You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/258830227

Preadolescent antecedents of depressive symptomatology at age 18: A


prospective study

Article  in  Journal of Youth and Adolescence · April 1991


DOI: 10.1007/BF01537609 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

44 197

2 authors, including:

Per Gjerde
University of California, Santa Cruz
44 PUBLICATIONS   2,320 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Per Gjerde on 10 September 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1988, VA 97, No. 4,475-486 0021-S43X/S8/S00.75

Depressive Symptoms and Personality During Late Adolescence:


Gender Differences in the Externalization-Internalization
of Symptom Expression

Per F. Gjerde, Jack Block, and Jeanne H. Block


University of California at Berkeley

This study examined the observed and self-attributed personality characteristics associated with
depressive symptoms in nondinical adolescents. Eighty-seven 18-year-olds completed the Center for
Epidemiological Studies Depression (CES-D) Scale, adjective self-descriptions, and the Multidimen-
sional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ). Separately, California Adult Q-sort (CAQ) ratings of the
subjects were obtained. Although mean CES-D scores did not differ for the sexes, dysthymic young
men were consistently more negatively evaluated than were dysthymic young women and nondysthy-
mic young persons of both sexes. Extensive differences were also observed between male and female
subjects in observer (CAQ) correlates of CES-D scores: 18-year-old dysthymic young men were seen
as disagreeable, aggressive, and antagonistic—an externalizing pattern of characteristics; 18-year-old
dysthymic young women, by contrast, were seen as ego-brittle, unconventional, and ruminating—
an internalizing pattern of characteristics, [n their self-recognition, however, both male and female
adolescents described themselves as aggressive and alienated. Nonlinear relations between CES-D
scores and personality were also observed. The discussion focused on gender differences in depressive
tendencies, on the role of hostility and aggression in dysthymia, and on the psychological implica-
tions of nonlinear relations between dysthymia and personality.

Appreciable changes occur in the frequency and nature of sion-vulnerable personality structure (for reviews, see Akiskal,
depressive symptoms as individuals approach and pass through Hirschfeld, & Yerevanian, 1983; Chodoff, 1972), these efforts
adolescence. Following puberty, depression becomes more com- have provided limited insight into the diathesis underlying ado-
mon, especially among girls (Kandel & Davies, 1982; Rutter, lescent depression if only because studies of the character struc-
1986). Although depressive states comparable to those seen in ture associated with depression have typically not focused on
adults also begin to appear (Bemporad & Wilson, 1978; Weiner, the adolescent period. Furthermore, even with respect to age
1975), adolescents sometimes seem to express their underlying groups other than adolescence, controversy persists over the rel-
depression through behaviors differing from the traditional ative contributions of singular personality attributes such as ex-
manifestations of adult depression. For example, maladaptive cessive dependency needs (e.g., Arieti & Bemporad, 1980; Cho-
adolescent behavior (e.g., drug abuse) has been viewed as de- doff, 1972; Hirschfeld, Klerman, Chodoff, Korchin, & Barrett,
pression related (Malmquist, 1971a, 1971b; Weiner, 1975). 1976), dysfunctional belief systems (Abramson, Seligman, &
The severity of a depressive episode during adolescence may, Teasdale, 1978; Beck, 1967), absence of competence (Cicchetti
at least partly, be influenced by the extent of vulnerability inher- & Schneider-Rosen, 1986), and introversion (Akiskal et al.,
ent in the individual's underlying personality organization 1983). In addition, we do not yet have perspective on the rele-
(Chodoff, 1970, 1972; Salzman, 1975). Although much re- vance of contemporaneous psychosocial factors in the genesis
search has been devoted to identifying attributes of a depres- of adolescent depression (Robbins & Kashani, 1986).
Various writers, in discussing the psychological dynamics of
depression, have conjectured that the basis for depression differs
This study was supported by National Institute of Mental Health for men and women (e.g., Kaplan, 1986; Nolen-Hoeksema,
Grant MH 16080 to Jack Block. Per F. Gjerde gratefully acknowledges 1987; Radloff, 1980; Radloff & Rae, 1979; Weissman & Kler-
additional support from the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Sci- man, 1979). These views may be seen as following from more
ence. Portions of this article were presented at the second biannual general developmental considerations of the differential social-
meeting of the Society for Research on Adolescence, Alexandria, Vir- ization of the sexes and from the different self and world views
ginia, 1988. that our culture creates in men and women (J. H. Block, 1976,
Jeanne H. Block is now deceased. 1979, 1983). Sroufe and Rutter (1984) have noted that depres-
We are indebted to Dan Ozer, Ruth Sharabany, Auke Tellegen, Brian
sion in young boys often is embedded within the context of con-
Vaughn, and two anonymous reviewers for their most thoughtful com-
duct disturbances—an externalizing pattern of symptom ex-
ments on an earlier version of this manuscript
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Per F. pression. In young girls, by contrast, depression is manifested
Gjerde or Jack Block, Department of Psychology, Tolman Hall, Univer- in passivity and a turning inward—an internalizing pattern of
sity of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California 94720. symptom expression. In a similar vein, Ebata and Peterson

475
476 P. F. GJERDE, J. BLOCK, AND J. H. BLOCK.

(1988) reported that depression among early adolescents was sion (Kovacs, Feinberg, Crouse-Novak, Paulauskas, &
related in boys but not in girls to "externalized" behavior (e.g., Finkelstein, 1984; Kovacs, Feinberg, Crouse-Novak, Pau-
getting into trouble at school) and to poor academic perfor- lauskas, Pollack, & Finkelstein, 1984), and depressive moods
mance. during adolescence have consequences for later psychological
This research was designed to examine (a) whether depressive adjustment and interpersonal functioning (Kandel & Davies,
symptoms in late adolescence are embedded concurrently 1986). However, despite these several justifications, the inferen-
within a distinct and identifiable constellation of personality at- tial leap made by many investigators, and also by us, from self-
tributes, and (b) whether the personality context incorporating reported depressive manifestations in a nonclinical sample to
depressive symptoms differs substantially for young men and clinically observed depression, as an organized psychiatric syn-
young women. We were particularly interested in gender differ- drome, still remains uncertain and arguable. Therefore,
ences in the "externalizing-internalizing" balance of depres- throughout this article, we use phrases such as depressive symp-
sion-related personality attributes: the direction of action taken toms, depressive tendencies, or dysthymia to acknowledge that
by individuals (externally, via action modes, or internally, via our results, based on self-reported depression symptoms in a
cognitive or psychosomatic channels of expression) in response nonclinical sample, may or may not generalize to clinical de-
to salient psychological motivations (J. H. Block & J. Block, pression as defined in psychiatry.
1980).
Based upon recognitions concerning gender-differentiated so- Method
cialization emphases (J. H. Block, 1983), and guided by recent
studies of the relation between depressive feelings and adjust- Subjects
ment in nonclinical samples of adolescents (Ebata & Peterson,
The subjects in this study participate in a longitudinal study of ego
1988), we anticipated that male subjects with depressive tend- and cognitive development, initiated in 1968 by Jack and Jeanne Block
encies in late adolescence will manifest their internal unhappi- at the University of California, Berkeley, and still ongoing (see J. H.
ness via overt action, hostility, and aggression—an externalizing Block & J. Block, 1980, for a comprehensive description of this study).
pattern of symptom expression. Conversely, we expected female The present sample consisted of the 106 adolescents (52 male, 54 fe-
subjects with depressive tendencies in late adolescence to be male) who took part in the age 18 assessment. After excluding individu-
characterized by an introspective concern with self and by inad- als who did not respond to the CES-D scale or who appeared to be
equate self-esteem—an internalizing pattern of symptom ex- responding invalidly to the validity scales embedded in the question-
pression. Given this gender difference in symptom expression, naire or for whom California Q-sort descriptions did not exist, a total
of 87 eighteen-year-olds remained (41 male, 46 female). Subjects live
we anticipated depressive symptoms in female adolescents to
primarily in urban settings, and are heterogeneous with respect to social
have less simple, less visible, and less straightforward behavioral
class and parents' educational levels. About two thirds of the 87 subjects
consequences than depressive symptoms in male adolescents. included in this study are White, one fourth are Black, and one twenti-
Whereas only modest gender differences in mean level of de- eth are Asian. Subjects were initially recruited into the study at age 3,
pression were expected, with girls likely to score somewhat while attending either a university-run nursery school or a parent-run
higher than boys (Kandel & Davies, 1982), we expected many cooperative nursery school.
and crucial gender differences in the correlates of depressive
symptoms. Measuring Depressive Symptoms: The Center for
In contrast to many other studies of depression, which have
Epidemic/logical Studies—Depression Scale (CES-D)
used the extreme group comparison design (depressed individ-
uals contrasted with nondepressed individuals), we analyzed During the age 18 assessment, the subjects responded to an extensive
depressive tendencies as a continuous variable. Our approach pool of personality inventory items. The set of 678 items included many
has the advantage of allowing us to examine the shape of the scales measuring various aspects of personality and family relation-
ships. Among these scales was the Center for Epidemiological Studies-
relation between depression and other, continuously measured
Depression (CES-D) Scale (Radloff, 1977), a well-established self-report
variables. If the underlying relation is nonlinear, the extreme
questionnaire of depressive tendencies.
group method may be misleading and may attenuate or even The CES-D scale has been widely used in assessing depressive symp-
obfuscate important relations (e.g., McNemar, 1960). toms in nonclinical samples of adolescents (Schoenbach, Kaplan,
Finally, the current study assesses depressive tendencies in a Grimson, & Wagner, 1982; Schoenbach, Kaplan, Wagner, Grimson, &
nonclinical sample and therefore our "depressed" subjects may Miller, 1983; Wells, Klerman, & Deykin, 1987). It has demonstrated
be viewed as perhaps reflecting only a dysthymic disorder, a rel- satisfactory reliability, can be completed by subjects of diverse back-
ative degree of disturbance, or extreme sadness, rather than the grounds, and discriminates well between clinical subgroups (Boyd,
major depressive disorder clinically recognizable as a distinct, Weissman, Thompson, & Myers, 1982; Craig & Van Natta, 1976; Lin,
Dean, & Ensel, 1986; Radloif, 1977; Radloff & Locke, 1985; Roberts,
diagnostic syndrome. Depressive tendencies in children and ad-
1980; Roberts & Vernon, 1983; Weissman, Scholomskes, Pollenger, Pru-
olescents, even if they fall short of major clinical depression,
soff,& Locke, 1977).
certainly warrant further study in their own right. It is also im-
The CES-D scale consists of 20 items tapping four content subdo-
portant to recognize that depressive tendencies appear to be mains: depressed affect, positive affect, somatic vegetative signs, and in-
continuous with clinical depression (Blatt, D'Afflitti, & Quin- terpersonal distress. Subjects responded to each item on a 4-point scale
lan, 1976; Hirschfeld & Cross, 1982). A considerable propor- (0 — does not apply at all, 1 = does not apply particularly well, 2 =
tion of individuals manifesting subclinical depression (dysthy- applies somewhat, and 3 = applies quite well). Four of the 20 items
mia) early in life appear to develop subsequent clinical depres- are worded in a positive direction, whereas the remaining 16 items are
DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS AND PERSONALITY 477

worded in a negative direction. For each subject, the scores on the 4 Self-Attributed Personality Descriptions of the Subjects
positively worded items are reflected and the total score is created by
summing the values of the 20 responses. Possible scores range from 0 to The Adjective Q-sort (AQ). During the age 18 assessment, each sub-
60, with higher scores indicating greater depressive mood. In the present ject was asked to describe himself or herself by using a 43-item Adjective
study, the internal consistency (alpha) reliability of the CES-D is .90. Q-sort (AQ) and a 7-step continuum (J. H. Block & J. Block, 1980). The
In the present usage of the CES-D scale, to prevent the subjects from adjectives were selected to permit comprehensive self-descriptions. In a
quickly realizing the common intention lying behind the 20 CES-D separate session, each subject also used this procedure to describe his
items, the 20 items were not presented consecutively, as is usually the or her "ideal self." Conceptually, when a person's characterization of
case. Rather, the CES-D items were interspersed among items belonging self is relatively congruent with his or her characterization of a personal
to other personality scales. In a further change from the usual way the ideal self, that person may be said to have high self-esteem. Operation-
CES-D scale is administered, subjects were asked how well each symp- ally, an estimate of the subject's self-esteem was thus obtainable by cor-
tom applied to them, with no time frame specified. Usually, the CES-D relating the two vectors of 43 adjective Q-sort descriptions: the one
asks how often each of the 20 symptoms included in the scale has oc- reflecting the self-concept, the second reflecting the ideal self. To the
curred during the last week. This change in item response format, a extent this correlation is high, the subject is said to possess high self-
consequence of our decision to intersperse the presentation of items esteem (J. Block & Thomas, 1955; Rogers & Dymond, 1954).
deriving from many scales, was deemed necessary to prevent too fre- The Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ). The MPQ
quent changes in the response format and because subject responses (Tellegen, 1982, 1985) was included in the item pool at age 18. The
were spread out over several weeks. Further, there is reason to believe MPQ is a factor-analytically developed self-report instrument, measur-
that subjects' responses to time-framed questions are not validly based ing 11 primary personality dimensions. The 11 scales are labeled as
on the time period specified (Bradburn, Ripo, & Shevell, 1987). The follows (the internal consistency reliabilities obtained in this study are
response format we used therefore deviates importantly from the con- provided in parentheses; the descriptions of the scales derive from Tel-
ventional CES-D format. We believe this change in the way the CES-D legen, 1982): (a) Well-Being (alpha = .89; high scorers have a happy
items were presented to the subjects and perhaps also our item inter- cheerful disposition, feel good about self), (b) Social Potency (alpha =
spersing increased the obtained depressive symptom scores, primarily .88; high scorers are decisive, like to take charge and to influence others),
because subjects were not limited to considering their emotional states (c) Achievement (alpha = .85; high scorers work hard, value accom-
during the last week only. Subjects participating in this study may also plishment, persistence, and perfection), (d) Social Closeness (alpha =
have given more frank responses because of their long-term involve- .83; high scorers are sociable, like people, and value interpersonal ties),
ment with this research. We note, however, that our subsequent analyses (e) Stress Reaction (alpha = .89; high scorers are vulnerable, easily up-
are correlational, depending on the ranking or ordering of subjects and set, and prone to worry), (f) Alienation (alpha = .83; high scorers feel
not on the absolute level of the scores obtained. It seems reasonable to mistreated and betrayed by others), (g) Aggression (alpha = .88; high
presume that the correlation between scores on the CES-D scale as usu- scorers are physically aggressive and vindictive), (h) Control (alpha »
ally administered and scores on the CES-D scale in the form we admin- .83; high scorers are reflective, cautious, and like to anticipate and plan),
istered would be very high. (i) Harm Avoidance (alpha = .84; high scorers prefer to avoid adventure
In responding to the inventory items, the subject was seated in front and danger), (j) Traditionalism (alpha = .81; high scorers endorse au-
of a personal computer. The questions appeared, one by one, on the thority, support religious values, and value propriety), and (k) Absorp-
display screen. The subject was required to provide his or her answer by tion (alpha = .88; high scorers are responsive to engaging sights and
pressing the appropriate keyboard key. The age 18 assessment program sounds, are readily captured by entrancing stimuli, and experience
included 6 separate sessions, each of which lasted, on the average, 2.5- states of expanded consciousness). In addition, the MPQ contains 6 va-
3 hr. In most cases, a full week separated different sessions. Either prior lidity scales.
to, or following each main session, each subject completed part of the Tellegen (1985) has, through factor analysis, created three higher or-
computer-assisted inventory. During each session, each subject worked der scales: Positive Aflectivity (primarily associated with the Well-Be-
on the inventory for approximately 20 min. Because the CES-D items ing, Social Potency, and Achievement scales—high scores reflect a sense
were never presented consecutively, most subjects used more than one of well-being and a positive evaluation of oneself as active and effectively
session to complete all 20 questions. engaged with other persons), Negative Aflectivity (primarily associated
with the Stress Reaction, Alienation, and Aggression scales—high
scores reflect a tendency to worry, to be anxious, to be victimized, and
Observer-Based Personality Descriptions of the Subjects
to experience life as stressful), and Constraint (Control, Harm Avoid-
During the age 18 assessment, four psychologists used the the Califor- ance, and Traditionalism scales—high scores reflect an appraisal of one-
nia Adult Q-sort (CAQ) (J. Block, 1961/1978) to describe each subject. self as cautious, restrained, and tendencies to accept conventional
The CAQ consists of 100 widely ranging statements about the personal- norms and morality).
ity, cognitive, and social characteristics of individuals, The assessors,
having observed each adolescent during different experimental sessions
Results
eliciting a wide variety of behaviors, described the subject by arranging
the Q-sort items in a forced-choice, nine-step distribution according to
Descriptive Statistics for the CES-D Scale
the evaluated salience of each item for that particular adolescent. The
psychologists worked independently of each other. The four indepen- The mean CES-D scale score was 19.77 (SD = 10.75, N =
dently obtained Q-descriptions were then composited to form a pooled 41) for male subjects and 22.50 (SD = 11.10, N = 46) for female
judgment of each adolescent. On the basis of correlations among ob-
subjects. This gender difference of approximately 3 points, al-
servers, the mean internal consistency reliability of the Q-items was
though not significantly different, <(85) = 1.16, ns, is in accord
found to be .59. This reliability information not only provides impor-
tant information about the relative quality of the personality data used,
with the slight but consistent gender differences reported in ear-
but also places perspective on the possible magnitude of correlations lier studies (e.g., Kandel & Davies, 1982). Our CES-D means
that can be expected when these measures are related to our criterion are appreciably higher than CES-D means reported in previous
variable, the CES-D scale (J. Block, 1963, 1964). studies (e.g., the mean CES-D score of 13 for adolescents re-
478 P. F. GJERDE, J. BLOCK, AND J. H. BLOCK

ported by Wells et al., 1987). As already noted, we believe this in that nondysthymic female subjects received somewhat lower
difference to be primarily due to the different way we adminis- values than dysthymic male subjects.
tered the CES-D in our study. For CAQ items not associated with an interaction effect, ad-
ditional main effects for sex and depressive symptoms were
Sex X Depressive Symptoms (CES-D Scores) Analysis of noted. A significant main effect for depressive symptoms was
Variance of Observer-Based (CAQ) Personality associated with 13 CAQ items, and a significant main effect for
gender was associated with 14 CAQ items. The relations be-
Descriptions
tween CES-D scale scores and CAQ items are reported later.
A first analysis involved a two-way Sex X Depressive Symp- Gender differences in CAQ evaluations will be reported in a
toms analysis of variance (ANOVA) in order to evaluate possible separate article.
interactions. Adolescents who had appreciably elevated scores
on the CES-D scale (i.e., those whose scores fell in the upper
Observer (CAQ) Item Correlates of Depressive
30% of the CES-D distribution) were assigned to the depressive
Symptoms (CES-D Scores)
symptoms group. Adolescents scoring in the lower 70% of the
CES-D distribution were considered to have few or no depres- A polynomial hierarchical regression procedure (Cohen &
sive symptoms. The CES-D score separating the upper 30% Cohen, 1983) for each of the male and female samples evaluated
from the lower 70% of the CES-D score distribution was 24 for the special stimulus value of individuals privately acknowledg-
male subjects and 26 for female subjects. We decided to use a ing depressive symptoms. The CES-D scale score and its sec-
CES-D cutting score of 25 for both sexes. Use of different CES- ond-degree power polynomial (i.e., the quadratic component
D cutting scores for the sexes would not appreciably alter the term) were, for each CAQ item, entered sequentially as predic-
results. tors. This approach permits, beyond a first test for the existence
A two-way ANOVA was conducted separately for each CAQ and strength of linear association, a following test of whether
item. A statistically significant (p < .05) Sex X Depressive quadratic nonlinear component adds significant or incremental
Symptoms interaction effect was obtained for 39 of the 100 variance.
CAQ items. For 38 of these 39 CAQ interactions, the CAQ-item For the sample of adolescent boys, the statistically significant
means for the four groups fell into a consistent pattern discrimi- linear and quadratic relations are presented in Table 1. Thirty-
nating young men with depressive tendencies from the remain- nine significant linear relations and 17 nonlinear relations
ing three groups (i.e., young men with few or no depressive (semipartials) were significant. When a semipartial coefficient
symptoms, young women with depressive symptoms, and has a positive sign, the regression line moves toward concavity
young women with few or no depressive symptoms). Compared (i.e., toward becoming U-shaped); when a semipartial coeffi-
with the other three groups, young male adolescents with de- cient has a negative sign, the regression line moves toward con-
pressive symptoms tended to be described as more negativistic vexity (i.e., toward becoming an inverted U). Note that a fully
(p < .001), more guileful (p < .001), more sensitive to demands concave (or convex) distribution is obtained only when the qua-
(p < .001), more rebellious (p < .01), more stretching of limits dratic component is not superimposed on an underlying linear
(p < .01), more self-indulgent (p < .01), more self- trend.
defeating (p < .01), more over-reactive to minor frustrations For the sample of adolescent girls, the statistically significant
(p < .01), more interpersonally hostile (p < .01), more power linear and quadratic relations are presented in Table 2. Six sig-
oriented (p < .01), more critical (p < .05), more lacking of nificant linear relations and 13 nonlinear (semipartials) corre-
meaning in life (p < .05), more extrapunitive (p < .05), more lations CAQ items were significant. The difference between the
projective (p < .05), more condescending in relations to others two sexes in the number of significant linear findings observed
(p < .05), more distrustful (p < .05), more exploitative of others is itself highly significant (p < .001).
(p < .05), more concerned with self-adequacy (p < .05), more The linear and quadratic CAQ item relations reported in Ta-
likely to perceive contexts in sexual terms (p < .05), more di- bles 1 and 2 may be directly evaluated and interpreted by the
rectly hostile (p < .05), and more appeciative of their own inde- reader. Their very number, however, makes them difficult to en-
pendence (p < .05). compass conceptually. Therefore, to summarize these CAQ
Relative to nondysthymic adolescents of both sexes and item relations and to reflect their coherent psychological
to dysthymic female adolescents, these dysmymic young male themes, the 44 discriminating items for the male subjects and,
adolescents also tended to be seen as less liked and accepted separately, the 18 discriminating items for the female subjects,
(p < .001), less dependable (p < .01), less sympathetic (p < .01), were submitted to principal-components analyses followed by
less warm (p < .01), less productive (p < .01), less likely to have Varimax rotation. We restricted our data-reducing analyses to
many interests (p < .05), less giving to others (p < .05), less include only the discriminating items because of the small sizes
fastidious (p < .05), less arousing of nurturance in others (p < of our samples. Four principal components accounted for 72%
.05), less likely to be turned to for advice (p < .05), less ready of the total variance in the male sample; three principal compo-
to feel guilt (p < .05), less appreciative of intellectual matters nents explained 71% of the variance in the female sample. Fac-
(p < .05), less self-insightful (p < .05), less ambitious (p < .05), tor loadings of the CAQ items on the four male and three female
less straightforward (p < .05), less cheerful (p < .05), and less factors are also included in Tables 1 and 2.
charming (p < .05). Only for one CAQ item, high intellectual Using unit weighting, four summarizing component mea-
capacity, did the mean group values deviate from this pattern sures were generated for the male sample: Interpersonal Antag-
DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS AND PERSONALITY 479

Table 1 Table 2
California Adult Q-Sort (CAQ) Correlates of Depressive California Adult Q-Sort (CAQ) Correlates of Depressive
Symptoms (Centerfor Epidemiological Studies- Symptoms (Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression
Depression Scores) at Age IS: Male Subjects (N - 41) Scores) at Age 18: Female Subjects (N = 46)

Factor Factor
CAQ item r sr" loading CAQ item r sr* loading

Factor 1 : Ego-Brittleness
Factor 1 : Interpersonal Antagonism
Is self-defeating .30* .75
Creates, exploits dependency in people 50*** .80 Has brittle ego-defense system 43*. .74
Guileful, deceitful, manipulative .51*** 52*** .73 Lack of personal meaning in life .38** .65
Subtly negativistic .45** 39* .71 Feels cheated, victimized by life .31* .61
Hostility toward others .39* 34* .67 Has personal fantasy, daydreams .36* .54
Tries to stretch limits .40** .65 Is verbally fluent -.41** -.78
Distrustful of people .39* .64 Is turned to for advice -.41** -.76
Is assertive .35* .62 Has high aspiration for self -.29* -.75
Extrapunitive, transfers blame .35* .32* .62 Has high degree of intellectual capacity .35* -.71
Perceives contexts in sexual terms .37* .49 Is cheerful -.32* -.46
Expresses hostility directly .37* .46
Capacity for close relationships —.36* —.33* -.83 Factor 2: Unconventionality
Has giving way with others -.43** -.81
Arouses nurturant feelings in others —.34* -.81 Rebellious, non-conforming .33* .86
Sympathetic, considerate -.40** -.41* -.80 Unconventional thought processes .43** .33* .84
Tendency to feel guilty -. 32* -.71 Favors conservative values —.36* -.93
Liked, accepted by people -.43** -. 32* -.66 Judges self, others in conventional terms — . 59*** -.87
Submissive, accepts domination -. 32* -.66
Straightforward, candid -.41** -.62 Factor 3: Rumination
Behaves in ethically consistent manner -.41** -.59
Tends to ruminate .29* .82
Anxiety outlet in body symptoms .44** .72
Factor 2: Unrestraint Vulnerable to threat .30* .67
Regards self as physically attractive —.30* -.78
Unable to delay gratification .38* .82
Unpredictable in behavior, attitudes .31* .65 * The semipartial correlation represents the contribution of the qua-
Self-indulgent .51*** .62 dratic component after the linear component has been semipartialed.
Withdraws in face of adversity .34* .62 "p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Projects feelings onto others .35* .50
Productive, gets things done -.45** -.30* -.85
Values intellectual matters -.41** -.82
Prides self on being objective -.32* -.78
Dependable, responsible -.44** - .31* -.72 onism (illustrative items are "Is guileful, deceitful, and manipu-
Has high aspiration for self -.34* -.62 lative," "Has hostility towards others," and "Is subtly negativis-
Is fastidious -.36* -.52 tic"), Unrestraint (illustrative items are "Is self-indulgent,"
Clear-cut, consistent personality -.3 1 * -.48
"Unable to delay gratification," and "Is unpredictable in behav-
ior and attitudes"), Discontentment With Self (illustrative items
Factor 3: Discontentment With Self are "Feels cheated, victimized by life," "Feels a lack of personal
Lack of personal meaning in life .36* .79 meaning in life," and "Concerned with own adequacy"), and
Feels cheated, victimized by life .40** .78 Unconventionality (illustrative items are "Tends to be rebel-
Concerned with own adequacy as .36* .75 lious, non-conforming," "Favors conservative values" [re-
person flected], and "Judges self, others in conventional terms" [re-
Self-defeating .35* .68
flected]).
Tends to be sen-defensive .41** .64
Overreacts to minor frustrations .31* .32* .59 For the female sample, three summarizing measures were de-
Sensitive to demands .35* .47* .56 veloped: Ego-Brittleness (illustrative items are "Has a brittle
Is personally charming -.41** -.85 ego-defense system," "Is self-defeating," and "Is verbally fluent"
Is cheerful -.39* -.79
[reflected]), Unconventionality (illustrative items are "Tends to
Rapid personal tempo -.3 1 * -.55
be rebellious, non-conforming," "Favors conservative values"
[reflected], and "Judges self, others in conventional terms" [re-
Factor 4: Unconventionality
flected]), and Rumination (illustrative items are "Is rumina-
Rebellious, nonconforming .41** .65 tive," "Anxiety finds outlet in bodily symptoms," and "Vulner-
Favors conservative values -.41** -.82 able to threat"). These summarizing, observer-based measures,
Judges self/others in conventional terms -.31* - .30* -.74
each based on an aggregation of individual CAQ items, can be
' The semipartial correlation represents the contribution of the qua- expected to be more dependable than the individual CAQ items
dratic component, after the linear component has been semipartialed. on which they are based and yet to represent their core mean-
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. ings in a conceptually reasonably sufficient way. Using these im-
480 P. F. GJERDE, J. BLOCK, AND J. H. BLOCK

TableS
Polynomial Hierarchical Regression Analyses of California Adult Q-Sort (CAQ) Components: Male Subjects

Increment in %
% of variance of variance
CAQ components and model R explained (R2) F df explained F" sr»

Interpersonal Antagonism
CES-D linear component .46 .21 10.44" 1,39
Quadratic component .62 .39 12.05**' 2,38 .18 10.98** .42
Unrestraint
CES-D linear component .48 .23 11.90** 1,39
Quadratic component .54 .30 8.02** 2,38 .07 3.40* .25
Discontentment With Self
CES-D linear component .47 .22 11.23** 1,39
Quadratic component .51 .26 6.73** 2,38 .04 1.94 .19
Unconventionality
CES-D linear component .42 .17 8.26** 1,39
Quadratic component .46 .21 5.14** 2,38 .04 1.85 .20

Note. CES-D = Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.


" F value associated with increment in R2. k The semipartial correlation represents the contribution of the quadratic component after the linear
component has been semipartialled.
V < .10. **p < .01. ***p< .001.

proved, data-reducing measures, we again related the CES-D predicted as a function only of the underlying linear relation, as
scale score to CAQ evaluations. if the absence of even slight depressive tendencies frees these
Multiple polynomial regression analyses: Male subjects. For young men to be more aggressive, assertive, and self-indulgent.
each component measure, the CES-D scale score and then the However, because the quadratic component is superimposed on
second-degree power polynomial (i.e., the quadratic component an underlying linear trend, male adolescents receiving low CES-
term) were entered sequentially as predictors. As noted earlier, D scores are not seen, in an absolute sense, to be more antago-
this approach permits a test for the existence and strength of nistic or more unrestrained than male adolescents scoring in
linear association, followed by a test of whether the quadratic the middle range on the CES-D scale.
nonlinear component adds significant incremental variance. Multiple polynomial regression analyses: Female subjects. In
Tests for nonlinearity were included for observer CAQ evalua- an equivalent analysis for the female sample, CES-D scores were
tions to examine the shape of the relation between privately ex- linearly unrelated to Ego-Brittleness (r = .08, ns) but were lin-
perienced dysthymia and social stimulus value. Table 3 summa- early related to Unconventionality (r = .42, p < .01) and to
rizes the results surrounding the composite measures for the Rumination (r = .32, p < .05). The addition of the quadratic
male sample. nonlinear component significantly increased the amount
CES-D scores were linearly associated in adolescent boys of variance accounted for with respect to Ego-Brittleness, F(l,
with observer evaluations of Interpersonal Antagonism (r = .46, 43) = 8.30, p < .01, Unconventionality, F(l, 43) = 7.82, p <
p < .01), Unrestraint (r = .48, p < .01), Discontentment With .01, and Rumination, P(l, 43) = 8.94, p < .01. The results of
Self (r = .47, p < .01), and Unconventionality (r = .42, p < .01). these analyses are reported in Table 4.
However, the linear model is not sufficiently descriptive of the Relative to female adolescents scoring in the middle range on
underlying relation. The quadratic aspect of the regression the CES-D scale, relatively low and relatively high scorers were
function significantly increased the variance explained for In- seen as more ego-brittle. The scatterplot of the relation between
terpersonal Antagonism, F(l, 38) = 10.98, p < .01, and, mar- CES-D scale scores and Ego-Brittleness revealed a nonlinear
ginally, for Unrestraint, F(l, 38) = 3.40,p < .07. heteroscedastic score configuration, a pattern also known as
Inspection of the scatter plots revealed that when CES-D "the twisted pear." This configuration, characterized by a qua-
scale scores are comparatively high, there is a relative intensifi- dratic (in this case, concave) regression component in conjunc-
cation of the behavioral mode of Interpersonal Antagonism as tion with greater variability of Ego-Brittleness scores at one (in
a function of the cumulative effect of the linear and curvilinear this case, the lower) end than at the other end of the CES-D
component: adolescent males scoring very high on the CES-D variable, suggests that behaviors are differentially predictable
scale tend to display even more Interpersonal Antagonism than in different segments of the predictor-criterion relation (Fisher,
is predicted by the underlying positive linear relation of the 1959). This differential variability suggests that young women
CES-D scale with Interpersonal Antagonism. Similar, but not scoring low on the CES-D are a more heterogeneous group than
so striking, is the curvilinear relation of the CES-D scale with young women receiving high CES-D scores.
Unrestraint. This finding is reminiscent of the agitation some- For Unconventionality and Rumination, a quadratic (con-
times observed in clinical depression. cave) relation was superimposed on the underlying linear trend.
When CES-D scale scores are relatively low, there is again Relatively high scorers on the CES-D scale were judged to be
more Interpersonal Antagonism and Unrestraint than would be more unconventional and more ruminative than would be ex-
DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS AND PERSONALITY 481

Table 4
Polynomial Hierarchical Regression Analyses of California Adult Q-Sort (CAQ) Components: Female Subjects

Increment in %
% of variance of variance
CAQ components and model R explained 0«2) F df explained (R2) F" sr"

Ego-Brittleness
CES-D linear component .08 .00 .26 1,44
Quadratic component .41 .16 4.30* 2,43 .16 8.30** .40
Unconventionality
CES-D linear component .42 .18 9.4 !•* 1,44
Quadratic component .52 .27 5.30* 2,43 .09 7.82** .30
Rumination
CES-D linear component 32 .10 5.07* 1,44
Quadratic component .51 .26 7.42** 2,43 .16 8.94** .39

Note. CES-D = Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression.


" F value associated with increment in RJ.b The semipartial correlation represents the contribution of the quadratic component after the linear
component has been semipartialled.
*p<.05.**p<.0l.

pected on the basis of the underlying linear relation, reflecting p< .01), more easily upset (r= .38, p< .01), more shy (r= .29,
an apparent exacerbation or potentiation of these tendencies. p < .05), and as relatively less energetic (r = -.39, p < .01), less
Relatively low scorers on the CES-D scale also were evaluated as orderly (r = -.39, p < .01), and less trusting (r = -.35, p < .05).
more unconventional and ruminative than would be expected These are all qualities, self-recognized and self-influencing, but
simply on the basis of the linear relations observed. Also in re- simultaneously unapparent (withheld?) to the observers of these
gard to these two latter personality characteristics, greater vari- female adolescents in interaction.
ability characterized the subgroup of young women receiving
low CES-D scores compared with the subgroup scoring in the
MPQ Scales Related to Depressive Symptoms (CES-D
upper range.
Scores)

Adjective Self-Descriptions Related to Depressive Table 5 presents the correlations between the CES-D scores
Symptoms (CES-D Scores) and MPQ self-evaluations. Among young men, relatively high
CES-D scores were associated with relatively high scores on
To evaluate the self-concept of dysthymic young men and
women, CES-D scale scores were related to self-attributed ad-
jective descriptions obtained at age 18. In a first analysis, a two-
way Sex X Depressive Symptoms ANOVA was conducted, sepa- Table 5
rately for each AQ item, to identify possible interactions. In Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ)
contrast to results for observer-based CAQ items, Sex X Depres- Correlates of Depressive Symptoms (Centerfor
sive Symptoms interactions were not observed for self-attrib- Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scores) at Age 18
uted AQ characteristics.
Female subjects Male subjects
In a correlational analysis, CES-D scores were related, sepa-
MPQ scales (AT =46) (JV=41)
rately for each sex, to AQ self-descriptions. In 18-year-olds of
both sexes, depressive symptoms were associated with negative Primary MPQ scales
self-descriptions. Relative to nondepressed young men, young Stress Reaction .52**** .58****
Aggression .37** .42***
men with depressive tendencies described themselves as rela-
Alienation .34** .51****
tively more rebellious (r = .40, p < .01), more shy (r = .32, p < Absorption .32** .18
.05), more masculine (r = .31, p < .05), and also as relatively Well-Being -.58**** -.46***
less helpful (r = -.42, p < .01), less considerate (r = -.41, p < Traditionalism -.42*** -.11
.01), less cheerful (r = -.40, p < .01), less playful (r = -.32, Control -.41*** .03
Social Closeness -.25* 17*
p < .05), and less energetic (r = -.31, p < .05). These self-
Achievement -.23 .00
recognized personality qualities were, in their essence, recog- Social Potency -.22 .00
nized by the Q-sort observers from their interaction with Harmavoidance -.09 .02
these young men. Higher order MPQ factors
Negative Aflectivity .47**** .67****
Relative to nondepressed young women, young women with
Positive ArFectivity -.40*** -.18
depressive tendencies described themselves as relatively more Constraint _41«*« .06
restless(r= .55,p<.001),mor y edistractible(r= .53,p<.001),
more self-centered (r = .44, p < .01), more rebellious (r = .38, *p <. 10. **p < .05. ***p < .01. ****p < .001.
482 P. F. GJERDE, J. BLOCK, AND J. H. BLOCK

MPQ Stress Reaction, MPQ Aggression, MPQ Alienation, and servers or, alternatively, they may have been relatively insensi-
MPQ Negative Affectivity scales. They were also inversely asso- tive to the intrapersonal anguish experienced by these dysthy-
ciated with scores on the MPQ Well-Being scale. mic male adolescents because of their interpersonal unpleasant-
Among young women, relatively high CES-D scores were as- ness.
sociated with relatively high scores on MPQ Stress Reaction, Female 18-year-olds with depressive tendencies were not ob-
MPQ Aggression, MPQ Alienation, MPQ Absorption, and served by Q-sorters to be aggressive or to have poor interper-
MPQ Negative Affectivity scales. They were also inversely asso- sonal relations; however, in their self-perceptions, dysthymic
ciated with MPQ Well-Being, MPQ Traditionalism, MPQ Con- young women pictured themselves as being relatively aggres-
trol, MPQ Constraint, and MPQ Positive Affectivity scales. sive, unrestrained, and alienated from others. These discrepan-
None of the MPQ correlations are significantly different for the cies between what was observed by the judges and what was
sexes when examined via McNemar's (1969) formula for com- privately expressed by the female adolescents suggest a ten-
paring independent correlations. dency in the young women to withhold or internalize depres-
sion-related feelings that are aversive to others. Whereupon,

Self-Esteem Related to Depressive Symptoms (CES-D because of these gender differences in the expression of depres-
sive affect, dysthymic young men and women may evoke differ-
Scores)
ent reactions from others (see also Hammen & Peters, 1978).
In the male sample, depressive symptoms were unrelated to
self-esteem (r = —.06, ns). However, in the female sample, de- Gender Differences in Adolescents' Depressive
pressive symptoms were strongly associated with low scores on Manifestations
the self-esteem index (r = -.48, p < .001). The difference be-
tween these two correlations is significant at the .05 level. Our results suggest that young men and young women differ
in their manifestations of depressive affect. Male adolescents
with depressive tendencies appear more likely to act on their
Discussion
world with aggressive, externalized behaviors. Depressive tend-
Before proceeding to a discussion of our findings, we ac- encies in female adolescents, by contrast, are more likely to be
knowledge possible concerns regarding the large number of sig- associated with introspection and a concern with the adequacy
nificant tests calculated during the course of our analyses. With of self, perhaps even self-dislike. It will be remembered that
so many tests, are we reporting primarily relations based on among the female 18-year-olds, depressive tendencies went
Type I errors? We think not. along with low self-esteem. Not so for male 18-year-olds. (In
There is much misunderstanding in psychology regarding evaluating these results, the reader may also consult two related,
how to interpret multiple significance tests. In practice, chance prospective studies of the childhood and preadolescent person-
expectations generally are not what they are commonly as- ality antecedents of adolescent dysthymia: J. Block & Gjerde,
sumed to be (J. Block, 1960; Pollard & Richardson, 1987). in press; Gjerde & J. Block, 1988.)
Moreover, in reflexively protecting against Type I errors, psy- These several findings may be seen as following from long rec-
chologists are often without protection against Type II errors ognized gender differences in socialization emphases and from
that it may be more crucial to avoid (J. Block, 1960; Rosnow & the different self and world views that our culture fosters in men
Rosenthal, 1988). Beyond these recognitions, we suggest that and women. Boys more than girls are socialized to develop a
the present set of relations, deriving from several data sources, sense of instrumental competence and mastery. Girls, on the
by their convergence provide support for their believability. other hand, are less likely to develop a sense of efficacy upon
Replication is of course in order, but given current knowledge, which later instrumental competence depends (J. H. Block,
the present findings warrant interest and interpretation. 1983). Boys are therefore encouraged to rely upon assertive,
Returning now to the implications of our findings, male ado- even aggressive behaviors to obtain important life objectives
lescents with depressive tendencies in late adolescence were, as (e.g., Eron, 1980); girls are taught to be diffident, self-evaluat-
anticipated, characterized by independent observers as mani- ing, even self-effacing. Thus, because relative to boys, girls are
festing an externalizing pattern of characteristics: they were rel- less likely to be socialized toward an active and mastery-ori-
atively antagonistic, unrestrained, manifestly discontented with ented relation to the environment, dysthymic young women
self, and unconventional in thought and behavior. Consistent may therefore be more likely to turn inward—to become self-
with these personality evaluations by observers, these dysthy- focused, passive, and introspectively concerned with self. The
mic young men in their self-perceptions also described them- absence of overt expressions of aggression in dysthymic young
selves as being relatively aggressive and alienated from their women may also be seen as devolving from the early social sanc-
social surround. However, beyond these similarities between tions imposed on girls who are overt in their expression of ag-
manifest behavioral characteristics and self-reported char- gressive affect. Our gender-differentiated results are also com-
acteristics, there are some notable discrepancies. Although not patible with the recent view that men tend to respond to depres-
recognized by observers, these young men viewed themselves sion by engaging in action whereas depressed women tend to
on MPQ self-report scales as prone to worry, vulnerable, and resort to rumination, analysis, and contemplative self-concern
without a sense of psychological well-being. Why did the ob- (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987), and with the finding that depressed
servers not perceive these self-recognitions? Perhaps the nature women are more likely than depressed men to be characterized
of the assessment situation limited the perceptivity of the ob- by self-dislike (Hammen & Padesky, 1977).
DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS AND PERSONALITY 483

An alternative way of construing these findings is in terms with depressive tendencies see themselves as aggressive, the
of gender differences in the tendency to perceive the world as open expression of aggression is strongly moderated by gender.
noncontingent, that is, a sense of helplessness in controlling the Among dysthymic male adolescents, hostility and aggression
environment. It has been suggested that women experience are externalized—an extrapunitive behavior pattern seemingly
higher levels of helplessness than men do (Nolen-Hoeksema, inconsistent with the classical psychoanalytic position. Also, as
1987; Radloff, 1980). McCranie (1971) has proposed that if male dysthymic adolescents develop more manifest depressive
feelings of hopelessness become a prominent component of the tendencies, there is further potentiation of their antagonistic
depressive experience, there is a greater tendency to submit to tendencies.
depression without overt expressions of anger and hostility, pri- Among dysthymic female adolescents, behavioral manifesta-
marily because the actively defensive functions of anger and tions of aggression were not discernible, but aggressive affects
hostility are perceived as unlikely to succeed. Feelings of help- were nonetheless consciously accessible to these young
lessness should have similar consequences, because prolonged women—a pattern also seemingly incompatible with the psy-
helplessness may lead to a sense of hopelessness. Owing to their choanalytic position that has emphasized in depressives the re-
greater likelihood of experiencing a sense of helplessness, young pressed or unconscious quality of hostility or aggression (e.g.,
women with depressive tendencies may therefore be less likely Abraham, 1927).
to externalize hostile and aggressive affects than are young men Because our findings are correlational, they do not permit
with depressive tendencies. Finally, it follows that depressed in- claims about causality. The following conjectures are therefore
dividuals who believe that their helplessness is personally based presented as suggestions only. First, hostile and aggressive
rather than externally based can be expected to have lower self- affects may be secondary to depression—they may either be re-
esteem (Abramson et al., 1978). If women are more likely to actions to or (albeit only partially successful) attempts to cope
experience helplessness than are men, then it follows that an with depressive experiences (or both). This position is similar
inverse association between self-esteem and depressive tenden- to that adopted by Fenichel (1945), who viewed antisocial tend-
cies is more likely to obtain in the female sample than in the encies and undercontrolled behaviors as serving to ward off de-
male sample—the result found in this study. pression, because these behaviors satisfy narcissistic needs (see
We note that for both young women and young men, depres- also Balint, 1952; McCranie, 1971; Schless et al., 1974). Alter-
sive symptoms were related to Negative Affectivity as measured natively, we cannot exclude the possibility that hostile and ag-
via the MPQ. For young women only, depressive symptoms cor- gressive affects precede, in some form, depressive tendencies.
related negatively with MPQ-measured Positive Affectivity. De- Because aggressive behaviors are aversive to others, they may
pressive states are thought to be characterized by a distinctly elicit negative reactions. Hence, supportive feedback may be
low positive-affect component and so female adolescents seem less likely to be forthcoming, resulting in the possible loss of
to respond to the CES-D Scale as a true measure of depression. social reinforcers (Ferster, 1973). The ensuing social isolation
Male adolescents, by contrast, may be said to respond to this may give rise to depressive tendencies. Or it may be that the
scale as if it were a measure of aversive affects other than dysthy- negative irrational thinking style that appears to accompany de-
mia, such as generalized distress or anger. It may well be that pression leads to increased aggression and hostility because it
emotional crises among female adolescents are centered around creates unrealistic and oversensitive interpretations of other
issues of communion versus isolation, with depression occur- people's behavior.
ring when communion is not attainable. For male adolescents The "acting out" and delinquent behaviors of depressed ado-
with depressive symptoms, on the other hand, the theme of frus- lescents have been said to mask depressive experiences (Toolan,
trated agency is more likely to dominate, resulting in high nega- 1962, 1974; see also Chwast, 1961; Glaser, 1967; Lesse, 1974;
tive affect (A. Tellegen, personal communication, May, 1988). Weiner, 1975). The concept of "masked depression" generally
The lowered positive affectivity seen among young women is implies, however, that the depressive experience is hidden from
also consistent with views of depression emphasizing inability both the individual's self-recognitions and from others (Blatt
to affectively respond to pleasure (Klein, 1974) and blockage & Shichman, 1981). Because the externalization of dysthymic
of reward-seeking behavior and loss of reinforcer effectiveness affect seen among the young men in this study takes place in the
(Eastman, 1976). context of subjectively recognized depressive experiences, our
results do not appear to support the notion of "masked" de-
pression as usually defined.
The Role of Hostility and Aggression in Depression
A final observation regarding gender differences is the degree
The role of hostility in the psychodynamics of depression re- to which depressive symptoms are coordinated with overt hos-
mains problematic. Although classical psychoanalytic theory tility and aggression. It is possible that the discrepancies be-
held that hostility (or aggression) was suppressed, or directed tween self-report and observable behavior characterizing dys-
inwards in depression (Abraham, 1927; Freud, 1968), more re- thymic young women in regard to these negative affects are lim-
cent evidence has cast doubt on this proposition (e.g., Fried- ited to specific interpersonal relationships. For example,
man, 1970; Gershon, Cromer, & Klerman, 1968; Gottschalk, Weissman et al. (1971) reported that depressed women, al-
Glesser, & Springer, 1963; Kahn, Coyne, & Margolin, 1985; though not hostile during an initial interview, expressed consid-
Schless, Mendels, Kipperman, & Cochrane, 1974; Weissman, erable hostility in close family relationships. This kind of situa-
Klerman, & Paykel, 1971; Wessman, Ricks, & Tyl, 1960). Our tion-dependent hostility and aggression may also characterize
results suggest that although both male and female 18-year-olds our female subjects in that their privately experienced aggres-
484 P. F. GJERDE, J. BLOCK, AND J. H. BLOCK

sion, although inaccessible to observers in this study, may none- relationship of personality to affective disorders. Archives of General
theless be more overtly manifested in intimate relationships. Psychiatry, 40, 801-810.
Arieti, S., & Bemporad, J. (1980). The psychological organization of
depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1360-1365.
Methodological and Substantive Implications of Balint, M. (1952). New beginning and the paranoid and depressive syn-
Curvilinearity in Relations Between Depression and dromes. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 33, 214-224.
Personality Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical
aspects. New York: Hoeber.
The concave relations observed between CES-D scale scores Bemporad, J., & Wilson, A. (1978). A developmental approach to de-
and independent personality evaluations are particularly inter- pression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of the American
esting. Male 18-year-olds scoring either very low or very high on Academy of Psychoanalysis, 6, 325-352.
Blatt, S. J., D'Afflitti, J. P., & Quinlan, D. M. (1976). Experiences of
the CES-D scale were described as relatively more interperson-
depression in normal adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85,
ally antagonistic than would be expected on the basis of linear
383-389.
relations alone. Compared with female adolescents earning
Blatt, S. J., & Shichman, S. (1981). Antisocial behavior and peraonality
CES-D scores in the middle range, female adolescents scoring organization. In S. Tuttman, C. Kaye, & M. Zimmerman (Eds.), Ob-
high or low were also seen as relatively more ego-brittle, more ject and self: A developmental approach (pp. 325-367). New York:
unconventional, and more ruminative than linear relations International Universities Press.
alone would predict. These results suggest that some adoles- Block, J. (1960). On the number of findings expected by chance. Psy-
cents with low CES-D scale scores, perhaps young women in chometrika, 25, 369-380.
particular, are so committed to maintaining a certain self-un- Block, J. (1963). The equivalence of measures and the correction for
derstanding or public image (or both) that they fail to acknowl- attenuation. Psychological Bulletin, 60, 152-156.
edge depressive tendencies (to themselves or to others). Ragrant Block, J. (1964). Recognizing attenuation effects in the strategy of re-
search. Psychological Bulletin, 62,214-216.
absence of normally expectable dysthymic symptoms thus may
Block, J. (1978). The Q-sort method in personality assessment and psy-
be an important sign that further inquiry is warranted. It should
chiatric research. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
be possible and theoretically important to distinguish (a) truly
(Original work published by C. C. Thomas, 1961)
nondepressed individuals from (b) those who deliberately re-
Block, J., & Gjerde, P. F. (in press). Depressive symptomatology in late
port an absence of depressed moods for reasons of public im- adolescence: A longitudinal perspective on personality antecedents.
pression management, and (c) those who without deliberation In J. E. Rolf, A. Masten, D. Cicchetti, K. Neuchterlein, & S. Wein-
or self-awareness incorrectly deny in themselves the presence of traub (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psycho-
dysthymic affects. This last group of deniers seems particularly pathology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
important to study (Boyd et al., 1982; Radloff & Locke, 1985), Block, J., & Thomas, H. (1955). Is satisfaction with self a measure of
to delineate the strategies used to avoid disturbing self-cogni- adjustment? Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 254-
tions, and to study the developmental pathways leading to de- 259.
Block, J. H. (1976). Issues, problems, and pitfalls in assessing sex
nial of depression as compared to frankly acknowledged de-
differences: A critical review of The psychology of sex differences.
pression.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 22, 283-308.
Investigators of depression should seek closer understanding
Block, J. H. (1979). Another look at sex differentiation in the socializa-
of the psychological processes underlying scores on the depres- tion behavior of mothers and fathers. In J. Sherman & F. L. Denmark
sion inventories that are generally used. In particular, they (Eds.), Psychology of women: Future directions of research (pp. 31-
should be alert to the possibility that seemingly similar response 87). New York: Psychological Dimensions.
patterns—among low scorers in particular—may have impor- Block, J. H. (1983). Differential premises arising from difierential so-
tantly different psychological implications. Finally, when low cialization of the sexes: Some conjectures. Child Development, 54,
and high scoring individuals are to be compared with respect 1335-1354.
to other relevant psychological dimensions or measures of inter- Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resil-
est, it behooves the researcher to examine empirically and to iency in the organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), The
Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39-101).
consider conceptually the shape of the relation between the
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
variables being related. If this relation is found to be or is ex-
Boyd, J. H., Weissman, M. M., Thompson, W. D., & Myere, J. K.
pected to be curvilinear, analyses other than simple extreme
(1982). Screening for depression in a community sample. Archives of
group comparisons will be required.
General Psychiatry. 39, 1195-1200.
Bradburn, N. M., Ripo, L. J., & Shevell, S. K. (1987). Answering auto-
biographical questions: The impact of memory and inference on sur-
References
veys. Science, 236, 157-161.
Abraham, K. (1927). Notes on the psycho-analytical investigation and Chodoff, P. (1970). The core problem in depression: Interpersonal as-
treatment of manic-depressive insanity and allied conditions. In K. pects. In J. H. Masserman (Ed.), Depression: Theories and therapies
Abraham (Ed.), Selected papers of psychoanalysis (pp. 137-156). (pp. 56-61). New York: Grune & Stratton.
London: The Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1911) Chodoff, P. (1972). The depressive personality. Archives of General Psy-
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned chiatry, 27, 666-673.
helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Ab- Chwast, J. (1961). Depressive reactions as manifested among adolescent
normal Psychology, 87, 49-74. delinquents. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 21, 575-584.
Akiskal, H. S., Hirschfeld, R. M. A., & Yerevanian, B. I. (1983). The Cicchetti, D., & Schneider-Rosen, K. (1986). An organizational ap-
DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS AND PERSONALITY 485

proach to childhood depression. In M. Rutter, C. E. Izard, & P. B. tudinal prospective study of characteristics and recovery. Archives of
Read (Eds.), Depression in young people (pp. 71-134). New York: General Psychiatry, 41,229-237.
Guilford Press. Kovacs, M., Feinberg, T. L., Crouse-Novak, M. A., Paulauskas, S. L.,
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correla- Pollack, M., & Finkelstein, R. (1984). Depressive disorders in child-
tional analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: hood: II. A longitudinal study of the risk for a subsequent major de-
Erlbaum. pression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 636-644.
Craig, T. J., & Van Natta, P. A. (1976). Recognition of depressed affect Lesse, S. (1974). Depressive equivalents and the multivariant masks of
in hospitalized psychiatric patients: Staff and patient perceptions. depression. In S. Lesse (Ed.), Masked depression (pp. 3-23). New
Journal of the Diseases of the Nervous System, 37, 561-566. York: Jason Aronson.
Eastman, C. (1976). Behavioral formulations of depression. Psychologi- Lin, N., Dean, A., & Ensel, W. M. (1986). Social support, life events,
cal Review, 83, 277-291. and depression. New "York: Academic Press.
Ebata, A. T., & Peterson, A. C. (1988). Patterns of adjustment during Malmquist, C. (197 la). Depression in childhood and adolescence: I.
early adolescence: Gender differences in depression and achievement. New England Journal of Medicine, 284, 887-893.
Manuscript submitted for publication. Malmquist, C. (1971b). Depression in childhood and adolescence: II.
Eron, L. D. (1980). Prescription for reduction of aggression. American New England Journal of Medicine, 284, 955-961.
Psychologist, 35, 244-252. McCranie, E. J. (1971). Depression, anxiety, and hostility. Psychiatric
Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New \fork: Quarterly, 45, 117-133.
W. W. Norton. McNemar, Q. (1960). Sense and nonsense. American Psychologist, 15,
Ferster, C. B. (1973). A functional analysis of depression. American Psy- 295-300.
chologist, 28, 857-870. McNemar, Q. (1969). Psychological statistics. New York: Wiley.
Fisher, J. (1959). The twisted pear and the prediction of behavior. Jour- Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Ev-
nal of Consulting Behavior, 23, 400-405. idence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101,259-282.
Pollard, P., & Richardson, J. T. E. (1987). On the probability of making
Freud, S. (1968). Mourning and melancholia. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The
Type I errors. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 159-163.
standard edition of the complete works ofSigmund Freud(Vol. 14, pp.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale
243-258). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1917)
for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Mea-
Friedman, A. S. (1970). Hostility factors and clinical improvement in
surement, 3, 385-401.
depressed patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 23, 524-537.
Radloff, L. S. (1980). Risk factors for depression: What do we learn
Gershon, E. S., Cramer, M., & Klerman, G. L. (1968). Hostility and
from them? In M. Guttentag, S. Salasin, & D. Belle (Eds.), The men-
depression. Psychiatry, 31.224-235.
ial health of women (pp. 93-108). New \brk: Academic Press.
Gjerde, P. F. & Block, J. (1988). Preadolescent antecedents of depressive
Radloff, L. S., & Locke, B. Z. (1985). The community mental health
symptomatology at age 18: A prospective study. Manuscript submit-
assessment survey and the CES-D scale. In M. M. Weissman, J. K.
ted for publication.
Myers, & C. E. Ross (Eds.), Community surveys of psychiatric disor-
Glaser, K. (1967). Masked depression in children and adolescents.
ders (pp. 177-189). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
American Journal of Psychotherapy, 21,565-574.
Radloff, L. S., & Rae, D. S. (1979). Susceptibility and precipitating fac-
Gottschalk, L. A, Glesser, G. C., & Springer, K. J. (1963). Three hostil-
tors in depression: Sex differences and similarities. Journal of Abnor-
ity scales applicable to verbal samples. A rchives of General Psychiatry,
mal Psychology, 88, 174-181.
9, 254-279.
Robbins, D. R., & Kashani, J. H. (1986). Depression in adolescence. In
Hammen, C. L., & Padesky, C. A. (1977). Sex differences in the expres-
R. A. Feldman & A. B. Stiffman (Eds.), Advances in adolescent men-
sion of depressive responses on the Beck Depression Inventory. Jour-
tal health (Vol. 1, Part B, pp. 59-89). New York: JAI Press.
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 609-614.
Roberts, R. E. (1980). Reliability of the CES-D scale in different ethnic
Hammen, C. L., & Peters, S. D. (1978). Interpersonal consequences of contexts. Psychiatry Research, 2, 125-134.
depression: Responses to men and women enacting a depressed role. Roberts, R. E., & Vernon, S. W. (1983). The Center for Epidemiological
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 322-332. Studies depression scale: Its use in a community sample. American
Hirschfeld, R. M., & Cross, C. K. (1982). Epidemiology of affective Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 4-46.
disorders: Psychosocial risk factors. Archives of General Psychiatry, Rogers, C., & Dymond, R. (1954). Psychotherapy and personality
39, 35-46. change. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Chodoff, P., Korchin, S., & Bar- Rosnow, R. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Focused tests of significance and
rett, J. (1976). Dependency—self-esteem—clinical depression. Jour- effect size estimation in counseling psychology. Journal of Consulting
nal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 4, 373-388. Psychology, 35, 203-208.
Kahn, J., Coyne, J. C., & Margolin, G. (1985). Depression and marital Rutter, M. (1986). The developmental psychopathology of depression:
disagreement: The social construction of despair. Journal of Social Issues and perspectives. In M. Rutter, C. E. Izard, & P. B. Read (Eds.),
and Personal Relationships, 2,447-461. Depression in young people (pp. 3-32). New \brk: Guilford Press.
Kandel, D. B., & Davies, M. (1982). Epidemiology of depressive moods Salzman, L. (1975). Interpersonal factors in depression. In F. F. Flack
in adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 1205-1212. & S. C. Draghi (Eds.), The nature and treatment of depression (pp.
Kandel, D. B., & Davies, M. (1986). Adult sequelae of adolescent de- 43-56). New York: Wiley.
pressive symptoms. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43,255-262. Schless, A. P., Mendels, J., Kipperman, A., &Cochrane, C. (1974). De-
Kaplan, A. (1986). The "self-in relation": Implications for depression pression and hostility. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
in women. Psychotherapy, 23,234-242. 159,91-100.
Klein, D. F. (1974). Endogenomorphic depression. Archives of General Schoenbach, V. J., Kaplan, B. H., Crimson, R. C., & Wagner, E. H.
Psychiatry, 31, 447-454. (1982). Use of a symptom scale to study the prevalence of a depressive
Kovacs, M., Feinberg, T. L., Crouse-Novak, M. A., Paulauskas, S. L., & syndrome in young adolescents. American Journal of Epidemiology,
Finkelstein, R. (1984). Depressive disorders in childhood: I. A longi- 116,191-799.
486 P. F. GJERDE, J. BLOCK, AND J. H. BLOCK.

Schoenbach, V. J., Kaplan, B. H., Wagner, E. H., Crimson, R. C., & Weissman, M. M., & Klerman, G. L. (1979). Sex differences and the
Miller, F. T. (1983). Prevalence of self-reported depressive symptoms etiology of depression. In E. S. Gomberg & V. Franks (Eds.), Gender
in young adolescents. American Journal of Public Health, 73, 1281- and disordered behavior (pp. 381-425). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
1287. Weissman, M. M., Klerman, G. L., & Paykel, E. S. (1971). Clinical eval-
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psy- uation of hostility in depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 128,
chopathology, Child Development, 55, 17-29. 261-266.
Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief manual for the Multidimensional Personality Weissman, M. M., Scholomskes, D., Pollenger, M., Prusoif, B. A., &
Questionnaire (MPQ). (Unpublished manuscript available from A. Locke, B. Z. (1977). Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiat-
Tellegen, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Min- ric populations: A validation study. American Journal of Epidemiol-
neapolis, MN 55455). ogy, ;06, 203-214.
Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their rele- Wells, V. E., Klerman, G. L., & Deykin, E. Y. (1987). The prevalence
vance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In A. H. of depressive symptoms in college students. Social Psychiatry, 22,18-
Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and anxiety disorders (pp. 681- 20.
706). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum. Wessman, A. E., Ricks, D. F., & Tyl, M. M. (1960). Characteristics and
Toolan, J. M. (1962). Depression in children and adolescents. American
concomitants of mood fluctuation in college students. Journal oj'Ab-
Journal of Orlhopsychiatry, 32, 404-415.
normaland Social Psychology, 60, 117-126.
Toolan, J. M. (1974). Masked depression in children and adolescents.
In S. Lesse (Ed.), Masked depression (pp. 141-164). New \brk: Jason
Aronson.
Weiner, I. B. (1975). Depression in adolescence. In F. F. Flack & S. C. Received December 21, 1987
Draghi (Eds.), The nature and treatment of depression (pp. 99-117). Revision received June 10, 1988
New York: Wiley. Accepted June 13, 1988

View publication stats

You might also like