You are on page 1of 6

Acta Pñdiatr 88: 1327±32.

1999

Duration of breastfeeding and developmental milestones during the


latter half of infancy
M Vestergaard1, C Obel1, TB Henriksen1, HT Sørensen2, E Skajaa1 and J Østergaard3
Perinatal Epidemiological Research Unit1, Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Department of Internal Medicine V2 and
Department of Paediatrics3, Aarhus University Hospital, Denmark

Vestergaard M, Obel C, Henriksen TB, Sørensen HT, Skajaa E, Østergaard J. Duration of


breastfeeding and developmental milestones during the latter half of infancy. Acta Pædiatr 1999;
88: 1327–32. Stockholm. ISSN 0803–5253
Several studies have suggested that breastfeeding has a long-term influence on brain development.
However, interpretation of these findings is complicated by the presence of many potential
confounding factors. Only a few studies have examined infants before 1 y of age, although very
early assessment might reduce the role of environmental influence. We investigated the association
between exclusive breastfeeding and three developmental milestones related to general and fine
motor skills and early language development at the age of 8 mo. We followed 1656 healthy,
singleton, term infants, with a birthweight of at least 2500 g, born between May 1991 and February
1992 in Aarhus, Denmark. Information was collected at 16 wk gestation, at delivery and when the
infant was 8 mo old. Motor skills were evaluated by measurement of crawling and pincer grip.
Early language development was defined as the ability to babble in polysyllables. The proportion
of infants who mastered the specific milestones increased consistently with increasing duration of
breastfeeding. The relative risk for the highest versus the lowest breastfeeding category was 1.3
(95% CI: 1.0–1.6) for crawling, 1.2 (95% CI: 1.1–1.3) for pincer grip and 1.5 (95% CI: 1.3–1.8)
for polysyllable babbling. Little change was found after adjustment for confounding. In conclusion,
our data support the hypothesis that breastfeeding benefits neurodevelopment. & Breastfeeding,
developmental milestones, infant development

M Vestergaard, Perinatal Epidemiological Research Unit, Department of Obstetrics and


Gynaecology, Aarhus University Hospital, DK-8200 Aarhus N, Denmark (Tel. ‡45 89496380, fax.
‡45 89496373)

Experiments in animals have indicated that prenatal and fields of development into a common score. However, it
early postnatal malnutrition affects the maturation and is also important to assess the association between
function of the developing central nervous system breastfeeding and specific aspects of neurodevelop-
(1, 2). At the time of birth, the human brain experiences ment.
a growth spurt (3), which continues for 12–18 mo Breastfed and formula-fed infants could differ due to
postpartum (4, 5), suggesting that optimal nutrition may differences in parental skills, genetic potential or other
be particularly important during this period. Several potential confounding factors. However, another possi-
studies have examined the relationship between breast- ble influential factor is that human milk contains
feeding and development. The majority found that substances essential for optimal neurodevelopment.
neurodevelopment was more advanced among indivi- Most evidence is related to the long-chain polyunsatu-
duals who were breastfed rather than formula-fed in rated fatty acids, particularly docosahexaenoic acid
early life (6–14), but some found no difference (15–17). (DHA), which is present in human milk, but not in
However, the association between breastfeeding and standard formulas (18). DHA is an important compo-
development of infants below 1 y of age has seldom nent of cell membranes in the cortex and retina (19–22).
been studied. One would expect differences to be Randomized trials have shown that infants who were
present early in life if breastfeeding benefits neurode- fed formula supplemented with DHA had more ad-
velopment directly. In two studies (9, 10) infants were vanced neural maturation than infants fed standard
examined at various ages beginning at 6 mo, but no formula (19, 20, 23). Furthermore, human milk contains
differences were found until age 1 y (10) and 2 y (9). hormones, oligosaccharides and other trophic factors
Most investigators have measured infant development (24, 25) that might influence brain growth and matura-
with standardized tests such as “the Bayley Scales of tion.
Infant Development”. These tests summarize different The aim of the present study was to examine the

 Scandinavian University Press 1999. ISSN 0803-5253


1328 A Vestergaard et al. ACTA PÆDIATR 88 (1999)

association between breastfeeding and three develop- were not feeling well. The health visitors were not
mental milestones related to fine motor skills, general aware of the specific aim of this study.
motor skills and early language development at the age Duration of breastfeeding was classified into four
of 8 mo. categories based on the number of months of exclusive
breastfeeding, i.e. no food apart from mother’s milk,
water and vitamins. Information about the duration of
partial breastfeeding was available for the infants who
were never exclusively breastfed.
Patients and methods General motor skills were evaluated, i.e. infants
The infants constituted a well-defined part of a prenatal crawling with arms and legs moving in a diagonal
cohort established at the Department of Obstetrics, pattern, and with the stomach above the mat.
Aarhus University Hospital, Denmark, from August Fine motor skills were evaluated, i.e. infants demon-
1989 to September 1991. The design of the study has strating the pincer grip, that is the ability to pick up a
been described previously (26). Briefly, all women tiny object between thumb and index finger with right or
attending the routine antenatal care programme were left hand.
invited to participate. The department, which is the only Early language development was defined as the
maternity ward in the city, serves a well-defined infant’s babbling abilities and use of syllables and
geographical area with a population of about 250 000. consonant change. The infants were categorized as
At 16 wk gestation the women were asked to fill in a polysyllable babblers if they used syllables and changed
basic questionnaire about medical and obstetric history, consonants within the same string, e.g. “nonono-
and a study entry questionnaire providing information mamama-najnajnaj”.
about the sociodemographic conditions of the family. Corrected examination age was defined as the age at
The tending midwife filled in a specific registration examination adjusted for gestational age at birth.
form providing information about the delivery and the The research protocol was approved by the regional
newborn (sex, weight, length, Apgar score and mal- committee for ethics in science and by the Danish
formations). A research midwife who reviewed all the committee of registers.
hospital records validated the obstetric data. Any We analysed fine and general motor skills and
complication during the first days after delivery was language development using contingency tables in
registered. relation to duration of breastfeeding. The results from
The infants were born between May 1991 and the bivariate analyses are presented as relative risks
February 1992. Inclusion criteria for the study were as (RR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI). We subse-
follows: singleton infants whose mothers were included quently stratified the contingency tables according to
in the basic study and who were still living in the each of the potential confounding factors listed and
municipality of Aarhus at the age of 8 mo; Danish- categorized as shown in Table 3. Adjustment for
speaking parents; gestational age 37–42 wk; birth- multiple variables was carried out by logistic regression
weight 2500 g; Apgar score 7 at 5 min after birth; no analyses. A priori important variables, and variables
known handicaps; and no referral to the neonatal that changed the estimates of association, were chosen
department. Of the 2863 liveborn infants, 2108 were for the final model (29, 30). Unadjusted and adjusted
eligible for the study. Of these, 1883 (89%) participated, odds ratios (OR) are presented in Table 4 because
but because of missing information on breastfeeding logistic regression analyses do not provide RR.
and milestone evaluation, only 1656 (79%) were
included in the analyses.
A health visitor interviewed the parents about the
duration of breastfeeding, periods of illness, the use of
day care, changes of cohabitation status, parents’ job Results
status and the parents’ experience with child rearing. A We found that 644 (38.8%) infants were able to crawl,
questionnaire completed by the parents provided 1243 (75.1%) had mastered the pincer grip with right or
information about mothers’ alcohol and smoking habits left hand and 1021 (61.7%) were polysyllable babblers.
during the first months postpartum. Milestone evalua- One thousand five hundred and fifty-two (93.7%)
tions of fine motor skills, general motor skills and women breastfed their infants exclusively for at least
language development were tested according to well- 1 mo, and 1088 (65.7%) continued for 4 mo, as
described methods (27, 28). The assessments were recommended by the WHO. The mean corrected
made by the health visitors (n = 74) in the homes of examination age was 35.7 wk, and 95% of the infants
the infants in the presence of at least one parent. The were examined within the age interval 30.7–40.7 wk.
assessment was based on this single visit to the home, The proportion of infants who mastered the specific
which lasted between 1 and 1.5 h. Milestone evaluation milestones increased consistently with increasing dura-
was exclusively based on observations of the health tion of breastfeeding. The RR for the highest vs. lowest
visitors. Examinations were postponed when the infants breastfeeding category was 1.3 (95% CI: 1.0–1.6) for
ACTA PÆDIATR 88 (1999) Breastfeeding and developmental milestones 1329

Table 1. Developmental milestones according to the duration of exclusive breastfeeding among 1656 infants.

Crawling Pincer grip Polysyllable babblers


Exclusive Total
breastfeeding (mo) count (n) % RR (95% CI) % RR (95% CI) % RR (95% CI)
0–1 266 35.0 Reference 70.7 Reference 48.5 Reference
2–3 302 33.8 1.0 (0.8–1.2) 71.9 1.0 (0.9–1.1) 58.3 1.2 (1.0–1.4)
4–5 866 40.2 1.1 (1.0–1.4) 75.6 1.1 (1.0–1.2) 63.9 1.3 (1.2–1.5)
6‡ 222 45.5 1.3 (1.0–1.6) 82.4 1.2 (1.1–1.3) 73.4 1.5 (1.3–1.8)

crawling, 1.2 (95% CI: 1.1–1.3) for pincer grip and 1.5 Discussion
(95% CI: 1.3–1.8) for polysyllable babbling (Table 1).
Table 2 shows the association between partial By contrast with two previous studies (9, 10), we found
breastfeeding and the three specific milestones for an association between breastfeeding and neurodeve-
infants never exclusively breastfed (n = 101). The lopment in infants below 1 y of age, which persisted
results failed to reach statistical significance. However, after adjustment for confounding. Small sample sizes
the advantages of exclusive breastfeeding were even may explain lack of association in the previous studies
more pronounced when we excluded infants who were (9, 10). The role of environmental confounding might
partially breastfed (data not shown). be reduced in our study because the infants were
Table 3 shows the proportion of infants who mastered examined very early in life, at a time before other
the developmental milestones according to potential factors can have had much influence.
confounders. Mastery of the three developmental mile- The loss to follow-up in our study of 21% was mainly
stones was more prevalent among infants with a higher due to unwillingness to participate and to missing
corrected age at examination. Furthermore, the motor information on breastfeeding or milestone develop-
skills were associated with maternal education. Infants ment. Selection bias would have been introduced if non-
delivered by Caesarean section were less likely to responders were breastfed for a longer time and had less
crawl. Girls and infants with no or at least two siblings advanced development than the infants who remained in
at home were more likely to have mastered the pincer the study, or if non-responders were breastfed for a
grip and to be polysyllable babblers. The pincer grip shorter time and had more advanced development than
was more frequently found if gestational age at delivery the study infants. Unfortunately, there was insufficient
and birthweight were high. Infants were more likely to information on non-responders.
be polysyllable babblers if their mothers were non- Ninety-five percent of the infants were examined
smokers or smoked <9 cigarettes/d during pregnancy, within the age interval 30.7–40.7 wk. We found no
and if they spent time in day care at the time of the change in the estimate of interest when the data were
examination. stratified according to the corrected examination age.
Data were stratified according to the potential Thus, variation in the examination age did not introduce
confounders listed and categorized as shown in Table bias. However, the corrected examination age was
3; there was little or no confounding from any single included in the final logistic regression model.
variable. Multiple logistic regression models were Perinatal problems or illness of the infant may
constructed, incorporating family’s social group, interfere with the duration of breastfeeding. Children
mother’s education, smoking during pregnancy, birth- nursed for a longer time might therefore be a selected
weight, gestational age at delivery, corrected examina- group of more healthy individuals. We carefully
tion age, whether the infant was living with one or two selected a homogeneous cohort of healthy, full-term
parents and the number of illnesses during the first 8 mo babies with a birthweight of at least 2500 g in order to
of life. Table 4 shows that the unadjusted and adjusted minimize the risk that this “healthy child selection”
OR are almost identical. Thus, there was little aggregate might possibly affect later development. In addition, we
confounding from the factors mentioned. included the number of periods with illnesses in the

Table 2. Developmental milestones according to the duration of partial breastfeeding for children never exclusively breastfed (n = 101).
Crawling Pincer grip Polysyllable babblers
Partial Total
breastfeeding (mo) count (n) % RR (95% CI) % RR (95% CI) % RR (95% CI)
0 25 20.0 Reference 72.0 Reference 32.0 Reference
1 27 22.2 1.1 (0.4–3.2) 66.7 0.9 (0.6–1.3) 63.0 2.0 (1.1–3.6)
2 14 35.7 1.8 (0.6–5.2) 71.4 1.0 (0.7–1.5) 57.1 1.8 (0.8–3.8)
3‡ 35 40.0 2.0 (0.9–4.6) 68.6 1.0 (0.7–1.3) 37.1 1.2 (0.6–2.4)

Information about duration of partial breastfeeding was missing for 3 children.


1330 A Vestergaard et al. ACTA PÆDIATR 88 (1999)

Table 3. Developmental milestones according to child and maternal characteristics, sociodemographic factors and maternal lifestyle factors
(n = 1656).
Variable n Crawling (%) Pincer grip (%) Polysyllable babblers (%)
Family’s social status
High 271 40.2 73.1 59.4
Middle 582 37.6 77.5 64.8
Low 676 39.2 73.1 58.9
Mother’s education (y)
7–9 114 34.2 65.8 56.1
10–11 400 33.8 73.8 59.3
12‡ 1013 41.5 76.2 62.9
Mother’s age (y)
<25 239 42.7 73.6 59.0
25–29 720 38.8 74.3 61.3
30–34 499 39.3 75.2 60.7
35‡ 198 33.8 79.3 68.7
Smoking during pregnancy (cigarettes/d)
0 1097 39.7 75.8 62.6
1–9 232 39.2 75.4 62.5
10‡ 259 35.5 71.4 54.1
Alcohol during pregnancy (units/wk)
<1 1032 39.9 75.1 62.4
1–2 387 37.0 73.9 59.4
3–4 136 34.6 73.5 61.8
5–14 38 34.2 68.4 63.2
Sex
M 852 39.2 70.2 58.9
F 804 38.6 80.2 64.6
Gestational age (wk)
37–38 190 33.7 71.1 58.9
39–40 991 39.4 73.8 60.5
41‡ 464 40.3 79.3 64.7
Birthweight (g)
2500–2999 180 38.3 66.1 60.0
3000–3499 504 39.5 75.4 61.5
3500–3999 663 37.7 76.2 61.4
4000‡ 306 41.2 77.5 63.7
Apgar score (5 min)
7–9 59 35.6 64.4 57.6
10‡ 1583 39.0 75.6 61.7
Delivery
Vaginal 1558 39.7 75.0 61.3
Caesarean section 98 26.5 75.5 67.3
Siblings at home
0 791 40.1 77.1 64.3
1 563 36.8 70.9 57.5
2‡ 172 37.8 76.2 59.9
Day care (h/wk)
0 538 39.4 75.8 56.7
1–21 219 43.4 78.5 65.8
22–50 834 37.9 74.3 64.7
Periods of illness (n/8 mo)
0–1 862 38.6 75.8 62.4
2–3 571 39.1 74.3 62.0
4‡ 181 38.7 76.2 57.5
Parental experience with child rearing
Yes 969 37.8 74.1 59.4
No 457 39.2 74.8 63.9
Infants living with
Two parents 1484 38.8 74.4 62.0
One parent 126 38.9 80.2 57.1
Mother’s working status
Employed 1179 38.6 75.0 63.0
Unemployed 460 39.3 74.8 58.3
Father’s working status
Employed 1402 38.9 74.8 61.6
Unemployed 186 39.2 74.7 62.9
Corrected examination age (wk)
<33 145 19.3 52.4 45.5
33–34 456 29.8 63.4 50.4
35–36 503 36.4 76.5 62.0
37‡ 552 53.8 89.5 74.8

Due to missing values the numbers do not always add up to 1656.


ACTA PÆDIATR 88 (1999) Breastfeeding and developmental milestones 1331

Table 4. Developmental milestones according to duration of exclusive breastfeeding for 1348 infants with no missing information on the
confounding factors.

OR (95% CI)
Crawling Pincer grip Polysyllable babblers
Exclusive
a a
breastfeeding (mo) Unadjusted Adjusted Unadjusted Adjusted Unadjusted Adjusted a

0–1 Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference Reference


2–3 0.9 (0.6–1.3) 0.7 (0.5–1.1) 1.2 (0.8–1.8) 1.1 (0.7–1.8) 1.2 (0.8–1.7) 1.1 (0.8–1.7)
4–5 1.3 (1.0–1.8) 1.2 (0.8–1.7) 1.5 (1.1–2.1) 1.4 (1.0–2.1) 1.7 (1.3–2.4) 1.6 (1.1–2.3)
6‡ 1.5 (1.0–2.3) 1.4 (0.9–2.1) 2.3 (1.5–3.7) 2.2 (1.3–3.7) 2.8 (1.8–4.2) 2.5 (1.6–3.9)
a
Adjusted for family’s social group, mother’s education and age, smoking during pregnancy, birthweight and gestational age at delivery, corrected
examination age, number of periods with illness and whether the child was living with one or two parents. Variables categorized as in Table 4 and
entered into the logistic regression model as dummy variables equal to the number of categories ÿ1.

regression model, but the association remained un- Medical Research Council (j.nr. 12–1663), the Health Fund, Aarhus
changed. University Research Fund, the Egmont Fund and the Municipality of
Aarhus.
Developmental milestones were used as outcome
measures to simplify evaluation and interpretation of
the results. Although the definitions were simple, and
the health visitors were trained to evaluate develop-
mental milestones, some variation in the judgements References
must be expected because many health visitors partici-
pated in the study. The health visitors were not blinded 1. Morgane PJ, Austin LaFrance R, Bronzino J, et al. Prenatal
malnutrition and development of the brain. Neurosci Biobehav
concerning breastfeeding, but they were unaware of the Rev 1993; 17: 91–128
specific aim of the study; thus, any misclassification is 2. Smart JL. Vulnerability of developing brain to undernutrition.
probably non-differential, therefore causing bias to- Ups J Med Sci 1990; 48 Suppl: 21–41
wards the null (30). 3. Klein PS, Forbes GB, Nader PR. Effects of starvation in infancy
(pyloric stenosis) on subsequent learning abilities. J Pediatr
Although we adjusted for a variety of potential 1975; 87: 8–15
confounding factors, residual confounding may still be 4. Dobbing J, Sands J. Timing of neuroblast multiplication in
present. Some factors are very difficult to control for, developing human brain. Nature 1970; 226: 639–40
e.g. children’s genetic potentiality and environmental 5. Dobbing J. Undernutrition and the developing brain. The
stimulation. Three studies have examined the associa- relevance of animal models to the human problem. Bibl Nutr
Dieta 1972; 17: 35–46
tion between breastfeeding and infant development 6. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, Lister G, Leeson Payne C. Breast
after adjustment for maternal intelligence. The associa- milk and subsequent intelligence quotient in children born
tion remained significant in two (10, 14), but was preterm. Lancet 1992; 339: 261–4
insignificant in the third (17). Information about 7. Lanting CI, Fidler V, Huisman M, Touwen BC, Boersma ER.
Neurological differences between 9-year-old children fed breast
parental intelligence was not available in the present milk or formula milk as babies. Lancet 1994; 344: 1319–22
study. However, the results remained unchanged after 8. Florey CDuV, Leech AM, Blackhall A. Infant feeding and
adjustment for the main proxy variables, maternal mental and motor development at 18 months of age in first born
education and social class. Furthermore, adjustment singletons. Int J Epidemiol 1995; 24: S21–6
for parental intelligence might be less important when 9. Rogan WJ, Gladen BC. Breast-feeding and cognitive develop-
ment. Early Hum Dev 1993; 31: 181–93
studying the development of milestones than the 10. Morrow Tlucak M, Haude RH, Ernhart CB. Breastfeeding and
performance in mental tests. Breastfeeding might cognitive development in the first 2 years of life. Soc Sci Med
intensify the early mother–infant relationship, which 1988; 26: 635–9
again could influence the degree of stimulation offered 11. Pollock JI. Long-term associations with infant feeding in a
clinically advantaged population of babies. Dev Med Child
the infant. We acknowledge that our measures of Neurol 1994; 36: 429–40
environmental stimulation might be insufficient. 12. Niemelä A, Järvenpää AL. Is breastfeeding beneficial and
A causal relationship between breastfeeding and maternal smoking harmful to the cognitive development of
neurodevelopment has major public health implica- children? Acta Paediatr 1996; 85: 1202–6
tions. Future research should therefore explore causality 13. Temboury MC, Otero A, Polanco I, Arribas E. Influence of
breast-feeding on the infant’s intellectual development. J Pediatr
and try to reduce the role of genetic and environmental Gastroenterol Nutr 1994; 18: 32–6
confounding even further. 14. Fergusson DM, Beautrais AL, Silva PA. Breast-feeding and
cognitive development in the first seven years of life. Soc Sci
Med 1982; 16: 1705–8
Acknowledgements.—We are indebted to Professor Niels Jørgen Secher 15. Silva PA, Buckfield P, Spears GF. Some maternal and child
and Morten Hedegaard for their key roles in the initiation of the cohort, developmental characteristics associated with breast feeding: a
and to the health visitors of the municipality of Aarhus for meticulous report from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Child Development
data collection. The study was supported by grants from the Danish Study. Aust Paediatr J 1978; 14: 265–8
1332 A Vestergaard et al. ACTA PÆDIATR 88 (1999)

16. Gale CR, Martyn CN. Breastfeeding, dummy use, and adult 23. Willatts P, Forsyth JS, DiModugno MK, Varma S, Colvin M.
intelligence. Lancet 1996; 347: 1072–5 Effect of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in infant formula
17. Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. Breastfeeding and intelligence on problem solving at 10 months of age. Lancet 1998; 352: 688–
[letter]. Lancet 1992; 339: 926 91
18. Jorgensen MH, Hernell O, Lund P, Holmer G, Michaelsen KF. 24. Donovan SM, Odle J. Growth factors in milk as mediators of
Visual acuity and erythrocyte docosahexaenoic acid status in infant development. Ann Rev Nutr 1994; 14: 147–67
breast-fed and formula-fed term infants during the first four 25. Uauy R, De Andraca I. Human milk and breast feeding for
months of life. Lipids 1996; 31: 99–105 optimal mental development. J Nutr 1995; 125: S2278–80
19. Agostoni C, Trojan S, Bellù R, Riva E, Giovannini M. 26. Hedegaard M, Henriksen TB, Sabroe S, Secher NJ. Psychologi-
Neurodevelopmental quotient of healthy term infants at 4 months cal distress in pregnancy and preterm delivery. Br Med J 1993;
and feeding practice: the role of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty 307: 234–9
acids. Pediatr Res 1995; 38: 262–6 27. Illingworth RS. The development of the infant and young child.
20. Makrides M, Neumann M, Simmer K, Pater J, Gibson R. Are Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone, 1987
long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids essential nutrients in 28. Holle B. Motorisk—perceptuel udvikling. Copenhagen: Munks-
infancy? Lancet 1995; 345: 1463–8 gaard, 1977
21. Neuringer M, Anderson GJ, Connor WE. The essentiality of n-3 29. Greenland S. Modeling and variable selection in epidemiologic
fatty acids for the development and function of the retina and analysis. Am J Public Health 1989; 79: 340–9
brain. Ann Rev Nutr 1988; 8: 517–41 30. Rothman KJ. Modern epidemiology. Boston: Little, Brown, 1986
22. Farquharson J, Cockburn F, Patrick WA, Jamieson EC, Logan
RW. Infant cerebral cortex phospholipid fatty-acid composition Received Aug. 31, 1998; revision received Nov. 27, 1998 and June
and diet. Lancet 1992; 340: 810–3 28, 1999; accepted July 21, 1999

You might also like