You are on page 1of 8

ARTICLE IN PRESS

THE JOURNAL OF PEDIATRICS • www.jpeds.com ORIGINAL


ARTICLES
Preschool Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Children with
Congenital Heart Disease
Cheryl L. Brosig, PhD1,2, Laurel Bear, MD1,2, Sydney Allen, MPH1, Raymond G. Hoffmann, PhD1, Amy Pan, PhD1,
Michele Frommelt, MD1,2, and Kathleen A. Mussatto, PhD, RN1,2

Objective To describe preschool neurodevelopmental outcomes of children with complex congenital heart disease
(CHD), who were evaluated as part of a longitudinal cardiac neurodevelopmental follow-up program, as recom-
mended by the American Heart Association and the American Academy of Pediatrics, and identify predictors of
neurodevelopmental outcomes in these children.
Study design Children with CHD meeting the American Heart Association/American Academy of Pediatrics high-
risk criteria for neurodevelopmental delay were evaluated at 4-5 years of age. Testing included standardized neu-
ropsychological measures. Parents completed measures of child functioning. Scores were compared by group (single
ventricle [1V]; 2 ventricles [2V]; CHD plus known genetic condition) to test norms and classified as: normal (within
1 SD of mean); at risk (1-2 SD from mean); and impaired (>2 SD from mean).
Results Data on 102 patients were analyzed. Neurodevelopmental scores did not differ based on cardiac anatomy (1V
vs 2V); both groups scored lower than norms on fine motor and adaptive behavior skills, but were within 1 SD of norms.
Patients with genetic conditions scored significantly worse than 1V and 2V groups and test norms on most measures.
Conclusions Children with CHD and genetic conditions are at greatest neurodevelopmental risk. Deficits in chil-
dren with CHD without genetic conditions were mild and may not be detected without formal longitudinal testing.
Parents and providers need additional education regarding the importance of developmental follow-up for children
with CHD. (J Pediatr 2017;■■:■■-■■).

C
hildren with congenital heart disease (CHD) are at higher risk for neurodevelopmental problems than healthy chil-
dren, across all time points in development, from infancy through adolescence.1 Although IQ is often in the low average/
average range, a characteristic pattern of mild deficits in multiple other domains, including motor and visual spatial
skills, adaptive behavior, executive functioning, language, and social cognition, is common, and found in children with a wide
range of CHD diagnoses.2-7 Deficits are thought to be related to a number of factors including altered prenatal brain maturation,8
comorbid genetic conditions,9,10 perioperative and postoperative events,11 socioeconomic status,12 and parenting style.13 As a
result of these deficits, children with CHD are more likely than healthy children to require special education services,14 result-
ing in a significant impact on them, their families and society.15
To promote early detection of delays and optimize outcomes, the American Heart Association (AHA) and the American Academy
of Pediatrics (AAP) now recommend systematic evaluation of development in children with CHD throughout childhood.1 Cardiac
centers have begun to incorporate developmental follow-up programs as part of routine cardiac care.16,17 We have previously
reported developmental outcomes of children who were evaluated in our longitudinal developmental follow-up program over
the first 3 years of life and found that 46% of patients were delayed in at least 1 domain (cognitive, language, or motor skills);
feeding difficulty and medical and genetic comorbidities increased risk for delays.10,18 The aim of this study was to summarize
and identify predictors of neurodevelopmental outcomes for preschoolers who were seen as part of a longitudinal develop-
mental evaluation program for children with CHD.

Methods
Children with CHD believed to be at high risk for developmental delay as defined
by the AHA/AAP guidelines,1 were recruited from the Herma Heart Center
From the 1Department of Pediatrics, Medical College of
Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI; and 2Herma Heart Center,
Children’s Hospital of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI
1V Single ventricle Supported in part by the National Center for Research
2V Two ventricles Resources and the National Center for Advancing
Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health (NIH;
AAP American Academy of Pediatrics 8UL1TR000055). Its contents are solely the responsibility
AHA American Heart Association of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official
CPB Cardiopulmonary bypass views of the NIH. The authors declare no conflicts of
interest.
DHCA Deep hypothermic circulatory arrest
LOS Length of stay 0022-3476/$ - see front matter. © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights
WPPSI Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence reserved.
http://dx.doi.org10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.12.044

FLA 5.4.0 DTD ■ YMPD8902_proof ■ January 9, 2017


THE JOURNAL OF PEDIATRICS • www.jpeds.com Volume ■■

Developmental Follow-up Clinic at Children’s Hospital of Wis- attention problems (mean T score: 50 ± 10; higher scores in-
consin. Eligibility criteria and operation of the Herma Heart dicate more problems). The Total Problems score of the Child
Center Developmental Follow-Up Clinic have been previ- Behavior Checklist (completed by parent)28 was used as a
ously described.10,16,18 Children were deemed to be at high risk measure of child behavior problems (mean T scores: 50 ± 10;
for developmental delay and eligible for the clinic if they higher scores indicate more problems). The Total score of the
had any cardiac surgery as a neonate, surgery using cardio- Social Responsiveness Scale, First or Second Edition (com-
pulmonary bypass (CPB) in the first year of life, a cardiac defect pleted by parent)29,30 was used as a measure of child social prob-
resulting in cyanosis, or other comorbid conditions or com- lems (mean T scores: 50 ± 10; higher scores indicate more
plications such as prematurity, genetic syndromes, seizures, or problems). The Total score is comparable across versions of
cardiac arrest that placed them at higher risk for delay. Genetic this measure, as items are exactly the same. The second edition
testing was used to confirm a diagnosis when a genetic syn- of this measure allows for administration across a wider age
drome was suspected, but all patients did not routinely undergo range, and 1 subscale name was changed.
genetic testing. All families whose children met the AHA/ For children who were too developmentally impaired to com-
AAP high risk for delay criteria were contacted by letter and plete a task (n = 10), the lowest possible score for that test was
subsequently called to schedule a preschool evaluation. Chil- assigned. Children who did not complete a task for other
dren were seen for neurodevelopmental testing within the car- reasons (separation anxiety, n = 1; distractibility, n = 1; lan-
diology clinic; appointments lasted approximately 2-3 hours. guage delay, n = 1; fatigue, n = 4; oppositional behavior, n = 7)
Parents provided informed consent to have their child’s data were excluded from analysis for tasks they did not complete.
included in a databank approved by the Institutional Review
Board at Children’s Hospital of Wisconsin. No subjects were Statistical Analyses
excluded based on race or other coexisting medical or genetic Sample characteristics and clinical variables are presented as
condition. Only children who spoke English were included, as medians with IQR (25th percentile-75th percentile) for con-
tests were administered in English. tinuous data and frequencies (%) for categorical data.
Children completed a variety of neurodevelopmental mea- Neurodevelopmental test scores were converted to standard z
sures that were selected based on developmental challenges that scores based on test norm means/SDs and compared with the
are commonly seen in children with CHD. In addition, parents population mean using a Wilcoxon signed rank test. Convert-
completed several measures of child functioning and behav- ing neurodevelopmental test scores to standard z scores, a
ior. All measures are validated and have normative values based common metric, allowed for comparison of scores across mea-
on a healthy population. New editions of some measures were sures, as not all neurodevelopmental tests have the same scales.
published during the 4 years in which evaluations were com- To adjust for multiple comparisons, a step-down Bonferroni
pleted; the testing protocol was updated to include the most procedure was used because it is less conservative than the
current version of all measures at the time of assessment. Bonferroni in controlling for the family of hypotheses error
The full scale IQ score from the Wechsler Preschool and rate.31 To perform this adjustment, raw P value needs to be a
Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI), Third or Fourth number. Therefore, a P value of <.0001 was treated as .0001.
Edition19,20 (mean: 100 ± 15) was used as a measure of cogni- Scores were classified as: normal (within 1 SD of test mean);
tive functioning. The WPPSI-Fourth Edition full scale IQ score at risk (1-2 SDs from test mean); or impaired (>2 SDs from
correlates .86 with the WPPSI-Third Edition full scale IQ score. test mean). A 1-sample proportion test was used to examine
The Letter-Word Identification, Applied Problems, and Spell- whether the observed percentages were different from the ex-
ing subtests of the Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Achievement21 pected percentages for impaired (2.5%) and at risk (13.5%)
were used to assess prereading, premath, and prespelling skills, categories. A Kruskal-Wallis test or Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon
respectively (mean: 100 ± 15). The Developmental Test of Visual test was used to compare test scores by group (single ven-
Motor Integration, Sixth Edition22 was used to assess visual tricle [1V] without genetic condition; 2 ventricles [2V] without
motor integration ability (mean: 100 ± 15). The Pegboard genetic condition; CHD with genetic condition). A Cochran-
subtest of the Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities23 Armitage trend exact test was used to examine the trend in pro-
was used to assess fine motor skills (mean: 100 ± 15). The portions of the number of domains that fell in the normal,
General Communication Composite score of the Children’s at risk, or impaired range for the genetic vs 1V and 2V
Communication Checklist-2 (completed by parent)24 was used nongenetic groups. Univariable and multivariable logistic re-
as a measure of language skills (mean: 100 ± 15). The General gression analyses were used to assess the impact of patient and
Adaptive Composite score of the Adaptive Behavior Assess- clinical factors on binary neurodevelopmental test scores (at
ment System-Second Edition (completed by parent)25 was used risk/impaired vs normal). Predictors with P value of < .1 from
as a measure of adaptive behavior (mean: 100 ± 15). The Global the univariate analyses were included in the multivariable lo-
Executive Composite score of the Behavior Rating Inventory gistic regression models (1 model for each neurodevelopmental
of Executive Function-Preschool Version (completed by test) using a forward selection method. The following vari-
parent)26 was used as a measure of executive functioning (mean ables were included as predictors based on our previous
T score: 50 ± 10; higher scores indicate more problems). The findings and a review of the literature: sex, race (White, non-
Total score of the Conners’ Parent Rating Scale-Revised- Hispanic, vs others), maternal education (completed beyond
Short Form (completed by parent)27 was used as a measure of high school vs completed high school or less), prenatal
2 Brosig et al

FLA 5.4.0 DTD ■ YMPD8902_proof ■ January 9, 2017


2017 ORIGINAL ARTICLES

diagnosis, cardiac anatomy (1V vs 2V), comorbidities (none


Table I. Sample demographics (n = 102)
vs other medical vs genetic), age at first cardiac surgery, cu-
mulative hospital length of stay (LOS), cumulative CPB time, Demographics n (%)
cumulative deep hypothermic circulatory arrest (DHCA) time, Sex: male 64 (63)
history of early intervention (age 0-3 years). Statistical sig- Race/ethnicity
White, non-Hispanic 72 (71)
nificance in the final model was defined as P value of < .05. Hispanic 13 (13)
All statistical analyses were performed using SAS v 9.4 (SAS Black 8 (8)
Institute, Cary, North Carolina) software. Other 9 (9)
Maternal education
Post-high school 80 (78)
Results High school or less 16 (16)
Missing 6 (6)
Family constellation
From March 2011 through April 2015, 108 subjects with CHD Married 73 (72)
completed preschool neurodevelopmental testing as part of the Single 17 (17)
cardiac neurodevelopmental follow-up clinic. This repre- Other 11 (11)
Missing 1 (<1)
sented 28% of the potentially eligible population. Of those who Prenatal diagnosis: yes 54 (53)
did not attend the clinic, 212 (80%) did not respond to letters Premature (GA <37 wk) 18 (18)
or phone calls, so the reason they did not participate is Anatomy
2V 68 (67)
unknown. Participants were compared with nonparticipants 1V 34 (33)
on demographic and treatment characteristics. There were no Comorbidities
significant differences between groups on sex, presence of None 59 (58)
Other medical 26 (25)
known genetic or noncardiac morbidity, insurance (public or Genetic 17 (17)
private) at time of surgery, history of CPB, gestational age, age
GA, gestational age.
at first cardiac operation, or total surgical LOS. When com-
pared with nonparticipants, participants were more likely to
have 1V anatomy (35% vs 24%, P = .02); were more likely to
have been seen in the 0- to 3-year-old clinic (74% vs 58%, Of these, 16 of 17 children had 2V anatomy. One subject with
P = .002); had more cardiac operations (3.8 vs 2.9, P = .01); double inlet left ventricle had Pierre Robin syndrome.
had more minutes of CPB time (286 vs 235, P = .013); and had The majority of subjects, 92 of 102 (90%) had undergone
higher maximum Society of Thoracic Surgeons-European As- at least 1 heart surgery requiring CPB; median cumulative CPB
sociation for Cardio-Thoracic Surgery Congenital Heart Surgery time (to the time of preschool evaluation) was 221 minutes
Mortality Categories (3.8 vs 3.4, P = .002). (141-362 minutes). Of the 37 subjects who had DHCA, median
Ninety-eight percent of the subjects consented to partici- cumulative DHCA time (to the time of preschool evalua-
pation in our research databank. Four patients were ex- tion) was 12 minutes (7-28 minutes). Median cumulative hos-
cluded from this analysis because their primary diagnosis was pital LOS was 46 days (24-82 days). A majority of subjects, 85
acquired cardiomyopathy, resulting in a final sample of 102 of 102 (83%), had received early intervention services (eg, physi-
subjects with structural CHD. Of these, 75 of 102 (74%) had cal, occupational or speech therapy) previously; 42 of 102 (41%)
been previously evaluated in our developmental follow-up were currently enrolled in early intervention services at the time
program before 3 years of age. All patients who had cardiac of their preschool evaluation.
surgery at less than 1 year of age were previously referred for The percentages of the subjects for the entire cohort that
early intervention services. Characteristics of the sample are fell within the normal (within 1SD of test norm), at risk (1-2
presented in Table I. Median gestational age at birth was 39 SD from test norm) and impaired (>2 SD from test norm)
weeks (37-40 weeks). Median age at assessment was 4.5 years ranges are illustrated in Figure 1 (available at www.jpeds.com).
(4.3-4.7 years). Anatomy was classified according to the child’s In a normal distribution, 84% of a given sample should fall
diagnosis at birth. Thirty-three percent of the subjects had within the normal or above normal range, 13.5% within the
anatomy that required surgical palliation resulting in a func- at risk range, and approximately 2.5% within the impaired
tional single ventricle (list of primary cardiac diagnoses in range. Assuming a normal distribution, this cohort of chil-
Table II; available at www.jpeds.com). Twenty-five percent dren with CHD had more scores in the impaired range for all
(n = 26) of the subjects had a known medical comorbidity, in domains except parent-reported behavior problems and more
addition to their CHD, involving the following systems: airway scores in the at risk range for fine motor skills than would be
(n = 10), gastrointestinal/genitourinary (n = 5), hearing (n = 3), expected (all adjusted P < .05).
neurologic (n = 3), chronic lung disease (n = 2), multisystem Child neurodevelopmental scores and parent rating scale
(n = 2), and orthopedic (n = 1). Seventeen percent (n = 17) of results are presented in Table III. To address concerns that scores
the subjects had a diagnosed genetic condition (confirmed by for the patients with known genetic conditions would skew the
genetic testing): trisomy 21 (n = 7); 22q11 deletion (n = 3); chro- results for the entire cohort, scores were compared with test
mosomal deletion (n = 3); Turner syndrome (n = 2); VACTERL norms based on patient diagnostic group (1V without genetic
syndrome (vertebral, anal, cardiac, tracheo-esophageal, renal, condition, 2V without genetic condition, or CHD with genetic
and limb defects) (n = 1); and Pierre Robin syndrome (n = 1). condition). Scores for each diagnostic group were also
Preschool Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Children with Congenital Heart Disease 3

FLA 5.4.0 DTD ■ YMPD8902_proof ■ January 9, 2017


THE JOURNAL OF PEDIATRICS • www.jpeds.com Volume ■■

Table III. Child completed neurodevelopmental measures and parent rating scale results
Standardized median z Raw P (z score Step-down
Median (IQR: 25th-75th (IQR: 25th-75th percentile) compared Bonferroni
Tests Domains percentile) for cases for cases with 0) adjusted P
WPPSI-III/IV IQ* Cognitive 1V 97.0 (80.0-105.0) −0.2 (−1.3 to 0.3) .066 .59
2V 102.0 (87.5-110.5) 0.1 (−0.8 to 0.7) .90 >0.99
Genetic 68.00 (40.0-79.0) −2.1 (−4.0 to −1.4) .001 .01
WJTA-III Letter-Word Pre-reading 1V 104.0 (90.0-112.0) 0.3 (−0.7 to 0.8) .56 >.99
Identification* 2V 104.0 (91.0-114.0) 0.3 (-0.6 to 0.9) .13 .78
Genetic 79.0 (40.0-106.0) −1.4 (−4.0 to 0.4) .024 .10
WJTA-III Applied Pre-math 1V 106.0 (94.0-118.0) 0.4 (−0.4 to 1.2) .34 >.99
Problems* 2V 109.0 (98.0-117.0) 0.6 (−0.1 to 1.1) .006 .06
Genetic 81.0 (40.0-94.0) −1.3 (−4.0 to −0.4) .007 .04
WJTA-III Spelling* Pre-spelling 1V 98.0 (86.0-110.0) −0.1 (−0.9 to 0.7) .53 >.99
2V 102.0 (87.0-111.0) 0.1 (−0.9 to 0.7) .88 >.99
Genetic 61.0 (40.0-92.0) −2.6 (-4.0 to −0.5) .003 .02
VMI* Visual motor 1V 95.0 (90.0-101.0) −0.3 (-0.7 to 0.1) .023 .23
2V 98.5 (93.0-106.5) −0.1 (−0.5 to 0.4) .31 >.99
Genetic 87.0 (66.0-92.0) −0.9 (−2.3 to −0.5) .001 .01
WRAVMA* Fine motor 1V 88.0 (80.0-100.0) −0.8 (−1.3 to 0.0) .0006 .007
2V 93.0 (83.0-101.5) −0.5 (−1.1 to 0.1) .0001 .001
Genetic 63.0 (45.0-76.0) −2.5 (−3.7 to −1.6) <.0001 .001
ABAS-II* Adaptive behavior 1V 88.0 (79.0-104.0) −0.8 (−1.4 to 0.3) .003 .033
2V 94.0 (77.0-101.0) −0.4 (−1.5 to 0.1) .0006 .007
Genetic 66.0 (60.0-90.0) −2.3 (−2.7 to −0.7) .0002 .002
BRIEF-P† Executive functioning 1V 50.5 (39.0-65.0) 0.1 (−1.1 to 1.5) .50 >.99
2V 48.0 (39.0-57.0) −0.2 (−1.1 to 0.7) .66 >.99
Genetic 56.5 (46.0-69.5) 0.7 (−0.4 to 2.0) .81 >.99
CPRS† Attention problems 1V 50.0 (43.0-64.0) 0.0 (−0.7 to 1.4) .28 >.99
2V 54.0 (44.0-64.0) 0.4 (−0.6 to 1.4) .06 .48
Genetic 53.0 (47.0-79.0) 0.3 (−0.3 to 2.9) .16 .48
CBCL† Behavior problems 1V 49.0 (38.5-58.5) −0.1 (−1.2 to 0.9) .54 >.99
2V 46.0 (39.5-56.0) −0.4 (−1.1 to 0.6) .10 .70
Genetic 50.0 (45.0-56.0) 0.0 (−0.5 to 0.6) >.999 >.99
SRS/SRS-2† Social problems 1V 51.0 (43.0-61.0) 0.1 (−0.7 to 1.1) .21 >.99
2V 50.5 (45.0-58.0) 0.1 (−0.5 to 0.8) .40 >.99
Genetic 59.5 (54.0-66.0) 1.0 (0.4 to 1.6) .0039 .03
CCC-2* Language 1V 102.0 (86.0-115.0) 0.1 (−0.9 to 1.0) .93 >.99
2V 109.0 (96.5-116.0) 0.6 (−0.2 to 1.1) .018 .16
Genetic 72.0 (40.0-89.0) −1.9 (-4.0 to −0.7) .0037 .03

ABAS-II, Adaptive Behavior Assessment System, Second Edition; BRIEF-P, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; CCC-2, Children's Commu-
nication Checklist, Second Edition; CPRS, Conners' Parent Rating Scales; SRS/SRS-2, Social Responsiveness Scale, First or Second Edition; VMI, Visual Motor Integration; WJTA-III, Woodcock Johnson
Tests of Achievement-Third Edition; WPPSI-III/IV, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence, Third or Fourth Edition; WRAVMA, Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities.
Bold P values indicate statistically significant.
*Test mean/SD 100 ± 15; higher scores indicate better functioning.
†Test mean/SD 50 ± 10; higher scores indicate worse functioning.

compared with each other. Children with 1V or 2V anatomy domain scores in the at risk/impaired range (ie, all scores within
without genetic conditions scored lower than test norms on the normal range); (2) 1 domain score in the at risk range;
fine motor and adaptive behavior skills (adjusted P < .05). Chil- (3) ≥ 2 domain scores in the at risk range; or (4) ≥ 1 domain
dren with CHD and genetic conditions scored lower than scores in the impaired range (Figure 2). Children with CHD
test norms and lower than children with 1V anatomy without genetic conditions were less likely than children with
without genetic conditions on most child completed CHD and genetic conditions to have ≥1 domain score in the
neurodevelopmental measures; they also scored lower on adap- impaired range (P = .011).
tive behavior skills and had higher rates of parent-reported Logistic regression was performed on the dichotomized
social problems (adjusted P < .05). Children with CHD and neurodevelopmental test scores (at risk/impaired vs normal)
genetic conditions scored lower than children with 2V anatomy for each neurodevelopmental test. The significant predictors
without genetic conditions on IQ, premath, and fine motor of child neurodevelopmental outcomes in the multivariable
skills (adjusted P < .05). No significant differences in models are presented in Table IV. An OR >1 represented an
neurodevelopmental scores were found between children with increased likelihood of having an “at risk/impaired” score. Pres-
1V vs 2V anatomy without genetic conditions. ence of a genetic or other medical condition was associated
Children with 1V anatomy and 2V anatomy without genetic with worse neurodevelopmental outcomes in most domains.
conditions were combined into 1 group and compared with Non-white race or Hispanic ethnicity was associated with worse
children with CHD and genetic conditions on the percent- outcomes in prereading, premath, visual motor, adaptive be-
age of children who fell into the following categories: (1) no havior, and language skills. Male sex was associated with worse
4 Brosig et al

FLA 5.4.0 DTD ■ YMPD8902_proof ■ January 9, 2017


2017 ORIGINAL ARTICLES

represent at least 20% of the CHD population, and this number


is likely an underestimate, as not all children with CHD undergo
formal genetic testing.10 Children with CHD and comorbid
genetic conditions meet the AHA/AAP1 criteria regarding which
children should be referred for serial neurodevelopmental as-
sessment. Therefore, we believed that it was important to
include these children in our results.10,16
Of note, neurodevelopmental outcomes did not differ based
on cardiac anatomy (1V without genetic condition vs 2V
without genetic condition), which is consistent with previ-
ous research on preschool neurodevelopmental outcomes in
CHD.34 In contrast to the current preschool findings, re-
search conducted with adolescents with CHD found worse ex-
Figure 2. Number of domains at risk or impaired by group (1V/ ecutive functioning in subjects with single-ventricle anatomy
2V without genetic condition vs CHD with genetic condition). compared with biventricular groups. 2 It is possible that
neurodevelopmental risks between 1V and 2V groups widen
over the course of development, as societal demands for in-
outcomes in prespelling and fine motor skills. A longer cu- dependent problem solving and executive functioning skills
mulative hospital LOS was associated with worse outcomes in increase.
fine motor skills. Of note, maternal education, prenatal diag- Presence of a comorbid medical or genetic condition was
nosis, cardiac anatomy, age at first cardiac surgery, or minutes the strongest predictor of at risk/impaired neurodevelopmental
of CPB/DHCA were not significantly associated with any outcomes across domains. In addition, minority race, male
neurodevelopmental domains. None of the predictors in the sex, and cumulative hospital LOS predicted adverse
regression model were associated with executive function- neurodevelopmental outcomes in some domains. Early patient
ing, attention, behavior, or social skills. or treatment characteristics, such as prenatal diagnosis, age
at first cardiac surgery, or CPB/DHCA time, did not predict
Discussion preschool neurodevelopmental outcomes. It appears that early
treatment factors have less impact on development the farther
In this cohort of preschool-age patients who underwent the child is out from surgery; whereas intrinsic factors such
neurodevelopmental evaluation as part of a clinical follow- as ongoing medical/genetic problems (which may contribute
up program, findings indicate that most children with CHD to longer hospital LOS) and minority race (which may be a
without comorbid genetic conditions do not have severe delays. proxy for socioeconomic disadvantage), may have more impact
However, 50% of the children with CHD without known on development over time. It was surprising that males did
genetic conditions had multiple scores in the at risk range or worse on fine motor and prespelling skills, as sex was not
at least 1 score in the impaired range, with adaptive behavior found to be a risk factor in our previous studies.10,16,18 However,
and fine motor skills being particular areas of concern. findings of the current study are consistent with recent reports
Not surprisingly, children with a comorbid genetic condi- that have shown similar differences by sex in motor and
tion had worse neurodevelopmental outcomes, which is writing skills in healthy children.35,36 It is interesting to note
consistent with previous findings.1,10,32 Although many studies that no factors examined in the present study predicted prob-
have excluded children with CHD and comorbid genetic lems with executive functioning, attention, or behavior, deficits
conditions,2,3,33 children with CHD and genetic conditions that are frequently seen in older children with CHD.2 In the

Table IV. Significant predictors (P < .05) of at risk/impaired neurodevelopmental outcomes in multivariable regres-
sion models
Cognitive Prereading Premath Prespelling Visual motor Fine motor Adaptive behavior Language
OR (95% CI)
Male vs female 3.7 (1.04, 13.16) 6.7 (1.69, 26.15)
White, 0.3 (.08, 0.92) 0.2 (.06, 0.64) 0.2 (.07, 0.69) 0.3 (0.10, 0.87) 0.3 (0.10, 0.97)
non-Hispanic
vs others
Genetic condition 15.6 (3.55, 68.16) 12.2 (2.85, 52.31) 9.4 (2.14, 40.98) 11.3 (2.63, 48.50) 6.9 (1.75, 26.88) 33.2 (5.46, 201.96) 6.3 (1.61, 25.05) 7.8 (2.10, 29.04)
vs none
Other medical 4.7 (1.59, 13.69) 1.3 (0.27, 5.75) 2.2 (0.69, 7.13) 2.5 (0.66, 9.91) 1.3 (0.42, 3.97) 1.9 (0.66, 5.79) 1.1 (0.28, 4.10)
condition
vs none
Hospital LOS* 1.01 (1.00, 1.02)

*Cumulative to time of preschool assessment.

Preschool Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Children with Congenital Heart Disease 5

FLA 5.4.0 DTD ■ YMPD8902_proof ■ January 9, 2017


THE JOURNAL OF PEDIATRICS • www.jpeds.com Volume ■■

present study, executive functioning, attention, and behavior 2. Cassidy AR, White MT, DeMaso DR, Newburger JW, Bellinger DC.
were assessed exclusively via parent questionnaires, which Executive function in children and adolescents with critical cyanotic
congenital heart disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2015;21:34-49.
may have influenced the results. Nonetheless, findings suggest 3. Bellinger DC, Wypij D, Rivkin MJ, DeMaso DR, Robertson RL Jr, Dunbar-
that there is no clear profile of factors that consistently pre- Masterson C, et al. Adolescents with d-transposition of the great
dicts all areas of development in patients with CHD over arteries corrected with the arterial switch procedure: neuropsychologi-
time. cal assessment and structural brain imaging. Circulation 2011;124:1361-
There are some important limitations to the current study. 9.
4. Sananes R, Manlhiot C, Kelly E, Hornberger LK, Williams WG, MacGregor
Results are based on a single center experience, and not all pa- D, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes after open heart operations before
tients who were eligible for the clinic participated; thus, find- 3 months of age. Ann Thorac Surg 2012;93:1577-83.
ings may not be generalizable to the CHD population as a 5. Alton G, Taghados S, Joffe A, Robertson C, Dinu I, The Western Cana-
whole. It is possible that results represent a “best-case sce- dian Pediatric Therapies Follow-Up Group. Prediction of preschool func-
nario,” as the majority of the sample (74%) had been previ- tional abilities after early complex cardiac surgery. Cardiol Young
2015;25:655-62.
ously evaluated in our neurodevelopmental follow-up program, 6. Calderon J, Angeard N, Pinabiaux C, Bonnet D, Jambaqué I. Facial ex-
and the majority (83%) had received early intervention pression recognition and emotion understanding in children after neo-
services. Thus, results may underestimate the level of natal open-heart surgery for transposition of the great arteries. Dev Med
neurodevelopmental deficits in this age group, particularly Child Neurol 2014;56:564-71.
for patients with CHD who are not receiving systematic 7. Gerstle M, Beebe D, Drotar D, Cassedy A, Marino B. Executive function-
ing and school performance among pediatric survivors of complex
neurodevelopmental evaluation or early intervention ser- congenital heart disease. J Pediatr 2016;173:154-9.
vices. As evaluations were conducted as part of a clinical 8. Licht DJ, Shera DM, Clancy RR, Wernovsky G, Montenegro LM, Nicolson
program, and not as part of a research study, it was not pos- SC, et al. Brain maturation is delayed in infants with complex congeni-
sible for us to recruit a healthy control sample, which is an ad- tal heart defects. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 2009;137:529-36, discussion
ditional limitation. As patients were evaluated in the clinic over 536-537.
9. Fahed AC, Gelb BD, Seidman JG, Seidman CE. Genetics of congenital heart
a period of several years, it is possible that outcomes could have disease: the glass half empty. Circ Res 2013;112:707-20.
been influenced by changes in surgical or clinical manage- 10. Mussatto KA, Hoffmann RG, Hoffman GM, Tweddell JS, Bear L, Cao Y,
ment; however, this is unlikely, as our comprehensive et al. Risk and prevalence of developmental delay in young children with
neuroprotective strategy remained stable throughout the course congenital heart disease. Pediatrics 2014;133:e570-7.
of this study. Finally, although multiple neurodevelopmental 11. Mahle WT, Clancy RR, McGaurn SP, Goin JE, Clark BJ. Impact of pre-
natal diagnosis on survival and early neurologic morbidity in neonates
domains were assessed as part of the preschool evaluation, it with the hypoplastic left heart syndrome. Pediatrics 2001;107:1277-82.
is not known whether preschool neurodevelopmental out- 12. Cassedy A, Drotar D, Ittenbach R, Hottinger S, Wray J, Wernovsky G, et al.
comes will predict neurodevelopmental outcomes at school- The impact of socio-economic status on health related quality of life for
age and beyond. children and adolescents with heart disease. Health Qual Life Outcomes
Cardiology providers, primary care providers/pediatricians, 2013;11:99.
13. Rempel GR, Harrison MJ. Safeguarding precarious survival: parenting
and other members of the child’s medical team should counsel children who have life-threatening heart disease. Qual Health Res
parents that formal developmental testing can identify the 2007;17:824-37.
child’s strengths and weaknesses, which often change as the 14. Riehle-Colarusso T, Autry A, Razzaghi H, Boyle CA, Mahle WT, Van
child gets older. The information obtained can also help parents Naarden Braun K, et al. Congenital heart defects and receipt of special
advocate for their child’s unique needs as they enter the school education services. Pediatrics 2015;136:496-504.
15. Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Reauthorization 2004. Pub L
system. As more cardiac centers incorporate developmental No. 108-446, 118 Stat 2647.
follow-up programs into their clinical care,17 quality improve- 16. Soto CB, Olude O, Hoffmann RG, Bear L, Chin A, Dasgupta M, et al.
ment initiatives designed to increase family participation will Implementation of a routine developmental follow-up program for chil-
be beneficial. ■ dren with congenital heart disease: early results. Congenit Heart Dis
2011;6:451-60.
17. Brosig CL, Butcher J, Butler S, Ilardi DL, Sananes R, Sanz JH, et al. Moni-
We acknowledge the support of Mara Koffarnus, Kristen Andersen, and toring developmental risk and promoting success for children with con-
Katie Wilde for patient scheduling and data management support. We genital heart disease: recommendations for cardiac neurodevelopmental
thank the children and parents who attended developmental follow- follow-up programs. Clin Pract Pediatr Psychol 2014;2:153-65.
up and were willing to participate in this research. 18. Mussatto KA, Hoffmann R, Hoffman G, Tweddell JS, Bear L, Cao Y, et al.
Risk factors for abnormal developmental trajectories in young children
Submitted for publication Sep 2, 2016; last revision received Nov 1, 2016; with congenital heart disease. Circulation 2015;132:755-61.
accepted Dec 14, 2016 19. Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Ad-
Reprint requests: Cheryl L. Brosig, PhD, Department of Pediatrics, Medical ministration and Scoring Manual. 3rd ed. San Antonio (TX): Psycho-
College of Wisconsin, 8701 Watertown Plank Rd, Milwaukee, WI 53226. logical Corporation; 2002.
E-mail: cbrosig@chw.org 20. Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence,
Adminstration and Scoring Manual. 4th ed. Bloomington, (MN): NCS
Pearson, Inc; 2012.
References 21. Woodcock R, McGrew K, Mather N. Woodcock Johnson III Tests of
1. Marino BS, Lipkin PH, Newburger JW, Peacock G, Gerdes M, Gaynor JW, Achievement. Itsaca (IL): Riverside Publishing; 2001.
et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes in children with congenital heart 22. Beery K, Beery N. Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-
disease: evaluation and management: a scientific statement from the Ameri- Motor Integration Administration, Scoring and Teaching Manual. 6th ed.
can Heart Association. Circulation 2012;126:1143-72. Bloomington (MN): NCS Pearson, Inc; 2010.

6 Brosig et al

FLA 5.4.0 DTD ■ YMPD8902_proof ■ January 9, 2017


2017 ORIGINAL ARTICLES

23. Adams W, Sheslow D. Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor 31. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J
Abilities Manual. Lutz (FL): Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; Stat 1979;6:65-70.
1995. 32. Atallah J, Joffe A, Robertson C, Leonard N, Blakely P, Nettel-Aguirre A,
24. Bishop D. Children’s Communication Checklist—2 Manual. San Antonio et al. Two-year general and neurodevelopmental outcome after neonatal
(TX): Pearson Clinical Assessment; 2003. complex cardiac surgery in patients with deletion 22q11.2: A compara-
25. Harrison P, Oakland T. Adaptive Behavior Assessment System-Manual. tive study. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 2007;134:772-9.
2nd ed. San Antonio (TX): Harcourt Assessment, Inc; 2003. 33. Mackie AS, Vatanpour S, Alton GY, Dinu IA, Ryerson L, Moddemann DM,
26. Gioia G, Epsy K, Isquith P. BRIEF-P Behavior Rating Inventory of et al. Clinical outcome score predicts adverse neurodevelopmental outcome
Executive Function-Preschool Version. Lutz (FL): Psychological Assess- after infant heart surgery. Ann Thorac Surg 2015;99:2124-32.
ment Resources, Inc; 2003. 34. Gaynor JW, Ittenbach RF, Gerdes M, Bernbaum J, Clancy RR, McDonald-
27. Conners C. Conners’ Rating Scales-Revised User’s Manual. North McGinn DM, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes in preschool
Tonawanda (NY): Multi-Health Systems Inc; 1997. survivors of the Fontan procedure. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg
28. Achenbach T, Rescorla L. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms & 2014;147:1276-82, discussion 1282-83, e5.
Profiles. Burlington (VT): University of Vermont, Research Center for Chil- 35. Reynolds MR, Scheiber C, Hajovsky DB, Schwartz B, Kaufman AS. Gender
dren, Youth & Families; 2000. differences in academic achievement: is writing an exception to the gender
29. Constantino J. Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) Manual. Los Angeles (CA): similarities hypothesis? J Genet Psychol 2015;176:211-34.
Western Psychological Services; 2005. 36. Morley D, Till K, Ogilvie P, Turner G. Influences of gender and socio-
30. Constantino J, Gruber C. Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2). 2nd ed. economic status on the motor proficiency of children in the UK. Hum
Torrance (CA): Western Psychological Services; 2012. Mov Sci 2015;44:150-6.

Preschool Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Children with Congenital Heart Disease 7

FLA 5.4.0 DTD ■ YMPD8902_proof ■ January 9, 2017


THE JOURNAL OF PEDIATRICS • www.jpeds.com Volume ■■

Figure 1. Percentage of patients in the normal, at risk, and impaired ranges on neurodevelopmental measures.

Table II. Primary cardiac diagnoses


2V congenital heart defects n = 68
Aortic arch hypoplasia or coarctation 12
Atrioventricular septal defect 8
Ventricular septal defect 8
Transposition of the great arteries with intact ventricular septum 6
Aortic valve stenosis 4
Pulmonary atresia + ventricular septal defect 4
Double outlet right ventricle 3
Interrupted aortic arch 3
Tetralogy of Fallot 3
Total anomalous pulmonary venous connection 3
Transposition of the great arteries with ventricular septal defect 3
Atrioventricular septal defect with tetralogy of Fallot 2
Pulmonary atresia with intact ventricular septal septum 2
Truncus arteriosus 2
Ebstein malformation of tricuspid valve 1
Double aortic arch 1
Mitral valve stenosis 1
Pulmonary valve stenosis 1
Congenitally corrected transposition of the great arteries with ventricular septal defect 1

1V congenital heart defects n = 34


Hypoplastic left heart syndrome 20
Double inlet left ventricle 6
Double outlet right ventricle 3
Pulmonary atresia + intact ventricular septum 3
Congenitally corrected transposition of the great arteries 1
Tricuspid atresia 1

7.e1 Brosig et al

FLA 5.4.0 DTD ■ YMPD8902_proof ■ January 9, 2017

You might also like