the Caldwell, D. F., & Burger, J. M. (1997). Personality and social influ-
science of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to ence strategies in the workplace. Personality and Social Psychology
actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 396–403. Bulletin, 23, 1003–1012.
Beaton, E. A., Schmidt, L. A., Ashbaugh, A. R., Santesso, D. L., Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I., et al.
Antony, M. M., McCabe, R. E., Segalowitz, D. J., & Schulkin, J. (2002, August 2). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in mal-
(2006). Low salivary cortisol levels among socially anxious young treated children. Science, 297, 851–854.
adults: Preliminary evidence from a selected and a non-selected sam-
ple. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 1217–1228. Caspi, A., Roberts, B., & Shiner, R. (2005). Personality development:
Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453–484.
Beaton, E. A., Schmidt, L. A., Schulkin, J., Antony, M. M., Swinson, R. P., &
Hall, G. B. (2008). Different neural responses to stranger and person- Cattell, R. B. (1943). The description of personality: Basic traits resolved
ally familiar faces in shy and bold adults. Behavioral Neuroscience, into clusters. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38,
122, 704–709. 476–506.
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances Chaplin, W. F. (1991). The next generation of moderator research in per-
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York: sonality psychology. Journal of Personality, 59, 143–178.
Academic Press. Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York:
Bem, D., & Funder, D. (1978). Predicting more of the people more of the Academic Press.
time: Assessing the personality of situations. Psychological Review, Colvin, C. R. (1993).“Judgable” people: Personality, behavior, and com-
85, 485–501. peting explanations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Benet-Martínez, V., & Haritatos, J. (2005). Bicultural identity integration: 64, 861–873.
Components and psychosocial antecedents. Journal of Personality, Colvin, C. R., & Bundick, M. J. (2001). In search of the good judge of per-
73, 1015–1050. sonality: Some methodological and theoretical concerns. In J. A. Hall &
Bernhardt, P. C., Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Fielden, J. A., & Lutter, D. C. (1998). F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measure-
Testosterone changes during vicarious experiences of winning and losing ment (pp. 47–65). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
among fans at sporting events. Physiology and Behavior, 65, 59–62. Colvin, C. R., & Funder, D. C. (1991). Predicting personality and behavior:
Bernieri, F. J., Zuckerman, M., Koestner, R., & Rosenthal, R. (1994). A boundary on the acquaintanceship effect. Journal of Personality and
Measuring person perception accuracy: Another look at self–other Social Psychology, 60, 884–894.
agreement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 367–378. Crews, F. (1996). The verdict on Freud. Psychological Science, 7, 63–67.
Blackman, M. C., & Funder, D. C. (1998). The effect of information on con- Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth.
sensus and accuracy in personality judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychological Review, 108, 593–623.
Social Psychology, 34, 164–181. Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Processes affecting scores on “understanding of
Blass, T. (2004). The man who shocked the world: The life and leqacy of others” and “assumed similarity.” Psychological Bulletin, 52, 177–193.
Stanley Milgram. New York: Basic Books. Cutrona, C. E. (1982). Transitions to college: Loneliness and the process
Block, J. (1978). The Q-sort method in personality assessment and psy- of social adjustment. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness:
chiatric research. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. A sourcebook of current theory, research, and therapy (pp. 291–309).
(Original work published 1961.) New York: Wiley.
Block, J. (2007). The Q-sort in character appraisal: Encoding subjec- Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Morris, R. (1990). Testosterone, social class, and anti-
tive impressions of persons quantitatively. Washington, DC: American social behavior in a sample of 4,462 men. Psychological Science, 1,
Psychological Association. 209–211.
Block, J., & Block, J. H. (1981). Studying situational dimensions: A grand Darley, J. M., & Batson, C. D. (1973). “From Jerusalem to Jericho”: A
perspective and some limited empiricism. In D. M. Magnusson (Ed.), study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behavior.
Toward a psychology of situations: An interactional perspective Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 100–108.
(pp. 85–103). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergen-
Block, J., & Block, J. H. (2006). Nursery school personality and political orien- cies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social
tation two decades later. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 734–749. Psychology, 28, 377–383.
Blum, K., Cull, J. G., Braverman, E. R., & Comings, D. E. (1996). Reward Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., & Frisesen, W. V.
deficiency syndrome. American Scientist, 84, 132–146. (1990). Approach/withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional
Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1995). Observable attributes as manifestations and expression and brain physiology. Journal of Personality and Social
cues of personality and intelligence. Journal of Personality, 63, 1–25. Psychology, 58, 330–341.
Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F., & Angleitner, A. (2004). Davis, W. L., & Davis, D. E. (1972). Internal–external control and attribu-
Thin slices of behavior as cues of personality and intelligence. Journal tion of responsibility for success and failure. Journal of Personality,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 599–614. 40, 123–136.
Bornstein, R. F. (1999). Source amnesia, misattribution, and the power of Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Personality and subjective well-being. In
unconscious perceptions and memories. Psychoanalytic Psychology, D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The founda-
16, 155–178. tions of hedonic psychology (pp. 213–229). New York: Russell Sage.
694 Personality in Social Psychology

Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1991). Explorations in behavioral con-
model. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of sistency: Properties of persons, situations, and behaviors. Journal of
psychology (Vol. 41, pp. 417–440). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews. Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 773–794.
Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1997). Congruence of self and others’
in aggressive boys. Child Development, 53, 620–635. judgments of personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.),
Downey, G., & Feldman, S. I. (1996). Implications of rejection sensi- Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 617–647). Orlando, FL:
tivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Academic Press.
Psychology, 70, 1327–1343. Funder, D. C., & Dobroth, K. M. (1987). Differences between traits:
Erdelyi, M. H. (1974). A “new look” at the New Look in perception. Properties associated with inter-judge agreement. Journal of
Psychological Review, 81, 1–25. Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 409–418.
Erdelyi, M. H. (1985). Psychoanalysis: Freud’s cognitive psychology. San Funder, D. C., Furr, R. M., & Colvin, C. R. (2000). The Riverside
Francisco: Freeman. Behavioral Q-sort: A tool for the description of social behavior. Journal
of Personality, 68, 450–489.
Estes, S. G. (1938). Judging personality from expressive behavior. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 33, 217–236. Funder, D. C., & Harris, M. J. (1986). On the several facets of personal-
ity assessment: The case of social acuity. Journal of Personality, 54,
Fast, L., & Funder, D. C. (2008). Personality as manifest in word use:
528–550.
Correlations with self-report, acquaintance report, and behavior.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 334–346. Funder, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (1983). Behavior as a function of the situation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 107–112.
Fast, L., Reimer, H., & Funder, D. C. (2008). The social behavior and
reputation of the attributionally complex. Journal of Research in Funder, D. C., & Wagerman, S. (2008, July). The Riverside Situational
Personality, 42, 208–222. Q-sort: A tool for assessing situations. Paper presented at the European
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of Conference on Personality, Tartu, Estonia.
forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, Gage, N. L., & Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Conceptual and methodological
203–210. problems in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 62,
Fiske, D. W. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality 411–422.
ratings from different sources. Journal of Abnormal and Social Galton, F. (1884). Measurement of character. Fortnightly Review, 36,
Psychology, 44, 329–344. 179–185.
Fletcher, G. J. O., Danilovics, P., Fernandez, G., Peterson, D., & Reeder, G. D. Gazzaniga, M. S., & Heatherton, T. F. (2006). Psychological science (2nd
(1986). Attributional complexity: An individual differences measure. ed.). New York: W. W. Norton.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 875–884. Gifford, R., Ng, C.-F., & Wilkinson, M. (1985). Nonverbal cues in the
Follett, K., & Hess, T. M. (2002). Aging, cognitive complexity, and the employment interview: Links between applicant qualities and inter-
fundamental attribution error. Journals of Gerontology: Series B: viewer judgments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 729–736.
Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, 312–323. Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group. (2000). Simple
Freeberg, N. E. (1969). Relevance of rater–ratee acquaintance in the validity heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.
and reliability of ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 518–524. Gilbert, D. T. (1998). Ordinary personology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &
Friedman, H. S., Tucker, J. S., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Schwartz, J. E., G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp.
Wingard, D. L., & Criqui, M. H. (1993). Does childhood personality 49–150). New York: McGraw-Hill.
predict longevity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraint:
176–185. Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and
Fuhrman, R. W., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Convergence between self and Social Psychology, 50, 269–280.
peer in the response-time processing of trait-relevant information. Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 961–974. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38.
Funder, D.C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P.
social judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75–90. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective fore-
Funder, D. C. (1994). Explaining traits. Psychological Inquiry, 5(2), casting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617–638.
125–127. Gilovich, T. (1993). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human
Funder, D. C. (1995). On the accuracy of personality judgment: A realistic reason in everyday life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
approach. Psychological Review, 102, 652–670. Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1956). Physique and delinquency. New York:
Funder, D. C. (1999). Personality judgment: A realistic approach to per- Harper.
son perception. San Diego: Academic Press. Goldberg, L. R. (1981). Language and individual differences: The search
Funder, D. C. (2000). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, for universals in personality lexicons. In L. Wheeler (Eds.), Review
197–221. of personality and social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 141–165). Beverly
Funder, D. C. (2006). Towards a resolution of the personality triad: Hills, CA: Sage.
Persons, situations and behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality, Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”:
40, 21–34. The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social
Funder, D. C. (2007). The personality puzzle (4th ed.). New York: Psychology, 59, 1216–1229.
W. W. Norton. Gosling, S. D., John, O. P., Craik, K. H., & Robins, R. W. (1998). Do peo-
Funder, D. C., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1983). Delay of gratification: ple know how they behave? Self-reported act frequencies compared
Some longitudinal personality correlates. Journal of Personality and with on-line codings by observers. Journal of Personality and Social
Social Psychology, 44, 1198–1213. Psychology, 74, 1337–1349.
Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1988). Friends and strangers: Gosling, S., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A room with
Acquaintanceship, agreement, and the accuracy of personality judg- a cue: Personality judgments based on offices and bedrooms. Journal
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 149–158. of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379–398.
References 695

Graziano, W., & Bryant, W. H. (1998). Self-monitoring and the self- Knutson, B., Wolkowitz, O. M., Cole, S., Chan, T., Moore, E. A.,
attribution of positive emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Johnson, R. C., et al. (1998). Selective alteration of personality and
Psychology, 74, 250–261. social behavior by serotonergic intervention. American Journal of
Greenberg, J. R., & Mitchell, S. A. (1983). Object relations in psychoana- Psychiatry, 155, 373–379.
lytic theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kolar, D. W. (1996). Individual differences in the ability to accurately
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring judge the personality characteristics of others. Unpublished doctoral
individual differences in implicit cognition. The implicit association dissertation, University of California, Riverside.
task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. Kross, E., Egner, T., Ochsner, K., Hirsch, J., & Downey, G. (2007). Neural
Haidt, J. (2008). What makes people vote republican? Retrieved January 23, dynamics of rejection sensitivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
2009, from www.edge.org/3rd_culture/haidt08/haidt08_index.html. 19, 945–956.
Hampson, S. E., Goldberg, L. R., Vogt, T. M., & Dubanoski, J. P. (2006). Krueger, J. I., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Towards a balanced social psychol-
Forty years on: Teachers’ assessments of childran’s personality traits ogy: Causes, consequences and cures for the problem-seeking approach
predict self-reported health behaviors and outcomes at midlife. Health to social behavior and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27,
Psychology, 25(1), 57–64. 313–376.
Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (2000). Rewarding competence: The Leary, M. R. (1999). Making sense of self-esteem. Current Directions in
importance of goals in the study of intrinsic motivation. In C. Sansone & Psychological Science, 8, 32–35.
J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The Letzring, T. D. (2008). The good judge of personality: Characteristics,
search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 79–103). San behaviors, and observer accuracy. Journal of Research in Personality,
Diego: Academic Press. 42(4), 914–932.
Haselton, M. G., & Funder, D. C. (2006). The evolution of accuracy and Letzring, T. D., & Funder, D. C. (2006, January). Relations between judge’s
bias in social judgment. In M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick personality and types of realistic accuracy. Paper presented at the
(Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 15–38). New York and annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology,
London: Psychology Press. Palm Springs, CA.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1990). Love and work: An attachment– Letzring, T. D., Wells, S., & Funder, D. C. (2006). Quantity and quality of
theoretical perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, available information affect the realistic accuracy of personality judg-
59, 270–280. ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 111–123.
Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2004). Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topographical psychology. New York:
On the selective relation of frontal cortical asymmetry and anger-out McGraw-Hill.
versus anger-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
87, 926–939. Lucas, R. E. (2007). Personality and the pursuit of happiness. Personality
and Social Psychology Compass, 1, 168–182.
Hogan, R., & Nicholson, R. A. (1988). The meaning of personality test
scores. American Psychologist, 43, 621–626. Lucas, R. E. (2008). Personality and subjective well-being. In M. Eid &
R. J. Larsen (Eds.). The science of subjective well-being (pp. 171–194).
Hogan, R., & Roberts, B. W. (2000). A socioanalytic perspective on New York: Guilford Press.
person–environment interaction. In W. B. Walsh, K. H. Craik, &
R. H. Price (Eds.), Person–environment psychology: New directions Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon.
and perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 1–23). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychological Science, 7, 186–189.

Isom, J., & Heller, W. (1999). Neurobiology of extraversion: Pieces of the Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others? The
puzzle still missing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 524. role of cognitive and motivational processes in well-being. American
Psychologist, 56, 239–249.
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1993). Determinants of interjudge agreement
on personality traits: The Big Five domains, observability, evaluative- Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing hap-
ness, and the unique perspective of the self. Journal of Personality, 61, piness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General
521–551. New York: Random House. Psychology, 9, 111–131.
Jones, E. E. (1985). Major developments in social psychology during the Markus, H. R. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the
past five decades. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63–78.
social psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 47–101). Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98,
Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24. 224–253.
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political Marsch, A. A., Kozak, M. N., & Ambady, N. (2007). Accurate identifica-
conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, tion of fear facial expressions predicts prosocial behavior. Emotion, 7,
129, 339–375. 239–251.
Jourard, S. M. (1971). Self-disclosure: An experimental analysis of the Maslow, A. (1987). Motivation and personality (3rd ed.). New York: Harper.
transparent self. New York: Wiley. Masson, J. M. (1984). The assault on truth: Freud’s suppression of the
Kelley, H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine seduction theory. New York: Farrar, Straus, & Giroux.
(Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15). Lincoln: Matsumoto, D. (2007). Culture, context and behavior. Journal of
University of Nebraska Press. Personality, 75, 1285–1320.
Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A social relations analy- McArthur, L. A. (1972). The how and what of why: Some determinants and
sis. New York: Guilford Press. consequences of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social
Kenny, D. A., Albright, L., Malloy, T. E., & Kashy, D. A. (1994). Psychology, 22, 171–193.
Consensus in interpersonal perception: Acquaintance and the Big Five. McClelland, D. C. (1972). Opinions reflect opinions: So what else is new?
Psychological Bulletin, 116, 245–258. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 38, 325–326.
Kenny, D. A., West, T. V., Cillessen, A. H. N., Cole, J. D., Dodge, K. A., McCrae, R. R. (1982). Consensual validation of personality traits:
Hubbard, J. A., et al. (2007). Accuracy in judgments of aggressiveness. Evidence from self-reports and ratings. Journal of Personality and
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 1225–1236. Social Psychology, 43, 293–303.
696 Personality in Social Psychology

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of the five-factor Reis, H. T. (2008). Reinvigorating the concept of situation in social psy-
model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of chology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 311–329.
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90. Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., Jr., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred
Mealy, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolu- years of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General
tionary model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 523–599. Psychology, 7, 331–363.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007).
Structure, dynamics and change. New York: Guilford Press. The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits,
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life
and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378. outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 313–345.

Milgram, S. (2004). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. New Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-
York: Harper and Row. (Original work published 1974.) level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis
of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 1–25.
Milgram, S. (1977). The individual in a social world: Essays and experi-
Rogers, C. (1951). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice, implica-
ments. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
tions, and theory. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.
Rorer, L. G. (1990). Personality assessment: A conceptual survey. In
Mischel, W. (1999). Personality coherence and dispositions in a cogni- L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New
tive–affective personality system (CAPS) approach. In D. Cervone & York: Guilford Press.
Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality: Social-cognitive bases
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
of consistency, variability and organization (pp. 37–60). New York:
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Guilford Press.
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1978). Interpersonal expectancy effects:
Mischel, W. (2009). From Personality and Assessment (1968) to personal-
The first 345 studies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 377–415.
ity science. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 282–290.
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982). A simple, general purpose display of
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive–affective system theory of
magnitude of experimental effect. Journal of Educational Psychology,
personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics,
74, 166–169.
and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102,
246–268. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings:
Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
Mook, D. G. (1983). In defense of external invalidity. American
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). New York:
Psychologist, 38(4), 379–387.
Academic Press.
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of nar-
Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The false consensus effect: An
cissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological
egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal
Inquiry, 12, 177–196.
of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279–301.
Napier, J. L., & Jost, J. T. (2008). Why are conservatives happier than
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R.E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives
liberals. Psychological Science, 19, 565–572.
of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Nave, C., Sherman, R., & Funder, D. (2008). Beyond self-report in the
Ross, M., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1985). Attribution and social perception. In
study of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being: Correlations with
G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
acquaintance reports, clinician judgments and directly observed social
(3rd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 73–122). New York: Random House.
behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 643–659.
Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning theory and clinical psychology.
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
violence in the South. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Rotter, J. B. (1982). The development and applications of social learning
Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and short-
theory: Selected papers. New York: Praeger.
comings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Rowatt, W. C., Cunningham, M. R., & Druen, P. B. (1998). Deception to get
Norman, W. T. (1967). 2800 personality trait descriptors: Normative
a date. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1228–1242.
operating characteristics for a university population. Ann Arbor:
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan. Rule, N. O., Ambady, N., Adams, R. B., Jr., & Macrae, C. N. (2008).
Accuracy and awareness in the perception and categorization of male
Oishi, S., Diener, E., Napa Scollon, C., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2004). Cross-
sexual orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
situational consistency of affective experience across cultures. Journal
95(5), 1019–1028.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 460–472.
Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & the PDP Research Group (1986).
Osborn, S. M., Field, H. S., & Veres, J. G. (1998). Introversion–
Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of
extraversion, self-monitoring and applicant performance in a
cognition. Foundations (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
situational panel interview: A field study. Journal of Business and
Psychology, 13, 143–156. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials:
A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual
Ozer, D. J. (1985). Correlation and the coefficient of determination. Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166.
Psychological Bulletin, 97, 307–315.
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. (2003). The structure of personality attri-
Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of
butes. In M. R. Barrick & A. M. Ryan (Eds.), Personality and work:
consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401–421. Reconsidering the role of personality in organizations (pp. 1–29). San
Park, B., & Judd, C. M. (1989). Agreement in initial impressions: Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Differences due to perceivers, trait dimensions, and target behaviors.
Saucier, G., Bel-Bahar, T., & Fernandez, C. (2007). What modifies the
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 493–505. expression of personality tendencies? Defining basic domains of situa-
Pervin, L. A. (1994). A critical analysis of current trait theory. Psychological tion variables. Journal of Personality, 75, 479–504.
Inquiry, 5(2), 103–113. Schaller, M., Boyd, C., Yohannes, J., & O’Brien, M. (1995). The preju-
Quattrone, G. A. (1982). Overattribution and unit formation: When behav- diced personality revisited: Personal need for structure and formation
ior engulfs the person. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, of erroneous group stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social
42, 593–607. Psychology, 68, 544–555.
References 697

Sears, R. R. (1963). Dependency motivation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Tsai, J. L., & Chentsova-Dutton, Y. (2003). Variation among European
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Americans in emotional facial expression. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Press. Psychology, 34(6), 650–657.
Snyder, M. (1974). The self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Tupes, E. C., & Christal, R. E. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based
Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526–537. on trait ratings (Technical Report No. ASD-TR-61–97). Lackland Air
Snyder, M., & Ickes, W. (1985). Personality and social behavior. In Force Base, TX: U.S. Air Force.
G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (3rd Turkheimer, E. (1998). Heritability and biological explanation.
ed., Vol. 2, pp. 883–948). New York: Random House. Psychological Review, 105, 782–791.
Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. (1978). Confirmation in social interaction: Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty:
From social perception to social reality. Journal of Experimental Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.
Social Psychology, 14, 148–162. Uttal, W. R. (2001). The new phrenology: The limits of localizing cogni-
Strickland, L. H. (1958). Surveillance and trust. Journal of Personality, tive processes in the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
26, 200–215. Vazire, S., & Funder, D. C. (2006). Impulsivity and the self-defeating
Swann, W. B., Chang-Schneider, C. S., & Angulo, S. K. (2007). Self- behavior of narcissists. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10,
verification in relationships as an adaptive process. In J. Wood, 154–165.
A. Tesser, & J. Holmes (Eds.), Self and relationships. New York: Vazire, S., & Mehl, M. R. (2008). Knowing me, knowing you: The
Psychology Press. accuracy and unique predictive validity of self and other ratings of
Swann, W. B., & Seyle, C. (2005). Personality psychology’s comeback and daily behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
its emerging symbiosis with social psychology. Personality and Social 1202–1216.
Psychology Bulletin, 31, 155–165. Vogt, D. S., & Colvin, C. R. (2003). Interpersonal orientation and the accu-
Taft, R. (1955). The ability to judge people. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 1–23. racy of personality judgment. Journal of Personality, 71, 267–295.
Tagiuri, R., & Petrullo, L. (1958). Person perception and interpersonal Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., & Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly high
behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cogni-
Taylor, P., Funk, C., & Craighill, P. (2006). Are we happy yet? tion. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(3), 274–290.
Pew Research Center. Retrieved November 17, 2008, from http:// Wagerman, S. A., & Funder, D. C. (2009). Situations. In P. J. Corr &
pewresearch.org/assets/social/pdf/AreWeHappyYet.pdf. G. Matthews (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of personality (pp. 27–42).
Thomas, G., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (2000, June). Empathic accuracy. Paper Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
presented at the International Conference on Personal Relationships, Watson, D. (1989). Strangers’ ratings of the five robust personality fac-
Brisbane, Australia. tors: Evidence of a surprising convergence with self-report. Journal of
Thurstone, L. L. (1934). The vectors of mind. Psychological Review, 41, Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 120–128.
1–32. Watson, J. (1930). Behaviorism (rev. ed.). New York: W. W. Norton.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). On the universality of human nature and Westen, D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a
the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and adaptation. psychodynamically informed psychological science. Psychological
Journal of Personality, 58, 17–67. Bulletin, 124, 333–371.
Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York: Zuckerman, M. (1991). Psychobiology of personality. Cambridge,
McGraw-Hill. England: Cambridge University Press.
Triandis, H. C. (1997). Cross-cultural perspectives on personality. In Zuckerman, M. (1998). Psychobiological theories of personality. In
R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality D. F. Barone, M. Hersen, & V. B. Van Hasselt (Eds.), Advanced per-
psychology (pp. 440–464). San Diego: Academic Press. sonality (pp. 123–154). New York: Plenum.
Chapter 19

Health
SHELLEY E. TAYLOR

Social psychologists have had an enduring interest in perspectives on health and not the endorsement of specific
health. As early as the 1950s, Irving Janis conducted land- therapeutic techniques. Adopting this holistic perspective,
mark studies with hospital patients to understand how fear this chapter reviews the current status of health psychol-
affects the understanding of information about surgery and ogy research, especially from the vantage point of social
the recovery process (Janis, 1958). Not until the mid- to psychology.
late 1970s, however, was a field of health psychology for- Although social psychologists have contributed to vir-
malized. Since that time, social psychologists have consis- tually every aspect of health psychology, in this chapter,
tently made important contributions to this field. I focus on three areas in which the contributions have
Health psychology is devoted to understanding psycho- been most plentiful and fruitful. These are health behav-
logical influences on how people stay healthy, why they iors, stress and coping, and social support. The reader
become ill, and how they respond when they do get ill. should note that the previous edition of The Handbook
Health psychologists conduct research on these issues of Social Psychology included a chapter on health behav-
and promote interventions to help people stay well or iors (Salovey, Rothman, & Rodin, 1998) that covered this
get over illness. The field is guided by the World Health area in depth. Accordingly, the current coverage avoids
Organization’s 1948 holistic definition of health as “a substantial overlap with that more detailed contribution,
complete state of physical, mental, and social well-being which remains an important review of social psychological
and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” (World contributions to health behaviors and their modification.
Health Organization, 1948). The breadth of this definition Several central themes guide the coverage in this chap-
indicates that health psychology is concerned with health ter. The first is that social psychologists who study health
promotion and illness prevention; the treatment of illness; are not applied social psychologists. Rather, health is an
the etiology and correlates of health, illness, and disability; important domain in which social psychological theories
improvement of the health care system; and formulation can be tested, and the results of investigations in health
of health policy. Thus, there is a broad role for social psy- settings often feed back to suggest modifications to the
chologists to play in these endeavors. theories and provide guidance about underlying mecha-
There are at least two important things to note about the nisms. A second theme is that through our contributions
WHO’s guiding definition of health. The first is the date: to health, social psychology is integrated into the biologi-
this holistic view of health was recognized to be important cal and medical sciences and thus contributes to the inte-
in 1948, yet health psychologists still encounter outmoded grative science that unites insights from psychology with
beliefs about health as purely physical in origin. Second is those of other scientific disciplines. A third theme is that
the World Health Organization’s specific endorsement of health is an important area in which social psychologists
“holism.” In recent years, this term has come to be asso- have implemented interventions that make a difference
ciated with alternative medical practices, such as herbal in people’s lives. As such, social psychological contri-
medicine, acupuncture, and other less traditional interven- butions to health embody Kurt Lewin’s famous dictum:
tions (at least for Western societies). However, holism, in “There is nothing so practical as a good theory” (Lewin,
fact means the integration of physical, mental, and social 1943, 9. 118).

Preparation of this chapter was supported by research grants from the National Institute of Aging (AG030309) and the National Science
Foundation (SES-0525713 and BCS-0729532).

698
Handbook of Social Psychology, edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, and Gardner Lindzey.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Health Behaviors 699

HEALTH BEHAVIORS change across the life span. For example, most smokers
begin to smoke because of contact with a smoking peer
The study of health behaviors is guided by the philosophy group, but long-term smoking may be maintained because
of health promotion, namely, the idea that good health is it helps reduce feelings of stress.
a personal and collective achievement. For the individual, Accordingly, a debate is taking shape as to whether
it involves developing good health habits early in life and social engineering solutions to health problems might be
carrying them through to old age. For the health practitio- more practical and, ultimately, more successful than indi-
ner, it involves teaching people how to achieve a healthy vidual or group interventions developed by social psy-
lifestyle and helping people at risk for a health problem chologists. For example, to influence obesity levels, one
offset or monitor that risk. For the psychologist, health could intervene at the individual or community level to
promotion involves research on and the development of teach people about good eating habits or one could ban
interventions to help people practice healthy behaviors trans fats from foods and eliminate vending machines in
and change poor ones. This, in turn, feeds into commu- schools. Similarly, a health intervention could help smokers
nity and national health policy to help people to develop develop the motivation and skills to stop smoking, or
and maintain healthy lifestyles and to create resources and smoking could be heavily taxed and restricted to particular
facilities that aid that process. areas. Do social engineering solutions to major health
At the turn of the 20th century, the major health problems problems obviate the need for social psychological inter-
facing the United States were acute infectious disorders ventions? Absolutely not. The two approaches complement
such as tuberculosis, influenza, measles, and poliomyelitis, each other. For example, unless people are persuaded of the
conditions that are not strongly influenced by personal need for social engineering solutions, the implementation
attitudes and behavior. As a result of treatment innova- of such solutions can produce resentment and retaliatory
tions and changes in public health standards, these health political action. Moreover, many health habits cannot be
problems have been made more tractable. However, there controlled or controlled completely through social engi-
has been an increase in what have been called the prevent- neering. For example, people who consume a high-fat
able disorders, including cancers, especially lung cancer, diet may still find the products they want on their super-
cardiovascular disease, alcohol and other substance abuse, market shelves, and smokers who may find their access to
and vehicular accidents. The role of behavioral factors smoking areas restricted nonetheless know where they can
in these disorders is clear. It is estimated that nearly half smoke. Other health habits can be only minimally changed
the deaths in the United States are caused by preventable by social engineering solutions. For example, legislation
factors, with smoking, obesity, and alcoholism being that bans premarital or extramarital sex would likely be
the top three. Uncovering the psychological technology quite ineffective, as would legislation requiring the use of
that will make a dent in these problems is an important condoms. Laws that cannot be enforced have little impact
national priority. on behavior. Accordingly, social engineering solutions to
What are the important health habits that are targets for health problems work hand-in-hand with social psycho-
modification? Sleeping 7 to 8 hours a night, not smoking, logical interventions to educate, coax, and nudge people
consuming a healthy diet, having no more than one or two into the health habits that will serve them well. Moreover,
alcoholic drinks each day, getting regular exercise, being no social psychologists have particular skills that can be used
more than 10% overweight, and practicing sun protection to improve the nature of social engineering interventions at
are among the most important health habits for ensuring the individual, community, state, or even federal level.
good health (e.g., Belloc & Breslow, 1972; Taylor, 2009). Social psychologists are masters at identifying and mod-
Yet health habits are notoriously difficult to modify. ifying situational influences on behavior, and as such, they
Many of them become ingrained early in life and, once can identify and make creative use of those circumstances
learned, are difficult to change. Moreover, habits devel- when people may be most motivated to change their health
oped during childhood and adolescence when most people behaviors. For example, the concept of teachable moment
are healthy provide little incentive for change because the refers to the fact that certain times are better than others
cumulative damage that these behaviors may cause will not for changing particular health practices. Many teachable
be apparent for years (Johnson, McCaul, & Klein, 2002). moments arise in early childhood when parents teach their
Health habits are typically only modestly related to each children basic safety behaviors, such as using a car seat or
other. The person who controls her weight may continue a seatbelt. Moreover, because well-baby visits are a fixture
to smoke, and the person who exercises faithfully does of early health care, well-baby care visits provide teachable
not necessarily wear his seatbelt. Different health habits moments for pediatricians to check on the basics of health
are controlled by different factors, and those factors may habits, accident prevention, and safety in the home. Late
700 Health

elementary school and junior high school represent teachable may occur if it is not undertaken will be most effective.
moments for smoking and drug abuse, and many social For health promotion behaviors (such as sunscreen
psychological interventions that have been implemented use), emphasizing the benefits to be gained may be
through academic classrooms have made use of these more effective.
windows of opportunity (e.g., Evans, Powers, Hersey, & • If the audience is receptive to changing a health habit,
Renaud, 2006). Pregnancy represents a teachable moment then the communication should include only favorable
for several health habits, including stopping smoking and points, but if the audience is not inclined to accept the
improving diet. When people are newly diagnosed with a message, the communication should discuss both sides
chronic disease, such as lung cancer or heart disease, they of the issue. For example, messages to smokers ready
may be especially motivated to modify their health behav- to stop should emphasize the health risks of smoking.
iors as well. On what social psychological technologies Smokers who have not yet decided to stop may be more
have such interventions drawn? persuaded by a communication that points out its risk
while acknowledging and rebutting its pleasurable
effects.
Attitude Change

Principles of attitude change from social psychological Providing information does not ensure that people will
research have been useful for designing persuasive com- perceive that information accurately. When people receive
munications that address health habits. Some of these negative information about risks to their health, they
involve educating the public about health risks. Educational sometimes process that information defensively (Millar &
appeals make the assumption that people will change their Millar, 1996). Instead of making appropriate health behav-
health habits if they have correct information. Attitude ior changes, the person may come to view the problem as
change research, derived from social learning perspectives less serious or more common than previously believed,
and from dual processing perspectives, has provided the particularly if the person intends to continue the behavior
following guidelines as to the best ways to persuade people (Gerrard, Gibbons, Benthin, & Hessling, 1996). Smokers,
through educational appeals: for example, know that they are at a greater risk for lung
cancer than are nonsmokers, but they see lung cancer as
• The communicator should be expert, prestigious, trust- less likely or problematic and smoking as more common
worthy, likable, and similar to the audience. For example, than do nonsmokers.
a health message will be more persuasive if it comes
Fear Appeals
from a respected, credible physician rather than from
the proponent of a health fad. In part because of these problems, attitudinal approaches
• Communications should be colorful and vivid rather to changing health habits often use fear appeals. This
than steeped in statistics and jargon. If possible, they approach assumes that if people are fearful a particular
should also use case histories. For example, a vivid habit is hurting their health, they will change their behav-
account of the health benefits of regular exercise, cou- ior to reduce their fear. Common sense suggests that the
pled with a case history of someone who took up bicy- relationship between fear and behavior change should be
cling after a heart attack, may be persuasive to someone direct: The more fearful an individual is, the more likely
at risk for heart disease. he or she will be to change the relevant behavior. However,
• Strong arguments should be presented at the beginning this relationship does not always hold. Persuasive mes-
and end of a message, not buried in the middle. sages that elicit too much fear may actually undermine
• Messages should be short, clear, and direct. health behavior change (Becker & Janz, 1987). Moreover,
• Messages should state conclusions explicitly. For research suggests that fear alone may not be sufficient to
example, a communication extolling the virtues of a change behavior. Sometimes fear can affect intentions
low-cholesterol diet should explicitly advise the reader to change health habits (for example, Sutton & Eiser, 1984),
to alter his or her diet to lower cholesterol. but it may not produce long-lasting changes in health
• Extreme messages produce more attitude change, but habits unless it is coupled with recommendations for action
only up to a point. Very extreme messages are discounted. or information about the efficacy of the health behavior
For example, a message that urges people to exercise for (Self & Rogers, 1990).
at least 3 hours a week will be more effective than one Building on these findings, protection motivation the-
that recommends 3 hours of exercise a day. ory (Maddox & Rogers, 1983) maintains that the effect
• For illness detection behaviors (such as HIV testing or of fear appeals on health behavior change is importantly
obtaining a mammogram), emphasizing the problems that influenced by whether people are motivated to protect
Health Behaviors 701

themselves. Feelings of fear motivate behavior by altering people adopt a health behavior on the basis of its expected
perceptions of vulnerability and severity. An original utility; utility is the subjective value attached to each pos-
component of this theory also included the prediction that sible outcome associated with the behavior weighted by
strong fear motivates behavior only if it is accompanied by the perceived likelihood of the outcome. So, for example,
feelings of self-efficacy. However, self-efficacy appears to if a person is choosing between continuing to smoke and
exert effects on behavioral intentions directly, not necessar- attempting to quit, the expected utility of each alternative
ily in conjunction with fear. Perceived vulnerability appears would be based on the relative importance attached to
to affect behavioral intentions directly as well, whereas per- the pleasure of smoking versus the importance of health
ceived severity appears to have a lesser impact on behavior. weighted by the likelihood that stopping smoking would
Generally speaking, the predictions of the model have been actually improve health. According to the theory, a person
supported, albeit weakly, and critics have suggested that pro- chooses among possible courses of action by selecting
tection motivation theory may be more useful when mod- the choice that provides the greatest utility. Usually, this
erators are considered along with the primary elements of means comparing one’s current behavior with some new
the theory (Salovey et al., 1998). health behavior.
The theory we consider in this context, the health behav-
Message Framing ior model, posits utility functions for choosing to practice
Any health message can be phrased in positive or negative (or not) a recommended health behavior and generates
terms. For example, a reminder letter to get a flu immunization predictions about what beliefs might be targeted to change
can emphasize the benefits of being immunized or empha- behavior. As will become evident, however, subjective
size the discomfort of the flu itself (McCaul, Johnson, & expected utility theory does not take account an important
Rothman, 2002). Which of these methods is more success- aspect of behavior change in the health domain, namely,
ful? Messages that emphasize potential problems work the perception that one will be able to change the behav-
better for behaviors that have uncertain outcomes, whereas ior in question (i.e., self-efficacy). This point, as will be
messages that stress benefits may be more persuasive for seen, is important because such behaviors as altering diet
behaviors with certain outcomes (Apanovitch, McCarthy, & or stopping smoking are often difficult to change.
Salovey, 2003). As is the case with fear appeals, recom-
mendations regarding exactly how to take the action Health Belief Model
increase effectiveness (McCaul et al., 2002).
Which kind of message framing will most affect behavior The earliest attitude theory of why people practice health
also depends on people’s own motivation. Some people behaviors was the health belief model (Hochbaum, 1958;
have a promotion or behavioral approach orientation Rosenstock, 1966). This model states that whether a person
(Gray, 1990) that emphasizes approaching opportunities. practices a particular health behavior can be understood by
People with a behavioral approach orientation respond knowing two factors: whether the person perceives a per-
positively to such statements as “I go out of my way to sonal health threat and whether the person believes that a
get things I want.” Such people are especially influenced particular health practice will be effective in reducing that
by messages phrased in terms of benefits (e.g., “calcium threat. The perception of a personal health threat is itself
will keep your bones healthy”). Other people have a pre- influenced by at least three factors: general health values,
vention or avoidance orientation that emphasizes minimiz- which include concern about health; specific beliefs about
ing risks. They respond positively to such statements as personal vulnerability to a particular disorder; and beliefs
“I worry about making mistakes.” They are more influ- about the consequences of the disorder, such as whether
enced by messages that stress the risks of not performing they are serious. For example, people may change their
a health behavior (e.g., “a low calcium intake will increase diet to include low cholesterol foods if they value health,
bone loss”). Messages that match a person’s behavioral feel threatened by the possibility of heart disease, and
orientation produce more behavior change than those that perceive that the threat of heart disease is severe (e.g.,
do not (Mann, Sherman, & Updegraff, 2005). Brewer et al., 2007).
Most of the preceding points regarding how to change Whether a person believes that a health measure will
health behaviors have implicitly drawn on either a social reduce a threat has two subcomponents: whether the per-
learning approach to attitude change or on social cogni- son thinks a health practice will be effective and whether
tion research. Expectancy value theories are also useful the cost of undertaking the measure exceeds its benefits.
in the health domain to both explain why people practice For example, a person urged to change his diet in response
the health habits they do and to provide frameworks for to vulnerability to heart disease may believe that dietary
changing health behaviors. These theories assume that change alone would not reduce the risk of a heart attack or
702 Health

that changing his diet would interfere with his enjoyment subjective norms regarding the action, and perceived
of life too much to justify taking the action. Thus, although behavioral control. Attitudes toward the action include
he may recognize his personal vulnerability to heart dis- beliefs about the likely outcomes of the action and eval-
ease, if he lacks the belief that dietary change would uations of those outcomes. Subjective norms are what a
reduce his risk, he would likely not make any changes in person believes others think that person should do (norma-
his behavior. tive beliefs) and the motivation to comply with those nor-
The health belief model explains people’s practices of mative beliefs. Perceived behavioral control occurs when
health habits quite well and has been widely used for sev- an individual feels able to perform the action and that the
eral decades to understand health behaviors (Taylor, 2009, action undertaken will have the intended effect; this com-
for a review). The health belief model also predicts some ponent of the model is similar to self-efficacy. These factors
of the circumstances under which people’s health behav- combine to produce a behavioral intention and, ultimately,
iors will change. Interventions that highlight perceived behavior change. To take a simple example, smokers who
vulnerability but simultaneously increase the perception believe that smoking causes serious health outcomes,
that a particular health behavior will reduce the threat are who believe that other people think they should stop smok-
somewhat successful in changing health behaviors such as ing, who are motivated to comply with those normative
smoking and preventive dental care (e.g., Eiser, van der beliefs, and who believe that they are capable of stopping
Plight, Raw, & Sutton, 1985; Ronis, 1992). smoking will be more likely to intend to stop smoking
Criticisms of the health belief model focus on three than individuals who do not hold these beliefs. Behavioral
issues. The first is that the model addresses risk-related intentions then predict behavior.
beliefs rather than emotional responses to perceived risk; The theory of planned behavior suggests points of
emotional responses may better predict behavior (e.g., attack in the design of persuasive messages designed
Beckjord, Rutten, Arora, Moser, & Hesse, 2008; Lawton, to change health behaviors. A first intervention point is to
Conner, & Parker, 2007; Weinstein et al., 2007). Second, change attitudes toward the specific action, as by provid-
the health belief model does not provide an analysis ing information about the likely outcomes of behavior
of behavior and the situational forces that may undermine change. Subjective norms, that is, what people believe
behavior change. In important respects, this represents a others think they should do, can also be altered through
reflection of the attitude–behavior problem, namely, that persuasive communications. For example, binge drink-
attitudes predict behavior best under certain circumstances. ing among college students is a serious problem, but most
Environmental factors that exert tugs on behavior but not college students think they are in a minority in regarding
necessarily on attitudes are one source of this discrepancy. it as such. The statistics, in fact, suggest the majority of
Behavior is often under the control of specific situational college students are against binge drinking, and so making
cues or social norms that may supplant attitudes that would these subjective norms more salient can motivate students
otherwise influence behavior. For example, a person who to alter their behavior (Chan, Neighbors, Gilson, Larimer, &
is trying to stop smoking may find that the urge for an Marlatt, 2007). Perceived behavioral control, that is, the
after-dinner cigarette undermines his resolve. person’s perceived ability to perform the action and belief
A third more general issue concerns the ability to that it will have its intended effect, can also be directly
actually perform the health behaviors in question (e.g., addressed through persuasive messages. If people are told,
Bandura, 1991; Murphy et al., 2001). Self-efficacy refers for example, about the numbers of smokers who succeed
to the belief that one will be able to perform a specific in stopping on their own and that often it takes several
behavior, and often, sense of self-efficacy is low. If a per- efforts before one is successful, this may help to motivate
son does not believe he will be able to stop smoking, for the would-be ex-smoker to change behavior.
example, then he may be unlikely to try. A theory that Finally, the theory of planned behavior provides a novel
links attitudes directly to behavior and that takes account point of intervention at the level of behavioral intentions.
of the need for a sense of behavioral control is the theory of Schweiger Gallo and Gollwitzer (2007), for example,
planned behavior. have found that when people are induced to set a specific
day, time, and place to begin a target behavior, they are
more likely to do so than if they merely express a desire
Theory of Planned Behavior
to change a behavior. This technology can be profitably
According to the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen & used to modify health behaviors as well (Sullivan &
Madden, 1986), a health behavior is the direct result of a Rothman, 2008).
behavioral intention. Behavioral intentions are made up Thus, the theory of planned behavior is not only a
of three components: attitudes toward the specific action, strong theoretical model but also acts as a methodological
Health Behaviors 703

heuristic for which cognitions and behaviors can be HIV infection. However, whether such messages actually
targeted in interventions. The theory of planned behavior change behavior has been in doubt. The reduction in smok-
has been applied to many health behaviors, including exer- ing following the Surgeon General’s announcement was
cise (Baker, Little, & Brownell, 2003), consumption of soft not especially impressive. Presenting a consistent media
drinks among adolescents (Kassem & Lee, 2004), partici- message over time, however, can have a cumulative effect
pation in health screening programs (Sheeran, Conner, & in changing opinions and values associated with health
Norman, 2001), and follow-up appointments for abnormal practices. For example, although initially many people
cervical screening results (Orbell & Hagger, 2006). counterargued the antismoking messages that the media
put forth, the climate of public opinion shifted over time to
the side of the nonsmoker (Lichtenstein & Cohen, 1990).
Attitude–Behavior Relationship
Whether mass media messages alone and, by impli-
As theories of health behaviors move closer to predicting cation, persuasive communications alone can actually
behavior, the attitude–behavior relation assumes expected change behavior without behavior change technology, such
importance. Many communications designed to change as cognitive–behavioral interventions, is unclear. Although
people’s attitudes can evoke defensive or irrational pro- cognitive–behavioral interventions are one of the most
cesses that undermine behavior change. People may per- effective ways of attacking health behaviors (see Taylor,
ceive a threat to be less relevant to themselves than it really 2009, for a review), the effectiveness of mass media mes-
is (e.g., Liberman & Chaiken, 1992), they may falsely see sages may have been underestimated. For example, follow-
themselves as less vulnerable than others with the same ing the initial release of the Surgeon General’s report in
risks (Clarke, Lovegrove, Williams, & Macpherson, 2000), 1982, many millions of people quit smoking on their own in
or they may perceive themselves as dissimilar to people the years that followed. It often takes several efforts before
who have succumbed to a particular health threat, thereby a person is successful in stopping smoking (Lichtenstein &
distancing themselves from the threat (Thornton, Gibbons, & Cohen, 1990). As such, the mass media may have motivated
Gerrard, 2002). Moreover, continued practice of a risky quitting efforts that eventually were successful. The fact
behavior can itself compromise the assessment of personal that so many people alter their health habits successfully
risk (Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001). With these multiple on their own without medical or psychological interven-
abilities to distort the relevance of health threats, even tions implicitly suggests that the cumulative effects of mass
carefully designed messages may be unable to get around media educational interventions may have been underesti-
biases in information processing. mated (McBride et al., 2001). Whether currently obese or
overweight people will be able to lose weight on their own
will provide another interesting test of this point.
Mass Media Messages
Even soap operas have been used in some countries to
Messages in the mass media have been both blamed for get people to change their health habits (Williams, 2001),
poor health habits and credited with helping to change bad and this venue can be more successful in producing behav-
ones. On the negative side, when people are confronted ior change than lectures or pamphlets, especially in devel-
with images in the media of people exhibiting poor health oping countries. When people listen to the radio or watch
behaviors, such as smoking or restricted eating, it can affect the stars of their favorite TV dramas practice good health
their behavior (Anschutz, Van Strein, & Engels, 2008). habits, they are more inclined to do the same. For example,
Yet theories and methods of attitude change can also be with problems such as teen pregnancy and AIDS, dra-
used to design mass media messages. The goal of health matic portrayals show some success in changing behavior
promotion is to reach as many people as possible and con- (Vaughan, Rogers, Singhal, & Swalehe, 2000).
sequently, the mass media can achieve this goal. Evaluations An additional benefit of attitude change campaigns
of the effectiveness of mass media appeals, however, sug- through the mass media is that they may foster a receptive
gest some qualifications regarding their success (e.g., Lau, climate for social engineering responses to health prob-
Kane, Berry, Ware, & Roy, 1980). The mass media appear lems. For example, without public service announcements
to be most effective in alerting people to health risks addressing the dangers of secondhand smoke or safety
that they might otherwise not know about. For example, restraints for infants, many state legislatures might have left
mass media attention to the Surgeon General’s report on the passive smoking and infant car seat issues unaddressed.
the health risks of smoking alerted millions of people to the A promising but as yet underutilized tool for modifying
problem faster than would otherwise have occurred (U.S. health habits is the Internet. The Internet provides low-cost
Public Health Service, 1982). The media helped spread the access to health messages for millions of people who can
safe sex message after sexual practices were implicated in benefit from the information, suggestions, and techniques
704 Health

for behavior change offered on websites. The Internet also negative emotional experience accompanied by predictable
allows researchers to recruit large numbers of participants biochemical, cognitive, and behavioral changes that are
for studies at relatively low cost, thus enabling data collec- directed either toward altering the stressful event or accom-
tion efforts as well (e.g., Lenert & Skoczen, 2002). modating to its effects (Baum, 1990). The study of stress
In summary, social psychologists have been heavily and how people deal with it is important for several rea-
involved in the development and testing of models of health sons. First, many people experience their lives as stressful,
behavior change, with considerable success. These mod- and so the problem is widespread. Second, stress compro-
els are widely used to develop persuasive communications mises health. Chronic stress leads to predictable changes
to motivate people to change their health habits. In addi- in biological regulatory systems—in particular, the sym-
tion, social psychological research has provided inputs to pathetic nervous system and the hypothalamic-pituitary-
understanding the attitude–behavior discrepancy that can adrenocortical axis (McEwen, 2008; see Figure 19.1).
characterize health behaviors. Clearly social psychological When a person encounters an event that is perceived as
approaches to health behavior change will not be sufficient harmful or threatening, a set of biobehavioral reactions is
in their own right. For example, social engineering solutions initiated. Information from the cortex is transmitted to the
that address unhealthy behaviors by simply outlawing or hypothalamus, which initiates sympathetic nervous system
controlling their practice have the potential to achieve mass arousal, or the fight-or-flight response first described by
behavior change. Without social psychological research, Walter Cannon (1932). Sympathetic arousal stimulates
though, understanding the particular times and ways in which the medulla of the adrenal glands, which in turn secrete the
people will adhere to health behavior change or comply catecholamines epinephrine (EP) and norepinephrine
with social engineering solutions would be incomplete. (NE). This sympathetic arousal in turn leads to increases
in blood pressure, heart rate, sweating, and constriction
of peripheral blood vessels, among other changes. The
STRESS AND ITS MANAGEMENT result is the cranked up feeling that people usually experi-
ence in response to stress. In addition, the hypothalamic-
Stress and how people manage it is one of the central topics pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is also activated. The hypothal-
of health psychology, and social psychologists have made amus releases corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF), which
substantial contributions to this research area. Stress is a stimulates the pituitary gland to secrete adrenocorticotropic

Demands from the Changes in blood


environment act on the cortex pressure, heart rate,
Higher brain sweating, peripheral
regions blood vessels, etc.

Hypothalamus

Autonomic CRH
nervous system

Catecholamines
Pituitary Gland Corticosteroids (EP, NE)
ACTH

Adrenal cortex Adrenal medulla

Immune System

Figure 19.1 The body’s stress systems. ACTH  adrenocorticotropic hormone; CRH  corticotrophin-releasing hormone; EP 
epinephrine; NE  norepinephrine.
Stress and Its Management 705

hormone (ACTH), which in turn stimulates the adrenal Specifically, Taylor and colleagues (2000) suggested that in
cortex to release glucocorticoids. Of these, cortisol is espe- addition to behavioral manifestations of fight-or-flight,
cially significant, because it helps conserve stores of car- people tend-and-befriend. Tending involves nurturant
bohydrates and helps reduce inflammation in the case of activities that protect offspring and that promote safety and
an injury. It also helps the body return to its steady state reduce distress. Befriending is the creation and mainte-
following stress. nance of social networks that aid in this process. We return
Engagement of these two stress systems on the short to this issue in the section on social support.
term mobilizes an organism for action, and, thus, these sys-
tems are critical for meeting stressful or threatening circum- Perceptions of Stress
stances. In human prehistory, when these systems evolved,
many threats (e.g., attack by a predator, attack by a conspe- Social cognition approaches to stress have led to several
cific, flood or other natural disaster) required instantaneous important insights. Because the construal of events so heav-
action, and so biological systems that respond quickly and ily influences the experience of stress, psychologists often
strongly would have been selected for through the pro- look at both the objective and the subjective experience of
cess of natural selection. However, at present, most of the stress to predict mental and physical health outcomes. For
stressors that people encounter are not threats of this nature example, Cohen, Tyrrell, and Smith (1993) asked com-
but are rather grinding, chronic problems such as work munity residents to indicate whether specific events had
stress, commuting, and chronic unpleasant relationships, occurred in their lives that are consensually judged to
but these threats, nonetheless, engage these same biologi- be stressful and also to complete a measure of perceived
cal stress regulatory systems. So protective on the short stress. They then exposed these participants to a common
term, the chronic or recurring engagement of these systems cold virus and found that both objectively assessed stress-
is destructive on the long term. The stress systems may lose ful life events and perceived stress both predicted whether
their resilience or elasticity and, over time, lay the ground- people developed a cold. Studies such as these indicate
work for a broad array of chronic illnesses (McEwen, clearly that the perception of stress is important to the psy-
1998). Accordingly, understanding what experiences are chological and physical fallout that results.
stressful and what individual differences and group pro- Because events are not always inherently stressful, peo-
cesses help people manage stress better is a high-priority ple respond to cues that make them more likely to appraise
topic in health psychology. events as stressful. What are those cues? People are more
Initially, researchers focused on stressful events them- vulnerable to stress when the events occur in central
selves, called stressors. Such events include noise, crowd- life domains than in peripheral ones. Work and relation-
ing, a bad relationship, or a round of job interviews, for ship stressors, for example, are typically important ones.
example. However, in the best tradition of Emmanuel Events are also more likely to be perceived as stressful if
Kant and Kurt Lewin, researchers soon came to realize they are negative, uncontrollable, ambiguous, and over-
that stress is not inherent in events themselves; it depends whelming. Although any event that produces change in
heavily on how it is appraised and interpreted. Events are one’s life activities can be perceived as stressful, negative
perceived as more stressful when personal resources experiences, such as coping with a death in the family or
are perceived to be insufficient to meet the demands of getting divorced, produce more psychological distress and
the environment. Stress, then, is determined by person– physical symptoms then do positive ones (see Taylor, 2009,
environment fit (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). for a review).
Early research on stress focused on the fight-or-flight Uncontrollable or unpredictable events are more stress-
response, a perspective that initially grew out of animal ful than controllable or predictable ones. When people
research addressing aggression and fleeing in response feel that they can predict, modify, or terminate an aversive
to stressful events. Walter Cannon (1932) imported this event, or at least feel that they have access to someone who
perspective to human behavior. That is, in response to can, they experience it as less stressful (Thompson, 1981).
stress a person may fight or mount aggressive or asser- Ambiguous events are typically perceived to be more
tive responses to stress, or flee, which in humans, is often stressful than clear-cut ones because in the former case,
manifested as social withdrawal or withdrawal through energy must be devoted to understanding the stressor rather
substance use. Even such behaviors as television watching than finding solutions to it. The ability to take confrontative
may be interpreted as “flight.” Social psychologists, how- action or at least adjust emotionally to a well-defined stressor
ever, have noted that social responses to stress are the most usually produces less distress, better coping, and lower
common ways in which human beings deal with it. This biochemical responses to stress. Finally, overloaded people
has given rise to a focus on affiliative responses to stress. are more stressed than people with fewer tasks to perform.
706 Health

In the work literature, job overload, or the perception that one’s life. The protective effects of employment, marriage,
one must do too much in too short a period of time, is a and parenting on psychological distress and health attest
chief cause of job dissatisfaction (e.g., Pearson, 2008), and to the salutary effects of meaningful social roles (Burton,
it contributes to adverse health outcomes as well (Steptoe, 1998). We return to this issue.
Siegrist, Kirschbaum, & Marmot, 2004).
Stressors need not be present for people to experience Coping With Stress
stress. The anticipation of stress can be at least as threaten-
ing as its occurrence (Wirtz et al., 2006), and aftereffects People manage to tolerate stress in their lives primarily
of stress often last indefinitely. For example, in one study, because they develop effective ways of coping. Coping
medical students wore blood pressure monitors on an is defined as the thoughts and behaviors that people use
unstressful lecture day, the day before an important exam, to manage the internal and external demands of situations
and during the exam itself. Although the lecture day was that are appraised as stressful (Taylor & Stanton, 2007).
characterized by stable patterns of cardiovascular activity, Because stress is so heavily dependent on appraisals, social
cardiovascular activity on the preexamination day when psychologists have studied individual differences in how a
the students were worried about the exam was as high as potentially stressful event is appraised and how people will
that seen during the examination itself (Sausen, Lovallo, cope with that event as a result.
Pincomb, & Wilson, 1992). First, we consider people who do not handle stress well.
Although the anticipation of stress and its aftereffects Certain people are predisposed by their personalities to
can be inherently stressful, people also are resilient. They experience stressful events as especially stressful, which
have the ability to adapt psychologically to moderate or in turn affects their psychological distress, social relation-
predictable stressors. With the exception of vulnerable ships, physical symptoms, and rates of illness. This research
people, such as children, the elderly, and people already has focused especially on negative affectivity, a pervasive
under intense stress, most people are able to incorporate negative mood marked by anxiety, depression, and hostility.
some stress into their lives (Wirtz et al., 2006). What makes Closely related to neuroticism, negative affectivity predicts
people resilient? both psychological distress and stronger biological stress
An early insight from social psychology (Linville, responses, which may provide a biopsychosocial pathway
1987) maintained that when people are invested in multi- linking negative affectivity to adverse health events (e.g.,
ple aspects of their lives, as opposed to focusing their per- Polk, Cohen, Doyle, Skoner, & Kirschbaum, 2005).
sonal rewards primarily on one life domain, they may be In some social psychological research, negativity is
buffered against setbacks in other areas. Research on occu- treated as a nuisance variable, in that people who report
pational stress has provided substantial support for this high levels of negative affectivity also report high levels
hypothesis. Married, working women have been heavily of distressing physical symptoms (Watson & Pennebaker,
studied because of the potential for role conflict among 1989); in many cases, there is no evidence of an underlying
the worker, wife, and mother roles and for potential role physical disorder (see, for example, Cohen, Doyle, Turner,
overload due to the sheer number of activities those roles Alper, & Skoner, 2003). Although negative affectivity no
encompass. Initially, psychologists anticipated that work- doubt inflates people’s perception of stress and contrib-
ing, married women with responsibilities for child care utes to self-reports of physical symptoms and psychologi-
would be overwhelmed and, consequently, be one of the cal distress, chronic negative affect also directly affects
most stressed groups among working adults. Surprisingly, the likelihood of physical health disorders (e.g., Charles,
there appear to be positive effects of combining home and Gatz, Kato, & Pedersen, 2008). For example, depression
work responsibilities. On the one hand, juggling heavy is a well-documented contributor to a risk for coronary
responsibilities at both work and home can undermine artery events and for a repeat event following an initial one
the enjoyment of both sets of tasks, but combining moth- (Bleil, Gianaros, Jennings, Flory, & Manuck, 2008;
erhood with employment can be beneficial for women’s Musselman & Nemeroff, 2000). As such, chronic negative
well-being, improving self-esteem, self-efficacy, and life affect needs to be considered as not only a potential nui-
satisfaction (Verbrugge, 1983). These findings occur pri- sance variable that conflates reports of symptoms, but also
marily for women who have enough help in the home. as a psychobiological pathway to disease.
Combining employment and marriage may be protective
Coping Resources
for men as well, with respect to both health and mental
health (Burton, 1998). Just as negative affect and neuroticism adversely affect
This research on multiple roles is converging on the idea health, positive mental states and psychosocial resources
that stress is lower when one has sources of meaning in are associated with better mental and physical health
Stress and Its Management 707

(Cohen & Pressman, 2006; Ironson & Hayward, 2008). would otherwise be the case. However, if the salient cues in
Social psychologists have been actively involved in identi- the environment suggest self-control, for example, cues sug-
fying the specific psychosocial resources that people bring gesting the importance of a healthy diet, then limitations on
to stressful events to help them cope more effectively. One attention may actually lead to more self-control rather
such resource is optimism (Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, than less self-control. The attentional myopia model has
1994). People who are high in dispositional optimism cope been examined with respect to eating and smoking, among
more effectively with stress and reduce their risk for illness other health behaviors, and, as such, implicates stress and
and improve their chances for recovery (e.g., Ironson & attentional processes in the ability to exert self-control over
Hayward, 2008; Segerstrom, 2006). An important reason health behaviors (Mann & Ward, 2007).
why optimism is a valuable resource is because it leads Research on the self has been an especially valuable
people to take more active coping measures (Nes & source of insights and interventions for understanding suc-
Segerstrom, 2006). For example, optimists cope more cessful coping. Self-esteem can act as a resource to help
effectively with the stress of college because they are people appraise stressful events as less so. For example, in
more likely to seek out social support and to reinterpret one study of students facing exams, those with high self-
positively the stressful circumstances they encounter esteem were less likely to be anxious (Shimizu & Pelham,
(Brissette, Scheier, & Carver, 2002). 2004). These appraisal effects of self-esteem appear to
When people have a dispositional sense of psychological be most protective at low to moderate effects of stress;
control, namely, that they can determine their own behav- at higher levels of stress, the events themselves can over-
ior, influence the environment, and bring about desired whelm the beneficial contribution of self-esteem (e.g.,
outcomes, they cope more successfully. Perceived control Whisman & Kwon, 1993). Self-esteem appears to affect
is related to self-efficacy, which is the more narrow percep- health at least in part by affecting the quality of social ties
tion that one can take the necessary actions to obtain a spe- (Stinson et al., 2008).
cific outcome in a specific situation (Bandura, 1977). Both Interventions to enhance a sense of self improve
types of beliefs (control, self-efficacy) help people cope responses to stressful events. Drawing on Steele’s (1988)
with a wide variety of stressful events (e.g., Schwerdtfeger, self-affirmation theory, for example, Creswell and associ-
Konermann, & Schönhofen, 2008; Wrosch, Schulz, Miller, ates (Creswell, Welch, Taylor, Sherman, Gruenewald, &
Lupien, & Dunne, 2007). Mann, 2005) assigned some people to focus on and write
A sense of control is especially important for vulner- about their most important value, whereas others wrote about
able populations such as medical patients, children, and a less important value. All participants then went through
the elderly who are at risk for health problems (Wrosch laboratory stress tasks including mental arithmetic and
et al., 2007). Initially spawned by social psychologist delivering a speech to an unresponsive audience. People
Irving Janis’s work with medical patients, the princi- who had affirmed an important personal value showed
ple of psychological control is so powerful that it is now lower biological responses to stress and, among those
used extensively in medical interventions to help people with high self-esteem, experienced less psychological dis-
cope with surgery and other noxious medical procedures. tress as well.
As noted earlier, it is also used in interventions to Related to self-esteem is a cluster of personal quali-
promote good health habits. Certainly there are qualifications ties that has been called ego strength, characterized by
to the idea that psychological control is always beneficial dependability, emotional stability, trust, and lack of
(e.g., Thompson, Cheek, & Graham, 1988). For example, con- impulsivity (e.g., Terracciano, Löckenhoff, Zonderman,
trol may be aversive if it gives people more responsibility Ferrucci, & Costa, 2008). This cluster also has health
than they want or feel able to assume (e.g., Chipperfield & benefits. For example, in a longitudinal investigation,
Perry, 2006), but on the whole, it is a beneficial resource Friedman and colleagues (1995) studied children who
(e.g., P. Smith, Frank, Bondy, & Mustard, 2008). had first been interviewed in 1947. Some had impulsive
However, limitations on attention can themselves lead and undercontrolled personalities, whereas others showed
to loss of control. When attentional resources are limited, signs of ego strength. Those high in ego strength as chil-
people focus on the most salient cues in the environment dren lived longer as adults. One reason is that those high
and neglect less salient stimuli. The resulting “attentional in ego strength were less likely to practice bad health hab-
myopia” can lead to a loss of control over behavior if the its, including smoking and excessive alcohol consump-
cues that are salient promote a behavior that violates per- tion. Related to these findings, conscientiousness also
sonal standards or intentions (Mann & Ward, 2007). Thus, affects health and longevity. In a meta-analysis of 20 stud-
for example, with attentional load, self-control over eating ies, Kern and Friedman (2008) found that conscientious-
may erode, and a person may end up eating much more than ness was strongly related to health across the life span.
708 Health

Conscientious people may be more successful in avoiding Problem-Focused and Emotion-Focused Coping
situations that could harm them or may more reliably
Another useful distinction in the science of coping is
practice good health habits (Hampson, Goldberg, Vogt, &
between problem-focused coping, which involves attempts
Dubanoski, 2006).
to do something active or constructive about the stressful
In short, a broad array of individual differences that
event, and emotion-focused coping, which involves a focus
include both protective and vulnerability factors have
on or effort to regulate emotions experienced in response to
been studied by personality and social psychologists and
the stressful event (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Typically,
yielded the conclusion that, just as some people appear to
people use both problem-focused and emotion-focused
have an illness-prone personality marked by neuroticism
coping to deal with stressful events.
and negative affect, others possess a health-prone person-
Emotion-focused coping, however, includes coping of
ality characterized by optimism, a sense of control, consci-
two kinds. Ruminating over a stressful event, that is, hav-
entiousness, self-esteem, and resilience.
ing negative, recurring thoughts, is generally maladaptive
for both psychological and physical health (e.g., Thomsen
Approach and Avoidance Coping et al., 2004). However, emotional-approach coping, which
involves clarifying, focusing on, and working through
Social psychologists have also studied broad proclivities
the emotions experienced in conjunction with a stressor
for managing stressful events. For example, some people
(Stanton, Danoff-Burg, Cameron, & Ellis, 1994), has ben-
cope with threatening events by using an avoidant or mini-
efits for a broad array of stressful situations.
mizing coping style, whereas others are more likely to use
an approach-oriented style that is confrontative or at least
vigilant, by gathering information or taking direct action. Resilience
Reflecting a core motivational construct (e.g., Davison,
Pennebaker, & Dickerson, 2000), the approach–avoidance Increasingly, social psychologists have turned their atten-
continuum maps onto broader theories of biobehavioral tion to the origins and effects of resilience. Positive life
functioning, including Gray’s (1990) behavioral approach events, positive emotions, and opportunities for rest, relax-
and inhibition orientations, referred to earlier. Examples of ation, and renewal can help people cope more effectively
active and approach-oriented coping are problem solving, with life stressors or prevent stressful events from taking a
seeking social support, and creating outlets for emotional toll on health (e.g., Ong, Bergeman, Bisconti, & Wallace,
expression. Coping through avoidance includes both cog- 2006; Ryff & Singer, 2000). Psychological well-being
nitive (distraction) and behavioral (substance abuse) meth- reliably affects long-term survival in both healthy people
ods. Some approaches, such as spiritual coping, can serve and people with health disorders (Chida & Steptoe, 2008).
either approach-oriented or avoidance goals. Although Even an action as simple as taking a vacation is benefi-
neither style is always more effective in managing stress, cial for middle-aged men at risk for heart disease (Gump &
approach-related coping is more successful when one can Matthews, 2000) and perhaps for other groups as well.
focus on the information in the situation and if there are Resilience is also a reliable individual difference. Some
specific actions that can ameliorate the stressor (Taylor & people recover from stressful events quickly, whereas others
Stanton, 2007). do not. Psychological resilience is characterized by the
Why are approach-related coping methods generally ability to bounce back from negative emotional experi-
more successful than avoidant ones? People who cope ences by adapting flexibly to the changing demands of the
with threatening events through approach-related methods environment. Research on emotions, such as Fredrickson’s
engage in the cognitive and emotional efforts needed to theory that positive emotions allow people to “broaden
manage long-term threats. Although they may pay a price and build” their coping resources (Tugade & Fredrickson,
in anxiety and biological reactivity in the short term (e.g., 2004), has provided an important theoretical and empirical
T. W. Smith, Ruiz, & Uchino, 2000), in the long term, they base for both understanding people’s reactions to stressful
may successfully modify the stressor and change their events and creating interventions to improve coping (e.g.,
reactions to it as well. For example, people who coped Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Larkin, 2003). Being able
with the September 11 attacks through avoidant coping to experience positive emotions in the context of otherwise
strategies fared worse psychologically over the long term intensely stressful events is one of the coping methods
compared with those who coped with their distress through on which resilient people typically draw. For example,
more active coping (Silver, Holman, McIntosh, Poulin, & being able to experience positive events such as grati-
Gil-Rivas, 2002). Disengaging from coping efforts, then, tude or love following the September 11 attacks enabled
can predict psychological difficulties over time. many people to cope with these distressing events and to
Stress and Its Management 709

experience posttraumatic growth (Tugade & Fredrickson, such as postoperative recovery (Solano, Donati, Pecci,
2004). Resilience can be characterized by posttraumatic Persichetti, & Colaci, 2003).
growth: people report many ways in which they have actu- Examining the process of emotional disclosure in health
ally benefited from traumatic events, such as reordering settings has been helpful for understanding the underlying
their priorities, more fully appreciating what they have, mechanisms whereby these effects occur, and as a result,
and valuing personal relationships more (Low, Bower, has enriched the theory that gave rise to these studies.
Kwan, & Seldon, 2008; Taylor, 1983). There are cognitive benefits associated with talking about
or writing about a traumatic event such as organizing one’s
thoughts and being able to find meaning in the experi-
Emotional Disclosure
ence (Lepore, Ragan, & Jones, 2000). Talking or writing
Considerable social psychological research has examined about traumatic or stressful events provides an opportunity
the mental and physical health benefits of emotional dis- for emotional-approach coping (Lepore & Smyth, 2002)
closure, especially in the context of traumatic or stressful and for affirming personal values (Creswell et al., 2007).
events. Fairly consistently, this research finds beneficial Talking with others allows one to gain information or
effects on health (Pennebaker & Chung, 2007). The benefits insight about a stressful event or about effective coping.
of disclosure stem in part from the research just discussed, It may also elicit emotional support from others.
which reveals the benefits of emotion-focused coping.
For years, researchers suspected that when people Coping With Chronic Illness
undergo traumatic events and cannot or do not communicate
about them, those events may fester inside them, producing Social psychologists have also studied how people cope
obsessive thoughts and physiological reactivity. Indeed, the with, adjust to, and manage the threats associated with
more people are forced to inhibit thoughts, emotions, and chronic illness, and these insights have enriched social
behaviors, the more their underlying physiological activ- psychological theories. Clinical health psychologists study-
ity increases (Pennebaker, 1997). Consequently, the ability ing coping with chronic illness tend to focus on the prob-
to confide in others or to consciously confront one’s feel- lems people experience and on psychological distress. By
ings may eliminate the need to obsess about and inhibit the contrast, social psychologists have focused on responses to
trauma and reduce concomitant physiological reactivity. chronic illness that help restore or maintain psychological
In an initial investigation of this intriguing hypothesis, functioning. As the next section shows, attribution theory
Pennebaker and Beall (1986) had 46 undergraduates write and social cognition provide valuable frameworks for
about either the most traumatic and stressful event in their addressing these issues.
lives or about trivial topics. Although writing about trau-
Beliefs About the Cause of an Illness
mas was more upsetting immediately after the essays were
completed than writing about trivial topics, the students People with chronic illnesses often develop theories about
were less likely to visit the student health center for illness the origins of the illness (e.g., Costanzo, Lutgendorf,
for the following 6 months. Subsequent research found Bradley, Rose, & Anderson, 2005). These theories include
that when people talked about traumatic events, their skin stress, physical injury, disease-causing bacteria, and God’s
conductance, heart rate, and blood pressure all decreased will. Of potentially greater significance is where patients
(Pennebaker, Hughes, & O’Heeron, 1987). Research has ultimately place the blame or responsibility for their ill-
also uncovered beneficial long-term effects of emotional ness: Do they blame themselves, another person, the envi-
disclosure on immune functioning (e.g., Petrie, Booth, ronment, or a quirk of fate?
Pennebaker, Davison, & Thomas, 1995). Self-blame for chronic illness is widespread. Patients
Drawing on this method, interventions have employed frequently believe they brought on their illnesses through
written or oral exercises designed to encourage emotional their own actions. Sometimes, these perceptions are to
expression. Such interventions have lead to improved some extent correct. Poor health habits, such as smoking,
health among AIDS patients (e.g., Petrie, Fontanilla, improper diet, or lack of exercise, contribute to heart dis-
Thomas, Booth, & Pennebaker, 2004), breast cancer ease, stroke, or cancer. But in many cases, a patient’s self-
patients (Stanton et al., 2002), and asthma and rheuma- blame is ill placed, as when a disease is caused primarily
toid arthritis patients, among other conditions (Norman, by a genetically based defect. What are the consequences of
Lumley, Dooley, & Diamond, 2004). Even writing about self-blame? Unfortunately, there is no definitive answer to
emotional topics via e-mail can improve health (Sheese, this question. Some researchers have found that self-blame
Brown, & Graziano, 2004). Writing interventions have also can lead to guilt, self-recrimination, or depression (e.g.,
been used to help people cope with debilitating treatments Bennett, Compas, Beckjord, & Glinder, 2005). However,
710 Health

perceiving the cause of one’s illness as self-generated may pulmonary disease found that those with high self-efficacy
also represent an effort to assume control over the disor- expectations lived longer than those without such expecta-
der; such feelings can be adaptive for coming to terms with tions (Kaplan, Ries, Prewitt, & Eakin, 1994).
the disorder. It may be that self-blame is adaptive under Not all studies find that feelings of control are adap-
certain conditions but not others (Taylor, 2009). tive in adjusting to chronic conditions. When real control
Research uniformly suggests that blaming another per- is low, efforts to induce it or exert it may be unsuccessful
son for one’s disorder is maladaptive (Affleck, Tennen, and backfire (Burish et al., 1984; Tennen, Affleck, Urrows,
Pfeiffer, & Fifield, 1987; Taylor, Lichtman, & Wood, 1984). Higgins, & Mendola, 1992; Toshima, Kaplan, & Ries,
For example, some patients believe that their disorder 1992). When perceived control is dramatically discon-
was brought about by stress caused by family members, firmed, as with a cancer recurrence, worse adjustment may
ex-spouses, or colleagues at work. Blame of this other per- result. For example, Tomich and Helgeson (2006) found
son or persons may be tied to unresolved hostility, which that initial perceptions of personal control over breast can-
can interfere with adjustment to the disease. By contrast, cer were associated with women’s reports of worse mental
forgiveness is tied to fewer health complaints (Lawler et al., and physical functioning 5 years later if they had sustained
2005). a recurrence. On the whole, however, control appears to be
helpful for coping with many aspects of chronic illness.
Beliefs About the Controllability of the Illness
Patients develop a number of control-related beliefs with Positive Changes in Response to Chronic Illness
respect to chronic illness. They may believe, as do many
cancer patients, that they can prevent a recurrence of the As noted, clinical health psychology research has focused
disease through good health habits or even sheer force of disproportionately on the negative emotions and expe-
will. They may believe that by complying with treatments riences that are produced by chronic illness. However,
and physicians’ recommendations, they achieve vicarious many people experience positive emotions or beliefs
control over their illness (e.g., Helgeson, 1992). They may (Cordova, Cunningham, Carlson & Andrykowski, 2001;
believe that they personally have direct control over the ill- McFarland & Alvaro, 2000; Ryff & Singer, 1996; Scheier,
ness through self-administration of a treatment regimen. Weintraub, & Carver, 1986), including personal growth or
These control-related beliefs may or may not be accurate. a sense of meaning in life (Taylor, 1983). These reactions
For example, if patients do maintain a treatment regimen, may occur because chronically ill people perceive that
they can exercise real control over the possibility of recur- they have narrowly escaped death or because they have
rence or exacerbation of their illness. On the other hand, reordered their priorities in a more satisfying way.
the belief that one’s illness can be controlled through a Studies with a broad array of chronically ill populations
positive attitude may or may not be correct. have found that many, sometimes most, chronically ill patients
Beliefs in control (or a sense of self-efficacy) with find at least some beneficial changes in their lives, including
respect to the disease and its treatment are generally but an increased ability to appreciate each day and the inspiration
not always adaptive. For example, cancer patients who to do things now rather than postponing them (R. L. Collins,
believe that they have control over their illness are better Taylor, & Skokan, 1990; Ostir, Berges, Ottenbacher, Clow, &
adjusted than are patients without such beliefs (Thompson, Ottenbacher, 2008; Mohr et al., 1999). People report that they
Nanni, & Levine, 1994). A sense of control or self-efficacy are putting more effort into their relationships and believe
improves adjustment among people with a variety of they have acquired more awareness of others’ feelings and
chronic health problems, such as sickle-cell disease more empathy and compassion for others. They report feel-
(Edwards, Telfair, Cecil, & Lenoci, 2001), chronic obstruc- ing stronger and more self-assured as well. Benefit finding
tive pulmonary disease (Kohler, Fish, & Greene, 2002), and positive emotions correlate not only with psychological
AIDS (Taylor, Helgeson, Reed, & Skokan, 1991), ovarian adjustment but also with better social functioning and health
cancer (Norton et al., 2005), and patients recovering from (Aspinwall & MacNamara, 2005; Danoff-Burg & Revenson,
angioplasty (Helgeson & Fritz, 1999). Children also benefit 2005; Low, Stanton, & Danoff-Burg, 2006). Two studies
from perceived control. Griffin and Chen (2006), for exam- compared the quality of life experienced by cancer patients
ple, found that high perceived control was associated with with a normal sample free of chronic disease, and both stud-
better asthma control. Even for patients who are physically ies found the quality of life experienced by the cancer sample
or psychosocially badly off, perceptions of control facili- to be higher than that of the non-ill sample (Danoff, Kramer,
tate psychological adjustment (McQuillen, Licht, & Licht, Irwin, & Gottlieb, 1983; Tempelaar et al., 1989).
2003). Perceived control or self-efficacy may even help to Helgeson, Reynolds, and Tomich (2006) completed a
prolong life. A study of patients with chronic obstructive meta-analysis of the benefit-finding literature and found
Social Support 711

that, for the most part, perceived growth is related to posi- but neither is perceived growth a definitive pathway to
tive emotional functioning and low risk of depression. The good psychological functioning.
ability to reappraise one’s situation positively is related As these findings suggest, the time that has elapsed
to a more positive mood (Pakenham, 2005) and to post- since a trauma may be an important moderator predicting
traumatic growth in women with breast cancer (Manne the relation of personal growth to psychological adjust-
et al., 2004; Sears, Stanton, & Danoff-Burg, 2003), espe- ment (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996). Personal growth can-
cially among women with more advanced disease. Finding not occur immediately following an event, and in the early
meaning in a chronic illness and coping through religion stages of adjusting, benefit finding is likely to co-occur
can also improve adjustment to chronic illness (Calhoun, with psychological distress. Over time, however, distress
Cann, Tedeschi, & McMillan, 2000; Helgeson, 2003; may recede, whereas personal growth may be maintained
Schanowitz & Nicassio, 2006). Perceptions of growth are or enhanced (Helgeson et al., 2006). From the standpoint of
related to lower anxiety, reduced distress, better quality of basic research, the findings suggest the importance of dif-
life, and a more positive subjective rating of physical health. ferent coping strategies for different phases of adaptation
Some investigators have used the theory of cognitive to disorders, indicating the fact that the search for meaning
adaptation to examine patients’ reactions to chronic illness. may be more beneficial at some times than at others. In
The theory of cognitive adaptation (Taylor, 1983) main- terms of applications, the findings suggest that interven-
tains that following a severe threat to the self, such as a tions to help people restore their sense of self-esteem, opti-
chronic illness diagnosis, people are able to restore their mism, and mastery about the future may be useful.1
psychological functioning by shoring up their self-esteem, In the realm of psychosocial resources and coping, one
their sense of mastery over the events around them, opti- resource in particular stands out, and that is social support.
mism about the future, and the experience of meaning Because social psychologists have made extensive contri-
in the event or in their lives more generally. In a test of butions to this literature, I cover it in detail here.
this theory, Moore, Norman, Harris, and Makris (2007)
recruited patients with venous thrombosis, had them com-
plete measures of these variables, and related them to out- SOCIAL SUPPORT
come variables of anxiety, depression, thrombosis-related
worries, and quality of life. Mastery, self-esteem, and opti- A topic central to social psychologists’ health-related
mism were significantly associated with good adjustment, research is social support. Social support is the percep-
as the theory predicts. Similarly, using this theoretical tion or experience that one is loved and cared for by
framework, Helgeson (2003) found that a positive sense of others, esteemed and valued, and part of a social net-
self, optimism, and personal control were associated pro- work of mutual assistance and obligations (Wills, 1991).
spectively with good adjustment to coronary heart disease Social support may come from a partner, relative, friends,
and a reduced likelihood of a repeat cardiac event over a coworkers, social and community ties, or even a devoted
4-year follow-up, controlling for initial adjustment (see pet (dogs are the best; Allen, 2003). The benefits of social
also Helgeson & Fritz, 1999). support are thought to have evolved from human beings’
There is, however, some contradictory evidence most significant adaptation to threat, namely, group living.
regarding perceived control and recurrence. Because per- Whereas other animals are armed with sharp teeth or claws
sonal growth also arises in the context of coping with an and defensive resources such as thick skin or speed, pri-
extremely stressful event, perceived growth can be tied to mates depend critically on group living for survival.
more intrusive thoughts about the event. This seemingly Taxonomies of social support have suggested several
paradoxical relationship between experiencing intrusive specific beneficial forms. Informational support occurs
thoughts and the perception of personal growth may be when one person helps another person to understand
signs that people are working through the implications of stressful conditions better and to select what resources
the event for their lives. In the process of conducting this
cognitive and affective work, personal growth may result,
but intrusive thoughts may be inevitable as well. For exam- 1Of interest is the fact that benefit finding is related to reduced
ple, Moore and colleagues (2007) found that finding mean- distress especially for health stressors, relative to other stressors
ing in the illness experience was associated with elevated involving personal trauma. However, studies of personal growth
distress, perhaps indicative of enduring concern about the and health disproportionately use cross-sectional as opposed to
disorder (see also Tomich & Helgeson, 2004). This pattern longitudinal data and show a modest gender skew in the direc-
indicates that perceived growth is not merely the result of tion of more female participants, and so the contribution of these
a halo effect induced by successful coping with a stressor issues to the conclusions in the literature is difficult to assess.
712 Health

and coping strategies may be needed to deal with these isolation is a significant risk for morbidity and mortality
conditions. Instrumental support involves the provision of with effect sizes on par with or exceeding those of smok-
tangible assistance such as services, financial support, and ing, blood pressure, lipids, obesity, and physical activity
other forms of specific aid or goods. Emotional support (House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988).
involves providing warmth or nurturance to another per- By what biological routes does social support affect
son and reassuring the person that he or she is a valuable health outcomes? Much research has focused on stress
individual for whom others care. Critical to the definition pathways. As noted, although sympathetic and HPA axis
of social support, however, is the fact that the perception responses to stress have short-term protective effects, over
that these resources are available, should they be needed, time they have adverse long-term implications for health,
is beneficial, and not only the actual experience of social including an elevated risk for many chronic diseases
support. Indeed, as will be evident later in this section, (McEwen, 2008; Saxbe, Repetti, & Nishina, 2008). Stress
sometimes the actual use of social support resources has also affects the risk for adverse health outcomes by altering
hidden costs. immune functioning in ways that leave a person vulnerable
Social support is typically measured either in terms to opportunistic diseases and infections or by diminishing
of structural social support or functional support (Thoits, immune system sensitivity to cortisol, which would nor-
1995). Structural support is the number of social relation- mally terminate stress-induced inflammation; these twin
ships a person has and the interconnectedness among those effects account for the fact that stress may both increase the
relationships, an assessment also known as social integra- risk for infectious disorders (immunosuppression) and also
tion. Functional support is assessed in terms of the specific exacerbate or confer risk for diseases in which the central
functions (informational, instrumental, and emotional) feature is excessive inflammation, such as allergies, auto-
that a specific member of one’s network may serve for immune disease, rheumatoid disorders, and cardiovascu-
a person. lar disorders (Miller, Cohen, & Ritchey, 2002). All of the
An early debate in the social support literature ques- bodily systems’ functioning is interrelated. To the extent
tioned whether social support is generally beneficial to that social support can keep sympathetic nervous system
mental and physical health during nonstressful as well as and HPA axis responses to stress low, it may have benefi-
stressful times (the direct effects hypothesis) or whether cial effects on other systems, such as immune functioning,
the health and mental health benefits of social support are as well and thus affect health in a positive direction.
evident chiefly during periods of high stress (the buffering There may be other biological systems that underlie
hypothesis; Cohen & Wills, 1985). Both effects appear to the benefits of social support. Oxytocin and the opioid
hold. When social support is assessed through measures of system (the body’s natural pain reduction system) may be
social integration, direct effects of support on mental and implicated in the neuroendocrine and physiological ben-
physical health are typically found (Thoits, 1995), but not efits of social support. For example, oxytocin is known to
buffering effects, whereas the perception that emotional be released in response to social contact, and it has been
support is available is tied to both direct and buffering tied to reduced sympathetic nervous system and HPA axis
effects (e.g., Wethington & Kessler, 1986). responses to stress (Taylor & Seeman, 2000).
The benefits of social support are experienced, in part,
because it helps people to stave off illness altogether. For
Benefits of Social Support
example, Cohen and associates intentionally infected
Hundreds of studies attest to the fact that social support healthy community volunteers with a cold or flu virus by
consistently reduces psychological distress during times swabbing the inside of their nasal passages with virus-
of stress, such as depression and anxiety. It also promotes soaked cotton swabs. They found th

You might also like