You are on page 1of 16

Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Molecular Aspects of Medicine


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / m a m

Review

Non-pharmacological interventions for adults with mild


cognitive impairment and early stage dementia:
An updated scoping review
Juleen Rodakowski a,b, Ester Saghafi c, Meryl A. Butters d,
Elizabeth R. Skidmore a,b,*
a
Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Health & Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Pittsburgh (Rodakowski, Skidmore),
Pittsburgh, PA, USA
b Clinical and Translational Science Institute, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
c Health Sciences Library System, University of Pittsburgh (Saghafi), Pittsburgh, PA, USA
d
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh (Butters), Pittsburgh, PA, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: The purpose of this scoping review was to examine the science related to non-
Received 2 March 2015 pharmacological interventions designed to slow decline for older adults with Mild Cognitive
Revised 6 June 2015 Impairment or early-stage dementia. We reviewed 32 unique randomized controlled trials
Accepted 6 June 2015
that employed cognitive training (remediation or compensation approaches), physical ex-
Available online 10 June 2015
ercise, or psychotherapeutic interventions that were published before November 2014.
Evidence suggests that cognitive training focused on remediation and physical exercise in-
Keywords:
terventions may promote small improvements in selected cognitive abilities. Cognitive
Mild cognitive impairment
Dementia training focused on compensation interventions and selected psychotherapeutic interven-
Alzheimer’s disease tions may influence how cognitive changes impact daily living. However, confidence in these
Cognitive rehabilitation findings is limited due to methodological limitations. To better assess the value of non-
Non-pharmacological interventions pharmacological interventions for this population, we recommend: (1) adoption of universal
Behavioral interventions criteria for “early stage cognitive decline” among studies, (2) adherence to guidelines for
the conceptualization, operationalization, and implementation of complex interventions,
(3) consistent characterization of the impact of interventions on daily life, and (4) long-
term follow-up of clinical outcomes to assess maintenance and meaningfulness of reported
effects over time.
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................ 39
2. Methods ................................................................................................................................................................................................................................... 40
3. Results ...................................................................................................................................................................................................................................... 40
3.1. Participant characteristics .................................................................................................................................................................................... 40
3.2. Intervention characteristics ................................................................................................................................................................................. 40
3.3. Primary and secondary outcomes ..................................................................................................................................................................... 48
3.4. Intervention effects ................................................................................................................................................................................................ 48

* Corresponding author. Department of Occupational, Therapy University of Pittsburgh, 5012 Forbes Tower, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA. Tel.: +1 412 383
6617; fax: +1 412 383 6617.
E-mail address: skidmore@pitt.edu (E.R. Skidmore).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mam.2015.06.003
0098-2997/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53 39

4. Discussion ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................... 50
Acknowledgements .............................................................................................................................................................................................................. 52
References ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................... 52

1. Introduction Non-pharmacological interventions that address cogni-


tive function and the impact of cognitive function on daily
An estimated 5.2 million older adults are suspected to living have been widely studied in a variety of clinical popu-
have dementia in the United States (Alzheimer’s Association, lations (e.g., learning disabilities, stroke, traumatic brain
2014). Due to aging of one of the largest birth cohorts in injury, dementia) (Chung et al., 2013; Seitz et al., 2012;
United States history, up to 16 million older adults are ex- Skidmore et al., 2014; Young and Amarasinghe, 2010). These
pected to have dementia by the year 2050 (Alzheimer’s non-pharmacological interventions tend to be complex,
Association, 2014). Costs associated with dementia are es- multimodal interventions, as defined by the Medical Re-
timated to exceed $1 trillion USD (in current market value) search Council (Craig et al., 2013). Chief among these
by 2050 (Alzheimer’s Association, 2014). Mild cognitive im- interventions are cognitive training interventions that can
pairment (MCI) is the state between normal cognitive aging be grossly categorized as either remediation or compensa-
and dementia. Approximately 16% of older adults have MCI tion approaches (Cicerone et al., 2011). Cognitive remediation
(Mitchell and Shiri-Feshki, 2009; Petersen et al., 1999), and approaches attempt to improve cognitive function through
these older adults are at high risk of developing dementia. focused training and practice (Barnes et al., 2009). Com-
Thus, older adults with MCI are at high risk for disability pensation interventions focus on training individuals to
in daily activities and costly support in the form of care- maintain independence, safety, or engagement in daily ac-
giver assistance, community resources, and long-term care. tivities through the use of external aids or adapted methods
Interventions that slow or reverse the decline from MCI to without seeking to improve cognitive functions, per se
dementia could have a significant impact on individual, fa- (Parker and Thorslund, 2007). In addition to cognitive train-
milial, and societal burden. ing interventions, physical exercise may also influence
The measure of efficacy of these interventions may best cognitive function. Although the data are limited, physical
be detected through changes in (or at least maintenance of) exercise has been associated with improvements in cogni-
cognitive function and the impact of these changes on daily tive function healthy older adults (Kelly et al., 2014).
living. Individuals with MCI may have subjective memory Psychotherapeutic interventions have also been examined
complaints and objective evidence of cognitive impair- for their value for helping individuals with cognitive im-
ment beyond those expected for their age and education pairments cope with the changes that cognitive impairments
levels. These cognitive impairments may be detected through bring about in daily life (Simon et al., 2015; Ueda et al., 2013).
domain-specific or global measures of cognitive function. While studied in other clinical populations, these inter-
The impact of cognitive changes on daily living may be as- ventions have only recently become the subject of interest
sessed through measures of daily activity performance or for older adults with MCI. Recent reviews suggest that cog-
quality of life. Despite the common conception that indi- nitive remediation interventions show promise for
viduals in the early stages of cognitive decline do not have promoting small improvements in attention, memory, pro-
disability in daily activities, evidence suggests that perfor- cessing speed, and executive functioning (Huckans et al.,
mance of complex cognitively-focused daily activities may 2013; Li et al., 2011; Reijnders et al., 2013; Simon et al.,
be affected (Rodakowski et al., 2014) and this may have im- 2012). However, given the small magnitude of reported
plications for overall quality of life. changes in these few reviews, and the lack of reported
Several pharmacological interventions (e.g., donepezil, changes in other reviews (Cooper et al., 2013; Martin et al.,
huperzine A, vitamin E, and cholinesterase inhibitors) have 2011; Teixeira et al., 2012), the benefits of these improve-
been examined as potential agents for slowing or revers- ments are unclear. Furthermore, the reviews could not
ing cognitive decline. However, evidence suggests that these comment on the impact of these improvements on every-
agents do not alter cognitive function outcomes or slow pro- day life, as the impact was infrequently addressed in the
gression to dementia (Birks and Flicker, 2006; Farina et al., reviewed articles (Huckans et al., 2013; Kurz et al., 2011;
2012; Russ, 2014; Yue et al., 2012). Thus, more recent Reijnders et al., 2013). The benefits of compensation ap-
efforts have focused on non-pharmacological interven- proaches, physical exercise, and psychotherapeutic
tions. Non-pharmacological interventions may be promising interventions also remain unclear (Cooper et al., 2013;
for a variety of reasons. First, older adults may prefer Huckans et al., 2013; Simon et al., 2012; Teixeira et al., 2012).
non-pharmacological strategies to maintain cognitive Furthermore, many of the reviews combined clinical popu-
function and community independence rather than phar- lations (i.e., healthy older adults and MCI, or MCI and various
macological strategies that may have adverse side-effects. stages of dementia) (Kurz et al., 2011; Martin et al., 2011;
Second, non-pharmacological strategies have less risk Reijnders et al., 2013; Thom and Clare, 2011), making it dif-
than pharmacological strategies (i.e., low likelihood of ficult to isolate the benefits of interventions for older adults
contraindications or problems that occur with polyphar- in the early stages of cognitive decline. The lack of clarity
macy); therefore, they are likely to be more broadly in the findings of these reviews was strongly influenced by
generalizable. state of the science at the time when these reviews were
40 J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53

conducted. The purpose of this scoping review is to update Table 1


and summarize the current state of science addressing non- Study characteristics.

pharmacological approaches (cognitive remediation, Number of


compensation, physical exercise and psychotherapeutic in- studies
terventions) for adults with MCI and early-stage dementia. Clinical population
We also present specific suggestions to guide future Mild Cognitive Impairment (or similar condition) 20
investigations. Early Alzheimer’s disease 8
Both of the above 4
Intervention method
2. Methods Cognitive training interventions 17
Physical exercise interventions 6
We searched PubMed, PsycINFO, EMBASE, and Co- Psychotherapeutic interventions 3
Multimodal interventions 6
chrane Database of Systematic Reviews for randomized,
Comparison group(s)
controlled clinical trials examining non-pharmacological in- Non-active control 12
terventions for improving cognitive function, activities of Active control 13
daily living, or quality of life for individuals with MCI or Active treatment 7
early-stage dementia. We tested and refined the initial search Cognitive function as primary or secondary outcome
Yes 28
strategy in PubMed with subsequent translations into No 4
PsycINFO and EMBASE search languages. We implemented
searches with controlled vocabulary in each database supple-
mented with free-text searching in keywords and titles.
Database specific conventions such as mapping and use of 3.1. Participant characteristics
multiple search fields and filters customized the strategy
for implementation in each individual database. A random- Across the 32 studies, participants were recruited from
ized clinical trial publication filter was applied to locate a variety of settings, including university-based research
studies evaluating non-pharmacological interventions with centers, community-based centers or outreach, and clini-
MCI. English language and age filters (middle-aged and cal practices. Participants were generally 70 years of age or
elderly) were then applied to all search results. For the pur- older. There were more female and male participants, and,
poses of this review we excluded studies that focused on if race was reported, the majority of the samples con-
adults below the age of 60, adults with moderate or more sisted of white participants. On average, participants in these
severe dementia (MMSE ≤ 20), or caregivers or family studies completed 12 years of education.
members as the primary target of intervention. We also ex-
cluded studies that did not examine cognitive function or 3.2. Intervention characteristics
activities of daily living as a primary or secondary outcome.
Studies identified through the search were screened for in- Interventions in the reviewed studies could be classi-
clusion and exclusion by examining the title of the paper, fied as addressing one or more of the following three
the abstract, and the text. Two authors (JR and ES) com- themes: cognitive training interventions (either remediation
pleted screening, and compared included and excluded or compensation), physical exercise interventions, and
studies. Discrepancies were discussed and resolved. A total psychotherapeutic interventions. Most of the reviewed
of 33 articles met criteria. These articles were based on 32 studies (n = 22) examined cognitive training interven-
distinct studies; 1 study used the same sample but re- tions, either alone (Barnes et al., 2009; Boripuntakul et al.,
ported different outcomes in two separate papers. Therefore, 2012; Buschert et al., 2011, 2012; Carretti et al., 2013;
we combined findings from these two publications to rep- Finn and McDonald, 2011; Gaitan et al., 2013; Greenaway
resent the single study. All studies were published in peer- et al., 2013; Jean et al., 2010; Jelcic et al., 2012; Kurz et al.,
reviewed journals. 2012; Kwok et al., 2011; Moro et al., 2012; Olchik et al.,
We extracted data pertaining to the study sample, content 2013; Rozzini et al., 2007; Tappen and Hain, 2014; van
of the experimental and control interventions, primary and Paasschen et al., 2013; Zhuang et al., 2013) or in combina-
secondary outcomes, and results including within and tion with psychotherapeutic interventions (interpersonal
between subject effects. A meta-analysis for this particu- therapy, cognitive-behavioral therapy, coping strategies)
lar review was not considered informative due to the wide (Bottino et al., 2005; Rapp et al., 2002; Troyer et al., 2008;
variations in intervention methods and outcomes as- Tsolaki et al., 2011). Cognitive training interventions ad-
sessed. Results are summarized descriptively. dressed either remediation (via stimulation or memory
training exercises such as attention focusing exercises,
3. Results mnemonics, or procedural memory cues) (Barnes et al.,
2009; Boripuntakul et al., 2012; Buschert et al., 2011,
Table 1 summarizes the selected studies. With a few ex- 2012; Carretti et al., 2013; Finn and McDonald, 2011;
ceptions, studies recruited fairly small samples. Table 2 Gaitan et al., 2013; Jean et al., 2010; Jelcic et al., 2012;
summarizes sample characteristics. Table 3 describes in- Moro et al., 2012; Olchik et al., 2013; Rozzini et al., 2007;
tervention characteristics, primary and secondary outcomes, Tappen and Hain, 2014; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al.,
and major findings from each of the selected studies. Re- 2011; van Paasschen et al., 2013; Zhuang et al., 2013) or
viewed articles are marked with an asterisk in the reference compensation (via external memory aids or adaptive strat-
section. egies) (Bottino et al., 2005; Greenaway et al., 2013; Jelcic
Table 2
Sample characteristics.

Author (Year) Population Sample size Gender, race Mean age (years) Mean education (years)

Baker et al. Amnestic mild cognitive Intervention (n = 19) Male, 47.4% Men 70.9 ± 6.7, Women 65.3 ± 9.4 NR
(2010) impairment White, NR
Control (n = 10) Male, 50% Mean 70.6 ± 6.1, Women 74.6 ± 11.1 NR
White, NR
Barnes et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 22) Male, 59.1% 74.1 ± 8.7 16.8 ± 3.2
(2009) White, NR
Control (n = 25) Male, 60.0% 74.8 ± 7.2 16.6 ± 2.9
White, NR
Boripuntakul Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 5) Male, 40.0% 77.6 ± 6.1 9.0 ± 5.2
et al. (2012) White, NR
Control (n = 5) Male, 40.0% 78.4 ± 5.0 12.4 ± 3.0

J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53


White, NR
Bottino et al. Early-stage Alzheimer’s Intervention (n = 6) Male, 16.7% 74.7 ± 7.0 7.5 ± 4.0
(2005) disease White, NR
Control (n = 7) Male, 42.9% 72.9 ± 6.3 8.6 ± 3.7
White, NR
Burgener et al. Early-stage dementia Intervention (n = 24) Male, 54.2% 77.9 ± 7.9 16.2 ± 3.7
(2008) White, NR
Control (n = 19) Male, 52.6% 76.0 ± 8.1 15.4 ± 3.9
White, NR
Burns et al. Early-stage dementia Intervention (n = 20) Male, 50.0% 73.9 ± 13 NR
(2005) White, NR
Control (n = 20) Male, 55.0% 77.7 ± 16 NR
White, NR
Buschert et al. Amnestic-mild cognitive aMCI: Intervention (n = 12) aMCI: Male , 50.0% aMCI: 71.8 ± 8.6 aMCI: 12.3 ± 3.6
(2011) impairment or early-stage aMCI: White, NR
Alzheimer’s disease aMCI: Control (n = 12) aMCI: Male, 50.0%, White, NR; aMCI: 70.7 ± 5.7 aMCI: 13.3 ± 2.2
Early stage AD: Control early AD: Male, 42.9%, White, Early AD: 74.2 ± 9 Early AD: 13.6 ± 6.5
(n = 7) NR
Buschert et al. Amnestic-mild cognitive Intervention (n = 12) Male, 50.0% 71.8 ± 8.6 12.3 ± 3.6
(2012) impairment White, NR
Control (n = 12) Male, 50.0% 70.7 ± 5.7 13.3 ± 2.2
White, NR
Carretti et al. Amnestic-mild cognitive Intervention (n = 10) Male, 60.0% 71.8 ± 2.2 6.5 ± 2.8
(2013) impairment White, NR
Control (n = 10) Male, 40.0% 70.6 ± 2.6 7.2 ± 3.3
White, NR
Finn and Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 8) Male, 37.5% 69.0 ± 7.7 13.3 ± 2.2
McDonald White, NR
(2011) Control (n = 8) Male, 62.5% 76.4 ± 6.5 12.0 ± 2.8
White, NR
Gaitan et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 23) Male, 65.2% 74.9 ± 4.9 4.8 ± 3.8
(2013) and early-stage White, NR
Alzheimer’s disease Control (n = 16) Male, 25% 76.0 ± 6.6 3.9 ± 3.2
White, NR
Greenaway et al. Amnestic-mild cognitive Intervention (n = 20) Male, 40.0% 72.7 ± 6.9 16.4 ± 2.8
(2013) impairment White, 90%
Control (n = 20) Male, 38.0% 72.3 ± 7.9 16.4 ± 2.8
White, 85%
(continued on next page)

41
42
Table 2 (continued)

Author (Year) Population Sample size Gender, race Mean age (years) Mean education (years)

Jean et al. (2010) Amnestic-mild cognitive Intervention (n = 11) Male, 36.0% 68.6 ± 9.2 14.5 ± 3.2
impairment White, NR
Control (n = 11) Male, 45.0% 68.6 ± 5.9 14.6 ± 4.2
White, NR
Jelcic et al. Early-stage Alzheimer’s Intervention (n = 20) Male, 10.0% 82.9 ± 3.6 6.7 ± 2.9
(2012) disease White, NR
Control (n = 20) Male, 25.0% 81.8 ± 5.5 8.3 ± 3.6
White, NR
Kurz et al. (2012) Early-stage dementia Intervention (n = 100) Male, 50.0% 72.4 ± 8.6 12.8 ± 3.1
White, NR
Control (n = 101) Male, 62.4% 75.0 ± 7.1 12.2 ± 3.0

J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53


White, NR
Kwok et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 14) Male, 35.7% 85.8 ± 4.9 ≥12 years, 14.3%
(2011) White, NR
Control (n = 17) Male, 6.3% 72.4 ± 8.6 ≥12 years, 11.8%
White, NR
Moro et al. Amnestic-mild cognitive Intervention group A Male, NR 73.3 ± 6.9 8.9 ± 4.4
(2012) impairment (n = 15) White, NR
Intervention group B Male, NR 68.5 ± 8.7 11.1 ± 2.8
(n = 15) White, NR
Nagamatsu et al. Probable mild cognitive Intervention group A Male, NR 75.6 ± 3.6 ≤12 years: 13.9%
(2012) impairment (n = 30) White, NR
Intervention group B Male, NR 73.9 ± 3.4 ≤12 years: 10.4%
(n = 28) White, NR
Control (n = 28) Male, NR 75.1 ± 3.6 ≤12 years: 13.9%
White, NR
Olchik et al. Normal cognition (NC) and NC Intervention (n = 23) NC intervention: Male, 4.3% NC intervention: 67 ± 6.1 NC intervention: 13.0 ± 4.6
(2013) mild cognitive impairment NC intervention: White, NR
(MCI) MCI Intervention (n = 16) MCI intervention: Male, 25.0% MCI intervention: 70.3 ± 4.3 MCI intervention: 14.3 ± 4.9
MCI intervention: White, NR
NC Control1 (n = 20) NC control 1: Male, 10.0% NC control 1: 66.7 ± 5.1 NC control 1: 14.9 ± 3.9
NC control 1: White, NR
NC Control2 (n = 22) NC control 2: Male, 22.7% NC control 2: 68.2 ± 6.8 NC control 2: 12.4 ± 4.5
NC control 2: White, NR
MCI Control1 (n = 17) MCI control 1: Male, 17.6% MCI control 1: 72.2 ± 6.3 MCI control 1: 13.2 ± 5.2
MCI control 1: White, NR
MCI Control2 (n = 14) MCI control 2: Male, 14.3% MCI control 2: 70.2 ± 5.7 MCI control 2: 11.2 ± 4.2
MCI control 2: White, NR
Rapp et al. (2002) Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 9) Male, 11.0% 73.3 ± 6.6 ≥12 years, 100%
White, 100.0%
Control (n = 10) Male, 70.0% 75.1 ± 7.0 ≥12 years, 100%
White, 90.0%
Rozzini et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention 1 (n = 15) Male, NR Between 63 and 78 NR
(2007) White, NR
Intervention 2 (n = 22) Male, NR Between 63 and 78 NR
White, NR
Control (n = 22) Male, NR Between 63 and 78 NR
White, NR
(continued on next page)
Table 2 (continued)

Author (Year) Population Sample size Gender, race Mean age (years) Mean education (years)

Scherder et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention A (n = 15) Male, 13.3% Intervention group A: 84 ± 6.4 Intervention group A: 2.6 ± 1.1
(2005) White, NR
Intervention B (n = 13) Male: 15.4% Intervention group B: 89 ± 2.4 Intervention group B: 2.9 ± 1.1
White, NR
Control (n = 15) Male, 6.7% Control group: 86 ± 5.1 Control group: 2.7 ± 1.7
White, NR
Stanley et al. Early-stage dementia Intervention (n = 16) Male, 37.5% 77.6 ± 10.5 ≤12 years: 37.5%
(2013) White, 75.0%
Control (n = 16) Male, 43.7% 79.6 ± 9.0 ≤12 years: 56.3%
White, 56.3%
Suzuki et al. Amnestic-mild cognitive Intervention (n = 25) Male, 52.0% 75.3 ± 7.5 11.1 ± 2.4
(2012) impairment White, NR

J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53


Control (n = 25) Male, 56.0% 76.8 ± 6.8 10.8 ± 2.7
White, NR
Tappen et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 37) Male, 59.0% 80.9 ± 5.5 14.3 ± 3.6
(2014) and early-stage White, NR
Alzheimer’s disease Control (n = 31) Male, 61.0% 81.8 ± 5.8 14.1 ± 2.6
White, NR
Troyer et al. Amnestic-mild cognitive Intervention (n = 24) Male, 45.8% 76.0 ± 5.6 15.2 ± 3.3
(2008) impairment White, NR
Control (n = 24) Male, 45.8% 74.8 ± 7.7 14.3 ± 3.1
White, NR
Tsolaki et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 122) Male, 22.8% 68.5 ± 7.0 9.3 ± 4.1
(2011) White, NR
Control (n = 79) Male, 26.2% 66.9 ± 8.8 9.0 ± 4.2
White, NR
van Paasschen Early-stage Alzheimer’s Intervention (n = 7) Male, 42.9% 72.6 ± 7.3 10.1 ± 2.5
et al. (2013) disease White, NR
Control (n = 12) Male, 33.3% 75.8 ± 8.7 10.0 ± 1.4
White, NR
van Uffelen et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention 1 (n = 77) Male, 48.1% Intervention 1 men 74.0 ± 2.7 Men, ≤12 years 57%
(2008) White, NR Intervention 1 women 76.0 ± 2.9 Women, ≤12 years 64%
Intervention 2 (n = 78) Male, 56.4% Intervention 2 men 75.0 ± 2.7 Men, ≤12 years 61%
White; NR Intervention 2 women 76.0 ± 2.9 Women, ≤12 years 52%
Control 1 (n = 75) Male, 64% Control 1 men 75.0 ± 2.8 Men, ≤12 years 42%
White, NR Control 1 women 75.0 ± 2.9 Women, ≤12 years 70%
Control 2 (n = 74) Male; 55.4% Control 2 men 74.0 ± 2.9 Men, ≤12 years 34%
White; NR Control 2 women 76.0 ± 2.9 Women, ≤12 years 82%
Wells et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 7) Male, NR 73.0 ± 8.0 NR
(2013a, 2013b) White, NR
Control (n = 7) Male, NR 75.0 ± 7.0 NR
White, NR
Wesson et al. Early-stage dementia Intervention (n = 11) Male, 54.5% 78.7 ± 4.2 10.6 ± 2.4
(2013) White, NR
Control (n = 11) Male, 63.6% 80.9 ± 5 12.0 ± 4.3
White, NR
Zhuang et al. Mild cognitive impairment Intervention (n = 19) Male, 21.1% 83.5 ± 7.3 8.6 ± 4.2
(2013) and early-stage dementia White, NR
Control (n = 14) Male, 28.6% 82.6 ± 7.1 6.6 ± 5.4
White, NR

43
NR = not reported.
44
Table 3
Intervention characteristics and study outcomes.

Author (year) Intervention Primary outcomes Secondary outcomes Statistically significant between
group findings

Baker et al. (2010) 2 groups: Symbol-Digit Modalities, Verbal Fluency, Fasting plasma levels of insulin, Cortisol, Exercise demonstrated modestly larger
(1) High intensity aerobic exercise Stroop, Trail Making B, Task Switching, Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor, Insulin improvements in Symbol-Digit Modalities
intervention group Story Recall, and List Learning growth factor-I, and B amyloids 40 and 42 and Verbal Fluency relative to control.
(2) Stretching control group
Barnes et al. (2009) 2 groups: Repeatable Battery for Assessment of RBANS Index, California Verbal Learning Among the outcomes, computer-based
(1) Computer-based brain games Neuropsychological Status (total score) Test-II, Controlled Oral Word Association brain games demonstrated larger
intervention group Test, Boston Naming Test, California Trail improvements in only Spatial Span Test
(2) Passive computer games control Making Test and Design Fluency test from compared to control based on reported
group the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Scale, effect sizes.
Spatial Span Test

J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53


Boripuntakul et al. 2 groups: Memory, Attention, Executive Functions – Neurochemistry biomarkers, the No between group analyses were reported.
(2012) (1) Cognitive training intervention Logical memory Part I and II, Digit Span, myoinositol/creatine (mI/Cr) ratio
group Trail Making A and B
(2) No treatment control group
Bottino et al. (2005) 2 Groups: Mini-Mental State Examination, CDR, and Vocabulary and Block design subtests from Cognitive training demonstrated modestly
(1) Cognitive training intervention Cognitive Subscale of the Alzheimer’s Wechsler Intelligence Revised Scale, larger improvements in Mini-Mental State
group Disease Assessment Scale Forward and Backward Digit Span from Examination and Backward Digit Span
(2) Routine treatment group Wechsler Memory Revised Scale, Trails scores relative to control.
Both groups received Making Test A and B, Verbal Fluency
pharmacotherapy Semantic, Boston Naming Test, Fuld Object
Memory Evaluation; Caregivers completed
Hamilton Anxiety Scale and Montgomery-
Asberg Depression Rating Scale
Burgener et al. (2008) 2 groups: Mini-Mental State Examination, Single Leg Geriatric Depression Scale, Rosenberg’s At the mid-point in follow-up (20 weeks),
(1) Taiji exercises, cognitive- Stance, Berg Balance Scale, and CIRS Self-Esteem Scale the multimodal intervention group
behavioral therapies, support demonstrated higher Mini-Mental State
intervention group Examination scores and self-esteem than
(2) Educational control group the attention control.
Burns et al. (2005) 2 groups: Cornell Scale for Depression, Mini-Mental Bristol ADL Scale No between group differences were found.
(1) Psychodynamic interpersonal State Examination
therapy intervention group
(2) No treatment control group
Buschert et al. (2011) 2 groups: Alzheimer’s disease assessment scale, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Between group differences were only
(1) Multicomponent cognitive training Mini-Mental State Examination Neuropsychological Status, Trail Making found for those with aMCI. Cognitive
intervention group Test A and B, Montgomery Asberg training demonstrated modestly larger
(2) Self-study of exercises control Depression Rating Scale improvements in Alzheimer’s Disease
group Assessment Scale and Montgomery Asberg
Depression Rating Scale relative to control.
Buschert et al. (2012) 2 groups: Mini-Mental State Examination, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of No between group differences were found.
(1) Multicomponent cognitive training Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – Neuropsychological Status, Trail Making
intervention group cognitive domain Test A and B, Montgomery Asberg
(2) Self-study of exercises control Depression Rating Scale, Quality of Life-
group Alzheimer’s Disease scale
Carretti et al. (2013) 2 groups: Categorization Working Memory Span Test Forward and Backward Digit Span, Dot No between group analyses were reported.
(1) Working memory training matrix, List recall, Cattell test
intervention group
(2) Educational activities control
group
(continued on next page)
Table 3 (continued)

Author (year) Intervention Primary outcomes Secondary outcomes Statistically significant between
group findings

Finn and McDonald 2 groups: Cambridge Automated Memory Functioning Questionnaire, Computer-based cognitive training
(2011) (1) Computer-based cognitive training Neuropsychological Test Battery, Paired Memory Controllability Inventory, demonstrated modestly larger
(2) No treatment control group associates learning, Intra/extra- Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale improvements in visual sustained
dimensional set shifting, Rapid visual attention relative to control.
information processing
Gaitan et al. (2013) 2 groups: Digits forward span, Forward Spatial Span, Iowa Gambling Task (decision making), Computer-based plus traditional cognitive
(1) Traditional in-person cognitive Stroop Test, Digit backward span, Spatial Memory failures in Everyday Memory, training demonstrated modestly larger
training intervention group Span backward, List learning, List learning Geriatric Depression Scale improvements in Mini-Mental State
(2) Computer-based plus traditional Free Recall List Learning Recognition, and Examination Scores, disadvantageous deck,
cognitive training intervention more and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory relative
group to control.

J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53


Greenaway et al. 2 groups: Mini-Mental State Examination, Everyday Adherence, Center for Epidemiological No between group differences were found.
(2013) (1) Memory Support System with Cognition, Dementia Rating Scale Studies-Depression, Quality of life-AD,
training intervention group Chronic Disease Self-Efficacy Scales
(2) Memory Support System without
training control group
Jean et al. (2010) 2 groups: Training Measure Dementia rating scale, California Verbal No between group differences were found.
(1) Errorless cognitive training Learning Test, Self-Esteem Scale, Mini-
intervention group Mental State Examination, Multifactorial
(2) Errorful learning cognitive training memory Questionnaire, Rivermead
intervention group Behavioral memory Test
Jelcic et al. (2012) 2 groups: Mini-Mental State Examination, Boston Forward Digit Span, Rey-Osterrieth After Bonferroni correction for multiple
(1) Lexical semantic stimulation (LSS) Naming Test, Verbal Naming Test, Complex Figure, Stroop Test, Attentive comparisons, LSS demonstrated modestly
intervention group Phonemic and Semantic Fluency, Brief Matrices, Trail Making Test A and B, ROCF larger improvements Mini-Mental State
(2) unstructured cognitive stimulation Story Recall, Rey Auditory Learning Copy, Clock Drawing Test, and Examination, Boston Naming Test, Verbal
(UCS) control group instrumental activities of daily living Naming Test, and Story Delayed Recall
relative to relative to UCS.
Kurz et al. (2012) 2 groups: Bayer-ADL scale Aachen Functional Item Inventory, Quality No between group differences were found.
(1) Cognitive training intervention of Life in Dementia, Geriatric Depression
group Scale, Neuropsychiatric Inventory
(2) Treatment as usual control group
Kwok et al. (2011) 2 groups: Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State None listed Calligraphy demonstrated moderately
(1) Calligraphy training intervention Examination large improvements in the Chinese Mini-
group Mental State Examination relative to
(2) No treatment control group control.
Moro et al. (2012) 2 groups: Auditory Verbal Learning Test Bell test, Omissions, False recognitions, Cognitive stimulation demonstrated
(1) Cognitive stimulation intervention Listening span test, Story recall, Verbal modest improvements for immediate and
group fluency, Tower of London, Analogies, delayed recall, listening span test, story
(2) No treatment control group Stroop test, Trail Making Test A and B recall, Trail-Making Test A, and verbal span
relative to control.
Nagamatsu et al. 3 groups: Stroop Test Trail Making Tests, Verbal Digits Tests, RT demonstrated higher Stroop test and
(2012) (1) Aerobic training (AT) intervention memorizing face-scene pairs, Everyday associative memory task as well as
group Problems Test, fMRI changes in 3 regions of the cortex relative
(2) Resistance training (RT) to control. AT demonstrated improved
intervention group balance, mobility, and cardiovascular
(3) Balance and tone control group capacity relative to control. No differences
were found between experimental
conditions.
(continued on next page)

45
46
Table 3 (continued)

Author (year) Intervention Primary outcomes Secondary outcomes Statistically significant between
group findings

Olchik et al. (2013) 3 groups: Verbal Fluency, FAS Verbal Fluency, Rey None listed No between group differences were found.
(1) Memory training intervention Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Rivermead
group Behavioral Memory Test
(2) Education control group
(3) No treatment control group
Rapp et al. (2002) 2 groups: Consortium of the registry of Alzheimer’s Memory Functioning Questionnaire, At follow-up, multifaceted intervention
(1) Multifaceted intervention group Disease, Mini-Mental State Examination Memory controllability Inventory, Profile group demonstrated better perceived

J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53


(2) No treatment control group of Mood States memory ability and mnemonic use relative
to control.
Rozzini et al. (2007) 3 groups: Short Story, Letter Verbal Fluency, Neuropsychiatric Inventory, Geriatric No between group differences were found.
(1) Neuropsychological Training with Semantic Verbal Fluency, Raven’s Colored Depression Scale
Cholinesterase Inhibitors Matrices, Rey’s Figure Copy, Rey’s Figure
intervention group Recall
(2) Cholinesterase inhibitors
intervention group
(3) No treatment control group
Scherder et al. (2005) 3 groups: Category Naming, Trail Making A and B, None listed When combining the 2 treatment groups,
(1) Walking intervention group Digit Span, Visual memory Span, Verbal the treatment group demonstrated
(2) Hand and face exercise learning and Memory Test, Rivermead medium effect sizes for improvement in
intervention group Behavioral Memory Test category naming relative to control.
(3) Social control group
Stanley et al. (2013) 2 groups: Neuropsychiatric inventory anxiety Penn State Worry Questionnaire, Geriatric At 3 months (not 6 months), Peaceful mind
(1) Peaceful Mind intervention group subscale, Rating Anxiety in Dementia Scale Anxiety Inventory, Geriatric Depression demonstrated a large effect on Rating
(2) No treatment control group Scale, Quality of life in Alzheimer’s Disease Anxiety in Dementia Scale and quality of
life relative to control.
Suzuki et al. (2012) 2 groups: Mini-Mental State examination, WMS-LM I None listed Multicomponent exercise demonstrated
(1) Multicomponent exercise and II, Digit Symbol-Coding, Letter Verbal modestly larger improvements for Mini-
intervention group Fluency Test, Category Verbal Fluency Test, Mental State Examination, immediate
(2) Education class control group Stroop Tests recall, and Letter Verbal Fluency Test
relative to control.
Tappen et al. (2014) 2 groups: Fuld Object Memory Evaluation, Direct None listed Cognitive training demonstrated modestly
(1) Cognitive training intervention Assessment of Functional Status, Face- larger improvements for Face-Name
group Name Association Task, Phonemic Fluency Association Task and financial
(2) Life story interview control group and Controlled Oral Word Association, management relative to control.
Picture Description Test, prospective
memory tasks
Troyer et al. (2008) 2 groups: Memory Toolbox, Multifactorial None listed Cognitive training showed modestly larger
(1) Cognitive training intervention Metamemory Questionnaire, Impact improvements for Mini-Mental State
group Rating Scale, Lifestyle Importance, Examination, immediate and delayed
(2) Lifestyle information control group Objective Memory Ability tests recall, and Digit Span Total relative to
control.
(continued on next page)
Table 3 (continued)

Author (year) Intervention Primary outcomes Secondary outcomes Statistically significant between
group findings

Tsolaki et al. (2011) 2 groups: Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test, Rey None reported. Cognitive training demonstrated modestly
(1) Cognitive training intervention Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Rey- larger improvements for executive
group Osterrieth Complex Figure Test-Delayed function, verbal memory, praxis, daily
(2) No treatment control group Recall, Montreal Cognitive Assessment, function, and general cognition relative to
Test of Everyday Attention, Digit Symbol, control.
Functional Cognitive Assessment Scale,
Trail Making B, Verbal Fluency Test, Boston

J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53


Naming Test, Boston Diagnostic Aphasia
Examination, Functional Rating Scale of
Symptoms of Dementia
van Paasschen et al. 3 groups: Canadian Occupation Performance fMRI With the two control groups combined,
(2013) (1) Cognitive rehabilitation Measure (COPM), Hospital Anxiety and cognitive rehabilitation demonstrated
intervention group Depression Scale modestly large improvements for COPM
(2) Relaxation therapy control group ratings for satisfaction and performance
(3) No treatment control group and fMRI (e.g., superior frontal gyrus and
medial front gyrus) relative to control.
van Uffelen et al. 4 groups: Memory by the Auditory Verbal Learning None listed Women in the walking program + vitamin
(2008) (1) Walking program + vitamin B Test, Verbal Fluency Test, Digit Symbol B group demonstrated modestly large
intervention group Substitute Test, and Stroop Color Word Test improvements on the Digit Symbol
(2) Walking program + placebo pill Substitute Test compared to those in the
intervention group control group.
(3) Low intensity activity + vitamin B
intervention group
(4) Low intensity activity + placebo pill
control group
Wells et al. (2013a, 2 groups: Safety and feasibility (a), seed-based Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale- No between group changes were shown for
2013b) (1) Mindfulness based stress reduction functional connectivity, volumetric (b) cognitive subscale behavioral measures. Mindfulness based
intervention group stress reduction group demonstrated
(2) No treatment control group modestly larger improvements for
functional connectivity between the
posterior cingulate cortex, bilateral medial
prefrontal cortex, and left hippocampus
compared to control.
Wesson et al. (2013) (1) Home hazard reduction and Interview for Deterioration of Daily None listed No between group differences were found.
exercise intervention program Activities, Cornell Scale for Depression in
(2) No treatment control group Dementia, Incidental and Planned Exercise
Questionnaire
Zhuang et al. (2013) 2 groups: Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination – None listed No between group differences were found.
(1) Human–computer interaction- Revised
based cognitive training
intervention group
(2) No treatment control group

47
48 J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53

et al., 2012; Kurz et al., 2012; Rapp et al., 2002; Tsolaki 3.3. Primary and secondary outcomes
et al., 2011). They were delivered in various formats,
including individual sessions (Barnes et al., 2009; A diversity of measures was used to assess the benefits
Boripuntakul et al., 2012; Carretti et al., 2013; Finn and of non-pharmacological interventions for addressing changes
McDonald, 2011; Greenaway et al., 2013; Jean et al., 2010; in cognitive function and the impact of these changes on
Jelcic et al., 2012; Kurz et al., 2012; Kwok et al., 2011; daily living. Measures of cognitive function were either
Moro et al., 2012; Rozzini et al., 2007; Tappen and Hain, domain specific or global in nature. Measures of specific cog-
2014; van Paasschen et al., 2013; Zhuang et al., 2013) and nitive abilities often aligned with hypothesized targets of
group sessions (Bottino et al., 2005; Buschert et al., 2011, intervention (e.g., assessments of memory after memori-
2012; Olchik et al., 2013; Rapp et al., 2002; Troyer et al., zation training). Specific cognitive abilities measured were
2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011). They also incorporated various verbal fluency, verbal learning, immediate and delayed
methods such as traditional training or cognitive stimula- memory, attention, information processing speed, and psy-
tion activities (Boripuntakul et al., 2012; Buschert et al., chomotor speed. The most commonly used global measures
2011, 2012; Carretti et al., 2013; Gaitan et al., 2013; of cognitive function were the Mini Mental State Examina-
Greenaway et al., 2013; Jean et al., 2010; Jelcic et al., tion (Folstein et al., 1975) and the Alzheimer’s Disease
2012; Kurz et al., 2012; Kwok et al., 2011; Moro et al., Assessment Scale-cognitive domain (Rosen et al., 1984).
2012; Olchik et al., 2013; Rapp et al., 2002; Tappen and Approximately 50% of the studies measured the impact
Hain, 2014; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011; van of cognitive changes on daily living as a primary or sec-
Paasschen et al., 2013) and computer-based cognitive ondary outcome (Bottino et al., 2005; Burns et al., 2005;
stimulation activities (Barnes et al., 2009; Finn and Buschert et al., 2012; Finn and McDonald, 2011; Gaitan et al.,
McDonald, 2011; Gaitan et al., 2013; Rozzini et al., 2007; 2013; Greenaway et al., 2013; Jean et al., 2010; Kurz et al.,
Zhuang et al., 2013). Among the 22 studies that examined 2012; Nagamatsu et al., 2012; Stanley et al., 2013; Tappen
some form of cognitive training (either alone, or in com- and Hain, 2014; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011; van
bination with psychotherapy), 10 studies compared cognitive Paasschen et al., 2013; Wells et al., 2013a, 2013b; Wesson
training to non-active control interventions (e.g., no treat- et al., 2013). Measures addressed daily activity perfor-
ment, waitlist) (Boripuntakul et al., 2012; Finn and mance, self-efficacy, and quality of life. Daily activity
McDonald, 2011; Kwok et al., 2011; Moro et al., 2012; performance measures included the Interview for Deteri-
Olchik et al., 2013; Rapp et al., 2002; Rozzini et al., 2007; oration of Daily Activities (Teunisse and Derix, 1997), Bayer
Tsolaki et al., 2011; van Paasschen et al., 2013; Zhuang ADL Scale (Hindmarch et al., 1998), and caregiver report
et al., 2013), and 12 studies compared cognitive training (Bottino et al., 2005). Self-selected goal attainment was mea-
to active control interventions that did not specifically sured with the Canadian Occupation Performance Measure
provide cognitive training (e.g., readings, education, usual (Law et al., 1990; van Paasschen et al., 2013). The impact
treatment) (Barnes et al., 2009; Bottino et al., 2005; Buschert of memory on daily activities was measured with the
et al., 2011, 2012; Carretti et al., 2013; Greenaway et al., Memory Failures in Everyday Memory (Gaitan et al., 2013;
2013; Kurz et al., 2012; Olchik et al., 2013; Rozzini et al., Sunderland et al., 1984). One study measured self-efficacy
2007; Tappen and Hain, 2014; Troyer et al., 2008; van for daily activities (Greenaway et al., 2013). Quality of life
Paasschen et al., 2013). Three studies compared cognitive was assessed with the Quality of Life-Alzheimer’s Disease
training to both non-active and active control conditions measure (Buschert et al., 2011, 2012; Greenaway et al., 2013;
(Olchik et al., 2013; Rozzini et al., 2007; van Paasschen Kurz et al., 2012; Stanley et al., 2013; Thorgrimsen et al.,
et al., 2013). Three additional studies compared two cog- 2003; Wells et al., 2013a, 2013b). Other studies used task
nitive training interventions (Gaitan et al., 2013; Jean specific indices (i.e., use of a schedule reminder) (Jean et al.,
et al., 2010; Jelcic et al., 2012). 2010) or measures of feasibility and safety of the interven-
Far fewer studies examined physical exercise or psycho- tion (Wells et al., 2013a).
therapeutic interventions. Of the 6 studies that examined Two additional categories of outcomes emerged even
physical exercise interventions, 4 studies examined aerobic though they were not the focus of our review. Several studies
exercise alone (Baker et al., 2010; Nagamatsu et al., 2012; examined the benefits of non-pharmacological interven-
Scherder et al., 2005; van Uffelen et al., 2008), 1 in combi- tions for mental health in older adults with MCI (Burgener
nation with balance and dual-task training (Suzuki et al., et al., 2008; Burns et al., 2005; Finn and McDonald, 2011;
2012), and 1 study examined strength and balance train- Gaitan et al., 2013; Kurz et al., 2012; Rapp et al., 2002;
ing plus a home safety program (Wesson et al., 2013). The Rozzini et al., 2007; Stanley et al., 2013; van Paasschen et al.,
frequent rationale for the physical exercise programs 2013; Wesson et al., 2013). In addition, a few studies ex-
was improved physical health, with the suggestion that amined changes in neurochemistry markers or fMRI
cognitive or other functional benefits may follow. Psycho- activation (Baker et al., 2010; Boripuntakul et al., 2012;
therapeutic interventions used interpersonal therapy (n = 1) Nagamatsu et al., 2012; van Paasschen et al., 2013; Wells
(Burns et al., 2005), coping strategies (n = 1) (Stanley et al., et al., 2013b).
2013), or mindfulness training (n = 1) (Wells et al., 2013a,
2013b). These interventions were designed to promote 3.4. Intervention effects
healthy adaptation to cognitive changes. Finally, one study
examined a combination of physical exercise (taiji) and psy- For clarity, only statistically significant differences are re-
chotherapy (cognitive-behavioral therapy) (Burgener et al., ported in the following sections. Examining the reviewed
2008). studies, the benefits of cognitive training for addressing
J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53 49

cognitive function among older adults with MCI are unclear. scores compared to unstructured stimulation combined with
The majority of studies examining cognitive training (20 of compensation-focused training in external memory aids
22) focused on improvements in cognitive function as the (Jelcic et al., 2012). However, best practices for remediation
primary or secondary outcome (Barnes et al., 2009; remain unclear. Jean and colleagues demonstrated that er-
Boripuntakul et al., 2012; Bottino et al., 2005; Buschert et al., rorless learning and errorful learning of face–name
2011, 2012; Carretti et al., 2013; Finn and McDonald, 2011; associations yielded similar improvements on the trained
Gaitan et al., 2013; Greenaway et al., 2013; Jean et al., 2010; task, with no between group differences (Jean et al., 2010).
Jelcic et al., 2012; Kwok et al., 2011; Moro et al., 2012; Olchik Gaitan and colleagues compared computer-delivered cog-
et al., 2013; Rapp et al., 2002; Rozzini et al., 2007; Tappen nitive training, traditional cognitive training (i.e., paper and
and Hain, 2014; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011; pencil activities), and a combination of computer-delivered
Zhuang et al., 2013). Statistically significant between group and traditional cognitive training. The three groups did not
differences in cognitive function favoring the cognitive train- differ with respect to cognitive function change scores.
ing group were reported in 12 of the 20 studies (Barnes et al., However, the group that participated in the combination
2009; Bottino et al., 2005; Buschert et al., 2011; Finn and program demonstrated better decision making on the Iowa
McDonald, 2011; Gaitan et al., 2013; Jelcic et al., 2012; Kwok Gambling Task (Gaitan et al., 2013). These findings might
et al., 2011; Moro et al., 2012; Rapp et al., 2002; Tappen and be attributed to the amount of training provided in the com-
Hain, 2014; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011), but the bination group, rather than the type of training.
between group differences were generally modest. Notably, Considering the findings from all studies examining cog-
intervention effects on cognitive function, when detected, nitive training interventions, it appears that remediation may
were generally attributed to remediation (and not com- be more likely to improve selected cognitive abilities than
pensation) approaches. Furthermore, these effects were for compensation. This is logical given the compensation focuses
the most part detected in indices of working memory and on teaching individuals to adapt to changes in their cogni-
delayed memory. Finally, studies that combined cognitive tive function, rather than addressing cognitive abilities per
training with some form of psychotherapeutic approach (i.e., se. Furthermore, the amount of training may have a stron-
social engagement, stress management) (Bottino et al., 2005; ger impact on outcome than the type of training, although
Rapp et al., 2002; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011) this requires additional investigation. The persistence of
all demonstrated small improvements in cognitive func- small improvements in cognition over time, particularly in
tion (compared to active and non-active controls), suggesting the face of cognitive decline, is unclear. Long-term follow-
that multimodal approaches may enhance cognitive train- up with study participants may be required to ascertain
ing. However, given small magnitude of improvements in whether there are benefits over time. Moreover, the ben-
cognitive function across studies, it is difficult to conclude efits of small improvements in cognitive abilities for
at this time that cognitive training interventions are ro- improving daily living are unclear.
bustly effective in influencing cognitive function in older With regard to physical exercise interventions, 5 of the
adults with MCI. 6 studies examined the benefits of aerobic exercise for im-
Among the 9 studies that focused on the impact of cog- proving cognitive function (Baker et al., 2010; Nagamatsu
nitive changes on daily living (Bottino et al., 2005; Buschert et al., 2012; Scherder et al., 2005; Suzuki et al., 2012; van
et al., 2012; Finn and McDonald, 2011; Gaitan et al., 2013; Uffelen et al., 2008). All 5 studies reported statistically sig-
Greenaway et al., 2013; Jean et al., 2010; Kurz et al., 2012; nificant between group differences in cognitive function
Tsolaki et al., 2011; van Paasschen et al., 2013), within group change scores favoring aerobic exercise relative to control,
improvements in performance of activities of daily living but the improvements in cognitive function attributed to
were detected in 3 studies (Greenaway et al., 2013; Jean et al., aerobic exercise were small in these studies. Baker and col-
2010; Tsolaki et al., 2011). Each of these studies included leagues detected greater improvements in measures of
compensatory training and self-efficacy training. Between executive functions when comparing aerobic exercise to
group differences were detected in only 2 studies (Kurz et al., stretching (Baker et al., 2010). Scherder and Suzuki and their
2012; van Paasschen et al., 2013). First, Kurz and col- colleagues reported greater improvements in measures of
leagues provided compensatory training in external processing speed, working memory and fluency, when com-
memory aids, and detected statistically significantly greater paring aerobic exercise to stretching and education (Scherder
improvements in self-reported quality of life, but not in in- et al., 2005; Suzuki et al., 2012). Van Uffelen and col-
dependence with daily activities, when training was leagues demonstrated greater improvements in measures
compared to treatment as usual. Second, Van Paaaschen and of processing speed for women (not men) in the walking
colleagues demonstrated superior goal-attainment through group compared to women in the low intensity activity
task-specific compensatory training, compared to a relax- group (van Uffelen et al., 2008). In this last study, the authors
ation intervention and a no treatment control. In both cases, reported only moderate adherence to the program, which
the reported effect sizes were relatively small. may have contributed to small effect sizes. None of these
From the reviewed studies, it is difficult to comment on studies examined the impact of aerobic exercise on daily
the relative benefits of different types of cognitive train- living. The sixth study examined the benefits of a multi-
ing interventions for older adults with MCI. Only 3 of the modal physical exercise program incorporating strength and
reviewed studies compared one form of cognitive training balance training in addition to an occupational therapist de-
to another. Jelcic and colleagues demonstrated that a livered home-safety intervention (Wesson et al., 2013).
remediation-focused cognitive training program, using lexical Cognitive function benefits were not assessed, but improve-
semantic stimulation, resulted in improved verbal memory ments in daily activity performance were noted in the
50 J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53

treatment group over time. Whether the improvements were long-term maintenance of these improvements. Cognitive
due to remediation through physical exercise or compen- training focused on compensation, as well as selected psy-
sation is not clear. chotherapy approaches, shows promise for addressing the
The studies that examined psychotherapeutic interven- impact of cognitive changes on daily living. However, con-
tions as a sole intervention (n = 3) (Burns et al., 2005; Stanley fidence in these findings is limited due to several limitations
et al., 2013; Wells et al., 2013a, 2013b) reported no signifi- in the studies performed to date. These limitations point to
cant changes in cognitive function, either within or between several directions for future investigations examining po-
groups. Given that the focus of psychotherapeutic approaches tential interventions to slow or reverse functional decline
is typically compensatory, teaching individuals to learn adap- in older adults with MCI or early-stage dementia.
tive skills to support daily living, lack of improvement in While many agree that identifying and studying older
cognitive functions is not surprising. Of interest, however, is adults at the early stages of cognitive decline hold partic-
whether these interventions reduce the negative impact of cog- ular clinical and scientific importance, a universal definition
nitive changes, as measured by mood and quality of life. Two of “early stage” with consistent application across the world
of the three studies examined mood as the primary outcome of specified criteria remains elusive. Several definitions have
(Burns et al., 2005; Stanley et al., 2013), and reported mixed existed over time, including questionable dementia, pos-
outcomes. Stanley found between group differences for anxiety sible incipient dementia, isolated short-term memory loss,
(favoring mindfulness training over treatment as usual; Stanley amnestic dementia, and mild neurocognitive disorder. The
et al., 2013), and Burns did not find mood-related between term Mild Cognitive Impairment, while widely used in med-
group differences (Burns et al., 2005). All 3 studies examined icine, research, and education, has been defined in a variety
impact on daily life, but only Stanley and colleagues found sig- of ways (Petersen, 2004; Portet et al., 2006; Winblad et al.,
nificantly greater improvements in quality of life attributed 2004). Among the studies we reviewed, MCI was the most
to training in psychosocial coping and self-monitoring com- commonly used classification, but diagnostic criteria varied
pared to treatment as usual (Stanley et al., 2013). As mentioned from study-specific clinical criteria to using recommenda-
previously, compensatory cognitive training interventions when tions of an international consensus committee (Winblad
offered in concert with psychotherapeutic interventions are et al., 2004). We must acknowledge that given the histor-
associated with statistically significant but small changes in ical variations in the definition of MCI and “early stage” it
cognitive function (Bottino et al., 2005; Rapp et al., 2002). We is possible that we unintentionally excluded important
only reviewed one study that examined the combined ben- studies that may have contributed to this review.
efits of physical exercise (taiji) and psychotherapy (cognitive Perhaps given the relatively “new” focus on early stages
behavioral therapy) (Burgener et al., 2008). Burgener and col- of cognitive decline, it is not surprising that the prepon-
leagues reported small improvements in a global measure of derance of studies that we examined contained fairly small
cognitive function that were greater in the multi-modal group and poorly defined samples. This may be due to the fact that
than the educational control group. In addition, Burgener and it is difficult to identify and recruit individuals at the early
colleagues reported more substantive improvements in balance stages of cognitive decline when “early intervention” may
and physical function, relative to educational control. be considered. Many older adults in the early stages of cog-
Impact on daily living was not assessed. Collectively, these nitive decline may not be aware of subtle changes in their
studies suggest that psychotherapeutic interventions show cognition, and therefore may not seek intervention. Fur-
promise, but require additional investigation before we can thermore, primary care providers and community agencies
draw strong conclusions about the benefits for older adults that interact with older adults may not notice these subtle
with MCI. cognitive changes if they are not looking for them. These
realities, added to the aforementioned disparities in defi-
4. Discussion nitions and criteria for identification, impede identification
and referral of older adults with early stages of cognitive
The current state of science examining non- decline. Community outreach through senior centers, li-
pharmacological interventions for changing (or, at least, braries, or other locations pertaining to cognitive health
maintaining) cognitive function and the impact of these literacy may empower older adults to seek services that
changes on daily living for individuals in the early stage of could support them in the quest to maintain cognitive func-
cognitive decline produces mixed results. A scoping review tion and community independence. Education and training
of the current literature suggests that cognitive training in- of primary care providers and community agencies to
terventions have been the most widely studied, but that improve recognition and screening for early stages of cog-
physical exercise and psychotherapeutic interventions have nitive decline may also improve awareness.
been investigated as well. In general, some evidence sug- In addition to clarifying the definitions of “early
gests that cognitive training focused on remediation has been stage” cognitive decline, clarification of the conceptualiza-
associated with significantly greater improvements in se- tion, operationalization and implementation of non-
lected cognitive abilities, but the magnitude of improvements pharmacological interventions for this population is also
is small. The same can be said for physical exercise inter- needed. For the purposes of summarization, we grossly char-
ventions. One may argue that these small improvements acterized interventions based on common intervention
could be meaningful, as small improvements or even main- concepts. However, wide variation existed in the concep-
tenance of cognitive function over time may be valuable in tualization and rationale for the interventions across studies,
the face of potential cognitive decline. However, before we which limits strong conclusions that can be drawn from this
can support this conclusion, we must first assess the literature. By definition, most studies used “complex
J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53 51

interventions” or “interventions that contain several inter- 2013). Connections between physical health and cognitive
acting components [including] the number and difficulty of health in late-life are also well-established. Cognitive changes
behaviors required by those administering and receiving the have been found to be contributors to limitations in mo-
intervention, … the number and variability of outcomes ad- bility, including among older adults without known
dressed by the intervention, and the degree of flexibility or neurological disease (Rosso et al., 2013); whereas, in-
tailoring of the intervention permitted.” (Craig et al., 2013, creased aerobic activity is associated with improved brain
2). Non-pharmacological interventions, if they are to be ben- health and cognitive function in healthy older adults
eficial for older adults with cognitive decline, are likely to (Erickson et al., 2011). The linkages among cognition, mood,
meet this definition. Therefore, careful conceptualization, and physical health are likely due to common underlying
operationalization and implementation of these interven- pathologies that cause general slowing of neurological func-
tions are critical. The Medical Research Council provides tion, and clinical manifestations in each of these domains
guidance on best practices for developing, piloting, evalu- (Rosano et al., 2014).
ating, and implementing complex interventions (Craig et al., These linkages suggest that it may be possible to iden-
2013), and should be incorporated into future studies seeking tify biomarkers that may be used to identify older adults
to test potential non-pharmacological interventions for older in the early stages of decline. Once rigorously validated, these
adults in the early stages of cognitive decline. biomarkers may prove to be important measures of change
In addition to integrating best practices for designing and attributed to intervention. For example, recent cross-
studying non-pharmacological interventions for this pop- sectional evidence suggests that there is a relationship
ulation, it seems important to embrace outcome assessment between pro-inflammatory cytokines and engagement in
strategies that more comprehensively assess the potential productive activities among older adults (Kim and Ferraro,
benefits and long-term outcomes. There is a long-held debate 2014). However, the nature of this relationship is unclear.
in cognitive rehabilitation research as to the “real world” The elevation of pro-inflammatory cytokines may contrib-
benefits of improvements in cognition, as detected with task- ute to reduced engagement productive activities, or
specific or norm-referenced tests (Simon et al., 2012). reductions in productive activities may contribute to ele-
Certainly large magnitude of changes (e.g., one standard de- vation of pro-inflammatory cytokines. Interventions that
viation as defined by norm-referencing or large effect sizes) address one of these factors may influence the other. For
can be one way to determine “clinically meaningful” change. example, non-pharmacological interventions that promote
However, assessment of the impact of changes in cogni- increased levels of productive activities (whether through
tive test scores on measures of independence, safety and remediation or compensation) may reduce pro-inflammatory
engagement in daily activities may be another way. This cytokines. Investigations focused on these types of ques-
argues for employing best practices in assessing daily ac- tions may clarify the nature of these important biological
tivities through performance observation or through well- and behavioral relationships.
validated assessments of disability and social engagement Finally, we focused this review on non-pharmacological
(McDonough et al., 2012; Rodakowski et al., 2014). Further- interventions. However, the synergistic benefits of phar-
more, long-term term follow-up, assessing for potential macological and non-pharmacological interventions may be
changes in trajectories of cognitive decline, may be another another promising area of study. Studies for adults with
way to assess the heuristic value of these interventions. related neurodegenerative disease (e.g., chronic nonpsychotic
For the purposes of our review, we focused on out- major depressive disorder) have found that combination
comes pertaining to changes in cognitive function and the therapies can be significantly more effective than medica-
impact of these changes on daily living as target outcomes tion or non-pharmacological therapy alone (Keller et al.,
of non-pharmacological interventions aiming to slow the 2000). Three of the reviewed studies (Bottino et al., 2005;
decline to dementia. That said, some of the reviewed studies Rozzini et al., 2007; van Uffelen et al., 2008) examined the
focused on additional measures of brain health that, when additive benefits of pharmacological substances (acetyl-
added to cognitive function, help to address the cacopho- cholinesterase inhibitors, vitamin B) as adjuncts to non-
ny of changes that contribute to dementia. Among these pharmacological interventions (aerobic exercise, cognitive
measures were indices of mood and physical health, as well training). Two of these studies reported a statistically sig-
as more proximal measures of neurophysiology and neu- nificant benefit of the combination of therapies relative to
rological connectivity. Mood, specifically, anxiety, appeared other conditions (Bottino et al., 2005; van Uffelen et al.,
to be most strongly influenced by psychotherapy interven- 2008). Bottino and colleagues demonstrated slightly better
tions that focused on relaxation (Stanley et al., 2013). changes in cognitive test scores for individuals who engaged
Proximal measures of neurophysiology and neurological con- in cognitive training while taking acetylcholinesterase in-
nectivity appeared to be most strongly influenced by physical hibitors relative to acetylcholinesterase inhibitors alone. Van
activity interventions (Baker et al., 2010). These findings are Uffelen and colleagues reported that women (not men)
not surprising given that several studies have demon- engaged in aerobic exercise (compared to low intensity ac-
strated strong connections between changes in cognitive tivity) while taking vitamin B (compared to placebo)
function and changes in mood in late life. In fact, approx- demonstrated modestly greater improvements in cogni-
imately 50% of older adults with MCI have concurrent tive test scores relatively. Given the size and scope of these
depressive symptoms (Apostolova and Cummings, 2008; two studies, additional investigation may be warranted.
Gabryelewicz et al., 2004; Monastero et al., 2009). Further- In summary, we conducted a scoping review of litera-
more, depressive symptoms in late-life are a risk factor for ture examining non-pharmacological interventions for older
MCI and dementia (Palmer et al., 2010; Rosenberg et al., adults in the early stages of cognitive decline. Descriptive
52 J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53

summaries of the literature indicate mixed findings with Erickson, K.I., Voss, M.W., Prakash, R.S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L.,
et al., 2011. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and
regard to the efficacy of cognitive training focused on
improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108 (7), 3017–3022.
remediation and aerobic exercise for improving cognitive Farina, N., Isaac, M.G., Clark, A.R., Rusted, J., Tabet, N., 2012. Vitamin E for
function. Cognitive training focused on compensation and Alzheimer’s dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane
psychotherapy show promise for addressing the impact of Database Syst. Rev. (11), CD002854.
*Finn, M., McDonald, S., 2011. Computerised cognitive training for older
cognitive changes in the lives of older adults in the early persons with mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study using a
stages of cognitive decline. In general, effect sizes were small. randomised controlled trial design. Brain Impair. 12 (3), 187–199.
Nonetheless, given variability in the samples, intervention Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., McHugh, P.R., 1975. “Mini-mental state”. A
practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the
methods, and outcomes measures, strong conclusions about clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12 (3), 189–198.
the efficacy of these interventions are difficult to draw. The Gabryelewicz, T., Styczynska, M., Pfeffer, A., Wasiak, B., Barczak, A.,
review identified specific areas for future research. Luczywek, E., et al., 2004. Prevalence of major and minor depression
in elderly persons with mild cognitive impairment – MADRS factor
analysis. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 19 (12), 1168–1172.
Acknowledgements *Gaitan, A., Garolera, M., Cerulla, N., Chico, G., Rodriguez-Querol, M.,
Canela-Soler, J., 2013. Efficacy of an adjunctive computer-based
cognitive training program in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and
This work was supported by the National Institutes of
Alzheimer’s disease: a single-blind, randomized clinical trial. Int. J.
Health grants KL2TR000146, P30 MH090333, P30 MH090333 Geriatr. Psychiatry 28 (1), 91–99.
Sub-project ID: 8315, and UL1 TR000005. *Greenaway, M.C., Duncan, N.L., Smith, G.E., 2013. The memory support
system for mild cognitive impairment: randomized trial of a cognitive
rehabilitation intervention. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 28 (4), 402–
References 409.
Hindmarch, I., Lehfeld, H., de Jongh, P., Erzigkeit, H., 1998. The Bayer
Alzheimer’s Association, 2014. 2014 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Activities of Daily Living Scale (B-ADL). Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord.
Alzheimers Dement. 10 (2), e47–e92. 9 (Suppl. 2), 20–26.
Apostolova, L.G., Cummings, J.L., 2008. Neuropsychiatric manifestations in Huckans, M., Hutson, L., Twamley, E., Jak, A., Kaye, J., Storzbach, D., 2013.
mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of the literature. Efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation therapies for mild cognitive
Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 25 (2), 115–126. impairment (MCI) in older adults: working toward a theoretical model
*Baker, L.D., Frank, L.L., Foster-Schubert, K., Green, P.S., Wilkinson, C.W., and evidence-based interventions. Neuropsychol. Rev. 23 (1), 63–80.
McTiernan, A., et al., 2010. Effects of aerobic exercise on mild cognitive *Jean, L., Simard, M., Wiederkehr, S., Bergeron, M.E., Turgeon, Y., Hudon,
impairment: a controlled trial. Arch. Neurol. 67 (1), 71–79. C., et al., 2010. Efficacy of a cognitive training programme for mild
*Barnes, D.E., Yaffe, K., Belfor, N., Jagust, W.J., DeCarli, C., Reed, B.R., et al., cognitive impairment: results of a randomised controlled study.
2009. Computer-Based Cognitive Training for Mild Cognitive Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 20 (3), 377–405.
Impairment: Results from a Pilot Randomized, Controlled Trial, third *Jelcic, N., Cagnin, A., Meneghello, F., Turolla, A., Ermani, M., Dam, M., 2012.
ed. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, USA, pp. 205–210. Effects of lexical-semantic treatment on memory in early Alzheimer
Birks, J., Flicker, L., 2006. Donepezil for mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane disease: an observer-blinded randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabil.
Database Syst. Rev. (3), CD006104. Neural Repair 26 (8), 949–956.
*Boripuntakul, S., Kothan, S., Methapatara, P., Munkhetvit, P., Sungkarat, Keller, M.B., McCullough, J.P., Klein, D.N., Arnow, B., Dunner, D.L.L., Glenberg,
S., 2012. Short-Term Effects of Cognitive Training Program for A.J., et al., 2000. A comparison of nefazodone, the cognitive behavioral-
Individuals with Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: a Pilot Study, analysis system of psychotherapy, and their combination for the
second ed. Taylor & Francis, UK, pp. 138–149. treatment of chronic depression. NEJM 342, 1462–1470.
*Bottino, C.M.C., Carvalho, I.A.M., Alvarez, A.M.M.A., Avila, R., Zukauskas, Kelly, M.E., Loughrey, D., Lawlor, B.A., Robertson, I.H., Walsh, C., Brennan,
P.R., Bustamante, S.E.Z., et al., 2005. Cognitive Rehabilitation Combined S., 2014. The impact of exercise on the cognitive functioning of healthy
with Drug Treatment in Alzheimer’s Disease Patients: a Pilot Study, older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev.
eighth ed. Hodder Arnold United Kingdom, UK, pp. 861–869. 16, 12–31.
*Burgener, S.C., Yang, Y., Gilbert, R., Marsh-Yant, S., 2008. The effects of a Kim, S., Ferraro, K.F., 2014. Do productive activities reduce inflammation
multimodal intervention on outcomes of persons with early-stage in later life? Multiple roles, frequency of activities, and C-reactive
dementia. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 23 (4), 382–394. protein. Gerontologist 54 (5), 830–839.
*Burns, A., Guthrie, E., Marino-Francis, F., Busby, C., Morris, J., Russell, E., *Kurz, A., Thone-Otto, A., Cramer, B., Egert, S., Frolich, L., Gertz, H.J., et al.,
et al., 2005. Brief psychotherapy in Alzheimer’s disease: randomised 2012. CORDIAL: cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive-behavioral
controlled trial. Br. J. Psychiatry 187, 143–147. treatment for early dementia in Alzheimer disease: a multicenter,
*Buschert, V.C., Friese, U., Teipel, S.J., Schneider, P., Merensky, W., Rujescu, randomized, controlled trial. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 26 (3),
D., et al., 2011. Effects of a newly developed cognitive intervention in 246–253.
amnestic mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease: a Kurz, A.F., Leucht, S., Lautenschlager, N.T., 2011. The clinical significance
pilot study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 25 (4), 679–694. of cognition-focused intervention for cognitively impaired older adults:
*Buschert, V.C., Giegling, I., Teipel, S.J., Jolk, S., Hampel, H., Rujescu, D., et al., a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Int. Psychogeriatr.
2012. Long-term observation of a multicomponent cognitive 23 (9), 1364–1375.
intervention in mild cognitive impairment. J. Clin. Psychiatry 73 (12), *Kwok, T.C., Bai, X., Kao, H.S., Li, J.C., Ho, F.K., 2011. Cognitive effects of
e1492–e1498. calligraphy therapy for older people: a randomized controlled trial in
*Carretti, B., Borella, E., Fostinelli, S., Zavagnin, M., 2013. Benefits of Training Hong Kong. Clin. Interv. Aging 6, 269–273.
Working Memory in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Specific and Law, M., Baptiste, S., McColl, M., Opzoomer, A., Polatajko, H., Pollock, N.,
Transfer Effects, fourth ed. Cambridge University Press, UK, pp. 617–626. 1990. The Canadian occupational performance measure: an outcome
Chung, C.S., Pollock, A., Campbell, T., Durward, B.R., Hagen, S., 2013. measure for occupational therapy. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 57 (2), 82–87.
Cognitive rehabilitation for executive dysfunction in adults with stroke Li, H., Li, J., Li, N., Li, B., Wang, P., Zhou, T., 2011. Cognitive intervention for
or other adult non-progressive acquired brain damage. Cochrane persons with mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis. Ageing Res.
Database Syst. Rev. (4), CD008391. Rev. 10 (2), 285–296.
Cicerone, K.D., Langenbahn, D.M., Braden, C., Malec, J.F., Kalmar, K., Fraas, Martin, M., Clare, L., Altgassen, A.M., Cameron, M.H., Zehnder, F., 2011.
M., et al., 2011. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review Cognition-based interventions for healthy older people and people with
of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. (1), CD006220.
92 (4), 519–530. McDonough, C.M., Tian, F., Ni, P., Kopits, I.M., Moed, R., Pardasaney, P.K.,
Cooper, C., Li, R., Lyketsos, C., Livingston, G., 2013. Treatment for mild et al., 2012. Development of the computer-adaptive version of the
cognitive impairment: systematic review. Br. J. Psychiatry 203 (3), Late-Life Function and Disability Instrument. J. Gerontol. A. Biol Sci.
255–264. Med Sci 67 (12), 1427–1438.
Craig, P., Dieppe, P., Macintyre, S., Michie, S., Nazareth, I., Petticrew, M., 2013. Mitchell, A.J., Shiri-Feshki, M., 2009. Rate of progression of mild cognitive
Developing and evaluating complex interventions: the new Medical impairment to dementia – meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort
Research Council guidance. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 50 (5), 587–592. studies. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 119 (4), 252–265.
J. Rodakowski et al. / Molecular Aspects of Medicine 43-44 (2015) 38–53 53

Monastero, R., Mangialasche, F., Camarda, C., Ercolani, S., Camarda, R., 2009. disability in the first 6 months after stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair
A systematic review of neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive doi:10.1177/1545968314562113.
impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 18 (1), 11–30. *Stanley, M.A., Calleo, J., Bush, A.L., Wilson, N., Snow, A.L., Kraus-Schuman,
*Moro, V., Condoleo, M.T., Sala, F., Pernigo, S., Moretto, G., Gambina, G., 2012. C., et al., 2013. The peaceful mind program: a pilot test of a cognitive-
Cognitive stimulation in a-MCI: an experimental study. Am. J. behavioral therapy-based intervention for anxious patients with
Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 27 (2), 121–130. dementia. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 21 (7), 696–708.
*Nagamatsu, L.S., Handy, T.C., Hsu, C.L., Voss, M., Liu-Ambrose, T., 2012. Sunderland, A., Harris, J.E., Gleave, J., 1984. Memory failures in everyday
Resistance training promotes cognitive and functional brain plasticity life following severe head injury. J. Clin. Neuropsychol. 6 (2), 127–
in seniors with probable mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Intern. Med. 142.
172 (8), 666–668. *Suzuki, T., Shimada, H., Makizako, H., Doi, T., Yoshida, D., Tsutsumimoto,
*Olchik, M.R., Farina, J., Steibel, N., Teixeira, A.R., Yassuda, M.S., 2013. K., et al., 2012. Effects of multicomponent exercise on cognitive function
Memory Training (MT) in mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) Generates in older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a randomized
Change in Cognitive Performance, third ed. Elsevier Science, The controlled trial. BMC Neurol. 12, 128.
Netherlands, pp. 442–447. *Tappen, R.M., Hain, D., 2014. The effect of in-home cognitive training on
Palmer, K., Di Iulio, F., Varsi, A.E., Gianni, W., Sancesario, G., Caltagirone, functional performance of individuals with mild cognitive impairment
C., et al., 2010. Neuropsychiatric predictors of progression from and early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. Res. Gerontol. Nurs. 7 (1), 14–
amnestic-mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease: the role 24.
of depression and apathy. J. Alzheimers Dis. 20 (1), 175–183. Teixeira, C.V., Gobbi, L.T., Corazza, D.I., Stella, F., Costa, J.L., Gobbi, S., 2012.
Parker, M.G., Thorslund, M., 2007. Health trends in the elderly population: Non-pharmacological interventions on cognitive functions in older
getting better and getting worse. Gerontologist 47 (2), 150–158. people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr.
Petersen, R.C., 2004. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. 54 (1), 175–180.
Intern. Med. 256 (3), 183–194. Teunisse, S., Derix, M.M., 1997. The interview for deterioration in daily living
Petersen, R.C., Smith, G.E., Waring, S.C., Ivnik, R.J., Tangalos, E.G., Kokmen, activities in dementia: agreement between primary and secondary
E., 1999. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and caregivers. Int. Psychogeriatr. 9 (Suppl. 1), 155–162.
outcome. Arch. Neurol. 56 (3), 303–308. Thom, J.M., Clare, L., 2011. Rationale for combined exercise and cognition-
Portet, F., Ousset, P.J., Visser, P.J., Frisoni, G.B., Nobili, F., Scheltens, P., et al., focused interventions to improve functional independence in people
2006. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in medical practice: a critical with dementia. Gerontology 57 (3), 265–275.
review of the concept and new diagnostic procedure. Report of the MCI Thorgrimsen, L., Selwood, A., Spector, A., Royan, L., de Madariaga Lopez,
Working Group of the European Consortium on Alzheimer’s Disease. M., Woods, R.T., et al., 2003. Whose quality of life is it anyway? The
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 77 (6), 714–718. validity and reliability of the Quality of Life-Alzheimer’s Disease
*Rapp, S.R., Brenes, G., Marsh, A.P., 2002. Memory Enhancement Training (QoL-AD) scale. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 17 (4), 201–208.
for Older Adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment: a Preliminary Study, Troyer, A.K., Murphy, K.J., Anderson, N.D., Moscovitch, M., Craik, F.I., 2008.
first ed. Taylor & Francis, UK, pp. 5–11. Changing everyday memory behaviour in amnestic mild cognitive
Reijnders, J., van Heugten, C., van Boxtel, M., 2013. Cognitive interventions impairment: a randomised controlled trial. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 18
in healthy older adults and people with mild cognitive impairment: (1), 65–88.
a systematic review. Ageing Res. Rev. 12 (1), 263–275. *Tsolaki, M., Kounti, F., Agogiatou, C., Poptsi, E., Bakoglidou, E., Zafeiropoulou,
Rodakowski, J., Skidmore, E.R., Reynolds, C.F., 3rd, Dew, M.A., Butters, M.A., M., et al., 2011. Effectiveness of nonpharmacological approaches in
Holm, M.B., et al., 2014. Can performance on daily activities patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurodegener Dis. 8 (3),
discriminate between older adults with normal cognitive function and 138–145.
those with mild cognitive impairment? J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 62 (7), Ueda, T., Suzukamo, Y., Sato, M., Izumi, S., 2013. Effects of music therapy
1347–1352. on behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia: a systematic
Rosano, C., Rosso, A.L., Studenski, S.A., 2014. Aging, brain, and mobility: review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 12 (2), 628–641.
progresses and opportunities. J. Gerontol. A. Biol Sci. Med Sci 69 (11), *van Paasschen, J., Clare, L., Yuen, K.S., Woods, R.T., Evans, S.J., Parkinson,
1373–1374. C.H., et al., 2013. Cognitive rehabilitation changes memory-related brain
Rosen, W.G., Mohs, R.C., Davis, K.L., 1984. A new rating scale for Alzheimer’s activity in people with Alzheimer disease. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair
disease. Am. J. Psychiatry 141 (11), 1356–1364. 27 (5), 448–459.
Rosenberg, P.B., Mielke, M.M., Appleby, B.S., Oh, E.S., Geda, Y.E., Lyketsos, *van Uffelen, J.G., Chinapaw, M.J., van Mechelen, W., Hopman-Rock, M.,
C.G., 2013. The association of neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI with 2008. Walking or vitamin B for cognition in older adults with mild
incident dementia and Alzheimer disease. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry cognitive impairment? A randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Sports Med.
21 (7), 685–695. 42 (5), 344–351.
Rosso, A.L., Studenski, S.A., Chen, W.G., Aizenstein, H.J., Alexander, N.B., *Wells, R.E., Kerr, C.E., Wolkin, J., Dossett, M., Davis, R.B., Walsh, J., et al.,
Bennett, D.A., et al., 2013. Aging, the central nervous system, and 2013a. Meditation for adults with mild cognitive impairment: a pilot
mobility. J. Gerontol. A. Biol Sci. Med Sci 68 (11), 1379–1386. randomized trial. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 61 (4), 642–645.
*Rozzini, L., Costardi, D., Chilovi, B.V., Franzoni, S., Trabucchi, M., Padovani, *Wells, R.E., Yeh, G.Y., Kerr, C.E., Wolkin, J., Davis, R.B., Tan, Y., et al., 2013b.
A., 2007. Efficacy of Cognitive Rehabilitation in Patients with Mild Meditation’s impact on default mode network and hippocampus
Cognitive Impairment Treated with Cholinesterase Inhibitors, fourth in mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study. Neurosci. Lett. 556, 15–
ed. John Wiley & Sons, USA, pp. 356–360. 19.
Russ, T.C., 2014. Cholinesterase inhibitors should not be prescribed for mild *Wesson, J., Clemson, L., Brodaty, H., Lord, S., Taylor, M., Gitlin, L., et al.,
cognitive impairment. Evid. Based Med. 19 (3), 101. 2013. A feasibility study and pilot randomised trial of a tailored
*Scherder, E.J., Van Paasschen, J., Deijen, J.B., Van Der Knokke, S., Orlebeke, prevention program to reduce falls in older people with mild dementia.
J.F., Burgers, I., et al., 2005. Physical activity and executive functions BMC Geriatr. 13, 89.
in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment. Aging Ment. Health 9 Winblad, B., Palmer, K., Kivipelto, M., Jelic, V., Fratiglioni, L., Wahlund, L.O.,
(3), 272–280. et al., 2004. Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards
Seitz, D.P., Brisbin, S., Herrmann, N., Rapoport, M.J., Wilson, K., Gill, S.S., a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild
et al., 2012. Efficacy and feasibility of nonpharmacological interventions Cognitive Impairment. J. Intern. Med. 256 (3), 240–246.
for neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia in long term care: a Young, S., Amarasinghe, J.M., 2010. Practitioner review: non-
systematic review. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 13 (6), 503–506.e2. pharmacological treatments for ADHD: a lifespan approach. J. Child
Simon, S.S., Yokomizo, J.E., Bottino, C.M., 2012. Cognitive intervention in Psychol. Psychiatry 51 (2), 116–133.
amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Neurosci. Yue, J., Dong, B.R., Lin, X., Yang, M., Wu, H.M., Wu, T., 2012. Huperzine A
Biobehav. Rev. 36 (4), 1163–1178. for mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. (12),
Simon, S.S., Cordas, T.A., Bottino, C.M., 2015. Cognitive behavioral therapies CD008827.
in older adults with depression and cognitive deficits: a systematic *Zhuang, J.P., Fang, R., Feng, X., Xu, X.H., Liu, L.H., Bai, Q.K., et al., 2013. The
review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 30 (3), 223–233. impact of human-computer interaction-based comprehensive training
Skidmore, E.R., Dawson, D.R., Butters, M.A., Grattan, E.S., Juengst, S.B., Whyte, on the cognitive functions of cognitive impairment elderly individuals
E.M., et al., 2014. Strategy training shows promise for addressing in a nursing home. J. Alzheimers Dis. 36 (2), 245–251.

You might also like