You are on page 1of 3

Forensic Science International: Genetics Supplement Series 6 (2017) e3–e5

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Forensic Science International: Genetics Supplement Series


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/fsigss

Hand activities during robberies—Relevance to consideration of DNA T


transfer and detection

C.J. Stellaa,b, R.J. Mitchellb, R.A.H. van Oorschota,b,
a
Office of the Chief Forensic Scientist, Victoria Police Forensic Services Department, Macleod, Victoria, Australia
b
School of Molecular Sciences, College of Science, health and Engineering, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: It is becoming increasingly relevant to address activity level issues relating to mechanisms explaining how DNA
Activity became located at the collection site. DNA transferred to and from a surface upon contact can be influenced by
Transfer the manner, frequency and relative timing of contact, plus substrate types and their histories. It is important to
DNA understand these factors during criminal activity. People touch many things during everyday activities, and in a
Hands
relatively short period of time, and this may influence how DNA of a postulated source is gained or lost. Here we
consider touch parameters (what, how, duration, frequency) of both hands during armed and unarmed rob-
beries. Data were generated from the video recording of > 50 events. During armed robberies, the dominant
hand made ∼5 touches/min and the non-dominant hand ∼12. The majority of touches by the dominant hand
was with personal items whereas the non-dominant hand mainly touched non-personal items. During unarmed
robberies, the dominant hand made ∼9 touches/min and the non-dominant hand ∼12. The majority of touches
for both hands was with non-personal items. These data increase our awareness of what an individual’s hand
touches during a crime and importantly how that may influence the likelihood of detecting a DNA profile
derived from a person of interest from items of interest. Such awareness can improve likelihood estimates of
proposed scenarios.

1. Introduction 2. Materials and methods

DNA can be transfered upon physical contact and various factors 47 videos taken by CCTV of robberies and attempted robberies (37
can impact transfer and detectability [1–15]. Awareness of these fac- armed, 10 unarmed), were analysed for a range of factors; number of
tors, and their impacts, is beneficial when addressing questions on how touches, types of items touched and their substrates, duration of tou-
DNA from a particular source became present on a sampled item. ches, which part of the hand touched, and type of contact (Table 1).
van Oorschot et al. [16] found that during everyday general activ- Videos were selected following general criteria: readily accessible;
ities (such as whilst office multi-tasking, sitting at a café, on public duration of ∼20 to 100 s; visability of both hands and what was tou-
transport, eating meals, cooking, being at home or in a park), people ched. The period immediately prior to the offence was considered se-
perform ∼15 touches per minute with their dominant hand. These parately from the period during the offence. Seven of eight ‘priors’ had
touches were of both personal (including self) and non-personal objects. associated ‘durings’ analysed.
To gain further insights into hand touches during, and just prior to, The offences were recorded during 2006–16 (majority during
an offence we analysed videos of a common offence for which DNA 2015–16), worldwide (chiefly USA and UK). All were committed inside
samples are regularly collected: robbery (armed and unarmed), ex- stores (including: convenience, jewellery, pharmacy, fast-food). The
amining both dominant and non-dominant hands. weapon used included in relative frequency order: handgun, machete,
Whilst video footage of a criminal offence can assist an investiga- knife, longarm firearm, other.
tion, in many cases it is unavailable, so consideration of data from other
sources is needed when interpreting DNA profiles from an ‘activity 3. Results
level’ perspective. This study provides a further resource.
Table 1 shows that during armed robberies, recorded for an average


Corresponding author at: Office of the Chief Forensic Scientist, Victoria Police Forensic Services Department, Macleod, Victoria, Australia.
E-mail address: roland.vanoorschot@police.vic.gov.au (R.A.H. van Oorschot).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.fsigss.2017.09.002
Received 23 August 2017; Accepted 2 September 2017
Available online 04 September 2017
1875-1768/ © 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
C.J. Stella et al. Forensic Science International: Genetics Supplement Series 6 (2017) e3–e5

Table 1
Summary of touches of personala and non-personal objects immediately prior and during armed and non-armed robberies by dominant and non-dominant handsb.

Activity – Hand Obs (n) Mean duration/ Obs Touches (n) Objects Duration
Robbery (s) (std)
Mean/obs Pers:non-pers Mean/obs Pers:non-pers Prop time touching Pers:non-pers obj
(std) obj (%) (std) obj (%) obj/obs (std) (%)

Armed – prior Dominant 5 65.6 (51.2) 6.2 (5.8) 84: 16 3.6 (1.7) 72: 28 0.70 (0.29) 91: 9
Non-dominant 5 65.6 (51.2) 5.6 (5.5) 79: 21 2.8 (1.9) 79: 21 0.81 (0.23) 90: 10
Armed – during Dominant 37 35.8 (26.8) 3.2 (2.6) 62: 38 2.4 (1.7) 60: 40 0.93 (0.14) 91: 9
Non-dominant 37 35.8 (26.8) 7.0 (5.4) 37: 63 3.4 (1.8) 37: 63 0.59 (0.31) 50: 50
Unarmed – prior Dominant 3 26 (1.7) 6.7 (4.0) 80: 20 2.3 (1.5) 71: 29 0.61 (0.43) 96: 4
Non-dominant 3 26 (1.7) 6.7 (2.1) 85: 15 3.0 (1.7) 67: 33 0.61 (0.31) 96: 4
Unarmed – Dominant 10 40.4 (28.2) 6.1 (3.7) 29: 71 4.5 (2.9) 18: 82 0.70 (0.28) 70: 30
during
Non-dominant 10 40.4 (28.2) 8.1 (5.5) 22: 78 4.3 (2.7) 16: 84 0.58 (0.27) 35: 65

a
Inclusive of self, assumed personal objects.
b
Data also collected regarding: part of hand making contact, types of contact and substrate of objects touched. Results are summarised within text.

of 36 s, the dominant hand made an average of 3.2 touches and the non- observed during general activities (∼15) [16]. The approximate
dominant hand 7.0 (extrapolating to ∼5 and ∼12 per min, respec- number of touches by the non-dominant hand (∼12/min) was higher
tively). The majority of touches by the dominant hand were with per- than the dominant hand during these activities, and close to the number
sonal objects whereas the non-dominant hand mainly touched non- of times observed during general activities by the dominant hand. The
personal. During unarmed robberies, recorded for an average of 40 s, proportions of non-personal objects touched by either hand during
the dominant hand made an average of 6.1 touches and the non- unarmed robberies, and the non-dominant hand during armed rob-
dominant hand 8.1 (extrapolating to ∼9 and ∼12 per min, respec- beries, were higher than for other activity-hand situations within this
tively). The majority of touches for both hands was with non-personal study and the majority of general activities reported previously [16].
objects. Whilst we recognise that the present data set is limited, and the
In armed robberies fewer touches were made by the dominant hand offence type and location analysed narrow, this study points to the need
‘during’, than ‘prior’. This difference was not evident with unarmed to consider general behaviours relating to an offence when determining
robberies (Table 1). The average number of objects touched during the the probability of the presence of a particular profile and/or profile
observed period, when extrapolated to a standard duration of 1 min, mixture configuration, when addressing activity level related questions.
was 3–7 objects with the fewest touched prior to armed robberies Further research of physical behaviours would be beneficial.
(Table 1). Hands were in contact with an object > 50% of the time Whilst not the focus of this study, observations of human behaviours
during each activity by either hand, with the dominant hand during could also assist with determining the most useful item(s), or area(s) of
armed robberies contacting an object for the longest period (93%) an item, from which to collect a DNA sample, given a particular si-
(Table 1). The durations of touches were longest with personal rather tuation.
than non-personal objects, except for the non-dominant hand during
armed and un-armed robberies (Table 1). 5. Conclusion
For each activity type and hand use, the majority of touches
(43–63%) involved the whole hand (palm plus fingers) rather than just Many things are touched by hands within a short timeframe. As
the palm, most of the fingers or fingertips, except for prior to unarmed each touch may influence the loss and gain of DNA from and to hands
robbery where touches with just fingertips were equally frequent and objects touched, it is important to gain insight into actions of hands
(38%). Also, for each activity and hand, the majority of contacts when interpreting crime related DNA profiles from an activity level
(67–91%) was deemed ‘pressure’ contact rather than ‘friction’ or ‘pas- perspective.
sive’ contact.
The substrate of the majority of personal objects touched was fabric Conflict of interest
(mainly own cloths or bag) (52–94%) for all activity types and hands,
except for the dominant hand during armed robberies where the most None.
touched substrate of personal objects was ‘hard’ (35%) (mainly weapon
areas made of metal or hard plastic). The substrate of the majority of References
non-personal objects touched were ‘hard’ (e.g. counter, register, stolen
goods) for all activities and hands, except for the dominant and non- [1] R.A.H. van Oorschot, M.K. Jones, DNA fingerprinting from fingerprints, Nature 387
dominat hands during unarmed robberies where the majority (57–58%) (1997) 767.
[2] M. Goray, E. Eken, R.J. Mitchell, et al., Secondary DNA transfer of biological sub-
were of wood/paper (e.g. cash, counter, paperbags), and the dominant stances under varying test conditions, Forensic Sci. Int. Genet. 4 (2010) 62–67.
hand prior to armed robbery, where substrates other than those defined [3] M. Goray, R.J. Mitchell, R.A.H. van Oorschot, Investigation of secondary DNA
as hard, fabric, skin/hair, or wood/paper (e.g. rubber, thin plastic bags) transfer of skin cells under controlled test conditions, Legal Med. 12 (2010)
117–120.
were as frequently touched (40%) as hard substrates. [4] T.J. Verdon, R.J. Mitchell, R.A.H. van Oorschot, The influence of substrate on DNA
During armed robberies the weapon was only, or mainly, held in the transfer and extraction efficiency, Forensic Sci. Int. Genet. 7 (2013) 167–175.
dominant hand in all situations, regardless of weapon type, except in [5] R.A.H. van Oorschot, R. McArdle, W.H. Goodwin, et al., DNA transfer: the role of
temperature and drying time, Legal Med. 16 (2014) 161–163.
one situation involving a longarm weapon. [6] B. Szkuta, M.L. Harvey, K.N. Ballantyne, et al., DNA transfer by examination tools
–a risk for forensic casework? Forensic Sci. Int. Genet. 16 (2015) 246–254.
[7] M. van den Berge, B. Bhoelai, J. Harteveld, et al., Advancing forensic RNA typing:
4. Discussion on non-target secretions a nasal mucosa marker, a differential co-extraction pro-
tocol and the sensitivity of DNA and RNA profiling, Forensic Sci. Int. Genet. 20
The approximate number of touches per minute by the dominant (2016) 119–129.
[8] R.A.H. van Oorschot, G. Glavich, R.J. Mitchell, Persistence of DNA deposited by the
hand during the assessed activities (∼5/min during armed robberies original user on objects after subsequent use by a second person, Forensic Sci. Int.
and ∼9/min during unarmed robberies) were lower than those

e4
C.J. Stella et al. Forensic Science International: Genetics Supplement Series 6 (2017) e3–e5

Genet. 8 (2014) 219–225. DNA from touched objects, Forensic Sc. Int.: Genet. 25 (2016) 26–33.
[9] M. Goray, S. Fowler, B. Szkuta, et al., Shedder status –an analysis of self and non- [13] B. Szkuta, K.N. Ballantyne, R.A.H. van Oorschot, Transfer and persistence of DNA
self DNA in multiple handprints deposited by the same individual over time, on the hands and the influence of activities performed, Forensic Sci. Int.: Genet. 28
Forensic Sci. Int. Genet. 23 (2016) 190–196. (2017) 10–20.
[10] V.J. Lehmann, R.J. Mitchell, K.N. Ballantyne, et al., Following the transfer of DNA: [14] M. Goray, R.A.H. van Oorschot, The complexities of DNA transfer during a social
how does the presence of background DNA affect the transfer and detection of a setting, Legal Med. 17 (2015) 82–91.
target source of DNA? Forensic Sci. Int. Genet. 19 (2015) 68–75. [15] F. Oldoni, V. Castella, D. Hall, Shedding light on the relative DNA contribution of
[11] G.E. Meakin, E.M. Butcher, R.A.H. van Oorschot, et al., Trace DNA evidence dy- two persons handling the same object, Forensic Sci. Int.: Genet. 24 (2016) 148–157.
namics: an investigation into the deposition and persistence of directly- and in- [16] R.A.H. van Oorschot, D.L. McColl, J.E. Alderton, et al., Activities between activities
directly-transferred DNA on regularly-used knives, Forensic Sci. Int.: Genet. 29 of focus –relevant when assessing DNA transfer probabilities, Forensic Sci. Int.:
(2017) 38–47. Genet. Suppl. Ser. 5 (2015) e75–e77.
[12] A.K. Buckingham, M. Harvey, R.A.H. van Oorschot, The origin of unknown source

e5

You might also like